Download Preview the material

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Heritability of IQ wikipedia , lookup

Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Developmental psychology wikipedia , lookup

Self-awareness wikipedia , lookup

Impact of health on intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Externalizing disorders wikipedia , lookup

Neurogenomics wikipedia , lookup

Heritability of autism wikipedia , lookup

Discrete trial training wikipedia , lookup

Autism therapies wikipedia , lookup

Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup

Autism spectrum wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Autism Spectrum Disorders – Part 1 Characteristics of ASD
INTRODUCTION
Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) encompass a group of developmental disorders
whose symptoms range on a continuum that runs from mild to severe in expression
[1].
ASD is typically present early in life, anytime from infancy or early childhood; however,
in the new DSM-V, there are changes in place for diagnostic age criteria in that deficits
from ASD may not be detected until later on in life. This later detection may result from
lower social demands placed on an individual as a result of assistance from parents or
caregivers earlier on in life.
The onset of ASD has been intensively studied
[2-4],
and it is largely accepted that the
time of diagnosis may occur far after the time of onset. Additionally, the time between
where parents indicate that the child shows early signs or symptoms of ASD or
abnormal development and the time until diagnosis may be quite lengthy. It is important
to note here that detecting early signs of ASD has been indicated to be quite difficult [5].
This is why ASD is sometimes only diagnosed once a child is put into situations that
require social abilities, and a lack is seen in these situations.
More than 500,000 people in the United States have some form of diagnosed autism [6].
Autism may keep a child from forming effective relationships with other people, due in
part to an inability to properly interpret facial expressions or emotions. Children with
autism spectrum disorders may be resistant to cuddling or change, and they may play
alone or have delays in speech development. People with autism also frequently repeat
body movements or have extreme attachments to certain objects. However, there are
positive aspects to autism, such as the fact that many people with autism excel on
certain mental levels, such as counting and measuring, or at art, music, or memory.
The precise causes of ASD are not known. However, it is known that genetic factors
play a role in ASD. It is also known that non-genetic, environmental factors play a role in
the development of ASD. It is thought that ASD is the result of a combination of genetic
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
1
and non-genetic factors. What basically happens is that non-genetic, environmental
factors come into play to make those who are genetically susceptible to ASD more likely
to develop ASD. ASD may then develop gradually, or the individual may experience
what is known as regression, where he or she may lose some or all of their acquired
skills. The loss of skills can happen suddenly or gradually. Overall, there are a number
of risk factors for ASD, including gender – boys are more likely than are girls to develop
ASD – genetics, certain prenatal and perinatal factors, neuroanatomical abnormalities
such as enlargement of the brain, and environmental factors.
There is no cure for ASD. The primary goals of ASD treatments are to lessen deficits
and to lessen family stresses. There are a number of ways to do these things, including
applied behavioral therapy, structured teaching, speech and language therapy, social
skills therapy, occupational therapy, and the use of medication. Additionally, educating
parents, caregivers, and siblings and providing these individuals with ways to cope with
the unique challenges that having an individual with ASD in the household brings helps
in alleviating the inevitable resulting family stress.
INCLUDED DISORDERS
Autism
The American Psychiatric Association has put forth a
new definition for autism as a part of the revisions made
for the new Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM). Autism will be seen as part of a
continuum of disorders termed autism spectrum disorder,
involving a range of disorders, such as autistic disorder,
[7]
Asperger’s syndrome, childhood disintegrative disorder,
and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified. Dr. James Scully of the
APA has stated that the criteria will “lead to more accurate diagnosis and will help
physicians and therapists design better treatment interventions” [8].
[7]
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
2
Autistic disorder is also known as autism or mindblindness. This disorder generally
presents sometime in the first three years of life, and the child appears to live within its
own world. The child also appears to show little interest in other people and displays
poor social skills. The focus of a child with autistic disorder is on a consistent routine,
with repetitive odd or peculiar behaviors. Children with autism frequently have problems
communicating, and they often will avoid making eye contact with others or will avoid
attaching to others.
Autistic disorder may be associated with a number of infirmities, including difficulties
with motor coordination and attention, intellectual disability, and physical health issues
such as gastrointestinal issues or sleep issues. However, despite difficulties, some
people with autistic disorder often excel in music, art, math, or with visual skills.
Autistic disorder seems to have roots in the early development of the brain, although the
most obvious signs and symptoms emerge between 2 – 3 years of age. Approximately
1 out of every 88 children in the United States will have an ASD; this has been a huge
increase in prevalence over the past 40 years. The increase is due in part to
improvements made in diagnostic tools as well as disease awareness. Another reason
for the increase may be environmental influence. Autistic disorder is much more
common in boys than it is in girls: approximately 1 out of every 54 boys and 1 out of
every 252 girls is diagnosed with an ASD in the U.S., with ASD affecting over 2 million
individuals in the U.S., and millions are affected worldwide.
There is no one cause for ASD, although a genetic predisposition appears to play a
role, as do environmental, or non-genetic, factors. Most cases of ASD appear to be the
result of a combination of both genetic and non-genetic factors, with environmental
stressors increasing the risk of development of ASD in children who already have a
genetic predisposition. The clearest evidence of these types of risk factors includes
events that happen before or during birth, such as maternal illness or birthing difficulties.
Asperger Syndrome
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
3
Asperger Syndrome is considered a sub-type of autism spectrum disorder. The cause of
Asperger Syndrome is unknown, and presents 4 times more frequently in boys than it
does in girls [9].
Many consider Asperger syndrome to be the mildest form of autism; therefore, many
with Asperger’s are considered to be the highest functioning autistic individuals.
However, even though many individuals with Asperger’s may be high functioning, they
still share certain key symptoms with others who suffer from autism spectrum disorders,
such as a lack of normal social conversation, lack of normal eye contact, deficits in body
language and facial expression, and difficulties in maintaining social relationships.
Those with Asperger syndrome also frequently engage in repetitive behaviors and may
become excessively attached to objects or routines.
The prevalence of Asperger syndrome is not well known. It is not even well recognized
prior to the age of 5 or 6, primarily because the individual has normal language
development. Asperger syndrome occurs in all ethnic groups and affects every age
group [10]. Additionally, it does not just affect the child during childhood; studies indicate
that children who suffer from Asperger syndrome carry their problems with them into
adulthood and some develop further psychiatric problems in adulthood.
Some of the common signs or symptoms of those with Asperger syndrome are having
monotone speech or lack of rhythm in speaking. Additionally, an individual with
Asperger syndrome may have problems modulating the volume in his or her voice and
may need to be continually reminded to talk more softly. Individuals with Asperger’s are
not generally isolated from the rest of the world as a result of their own withdrawal, but
rather they are isolated because they have poor social skills or because their interests
are narrow. For example, they may approach conversations by speaking only about
their own very narrow interests, making a normal conversation difficult.
Current research [10] indicates that brain abnormalities may be to blame, as some
research has indicated that those children who have Asperger syndrome have
differences in structure and function than do those children who are not affected.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
4
Researchers posit that these differences could be caused by an abnormal migration of
embryonic cells that in turn affects brain structure in early childhood. This then goes on
to affect the brain circuitry that affects thought and behavior. Scientists have also
suspected that there is a strong genetic component to Asperger syndrome, although no
specific gene for Asperger syndrome has ever been identified. Recent research has
indicated that Asperger syndrome is most likely the result of a grouping of genes in
which variations or deletions cause the individual to become vulnerable to the
development of Asperger syndrome. When combined with environmental factors, this
also determines the severity and the specific symptoms that each individual suffers.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) is sometimes
simply called pervasive developmental disorder, or may be used interchangeably with
autism spectrum disorder. There are some health professionals who refer to PDD-NOS
as sub threshold autism. PDD-NOS is a relatively new diagnosis, having been around
for only about 15 years. PDD-NOS is the diagnosis that has come to be commonly
applied to those who are on the autism spectrum but do not generally meet the criteria
for some other autism spectrum disorder in full, such as Asperger Syndrome.
Defining features of PDD-NOS are challenges in language development and social
functioning. Repetitive behaviors are frequently seen with PDD-NOS. Not all children
with PDD-NOS have the same symptoms. Symptoms may sometimes be mild, where
the individual exhibits only a few symptoms while at home or at school. Other
individuals may exhibit severe symptoms in all areas of their lives but still may not
qualify for a diagnosis of autistic disorder.
The signs and symptoms of PDD-NOS are divided into several categories, as outlined
below:

Social Signs and Symptoms:
Children with PDD-NOS have a desire to make and have friends, but they have
no idea how to make that happen. If there are language delays this may hinder
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
5
the ability to socialize with other people. There is frequently difficulty in
understanding body language, tone of voice, and facial expression in others, as
well as difficulty understanding emotions. Children with PDD-NOS often have
trouble distinguishing between emotions such as sadness, happiness and anger.

Communication Signs and Symptoms:
Those with PDD-NOS frequently have language or communication issues. Those
with PDD-NOS may not babble as babies. They also frequently take language
literally and do not have an understanding of things such as sarcasm or joking
language. It is easier to expand language skills in those children who have some
language skills than it is in those children who are entirely nonverbal.

Behavioral Signs and Symptoms:
Children with PDD-NOS may exhibit tantrums or emotional outbursts. These
children may also have a great need for routine. Additionally, individuals with
PDD-NOS can often misinterpret what is happening in a situation and in turn
become easily frustrated. Tantrums are the result of fear and anxiety. Another
common behavioral symptom is perseveration, which means the child will tend to
dwell on certain events or subjects. Individuals with PDD-NOS may become
fixated on one topic or play only with one toy that is associated with a particular
area.
Given the newness of this diagnosis, there has been some disagreement on how to
apply a diagnosis of PDD-NOS. Recently, some studies [11] have suggested that PDDNOS may best be utilized by placing individuals into one of three subgroupings of
diagnosis, as explained below:

High functioning PDD-NOS:
This involves approximately 25% of those with PDD-NOS); and, these individuals
generally have symptoms that overlap Asperger’s, but they may differ slightly in
that they have delays in language development or slight cognitive impairment.

Mid-function PDD-NOS:
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
6
This involves approximately 25% of those with PDD-NOS; and, these individuals
generally have symptoms that resemble those with autistic disorder.

Low function PDD-NOS:
This involves approximately 50% of those with PDD-NOS; and, these individuals
meet all of the criteria for autistic disorder, however, some of their symptoms are
noticeably mild.
Another way to diagnose PDD-NOS may be by placing individuals into one of five
subgroupings [12]:

Atypical autism: this category is for young children who may not have developed
a full-blown autistic disorder yet; these are individuals who almost, but not quite,
meet the criteria for autistic disorder.

Residual autism: this category is for individuals who have a history of having
autistic disorder yet may not at present meet the criteria; they therefore still have
some autistic features but as a result of interventions or development they do not
meet the criteria for autistic disorder.

Atypical Asperger syndrome: this category is for young children who may not
have developed full-blown Asperger syndrome as well as for individuals who are
almost, but not quite, to the point of meeting the full criteria for Asperger
syndrome.

Mixed clinical features of atypical Asperger syndrome: this category is for
children with an atypical autistic disorder.

Comorbid autism: this category is for children who have a medical or neurological
disorder, such as tuberous sclerosis, that is associated with some ASD-like
features.
Those who have PDD-NOS respond best to combined therapies. Therapies that have
been demonstrated to work well on PDD-NOS are applied behavioral analysis (ABA),
sensory integration therapy, play therapy, and social skills training. It is important for the
practitioner to pay attention to the child who may not be as easily diagnosed; the Yale
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
7
Developmental Disabilities Clinic [13] indicates that children who have PDD-NOS may
not get the treatment they need as easily as do those diagnosed with autistic disorder.
Additionally, many education systems in the United States do not have a special
education category for those individuals who have PDD-NOS, leaving these individuals
to be placed into programs designed for students with other problems, such as
intellectual disabilities, emotional disturbances, or behavior disorders. This can lead to
the individual getting lost in the crowd and not having their needs met. It is therefore
essential that the practitioner work closely with parents and caregivers to ensure that
the individual with PDD-NOS is getting his or her needs met in all areas.
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder
Childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD), also called Heller’s syndrome, is a condition
wherein children develop normally until approximately age 3 or 4. They then lose all of
the skills they have learned. This includes motor, language, social, and other skills. To
be diagnosed with CDD the child must lack or lose normal function in at least two
developmental skills areas that include: social interaction, communication, and repetitive
or stereotyped patterns of interest and behavior/activities. CDD may be caught at
developmental screenings performed at well-child check-ups; these screenings should
always be performed, although parents should also be encouraged to voice concerns
about their child’s development at other times as well. The cause of CDD is not known,
but a link to the brain and nervous system has been made [14].
Symptoms of CDD include the following: a delay or lack of language, impaired
nonverbal behavior, an inability to start or to maintain conversation, failure to play, loss
of control of the bladder or bowels, loss of previously established language or
communication skills, loss or motor or skills, and; problems forming relationships with
others have been identified. The loss of skills may occur abruptly or it may occur over a
period of time that is extended. Parents should express concerns to a practitioner when
a child loses any developmental skill, whether it is gradual or sudden loss.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
8
Outcome for CDD is poor. Many children with CDD are as severely impaired as those
with severe autistic disorder. Those with CDD almost always need support for the
duration of their lifetime and may need residential care in facilities such as group homes
or long-term care residential living. There is no cure for CDD, although there are some
treatments that may be used to lessen or relieve symptoms. These treatments include
the following:

Medication:
While there are not any medications that treat this disorder directly, some
medications may be used to treat the behaviors that result, such as anxiety or
depression. Additionally, epilepsy is highly co-occurring with CDD, and
anticonvulsant drugs may be utilized to control seizures.

Behavior therapy:
Behavior therapy programs are frequently utilized to help the individual with ASD
learn language or to help minimize language loss, as well as social skills and
self-care skills. Behavioral therapy programs use systems of reward and
discipline to reinforce behavior that is desirable and to discourage behavior that
is not desirable. It is important that the approach in behavior therapy is consistent
among all practitioners, caregivers, and teachers.
Rett Syndrome
Rett Syndrome is a neurodevelopmental disease that is seen almost entirely in females,
although it may sometimes rarely be seen in males. Rett Syndrome presents in infancy
or early childhood, and is caused by a mutation in the MECP2 gene on the X
chromosome [15]. Since boys have a different combination of chromosomes than do
females, males who have the mutation that causes the syndrome are generally affected
in ways that are devastating. Many die prior to birth or very early in infancy.
Symptoms vary in type and severity. Generally the child may appear to be developing
normally, up until about 6 months of age, and then symptoms begin to appear. This is
also when the rate of growth of the head as well as progress of certain skills such as
communication may start to slow. The most notable changes usually occur at around 12
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
9
– 18 months and occur over weeks to months. There are a number of signs and
symptoms that occur with Rett syndrome, including those listed below:

Slowed growth: the brain slows in growth following birth. One of the first signs
that a child has Rett syndrome is that the child has a smaller than usual head
size. Then, as the child ages, it becomes evident that there are delays in growth
in other body parts as well.

Loss of normal coordination and movement: the individual begins to lose a
significant amount of motor skills. This generally begins between 12 to 18 months
of age and includes a decreased ability to control the hands as well as a
decreased ability to crawl and walk in a normal way. This occurs rapidly at first
and then continues in a more gradual manner.

Loss of communication and the ability to think: individuals who have Rett
syndrome will lose their ability to communicate as well as to speak. They may
also become uninterested in toys, in people, and in their surroundings. The
change may be rapid in some children; for example, some children may
experience a sudden loss of their speech skills. Most children regain skills such
as eye contact over time as well as develop skills such as nonverbal
communication.

Abnormal hand movements: children who have Rett syndrome have stereotyped
hand patterns individual to each child; these may include wringing, clapping,
rubbing, squeezing, or tapping.

Unusual eye movements: those with Rett syndrome may have eye movements
that are unusual, such as blinking, staring intensely, or closing one eye.

Breathing problems: individuals with Rett syndrome may have breathing
problems that include apnea, forceful exhalation of air (or saliva), or rapid
breathing that is abnormal. These types of problems are likely to occur during
waking hours but not during sleep hours.

Irritability: Rett syndrome may cause individuals to become more and more
irritable and agitated as they age, having spells of screaming that can begin
suddenly and last for hours.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
10

Abnormal behaviors: abnormal behaviors may include sudden and odd facial
expressions or extended bouts of screaming or laughter that occurs for no
reason. Abnormal behaviors also include such behaviors as hand licking or
grasping clothing or hair.

Seizures: many who have Rett syndrome also experience seizures at some point
in their lifetime. The symptoms of these seizures vary, but they can range from
having a periodic muscle spasm to being struck with full-blown epilepsy.

Abnormal spinal curvature: this is also known as scoliosis. Scoliosis commonly
co-occurs with Rett syndrome, and generally begins at around 8 – 11 years of
age.

Irregular heartbeat, or dysrhythmia: many individuals with Rett syndrome
experience this life-threatening issue.

Constipation: constipation is a common issue for those who suffer from Rett
syndrome.
Rett syndrome is generally divided into 4 stages:

Stage I: In this stage the signs and symptoms of the disease may be easily
overlooked, as this stage begins between 6 – 18 months of age. Children who
are in this stage may begin to show less eye contact or begin to lose interest in
their toys. There may also be delays in sitting up or in crawling.

Stage II: Stage II occurs between 1 – 4 years of age. In this stage the child
begins to lose his or her speaking ability as well as the ability to use his or her
hands. Additionally, repetitive and purposeless hand motions begin. Some
children also start to hyperventilate or hold their breath as well as cry or scream
for no reason. It is also frequently difficult for the child to move on his or her own.

Stage III: Stage III is considered a plateau that starts between 2 – 10 years of
age. This stage may last for years. Even though issues with movement may
continue, behavior can improve. Children in this stage frequently cry less often,
and become less irritable.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
11

Stage IV: This last stage shows extremely reduced mobility as well as muscle
weakness and scoliosis. Communication skills, understanding, and hand skills
usually won’t decline any further in this stage.
Most people who suffer from Rett Syndrome require daily assistance with everyday
tasks. They can usually live moderately long lives, extending to 50 years of age or
longer.
EARLY ETIOLOGY
The precise causes of ASD are not known. However, many think that it is a complex
combination of genetic and environmental components that cause ASD symptoms.
ASD causes may be described in two ways: [16]
Primary ASD, which is also known as idiopathic ASD. This means that there is no
underlying medical condition, which can explain why there are symptoms of ASD.
Ninety percent of all ASD cases are primary ASD.
Secondary ASD, which means that there is an underlying medical condition that is
thought to be responsible – or at the very least, partially responsible – for the ASD
symptoms. Ten percent of all ASD cases are secondary ASD.
Primary ASD
Researchers have examined four possible causes to primary ASD. These causes are
outlined below:
Genetic causes:
There are certain genetic mutations, which may lead to a child being more likely to
develop ASD. ASD has been known to run in families, and there is 5 – 6% likelihood
that younger children born into families with an older child with ASD will also themselves
have ASD. Identical twins are also at risk for developing ASD. For example, if one twin
develops ASD, there is 60% likelihood that the other will develop ASD as well. However,
it is important to note that there are currently no specific genes that have been linked to
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
12
the development of ASD, and there is no way to currently test for the genetic
predisposition toward ASD.
Environmental causes:
A child may be exposed to certain environmental factors during the mother’s pregnancy
that may lead to the development of ASD. Some researchers think that ASD is caused
more by environment than by genetics, and that certain people may be born with a
predisposition toward ASD that is only triggered if exposed to certain environmental
stimuli. Some of the suggested environmental stimuli include: a mother who had a viral
or bacterial infection while pregnant; a mother who smoked while pregnant; an older
father; air pollution; and, pesticide exposure.
There is evidence to support some of these environmental factors. For example, women
who were exposed to the rubella infection while they were pregnant have a 7% risk of
giving birth to a child who develops an ASD. Additionally, women who smoked while
pregnant were 40% more likely to have a child who developed an ASD. Also, fathers
over 40 years of age were 6 times more likely to have a child who developed an ASD.
Researchers posit that this may be the case because a father’s genetic material
becomes more vulnerable to mutation as he ages. There is less evidence to support the
idea that air pollution or pesticide exposure causes the development of ASD; however,
studies are currently examining this idea under the CHARGE study [16].
Psychological causes:
A child may have thought processes that contribute to the expression of symptoms of
ASD. A great deal of the research behind the psychological factors that may contribute
to ASD is rooted in a psychological concept called “theory of mind” (TOM) [16]. TOM is
an individual’s ability to understand others’ emotional states; and, at its core involves
seeing the world through the eyes of the other person. The majority of children who do
not have ASD possess a full understanding of TOM by the time they are approximately
4 years of age. Children who have ASD possess a limited or no understanding of TOM.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
13
This may be one of the causes of the social problems that children with ASD
experience.
Neurological causes:
There are certain problems with how the brain and nervous system develops that may
contribute to ASD symptoms. A great deal of the research into neurological causes has
focused on the amygdala, which is the section of the brain that matches emotions to the
situation the individual is placed in. The amygdala selects emotional responses from the
limbic system and relays them to the cerebral cortex.
Brain studies that have been conducted on people with ASD indicate that the
connections between these systems are not fully functional. The result is that people
with ASD can suddenly experience extreme emotional reactions even to trivial objects
or events. This may explain why people who have ASD favor routines, since routines
and patterns tend not to promote extreme responses. An additional area of research
has been focused on mirror neurons, which enable an individual to mirror another
individual’s actions. For example, a mirror neuron is what allows a baby to smile in
reaction to a mother’s smile. Mirror neurons create more elaborate pathways in the
brain that may contribute to higher brain functions such as language, learning from
others, and the ability to recognize emotional states in others as an individual grows
older. Studies of children with ASD have discovered that mirror neurons in ASD
individuals do not respond in the ways they do those without ASD. Difficulties with
mirror neurons may contribute to the problems that some individuals with ASD
experience with learning and social interaction.
Secondary ASD
Sometimes another medical condition may be the cause of ASD. Some of these
conditions that can cause symptoms of ASD are listed below:
Fragile X syndrome:
This is an uncommon condition that occurs more frequently in boys than it does in girls.
It occurs in about 1 in every 3600 boys, and in about 1 in every 6000 girls [16], and
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
14
presents with certain distinct characteristics such as a long face, larger ears, and
flexible joints.
Tuberous sclerosis:
This is a rare condition that results in multiple tumors to grow throughout the body. The
tumors are not cancerous. This condition occurs in approximately 1 in every 6000
children.
Rett syndrome: This condition, also discussed above, is included here as a rare
secondary ASD and almost always occurs in girls. It causes extreme difficulty with
physical movement, and the individual suffering from Rett syndrome nearly always
requires full assistance with daily tasks. Approximately 1 in every 20,000 girls has Rett
syndrome.
RECENT CHANGES TO THE DSM-V
There have been several refinements made to the diagnostic criteria found in the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM), with suggestions based on limitations found in
previous diagnostic criteria [1]. The fourth edition of the DSM (DSM-IV) contained a large
number of diagnoses [17], including a large number of not otherwise specified (NOS)
diagnoses. Additionally, practice has changed in recent years to include the
consideration of the contribution of various comorbidities.
Autism Spectrum Disorders in the DSM-V
ASD in the DSM-V is an umbrella term that will include such disorders as pervasive
developmental disorders, autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, childhood disintegrative
disorder, and not otherwise specified disorders. ASD’s will consist of these groups of
developmental disorders that are seen on a continuum that ranges from mild to severe
and present in infancy or early childhood. However, in the new DSM-V, the age criteria
for diagnosis will be different; it will not be specified that diagnosis must occur during
childhood and that diagnosis may occur later on in life. Because caregivers might
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
15
compensate for a child’s lack of social intelligence, in school and other social
interactions, a diagnosis of ASD might not be made until later in the child’s life.
Onset of ASD has been extensively studied [2-4], and it is accepted by the majority of
practitioners that diagnosis may occur much later than the time the disorder begins
presenting itself. Additionally, parents and caregivers do not always acknowledge
symptoms immediately as signs of a problem, and the time at which they acknowledge
symptoms is important as well. It is not always easy to detect specific signs and
symptoms of ASD, particularly the early ones [5]. This aspect of ASD is, in particular,
important to the changes in the new DSM-V because onset of symptoms prior to age 3
has been removed. The deletion of the age criteria in the diagnosis of ASD in general
highlights the fact that some individuals are diagnosed later in life, as adolescents or
adults.
It has been suggested that the presence of language delays not be utilized as criteria
for applying a diagnosis of autistic disorder. This is because it has been well established
that a language delay is not a symptom that is specific to ASD. Further, children who
suffer from ASD may develop fluency of speech as they grow even if they suffered from
language delay as a younger child. However, it is important to note that the
development of language is crucial where it regards outcome because a severe
language delay in early childhood seems to predict poor outcome [18].
Considerations regarding specific disorders in the DSM-V are outlined below:
Asperger disorder:
A highly debated change to the revision of the DSM-V is the suggestion to remove
Asperger syndrome. However, the suggestion to remove Asperger syndrome is based
on study evidence that indicates that there is no clear difference between Asperger’s
and autistic disorder where outcome is concerned [1].
Disintegrative disorder:
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
16
While the validity of disintegrative disorder has been debated recently, the debate
focuses on the problems that stem from precisely defining developmental regressions
as well as pinpointing the time of onset of the regression. Additionally, there is the
problem of defining whether developmental delays were present prior to regression.
Further, consideration has been given to whether symptoms or changes present
gradually or suddenly. Due to these ambiguities, the DSM-V therefore suggests removal
of the category of childhood disintegrative disorders.
A goal of this new criterion is to stabilize validity of diagnostic criteria across types of
ASD. Earlier studies have indicated that various types of ASD are not easily
distinguishable from one another [19, 20], which was supported by a recent review [21],
which concluded that criteria for various types of ASD indeed overlap. One study [22]
indicated that this led to differing definitions between such disorders as autistic disorder
and Asperger syndrome between assessment sites, which compromised diagnostic
validity. The conclusion was that distinctions between various types of ASD’s are often
dependent upon the severity of symptoms such as the presence of a learning disability
or language skills. Utilizing an umbrella term such as autistic spectrum disorders helps
place symptoms on a continuum and provides one clear, concise defining term,
therefore preserving diagnostic validity. The major goal of changes to the diagnostic
criteria in the new DSM-V is to make criteria for ASD’s more clear as well as increase
validity of diagnosis.
CHARACTERISTICS OF ASD
Social deficits
Infants and children with ASD are different from typical infants when it comes to social
development [24]. Typically developing infants are very social. They gaze toward faces,
turn toward speaking voices, grasp fingers extended towards them, and smile by the
time they are 2 – 3 months old. Conversely, children who develop ASD have difficulty
with social interactions with other human beings. By 8 – 10 months of age, infants who
go on to develop ASD are generally showing some symptoms such as the failure to
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
17
respond to their names, a reduced interest in other people, and a delay in babbling. By
the time they are toddlers, many of these children have difficulties playing socially with
other children. Additionally, they don’t imitate others and they often prefer to play on
their own instead of with other children. They may not seek comfort from parents or
caregivers as well as show a failure to respond to anger or affection in ways that are
typical.
Research has indicated that children with ASD are attached to caregivers. However, the
way this attachment is expressed is often unusual, and caregivers frequently interpret
the child’s expressions as disconnected or emotionless. It is important to remember that
both children and adults who suffer from ASD have difficulty determining what others
are thinking or feeling. Where children who develop normally learn to accurately
interpret such social cues as smiling, waving or grimacing, these social cues hold little
meaning for the individual who suffers from autism. Individuals with ASD also have
difficulty seeing things from another person’s perspective, meaning that they have little
empathy. While most normally developing five year olds have learned to see things from
another person’s perspective, the person suffering from ASD has not learned this skill,
leading to poor understanding. This may interfere with an ability to predict or understand
actions as well as lead to an inability to understand why another person is feeling the
way they are feeling.
Finally, it is common for individuals who suffer ASD to have difficulty regulating their
emotions. They may come across as emotionally immature, having crying outbursts or
displaying emotions that are inappropriate for the situation. They may also be disruptive
or physically aggressive. These expressions may be particularly pronounced if the
individual with ASD is feeling overwhelmed or frustrated, and can lead to self-injurious
behaviors such as biting or head banging.
Communication difficulties
Infants and children with ASD are different from typical children when it comes to
communication [24]. The majority of children pass predictable milestones to learning
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
18
language by 3 years of age. The earliest of these is babbling; and, by the age of 1, most
typically developing children will say a few words, acknowledge their names, and point
to or display objects they want. Additionally, children of this age have the ability to
communicate clearly either through sounds or expression when they do not want
something that is given to them. Conversely, children suffering from ASD generally
experience delayed babbling and speaking and may also experience delays in learning
how to utilize gestures to indicate their preferences. Some individuals who develop ASD
may possess these abilities early on and then lose them. Others experience delays and
then gain language ability later on in life.
With therapy, many with ASD do learn how to use spoken language and all with ASD
can learn how to communicate in some way, either verbally or nonverbally. Those who
communicate nonverbally or nearly nonverbally learn to use systems such as pictures,
sign language, word processors, or devices such as speech-generators.
When language does begin to develop, an individual with ASD may utilize speech in
ways that are unusual. For example, some individuals have difficulty forming complete
or meaningful sentences. They may speak single words or phrases or repeat the same
word or phrase over and over again. Others may experience a stage where they repeat
everything they hear word for word, a condition called echolalia. There are others who
have difficulty sustaining conversation despite developing large vocabularies. Still
others carry on long monologues on favorite subjects, giving those listening little
opportunity to respond and having little understanding of the give and take of social
conversation.
Another common communication difficulty is the inability to interpret body language and
tone of voice. For example, sarcasm is particularly difficult for those with ASD to
interpret. An individual who has developed normally may accurately interpret the
sarcasm in a comment such as “Oh, that’s just great!” whereas the individual with ASD
would miss it and interpret the comment as referencing something that actually is just
great. The individual with ASD may also not exhibit typical body language himself or
herself. For instance, their body language may not match what they are saying.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
19
Tone of voice may not reflect the emotion and individual with ASD is trying to convey.
Some individuals suffering from ASD utilize flat or robotic sounding voices. These
displays make it difficult for others to understand what individuals with ASD need or
want. This failure in communication can in turn lead to frustration and inappropriate
displays made by the person suffering ASD, such as screaming or grabbing.
Stereotyped or repetitive behaviors and interests
Infants and children with ASD are different from typical children when it comes to
behaviors and interests [24]. Individuals who develop ASD generally engage in repetitive
behaviors as well as have a tendency to engage in a range of activity that is tightly
restricted. Common repetitive behavior includes hand flapping, jumping, rocking,
twirling, arranging or rearranging objects, and the repetition of sounds, words, or
sentences. Occasionally the repetition involves behaviors such as wiggling the hands or
fingers.
Restrictive activities can be clearly seen in the ways children with ASD play with their
toys. Instead of actually playing with their toys, these children will often spend hours
lining the toys up in a specific order. In adults this is seen in the way the individual is
preoccupied with having certain objects lined up in a certain order. Repetitive behavior
may be intensely obsessive and very unusual. It can take any form, including unusual
content or knowledge. This is why some individuals with ASD develop extreme interest
and knowledge of numbers or scientific topics. Individuals with restrictive activities can
become extremely upset if someone or something disrupts the order of their things. This
shows how essential consistency is for many individuals with ASD. Even the slightest
changes in environment or routine can prove very stressful for individuals suffering from
ASD and may lead to outbursts.
Individuals with ASD may have many different obsessions or behaviors, however there
are some that are particularly common among those with ASD. Activities where these
behaviors become notable include: identifying historical dates and events, computers,
certain television programs, trains, and science. Children with ASD in particular like
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
20
playing with toys such as Thomas the Train and dinosaurs. Older individuals with ASD
may develop repetitive obsessive interests with things like car registration numbers,
traffic lights, shapes, or body parts. Individuals with ASD are also frequently quite
interested in collecting objects; this may be collecting something that seems quite
common, such as a certain toy, or something that seems uncommon, such as leaves.
What marks the behavior as unusual is the duration and intensity that the person with
ASD shows. People with ASD will frequently learn a great deal about the thing they are
obsessed with, be intensely interested in it for an extended period of time, and feel very
strongly about the object or objects in question. There are a number of reasons that
people with ASD develop obsessions, including the fact that obsessions help provide
structure and order, obsessions offer a way to start conversations when social
interactions prove difficult, obsessions help the individual relax or feel happy, and
obsessions offer enjoyment and the opportunity to learn.
Repetitive behaviors can include arm and hand flapping, finger flicking, rocking,
jumping, twirling, spinning, head banging, and more complex body movements.
Repetitive movements also include the repetitive usage of an object, such as the
repetitive flick of a rubber band or repetitively stroking a piece of fabric. Many individuals
with ASD have what is called sensory sensitivity; this means that they are over or under
sensitive to sights, smells, touch, sounds, and tastes. Their balance and body
awareness may also be affected by this sensitivity, and the repetitive behavior is a way
to deal with the sensitivity. There are a number of reasons that people with ASD utilize
repetitive behavior, including attempts to obtain or reduce sensory input, finding ways to
deal with anxiety or stress, or as a way to obtain enjoyment or to occupy one self.
Routines and sameness are also important for the individual with ASD. Routines are
important because they bring order and predictability to the individual’s life and help to
manage anxiety. Repetitive behaviors and obsessions offer routine and order to the
individual who suffers from ASD. However, the need for repetition and routine and order
may extend beyond repetitive behaviors. Some individuals with ASD may have issues
with changes such as those to their physical environment. For instance, if a chair is
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
21
moved in a room or a new person enters the room, this could be difficult for some
individuals with ASD to handle. Some individuals with ASD may also have very rigid
preferences when it comes to things such as food. For example, they may only eat food
that is a certain color or begins with a certain letter of the alphabet. This may extend to
other areas of life, such as clothing (for example, only wearing clothing made of certain
fabrics) or even to everything objects (for example, only utilizing certain brands of soap
of toilet paper).
Some individuals with ASD may also develop a need to have a routine around daily
activities such as bedtime or meals. These routines may become nearly ritualistic.
Verbal rituals in addition to physically repetitive behaviors may also be seen in the
individual with ASD; and, they may repeat the same question over and over again or
need to hear a specific answer repeatedly. There may also be compulsive behavior in
addition to obsessive behavior that is developed. This means that the individual may do
things like wash their hands or check locks constantly. This behavior is not the same as
having obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), although it strongly mirrors it.
Cognitive delays
Children with ASD are different from typical children when it comes to cognitive
development [25]. While cognitive abilities vary, a great many children who have ASD
also have some level of mental retardation. About 75% of people who have ASD have a
non-verbal IQ that is below 70, although ASD may also occur in individuals who are of
normal or high intelligence. There are some children with ASD who also have a high
level of giftedness in a certain area, such as art, music, or math. However, this category
or individuals typically referred to as savants is typically very small and is estimated to
comprise less than 1% of ASD children [25]. If a child with ASD has abilities such as
these they usually manifest by the age of 10.
Children with cognitive delay often present with other delays as well. They also miss
general developmental milestones, particularly the following
[26]:
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
22

Speaking their first words, generally at age 10 – 18 months

Responding to simple instructions, exploring on one’s own, utilizing trial and
error, generally at age 12 months

Walking without aid, generally at age 12 – 18 months

Naming body parts, generally at 18 months

Utilizing phrase speech, generally before the age of 24 – 30 months
Some studies indicate that cognitive delays may be in part the result of drug usage. One
study, the Neurodevelopmental Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs (NEAD), confirmed that
fetal exposure to the drug valproate impairs a child’s IQ well into childhood [25]. This
study was conducted on 310 pregnant women in the United States and the United
Kingdom, with the purpose being to determine if there were differential long-term
neurodevelopmental effects across four frequently utilized drugs: carbamazepine,
phenytoin, lamotrigine, and valproate. Researchers indicate that the use of valproate
during pregnancy led to significant cognitive effects in children, with the child IQ being 8
– 11 points lower (as compared with the other drugs). Valproate was therefore
considered a very poor choice for women who were of childbearing age. While these
results seem to indicate one possible culprit of cognitive delay in those with ASD, further
study is needed to determine how drugs could be contributing to cognitive delays.
A child with ASD may also experience additional mental health conditions or learning
disabilities. For example, they may experience attention difficulties, problems controlling
emotions, or have difficulties learning. There may also be mood or anxiety disorders
present. For example, children with ASD sometimes suffer specific phobias in addition
to ASD. It is also important to note that medication that is utilized to treat symptoms may
affect a child with ASD cognitively. For example, children with ASD who also have
attention deficits do not respond predictably to stimulant medications (i.e.
methylphenidate) that may be prescribed for children with attention deficit disorder.
Keeping in mind that medication may affect the ASD child in unpredictable ways allows
for customization of treatment protocol.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
23
ASSOCIATED FEATURES OF ASD
Genetic disorders
One of the biggest advances in understanding the pathophysiology of ASD has been
appreciating the significance of the contribution genetics makes to the etiology of ASD.
There are three main areas of evidence that support this genetic contribution: twin
studies, which compare monozygotic twins (MZ) and dizygotic twins (DZ); family
studies, which compare the rates of ASD in the first-degree relatives versus the general
population; and, studies of genetic syndromes that also co-occur with a diagnosis of
ASD [27]. Each of these will be addressed in turn in the following sections.
Since MZ twins share 100% of genetic material, and DZ twins share 50% of genetic
material (which is similar to siblings who are not twins), and both of these types of twins
share an environment in utero, the higher disease occurrence in MZ twins as opposed
to DZ twins supports the genetic etiology. This has been supported in more than one
twin study and is overall consistent with estimates of heritability at around 70 – 80% [28,
29].
Studies in families indicate that first-degree relatives of those who are autistic have a
marked increased risk of developing ASD as compared to those in the general
population. This is consistent with the strong familial, genetic tie that was observed in
the twin studies [30]. This is not to say that environment does not play a role, but rather
to display that genetics also plays an important role as well. Further, first degree
relatives of those with ASD display an increase in the behavioral and cognitive features
that are associated with ASD, such as language deficits or autistic-like social
impairments [31]; however these often manifest in lesser forms. This is as compared to
the general population [32].
It has also been common knowledge for several decades that there are a number of
medical and genetic conditions that are associated with ASD. For instance, conditions
such as Joubert syndrome, Smith-Lemli-Opitz syndrome, Tuberous Sclerosis, and
Fragile X are all known to cause ASD, although many of these with a lower than 50%
penetrance [33, 34]. Many genes have been identified for ASD; however, few of these
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
24
genes are specific to ASD but instead contribute to a genetic risk for an associated
disorder that causes ASD.
Epilepsy
Epilepsy is very common in those who suffer from ASD, and increasingly, practitioners
are recognizing it as a problem that must be dealt with in addition to the problems that
come with an ASD diagnosis. Approximately 20 – 30% of those who suffer ASD will
develop epilepsy by the time they become adults
[35].
However, actual rates of
comorbidity vary with age and the type of disorder.
Major risk factors for occurrence of a seizure are mental retardation as well as the
presence of additional neurological disorders [36]. Therapeutic approaches to epilepsy in
ASD include conventional treatments; however, should seizures not be evident, there is
controversy as to what treatments should be utilized. Anticonvulsant medication may
interfere with moods or behaviors, and disturbances in moods and behaviors are often
observed in those patients with ASD. There is currently limited understanding regarding
the link between ASD and epilepsy; however, from a clinical standpoint this link should
not be dismissed.
Intellectual disorders
Recent findings [37] indicate that those with ASD also have a high prevalence of
intellectual disorders. Intellectual disabilities are characterized by cognitive, social, and
adaptive deficits, leading to the co-occurrence with other disorders, such as ADHD,
mood disorders, and catatonia and repetitive behaviors, which further complicate
matters. These problems may be problematic not only for the individual suffering from
the disability but also for parents, caregivers, and providers. The disorder may be so
severe as to be debilitating [38].
Matson [39] divides individuals into three distinct groups: those with intellectual disability
(ID), those with ASD, and those with both ASD and ID. Matson states that it is important
for the practitioner to know which type of person he or she is treating, as that will
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
25
determine the best course of treatment. For instance, an individual with both ID and
ASD will have different needs than the individual who only has ASD.
The combination of intellectual disorders and ASD presents a number of challenges as
well as deficits across a wide range of behaviors or skills that are not seen in those
individuals who have only ID or only ASD. For example, it has been observed
[40]
that
those individuals with severe autism had more feeding problems that were behaviorally
based, in particular with selecting or refusing food, as compared with those who only
had ID. The combination also increases challenging behaviors. For example, as IQ
goes down, the severity of challenging behaviors in ASD increases. Murphy et al
[41]
discovered that self-injury in particular increased. Further, those with ASD tend to not
“grow out” of these types of behaviors; rather, they continue to present significant
challenges over the lifespan, as observed by Murphy et al [41] in a 12-year follow-up
conducted on 141 individuals with severe ASD and ID.
DEVELOPMENTAL COURSE OF ASD
It is commonly believed that those with ASD fall into one of two major developmental
categories [42]. One category involves early onset of impairment and signs and
symptoms of ASD without signs of regression. This is termed gradual onset course. The
other category involves a rather typical development. This typical development is then
followed by loss of language or social skills, sometimes with loss of both, that is paired
with an emergence of ASD-type behaviors such as repetitive or stereotypical behaviors.
This is termed regression course. There is mixed evidence when it comes to prognostic
implications where the regression course is concerned. Some studies indicate that the
regression course is associated with worse outcomes than is the gradual onset course
[43].
Both of these courses may be diagnosed in early to late infancy [44].
There are two approaches that have been taken in order to understand the
development of ASD. These approaches are retrospective and prospective.
Retrospective approaches are mainly based on information gained from reviews of
medical records, parental recall, or observational coding of videotapes made in the
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
26
home environment during the first or second year of life prior to the ASD diagnosis.
Retrospective studies have generated information that indicates that the core deficits of
ASD are social functioning [45]; however, they also indicate that children who suffer from
ASD also exhibit disruptions in other areas of their lives within the first year of life,
including motor skills, attention, and temperament.
Prospective studies are considered optimal to investigate the timing and nature of how
ASD emerges [42] because prospective, longitudinal studies conducted from infancy
would provide a means of determining patterns of development in those children who
are later diagnosed with ASD. This would later eliminate such confounds as recall bias.
There have been five prospective studies to date that have provided longitudinal data
before the third birthday in children who have ASD as well as in children who are not
affected [46-50]. These studies have all indicated that the development of cognitive,
motor, language, and social skills all appear to be fine at age 6 months. Development
then slows. By the time the children enter pre-school, those with ASD frequently display
motor delays.
Gradual Onset Course
The gradual onset course occurs just as its name suggests; signs and symptoms
present themselves gradually. Parents or caregivers may notice increasing delays in
development or strange behavior, such as an unwillingness to communicate or
communicating in odd ways. One example of this would be a failure to look a person
who is speaking in the face. Other examples include failure to turn when the child’s
name is called or failure to display interests by pointing to objects the child desires.
Stereotypical behaviors may also develop, such as tapping or hand flapping. All of these
behaviors are early warning signs that a child is developmentally delayed and may need
to be screened for ASD.
Regression Course
The regression course occurs when a child is following a pattern of normal
development, generally for the first 12 – 24 months of life, and then he or she appears
to lose skills he or she has acquired. Language regression is considered the most
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
27
obvious form of regression, but it may also be accompanied by more global regression,
which can involve a loss of social skills or social interest. There is a late-onset
regression course that may occur after the age of 3, but the more common course
occurs prior to the age of 3.
Regression is thought to lead to the more severe course of autism [51], particularly when
young children abruptly lose acquired skills, according to a study published in the
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders [52]. There is a particular debate among
scientists as to whether children who regress form a distinct grouping of autistic
individuals, as some studies have indicated that those with regression have poorer
outcomes [53].
The study published in the Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders surveyed
the parents of 2,720 children and found that those children who regress were more
likely to display more severe symptoms than were those children who had early onset
delays. This was as measured through two standard questionnaires: the Social
Responsiveness Scale and the Social Communication Questionnaire. Children in the
regression group were more likely to receive a diagnosis of autistic disorder as opposed
to a diagnosis that was on the milder end of the spectrum, such as Asperger syndrome,
as compared to their earlier onset peers. Additionally, in a school setting, approximately
70% of children who regress are put into special education settings, with 56% needing a
professional aid.
Communication
Research indicates that regression in communication, particularly in speech and
gestures occurs in approximately 22 – 50% of those children with ASD [54]. The rate is
so variable because the definition for regression is fairly loose and can mean anything
from the loss of a minimum of five words for a period of 3 months to the loss of the
consistent usage of one word used in standard communication. Approximately 30% of
children who experience regression never manage conversational speech.
Social
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
28
Loss of social skills can mean that the child stops returning the caregiver’s gaze, for
example, or displays a lack of interest in other people when they are in the same space
with him or her. An increased disinterest in social games that the child previously
enjoyed, such as patty cake or peek-a-boo, may become obvious. One meta-analysis
[55]
indicates that approximately 38% of children suffer from social regression.
Cognitive
Cognitive decline in ASD manifests as more than a child simply losing what he or she
has learned to date. However, one of the manifestations most clearly seen in cognitive
decline is the loss of language skills. The individual may have been learning language
skills perfectly well and then suddenly loses the ability to learn new skills. Other features
of cognitive decline are the inability to learn new material as well as the development of
associated mental impairments. An example of an associated impairment would be
epilepsy, a common impairment associated with ASD.
Self-help skills
The loss of self-help skills occurs when a child loses the ability to continue to develop
independence. This means that the child fails to develop the ability to feed, clean, and
dress oneself. Children who regress lose the ability to understand how to complete
these tasks and can lack or lose the physical coordination necessary to complete these
tasks. Additionally, the child may lose or not develop an understanding of how or when
to ask for assistance with tasks. These types of tasks help children become socialized
into his or her culture; without them the child is poorly socialized. The fact that many
individuals with ASD are lacking in this area is one reason why they appear to be so
poorly socialized.
EFFECTS OF EARLY INTERVENTION
Early detection of ASD allows for practitioners to intervene in a more timely way with
behavioral therapies that may then improve outcomes. Currently, the American
Academy of Pediatrics guidelines call for the screening of all toddlers at the ages of 18
and 24 months [56]. These are the ages at which the existing screening methods are
most able to identify children at risk for ASD.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
29
However, research has shown that a brief questionnaire administered to parents at their
child’s one year well-baby screening may help practitioners identify those children who
have ASD or who are at higher risk for the development of ASD. One study involving
137 pediatricians who administered a 24-item checklist to all caregivers bringing in
children for routine one-year check-ups indicated that about 346 of the 10,500 children
screened were at risk for autism. These children were all referred to an autism clinic to
be evaluated further. Of these children, approximately 50% were followed to age 3; and,
32 of these children received a diagnosis of ASD. Another 56 children were diagnosed
with having a language delay. Of those children diagnosed with delayed language skills,
9 children were diagnosed with having a developmental delay, and; 36 children were
diagnosed with other conditions.
The screening utilized for autism in childhood – called the Communication and Symbolic
Behavior Scales Developmental Profile Infant-Toddler Checklist, took the parents
approximately 5 minutes to complete; and, the screening test predicted autism and
other developmental delays approximately 75% of the time. This indicates that this
questionnaire – or one similar – could be useful in identifying ASD earlier so that more
timely intervention methods may be put to use. Although most pediatricians do not
routinely screen early for ASD and other developmental delays (indeed, prior to the
study, only 30 of the 137 pediatricians participating (22%) had routinely screened for
ASD at 1 year of a child’s life) there seems to be some good evidence that early
intervention efforts do benefit those who suffer from ASD.
The Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), which is a behavioral intervention program
appropriate for children as young as 12 months who are suffering from ASD, has been
found in more than one study to be effective in improving brain response as well as
social skills. ESDM combines applied behavioral analysis (ABA) along with a
developmental relationship based approach to achieve gains in language, cognition,
and everyday living skills. ESDM is a unique approach in part because it works with
children who are very young, but also because it blends ABA with routines that are
based in play and focus on building relationships.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
30
One study [57] utilizing ESDM examined 48 children with ASD between 18 and 30
months of age. The children were randomly assigned to either receive ESDM or a
community based intervention regimen for a two-year period. Electroencephalogram
(EEG) activity was measured at the close of the two-year period. The hypothesis was
that children receiving ESDM would show higher levels of brain activity when viewing
faces than when viewing objects than would children who were receiving the typical
community regimen. The children were paired with typically developing children at the
time of their EEG assessment. Additionally a group of typical 4-year-old children was
tested as a comparison group. The children who were receiving the ESDM intervention
regimen were given therapy services for 20 hours per week and parents were also
trained to deliver the intervention regimen. Children who were in the community based
regimen received treatment per usual in their community and were given evaluation,
referrals, as well as resources and reading material at the start of services as well as
two times annually.
On the individual level, the study mentioned above showed that 11 of 15 children in the
ESDM group (73%) and 12 of 17 children (71%) in the control group had higher levels
of brain activity on their EEG assessment when viewing faces as opposed to viewing
objects. This is compared to 5 of 14 (36%) children in the community regimen group. At
the close of the study, the children in the ESDM group exhibited brain activity that was
comparable to typically developing children and that was significantly different from
those children who had received the community regimen. Dr. Geraldine Dawson, a
developer of the ESDM intervention and one of the researchers on the study, stated the
following: “For the first time, parents and practitioners have evidence that early
intervention can result in an improved course of both brain and behavioral development
in young children. It is crucial that all children with autism have access to early
intervention which can promote the most positive long-term outcomes” [57].
Another study [57], also examining the ESDM, found that this same model minimizes the
need for required therapy following the intervention as well as achieves the best
possible outcomes for the individual in terms of IQ, social interactions, and brain activity.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
31
The study compared 21 children who received ESDM to 18 children who received a
community intervention regimen during the two years they received their early
intervention regimen as well as the subsequent four years post-intervention. ESDM is
more expensive to deliver in the early years of intervention; there was an average
monthly cost of about $10,000 per child. Children on the community regimen had an
average monthly cost of approximately $5,200. However, the hypothesis was that
despite this greater up-front cost, ESDM would pay off greater dividends on the back
end of things, promoting higher IQ, greater ease in social interactions, increased brain
activity, and reduced therapy, leading to an overall reduced cost.
In the four years following the early intervention, the children who had received the
ESDM required an average of approximately $4,450 in related services such as speech,
physical, and occupational therapy, as well as ABA. The children who received the
community regimen required an average of approximately $5,550 in related speech,
physical, and occupational therapy, as well as ABA. Study researchers believe this is
very telling and differences in cost may be even broader than study results suggest, as
the defining factors for related expenses were kept fairly narrow. David S. Mandell,
study researcher expressed the following: “I believe the cost efficiencies would become
even more pronounced if there had been an evaluation on health costs and overall
family economics such as the ability of both parents to continue to work and earn
income while their child received services” [57].
These two studies offer powerful evidence for the idea that early intervention such as
early screening as well as methods such as ESDM may prove to offer better outcomes
for those individuals who suffer from ASD, and in turn offer parents and caregivers a
better outcome as well. Early intervention may help a child in the form of increased
cognitive and social skills, and a family in terms of long-term financial savings.
Therefore, there is strong evidence to suggest that early intervention benefits everyone
involved with ASD.
RISK FACTORS
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
32
There are a number of risk factors that contribute to ASD. There are both heritable and
non-heritable risk factors. These risk factors are discussed below.
Gender
While ASDs occur in all racial, ethnic, socioeconomic, and gender groupings, it is well
known that males have a greater likelihood than do females of developing an ASD. The
ratio is as high as 5:1 [58]. It is not known or well understood why there is this
discrepancy. It is also not known if concrete differences in development or presentation
occur between genders. Males with ASD do have certain advantages over females with
ASD. Research indicates that females with ASD tend to have lower Intelligence
Quotients (IQ) than do males [59]. Males also show stronger verbal, motor, and social
skills. However, when controlling for language, females display stronger nonverbal
problem solving abilities.
A recent study published in JAMA Pediatrics [60] that examined the records of more than
625,000 births indicated that birth that involved both augmented and induced labor was
linked to a 35% higher instance of ASD as compared with labor that did not receive
either treatment. The study further indicates that augmented or induced labor was linked
with a smaller increased risk in boys. In girls, only the augmented birth was linked with a
small increased risk. The researchers did control for a number of associated factors that
have been shown to increase the risk of autism, such as the health or age of the
mother. Researchers in the study added that the study results did not necessarily
indicate cause and effect; rather, there could be other factors at play that have as yet
been unidentified. One of these factors may be the usage of Pitocin (oxytocin) that is
utilized to induce or augment labor. Approximately 50 – 70% of women who undergo
induction in the United States receive Pitocin injections. There are other contributors
that may be a factor, such as pregnancy conditions or delivery events that lead up to the
need to induce labor. Further study is needed to determine why augmented and
induced labor is leading to this increased the risk of ASD in boys.
Genetics
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
33
Twin studies provide evidence to support that there is a heritable component to ASD
etiology, although there has not been any particular gene discovered that predisposes
an individual to ASD [61]. Twin studies have been done on both monozygotic (identical)
and dizygotic (fraternal) twins. Monozygotic twins share all of their genes, whereas
dizygotic twins share half of their genes, on the average. Studies have indicated that
there are increased disease concordance rates among monozygotic twins as compared
to dizygotic twins. A recent study indicated that there is a 60% monozygotic
concordance among 25 sets of twins as compared to a 0% dizygotic concordance
among 20 sets of twins [62]. This data suggests that there is a high rate of heritability.
However, the suggestion has been made that estimations from twin studies may be
overstated. Still, the large heritability discovered in twin studies is supported through
familial aggregation studies [63].
Several studies have also shown a heightened risk for ASD amongst siblings of cases.
This is termed “sibling relative risk” [61] and is estimated as the “ratio of the risk for ASD
among siblings of cases to the risk, or prevalence, in the general population” [61]. The
probability that a sibling of a case will develop ASD is estimated at between 2 % and
6% [64, 65], although there are some estimates that are as high as 7% for siblings of male
cases and 14% of siblings for female cases [66]. It is important to remember, however,
that these numbers are entirely dependent on the population prevalence estimates at
the time of sampling, leading to higher or lower estimations, depending on what is
happening in the population.
There is also another avenue of support for genetic association, and this is the overlap
of ASD with certain genetic disorders such as tuberous sclerosis [67], neurofibromatosis
[68],
and Fragile X syndrome [69, 70]. To that end, abnormalities on nearly every
chromosome have been associated with one form or another of ASD phenotype, most
notably on chromosomes 7, 15, and X [71]. The most frequently cited of these are
duplications and deletions of the proximal area of chromosome 15 [72-75]. Breakpoints for
chromosomal inversions that result in features of ASD frequently lie in fragile regions of
chromosomes, which lead to speculations about the possible role of unstable regions of
DNA and submicroscopic chromosomal deletions [76, 77].
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
34
Prenatal and Perinatal Factors
There are specific prenatal factors that may contribute to development of ASD. One
such factor is maternal infections. In several studies [61], maternal infections were
measured with non-specific indicators, which included maternal recall of symptoms such
as fever as well as information archived in medical records. While the studies did not
attain statistical significance for the infection measure, each reported a ratio of odds that
were above 1.0 [78-80].
There are specific infections that are known to affect developing brains; of these that
have been most commonly known to affect the developing brain as well as to be
commonly associated with ASD is rubella. However, it has also been shown that other
infections, such as herpes, syphilis, and varicella, as well as the flu, also have a higher
than normal association with ASD. Another factor is prenatal and intrapartum
pharmaceutical usage. For example, utilizing thalidomide during days 20-24 of gestation
has been associated with increased risk of ASD [81, 82]. This suggests that xenobiotics
may play a role in the etiology of ASD. Animal studies [83, 84] and case studies [85, 86]
reflect findings that valproic acid as well as other anticonvulsants may increase ASD
risk. This is an interesting association, as these same drugs may prove therapeutic for
non-epileptic children who suffer from ASD [87-89].
There are also some preconception factors that may be associated with development of
ASD. For instance, in the 1970s the idea that environmental exposure to certain
chemicals arose [90]. This hypothesis was revisited in the 1990s when parents without
incidence of ASD who lived close to plastic manufacturing plants appeared to have a
higher incidence of children with ASD [91]. However, upon examination, the
Massachusetts Department of Public Health concluded that further investigation of the
cases in question was not needed [92].
While preconception chemical exposure factors have not been thoroughly explored,
hypotheses of postnatal chemical exposures have been more thoroughly investigated,
primarily through looking at case studies. However, epidemiologic evidence for specific
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
35
postnatal environmental exposure that in turn leads to development of ASD is not
substantial. One of the more comprehensive investigations into the matter was
conducted in Brick Township, New Jersey, where there was a high local presence of
ASD near local landfills. This raised concerns that landfills were leaching chemicals into
the drinking water or into local swimming areas. The Agency for Toxic Substances and
Disease Registry looked into the possible exposure pathways, as well as evaluated data
on levels of trihalomethanes, tetrachloroethylene, and thrichloroethylene. While these
chemicals were present in the drinking water at various times during the study, the
levels were found to be low or in locations that did not correspond with the locations
being studied or with the timing of the pregnancies being studied [93, 94].
Neuroanatomical Abnormalities
Neuroanatomical abnormalities in the brains of individuals with ASD provide concrete
evidence that there is a neurobiological component to ASD
[95],
that the disease is more
than just a behavioral disorder with purely environmental contributions.
The neurobiology of the disorder has been examined since the dawn of the disorder,
and both genetic and non-genetic factors have been shown [96]. However, particular
etiologic factors are as yet undefined. Frontal lobe volume does appear to be decreased
in individuals with autism [97]; there also appears to be a decrease in gray matter (GM)
volume in the orbitofrontal cortex [98] as well as an abnormally thin frontotemporal cortex
[99].
However, conflicting studies [100-102] have reported that GM volume and thickness is
actually enlarged in these areas.
An increase in GM volume has also been indicated in areas involved in communicative
and social functions, to include the dorsal and medial prefrontal regions, the lateral and
medial temporal area, the parietal regions, and the auditory and visual association
cortices [103-106]. Likewise, discrepant white matter (WM) have been indicated in autism,
including regional increases [107-109], as have decreases in cross-sectional areas and the
microstructure of the corpus callosum [110, 111]. Concomitant WM disruptions have been
indicated in prefrontal, superior temporal, temporoparietal cortices and the corpus
callosum, but there have also been observations of an increase in whole brain WM [101,
109].
It is not clear how these anatomic abnormalities related to domain specific cognitive
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
36
impairment in social functioning, emotional functioning, language deficits,
communication deficits, and deficient executive function.
Microscopic observations of the brains of individuals with ASD have discovered reduced
cell size as well as increased cell-packing density (meaning an increased number of
nerve cells per unit volume) in the hippocampus, amygdala, mammillary body, anterior
cingulate gyrus, and medial septal nucleus [112]. These structures are recognized as
being connected to one another by interrelated circuits. They also comprise a major
portion of the limbic system. The limbic system is acknowledged to be important to
emotion and behavior, as well as learning and memory. It also plays a large role in the
integration, processing, and generalization of information. Abnormalities of the limbic
system may account for a lot of the major clinical features of ASD, including language
dysfunction as well as social deficits.
Additionally, abnormalities have been located in the cerebellum, where the amount of
Purkinje cells is much reduced, particularly in the posterior and inferior parts of the
hemispheres bilaterally. The vermis microscopically appears normal.
Abnormalities have also been seen in the deep cerebellar nuclei. These findings appear
to vary along with the age of the patient. Comparable observations have been seen in
the inferior olivary nucleus neurons as well as the neurons of the nucleus of the
Diagonal Band of Broca. The fact the the olivary neurons are preserved even in the face
of such markedly reduced number of Purkinje cells indicates that these brain lesions are
likely of prenatal origin.
The role of the cerebellum in ASD is not certain. Some studies [112] show that it may play
a role in the modulation of language, attention, emotional affect, mental imagery,
cognition, and anticipatory planning. Therefore, it is likely that the abnormalities in both
the limbic system and the cerebellum are important to understanding the clinical
features of ASD.
Brain enlargement
Brain changes prior to the age of 2 may lead to brain enlargement that in turn leads to
ASD. One study conducted by University of North Carolina (UNC) researchers found
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
37
that children with ASD who had enlarged brains at the age of 2 years also had enlarged
brains at the ages of 4 and 5. However, their brain growth was not markedly different
than it had been at 2 years old.
Researchers conducted their original study in 2005, with a follow-up study conducted in
2011. In 2005, researchers discovered that 2-year-old participants with ASD had brains
that were up to 10% larger than those of children at the same age that did not have
ASD. The follow-up study indicated that the children with ASD continued to have brain
enlargement in subsequent years, but at the same level they had it at 2 years old. This
finding led researchers to conclude that the changes they detected at the age of 2 were
due to growth prior to that time period. Additionally, the study discovered that the
enlargement was affiliated with an increase in folding on the surface of the brain, not an
increase in gray matter. Researchers posit that this increase is more than likely genetic
and results from “an increase in the proliferation of neurons in the developing brain”
[114].
The researchers suggest that the brain overgrowth may be occurring around the child’s
first birthday.
Another study conducted by the University of California Davis MIND Institute indicated
that those children who were later diagnosed with ASD were found to have an excess of
cerebrospinal fluid as well as enlarged brains in infancy. This brought up the possibility
that these types of brain abnormalities may serve “as potential biomarkers for the early
identification of the neurodevelopmental disorder” [115]. The study is the first to link the
excess of cerebrospinal fluid that existed during infancy to the development of ASD. A
potential positive outcome to the study is that it would offer practitioners a new way to
positively screen for ASD, because the brain anomaly would be detectable utilizing a
conventional MRI. Therefore, early detection would be possible; this is crucial in
children who have ASD because it allows for timely intervention.
Early intervention offers the most hope in decreasing the behavioral and cognitive
impairments associated with ASD and increasing positive long-term outcomes. This
study was conducted on 55 children who were between 6 – 26 months of age. Thirtythree of these children had an older sibling who had an ASD. Twenty-two were children
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
38
who had no family history of ASD. Researchers indicated that the brain anomaly was
more significantly detected in those infants who were high risk and who were later
diagnosed with ASD between 24 and 26 months of age.
Another study [116] examined abnormal brain growth and onset status: early onset or
regressive onset. This particular study examined 2 – 4 year olds whose status was
either non-regressive (n = 53) or regressive (n = 61). There was also a control group of
non-affected 2 – 4 year olds (n = 66). Researchers discovered that abnormal brain
enlargement was most commonly discovered in boys with regressive ASD. Brain size in
boys who were non-regressive did was not different from the control group.
Retrospective head circumference measurements were also taken, and it was
discovered that head circumference in boys with regressive ASD is normal at birth but
then diverges at around 4 – 6 months of age. Girls who have autism do not have any
difference in brain size from those in the control group. Researchers posit that these
results indicate that there could be certain neural phenotypes that are associated with
the different types of onset of ASD. For instance, the rapid head growth may be a risk
factor for regressive type ASD.
Environmental Factors
There are several environmental factors that may be associated with the development
of ASD. One of these is childhood infection. There have been reports of sudden onset
of ASD symptoms in older children following herpes encephalitis [117-119]. There are other
infections that can result in secondary hydrocephalus; for example, meningitis. These
infections may lead to development of ASD [120]. One study indicated that mumps,
chicken pox, fever of unknown origin, and ear infections were significantly associated
with an increased risk of development of ASD [121].
Some vaccinations may also increase the risk for development of ASD. A paper
published in 1998 indicated that the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine might be linked to
ASD development [122]. However, epidemiologic studies have not provided evidence that
supports a link between the vaccine and the risk of developing ASD [123-127]. Similarly,
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
39
case study comparisons do not find any indication of post-vaccination increase in risk of
developing ASD [123, 128]. Further, a population based retrospective study that included
more than half a million children from Denmark who were born between 1991 and 1998,
82% who had received the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine, discovered no association
between the vaccine and development of ASD [129].
There are additional concerns over vaccines and ASD, which stem from the usage of
thimerosal, which is a preservative that contains ethylmercury. Ethylmercury is itself
similar to methylmercury, which is a known fetal neurotoxin that can cause severe brain
injury at high doses and leads to developmental delays and dysfunction at lower doses
[130].
There is limited data that suggests that higher doses of ethylmercury is similar to
higher doses of methylmercury [131, 132], and there is no data on low dose exposure to
ethylmercury. There is not much data on the association to date.
Data from the Vaccine Safety Datalink of the Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention indicates weak association between thimerosol related exposure to mercury
and related neurodevelopmental disorders, but not association to ASD itself [133].
Existing evidence is considered to be inconclusive. Although Thimerosal has been
removed from vaccines, there are still many individuals alive with ASD who have
received vaccinations that contained thimerosal.
Other environmental factors include birth complications, including umbilical cord
complications, fetal distress, injury or trauma during birth, multiple births, maternal
hemorrhage, summer births, low birth weights, congenital malformations, low 5-minute
Apgar score, feeding difficulty, neonatal anemia, meconium aspiration,
hyperbilirubenemia, and ABO or Rh incompatibility [134]. Parental age at the time of
conception is also a factor; and, this includes the age of both parents. One study [135]
indicated that firstborn children of 2 parents who were older were 3 times more likely to
develop ASD than were third or later born children of mothers who were 20 – 34 years
of age and fathers who were <40 years of age. Therefore, the risk of ASD increases
with both maternal and paternal age.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
40
Another environmental risk factor may be waiting less than a year between
pregnancies. One study [136] indicated that pregnancies that are closely spaced are
associated with an increase in ASD. The study examined pairs of first and second born
siblings in California that occurred between 1992 and 2002 and examined ASD
diagnoses in these siblings. Results of the study indicated that children who were born
after shorter intervals between the pregnancies were at an increased risk of developing
an ASD, with the highest risk being associated with pregnancies that were spaced less
than a year apart.
One more environmental risk factor may be not taking prenatal vitamins, as taking
prenatal vitamins has been shown to slightly reduce the odds that a child will develop
ASD. One study examining Northern California families enrolled in the Childhood
Autism Risks from Genetics and Environment (CHARGE) study [137] concluded that
mothers who had children with ASD were less likely than were mothers of non-affected
children to have taken a prenatal vitamin during the 3 months prior to pregnancy or in
the first month of pregnancy. This led researchers to conclude that the peri-conceptional
use of prenatal vitamins may reduce risk of birthing a child with ASD, particularly in
those who are already genetically susceptible to ASD.
What is important to note regarding environmental risk factors is they do not affect a
child in a vacuum. Many children are exposed to environmental risk factors without
developing ASD. This leads researchers to conclude that if a child is genetically
predisposed to ASD, for example, then these types of environmental risk factors may
increase the risk of development of an ASD. Studies have indicated that there are other
environmental factors - such as the maternal use of antipsychotics and mood
stabilizers – that may increase the risk of development of ASD. However, the risk must
also be weighed against the mother’s need, which can also affect the health of the child
she will be having. Additionally, most risk factors that are not yet well documented may
only increase the risk of development of ASD slightly as compared with other risk
factors, such as genetic predisposition, making it difficult to pinpoint how, exactly,
environmental factors contribute to the larger overall picture of each single ASD
diagnosis.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
41
References
1.
Lauritson MB. Autism spectrum disorders. Ear Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 22(Supp1):S37-S42.
2.
Ozonoff S, Heung K, Byrd R, Hansen R, Hertz-Picciotto. The onset of autism: patterns of symptom
emergence in the first years of life. Autism Res 2008; 1:320–328.
3.
Barbaro J, Dissanayake C. Autism spectrum disorders in infancy and toddlerhood: a review of the
evidence on early signs, early identification tools, and early diagnosis. J Dev Behav Pediatr
2009;30:447–459.
4.
Yirmiya N, Charman T. The prodrome of autism: early behavioral and biological signs, regression,
peri- and post-natal development and genetics. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010;51:432–458.
5.
Rogers SJ. What are infant siblings teaching us about autism in infancy? Autism Res 2009;2:125–
137.
6.
Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin. Autistic disorder. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.chw.org/display/PPF/DocID/22122/router.asp
7.
Bryce L. Analyzing the 2012 CDC numbers on autism rates. 2012. Picture obtained October 14,
2013 from http://thautcast.com/drupal5/category/tags/race
8.
CMAJ. 2012;184(4):E219.
9.
Hayes S. A look at Asperger’s Syndrome. Scol Parent & Child 2009;17,2:86-88.
10.
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. Asperger syndrome fact sheet. Obtained
October 14, 2013 from http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/asperger/detail_asperger.htm
11.
Autism Speaks. PDD-NOS. Obtained October 14, 2013 from http://www.autismspeaks.org/whatautism/pdd-nos
12.
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. Bullying Resource Center. Obtained October
14, 2013 from
http://www.aacap.org/AACAP/Families_and_Youth/Resource_Centers/Autism_Resource_Center/F
AQ.aspx
13.
National Autism Resources. PDD-NOS signs, symptoms and treatments. Obtained October 14,
2013 from http://www.nationalautismresources.com/autismsymptoms.html
14.
MedlinePlus. Childhood disintegrative disorder. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/ency/article/001535.htm
15.
International Rett Syndrome Foundation. About Rett Syndrome. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.rettsyndrome.org/about-us/about-rett-syndrome/frequently-asked-questions#001
16.
NHS Choices. Autism and Asperger syndrome – causes. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Autistic-spectrum-disorder/Pages/Causes.aspx
17.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American
Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA. 1994.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
42
18.
Howlin P. Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays:
implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord
2003;33:3–13.
19.
Walker DR, Thompson A, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Specifying PDD-NOS: a comparison of PDDNOS, Asperger syndrome, and autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004;43:172–180.
20.
Witwer AN, Lecavalier L. Examining the validity of autism spectrum disorder subtypes. J Autism
Dev Disord 2008;38:1611–1624.
21.
Sharma S, Woolfson LM, Hunter SC. Confusion and inconsistency in diagnosis of Asperger
syndrome: a review of studies from 1981 to 2010. Autism 2011;16:465–486
22.
Lord C, Petkova E, Hus V, Gan W, et al. A multisite study of the clinical diagnosis of different
autism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012;69:306–13.
23.
Fein E. German tech company seeks to hire people with autism. Photo obtained from
http://wondergressive.com/news/4991/german-tech-company-seeks-to-hire-people-with-autism/
24.
Autism Speaks. Symptoms. Obtained October 14, 2013 from http://www.autismspeaks.org/whatautism/symptoms
25.
Massachusetts General Hospital. Autism Spectrum Disorders. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www2.massgeneral.org/schoolpsychiatry/info_autism.asp
26.
BMJ. Learning difficulty and cognitive delay. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.bestpractice.bmj.com/best-practice/monograph/884/diagnosis/html
27.
Geschwind DH. Genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Trends in Cog Sciences 2011;15(9):40916.
28.
Bailey A, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol
Med 1995;25:63–77.
29.
Rosenberg RE, et al. Characteristics and concordance of autism spectrum disorders among 277
twin pairs. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2009;163:907–14.
30.
Bolton P, et al. A case-control family history study of autism. J Child Psychol. Psychiatry
1994;35:877–900.
31.
Constantino JN, Todd RD. Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the
general population. Biol Psychiatry 2005:57:655–60.
32.
Losh M, et al. Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 2009;66:518–26.
33.
Abrahams BS and Geschwind DH. Advances in autism genetics: on the threshold of a new
neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet 2008;9:341–55.
34.
Wang K, et al. Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism spectrum disorders.
Nature 2009;459:528–33.
35.
Autism Speaks. Autism and epilepsy resources. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.autismspeaks.org/family-services/epilepsy
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
43
36.
Canitano R. Epilepsy in autism spectrum disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007;16(1):61-6.
37.
McCarthy J. Children with autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. Curr Opin
Psychiatry 2007;20:472-76.
38.
Matson JL, Shoemaker M. Intellectual disability and its relationship to autism spectrum disorders.
Res in Dev Disabil 2009;30:1107-14.
39.
Matson JL, Dempsey T, LoVullo SV. Characteristics of social skills for adults with intellectual
disability, autism, and PDD-NOS. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 2009;3:207–13.
40.
Fodstad JC, Matson JL. A comparison of feeding and mealtime problems in adults with intellectual
disabilities with and without autism. J Dev and Phys Disabil 2008;20:541–50.
41.
Murphy O, Healy O, Leader G. Risk factors for challenging behaviors among 157 children with
autism spectrum disorders in Ireland. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2009;3:474–82.
42.
Landa RJ et al. Developmental trajectories in children with and without autism spectrum disorders:
the first 3 years. Child Dev 2013;84(2):429-42.
43.
Kalb LG, Law JK, Landa R, et al. Onset patterns prior to 36 months in autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2010;40:1389–1402.
44.
Werner E, Dawson G. Validation of the phenomenon of autistic regression using home videotapes.
Archives of General Psychiatry 2005;62(8):889-95.
45.
Pelphrey KA, Carter EJ. Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain.
Development and Psychopathology 2008:20:1081–1102.
46.
Landa R, Garrett-Mayer E. Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective
study. J of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2006;47:629–38.
47.
Landa RJ, Holman KC, Garrett-Mayer E. Social and communication development in toddlers with
early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry
2007;64:853–64.
48.
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, et al. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A Baby
Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics 2011;128:e488–e495.
49.
Sullivan M, Finelli J, Marvin A, et al. Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism
spectrum disorder: A prospective study. J Aut and Dev Disord 2007;37:37–48.
50.
Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, et al. Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of
life. Int J Dev Neuroscience, 2005;23:143–52.
51.
deWeerdt S. Dramatic regression leads to severe autism, study finds. SFARI. Obtained October
14, 2013 from http://sfari.org/news-and-opinion/news/2010/dramatic-regression-leads-to-severeautism-study-finds.
52.
Kalb L. G. et al. J. Autism Dev. Disord. Epub.
53.
Bernabei P. et al. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2007;37:580-588
54.
Brereton AV. Regression in autism. ACT-NOW Fact Sheet 54. 2010.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
44
55.
Barger BD, Campbell JM, McDonough JD. Prevalence and onset of regression within autism
spectrum disorders: a met-analytic review. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(4)817-28.
56.
Pierce K, Carter C, Weinfeld M, et al. Detecting, studying, and treating autism early: the one-year
well-baby check-up approach. J Pediatr 2011;159(3):458-65.
57.
Autism Speaks. The Early Start Denver Model (ESDM). Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.autismspeaks.org/what-autism/treatment/early-start-denver-model-esdm
58.
CDC. Facts about ASDs. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/facts.htm.
59.
Johnston A, Tam C, Doerr J, et al. Gender differences in presentation of autism spectrum
disorders. Developmental Research Program, Children’s Hospital Boston, Harvard Medical School.
60.
Autism Speaks. Study links inducing/augmenting labor with modestly higher autism risk. Obtained
October 14, 2013 from http://www.autismspeaks.org/science/science-news/study-linksinducingaugmenting-labor-modestly-higher-autism-risk
61.
Newschaffer CJ, Fallin D, Lee NL. Heritable and nonheritable risk factors for autism spectrum
disorders. Epidemiol Rev 2002;24(2):137-53
62.
Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a
British twin study. Psychol Med 1995;25:63–77.
63.
Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, Waldmann R, et al. The UCLA University of Utah epidemiologic
survey of autism: genealogical analysis of familial aggregation. Am J Med Genet 1990;36:85–8.
64.
Smalley SL, Asarnow RF, Spence MA. Autism and genetics. A decade of research. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1988;45:953–61.
65.
Jorde LB, Hasstedt SJ, Ritvo ER, et al. Complex segregation analysis of autism. Am J Hum Genet
1991;49:932–8.
66.
Ritvo ER, Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, et al. The UCLA University of Utah epidemiologic survey of
autism: recurrence risk estimates and genetic counseling. Am J Psychiatry 1989;146:1032–6.
67.
Smalley SL. Autism and tuberous sclerosis. J Autism Dev Disord 1998;28:407–14.
68.
Folstein SE, Rutter ML. Autism: familial aggregation and genetic implications. J Autism Dev Disord
1988;18:3–30.
69.
Brown WT, Jenkins EC, Cohen IL, et al. Fragile X and autism: a multicenter survey. Am J Med
Genet 1986;23:341–52.
70.
Folstein SE, Piven J. Etiology of autism: genetic influences. Pediatrics 1991;87:767–73.
71.
Gillberg C. Chromosomal disorders and autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1998;28:415–25.
72.
Martinsson T, Johannesson T, Vujic M, et al. Maternal origin of inv dup (15) chromosomes in
infantile autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;5:185–92.
73.
Cook EH Jr, Lindgren V, Leventhal BL, et al. Autism or atypical autism in maternally but not
paternally derived proximal 15q duplication. Am J Hum Genet 1997;60:928–34.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
45
74.
Schroer RJ, Phelan MC, Michaelis RC, et al. Autism and maternally derived aberration of
chromosome 15q. Am J Med Genet 1998;76:327–33.
75.
Wassink TH, Piven J, Patil SR. Chromosomal abnormalities in a clinic sample of individuals with
autistic disorder. Psychiatr Genet 2001;11:57–63.
76.
Ashley-Koch A, Wolpert CM, Menold MM, et al. Genetic studies of autistic disorder and
chromosome 7. Genomics 1999;61:227–36.
77.
Bass MP, Menold MM, Wolpert CM, et al. Genetic studies in autistic disorder and chromosome 15.
Neurogenetics 2000;2:219–26.
78.
Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Pregnancy, delivery, and neonatal complications among autistic children.
Am J Dis Child 1980;134:860–4.
79.
Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, et al. The UCLA University of Utah epidemiologic survey
of autism: prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics 1990;86:514–19.
80.
Juul-Dam N, Townsend J, Courchesne E. Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors in autism,
pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, and the general population. Pediatrics
2001;107:E63.
81.
Stromland K, Nordin V, Miller M, et al. Autism in thalidomide embryopathy: a population study. Dev
Med Child Neurol 1994;36:351–6.
82.
Miller MT, Stromland K. Teratogen update: a review, with a focus on ocular findings and new
potential uses. Teratology 1999;60:306–21.
83.
Rodier PM, Ingram JL, Tisdale B, et al. Linking etiologies in humans and animal models: studies of
autism. Reprod Toxicol 1997;11:417–22.
84.
Ingram JL, Peckham SM, Tisdale B, et al. Prenatal exposure of rats to valproic acid reproduces the
cerebellar anomalies associated with autism. Neurotoxicol Teratol 2000;22:319–24.
85.
Moore SJ, Turnpenny P, Quinn A, et al. A clinical study of 57 children with fetal anticonvulsant
syndromes. J Med Genet 2000;37:489–97
86.
Bescoby-Chambers N, Forster P, Bates G. Foetal valproate syndrome and autism: additional
evidence of an association. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;43:847–8.
87.
DiMartino A, Tuchman RF. Antiepileptic drugs: affective use in autism spectrum disorders. Pediatr
Neurol 2001;25:199–207.
88.
Hollander E, Dolgoff-Kaspar R, Cartwright C, et al. An open trial of divalproex sodium in autism
spectrum disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 2001;62:530–4.
89.
Plioplys AV. Autism: electroencephalogram abnormalities and clinical improvement with valproic
acid. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1994;148:220–2.
90.
The autistic syndromes. New York, NY: American Elsevier Publishing Company, Inc, 1976.
91.
Spiker D, Lotspeich L, Hallmayer J, et al. Failure to find cytogenetic abnormalities in autistic
children whose parents grew up near plastics manufacturing sites. J Autism Dev Disord
1993;23:681–2.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
46
92.
Massachusetts Department of Public Health. Leominster environment and health investigation.
Boston, MA: Environmental Epidemiology Program, The Bureau of Environmental Health
Assessment, Massachusetts Department of Public Health, 1997.
93.
Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry. Chemical specific consultation: hazardous
substance exposures and autism. Atlanta, GA: Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry,
Division of Toxicology, Emergency Response and Scientific Assessment Branch, 1999.
94.
Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry. Public health assessment: Brick Township
investigation, Brick Township, Ocean County, New Jersey. Atlanta, GA: Agency for Toxic
Substances and Disease Registry, Division of Health Assessment and Consultation, Superfund
Site Assessment Branch, 2000.
95.
Bauman ML, Kemper TL. Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future
directions. Intl J Dev Neuroscience 2005;23(2-3)183-7.
96.
Zeilinski BA, Anderson JS, Froehlich AL, et al. scMRI reveals large-sclae brain network
abnormalities in autism. Obtained on October 10, 2013 from:
http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0049172;jsessionid=60B7
3ECF39A9FFCA4E2B243DC0BDDC83.
97.
Schmitz N, Daly E, Murphy D. Frontal anatomy and reaction time in autism. Neurosci Lett
2007;412:12–17.
98.
Hardan AY, Girgis RR, Lacerda AL, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging study of the orbitofrontal
cortex in autism. J Child Neurol 2006;21:866–71.
99.
Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, et al. Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and
social cognition network in autism. Cereb Cortex 2006;16:1276–82.
100. Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Gerig G, et al. Cortical gray and white brain tissue volume in adolescents and
adults with autism. Biol Psychiatry 2006;59:1–6.
101. Schumann CM, Bloss CS, Barnes CC, et al. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of
cortical development through early childhood in autism. J Neurosci 2010;30:4419–27.
102. Hardan AY, Muddasani S, Vemulapalli M, et al. An MRI study of increased cortical thickness in
autism. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1290–92.
103. Bonilha L, Cendes F, Rorden C, et al. Gray and white matter imbalance–typical structural
abnormality underlying classic autism? Brain Dev 2008;30:396–401.
104. Mitchell SR, Reiss AL, Tatusko DH, et al. Neuroanatomic alterations and social and communication
deficits in monozygotic twins discordant for autism disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2009;166:917–25.
105. Carper RA, Courchesne E. Localized enlargement of the frontal cortex in early autism. Biol
Psychiatry 2005;57:126–33.
106. Hyde KL, Samson F, Evans AC, et al. Neuroanatomical differences in brain areas implicated in
perceptual and other core features of autism revealed by cortical thickness analysis and voxelbased morphometry. Hum Brain Mapp 2010;31:556–66.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
47
107. Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Makris N, et al. Localization of white matter volume increase in autism and
developmental language disorder. Ann Neurol 2004;55:530–40.
108. Amaral DG, Schumann CM, Nordahl CW. Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends Neurosci 2008;31:137–
45.
109. Hazlett HC, Poe M, Gerig G, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of
brain size in autism: birth through age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:1366–76.
110. Vidal CN, Nicolson R, DeVito TJ, et al. Mapping corpus callosum deficits in autism: an index of
aberrant cortical connectivity. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:218–25.
111. Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in Autism.
Neuroimage 2007;34:61–73.
112. Bauman M, Kemper T. ASD. 2010.
113. Strock M. Autism spectrum disorders (pervasive developmental disorders).2007. Photo obtained
October 14, 2013 from http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Autismbrain.jpg
114. UNC School of Medicine. Brain enlargement in autism due to brain changes occurring before age
2. 2011. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.med.unc.edu/www/newsarchive/2011/may/brain-enlargement-in-autism-due-to-brainchanges-occurring-before-age-2
115. UC Davis Health System. Excessive cerebrospinal fluid and enlarged brain size in infancy are
potential biomarkers for autism. 2013. Obtained October 14, 2013 from
http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/publish/news/newsroom/7884
116. Nordahl CW, Lange N, Li DD, et al. Brain enlargement is associated with regression in preschoolage boys with autism spectrum disorders. PNAS. 1-6.
117. Delong GR, Bean SC, Brown FR. Acquired reversible autistic syndrome in acute encephalopathic
illness in children. Arch Neurol 1981;38:191–4.
118. Gillberg C. Onset at age 14 of a typical autistic syndrome. A case report of a girl with herpes
simplex encephalitis. J Autism Dev Disord 1986;16:369–75.
119. Gillberg IC. Autistic syndrome with onset at age 31 years herpes encephalitis as a possible model
for childhood autism. Dev Med Child Neurol 1991;33:920–4.
120. Gillberg C, Coleman M. Infectious diseases. The biology of the autistic syndromes. London, United
Kingdom: Mac Keith Press, 1992:218–25.
121. Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Viral exposure and autism. Am J Epidemiol 1979;109:628–38.
122. Birmingham K, Cimons M. Reactions to MMR immunization scare. Nat Med 1998;4:478–9.
123. Taylor B, Miller E, Farrington CP, et al. Autism and measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine: no
epidemiological evidence for a causal association. Lancet 1999;353:2026–9.
124. Fombonne E, Chakrabarti S. No evidence for a new variant of measles-mumps-rubella-induced
autism. Pediatrics 2001;108:E58.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
48
125. Dales L, Hammer SJ, Smith NJ. Time trends in autism and in MMR immunization coverage in
California. JAMA 2001;285:1183–5.
126. Kaye JA, Melero-Montez MDM, Jick H. Mumps, measles, and rubella vaccine and the incidence of
autism recorded by general practitioners: a time trend analysis. BMJ 2001;322:460–3.
127. Gillberg C, Heijbel H. MMR and autism. Autism 1998;2:423–4.
128. Farrington CP, Miller E, Taylor B. MMR and autism: further evidence against a causal association.
Vaccine 2001;19:3632–5.
129. Madsen K, Hvid A, Vestergaard M, et al. A population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella
vaccination and autism. N Engl J Med 2002;347:1477–82.
130. US Food and Drug Administration. Thimerosal in vaccines. Rockville, MD: Center for Biologics
Evaluation and Research, US Food and Drug Administration, 2002.
131. Magos L, Brown AW, Sparrow S, et al. The comparative toxicology of ethyl- and methylmercury.
Arch Toxicol 1985;57:260–7.
132. Zhang J. Clinical observations in ethyl mercury chloride poisoning. Am J Ind Med 1984;5:251–8.
133. Immunization Safety Review Committee, Board on Health Promotion and Disease Prevention,
Institute of Medicine. Immunization safety review: thimerosal-containing vaccines and
neurodevelopmental disorders. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001.
134. Gardener H, Spiegelman D, Buka SL. Perinatal and neonatal risk factors for autism: a
comprehensive meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2011;128(2)344-55.
135. Durkin MS, Maenner MJ, Newschaffer CJ. Advanced parental age and the risk of autism spectrum
disorder. Am J Epidemiol 2008;168(11):1268-76.
136. Cheslak-Postova K, Liu K, Bearman PS. Closely spaced pregnancies are associated with
increased odds of autism in California sibling births. Pediatrics 2011;127(2)246-53.
ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com ce4less.com
49