* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Smooth Pursuit Impairment in Schizophrenia— What Does It Mean?
Persistent vegetative state wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Neuroscience in space wikipedia , lookup
Irving Gottesman wikipedia , lookup
Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup
Transsaccadic memory wikipedia , lookup
Sluggish schizophrenia wikipedia , lookup
Superior colliculus wikipedia , lookup
Controversy surrounding psychiatry wikipedia , lookup
37 VOL 9, NO. 1, 1983 Smooth Pursuit Impairment in Schizophrenia— What Does It Mean? Abstract The significance and nature of smooth pursuit impairments in schizophrenia are considered. Studies of potential artifacts show that the phenomenon is not due to medications, age, inattention, or poor motivation. Abnormal pursuit eye movements in schizophrenics consist of intrusive saccades which are present during any visually guided slow r •. t movements, and in some patients are related to nonvoluntary attention. These results rule out a generalized motor or oculumotor dysfunction, as well as a specific pursuit system dysfunction. The findings are consistent with a cortical dysfunction which results in saccadlc disinhibition. Questions are raised for future research. The first question to be considered is whether the phenomenon of smooth pursuit impairment may be an artifact. Is it merely a drug effect? Is it related to subjects' age? Does it represent simple lack of interest in the tracking task? The data relating to these and other questions will be briefly summarized here. A more detailed review can be found in Lipton et al. (this issue). The nature of smooth pursuit impairment will also be discussed, and some hypotheses about the underlying dysfunction and its localization will be raised. Drug Effects The earliest reports of smooth pursuit impairment in psychiatric patients were made before the era of antipsychotic medications (Diefendorf and Dodge 1908; Couch and Fox 1934; White 1938). There can be little doubt, then, that eye tracking dysfunction (ETD) is not merely a motor side effect of neuroleptic or other antipsychotic medications. Later studies found no differences between medicated and unmedicated patients (Holzman et al. 1974; Shagass, Roemer, and Amadeo 1974; Karson 1979), or between patients' performance off and on medication (Spohn 1981). Quality of pursuit remains stable, in repeated measurements, in spite of changes in clinical state related to drug treatment (Levy, Lipton, and Holzman 1982). Furthermore, first-degree relatives who themselves were not ill and were not being treated pharmacologically showed the same kind of ETD as did patients (Holzman et al. 1974; Kuechenmeister et al. 1977; Holzman et al. 1978, 1980). Thus the available evidence suggests that ETD is not an artifact of antipsychotic medication. Age Effects Smooth pursuit is an age-dependent motor system (Sharpe and Sylvester 1978). Young adult subjects, whether healthy or psychiatrically affected, are better trackers than subjects over 50 years old (Shagass, Roemer, and Amadeo 1974; Holzman and Levy 1977; Kuechenmeister et al. 1977; Spooner, Sakala, and Baloh 1980), or children under 13 (Miller, Arboleda, and Holzman 1982). This raises the possibility that age accounts for the prevalence of ETD in psychiatric populations, particularly in those found in long-term custodial hospitals. Holzman and Levy (1977) reported, however, that age accounted for only 15 percent of the variance of a quantitative measure of ETD. Furthermore, ETD in a sample of young recent-break schizophrenics (mean age — 23 years, SD = 4) was significantly greater than in age-matched normal controls (Lipton, Levin, and Holzman 1980). ETD was also not related to age in groups of identical SCHIZOPHRENIA BULLETIN 38 twins discordant for schizophrenia (Holzman et al. 1978, 1980). Thus, although studies of eye movement function must control for age, the phenomenon of smooth pursuit impairment in psychiatric patients is not an artifact of age. Demographic Background and Chronicity Holzman et al. (1974) first reported that ETD was more prevalent in a state hospital sample of chronic schizophrenic patients than in patients from a private psychiatric institution. Up to 85 percent of schizophrenic patients had ETD in a state hospital, whereas only 50 percent did in a private psychiatric institution. Similar rates, respectively, were reported by Lipton, Levin, and Holzman (1980) for another private hospital, and by Latham et al. (1981) for a state facility. Two likely explanations for these findings must be considered. The sample differences may be related to idiosyncratic characteristics of different hospitals. Alternatively, they may reflect spurious correlations between the type of hospital and sample characteristics such as chronicity and socioeconomic status (SES). The first explanation is weakened by the similarity between types of hospitals across studies. The fact that findings of ETD have now been replicated in more than a dozen samples across the world (see review in Lipton et al., this issue) also provides a reasonable control for hospitalization variables. The second explanation is less easily dismissed. The structure of psychiatric services is such that the majority of patients in private psychiatric hospitals have a high enough SES to afford medical insurance; even these patients are generally recentbreak or ambulatory patients who have not exhausted their medical coverage resources. In contrast, patients in state hospitals have lower SES and have been sick most of their lives. SES is related to social, psychological, and organismic competence, and therefore also to illnessspecific variables such as chronicity and severity. Thus, sample differences in the prevalence of ETD may not be trivial artifacts, but rather provide meaningful inroads to further the study of etiological heterogeneity in schizophrenia. Attention Schizophrenic patients are emotionally and socially withdrawn; they are frequently lost in a world of delusions and hallucinations. It has been suggested that ETD is merely a reflection of these patients' inattention and lack of involvement in the tracking task (Couch and Fox 1934; Brezinova and Kendell 1977). Brezinova and Kendell (1977) studied the effects of stress, fatigue, and distraction in normal subjects and reported that distraction produced ETD. They concluded that ETD in schizophrenia was due to distraction. Acker and Toone (1978) also reported that pursuit in normal subjects deteriorated as a function of the degree of difficulty of a distracting task, and that among patients, anxious schizophrenics had the worst tracking. Holzman, Levy, and Proctor (1978) followed William James (1950) and Kraepelin (1919) in distinguishing between involuntary or passive attention and voluntary or active attention. They suggested that ETD in schizophrenia was related to a dysfunction of passive, involuntary attention and not to lack of motivation or inattentiveness. This conclusion was based on several findings. Verbal alerting instructions have no effect on the quality of pursuit, al- though patients who have stopped tracking do return to the task (Holzman, Proctor, and Hughes 1973; Shagass, Roemer, and Amadeo 1974; Pivik 1979a). Stronger motivational manipulations, such as giving patients cigarettes or money for good performance, are similarly ineffective in improving tracking (May 1979). The findings that saccadic eye movements of schizophrenic patients are normal, and particularly saccadic reaction times, further weaken the proposition that ETD is an artifact of inattention (Diefendorf and Dodge 1908; lacono, Tuason, and Johnson 1981; Levin, Lipton, and Holzman 1981; Levin et al., 1982b). Moreover, with respect to the finding of Brezinova and Kendell (1977), it is of interest that stress, fatigue, or moderate distraction did not impair tracking. Smooth pursuit in this study was only impaired when subjects had to do a difficult mental calculation (serially subtracting 13 from 200) and write down the result while following a moving target. Lipton, Frost, and Holzman (1980) showed that even this complex task, itself requiring eye movements, disrupts normal pursuit in a manner that can be reliably distinguished from ETD of schizophrenic patients. Distraction by competing tasks also did not affect schizophrenics' tracking compared to their baseline (Pass et al. 1978). Taken together, these findings suggest that mere lack of motivation or inattentiveness, while possibly affecting the subject's ability to track at all, does not produce the characteristic ETD in schizophrenia. Nonvoluntary attention, or the ability to center on and grasp the target, may be involved, however, in ETD in schizophrenia. Tasks which introduce unpredictable changes in the target—changing numbers, changing colors, intermittent second 39 VOL.9, NO. 1, 1983 target, or interruptions in tracking light—evoke optimal levels of visual centering (e.g., Holzman, Levy, and Proctor 1976; Shagass, Amadeo, and Overton 1976; Pivik 1979a). Patients are not aware that their tracking improves, or that it deteriorates again as soon as the attentional task is removed, suggesting that the effect is mediated by a nonvoluntary process. Furthermore, since attentional manipulations also improve ETD caused by central nervous system depressants, such as alcohol and barbiturates, ETD is not primarily related to poor motivation and inattention (Levy, Lipton, and Holzman 1981). A further clue about the role of nonvoluntary attention in ETD is provided by the finding of two basic types of smooth pursuit impairment (Holzman, Levy, and Proctor 1978). Type I tracking consists of pursuit that is almost completely replaced by large irregular saccades, whereas in type II, small amplitude saccades are superimposed on smooth pursuit tracking and give it a "spiky" or cogwheeled appearance. Type I tracking tends to normalize with an attentional task, suggesting that it is related to a dysfunction of nonvoluntary attention. In contrast, type II tracking is relatively unaffected by attentional tasks. This suggests that in a subgroup of poor trackers ETD is not directly related to a dysfunction of attentional focus or "auffassung" (Holzman, Levy, and Proctor 1978). The fact that in type I trackers ETD reappears as soon as the attentional task is removed indicates that, even in these patients, ETD is related to a central nervous system (CNS) disorder which the attentional tasks may only temporarily circumvent or override. In conclusion the available evidence suggests that ETD in schizophrenia is not an artifact of poor mo- tivation, inattention, or other aspects of voluntary attention. The ameliorative effect of attention-enhancing tasks on ETD in some patients indicates, however, that nonvoluntary attention may be involved in the pursuit impairment, although it is not the primary underlying dysfunction. Rather, the data suggest that ETD in the functional psychoses is closely related to a physiological, CNS dysfunction. The following section will review the nature of this dysfunction. Characterization of ETD in Schizophrenia There are three basic types of versional eye movements: smooth pursuit, saccades, and vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) movements. To define and characterize the nature of ETD in schizophrenic patients, it is useful to examine these versional eye movements under a variety of tracking conditions. Since smooth pursuit, saccades, and VOR movements are controlled in different parts of the brain, a comparison of these different eye movement systems can provide clues about both the nature of the dysfunction and its possible localization. Types of Impaired Eye Movements. With respect to the type of versional eye movement that is affected in schizophrenia, Levy, Holzman, and Proctor (1978) and Latham et al. (1981) found a dissociation between vestibular and visual eye movement control. Levy, Holzman, and Proctor (1978) used caloric stimulation to induce nystagmus eye movements, and reported that nystagmus response in schizophrenic patients was essentially normal. Latham et al. (1981) tested optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) under two types of conditions, a full-field stimulus which is thought to stimu- late a subcortical vestibular pathway, and a partial-field stimulus which is considered identical to visual pursuit with refixation saccades. Latham et al. (1981) found that schizophrenic patients who had smooth pursuit dysfunction also had impaired partial-field OKN, but their full-field OKN was intact. Several studies examined tracking under different tracking conditions. Lipton, Levin, and Holzman (1980) first reported that when schizophrenic patients generate slow eye movements via the vestibulo-ocular reflex—by fixing their eyes on a stationary target while rotating their head—these eye movements are quite smooth and free of intruding saccades. The same patients had smooth pursuit impairment when tracking a moving target. Levin et al. (1982a) confirmed this finding and further showed that compensatory VORs made while refixating a target with both head and eyes (head-eye saccades) also appeared normal and were free of saccades. In contrast, in patients tracking a moving target with both head and eyes (head-eye pursuit), pursuit eye movements were impaired by saccadic intrusions as they were with the head restrained. These findings suggest that generation of slow eye movements is adequate when it is based on vestibular signals but not when it is based on visual signals. These nystagmus and VOR findings have several functional and anatomical implications. First, they suggest that ETD in schizophrenia does not affect all eye movements, and not even all slow eye movements, but is restricted to slow pursuit of visual targets. Second, they suggest that the midbrain and brainstem mechanisms for control of VOR movements are intact in schizophrenia. These include the motor system SCHIZOPHRENIA BULLETIN 40 of eye movement control, which receives the direct vestibular-nudear projection in the VOR arc, and the vestibulocerebellum (the flocculonodule), which receives ascending vestibular-apparatus fibers (Cohen 1971, 1974). Since smooth pursuit control converges with the vestibular pathways at both the premotor cerebellar and oculomotor levels, the nystagmus and VOR findings argue against an involvement of these areas in pursuit impairment. Although the vestibular findings indicate that a lower motor pathway or a cerebellar dysfunction is unlikely, they do not address the possibility that ETD may be localized at the horizontal gaze center, in the paramedian pontine reticular formation (PPRF). This hypothesis is weakened by several findings that saccadic eye movements, whose subcortical control center converges with horizontal pursuit at the PPRF, are intact (Diefendorf and Dodge 1908; Iacono, Tuason, and Johnson 1981; Levin et al. 1981, 1982b). Visually guided saccades of schizophrenic patients who have smooth pursuit impairment have normal saccadic latencies (Diefendorf and Dodge, 1908; Iacono, Tuason, and Johnson 1981; Levin et al. 1981), and normal trajectories (Levin et al., 1982b). Furthermore, their dynamic characteristics—the relationships between the speed and duration of the eye movements, and their size—were the same as those seen in normal subjects (Levin et al. 1981, 1982b). In sum, the evidence for clear dissociations between pursuit impairment on the one hand and vestibuloocular and saccadic functions on the other suggests that ETD in schizophrenia is not related to a brainstem dysfunction. Furthermore, these findings argue against the hypothesis that pursuit impairment reflects a general- ized oculomotor dysfunction, or a dysfunction of slow eye movements in general. A generalized motor slowing is also ruled out by these findings, as well as by data showing that schizophrenics have normal visual but slowed hand reaction times (Iacono, Tuason, and Johnson 1981; Levin et al. 1981). Finally, the dissociation between pursuit and saccades, and the finding of impaired cortical OKN, both suggest that ETD in schizophrenia may be related to a cortical dysfunction. Precis* Pattern of Impairment. With respect to the precise pattern of ETD in schizophrenia, Diefendorf and Dodge (1908) showed photographic records of smooth pursuit eye movements which were disrupted by saccades. They called this pattern "stepping." A recent study using highresolution reflected light recording techniques showed that smooth pursuit impairments in schizophrenic patients consist of saccadic intrusions and saccadic smooth pursuit tracking (Levin et al. 1982a). Saccadic intrusions are small back-to-back saccades that are superimposed on smooth pursuit; they are also seen in normal subjects (Bahill, Clark, and Stark 1975; Herishanu and Sharpe 1981), as well as in people with dyslexia (Adler-Grinberg and Stark 1978), strabismus amblyopia (Ciuffreda, Kenyon, and Stark 1979a, 1979b), and cerebellar disease (Selhorst et al. 1976). Saccadic smooth pursuit tracking consists of small saccadic steps, which intermittently or continuously replace smooth pursuit tracking and are similar to the "staircase" tracking in patients with cerebellar atrophy (Baloh, Konrad, and Honrubia 1975). Both these types of saccadic interruptions coexist in pursuit eye movements of individual patients, and appear to correspond to type II tracking (Holzman, Levy, and Proctor 1978). Although several other studies reported that ETD in schizophrenia consists of saccadic interruptions of pursuit, these reports were put in question by studies showing discrepancies between eye movement records obtained with electro-oculographic (EOG) and infrared (IR) recording techniques (LJndsey et al. 1978; Iacono and Lykken 1981). The findings by Levin et al. (1982a) show unambiguously that smooth pursuit in some schizophrenic patients is in fact disrupted by saccadic intrusions. Since clear recordings of these eye movements require a recording system with a frequency response bandwidth of DC to 150 Hz, it may be possible that the IR recordings reported by both Lindsey et al. (1978) and Iacono and Lykken (1981) did not represent their data accurately, since they were traced by a polygraph with a slow pen and a lower high-frequency cutoff. This does not rule out, however, the possibility that EOG recordings reflect, in addition to smoothed out tracings of saccades, evidence for other bioelectric signals picked up by the skin-surface electrodes (Iacono and Lykken, this issue). The presence of saccadic intrusions and saccadic smooth pursuit tracking further clarifies the process that may underlie ETD in schizophrenia. Holzman, Levy, and Proctor (1978) suggested that smooth pursuit impairment may reflect a disinhibition of saccades during pursuit tracking, and related this dysfunction to other disinhibitory phenomena in psychopathology, such as sensory proprioceptive disorders (Holzman 1969) and thought disorder (Holzman 1978). This interpretation is consistent with the dual-mode control of visual tracking, whereby tracking is accomplished by both the saccadic and 41 VOL 9, NO. 1, 1983 smooth pursuit control systems working independently but in concert, with inhibitory feedback regulation (Rashbass 1961; Stark 1971). Such feedback mechanisms ensure that saccades will be inhibited as long as smooth pursuit is accurate and efficient. They are disinhibited when target position errors occur— for example, when pursuit becomes sluggish, when pursuit gain is too high, and when target velocity changes suddenly or exceeds the velocity capacity of the pursuit system. Saccadic eye movements may also interrupt smooth pursuit when other visual targets of interest compete for the tracker's attention. Thus normal smooth pursuit depends on the presence of a slowly moving visual target, on the ability of the smooth pursuit system to follow the target with appropriate speed, and on the ability of the saccadic system to remain silent except when position errors occur. According to the model of dualmode control of visual tracking, ETD in schizophrenia may be due to the following conditions: (1) Schizophrenic patients suffer from an attentional dysfunction which impairs their ability to focus on the moving target; consequently they make extraneous saccadic movements in the direction of distracting peripheral targets. (2) The pursuit system is inefficient in schizophrenia, with the result that pursuit velocity and gain do not match the target, generating position errors that require corrective saccades. (3) The feedback mechanisms whose function is to inhibit saccades during dual-mode control of pursuit are dysfunctional, so that unnecessary saccades are made during tracking. The data reviewed above suggest that for some schizophrenic patients—those with saccadic intrusions and type II tracking—the first hypothesis is not very likely. As previously mentioned, attentional tasks that aid attentional focus on the target did not improve tracking for type II trackers. The fact that saccadic intrusions are regular and small in size (.5-2.5 degrees of visual arc) suggests that they are not used to scan the visual periphery for distracting targets. The data do suggest, however, that another subgroup of schizophrenic patients—those with type I tracking—may in fact have an impaired ability to focus on the tracking target, and a predilection for distractibility by peripheral stimuli. The second hypothesis, of a specific pursuit dysfunction, has not been examined directly, although some converging evidence renders it unlikely. First, several studies reported no significant effect of target frequency on ETD in schizophrenia, suggesting that schizophrenic patients' pursuit mechanisms for velocity matching are comparable to those of normal subjects (Kuechenmeister et al. 1977; Cegalis and Sweeney 1979; May 1979; Pivik 1979fe; Levin, Lipton, and Holzman 1981). Second, visual inspection and frequency analyses of tracking records suggest that there is no relationship between saccadic intrusions and the phase of a tracking cycle, indicating that changes in target velocity do not affect the pursuit system (Bird, Levin, and Holzman 1982). Most important, saccadic intrusions appear not only during smooth pursuit, but also during fixation of a stationary target, suggesting that saccadic intrusions are not related to target movement, but to any condition of continuous visual tracking (Levin et al., 1982a). Taken together, these findings weaken an explanation positing a specific dysfunction of pursuit eye movements, although direct measurements of pur- suit velocity and gain in schizophrenic patients remain to be conducted. At the same time, the findings of saccadic intrusions and smooth pursuit tracking support the hypothesis of disinhibition of saccades. Since there is considerable evidence for the role of suprabulbar mechanisms in attention and eye movements, and particularly in disinhibition of saccades (e.g., Goldberg and Bushnell 1981; see review in Levin 1982), this argument is consistent with the proposition that ETD in schizophrenia reflects a cortical brain dysfunction. Conclusion Available studies of the nature of ETD in schizophrenia show that the phenomenon is not an artifact of antipsychotic drugs, age, inattention, or poor motivation. Other incidental artifacts such as length of hospitalization or SES are of relevance only inasmuch as they are related to the etiology and severity of the schizophrenic illness. Studies comparing ETD under a variety of tracking conditions show that ETD is not a generalized motor or oculomotor dysfunction, and that it is specific to conditions which require continuous visual tracking. The precise characterization of saccadic intrusions during pursuit and fixation eye movements renders a specific dysfunction of the pursuit system unlikely, and supports a hypothesis of a cortical dysfunction that results in saccadic disinhibition. The dissociation of pursuit dysfunction from vestibulo-ocular or saccadic movements also suggests a cortical localization for this eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia. Several open questions, however, invite future research. For example, the evidence for two types of tracking in schizophrenic patients, type I 42 and type II, raises the possibility that more than one underlying process may be responsible for ETD in schizophrenia. Since schizophrenia is most likely heterogeneous, it is useful to explore the relationship between ETD patterns and other behavioral and biological phenomena which may distinguish between subgroups of patients. Another question worthy of further study concerns the role of attention in schizophrenics' ETD, particularly with respect to other attentional and information processing dysfunctions. For example, is the process underlying long manual reaction times similar to that responsible for disrupted pursuit? If so, what is the process underlying visual reaction times? Future work on these and other questions will further our understanding of the nature of eye tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenia. References Acker, W., and Toone, B. Attention, eye tracking and schizophrenia. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17:173-181, 1978. Adler-Grinberg, D., and Stark, L. Eye movements, scanpaths, and dyslexia. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 55:557-570, 1978. Bahill, A.T.; Clark, M.R.; and Stark, L. Dynamic overshoot in saccadic eye movements is caused by neurological control signal reversals. Experimental Neurology, 48:107-122, 1975. Baloh, R.W.; Konrad, H.R.; and Honrubia, V. Vestibulo-ocular function in patients with cerebellar atrophy. Neurology, 25:160-168, 1975. Bird, M.; Levin, S.; and Holzman, P.S. "Phase-Related Analysis of Sac- SCHIZOPHRENIA BULLETIN cadic Intrusions During Smooth Pursuit of Psychiatric Patients." In preparation, 1982. Brezinova, V., and Kendell, R.S. Smooth pursuit eye movements of schizophrenics and normal people under stress. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130:59-63, 1977. Cegalis, J.A., and Sweeney, J.A. Eye movements in schizophrenics: A quantitative analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 14:13-26, 1979. Cohen, B. Vestibulo-ocular relations. In: Bach-y-Rita, P.; Collins, C.C.; and Hyde, J.E., eds. The Control of Eve Movements. New York: Academic Press, 1971. pp. 105-148. Cohen, B. The vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. In: Kornhuber, H.H., ed. Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Vestibular System, Part I: Basic Mechanisms. Vol. VI/1. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1974. pp. 478-540. Couch, F.H., and Fox, J.C. Photographic study of ocular movements in mental disease. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 34:556-578, 1934. Ciuffreda, K.J.; Kenyon, R.V.; and Stark, L. Saccadic intrusions in strabismus. Archives of Ophthalmology, 97:1673-1677, 1979a. Ciuffreda, K.J.; Kenyon, R.V.; and Stark, L. Suppression of fixational saccades in strabismlc and anisometropic amblyopia. Ophthalmic Research, 11:31-39, 1979b. Diefendorf, A.R., and Dodge, R. An experimental study of the ocular reactions of the insane from photographic records. Brain, 31:451-489, 1908. Goldberg, M.E., and Bushnell, M.C. Role of the frontal eye fields in visually guided saccades. In: Fuchs, A.F., and Becker, W., eds. Progress in Occulomotor Research. New York: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981. pp. 185-192. Herishanu, Y.O., and Sharpe, J.A. Normal square wave jerks. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 20:268-272, 1981. Holzman, P.S. Perceptual aspects of psychopathology. In: Zubin, ]., and Shagass, C , eds. Neurobiological Aspects of Psychopathology. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1969. pp. 144-178. Holzman, P.S. Cognitive impairment and cognitive stability: Towards a theory of thought disorder. In: Serban, G., ed. Cognitive Defects in the Development of Mental Illness. New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1978. pp. 361-376. Holzman, P.S.; Kringlen, E.; Levy, D.L.; and Haberman, S. Deviant eye tracking in twins discordant for psychosis: A replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37:627-631, 1980. Holzman, P.S.; Kringlen, E.; Levy, D.L.; Proctor, L.R.; and Haberman, S. Smooth pursuit eye movements in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 14:111-120, 1978. Holzman, P.S., and Levy, D.L. Smooth pursuit eye movements and functional psychoses: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 3:15-27, 1977. Holzman, P.S.; Levy, D.L.; and Proctor, L.R. Smooth-pursuit eye movements, attention, and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33:1415-1420, 1976. . Holzman, P.S.; Levy, D.L.; and Proctor, L.R. The several qualities of attention in schizophrenia. In: Wynne, L.C.; Cromwell, R.L.; and Matthysse, S., eds. The Nature of Schizophrenia. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1978. pp. 295-306. 43 VOL. 9, NO. 1, 1983 Holzman, P.S.; Proctor, L.R.; and Hughes, D.W. Eye tracking patterns in schizophrenia. Science, 181: 179-181, 1973. Levin, S.; Holzman, P.S.; Rothenberg, S.J.; and Upton, R.B. Saccadic eye movements in psychotic patients. Psychiatry Research, 5:47-58, 1981. pursuit eye movements, the oculocephalic reflex, and the functional psychoses. Psychiatry Research, 3:193-203, 1980. Holzman, P.S.; Proctor, L.R.; Levy, D.L.; Yasillo, N.J.; Meltzer, H.Y.; and Hurt, S.W. Eye-tracking dysfunction in schizophrenic patients and their relatives. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31:143-151, 1974. Levin S.; Jones, A.; Stark, L.; Merrin, E.L.; and Holzman, P.S. Identification of abnormal patterns in eye movements of schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39:1125-1130, 1982a. May, H.J. Oculomotor pursuit in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36:827, 1979. Iacono, W.G., and Lykken, D.T. Two-year retest stability of eye tracking performance and a comparison of electro-oculographlc and infrared recording techniques: Evidence of EEG in the electro-oculogram. Psychophysiology, 18:49-55, 1981. Levin, S.; Jones, A.; Stark, L.; Merrin, EX.; and Holzman, P.S. Saccadic eye movements of schizophrenic patients measured by reflected light techniques. Biological Psychiatry, 17:1277-1287, 1982b. Iacono, W.G.; Tuason, V.B.; and Johnson, R.A. Dissociation of smooth-pursuit and saccadic eye tracking in remitted schizophrenics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38:991-996, 1981. James, W. Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover, 1950. Karson, C.N. Oculomotor signs in a psychiatric population: A preliminary report. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136:1057-1060, 1979. Kraepelin, E. Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Edinburgh: Livingston, 1919. Kuechenmelster, C.A.; Linton, P.H.; Mueller, T.V.; and White, H.B. Eye tracking in relation to age, sex, and illness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34:578-599, 1977. Latham, C ; Holzman, P.S.; Manschrek, R.; and Tole, J. Optokinetic nystagmus and pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38:997-1003, 1981. Levin, S. "Localization of Eye Movements and Attention Deficits in Schizophrenia to Frontal Cortex: A Theoretical Review." In preparation, 1982. Levin, S.; Upton, R.B.; and Holzman, P.S. Pursuit eye movements in psychopathology: Effects of target characteristics. Biological Psychiatry, 16:255-267, 1981. Levy, D.L.; Holzman, P.S.; and Proctor, L.R. Vestibular responses in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35:972-981, 1978. Levy, D.L.; Upton, R.B.; and Holzman, P.S. Smooth-pursuit eye movements: Effects of alcohol and chloral hydrate. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 16:1-11, 1981. Levy, D.L.; Upton, R.B.; and Holzman, P.S. "Effects of Haldol on Eye Movements and Psychopathology in Schizophrenia." In preparation, 1982. Undsey D.T.; Holzman, P.S.; Haberman, S.; and Yasillo, N.J. Smoothpursuit eye movements: A comparison of two measurement techniques for studying schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87:491-496, 1978. Upton, R.B.; Frost, L.A.; and Holzman, P.S. Smooth:pursuit eye movements, schizophrenia, and distraction. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50:159-167, 1980. Upton, R.B.; Levin, S.; and Holzman, P.S. Horizontal and vertical Miller, M.J.; Arboleda, C ; and Holzman, P.S. "Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements: Development in Normal Children." In preparation, 1982. Pass, H.L.; Salzman, L.F.; Klorman, R.; Kaskey, G.B.; and Klein, R.H. The effects of distraction on acute schizophrenics' visual tracking. Biological Psychiatry, 13:587-593, 1978. Pivik, R.T. Smooth pursuit eye movements and attention in psychiatric patients. Biological Psychiatry, 14:859-879, 1979a. Pivik, R.T. Target velocity and smooth pursuit eye movements in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Research, 1:313-323, 1979b. Rashbass, C. The relationahip between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements. Journal of Physiology, 159:326-338, 1961. Selhorst, J.B.; Stark, L.; Ochs, A.L.; and Hoyt, W.F. Disorders in cerebellar ocular motor control. II. Macrosaccadic oscillation and oculographic control system and clinico-anatomical analysis. Brain, 99:509-522, 1976. Shagass, C ; Amadeo, M.; and Overton, D.A. Eye-tracking performance and engagement of attention. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33:121-125, 1976. Shagass, C ; Roemer, R.A.; and Amadeo, M. Eye-tracking performance in psychiatric patients. Biological Psychiatry, 9:245-260, 1974. Sharpe, J.A., and Sylvester, T.O. Effect of aging on horizontal smoothpursuit. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 17:465-470, 1978. SCHIZOPHRENIA BULLETIN Spohn, H.E. "Correlates of Eye Tracking in Chronic Schizophrenics." Presented at the NIMH workshop on eye tracking impairments in psychopathology, Cambridge, MA, 1981. Stark, L. The control system for versional eye movements. In: The Control of Eye Movements. New York: Academic Press, 1971. pp. 363-428. Spooner, J.W.; Sakala, S.M.; and Baloh, R.W. Effect of aging on eye tracking. Archives of Neurology, 37:575-576, 1980. White, H.R. Ocular pursuit in normal and psychopathological subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22:17-31, 1938. The Assessment of Smooth Tracking Dysfunction Abstract The two main techniques for recording eye movements, electro-oculography (EOG) and infrared reflection (IR), are discussed and compared. Methods of quantifying eye movement dysfunction are also reviewed and contrasted. It is concluded that EOG and IR recordings both contain information not present in the other and that both methods will continue to be useful. Similarly, although the root-mean-square (RMS) and the signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) indices are algebraic functions of one another, the function is not linear and one or the other may be more appropriate to a particular application. Recording Techniques There are two readily available and popular techniques for measuring eye movements: electro-oculography and infrared reflection. Comprehensive reviews of both recording methods can be found in a variety of sources (Shackel 1967; Tursky 1974; Young and Sheena 1975; Haines 1980; Oster and Stern 1980). Recording of the electro-oculogram (EOG) is possible because a standing potential of about 1 mV exists between the cornea and fundus of the eye. When the eyes move, the volt- Smadar Levin, Ph.D. Mailman Research Center McLean Hospital 115 Mill St. Belmont, MA 20178 age differences generated by shifts in the position of the two globes can be monitored from periorbital electrodes. Horizontal eye movements are usually recorded binocularly from electrodes at the lateral canthi. Monocular recording is also possible, but tends to be less reliable and more subject to artifact than measurements taken across both eyes. Vertical motion is generally monitored monocularly from electrodes at superior and inferior orbital rim placements and is often used to detect eye blinks. For infrared (IR) reflection, an IR light source is positioned slightly below the center of one eye and within several millimeters of the globe. Photodiodes which monitor reflected light from the eye are positioned on either side of the IR source. With this method, the position of the limbus is tracked. Since the sclera has greater reflectivity than the iris and the boundary between the two is relatively sharp, the ratio of reflected light to the sensors on either side of the limbus provides an accurate measure of eye movement. Placement of the light source and photodetectors is critical to obtain a good recording with this technique. Therefore, head position must be fixed in some way, or the IR apparatus must be mounted on goggles or frames that are securely attached to the subject's head. It is worth noting that ex-