Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 1 Psychophysiological correlates of the misinformation effect Katja Volz *, Rainer Leonhart **, Rudolf Stark ***, Dieter Vaitl *, Wolfgang Ambach * * Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Wilhelmstraße 3a, D79098 Freiburg, Germany ** Institute of Psychology, University of Freiburg, Engelbergerstraße 41, D-79085 Freiburg, Germany *** Bender Institute of Neuroimaging, University of Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Straße 10h, D-35394 Giessen, Germany Correspondence: Katja Volz Institut für Grenzgebiete der Psychologie und Psychohygiene, Freiburg, Germany Wilhelmstraße 3a D-79098 Freiburg Germany E-Mail: [email protected] PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 2 Abstract The misinformation effect refers to memory impairment that arises after exposure to misleading information (Loftus, 2005, p. 361). The present study focuses on the peripheral psychophysiology of false memories induced in a misleading information paradigm. On the basis of Sokolov’s orienting reflex and studies concerning the Concealed Information Test (CIT, Lykken, 1959), the main hypothesis assumes differences between true and false memories in terms of the accompanying autonomic measures. It also is assumed that a cued recall of original information preceding the recollection phase reduces misinformation effects. Seventy-five participants watched a video that included nine randomized details. After a tenminute retention phase, the subjects read a narrative text. Six out of the nine details were replaced by misleading details. Following this, the participants completed a cued recall task for three of the original items. In a subsequent CIT with truthful answering electrodermal responses, phasic heart rate, respiration, and response behavior were measured. Finally, the level of confidence and source monitoring were assessed. The misinformation effect was replicated with newly developed materials in three recollection tasks. Cued recall had no influence on the misinformation effect. Autonomic measures did not differ between true and false memories in the CIT. Electrodermal responses reflected the subjective importance the participants attributed to details in the source monitoring task. Therefore, electrodermal responses are interpreted as a correlate of subjective remembering in a misinformation paradigm. Keywords: False memory, paradigm, measures Concealed Electrodermal information activity, Cued test, Misinformation recall, Autonomic PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 1 3 Introduction Eyewitness testimony is crucial for concluding police investigations (Coupe & Griffiths, 1996; Paulo, Albuquerque, & Bull, 2013). However, most police officers do not know that eyewitness memory is often distorted (Kebbell & Milne, 1998). If memory impairment arises after exposure to misleading information, this is called a misinformation effect (Loftus, 2005, p. 361). The vulnerability to false memories has been a focus of memory and forensic research for nearly four decades. However, to date, there have been few studies investigating the physiological correlates of false memory (for a review, see Johnson, Raye, Mitchell, & Ankudowich, 2012; Schacter & Slotnick, 2004). In the present study, we examined peripheral physiological measures as possible indicators of false memories in a misinformation paradigm. Additionally, we aimed to reduce misinformation effects using a cued recall procedure. 1.1 The misinformation effect False memory research dates to the 1970s. In a series of five experiments, Loftus, Miller, and Burns (1978) evoked false memories of a traffic sign. The threestage procedure applied was named the misinformation paradigm and has since been used by various research groups (e.g., Belli, Lindsay, Gales, & McCarthy, 1994; McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985; Tversky & Tuchin, 1989). In the misinformation paradigm, the subjects first watch a video or slides typically showing crime or crimerelated plots. After a distractor or retention phase, the researchers introduced misinformation hidden in a narrative or questions about the event (e.g., “How fast was the car going when it ran the stop sign?” Loftus et al., 1978, p.19). Finally, the subjects complete memory tasks about the event. Typically, forced-choice, recognition, source identification, or level-of-confidence tests are used to gather memory data (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993; Tversky & Tuchin, 1989). If misleading information successfully provoked a misinformation effect, this is reflected in two ways: in reduced recall of original information and enhanced recall of misleading information (Loftus, 2005). To date, there is no clear explanation of how false memories emerge in a misinformation paradigm. Initially, it was assumed that misleading information replaces the original memory (Loftus, 1979; Loftus & Loftus, 1980). Later, an integration of misleading and original information into one mixed memory was PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 4 considered (Loftus & Hoffman, 1989). However, the coexistence of original and misleading information has been demonstrated by several research groups (e.g., Bekerian & Bowers, 1983; Belli, 1988; Wright, 1993). Lindsay and Johnson (1989) assumed that the sources of information are confused during retrieval. In the source monitoring framework (Johnson et al., 1993; for a summary, see Lindsay, 2008) false memory occurs when misleading information is misattributed to the source of the original information. Such source monitoring is driven by judgment processes that interact with several characteristics of memories, which are typical of a specific source that a memory could have (Johnson et al., 1993). Based on the assumptions of the source monitoring framework, the present study employed a cued recall task to reduce misinformation effects. 1.2 Reduction of misinformation effects As it is still unclear which exact processes drive the misinformation effect, it is an open question how it can be reduced reliably. Per the discrepancy detection principle, warnings before the misleading information (Eakin, Schreiber, & SergentMarshall, 2003; Greene, Flynn, & Loftus, 1982) or misinformation received from an unreliable source (Bodner, Musch, & Azad, 2009; Dodd & Bradshaw, 1980) can reduce misinformation effects. However, the evidence the evidence concerning warnings given in between the misleading information phase and the recollection phase is still equivocal (for a review, see Blank & Launay, 2014). The search for a procedure that can be applied after misleading information was given and that reduces the effect of misleading information on memory, is still ongoing. In applied forensic research, this is pursued with two approaches: the Cognitive Interview (CI; Fisher & Geiselman, 1992) and the Self-administered Interview (SAI; Gabbert, Hope, & Fisher, 2008). Both approaches rely mainly on a mental reinstatement of the crime (mentally recreating the context of an event, as well as the physiological, cognitive, and emotional states at the time of an event), that supports correct source identification (Fisher, Geiselman, & Amador, 1989; Memon & Bull, 1991; Paulo et al., 2013). Both interviews were investigated in applied contexts and achieved large effect sizes (Dodson, Powers, & Lytell, 2015; Fisher et al., 1989; Gabbert et al., 2008; Hope, Gabbert, Fisher, & Jamieson, 2014; Köhnken, Thürer, & Zoberbier, 1994; Köhnken, Milne, Memon, & Bull, 1999). For example, a recent metaanalysis found average effect sizes of d = 1.20 for comparisons between correctly PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 5 reported details in the Cognitive Interview compared to control interviews (Memon, Meissner, & Fraser, 2010). In our study, we employed a simple task preceding the recollection phase that aimed to strengthen correct source identification in a misinformation paradigm. We designed the task using the mental reinstatement principle that is utilized in both interview forms of applied forensic research. The simple procedure used the original scene in the video as a cue to facilitate correct source identification. This process resulted in a cued recall task, which will be described later. 1.3 Physiological correlates of false memory The main goal of our study was to examine peripheral physiological measures as possible indicators of false memories in a misleading information paradigm. Past research has mainly comprised studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), or event-related potential (ERP) and other false memory paradigms (for a review, see Johnson et al., 2012). To the authors’ knowledge, only one study has used autonomic measures (Baioui, Ambach, Walter, & Vaitl, 2012). Also, only few studies used the misinformation paradigm (e.g., Okado & Stark, 2005). The combination of autonomic measures and the misinformation paradigm, however, is promising. Autonomic measures might function as sensible indicators of false memories, which rely on the principles of the orienting reflex (OR; Sokolov, 1963). The OR is the physiological, cognitive, and behavioral response to a given stimulus (Sokolov, 1963). Autonomic measures like electrodermal activity (EDA), respiration line length (RLL), and phasic heart rate (pHR) are discussed to reflect this basal process (Sokolov, 1963). The strength of an OR is influenced by the novelty, intensity, and signficance of the stimulus (Sokolov, 1963). The stimulus significance is the special importance and meaning a subject attributes to an item (see also Ambach, Dummel, Lüer, & Vaitl, 2011), and, for this study, the stimulus significance is crucial for examining the physiological responses to true memories compared with false memories. The difference between less and highly significant stimuli is wellreflected, particularly by EDA (Barry, 1996). A method that uses autonomic measures to differentiate between stimuli of different significance is the Concealed Information Test (CIT; Lykken, 1959). PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 6 The CIT is a well-designed and valid method to detect information using physiological measures (for a review, see Ben-Shakhar & Elaad, 2003; Meijer, klein Selle, Elber, & Ben-Shakhar, 2014). The CIT assumes that physiological responses differ between crime-relevant and crime-irrelevant information if a subject has knowledge about a crime (Lykken, 1959). Besides other approaches, the OR is discussed as the main explanation of this difference (Verschuere, Ben-Shakhar, & Meijer, 2011). The CIT asks several questions referring to different crime-relevant categories. Typically, a question (e.g., “Was this fruit lying on the window sill?”) is combined with five items showing possible alternatives. Only subjects with crimerelated knowledge will recognize the right answer and react differently to crimerelated items. In a typical response pattern, test subjects respond to crime-relevant (significant) items with greater EDA and smaller RLL and pHR (Ambach, Stark, Peper, & Vaitl, 2008; Ben-Shakhar & Elaad, 2003; Elaad & Ben-Shakhar 2008; Gamer, Rill, Vossel, & Gödert, 2006; Verschuere, Crombez, de Clercq, & Koster, 2004). The CIT also reflects recognition if the information is not concealed. This outcome is especially true for EDA, which mainly reflects OR, whereas, pHR and RLL are also discussed in the light of concealment processes (Ambach et al., 2008; klein Selle, Verschuere, Kindt, Meijer, & Ben-Shakhar, 2015). In misinformation paradigms, the recognition of original information is impaired by misinformation. Based on orienting theory, we assumed that a special importance and meaning is attributed to the original but not the misleading information; therefore, original information is more significant to the person than misleading information. Referring to the typical response patterns in studies dealing with concealed information, it is assumed that the original information will be accompanied by greater EDA as well as smaller pHR and RLL responses, compared to the misleading or unknown information. Baioui and his colleagues (2012) already examined data on autonomic measures gathered in a Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm (DRM, Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995). In a DRM paradigm, participants first learn lists of closely related words (e.g., bed, pillow, sheet) and are asked to recognize the learned words in an upcoming recognition phase. Often, participants then falsely remember words related to the categories they have studied before (e.g., sleep) (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). In contrast to the misleading information paradigm, PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 7 false memories in a DRM paradigm are not evoked by misleading information but by the activation of a conceptual scheme of the studied items. Baioui and his colleagues (2012) also suggested that false memories are accompanied by less subjective importance and meaning and, thus, by a smaller OR in contrast to true memories. Their results yielded greater EDA responses associated with true memories rather than false memories. No significant effects of pHR or RLL were found. It is still an open question whether this response pattern can be replicated and transferred to a misinformation paradigm. 1.4 Aims of the present study 1.4.1 Methodologically advanced replication of the misinformation effect A methodologically advanced version of the typical misinformation paradigm was applied. The original information was presented in a video instead of slides because of advances in external validity; a video presents motion sequences of action and is thus easier for a person to perceive than slides, which only show a series of snapshots (for a review, see Takarangi, Parker, & Garry, 2006). Additionally, a fully randomized and balanced stimulus set with nine classes of objects comprising five objects each was created. Regarding the approach of an enhanced stimulus set by Takarangi and her colleagues (2006), we shot a video and filmed every scene in five variants, each containing a different critical object of the same object class. Each subject watched a different video, which was composed using a randomization scheme. Subjects were misled by a narrative that was also adjusted by the randomization scheme. Recollection tasks involved recognition, levelof-confidence, source identification, and forced-choice, thereby controlling the effects of single objects or classes. Furthermore, by using different memory tests and examining control categories as well as control items in each class of objects, a great range of memory data could be collected. It was investigated whether the described materials were appropriate to induce a misinformation effect. Also, all classes of objects used underwent exploratory analyses to examine which of them scored highest in evoking false memories. 1.4.2 Reducing misinformation effects by cued recall As a further question, it is proposed that, per the source monitoring framework, a cued recall before the recollection phase may reduce misinformation effects. The PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 8 cued recall task was adapted from the concept of mental reinstatement used in applied forensic research. For this study, the original source of information (i.e., visual information from the video) was used to support the recall of the original information. Successful recall is meant to strengthen the relation between the original source and the original information, which should reduce source confusion in the subsequent recollection tasks. 1.4.3 Autonomic correlates of the misinformation effect A CIT with truthful answering is used to differentiate between the autonomic responses of different types of items and memories. The core assumption of the study is that misinformation and false memories are accompanied by decreased physiological responses of recognition compared to the original information and true memories. This should be reflected in smaller EDA and greater pHR and RLL responses. It is further investigated whether the autonomic correlates differ between source confusion errors (confusing an original source with misleading one) and correct source identifications. 2 Methods 2.1 Participants Seventy-five healthy student participants (49 f., 26 m., age: 23.7 ± 2.7 y.) from different faculties except psychology and neuroscience were recruited via student services and university bulletins. Written consent was obtained prior to the experiment, which met all the ethical requirements per the declaration of Helsinki. Sixteen Euros were paid for participation. 2.2 Design Three different types of categories were varied within subjects: Categories in which no misleading information was induced (three control categories) were compared with categories in which misleading information was introduced (six misled categories). Whether a cued recall task reduces misinformation effects within the mislead categories or not, was investigated in three misled categories with cued recall and three misled categories without cued recall. Each misled category comprised three different types of items: Original items introduced as original information in a video, misleading items serving as misleading information in a PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 9 narrative text, and unknown information used as control items in the recollection phase. Control categories comprised one original and four control items. All factors were manipulated within-subject. 2.3 Materials and procedure The memory-related focus of the study was disguised by the cover story ‘moral sense and cognition’. The experimenter did not mention memory tests in advance. The video that was used to introduce original information lasted approximately 6.5 minutes and showed a young woman stealing a ring and money prior to a job interview. Nine particular items, each drawn from a different class of objects per a randomization procedure described later on, served as the original information. Object classes were name on a door plate, time, picture of a landscape, box, fruit, key pendant, color of an envelope, drink, and playing card. As one class of objects comprised five possible items, each scene showing a relevant object was filmed five times. Finally, the participants viewed a video composed of a basic plot and nine randomized video fragments, twice. They were asked to divide it into meaningful sections and rate every section on a scale of 0 (“morally not reprehensible”) to 100 (“morally reprehensible”). The subsequent distractor tasks were labeled as ‘cognitive performance tasks’. Subjects completed a five-minute Stroop-test and a five-minute two-back-task. Length and difficulty were adapted from former misinformation studies (e.g., Loftus, 1977; McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985; Belli et al., 1994; Chambers & Zaragoza, 2001; Wilford, Chan, & Tuhn, 2014). Subsequently, a narrative text describing the video plot served as misinformation source: Six out of nine original items were randomly replaced by misleading items (e.g., “kiwi” replacing “apple”); the remaining three were described by naming the object class (e.g., “fruit”) and served as control categories. Subjects were asked to read the narrative text, first divide it into meaningful sections, then rate every section (analog to the video task) and assign a brief title to each. Next, the participants received the cued recall disguised as a ‘scenic imagination’ task. Covering each misled category with cued recall, the participants viewed a screenshot of the video with the particular item (e.g., an apple lying on the PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 10 window sill) blackened out. They were asked to imagine the video scene as precisely and in as much detail as possible. Twenty seconds after onset of the screenshot, a verbal question was asked to indicate the class of objects of the searched item (e.g., “Which fruit was lying on the window sill?”), to facilitate the participants’ active recall of the original item. After a self-chosen delay (30 seconds minimum; no maximum), the participants wrote down a description of the blackened object and continued with the next screenshot. Subsequently, the participants were led to an experimental chamber and connected to polygraph leads to complete the CIT with truthful answering. The CIT consisted of nine blocks referring to nine categories. In each block, a question appeared two seconds before the presentation of a particular item (e.g., “Did she take the money out of this envelope?”). Items were presented for 10 seconds as textual pictures (640 x 480 pixels) on a 19-inch monitor at a distance of 90 centimeters, followed by a blank screen lasting for 8 to 10 seconds. Simultaneous with the presentation of an item, two indication fields (yes and no) prompted the participant to answer. If no answer was given, after two seconds, the indication fields disappeared. If an answer was given, the respective field remained visible for the rest of this trial. The participants were required to indicate whether the item shown was included in the video by giving a verbal yes or no answer and pressing one of the two response keys as quickly as possible. The key assignment was balanced between subjects. For each misled category, one original item, one misleading item, and three control items were shown one after the other. In control categories, one original and four control items were shown. The main run was preceded by a training run consisting of two blocks with five neutral items each. This resulted in a total of 55 item presentations. The first item of a category was always a control item, which was discarded from the analysis of physiological measures. The subjects were then detached from the leads and asked to complete three different memory tests. In all these tests, the items were presented as textual pictures. First, the subjects were told to indicate on a scale from -3 (“definitely not seen”) to +3 (“definitely seen”) whether the item appeared in the video (level of confidence). Second, the participants were instructed to decide if the shown item was presented in the video, the text, or during the physiological measurement (source identification). Third, all items of a class of objects were presented simultaneously PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 11 and the participants were asked to choose the item which appeared in the video (forced choice). Each participant received an individually cut video, an individual narrative text, and individual item sequences in all recollection tasks per a randomization scheme. Randomization procedures warranted that the role of each item as original, misleading, or control was balanced across subjects. For the CIT, the sequence of categories was balanced as well as the item sequence within categories; particularly, the relative position of crime-relevant and misleading items was balanced. Randomization was also applied to item sequences in the concluding three-fold memory test. 2.4 Physiological recording Physiological data was recorded in a dimly lit, electrically, and acoustically shielded experimental chamber (Industrial Acoustics GmbH, Niederkrüchten, Germany). The temperature was maintained by air conditioning and was set to approximately 22.4°C at the beginning with a maximum increase of 0.9°C throughout the recording. The subjects sat in an upright position; so they could easily reach the keyboard and watch the 19’-inch’-monitor. Skin conductance, electrocardiogram, respiratory activity, and finger plethysmogram were measured. Physiological data were logged using the Physiological Data System I 410-BCS (J&J Engineering, Poulsbo, Washington), and converted from analog to digital at a resolution of 14 bits, allowing skin conductance to be measured with a resolution of 0.01µS. Stimulus on-/offsets and physiological data were sampled at a rate of 510Hz. Standard Ag/AgCl electrodes (Hellige; diameter 0.8cm), electrode paste of 0.5% saline in a neutral base (TD 246 Skin Resistance, Mansfield R&D, St. Albans, Vermont, UK), and a constant voltage of 0.5V were used for recording skin conductance. Electrodes were placed over the thenar and hypothenar muscles of the non-dominant hand. Electrocardiogram (ECG) was measured using Hellige electrodes (diameter 1.3cm) according to Einthoven II. The thoracic and abdominal respiratory activity was registered by two PS-2 biofeedback respiration sensor belts (KarmaMatters, Berkeley, CA) with a built-in length-dependent electrical resistance. An infrared pulse sensor in a cuff around the PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 12 end phalanx of the middle finger of the non-dominant hand was used to record the finger plethysmogram. 2.5 Data analysis The recorded finger plethysmogram was not analyzed because of insufficient signal quality. Skin conductance data from eight subjects had to be discarded from the analysis because of electrodermal non-response (> 85% of responses < 0.01µS). Responses in skin conductance were defined as an increase in conductance that was initiated within a time period of one to five seconds after image onset. The amplitude of the response was automatically evaluated as the difference between response onset and the subsequent maximal value in the set time window (Furedy, Posner, & Vincent, 1991). Phasic heart rate data from one subject had to be discarded from analysis because of frequent extrasystoles. The remaining data were notch filtered at 50 Hz and underwent an automatic R-wave peak detection prior to visual inspection of the resulting data. The R-R intervals were transformed into heart rate and real-time scaled (Velden & Wölk, 1987). The heart rate during the last second before trial onset served as the pre-stimulus baseline. The phasic heart rate was calculated by subtracting this value from each second-per-second post-stimulus value. For extracting the trial-wise information of the phasic heart rate, the mean change in heart rate within 15 seconds after trial onset compared with the prestimulus baseline was calculated (Bradley & Janisse, 1981; Verschuere, Crombez, Koster, van Bockstaele, & de Clercq, 2007). Respiratory data was manually scanned and low-pass filtered (10dB at 2.8 Hz) for eliminating the artifacts. A method from Timm (1982), modified by Kircher and Raskin (2003) was used to process the data: RLL was computed over a time interval of ten seconds after trial onset. It integrates information on the frequency and depth of respiration. RLL data from both bolts were averaged. In all analyses within-subject z-standardized physiological data were used (Lykken & Venables, 1971; Ben-Shakhar, 1985; Gamer et al., 2006). 2.6 Statistical analysis All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). A sensitivity analysis was conducted using G*Power (version 3.1.9.2, Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). As defined a priori, all subjects with conspicuous values on at least three depending variables were discarded from analysis. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 13 Conspicuous values were defined as all values more than one-and-a-half interquartile range above the seventy-fifth or below the twenty-fifth percentile (Tukey, 1977). This resulted in the exclusion of three subjects. However, no changes in significance occurred when the analyses were conducted with all subjects. Prior to every analysis, the requirements were tested: If variables were not distributed normally a Wilcoxontest instead of a t-test for paired samples was used. For Wilcoxon-tests Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988) was calculated using the formulas r = z/ = (Field, 2009, p. 550) and d (Rosenthal, 1994, p. 239). If the Mauchly’s test indicated a violation of the assumption of sphericity, the degrees of freedom were corrected according to Greenhouse-Geisser. All tests were conducted with an alpha level of 0.05; the alpha level of all comparisons using t-tests was corrected per the Bonferroni-Holm method (Holm, 1979). Regarding the analysis of the misinformation effect, the number of affirmations in the CIT, level of confidence ratings, and the number of correct source identifications of original items were compared between the control and misled categories using a Wilcoxon test. It was further analyzed, if in misled categories with cued recall, source identification errors increased or source identification accuracy decreased, when compared with misled categories without cued recall. One-way repeated measurement ANOVAs were used to analyze the effects of the item type (original vs. misleading vs. control) on the physiological variables. The comparison of physiological variables between true (affirmed original items) and false (affirmed misleading items) memories was conducted using a t-test for paired samples. Finally, the answers in the source identification task were subdivided into confusing (original items attributed to text; misleading items to video), correct (original items to video; misleading items to text), and incorrect (original or misleading items to CIT) source identifications. For all physiological measures a one-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare the source identifications (confusing vs. correct vs. incorrect). 3 Results and discussion 3.1 Replication of the misinformation effect Regarding the behavioral data, the misinformation effect should be reflected in three measures: The number of affirmations in the CIT, the level of confidence (LOC) ratings, and the number of correct source identifications. Descriptive statistics of PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 14 affirmations in the CIT and LOC ratings are provided in Table 1; descriptive statistics of source identifications are presented in figure 1. In the CIT, 9.4% of misleading items were (falsely) affirmed (“false memory rate”). 3.0% of control items were (falsely) affirmed (“basic error rate”). False memory rate exceeded basic error rate, z = -3.37, p < .001, d = 0.57. In the misled compared with control categories the original items were less often affirmed, z = -2.60, p = .009, d = 0.64, the level of confidence for these items was lower, z = -3.19, p = .001, d = 0.81, and the correct source identification of these items decreased, z = -5.41, p < .001, d = 0.78. Figure 1. Answers to original items in the source identification task: percentage of correct (“video”), incorrect (“CIT”), and confusing (“text”) answers, seperately for the three types of categories (left). Answers to misleading items in the source identification task: percentage of correct (“text”), incorrect (“CIT”), and confusing (“video”) answers, seperately for the two types of categories (right). (Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.) PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 15 Table 1. Percentage of affirmations in the CIT and mean level of confidence (LOC) ratings, separate for control, original, and misleading items of the three types of categories. Category Item Control Control Misled without cued recall Original Control Original Misled with cued recall Misleading Control Original Misleading M SEM M SEM M SEM M SEM M SEM M SEM M SEM M SEM CIT 0.030 0.006 0.838 0.026 0.029 0.007 0.773 0.030 0.088 0.020 0.030 0.058 0.743 0.032 0.100 0.021 LOC 1.527 0.066 6.285 0.107 1.505 0.074 6.000 0.125 1.861 0.115 1.491 0.066 5.778 0.147 2.014 0.129 M = mean; SEM = standard error of the mean; LOC rating scale from 1 (“definitely not seen”) to 7 (“definitely seen”) This indicates that a misinformation effect was successfully induced, as reflected in the CIT answers, in the level of confidence, and in the source identifications. The newly developed materials proved to be suitable in replicating the misinformation effects, which were repeatable across three different recollection tasks. To test for differences between the employed classes of objects, the descriptive statistics of the misinformation effect (affirmations to original items in misled vs. control categories in the CIT) and of false memories (affirmations to misleading items in the CIT) were computed separately, for each of the nine classes. Adding up both criteria, drink, name on a doorplate, time, fruit, and key pendant scored highest in distorting memory. This outcome might indicate that memory for numeric or textual classes (e.g., time or name) may be impaired more easily than for pictorial classes (e.g., picture). It is tempting to speculate that visual original information is encoded better, and therefore, is less vulnerable to memory impairment than textual or numeric information. In line with the previous literature (Heath & Erickson, 1998; Paz-Alonso & Goodman, 2008) peripheral classes (not focused in the video, e.g., drink or key pendant) appeared to score higher than the central classes (e.g., box). In field applications of the CIT, it also is assumed that memory of places of stolen items (e.g., box or envelope) are typically remembered better than peripheral details (e.g., time) (Nakayama, 2002; Osugi, 2011). PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 3.2 16 Influence of the cued recall on the misinformation effect In misled categories with cued recall, the original and misleading items were not significantly better attributed to the correct source compared to the misled categories without cued recall, z = -.726, p = .468, d = 0.16. There is no difference between categories with or without cued recall with respect to the occurrence of source-confusing errors (original items attributed to the text or misleading items attributed to the video), z = -.061, p = .952, d = 0.01. This outcome indicates that there is no influence of the cued recall task on the misinformation effect. Evidence from former attempts made to decrease misinformation effects is equivocal. Priming of original information, but not of neutral or misleading information alters misinformation effects (Gordon & Shapiro, 2012). In some studies, warnings before the recollection phase reduced misinformation effects (Bodner et al., 2009; Eakin et al., 2003; Echterhoff, Hirst, & Hussy, 2005), in others they did not (Meade & Roediger, 2002; for a review see Blank & Launay, 2014). Echterhoff and his colleagues (2005) concluded that there were cognitive processes that could be influenced prior to the recollection to decrease false memories. The cued recall task may not have influenced the cognitive processes in a way that neither improved nor impaired memory recollection. The instructions and visual cues used (i.e., screenshots of the video) were apparently not sufficient to support the strengthening of the original source memory. Rather, it may have acted as a first recollection task. On summarizing, there is no reliable method yet, to reduce misinformation effects prior to the recollection phase. In the current state of research, the mechanisms leading to false memory are still unclear. No cognitive processes are known that may explain the majority of the mixed study results. The results of our study, therefore, numbers among in the mixed state of research concerning the mechanisms and decrease of the misinformation effect. 3.3 Autonomic correlates of the misinformation effect Table 2 provides an overview of the raw physiological data separated by item type. Figure 2 shows the temporal course of the phasic heart rate separately for each item type. In all following analyses, within-subject z-standardized physiological data were used (Lykken & Venables, 1971; Ben-Shakhar, 1985; Gamer et al., 2006). A significant effect of item type was found for EDA, F (1.76, 66) = 80.89, p < .001, = PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 17 0.55, but it was not found for pHR, F (1.81, 70) = 0.98, p = .371, = 0.01, or for RLL, F (1.63, 70) = 0.02, p = .976, = 0.00. The original items were accompanied by greater EDA responses when compared with the control items, t (65) = 12.06, p < .001, d = 2.63, and when compared with the misleading items, t (65) = 8.46, p < .001, d = 1.69. The misleading items were associated with greater EDA responses in contrast to the control items, t (65) = 2.61, p = .011, d = 0.50. Figure 2. Temporal course of the phasic heart rate separately for each item type. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 18 Table 2. EDA, pHR, and RLL separated for each item type Item type Original M EDA [nS] pHR [BPM] SEM Misleading M SEM Control M SEM 229.21 29.78 137.67 23.94 115.64 19.50 0.49 0.23 0.79 0.21 0.63 0.18 RLL [arb. units] 371.99 15.78 386.75 19.54 380.32 17.29 M = mean; SEM = standard error of the mean This indicates that the typically observed CIT effect (i.e., response differences between the known original and unknown control items) is replicated for EDA, but not for pHR or for RLL. These findings are in line with the former evidence from CIT studies: When subjects answer truthfully instead of deceptively, the RLL and pHR typically show much smaller effects, whereas, EDA shows equally large effects (Ambach et al., 2008; klein Selle et al., 2015). In those studies, it was argued that pHR and RLL do not reflect orienting, but rather inhibition or concealment-related processes. As a result, EDA can be regarded as the most sensible parameter in this study. As a new finding, EDA responses to the misleading items were greater than those to the control items, but smaller than those to the original items. In line with our hypothesis, the participants showed greater ORs to the original items than to the misleading ones. The original items must have gained special importance and meaning for the individual participant but not the misleading ones. A possible explanation is that the visual encoding of the original items might have been better than the textual encoding of the misleading items. It was found earlier that visual encoding exceeds textual encoding in depth, although textual encoded items were treated as familiar (Dodson & Markham, 1993). Furthermore, the answer given in the PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 19 CIT is an important confound of this finding: More original than misleading items were affirmed. Consequently, a direct comparison of the affirmed original items and affirmed misleading items was conducted. For the following analysis, false memories in the CIT refer to trials in which a misleading item was affirmed, whereas, correct memories refer to affirmations of original items. We selected participants who affirmed at least one misleading item and had valid physiological data; this reduced the sample size to 26 (EDA) and 31 (pHR, RLL), respectively. False memories compared to correct memories were not accompanied by smaller EDA, t (25) = 0.54, p = .298, d = 0.16 (see Figure 3), or by greater pHR, t (30) = 0.52, p = .305, d = 0.14, or by greater RLL responses, t (30) = 0.46, p = .328, d = 0.14. A sensitivity analysis was conducted using G*Power (version 3.1.9.2). For a two-tailed t-test for paired samples, assuming a power of 95 %, an alpha level of 5 %, and a sample size of 26, only effects of d = 0.74 or greater could be found, with a probability of 95 %. Hence, more participants showing false memories and more false memories per participant would have yielded more meaningful results. Figure 3. Z-transformed EDA of false (affirmed misleading items) and true memories (affirmed original items) in the CIT. (Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.) PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 20 The lack of differences in the physiological correlates between false and correct memories is contrary to the findings of Baioui and his colleagues (2012) who found greater EDA responses associated with true recognition than with false recognition in a Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. It was concluded that a physiological differentiation of falsely recognized and truly recognized items is possible. If so, the EDA would reflect the objective knowledge rather than the subjective belief of having it. Our findings, however, indicate that no differentiation between false and correct memories is possible by EDA in the misinformation paradigm. It has to be considered that false memories in the DRM paradigm are different from false memories induced by misinformation: False memories in the misinformation studies refer to previously encoded items for which a memory trace was created. In contrast, false memories in DRM studies occur without such encoding and memory trace. Assuming that EDA reflects not only the subjective feeling of recognition but also the objective existence of a memory trace would help to resolve the apparent contradiction between the findings of both studies. In misinformation studies, it is debated whether source confusion is involved in the formation of false memories. Hence, differences in physiological data between different source identification types were of special interest. In the source identification task, answers to original and misleading items were assigned to three categories: Correct (original item attributed to video, misleading item to text), incorrect (original or misleading item attributed to CIT), and confusing source identifications (original items attributed to text; misleading items to video). The physiological data gathered in the CIT then was assigned to the different types of source identification and compared. A significant effect of source identification type was found for EDA, F (1.19, 25) = 7.84, p = .007, = 0.25 (see Figure 4), but not for pHR, F (1.34, 28) = 1.30, p = .275, for RLL, F (1.70, 29) = 1.10, p = .334, = 0.05, or = 0.04. Compared with incorrect source identifications, greater EDA responses were found for correct source identifications, t (62) = 10.57, p < .001, d = 1.97, as well as for confusing source identifications, t (22) = -2.41, p = .025, d = 0.77. Confusing source identifications did not differ from correct source identifications, t (23) = 0.37, p = .718, d = 0.11. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 21 Figure 4. Z-transformed EDA of correct, incorrect, and confusing source identifications. (Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.) In summary, greater EDA responses were observed when items, be they original or misleading, were judged as presented in the video or text, compared to when items were reported as first seen in the CIT. If, however, original or misleading items were attributed as first seen in the CIT, they were not recognized as being significant. Although these items supposedly had gained special meaning during encoding, the orienting response, which was reflected in the EDA responses, significantly decreased with this misattribution. This indicates that EDA reflects subjective identification of items as important rather than their objective importance. 3.4 Limitations One main limitation restricts the amount of conclusions drawn by this study: For analyzing physiological correlates of false memories, the number of falsely affirmed misleading details and the number of source confusions between original and misleading details was relatively small, resulting in a limited test power. In further studies, more false memories should be induced so that differences in the PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 22 accompanying autonomic measures can be tested with more statistical power. First, this may be accomplished by optimizing the retention interval. Extending the duration of the retention interval to one or two weeks might be preferable over distractor tasks, which were commonly used and also employed in this study. With increasing retention intervals, higher false memory rates were observed (Pansky, Tenenboim, & Bar, 2011; Wilford et al., 2014). Additionally, the number of original details presented in the video should be increased, as meta-analytic studies regarding the CIT showed that physiological effects increase in power with increasing the number of stimulus categories (Ben-Shakhar & Elaad, 2003). Furthermore, the details used as original information should be presented in the video as peripheral instead of central details (Heath & Erickson, 1998; Paz-Alonso & Goodman, 2008). In this context, it remains unanswered whether textual or numeric details might be superior to pictorial information. A replication study is needed to face the main limitation of low statistical power. Considering the above suggestions might particularly allow differentiating the EDA correlates of subjective versus objective components of recognition. Another limitation of the study is the usage of more control than memorized items. Similar to other studies using the CIT, memorized items (i.e., original or misleading items) constitute a relative oddball among the more frequent control items. Differences in physiological responding between control items and memorized items can be due to any combination of these effects. Yet, this possible confound is limited to comparisons including control items. 3.5 Conclusion The present study replicated the misinformation effect with newly developed study materials. Three different recollection tasks showed similar misinformation effects. A randomization and balancing scheme methodologically improved the standard misinformation procedure. With regard to the expected decrease of misinformation effects by a cued recall task, the procedure employed in this study did not facilitate the correct source identification to a sufficient degree. It remains questionable whether cued recall is principally able to reduce misinformation effects or not. The typical CIT effect was replicated for EDA but not for pHR or RLL, which is in line with the current understanding of sub-processes ongoing in the CIT: EDA is discussed as reflecting orienting, whereas, pHR and RLL are related to concealment and inhibitory processes. As the source identification task revealed, EDA response PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 23 relates to the subjective importance a participant attributes to an item rather than its actual importance. In summing up, combining the misinformation paradigm with the CIT proved suitable to examine the psychophysiology of false memories. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 24 References Ambach, W., Stark, R., Peper, M., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Separating deceptive and orienting components in a Concealed Information Test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70 (2), 95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.002 Ambach, W., Dummel, S., Lüer, T., & Vaitl, D. (2011). Physiological responses in a Concealed Information Test are determinded interactively by encoding. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81 (3), 275-282. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.07.010 Baioui, A., Ambach, W., Walter, B., & Vaitl, D. (2012). Psychophysiology of false memories in a Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm with visual scenes. PloS one, 7 (1). Online Publication. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030416 Barry, R. J. (1996). Preliminary process theory: Towards an integrated account of the psychophysiology of cognitive processes. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 56 (1), 469–484. Bekerian, D. A., & Bowers, J. M. (1983). Eyewitness testimony: Were we misled? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 9 (1), 139-145. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.9.1.139 Belli, R. F. (1988). Color blend retrievals: Compromise memories or deliberate compromise responses? Memory & Cognition, 16 (4), 314–326 Belli, R. F., Lindsay, D. S., Gales, M. S., & McCarthy, T. T. (1994). Memory impairment and source misattribution in postevent misinformation experiments with short retention intervals. Memory & Cognition, 22 (1), 40–54. doi: 10.3758/BF03197042 Ben-Shakhar, G. (1985). Standardization within individuals: A simple method to neutralize individual differences in skin conductance. Psychophysiology, 22 (3), 292-299. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01603.x Ben-Shakhar, G., & Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (1), 131-151. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.131 Blank, H., & Launay, C. (2014). How to protect eyewitness memory against the misinformation effect. A meta-analysis of post-warning studies. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3 (2), 77–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.03.005 Bodner, G. E., Musch, E., & Azad, T. (2009). Reevaluating the potency of the memory conformity effect. Memory & Cognition, 37 (8), 1069-1076. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.8.1069 PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 25 Bradley, M. T., & Janisse, M. P. (1981). Accuracy demonstrations, threat, and the detection of deception: Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and pupillary measures. Psychophysiology, 18 (3), 307-315. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03040.x Chambers, K. L., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2001). Intended and unintended effects of explicit warnings on eyewitness suggestibility: Evidence from source identification tests. Memory & Cognition, 29 (8), 1120-1129. doi: 10.3758/BF03206381 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Coupe, T., & Griffiths, M. (1996). Solving residential burglary (Crime Detection and Preventions Series No 77). London: Home Office. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58 (1), 17-22. doi: 10.1037/h0046671 Dodd, D. H., & Bradshaw, J. M. (1980). Leading Questions and Memory: Pragmatic Constraints. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19 (6), 695-704. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90379-5 Dodson, M., & Markham, R. (1993). Imagery ability and source monitoring: Implications for eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology, 84 (1), 111-118. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1993.tb02466.x Dodson, C. S., Powers, E., & Lytell, M. (2015). Aging, confidence, and Misinformation: recalling information with the Cognitive Interview. Psychology and Aging, 30 (1), 46-61. doi: 10.1037/a0038492 Eakin, D. K., Schreiber, T. A., & Sergent-Marshall, S. (2003). Misinformation effects in eyewitness memory: The presence and absence of memory impairment as a function of warning and misinformation accessibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29 (5), 813–825. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.813 Echterhoff, G., Hirst, W., & Hussy, W. (2005). How eyewitnesses resist misinformation: Social postwarnings and the monitoring of memory characteristics. Memory & Cognition, 33 (5), 770-782. doi: 10.3758/BF03193073 Elaad, E., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2008). Covert respiration measures for the detection of concealed information. Biological Psychology, 77 (3), 284-291. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.001 Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT sciences. Behavior 10.3758/BF03193146 Research Methods, 29 (2), 26 175-191. doi: Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: SAGE. Fisher, R. P., & Geiselman, R. E. (1992). Memory-enhancing techniques for investigative interviewing: The Cognitive Interview. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas. Fisher, R. P., Geiselman, R. E., & Amador, M. (1989). Field test of the Cognitive Interview: Enhancing the recollection of actual victims and witnesses of crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74 (5), 722-727. doi: 10.1037/00219010.74.5.722 Furedy, J.J., Posner, R. T., & Vincent, A. (1991). Electrodermal differentiation of deception: Perceived accuracy and perceived memorial content. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 11 (1), 91-97. doi: 10.1016/01678760(91)90376-9 Gabbert, F., Hope, L., & Fisher, R. P. (2008). Protecting eyewitness evidence: Examining the efficacy of a Self-Administered Interview tool. Law and Human Behavior, 33 (4), 298-307. doi: 10.1007/s10979-008-9146-8 Gamer, M., Rill, H., Vossel, G., & Gödert, H. W. (2006). Psychophysiological and vocal measures in the detection of guilty knowledge. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60 (1), 76-87. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.006 Gordon, L. T., & Shapiro, A. M. (2012). Priming correct information reduces the misinformation effect. Memory & Cognition, 40 (5), 717-726. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0191-7 Greene, E., Flynn, M. S., & Loftus, E. F. (1982). Inducing resistence to misleading information. Journal of verbal Learning and verbal Behavior, 21 (2), 207–219. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.114.1.1 Heath, W. P., & Erickson, J. R. (1998). Memory for central and peripheral actions and props after varied post-event presentation. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3 (2), 321-346. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8333.1998.tb00369.x Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6 (2), 65-70. Hope, L., Gabbert, F., Fisher, R. P., & Jamieson, K. (2014). Protecting and enhancing eyewitness memory: The impact of an initial recall attempt on performance in an investigative interview. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28 (3), 304-313. doi: 10.1002/acp.2984 Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source Monitoring. Psychological bulletin, 114 (1), 3–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3 PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 27 Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., & Ankudowich, E. (2012). The Cognitive neuroscience of true and false memories. In Belli, R. F. (Ed.) Nebraska symposium on motivation (p. 15-52). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-1195-6_2 Kebbell, M. R., & Milne, R. (1998). Police officers‘ perceptions of eyewitness performance in forensic investigations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 138 (3), 323-330. doi: 10.1080/00224549809600384 Kircher, J. C., & Raskin, D. C. (2003). The Computerized Polygraph System II (Software Version 4.01). Salt Lake City, UT: Scientific Assessment Technologies. klein Selle, N., Verschuere, B., Kindt, M., Meijer, E. H., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2015). Orienting versus inhibition in the Concealed Information Test: Different cognitive processes drive different physiological measures. Psychophysiology, 53 (4), 579-590. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12583 Köhnken, G., Thürer, C., & Zoberbier, D. (1994). The Cognitive Interview: Are the interviewers‘ memories enhanced, too? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8 (1), 13-24. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350080103 Köhnken, G., Milne, R., Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1999). The Cognitive Interview: A meta-analysis. Psychology, Crime and Law, 5 (1-2), 3-27. doi: 10.1080/10683169908414991 Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition, 17 (3), 349–358. doi: 10.3758/BF03198473 Lindsay, D. S. (2008). Source Monitoring. In H. L. Roediger, III (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory. Vol. 2 of Learning and Memory: A comprehensive reference (J. Byrne, Ed.) (p. 325-347). Retrieved from: http://web.uvic.ca/~dslind/?q=node/128 Loftus, E. F. (1977). Shifting human color memory. Memory & Cognition, 5 (6), 696– 699. doi: 10.3758/BF03197418 Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness Testimony. Cambridge: University Press. Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12 (4), 361– 366. doi: 10.1101/lm.94705 Loftus, E. F., & Hoffman, H. G. (1989). Misinformation and memory: The creation of new memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118 (1), 100104. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.100 PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 28 Loftus, E. F., & Loftus, G. R. (1980). On the permanence of stored information in the human brain. American Psychologist, 35 (5), 409–420. doi: 10.1037/0003066X.35.5.409 Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4 (1), 19-31. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19 Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43 (6), 385-388. doi: 10.1037/h0046060 Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: A proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 8 (5), 656-672. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1971.tb00501.x McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114 (1), 1–16. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.1.36 Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L. (2002). Explorations in the social contagion of memory. Memory & Cognition, 30 (7), 995-1009. doi: 10.3758/BF03194318 Meijer, E. H., klein Selle, N., Elber, L., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2014). Memory detection with the Concealed Information Test: A meta-analysis of skin conductance, heart rate, and P300 data. Psychophysiology, 51 (9), 879-904. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12239 Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1991). The Cognitive Interview: Its origins, empirical support, evaluation and practical implications. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 1 (4), 291-307. doi: 10.1002/casp.2450010405 Memon, A., Meissner, C. A., & Fraser, J. (2010). The Cognitive Interview: A metaanalytic review and study space analysis of the past 25 years. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 16 (4), 340-372. doi: 10.1037/a0020518 Nakayama, M. (2002). Practical use of the Concealed Information Test for criminal investigation in Japan. In Kleiner, M. (ed.), Handbook of polygraph testing (p. 49-86). San Diego: Academic Press. Okado, Y., & Stark, C. E. L. (2005). Neural activity during encoding predicts false memories created by misinformation. Learning & Memory, 12 (1), 3-11. doi: 10.1101/lm.87605 Osugi, A. (2011). Daily application of the Concealed Information Test: Japan. In Verschuere, B., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Meijer, E. (eds.), Memory Detection: Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test (p. 253-275). Cambridge: University Press. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 29 Pansky, A., Tenenboim, E., & Bar, S. K. (2011). The misinformation effect revisited: Interactions between spontaneous memory processes and misleading suggestions. Journal of Memory and Language, 64 (3), 270-287. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.12.003 Paulo, R. M., Albuquerque, P. B., & Bull, R. (2013). The enhanced Cognitive Interview. Towards a better use and understanding of this procedure. International Journal of Police Science & Management, 15 (3), 190-199. doi: 10.1350/ijps.2013.15.3.311 Paz-Alonso, P. M., & Goodman, G. S. (2008). Trauma and memory: Effects of postevent misinformation, retrieval order, and retention interval. Memory, 16 (1), 58-75. doi: 10.1080/09658210701363146 Roediger, H. L. & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating False Memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21 (4), 803-814. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00004 Rosenthal, R. (1994). Parametric measures of effect size. In Cooper, H. & Hedges, L.V. (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (S. 232-244). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Schacter, D. L., & Slotnick, S. D. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion. Neuron, 44 (1), 149-160. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.017 Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Higher nervous functions: The Orienting Reflex. Annual Review of Psychology, 25, 545–580. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.25.030163.002553 Takarangi, M. K. T., Parker, S., & Garry, M. (2006). Modernising the misinformation effect: Development of a new stimulus set. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20 (5), 583-590. doi: 10.1002/acp.1209 Timm, H. W. (1982). Effect of altered outcome expectancies stemming from placebo and feedback treatments on the validity of the guilty knowledge technique. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67 (4), 391-400. doi: 10.1037/00219010.67.4.391 Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley. Tversky, B., & Tuchin, M. (1989). A reconciliation of the evidence on eyewitness testimony: Comments on McCloskey & Zaragoza. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118 (1), 86-91. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.86 Velden, M., & Wölk, C. (1987). Depicting cardiac activity over real time: A proposal for standardization. Journal of Psychophysiology, 1 (2), 173-175. Verschuere, B., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Meijer, E. (2011). Memory detection. Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test. Cambridge: University Press. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF THE MISINFORMATION EFFECT 30 Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., de Clercq, A., & Koster, E. H. W. (2004). Autonomic and behavioral responding to concealed information: Differentiating orienting and defensive responses. Psychophysiology, 41 (3), 461-466. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.00167.x Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Koster, E. H. W., Van Bockstaele, B., & de Cleruq, A. (2007). Startling secrets: Startle eye blink modulation by concealed crime information. Biological Psychology, 76 (1-2), 52-60. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.06.001 Wilford, M. M., Chan, J. C. K., & Tuhn, S. J. (2014). Retrieval enhances eyewitness suggestibility to misinformation in free and cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20 (1), 81–93. doi: 10.1037/xap0000001 Wright, D. B. (1993). Misinformation and warnings in eyewitness testimony: A new testing procedure to differentiate explanations. Memory, 1 (2), 153-166. doi: 10.1080/09658219308258229