* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Women and Men in Conflicting Social Roles: Implications from
Sex differences in psychology wikipedia , lookup
Muted group theory wikipedia , lookup
Gender roles in childhood wikipedia , lookup
Gender role wikipedia , lookup
Gender and development wikipedia , lookup
Special measures for gender equality in the United Nations wikipedia , lookup
Third gender wikipedia , lookup
Sex differences in humans wikipedia , lookup
Gender and security sector reform wikipedia , lookup
Feminism in the United States wikipedia , lookup
Media and gender wikipedia , lookup
Feminism (international relations) wikipedia , lookup
New feminism wikipedia , lookup
Michael Messner wikipedia , lookup
Gender systems wikipedia , lookup
Anarcha-feminism wikipedia , lookup
Gender apartheid wikipedia , lookup
Gender inequality wikipedia , lookup
Gender roles in Islam wikipedia , lookup
Judith Lorber wikipedia , lookup
Gender roles in non-heterosexual communities wikipedia , lookup
Social Issues and Policy Review, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011, pp. 191--226 Women and Men in Conflicting Social Roles: Implications from Social Psychological Research Kimberley A. Clow∗ University of Ontario Institute of Technology Rosemary Ricciardelli York University Despite legislation for gender equality in many nations, gender discrimination continues to be a problem. Psychological research from social role theory, the stereotype content model, and ambivalent sexism provide insights into the motivations behind gender inequality. This article reviews key research findings from these theoretical perspectives in the realm of gendered occupational inequalities and segregation. The emphasis of the article is on individuals fulfilling social roles that are perceived as conflicting and the consequences of those perceptions. Parents in the workforce, female leaders, and male nurses are used as specific examples of social role conflict. The policy implications from this research—and the issues facing parents in the workforce, female leaders, and male nurses in particular—are discussed. Women and men who attempt to enter fields typically dominated by the other gender frequently encounter stereotypes, prejudice, or discrimination (Eagly & Koenig, 2008; Evans & Frank, 2003; Heilman, Wallen, Fuchs, & Tamkins, 2004; Rudyk, 2010). A consistent theme underlying these negative reactions appears to be perceived conflict regarding conceptions of masculinity and femininity. Agentic attributes (e.g., assertive, ambitious, dominant, independent, self-confident, and competitive) focus on self-attainment and are central to perceptions of masculinity, whereas communal attributes (e.g., affectionate, helpful, kind, nurturing, sympathetic, and sensitive) are other focused and central to perceptions of femininity (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt; 2001; Kasen, Chen, Sneed, Crawford, & ∗ Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kimberley A. Clow, Faculty of Social Science & Humanities, University of Ontario Institute of Technology, 55 Bond St. East, Oshawa, ON, L1G 0A5 [e-mail: [email protected]]. 191 C 2011 The Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 192 Clow and Ricciardelli Cohen, 2006). Occupations that are numerically dominated by one gender tend to be stereotyped as masculine (if dominated by men) or as feminine (if dominated by women), possibly deriving from a need to rationalize the existing distribution of men and women into these occupations (Cejka & Eagly, 1999; Hoffman & Hurst, 1990). Thus, when a woman enters a male-dominated field or a man enters a female-dominated field, the assumed masculinity or femininity of the job and the person is perceived to conflict. This perceived incongruity can result in stereotypes that masculinize women or feminize men in these occupations or to perceptions that an individual’s behavior is not consistent—and often viewed as inappropriate—for his or her gender (Harding, 2007; Rudyk, 2010). Consider the case of Ann Hopkins. In 1982, after working at Price Waterhouse (one of the United States’ largest nationwide professional accounting firms) for five years, partners in her local office submitted her name as a candidate for partnership. Her supporters highlighted her successful acquisition of a $25 million contract with the Department of State and her record of securing major contracts that surpassed those of any other partnership candidates that year. Hopkins herself, as well as her accomplishments, was praised by the partners in her office. Her clients also spoke very positively about her, describing her as “extremely competent, intelligent,” and “strong and forthright, very productive, energetic and creative” (Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins, 1989, Tr. 150). Thirty-two partners chose to submit comments on Ann Hopkins regarding her bid for partnership. Of these 32 statements, 13 supported Hopkins, eight stated that they did not have sufficient information to comment, three recommended that her candidacy be placed on hold to be considered the following year, and eight recommended that she be denied partnership. One of the partners providing input on her candidacy mentioned, multiple times, that he did not feel that a woman could be a competent senior manager, let alone a partner. Another partner complained about her use of profanity “because it’s a lady using foul language” (Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins, 1989, Tr. 321). Despite the fact that the majority of statements supported Ann Hopkins, that Hopkins had a better record of securing contracts than the other candidates, and that Price Waterhouse did not have a limit on the number of individuals who could earn partnership each year, the Policy Board chose to put Ann Hopkins’ candidacy on hold. She was told that her chances for partner would improve if she made efforts to “walk more femininely, talk more femininely, dress more femininely, wear make-up, have her hair styled, and wear jewellery” (Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins, 1989, 618 F. Supp. at 1117). She was not considered for partner the following year and sued Price Waterhouse for gender discrimination. Although the Hopkins case is a blatant example, more subtle forms of gender discrimination still persist (e.g., Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2004; Eagly & Karau, 2002; Rudyk, 2010). Existing policies that attempt to prevent gender discrimination, and outline possible legal recourse should discrimination occur nonetheless, may not be eliminating the issues as intended. First of all, these policies almost Women and Men in Conflicting 193 exclusively focus on women as the targets of gender discrimination and do not consider the issues that men face in occupations traditionally numerically dominated by women. Second, although progress has been made in addressing gender discrimination directed at women in traditionally male-dominated occupations, gender discrimination still persists in these fields. As Hazel L. Sive, Associate Dean of the School of Science at M.I.T. claimed, “because things are so much better now, we can see an entirely new set of issues” (Zernike, 2011, p. 1). Policies Lack a Focus on Gender Discrimination Targeting Men Consider the following examples of gender discrimination targeting men. In the case of Blair v. Colonial Plaza (2009), a nursing home instituted a new policy barring male caregivers from working nightshifts. This new policy was established because the nursing home felt its patients would be most susceptible to sexual assault during these shifts. Blair, who had previously worked nightshifts for this nursing home as he worked a second job during the day, was suddenly denied the shifts he needed and had previously worked—not because of any behavior on his part, but because he was a man and men were no longer allowed to work those shifts. The United States District Court of Oklahoma ruled that Blair “has a legitimate right to sue for being cast in the role of a potential sexual predator simply on the basis of his male gender” (Legal Eagle Eye Newsletter for the Nursing Profession, 2008, p. 8). In England, Andrew Moyhing won a sex discrimination case against National Health Service (NHS) hospitals, where he was prohibited from completing personal procedures on female patients (e.g., cervical smears, electrocardiogram tests on breasts) without a female chaperone present (Carvel, 2006). Moyhing, as a male nurse, was required to have a chaperone in these intimate health care situations with female patients, whereas male doctors were not. In addition, women (nurses or doctors) were not required to have chaperones when conducting sensitive tests on male patients. Furthermore, this stipulation was not at the request of patients—a situation that did not bother Moyhing—but as a standard requirement for all (and only) male nurses. Unfortunately, these are not isolated cases. Men who enter nursing, a femaledominated profession, are generally stereotyped as sexually deviant, homosexual, feminine or “weird,” as if it were impossible to consider that a “normal” man would engage in what is currently considered “women’s work” (Evans & Frank, 2003; Harding, 2007). They are often disqualified from labor and delivery wards, gynecology procedures, and sometimes even access to their patients (Bartfay & Bartfay, 2007; Johnson, 2004). Men in female-dominated professions report that their sexuality and their ability to fulfill their jobs are often questioned (Harding, 2007; Lupton, 2006; Yang, Gau, Shiau, Hu, & Shih, 2004), whereas women in these fields (and men in comparable occupations that are more compatible with traditional gender roles) do not experience these reactions. There are few 194 Clow and Ricciardelli role models and mentors for men in these areas, men are not actively recruited (whereas the perception is that women are actively recruited), and people often react to their gender rather than their qualifications and abilities (Lupton, 2006; O’Lynn, 2004; Sherrod, Sherrod, & Rasch, 2006). Rather than valuing men as an important part of these professions, rules and regulations seem to work toward excluding men rather than including them. These sanctions do not occur simply because of gender—as male doctors, for example, are exempt from these concerns. These sanctions arise as a reaction to men in occupations that are not congruent with traditional masculine gender roles. Men in these fields are calling for changes that specifically work toward reducing gender discrimination for men in traditionally female occupations, similar to policies already in place for women in traditionally male occupations that have succeeded in increasing the number of qualified women in those fields (Meadus, 2000; Roth & Coleman, 2008; Sherrod et al., 2006). Existing Policies Targeting Women May Not Be Sufficient Existing policies, however, may not be sufficient. Research suggests that biases against women still exist (Cuddy et al., 2004; Eagly & Karau, 2002; Rudyk, 2010). To illustrate, consider M.I.T., an institution that acknowledged past gender discrimination targeting women in traditionally male-dominated fields (e.g., science, engineering) and attempted to proactively address the issue. Despite these efforts, gender equality has not been achieved (Zernike, 2011). For example, female professors at M.I.T. report that they feel their successes are interpreted by others as occurring because they are women (in light of policies that are attempting to reduce gender bias), rather than because they are competent. In addition, female faculty feel that current gender policies—although increasing the number of qualified women at M.I.T.—have unexpectedly placed a greater burden on them. For instance, at least one woman is required to sit on every university committee, but when there are fewer female faculty than male faculty at an institution or in a department, this rule leads to the average female professor sitting on more committees than her average male colleague—which is time taken away from her research program and her teaching. In addition, despite many improvements, a gender wage gap still exists. For example, in Canada women earn $0.83 for every dollar men earn (Statistics Canada, 2010). In the United States, female lawyers earn 68% of the salary earned by male lawyers (Hersch, 2003), whereas men in female-dominated professions, such as nursing, elementary school teaching, and library studies, are overrepresented in the higher paying and higher status positions within their fields (Evans, 1997; Williams, 1992). Participants even assign significantly higher salaries to the same occupations when they are described more masculine (e.g., industrial arts teacher, automotive magazine editor) than feminine (e.g., home economics Women and Men in Conflicting 195 teacher, gourmet food magazine editor) (Alksnis, Desmarais, & Curtis, 2008). Thus, it is important to have policies addressing gender discrimination, but it may be time to revisit these policies and ensure they are working as intended. Examples of Gender Discrimination Policies and Legislation Different countries have broached gender discrimination in slightly different ways. The United States Supreme Court ruled in 1986 that an employer could use gender as a factor to determine employment if it was used to correct for an existing gender imbalance (American Association for Affirmative Action, 2010a). Current American affirmative action policies are not based on quotas or preferential treatment, but instead focus on increasing the inclusion of groups that, historically, have been excluded from employment (American Association for Affirmative Action, 2010b). In Australia, the Equal Opportunity for Women in the Workplace Act of 1999 was developed. The Equal Opportunity for Women in the Workplace Agency administers the Act through education while working with employers to improve opportunities for women (Equal Opportunity for Women in the Workplace Agency, 2010). Similarly, in Canada, the Canadian Charter of Rights and Freedoms was established in 1982 (Human Resources and Skills Development Canada, 2010a). This provided all Canadians with the constitutional right to equality in employment. Later, in 1986, the Employment Equity Act was developed to ensure that federally regulated employers provided equal opportunities for employment to women, Aboriginal peoples, persons with disabilities, and members of visible minorities. The purpose of the Employment Equity Act was to ensure that individuals were not denied employment for reasons other than ability and to correct for existing disadvantages in the workplace that were experienced by the aforementioned groups (Canadian Human Rights Commission, 2010). However, the act did not include federal public service or procedures for enforcing employment equity. Thus, in 1991, the Canadian Human Rights Commission became responsible for ensuring compliance with the Employment Equity Act. It is important that such legislation exists. We believe policy makers should now question whether or not the current legislation is functioning as expected and whether or not further legislation (or modifications to existing legislation) might be necessary. In particular, we would like to draw attention to the obstacles men and women continue to face while working in occupations outside of their traditional gender roles. Applying Psychological Theory Three popular theories have emerged in the social psychological literature that have addressed issues of gender stereotypes and gender inequality in differing 196 Clow and Ricciardelli ways: social role theory, the stereotype content model, and ambivalent sexism. Although these theories were developed to address differing aspects of gender bias, we feel that these theories actually work together to contribute unique insights into the problem. Thus, this article will briefly review theory and research that lends insight into societal reactions to women and men fulfilling social roles that are perceived to conflict with traditional gender roles. We begin with social role theory because it explicitly addresses the importance of social roles and its impact on perceptions of gender roles. Theorizing derived from social role theory—such as role congruity theory of prejudice—specifically examines the consequences of individuals fulfilling social roles that are perceived to conflict with gender roles (e.g., women in leadership positions). Explanations of the stereotype content model and ambivalent sexism will follow. Although the stereotype content model and ambivalent sexism were not developed to specifically address perceptions of men and women in conflicting social roles, research findings derived from these theories are applicable. In addition, the contributions of these theories in the areas of gender stereotypes and individual differences in gender prejudice are particularly relevant. We feel that interpreting gender research findings through the lens of these three theories combined has the potential to advance our understanding of gender bias and societal reactions to men and women fulfilling incongruent social roles. The case of employees as parents, women in positions of leadership, and men in the nursing profession are used as specific examples to demonstrate how psychological theories and research can impact policy and practice. Three Theories Pertaining to Gender Inequality Despite their differences, there is convergence in the findings from social role theory, the stereotype content model, and ambivalent sexism. In this section, each theory will be briefly reviewed (see Figure 1), followed by a discussion of how the three theories might be integrated together and how they contribute to our understanding of societal reactions to women and men occupying social roles that are perceived to conflict. This section ends with an examination of gender research in three areas of perceived role conflict: employees as parents, women in positions of leadership, and men in nursing. Social Role Theory Social role theory takes a functionalist perspective to explain why societies come to develop gender stereotypes that generally bestow differing personalities on women and men that conveniently would assist them in completing the labor roles where men and women are typically employed (Eagly, 1987; Eagly, Wood, & Diekman, 2000). The main proposition of social role theory is that observed gender differences in behavior and personality actually arise from the gendered division Women and Men in Conflicting 197 Fig. 1. A visual summary of the main components of social role theory, the stereotype content model, and the model of ambivalent sexism. of labor in a society—especially family and occupational social roles (Eagly & Diekman, 2006; Eagly & Mitchell, 2004; Eagly et al., 2000). For example, labor is generally divided according to gender across cultures, suggesting a universality of dividing labor by gender, but there is variability across cultures and societies as to what specific activities are distributed to men and women (Wood & Eagly, 2002). Once labor is stratified by gender, the differential roles that men and women fulfill within a society encourage the development of gender roles—the normative expectations of what is considered appropriate masculine and feminine behavior (Eagly et al., 2000; Wood & Eagly, 2002). Gender roles, in turn, lead to the expectancy that individuals will occupy gender-specific roles and possess the characteristics that increase their success in those roles (Eagly et al., 2000; Wood & Eagly, 2002). Research does suggest that physical biological differences between women and men (primarily women’s ability to reproduce and breast feed and men’s greater size and strength)—combined with existing economic, technological, and ecological pressures—influence the tasks distributed to each gender (Eagly et al., 2000; Eagly & Mitchell, 2004; Wood & Eagly, 2002). For instance, sudden economic needs during World War II thrust a large number of women into the paid workforce for the first time. When World War II ended, labor roles were redistributed (for a discussion of other social consequences of women’s entry into the workforce, see Putnam, 2000). The labor roles that men and women currently fulfill in a society lead to the development of gender stereotypes. In other words, if “people observe women engaging in communal, nurturing behaviors such as child care . . . [they will] infer from these behaviors that women possess communal characteristics . . . Similarly, [if] people observe men engaging in assertive, task-oriented activities such as those involved in being a foreman or business executive . . . [they will] infer from these behaviours that men possess agentic characteristics” (Eagly & Mitchell, 2004, p. 190). Thus, gender stereotypes result from the different tasks that men and women engage in as they fulfill their differing roles, as opposed to deriving from biological driven personality differences between men and women (Bosak, Sczesny, & Eagly, 2008; Eagly et al., 2000). 198 Clow and Ricciardelli In addition to gender roles, the existing gendered division of labor forces most men and women into different experiences that reinforce different sets of skills and beliefs (Eagly & Mitchell, 2004; Eagly et al., 2000). These differential experiences could arise due to a variety of factors, such as motivations for engaging in role-congruent behavior, the gender stereotypic expectancies of others, or internalization of gender roles and self-stereotyping (Diekman & Eagly, 2008; Eagly et al., 2000). Thus, most individuals have greater opportunities to participate in activities that are deemed appropriate for their gender—as opposed to inappropriate for their gender. In other words, gender-segregated roles lead men and women to participate in different activities, providing men and women with differential training and expertise at different tasks. These differential experiences also affect beliefs about what is appropriate for men and women and the consequences of social behaviors. As children and youths are incorporated into these role expectations, they are acquiring the skills that society deems appropriate and useful for the majority of adults of their gender. Conforming to gender role expectations is presumed to assist individuals in becoming successful and productive members of society, as successful and productive members of communal (for women) or agentic (for men) social roles. Consider aggressive behavior as an example. As boys and young men are more likely than girls and young women to be encouraged to engage in activities that involve socially desirable displays of aggression, such as contact sports and military service, males end up engaging in more aggressive behavior than females and are more rewarded (or more tolerated) for doing so. Similarly, if more girls in comparison to boys are encouraged to (or employed to) babysit in their teenage years, then women would enter adulthood with more experience and knowledge related to the tending of children than do men, confirming stereotypic beliefs that women are better caregivers than men. These acquired skills and beliefs can be indirect manifestations of social roles. Research does suggest that particular personalities are associated with particular occupations (Clow & Esses, 2005; Holroyd, Bond, & Chan, 2002). More importantly, Hoffman and Hurst (1990) used fictional groups of individuals to experimentally demonstrate that stereotypes could develop solely from the distribution of groups of people into particular social roles—even when the personalities of those individuals did not actually differ. In addition, the tendency to stereotype based on the distribution of labor was greater when group differences were explained in biological terms or participants were asked to explain the unequal distribution of the groups in each role. Moreover, Cejka and Eagly (1999) found that participants rate feminine personalities as most important for success when considering female-dominated occupations (e.g., bank teller and dental hygienist) and male cognitive abilities as most important for success when considering male-dominated occupations (e.g., construction worker and civil engineer). Thus, seeing one gender primarily engaged in a particular social role seems to lead Women and Men in Conflicting 199 perceivers to assume that men or women generally have personality characteristics that make them especially suited to perform that given role. When people assume that there is something intrinsic to being a woman or a man that makes them inherently suited to perform a certain social role (e.g., women are naturally better caregivers, men are naturally better leaders), it implies that men and women in roles that are numerically dominated by the other gender are deviant, less qualified, and lacking the “natural” gift possessed by the normative gender. In addition, gender stereotypes suggest that men and women are born with personalities that would be counterproductive to fulfilling the social roles traditionally assumed by the opposite gender. Thus, men are seen as inadequate in fulfilling tasks associated with social roles traditionally fulfilled by women (e.g., men in communal professions such as nursing or day care) and women are seen as incapable of fulfilling tasks associated with the social roles traditionally fulfilled by men (e.g., women in agentic professions such as policing or politics). Moreover, gender stereotypes that arise may serve to rationalize and justify existing gender inequalities (Cejka & Eagly, 1999; Hoffman & Hurst, 1990; Jost & Banaji, 1994). Stereotype Content Model Whereas social role theory focuses on agency and communion and how the gendered division of labor gives rise to gender roles and the content of gender stereotypes, the stereotype content model (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002) proposed that the content of most social group stereotypes—including gender stereotypes—could be accounted for by perceptions of competence (similar to agency) and warmth (similar to communion), and that differing combinations of competence and warmth, based on the relative status and perceived interdependence of the social groups in question, led to the expression of qualitatively different forms of prejudice (for a review, see Clow & Ricciardelli, 2011). The differing combinations of competence (high; low) and warmth (high; low) correspond to four different forms of prejudice: paternalistic prejudice (low competence; high warmth), envious prejudice (high competence; low warmth), contemptuous prejudice (low competence; low warmth), and admiration (high competence; high warmth)—with this final form of prejudice being one of preference. Relative status and perceived interdependence lead to perceptions of competence and warmth, emotional reactions to groups, and behavioral intentions (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2007; Fiske et al., 2002). Groups that are perceived as low in status and are not competing for resources, power, or status with our own group memberships result in paternalistic prejudice (Caprariello, Cuddy, & Fiske, 2009). These groups are stereotyped as low in competence but high in warmth and people react to these groups with emotions of pity and sympathy. Research has found gender subtypes, such as housewives, secretaries, typical women, male softies, male senior citizens, male radicals, and male 200 Clow and Ricciardelli hippies, to be stereotyped according to paternalistic prejudices (Eckes, 2002; Fiske et al., 2002). Research in other nations (e.g., Germany, Japan, and South Korea) has found that women, more generally, fall into the paternalistic prejudice category (Asbrock, 2010; Cuddy et al., 2009). In Germany, gay men were clustered into the same stereotypes of high warmth and low competence as women and housewives (Asbrock, 2010), suggesting that gay men are perceived as nontraditional men who are low in status and not competing for resources. Thus, traditional women (e.g., housewives, secretaries) and nontraditional men (e.g., male softies, male hippies) tend to be stereotyped as high in warmth but low in competence. Cuddy et al. (2004) describe perceptions of these groups as the harmless and pathetic. Groups that are perceived as high in status and in competition with our own group memberships for resources, power, or status are subjected to envious prejudice (Caprariello et al., 2009). These groups are stereotyped as high in competence but low in warmth while eliciting feelings of envy and jealousy from perceivers. Research has found that some gender subtypes are stereotyped according to envious prejudice, such as feminists, business women, vamps, intellectual women, female hippies, career women, typical men, male managers, male yuppies, and career men (Eckes, 2002; Fiske et al., 2002). In particular, women in nontraditional roles and men in traditional roles seem to be the focus of this form of prejudice. Cuddy et al. (2004) describe perceptions of these groups as the “uppity outgroups” (p. 704), the social groups that are grudgingly respected (as they are high in status and competence) yet not liked (as they are competitive and low in warmth). Groups that are perceived as low in status and in competition for resources, power, or status are met with contemptuous prejudice (Caprariello et al., 2009). These groups are stereotyped as low in both competence and warmth (e.g., welfare recipients, the poor) and theorized to elicit feelings of contempt, disgust, anger, and resentment (Fiske et al., 2002). This form of prejudice is most similar to traditional, negative antipathy conceptualizations of prejudice (Allport, 1954; Dion, 2003) and does not adequately capture stereotypes or prejudice toward men and women (Eagly, 2004). Finally, groups that are perceived as high in status, but are not viewed as competing for resources, power, or status, are favored with admiration (Caprariello et al., 2009). These groups are stereotyped as high in both competence and warmth and elicit feelings of pride and admiration (Fiske et al., 2002). Admired social groups are often the in-groups (i.e., our own social groups) to which we belong, although they may also include mainstream social groups. Examples of admired social groups include full members of society in Japan, college graduates and married couples in Hong Kong, and middle class, White, Christians in the United States (Cuddy et al., 2004). Overall, paternalistic prejudice and envious prejudice seem most relevant to perceptions of men and women. Women in traditional gender roles (e.g., housewife, secretary) are targeted with paternalistic prejudice and women in roles that Women and Men in Conflicting 201 are incongruent with traditional gender roles (e.g., feminist, career woman) encounter envious prejudice. In contrast, men in traditional gender roles (e.g., career men, male managers) are targeted with envious prejudice, whereas men in roles that are incongruent with traditional gender roles (e.g., male hippies, male softies) seem to encounter paternalistic prejudice. Thus, whether or not a person fulfills a traditional or nontraditional role for one’s gender seems to determine whether that individual is perceived as high or low in status, in competition for resources and, consequently, whether or not the person is perceived as competent (but not liked) or liked (but not competent). Ambivalent Sexism Similar to social role theory, ambivalent sexism (Glick & Fiske, 1996, 1999) was developed to explain gendered attitudes. Whereas social role theory focused on the development of gender stereotypes, ambivalent sexism concerned itself with gender prejudice. It was argued that gender prejudice was a unique type of prejudice: one characterized by ambivalence (consisting of negative and positive components simultaneously) rather than uniform negativity (Glick & Fiske, 1996; Glick, Lameiras, & Castro, 2002; Glick et al., 2004; Viki, Abrams, & Hutchison, 2003). In the context of heterosexual relationships, one cannot afford to hate and isolate members of the opposite sex. In addition, the roles men and women occupy in many societies lead to frequent interaction and interdependence. Thus, instead of antipathy for the opposite sex, gender prejudices are a complicated balance of hostile and benevolent sexist ideologies. Hostile sexism refers to the negative aspect of gender attitudes involving domination, hostility, and resentment, whereas benevolent sexism is defined as the subjectively positive attitudes—from the perspective of the individual holding the attitude—of protection, benevolence, and idealization. Importantly, both hostile and benevolent sexism feed into existing power structures that serve to maintain the status quo, keeping men in a position of power and women subordinate to men (Glick & Fiske, 2001). Ambivalent sexism can target women (Ambivalent Sexism Inventory; ASI) or men (Ambivalence toward Men Inventory; AMI). Either form of sexism (targeting women or men) is derived from paternalism (relating to others as a father to his children), gender differentiation (gender stereotypes and gender roles), and heterosexual relationships (the interdependence of the genders for intimacy), and leads to the development of both hostile and benevolent sexist attitudes (Glick & Fiske, 1996; Glick et al., 2004). To give a brief example of ambivalent sexism targeting women, a heterosexual man is dependent upon women for love, affection, and intimacy. That dependency can lead to protective paternalism, where women are perceived as fragile or naı̈ve and in need of men’s protection (benevolent sexism), or to dominative paternalism, where feelings of superiority suggest a man is deserving of love, affection, and intimacy—whether a particular woman 202 Clow and Ricciardelli is willing to provide it or not (hostile sexism). Competitive gender differentiation can be used to justify men’s current and continued power, as men are perceived as possessing the characteristics (such as agentic traits) necessary to be leaders and to govern (hostile sexism), whereas complementary gender differentiation ascribes to women the idealized characteristics (such as communion traits) for motherhood and marriage (benevolent sexism), justifying their lack of power and feeding into protective and dominative paternalism. Desire for heterosexual intimacy may lead to similar idealizations of women (benevolent sexism), while resentment over this need for the “weaker” sex can lead to heterosexual hostility (hostile sexism). Ambivalent sexism targeting men is argued to stem from these same sources (paternalism, gender differentiation, and heterosexual relationships) (Glick & Fiske, 1999; Glick et al., 2004). For instance, women may resent men’s paternalism and sexual aggressiveness (hostile heterosexuality), yet they are bound to men through heterosexual attraction and daily living (heterosexual intimacy). In most societies, women hold a subordinate position to men, such that they are awarded less power and status than men. This subordination may lead women to respond with compensatory gender differentiation, where women distinguish themselves from men by self-stereotyping in a manner where women can be portrayed as superior (e.g., communal characteristics). Such compensatory gender differentiation may tie into maternalism, for which women show protective tendencies toward men, but with an air of superiority and condescension similar to paternalism, such as women needing to nurture men as they are incapable of caring for themselves. As women are drawn to men through heterosexual attraction, they may also experience complementary gender differentiation, where they respect and admire men’s higher status and power. Thus, resentment of paternalism, resentment of hostile heterosexuality, and compensatory gender differentiation constitute women’s hostile sexism toward men, whereas maternalism, heterosexual intimacy, and complementary gender differentiation create women’s benevolent sexism toward men. Hostile and benevolent sexism scores tend to be positively correlated, indicating that individuals who score higher in one form of sexism (hostile or benevolent) tend to score higher in the other form of sexism as well (Glick & Fiske, 2001). Research has established that hostile and benevolent sexist attitudes exist across cultures (e.g., Australia, Botswana, Chile, Cuba, Germany, Japan, Nigeria, Spain, South Korea, and the United States), and that a positive correlation between these sexist attitudes is consistent across cultures (Glick & Fiske, 2001; Glick et al., 2000, 2002, 2004). In addition, sexist attitudes toward men positively correlate with sexist attitudes toward women (Clow, Ricciardelli, & Bartfay, 2011; Glick et al., 2002, 2004). Despite these correlations, hostile and benevolent sexism do uniquely relate to particular attitudes and behaviors (for a review, see Clow & Ricciardelli, 2011). For example, hostile sexism toward women predicts attributing negative characteristics Women and Men in Conflicting 203 to women, whereas benevolent sexism toward women predicts attributing positive characteristics to women (Glick & Fiske, 1996; Glick et al., 2000). Hostile sexism toward women has been found to uniquely predict positive attitudes toward wife abuse in men and women from Turkey and Brazil (Glick, Sakalli-Ugurlu, Ferreira, & de Souza, 2002), whereas in both China and the United States, benevolent sexism generally predicted initial mate selection (especially among women), but hostile sexism generally predicted marriage norms (especially among men) (Chen, Fiske, & Lee, 2009). Together, hostile and benevolent sexism toward men and women predict gender inequality across nations (Glick et al., 2000, 2004). Research has found that benevolent sexism is related to positive evaluations of women in traditional roles (e.g., homemakers, chaste women), whereas hostile sexism is related to negative evaluations of women in nontraditional roles (e.g., career women, promiscuous women) (Sibley & Wilson, 2004; Takabayshi, 2007). Thus, benevolent sexism promotes women (and presumably men) fulfilling traditional gender roles while hostile sexism attempts to inhibit women (and presumably men) from engaging in social roles that are not traditionally in the domain of their gender. In this way, ambivalent sexism maintains current gender hierarchies and perpetuates gender inequality. Integrating the Theories These three theories are not in competition; rather they are complementary, each contributing different insight into the puzzle of gendered attitudes and stereotypes (see Figure 2). For example, although the stereotype content model was developed to explain stereotype content across differing social groups (e.g., social groups based on ethnicity, gender, nationalities, and disabilities), it has been used to explain differing gender subtype stereotypes and prejudices toward men and women in different roles (Cuddy et al., 2004; Eckes, 2002). In contrast, social role theory was developed specifically to explain the development of gender stereotypes, although it has been used to explain the development and use of other social categorizations and stereotypes as well (Clow & Esses, 2010; Eagly, Eastwick, & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2009). Both theories emphasize characteristics that have stereotypically been associated with men and women. Of the two theories, social role theory uses the more general variables of communion and agency, which incorporate the stereotype content model’s concepts of warmth and competence, but include aspects beyond these concepts as well. In trying to understand gender stereotyping and discrimination, social role theory uniquely contributes by focusing on the consequences of dividing a labor force according to gender and the influence of social roles on perceptions (see Figure 1). The stereotype content model adds to this understanding by focusing attention onto differing subtypes and subgroups of men and women, delineating the nature of differing forms of stereotypes and prejudices, as well as 204 Clow and Ricciardelli Fig. 2. A visual summary of how social role theory, the stereotype content model, and the model of ambivalent sexism work together to further our understanding of societal reactions to men and women. proposing factors that can lead to these differing stereotypes and prejudices (see Figure 1). Relating ambivalent sexism to the stereotype content model and social role theory, ambivalent sexism adds the individual difference component to gendered attitudes (see Figure 2) by seeking to explain why some people exhibit these stereotypes and prejudices more than others. There also appear to be certain parallels between ambivalent sexism and the other two theories. For example, gender differentiation seems to capture certain elements of social role theory, with its emphasis on gender roles and stereotypes, and the ambivalence of hostile and benevolent attitudes seems to parallel the ambivalence of respecting someone but not liking them (envious prejudice) or liking them but not respecting them (paternalistic prejudice) from the stereotype content model. Examples. Consider the following illustration of how these three theories can be used together to explain perceptions of women and men. Starting with social role theory, the influences of physical differences between men and women (e.g., reproduction, strength, and size), combined with the economic, technological, and ecological needs of the time, serve to distribute men and women into different labor roles (see Figure 1). Differing labor roles for men and women give rise to gender roles and stereotypes that suggest men and women are particularly suited to fulfill the labor roles that many men or women are currently engaged in. Existing gender roles and stereotypes enable gender differentiation Women and Men in Conflicting 205 to occur, both the competitive and complementary gender differentiation as discussed in ambivalent sexism (see Figure 1). Gender differentiation, and the gender roles and stereotypes themselves, lead to a variety of processes that essentially keep men and women in their existing labor roles—and possibly exacerbate and further divide conceptions of “men’s work” and “women’s work” (see Figure 2). Once a society feels there are different types of work, these tasks and labor roles can be evaluated differently and attributed more or less status (see Figure 1). As men’s work is generally held higher in status than women’s work, the stereotype content model’s perceived status and interdependence factors and ambivalent sexism’s paternalism seem particularly relevant. Women, who maintain their assigned labor roles generally involving communal tasks and activities (social role theory), are perceived as relatively low in status and nonthreatening to those higher in power (stereotype content model), resulting in communal stereotypes of warmth but not competence (stereotype content model), paternalistic prejudice (stereotype content model), and protective paternalism (ambivalent sexism). Men who maintain their assigned labor roles involving agentic tasks and activities (social role theory) are perceived as relatively high in status, which results in stereotypes of competence and agency (stereotype content model). If these men are perceived as a threat to those in power (e.g., competing for resources), they will be stereotyped as low in warmth (but high in competence) and targeted with envious prejudice (stereotype content model); if they are not perceived as a threat, they will be stereotyped as high in warmth (and high in competence) and admired (stereotype content model). As ambivalent sexism is an individual difference variable, some women may react to these men with resentment of paternalism, resentment of hostile heterosexuality, and compensatory gender differentiation, whereas other women may respond with maternalism, heterosexual intimacy, and complementary gender differentiation. Consider the situation, however, of a woman who does not want to engage in the low status and communal tasks that are generally allocated to women. For example, a woman who wants to be a lawyer or a partner in a competitive accounting firm (e.g., Ann Hopkins). This woman, trying to move out of the typical social role for her gender and into the labor tasks that are generally attributed to men (social role theory), could be perceived as a threat to existing power structures and possibly as competition for resources generally reserved for men (e.g., well-paying jobs), resulting in stereotypes of competence and agency but not liking, envious prejudice (stereotype content model), as well as hostile paternalism and compensatory gender differentiation (ambivalent sexism). In the Ann Hopkins case described at the beginning of the article, it was clear that some partners at the firm perceived her gender and the position of a firm partner as incompatible social roles (e.g., one partner commented that women were not competent enough to be senior managers or partners). Others did not 206 Clow and Ricciardelli like that she was displaying masculine gender role behavior (e.g., a lady using foul language) and wanted her to engage in more feminine gender role behavior instead (e.g., wear makeup). As swearing and wearing makeup are not requirements for the job of accountant, it is clear that gender stereotyping rather than performance issues were at the heart of the decision to not make Ann Hopkins a partner at Price Waterhouse (Fiske, Bersoff, Borgida, Deaux, & Heilman, 1991). Similarly, men who wish to engage in the labor tasks traditionally allocated to women (social role theory), such as men entering caregiver roles like nursing, can also be perceived as threatening to those in power—not because they compete for resources but because they challenge the legitimacy of existing power structures, gender roles and stereotypes, and gender hierarchies. These men are probably not liked by other men and—as they are attempting to enter communal occupations—not respected either, resulting in contemptuous prejudice (stereotype content model). In addition, other men may react toward these men as they respond to women in those same roles, with paternalism and gender differentiation (ambivalent sexism). In addition, society may feminize men in communal occupations or otherwise perceive them as different from “normal men” (i.e., as deviant in some way: sexual predators, homosexual, sissies) to justify their subordinate position with women. This would explain the issues experienced by the men in health care examples discussed earlier in the article (concerns about leaving male nurses alone with patients at night or allowing them to perform certain procedures unsupervised), where the men in these roles were perceived as a potential threat and danger—not due to any behavior they themselves exhibited, but merely from the fact that they were men in communal roles. Women may respond to these men with resentment, as they are entering the few fields that are dominated by women and reducing opportunities for maternalism and complementary gender differentiation (ambivalent sexism). Alternatively, they may welcome these men, seeing them as indicators of change and greater gender equality to come. Thus, as these examples have begun to demonstrate, these three theories can be synthesized to illuminate how men and women are often perceived when they occupy social roles that conflict with traditional gender roles (see Figure 2). Applying Theories to Individuals in Conflicting Social Roles Gender stereotypes that men are agentic and competent complement role stereotypes for the workforce (Eagly & Koenig, 2008; Eckes, 2002). Thus, people expect men to perform well in these social roles. Women, however, face more difficulty in the workplace—not because they are devalued, as women are often evaluated positively (and often more positively than men)—but because gender stereotypes of women as communal and warm conflicts with the Women and Men in Conflicting 207 role stereotypes of the business world (Eagly & Karau, 2002; Eagly & Koenig, 2008; Eckes, 2002). Similarly, men entering professions that society has stereotyped as caring or nurturing professions, such as nursing and teaching, encounter the same conflict between their gender role stereotype (masculine and agentic) and the stereotype of that occupational social role (feminine and communal). It is the conflicting roles and stereotypes, rather than negative feelings toward women or men per se, that lead to preferences for one gender over the other in these social roles (see role congruity theory of prejudice; Eagly & Koenig, 2008). Glick, Diebold, Bailey-Werner, and Zhu (1997) found that men higher in hostile sexism reported less favorable stereotypes toward career women than participants lower in hostile sexism, whereas men higher in benevolent sexism reported more favorable stereotypes of homemakers than men lower in benevolent sexism. Thus, one way ambivalent sexists may manage their conflicting beliefs about women is by targeting different types of women—women in different social roles—for their hostile and benevolent feelings. Homemakers fulfill a social role that is consistent with gender stereotypes, one that is stereotyped as high in communion and warmth but low in agency and competence, and targeted by paternalistic prejudice, whereas career women fulfill a social role that is less consistent with gender stereotypes, one that is stereotyped as high in agency and competence but low in communion and warmth, and targeted by envious prejudice (Eckes, 2002; Fiske et al., 2002). Thus, the match or mismatch of the gender role and social role is consistent with whether ambivalent sexists stereotype women in those roles more positively or more negatively. In addition, Sibley and Wilson (2004) reported similar findings among men reacting to a vignette of a promiscuous versus a chaste woman. These findings reveal how sexist individuals respond to women in roles that conflict with traditional gender stereotypes: with negative stereotypes and negative affect. There are a number of different conflicting social roles that men or women may find themselves encompassing. The remainder of this article will focus on three specific role conflicts: (1) employee versus married with children, (2) woman versus leader, and (3) man versus nurse. These three examples were selected for illustrative purposes because they are perceived role conflicts rather than actual conflicts (i.e., there is no ethical or legal conflict with individuals fulfilling both roles) and because research involving one or more of the psychological theories of interest has been done in the area. For employee versus married with children, we discuss the consequences for both male and female employees who simultaneously fulfill the social role of parent. We then chose to focus on a role conflict example for each gender separately (woman vs. leader; man vs. nurse). General social issues and policy implications that follow from these role conflicts, as well as specific concerns for each example, will be discussed in the “Policy Implications” section. 208 Clow and Ricciardelli Employee vs. Married with Children The social roles of marriage (being a husband or a wife) and parenting (having children) are perceived as significantly higher in communion for both employed men and employed women in comparison to the social roles of being single or childless (Eagly & Steffen, 1984). In addition, research has found that firsttime parents (in comparison to experienced parents) and women (in comparison to men) are more likely to exhibit greater gender role and gender stereotypic behavior following the birth of a child (Katz-Wise, Priess, & Hyde, 2010). Cuddy et al. (2004) found that the social role of parent was associated with greater warmth (communion) without impacting perceptions of competence (agency) when men and women were analyzed together. When men and women were analyzed separately, however, different findings emerged. Participants were asked to rate a male or female employee who was or was not expecting their first baby (target individuals who were expecting a child were indicated as married as well, whereas marriage and baby information was omitted for target individuals in the childless condition). Participants ascribed greater warmth to new parents regardless of gender, but new mothers were rated as more warm than competent, whereas new fathers were rated as equally warm and competent. Working women gained warmth but lost competence when they became mothers, whereas working men maintained their high competence and simply gained in warmth as well when they became fathers. In addition, participants were more willing to hire, promote, and train the female childless employee than the female employee expecting a child, but fatherhood did not significantly impact willingness to hire, promote, or train men. Competence ratings were also found to mediate willingness to hire, promote, and train employees, suggesting that working mothers are particularly at risk for workplace discrimination as motherhood itself lowers perceptions of competence— something which does not occur with the social role of parent for men. For men, the role of parent seems to shift perceptions from the envious prejudice (high competence, low warmth) that is generally ascribed to men to the admiration category that is usually reserved for select in-groups (high competence, high warmth). For women, the role of parent seems to shift perceptions from the envious prejudice of career women (high competence, low warmth) to the paternalistic prejudice for housewives (low competence, high warmth). These findings suggest that when men take on the social role of father, they are perceived as higher in communion than childless men but perceptions of their agency does not change, whereas when women take on the social role of mother, they are perceived higher in communion than childless women but at the cost of being perceived as lower in agency. Thus, women trade agency for communion when they become mothers—and occupational benefits are related to agency, not communion. Perceptions of men simply gain the benefits of communion from the Women and Men in Conflicting 209 parental social role without any trade-offs or perceived weaknesses from becoming fathers. Woman vs. Leader Considerable research has investigated differing perceptions of men and women in leadership roles, as well as actual differences in leadership behavior (for reviews, see Eagly, Johannesen-Schmidt, & van Engen, 2003; Eagly & Karau, 2002). Research has found that women engage in more interpersonally oriented work while in groups, whereas men engage in more task-oriented work (e.g., Eagly & Karau, 1991). As Carli and Eagly (1999) explained: [T]he tendency for men to emerge as leaders should not be interpreted as a biased tendency to choose men over women, despite behavioral equivalence of the sexes. Instead, the preference for men may primarily reflect a tendency to define leadership in terms of strictly task-oriented contributions, which men deliver somewhat more than women do, at least in part because of resistance to high levels of task-oriented contributions from women. (p. 219) Possibly, through gender role stereotyping, women have more experience and expertise in social skills such as facilitating collaborative processes, which in turn lead them to more democratic leadership styles. Alternatively, resistance to women’s leadership (Heilman et al., 2004) may deter women from autocratic leadership styles, where their ability and authority may be questioned. Eagly and Karau (2002) put forward role congruity theory to explain perceptions of—and reactions to—female leaders (see also Eagly & Koenig, 2008). This theory builds upon social role theory, specifically emphasizing the impact of individuals fulfilling social roles that are perceived to be incongruent. They argued that women are evaluated less favorably than men in regards to leadership ability because, in most societies, men are more likely to hold leadership positions in comparison to women. Moreover, this gendered division of labor has led to gender role stereotypes suggesting that men have personalities that make them inherently better suited for leadership tasks in comparison to woman. In other words, descriptive gender norms lead to prejudice against female leaders. In addition, women who do exhibit the personality characteristics that would be considered successful for a male leader are evaluated less favorably than their male colleagues (Eagly & Karau, 2002; Eagly & Koenig, 2008; Heilman et al., 2004; Rudman & Glick, 1999). One factor seems to be because leadership ability is perceived as less desirable in women than men (Eagly & Karau, 2002; Eagly & Koenig, 2008). In this case, injunctive gender roles—beliefs and expectations about how men and women ought to behave—lead to prejudice against female leaders. Agentic leadership styles among women, in particular, were proposed to elicit greater prejudice, as agentic leadership styles would be viewed as even less appropriate for women. Alternatively, female leaders may be victims of backlash, 210 Clow and Ricciardelli perceived as competent but interpersonally deficient and a threat to traditional gender stereotypes (Rudman & Fairchild, 2004; Rudman & Glick, 1999). Normative and injunctive gender roles and gender stereotype backlash result in subtle discrimination against women in leadership positions. Qualified women are disadvantaged, because if they have personalities consistent with leadership, then they are perceived as unfeminine and their personalities are used against them, whereas if they have personalities consistent with their gender role, then they are perceived as incapable of leading (Eagly & Koenig, 2008; Rudman & Fairchild, 2004; Rudman & Glick, 1999). Consequently, women have less access to leadership roles in comparison to men and, even if women manage to obtain leadership roles, they will have greater difficulty succeeding in those roles— especially in contexts or situations where the incongruity of the female gender role and leadership roles are stressed. Although it may be more difficult for women to obtain leadership positions, empirical evidence suggests that female managers may be more effective than male managers (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Female managers scored higher than male managers on transformational measures (e.g., motivating respect, expressing optimism and excitement about future goals, and focusing on mentoring subordinates), whereas men scored higher than women on more transactional measures, such as laissez-faire leadership (general absence and lack of involvement). These gender differences in leadership were confirmed by a meta-analysis of 45 studies (Eagly et al., 2003). Thus, although it may be more difficult for women to obtain leadership roles, research suggests that they are at least as effective as men once these roles are obtained. Man vs. Nurse Nursing is a female-dominated field in many nations (Meadus, 2000; O’Lynn & Tranbarger, 2007). To say that a randomly selected nurse was likely a woman would be a statement of fact, yet perceivers move beyond the factual gendered division of labor to gender stereotyping. For example, Bartfay and Bartfay (2007) found that 93% of their female nursing student sample agreed that nursing was more appropriate for women than men because women tend to be more caring and compassionate by their inborn nature. This corresponds to the finding that female-dominated professions are perceived as needing female personalities for success (Cejka & Eagly, 1999). In this way, the current gendered division of labor and resulting gender stereotypes function as a system of justification for current gender roles and gender inequality (Cejka & Eagly, 1999; Hoffman & Hurst, 1990; Jost & Banaji, 1994). Cunningham (1999) reported that the stereotype of a nurse is a White woman who is a mother-like figure. Bartfay, Bartfay, Clow, and Wu (2010) found that the majority of their nonnursing student sample agreed that society perceives Women and Men in Conflicting 211 female nurses as more caring and nurturing than male nurses and that nursing is not perceived as a very masculine career for men to pursue. Approximately 30% of nonnursing students (both men and women) and 60% of female nursing students sampled (although none of the male nursing students) agreed that nursing is more appropriate for women because they are naturally more caring and compassionate (Bartfay et al., 2010). As social role theory would predict, people move from the observation that many nurses are female to the assumption that there is something about being female that makes a person a better nurse (Eagly et al., 2000). For example, male nursing students in Taiwan routinely encounter individuals who question their ability to be nurses (Yang et al., 2004). One male nurse reported that he is frequently asked by patients and visitors if he is a real nurse, suggesting that being male and being a nurse are incompatible social roles. Interviews with men in nursing across a number of nations have revealed that the masculinity, sexuality, or competence of these men is often questioned (Burtt, 1998; Harding, 2007; Whittock & Leonard, 2003; Yang et al., 2004), which is perhaps indicative of society’s reaction to individuals in perceived incompatible social roles. Research suggests that many participants do find men and women in nontraditional occupations for their gender to occupy incompatible social roles (Oakhill, Granham, & Reynolds, 2005). Across a series of experiments, Oakhill et al. (2005) asked participants to respond whether or not two social roles (an occupation and a kinship role) could be fulfilled by the same person. For example, participants were presented with the occupation nurse and the kinship role brother and had to decide whether or not a brother could also be a nurse. Results indicated that participants were more likely to reject pairings that were gender role mismatched (e.g., nursebrother, engineer-mother) and even when participants did realize that the gender role mismatched pairings were possible, they tended to respond to gender role mismatched pairs slower than to gender role matched pairs (e.g., nurse-mother, engineer-brother). In addition, Cann (1993) found that participants remembered successful or positive behavior better when it was gender role consistent (e.g., John is a good engineer) and they remembered unsuccessful or negative information better when it was gender role inconsistent (e.g., John is a bad nurse). Clow et al. (2011) found that attitudes and stereotypes of male and female nurses were also related to ambivalent sexism. Specifically, benevolent sexism toward women predicted positive attitudes and stereotypes of female nurses, whereas hostile sexism toward women predicted negative attitudes and stereotypes of female nurses. Hostile sexism toward men predicted negative stereotypes of male nurses. In addition, nursing students reported more positive attitudes and stereotypes of nurses (regardless of the gender of the nurse) than did nonnursing students. Thus, participants’ social role (whether they were a nursing student or not) and ambivalent sexism scores appeared to influence attitudes and stereotypes about men and women in the field of nursing. 212 Clow and Ricciardelli Policy Implications The research findings from social role theory, the stereotype content model, and ambivalent sexism suggest that society (1) resists and negatively stereotypes men and women in social roles that conflict with traditional gender roles and (2) promotes and positively stereotypes men and women who fulfill social roles compatible with traditional gender roles (see Figure 2). This implies that current policies designed to prevent gender discrimination against women may not be as effective as desired, as subtle discrimination targeting women in nontraditional gender roles still persists. In addition, these findings insinuate that current policies do not typically consider gender discrimination targeting men and that men in communal occupations, in particular, appear to be targets of stereotypes and discrimination because they are perceived to be fulfilling incongruent roles. This section of the article will revisit the three perceived role conflicts examined earlier: employee versus married with children, woman versus leader, and man versus nurse. Particular relevant research findings will be highlighted and possible policy implications will be discussed for each perceived role conflict. Following the three perceived role conflict examples, more general policy implications from social role theory, the stereotype content model, and ambivalent sexism for men and women in conflicting social roles are suggested. Employee vs. Married with Children Research has found that parenthood affects perceptions of employees (Cuddy et al., 2004). Employed men and women who have children are perceived as more communal and warm than their childless peers, but mothers gain in warmth at the expense of their perceived competence, whereas fathers gain warmth without any cost. More worrisome, participants were less willing to hire, promote, or train a woman who was expecting her first child than a woman who did not have children. It appears that people have difficulty reconciling the role of mother with the role of employee, whereas people do not have difficulty seeing a man fulfill both the roles of father and worker. Cuddy et al. (2004) commented that men appear to be praised for their desire to be involved parents as well as dedicated employees, whereas women appear to be questioned as to their ability to manage both child care and work. Although policies do exist in multiple nations that allow parents (mothers or fathers) to take leave from work to handle child care, this approach appears to promote women taking leave (gender-congruent role) more than men taking leave (gender-incongruent role). For example, the specifics of the policies regarding parental leave influence the number of men who participate but not the number of women (Marshall, 2008). In countries where more men take parental leave, policies include high wage replacement and nontransferable leave for each parent Women and Men in Conflicting 213 (and additional leave that can be used by either parent), such as Sweden, Norway, and Iceland (84–90% of fathers taking leave), whereas countries with low wage replacement have low levels of men using parental leave, such as Austria and France (1–2% of fathers taking leave). As child care has traditionally been a role subsumed by women and, as such, a role that is underpaid and undervalued, increasing the number of men engaged in this role may help reduce this gendered division of labor, as well as increase the status and pay for people (both men and women) engaged in child care activities. In addition, as more men take advantage of caregiving policies, such as parental leave, society will become more accustomed to viewing both men and women taking time out of their careers for their children. It would follow from social role theory (see Figure 1) that reducing gender disparities in roles at work and at home should move toward changing gender stereotypes and, subsequently, reduce the perceived conflict of the role of parent and the role of employee. Seeing more men and women engaged in both child care and employed work may combine perceptions of child care [from the realm of paternalistic prejudice (high warmth, low competence)] with perceptions of career men and women [in the realm of envious prejudice (low warmth, high competence)], resulting in greater admiration for both male and female employees with children (high warmth and high competence). Ambivalent sexists, however, would resist such policies, as they would challenge existing gender hierarchies, reduce opportunities for paternalism, and promote men (via child care) and women (via workforce) into nontraditional gender roles. Historical cases, such as the Hopkins case, would suggest that legislation works toward countering blatant resistance by ambivalent sexists. However, research has found that when blatant prejudices are countered, more subtle prejudices tend to emerge in their stead (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2004; Goff, Eberhardt, Williams, & Jackson, 2008). As was suggested in interviews with female faculty at M.I.T., existing gender policies that were working toward greater gender equality in the university were also unintentionally creating new gender barriers (Zernike, 2011). Thus, even when blatant prejudice appears to be averted, policies and work places should be periodically examined to investigate whether current policies are functioning as intended or if new, more subtle prejudices have emerged that are not addressed by current legislation, suggesting a need for new or modified policies. In addition, existing gender divisions in household labor may continue to feed into gender stereotypes and resistance to working mothers. Employed women still engage in more household duties than employed men (Connell, 2009). In Sweden, for example, women who worked as many hours in the labor market as men devoted nearly four times as many additional hours to household chores (Boye, 2010). Findings from the European Social Survey (ESS), which includes Austria, Belgium, the Czech Republic, Denmark, Estonia, Finland, France, Germany, Greece, Hungary, Iceland, Luxembourg, the Netherlands, Norway, 214 Clow and Ricciardelli Poland, Portugal, Slovakia, Slovenia, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, Turkey, Ukrain, and the United Kingdom, suggests that these additional demands on women may affect their psychological well-being. Although men’s well-being was not related to the number of hours they spent engaged in paid work or housework, women’s well-being was positively related to hours engaged in paid work and was negatively related to hours engaged in housework (Boye, 2009). Unfortunately, this extra burden faced by many employed women (but fewer employed men) is rarely realized or acknowledged by others. For instance, when estimating the number of hours per day employed individuals spent doing household chores, participants only perceived target individuals with children to be engaged in more housework than target individuals without children; gender was not perceived as a factor (Eagly & Steffen, 1984). These perceptions put women at a disadvantage. Acknowledging the extra burden on career women (whether they have children or not) and finding ways to reward the extra work that women provide—or at the very least, to stop disadvantaging women for it—should improve women’s psychological well-being and move toward greater gender equality overall. Observing gender inequality in the distribution of labor in the home feeds into existing gender stereotypes and further disadvantages women in the workplace, as both men and women become accustomed to women doing more work for less reward. Thus, greater gender equality in the household may lead to greater gender equality in the workplace as well. Woman vs. Leader A woman has yet to become secretary general of the United Nations or head of the World Bank (Connell, 2009). Women typically constitute a very small proportion of national cabinets and few countries have voted a woman into the top role of their governments. For example, in 2007, women held 14% of cabinet seats in the United States and Ecuador, 8% of cabinet seats in Italy and Argentina— only in Sweden and Spain were women nearly equally represented. Women lack representation in the business world as well. Only 5 of the top 200 businesses listed on the Australian stock exchange had a female CEO and there were only 10 female CEOs in the Fortune 500 (Connell, 2009). Research has found that people interpret the behavior of men and women differently (Eagly & Karau, 2002; Heilman et al., 2004). Autocratic and other agentic leadership styles have been argued to be particularly likely to elicit unfavorable evaluations of female leaders due to role incongruity, injunctive gender role stereotypes that woman should be communal and concerned with others instead, and backlash mechanisms to sabotage deviant targets to maintain traditional gender stereotypes (Eagly & Karau, 2002; Rudman & Fairchild, 2004). Thus, women may choose leadership styles that are less likely to evoke negative reactions. Women and Men in Conflicting 215 Social role theory would suggest that greater exposure to women in leadership roles, and agentic leadership roles in particular, may reduce these negative reactions. Guiding and Scouting organizations (e.g., Girl Guides and Girl Scouts) do foster leadership skills in girls and young women. These organizations provide girls with the opportunity to engage in skills that are not normative for their gender (e.g., camping, technology, and public speaking). As the genders are segregated in Guides and Scouts, this helps women to learn how to become leaders—but only among other women (i.e., not among men). Leadership opportunities for women in coeducational settings appear to be lacking. If greater leadership opportunities were provided for young girls, then men and women would become more accustomed to seeing women in these roles and social role theory would suggest that resistance toward female leaders would diminish if the leadership role came to be perceived as normative for women (see Figure 1). For instance, if mandatory units were added to English classes—from grade school on through high school—where students needed to engage in debates and public speaking, then boys and girls would become more accustomed to seeing women in these roles. Group work is common in the classroom. If existing group work was modified to include team leaders and all students had to take turns being the leader of their group, then this would also provide more leadership experiences for women and make it appear more normative for women to be in leadership roles. Allowing leaders to emerge in group work or allowing students to volunteer to participate in public speaking will likely work toward greater gender segregation, as the role of leader is stereotypically associated with the male gender role and boys would be more encouraged to fulfill this role than girls. Thus, to overcome these stereotypic associations, policies need to be put in place that force society to become accustomed to viewing males and females in these roles. Not only would this increase exposure to women in leadership roles, it would also provide girls with greater leadership opportunities, allowing girls to further develop their leadership skills and to become more confident and comfortable in leadership roles. School curriculum seems an ideal setting for such work. Group work and potential for leadership is already prevalent in existing curriculum. Thus, simple modifications to current practices and procedures could easily increase women’s participation in leadership activities. This exposure to women in leadership roles from a young age may also work against the development of ambivalent sexist ideologies, preventing more boys and girls from becoming ambivalent sexists. Man vs. Nurse In 2007, there were 242,959 female registered nurses (RNs) in Canada, but only 15,002 male RNs (Canadian Nurses Association, 2009). Low percentages of men in the field of nursing are reported in a number of countries, such as 216 Clow and Ricciardelli the United States, Australia, England, Hungary, Mexico, and Thailand (Meadus, 2000; O’Lynn & Tranbarger, 2007). Although men have historically played a large role in the nursing field, this has not been the case in many countries since the influence of Florence Nightingale (Bartfay, Bartfay, Clow, & d’Astolfo, 2008; Meadus, 2000; O’Lynn, 2004). Nightingale herself wrote that “the whole reform in nursing at home and abroad has consisted of this: to take all power over the nursing out of the hands of men, and put it into the hands of one female trained head and make her responsible for everything” (quoted in Dossey, 1996, p. 291). Thus, a field that once enjoyed male participation evolved into a career dominated by women. The history of men in nursing is generally lacking from nursing courses and textbooks (Roth & Coleman, 2008; Sherrord et al., 2006). Nursing students are not typically taught about the history of military men in nursing, such as the Knights Hospitallers and the Teutonic Knights. Canadian nursing students are rarely told that the first nurses in Canada were actually Jesuit Catholic priests. The omission of this information is compounded by a lack of male role models in the classroom and in the field, the use of sexist language (e.g., text authors and faculty who exclusively use feminine pronouns to refer to nurses), no promotion of gender differences of care and communication, lack of mentorship for men, and failure to actively recruit men into nursing (O’Lynn, 2004; Roth & Coleman, 2008; Sherrod et al., 2006; Whittock & Leonard, 2003). These issues contribute to severe barriers for men in the nursing profession. Although these barriers may not be direct consequences of gender role stereotyping, stereotypes arising from the gendered division of labor may be influencing or exacerbating these issues. For instance, the perceived conflict between man and nurse may have influenced many textbook authors to omit the various contributions of men to the field of nursing, as those contributions are incompatible with current stereotypes. Social role stereotypes of women as nurses may contribute to the use of sexist language and the failure to consider gender differences in care or communication. Moreover, the female dominance and focus on “feminine” approaches to care in the field may further alienate men (Sherrod et al., 2006). The fewer men applying to nursing, or that stay enrolled in programs, further confirms the social role stereotyping of nursing as a feminine profession. Thus, although social role stereotypes alone do not bar men from the nursing profession, these stereotypes may impact, reinforce, and further exacerbate other barriers as well. Policies need to be developed to recruit more men into communal occupations, such as nursing. Scholarships and reserved seats for qualified male students in nursing programs, along with educating high school guidance counselors about promoting nursing as an appropriate career choice for men (Meadus, 2000), would help to reduce barriers for men who wish to enter the profession. Such incentives send the message that men are valued in nursing and that it is appropriate for men to fulfill this role. As more men enroll in nursing programs, the relevance of men’s Women and Men in Conflicting 217 historical contributions to the field may be more apparent and, thus, more likely to be included in textbooks and curriculum. Moreover, as society learns that nursing is a valid and rewarding career for men, more men may choose to pursue careers in nursing. Increasing the number of men in nursing will work toward changing social role stereotypes and may increase awareness and recognition of men’s ability to care. There are multiple reasons why society should accept men in communal occupations. One, research seems to suggest that men find nursing to be a fulfilling occupation that is in demand, with a stable income, where they can become leaders (Yang et al., 2004; Zysberg & Berry, 2005). Two, gender segregation leads to the devaluing of female-dominated occupations (e.g., Petersen & Morgan, 2005), whereas gender equality may encourage greater respect for these fields. Three, the aging of the world’s population is creating a greater need for individuals in communal occupations and actively recruiting greater numbers of men into these fields may assist in meeting these growing demands (Newell-Withrow & Slusher, 2001; Meadus, 2000; O’Lynn, 2004). Moreover, there is talk of a global nursing shortage (e.g., American Association of Colleges of Nursing, 2010; Canadian Nurses Association, 2010; Center for Nursing Advocacy, 2003). In Canada, for example, the ratio of RNs to the general population is one nurse for every 128 citizens and if no new policies are implemented, Canada is predicting a shortage of 60,000 RNs by 2022 (Canadian Nurses Association, 2010). Japan was already facing nursing shortages as early as 2008 (American Society of Registered Nurses, 2007). One proposal for addressing this serious nursing shortage is to make a concerted effort to actively recruit and retain nurses from current underrepresented subpopulations, such as increasing the number of men in the field (Meadus, 2000; Newell-Withrow & Slusher, 2001; O’Lynn, 2004). Not only would more men in nursing assist with nursing shortages, but greater diversity among practitioners may increase the quality of care provided. Jordan, for example, is one of the few countries where nursing is not currently femaledominated. By 2005, approximately 65% of enrolled nursing students were male (Ahmad & Alasad, 2007). Ahmad and Alasad (2007) reported that one-third of male patients in Jordan expressed a preference for male nurses over female nurses, whereas only 10% of the male patients preferred female nurses (the remaining men did not express a preference). Although the nursing profession in many countries has taken action to increase the ethnic diversity of the profession, similar attention has not been paid to gender diversity (Sherrod et al., 2006; Sullivan, 2000). For example, in the United States men constitute about 5.7% of the nursing field and 49.1% of the population (Sherrod et al., 2006). Patients may benefit from gender diversity in nursing staff, as embarrassment may be reduced and comfort in health care situations increased. As the number of men in communal occupations increases, resistance against men fulfilling these roles should similarly decrease (see Figure 1). 218 Clow and Ricciardelli Provisional Policy Implementation Research has found that conceptualizing social group differences as biologically determined, rather than as a product of social factors, leads to greater role stereotyping and prejudice (Hoffman & Hurst, 1990; Wiliams & Eberhardt, 2008). If mandatory curriculum in elementary schools included units on the social, economical, and environmental factors that contribute to perceived differences in social group behavior, perhaps this would reduce the reliance on biological theories of race and gender, consequently reducing prejudice and stereotypes. Furthermore, students could be educated about the consequences of the gendered division of labor on attitudes and stereotypes. In addition to educating students, policies need to be implemented to reduce the gendering of occupations and the gender division of occupational social roles. Although it is common for societies to divide labor according to gender (Wood, & Eagly, 2002), policies are needed that work toward ensuring greater gender equality. For example, high school guidance counselors actively attempt—and usually succeed—to dissuade interested young men from pursuing careers in nursing (Hart, 2005; Meadus, 2000). If new procedures were introduced where guidance counselors needed to promote the benefits of all careers, regardless of the gender of the interested student, this would be an important step toward removing barriers for students interested in careers that are not dominated by their gender and would work toward reducing the socially perceived gendering of occupations. Communal occupations, in particular, are undervalued in society. For example, being a full-time parent or taking care of elderly family members is unpaid work (Connell, 2009). With the aging of the world’s population, there is going to be an increasing demand upon, and for, caregivers (United Nations, 2009). Societies need to hold caregiving in higher esteem and value communal roles as equal to agentic roles to meet these growing demands. Even when communal roles are incorporated into paid occupations, they tend to be underpaid. For instance, in Canada, the wages of live-in caregivers vary by province, ranging from $8 per hour in British Columbia to $10.25 in Ontario (Human Resources and Skills Development Canada, 2010b). Based on the maximum hours for which a live-in caregiver can be paid (40–48 hours a week, depending upon the province), these employees—most of whom are women and many of whom are non-Canadians on work VISAs—gross a maximum of $16,640 in British Columbia (from which $4,500 is deducted annually for room and board), which places their earnings, prior to any deductions, below the poverty line in British Columbia for a single employable person (Canadian Council on Social Development, 2001). The situation in Ontario is not much better, as live-in caregivers gross a maximum of $25,584 (from which $2,340 is deducted annually for room and board), putting them below the poverty line in Ontario, prior to any deductions, for a couple with two children (Canadian Council Women and Men in Conflicting 219 on Social Development, 2001). Increasing the pay in these communal roles may increase the status of these occupations, the respect employees in these social roles receive, and may attract more men into traditionally “feminine” occupations. Greater gender equality in communal roles, to complement growing gender equality in agentic roles, should reduce gender stereotyping and provide benefits for all. There are other policies and practices that could increase the numbers of men entering communal roles. For example, schools and governments could create scholarships, awards, or other financial incentives to help encourage men to enroll in communal-based programs. Financial incentives do exist for women entering agentic and male-dominated programs and professions (for an example, see Society for Canadian Women in Science and Technology, 2011). Creating similar financial rewards for men in female-dominated areas will convey the message that men are valued in these fields and that it is acceptable for them to enter these occupations and programs. Also, advertising that men are desired in communal-based occupations may increase the likelihood that men will consider these occupations as a viable option, not only recruiting them into the field, but increasing the retention of men in these areas. For instance, the Oregon Center for Nursing (2010) released an advertisement portraying a number of very masculine men (including masculine props, such as a football and a snowboard) and ran the slogan, in large bold font, “Are you man enough . . . To be a nurse?” Another possibility is to reserve seats in programs that prepare students for careers that are numerically dominated by one gender or the other. A medical school in the United States reserves 16 out of 100 seats for qualified minority group members (American Association for Affirmative Action, 2010a). Similarly reserving a number of positions for qualified male students entering female stereotyped professions (and for qualified female students entering male stereotyped professions) could assist in recruiting more men into careers that are more communal in nature. Reserving seats for a particular group of students is not a new concept for many postsecondary institutions. For instance, many courses reserve a set number of seats for students majoring in that discipline and only after student majors have registered are any unused seats opened for students seeking electives. As such, programs that are numerically dominated by a particular gender could implement a similar policy, setting aside a certain number of seats for students in the underrepresented gender. This would force individuals responsible for admissions to actively consider members of the underrepresented gender. This alone should mean that qualified candidates would have a better chance of being recognized and accepted into programs that were nonnormative for their gender. In addition, if insufficient numbers of qualified candidates from the underrepresented gender applied, the formerly reserved seats would then become open to any qualified candidates from the dominant gender, in addition to the other nonreserved seats that they could already have obtained. 220 Clow and Ricciardelli Another concern is that the gender wage gap continues to exist across all levels of education (Statistics Canada, 2009). Women typically earn less than men and the higher the income, the greater the gender wage gap. In 2007, more women than men were working part-time and this gender difference was largest among college graduates. The average salary for male university professors in Canada is at times up to $20,000 more than their female colleagues (Cross, 2010). This does not necessarily reflect that men and women in the same faculty at the same university receive differential pay. Within a university, different disciplines offer different salaries. The disciplines that offer higher salaries, such as business and engineering, are typically male-dominated, whereas the disciplines that offer lower salaries, such as the social sciences and humanities, tend to have a greater proportion of women. Thus, what appears to be a gender wage gap may in fact be a discipline wage gap—although the discipline wage gap may be influenced by the nonequal distribution of men and women in these positions. Moreover, due to historical gender discrimination in hiring practices, gender and seniority are often confounded, such that there is a larger proportion of senior male professors than senior female professors and a larger proportion of junior female professors than junior male professors. As seniority is positively correlated with salary, the reported gender wage gap may also be a seniority wage gap—one that was created due to gender discrimination and is slowly being overcome. In addition to affirmative action (e.g., in the United States) and employment equity (e.g., in Canada) legislation, policies could be created to further standardize salaries within a field. Employees with the same responsibilities should earn equal pay, regardless of their gender. What seems to be lacking is a process of verifying and enforcing salary standardization across genders—ensuring that men are not being paid more than women for equal work. If employers needed to submit a document that outlined each employee’s responsibilities, salary, and gender, these documents could be used to investigate whether gender equality in wages was being practiced. Employers who failed to establish gender equality could be given recommendations and a timeline to rectify the situation and, failing that, could be penalized or prosecuted. Conclusions The unequal distribution of men and women in particular occupations leads to the development of stereotypes that assume men and women possess personality characteristics that would make them successful in those occupations (Eagly & Diekman, 2006; Eagly & Mitchell, 2004; Eagly et al., 2000). Furthermore, these stereotypes function as both descriptive norms, as well as injunctive pressures as to how men and women ought to behave (Eagly & Karau, 2002). In addition, paternalistic prejudice toward traditional women and nontraditional men, envious prejudice toward nontraditional women and traditional men, and ambivalent sexist Women and Men in Conflicting 221 attitudes work toward maintaining current gender inequalities (Fiske et al., 2002; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 1999). Not only do these forces justify current systems and power hierarchies (Jost & Banaji, 1994; Hoffman & Hurst, 1990), they also create barriers for men and women who attempt to fulfill social roles that society perceives as conflicting. Currently, the employee social role and the social role of parent are viewed as incongruous—especially for women. The role of leader is perceived as a better fit with the male gender role in comparison to the female gender role, whereas the role of nurse is perceived as a better fit with the female gender role than the male gender role. Greater exposure to parents who are also successful in their occupations, to women who are effective leaders, and to men who are effective caregivers should work toward reducing these stereotypes for future generations. Measures that are taken to lessen the impact of stereotypes may also serve to increase gender equality across professions. References Ahmad, M. M., & Alasad, J. A. (2007). Patients’ preferences for nurses’ gender in Jordan. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 13(4), 237–242. doi:10.1111/j.1440–172X.2007.00633.x. Alksnis, C., Desmarais, S., & Curtis, J. (2008). Workforce segregation and the gender wage gap: Is “women’s” work valued as highly as “men’s”? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(6), 1416–1441. doi:10.1111/j.1559–1816.2008.00354.x. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday Anchor Books. American Association for Affirmative Action. (2010a). Access, equity and diversity. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.affirmativeaction.org/media/AAAA_Generic_ Affirmative_Action_Presentation_20090908.pdf. American Association for Affirmative Action. (2010b). About affirmative action. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.affirmativeaction.org/about-affirmative-action.html. American Association of Colleges of Nursing. (2010). Nursing shortage. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.aacn.nche.edu/media/FactSheets/NursingShortage.htm. American Society of Registered Nurses. (2007). Nursing shortage update: Japan survey warns of serious nurse shortage. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.asrn. org/newsletter_article.php?journal = shortage&issue_id = 26&article_id = 146. Asbrock, F. (2010). Stereotypes of social groups in Germany in terms of warmth and competence. Social Psychology, 41(2), 76–81. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000011 Bartfay, W. J., & Bartfay, E. (2007). Canadian view of men in nursing explored. Men in Nursing, 2, 32–37. Bartfay, W. J., Bartfay, E., Clow, K. A., & d’Astolfo, J. (2008). Issues facing men in nursing in Canada: A discussion and five year strategic planning. Report prepared for the Canadian Men in Nursing Group (CMNG). Oshawa, Ontario: CMNG. Bartfay, W. J., Bartfay, E., Clow, K. A., & Wu, T. (2010). Attitudes and perceptions towards men in nursing education. The Internet Journal of Allied Health Sciences and Practice, 8(2). Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://ijahsp.nova.edu/articles/Vol8Num2/pdf/Bartfay.pdf. Blair v. Colonial Plaza. (2009). WL 3806778. W.D. Oklahoma. Bosak, J., Sczesny, S., & Eagly, A. H. (2008). Communion and agency judgments of women and men as a function of role information and response format. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1148–1155. doi:10.1002/ejsp.538. Boye, K. (2009). Relatively different? How do gender differences in well-being depend on paid and unpaid work in Europe? Social Indicators Research, 93(3), 509–525. doi:10.1007/s11205– 008-9434–1. 222 Clow and Ricciardelli Boye, K. (2010). Time spent working: Paid work, housework and the gender difference in psychological distress. European Societies, 12(3), 419–442. doi:10.1080/14616691003716928. Burtt, K. (1998). Male nurses still face bias. The American Journal of Nursing, 98(9), 64–65. doi:10.2307/3471876. Canadian Council on Social Development. (2001). Canadian welfare incomes as a percentage of the poverty line by family type and province. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.ccsd.ca/ factsheets/fs_ncwpl01.htm. Canadian Human Rights Commission. (2010). Frequently asked questions about employment equity. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.chrc-ccdp.ca/publications/ee_faq_ee-en.asp. Canadian Nurses Association. (2009). 2007 workforce profile of registered nurses in Canada. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.cna-aiic.ca/CNA/ documents/pdf/publications/2007_RN_Snapshot_e.pdf. Canadian Nurses Association. (2010). The nursing shortage: The nursing workforce. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.cna-aiic.ca/CNA/issues/hhr/default_e.aspx. Cann, A. (1993). Evaluative expectations and the gender schema: Is failed inconsistency better? Sex Roles, 28(11/12), 667–678. doi:10.1007/BF00289986. Caprariello, P. A., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Fiske, S. T. (2009). Social structure shapes cultural stereotypes and emotions: A causal test of the stereotype content model. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 12(2), 147–155. doi:10.1177/1368430208101053. Carli, L. L., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender effects on social influence and emergent leadership. In G. N. Powell (Ed.), Handbook of gender and work (pp. 203–222). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Carvel, J. (2006, June 10). Former male nurse wins sex discrimination case. The Guardian. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.guardian.co.uk/society/2006/jun/10/equality.health. Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(4), 413–423. doi:10.1177/0146167299025004002. Center for Nursing Advocacy, The (2003). Global shortage leads to exodus of experienced nurses from Philippines. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://nursingadvocacy.org/news/ 2003may15_philippines.html. Chen, Z., Fiske, S. T., & Lee, T. L. (2009). Ambivalent sexism and power-related gender-role ideology in marriage. Sex Roles, 60(11–12), 765–778. doi:10.1007/s11199–009-9585–9. Clow, K. A., & Esses, V. M. (2005). The development of group stereotypes from descriptions of group members: An individual difference approach. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8(4), 429–445. doi:10.1177/1368430205056469. Clow, K. A., & Esses, V. M. (2010). Mental representation of familiar others: The impact of occupation, sex, and race. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 32(2), 144–154. doi:10.1080/01973531003738718. Clow, K. A., & Ricciardelli, R. (2011). Ambivalence in stereotypes and attitudes: The implications of possessing positive and negative perceptions. In E. L. Simon (Ed.), Psychology of stereotypes (pp. 243–264). New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Clow, K. A., Ricciardelli, R., & Bartfay, W. J. (2011). Perceptions of male and female nurses from the social role theory and ambivalent sexism perspective. Manuscript under review. Connell, R. (2009). Gender: Short introductions. Malden, MA: Polity Press. Cross, A. (2010). Male faculty out-earn females at universities. Vancouver Sun. August 11, 2010, p. B2. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. Journal of Social Issues, 60(4), 701–718. doi:10.1111/j.0022–4537.2004.00381.x. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 631–648. doi:10.1037/0022– 3514.92.4.631. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S. Y., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. -P., et al. (2009). Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48(1), 1–33. doi:10.1348/014466608X314935. Cunningham, A. (1999). Nursing stereotypes. Nursing Standard, 13(45), 46–47. Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2008). Of men, women, and motivation: A role congruity account. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 434–447). New York: Guilford. Women and Men in Conflicting 223 Dion, K. L. (2003). Prejudice, racism, and discrimination. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Personality and social psychology: Vol. 5 of the Comprehensive handbook of psychology (pp. 507–536). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Dossey, B. M. (1996). Florence Nightingale: Mystic, visionary, healer. Springhouse, PA: Springhouse. Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Aversive racism. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 1–51). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Eagly, A. H. (2004). Prejudice: Toward a more inclusive understanding. In A. Eagly, R. M. Baron, & V. L. Hamilton (Eds.), The social psychology of group identity and social conflict: Theory, application, and practice (pp. 45–64). Washington, DC: APA Books. Eagly, A. H., & Diekman, A. B. (2006). Examining gender gaps in sociopolitical attitudes: It’s not Mars and Venus. Feminism & Psychology, 16(1), 26–34. doi:10.1177/0959–353506060817. Eagly, A. H., Eastwick, P. W., & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. (2009). Possible selves in marital roles: The implications of the anticipated division of labor on the mate preferences of women and men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(4), 403–414. doi:10.1177/0146167208329696. Eagly, A. H., & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2001). The leadership styles of women and men. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 781–797. doi:10.1111/0022–4537.00241. Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & van Engen, M. L. (2003). Tranformational, transactional, and laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 569–591. doi:10.1037/0033–2909.129.4.569. Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (1991). Gender and the emergence of leaders: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 685–710. Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109(3), 573–598. doi:10.1037/0033–295X.109.3.573. Eagly, A. H., & Koenig, A. M. (2008). Gender prejudice: On the risks of occupying incongruent roles. In E. Borgida & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Beyond common sense: Psychological science in the courtroom (pp. 63–81). MA: Blackwell Publishing. Eagly, A. H., & Mitchell, A. A. (2004). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: Implications for the sociopolitical attitudes of women and men. In M. Paludi (Ed.), Praeger guide to the psychology of gender (pp. 183–206). CT: Praeger. Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 735–754. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.46.4.735. Eagly, A. H., Wood, W. & Diekman, A. B. (2000) Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Taunter (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–74). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Eckes, T. (2002). Paternalistic and envious gender stereotypes: Testing predictions from the stereotype content model. Sex Roles, 47(3–4), 99–114. doi:10.1023/A:1021020920715. Equal Opportunity for Women in the Workplace Agency. (2010). About EOWA. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from: http://www.eowa.gov.au/About_EOWA.asp. Evans, J. (1997). Men in nursing: Issues of gender segregation and hidden advantage. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26(2), 226–231. doi:10.1046/j.1365–2648.1997.1997026226.x. Evans, J., & Frank, B. (2003). Contradictions and tensions: Exploring relations of masculinities in the numerically female-dominated nursing profession. The Journal of Men’s Studies, 11(3), 277–292. Fiske, S. T., Bersoff, D. N., Borgida, E., Deaux, K., & Heilman, M. E. (1991). Social science research on trial: Use of sex stereotyping research in Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins. American Psychologist, 46(10), 1049–1060. doi:10.1037/0003–066X.46.10.1049. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878–902. doi:10.1037/0022– 3514.82.6.878. Glick, P., Diebold, J., Bailey-Werner, B., & Zhu, L. (1997). The two faces of Adam: Ambivalent sexism and polarized attitudes toward women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(12), 1323–1334. doi:10.1177/01461672972312009. 224 Clow and Ricciardelli Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491–512. doi:10.1037/0022–3514 .70.3.491. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). The ambivalence toward men inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent beliefs about men. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23(3), 519–536. doi:10.1111/j.1471–6402.1999.tb00379.x. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complement justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56(2), 109–118. doi:10.1037/0003– 066X.56.2.109. Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Mladinic, A., Saiz, J. L., Abrams, D., Masser, B., et al. (2000). Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and benevolent sexism across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 763–775. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.79.5.763. Glick, P., Lameiras, M., & Castro, Y. R. (2002). Education and Caholic religiousity as predictors of hostile and benevolent sexism toward women and men. Sex Roles, 47(9–10), 433–441. doi:10.1023/A:1021696209949. Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S. T., Eckes, T., Masser, B., Volpato, C., et al. (2004). Bad but bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men predict gender inequality in 16 nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 713–728. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.86.5.713. Glick, P., Sakalli-Ugurly, N., Ferreira, M. C., & de Souza, M. A. (2002). Ambivalent sexism and attitudes toward wife abuse in Turkey and Brazil. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(4), 292–297. doi:10.1111/1471–6402.t01–1-00068. Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 292–306. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.94.2.292. Harding, T. (2007). The construction of men who are nurses as gay. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 60(6), 636–644. doi:10.1111/j.1365–2648.2007.04447.x. Hart, K.A. (2005). Breakthrough to nursing national survey results: Student comments focus on passion for nursing, diversity, and challenges. Imprint, 52(2), 30–32, 34. Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., & Tamkins, M. M. (2004). Penalties for success: Reactions to women who succeed at male gender-typed tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 416–427. doi:10.1037/0021–9010.89.3.416. Hersch, J. (2003). The new labor market for lawyers: Will female lawyers still earn less? Cardozo Women’s Law Journal, 10(1), 1–72. Hoffman, C., & Hurst, N. (1990). Gender stereotypes: Perception or rationalization? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 197–208. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.58.2.197. Holroyd, E. A., Bond, M. H., & Chan, H. Y. (2002). Perceptions of sex-role stereotypes, self-concept, and nursing role ideal in Chinese nursing students. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 37, 294–303. doi:10.1046/j.1365–2648.2002.02091.x. Human Resources and Skills Development Canada. (2010a). History of employment equity. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://www.hrsdc.gc.ca/eng/lp/lo/lswe/we/information/history.shtml. Human Resources and Skills Development Canada. (2010b). Regional wages, working conditions and advertisement requirements for the Live-In Caregiver Program (LCP). Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://www.rhdcc-hrsdc.gc.ca/eng/workplaceskills/foreign_workers/advertReq/ wageadreq.shtml. Johnson, C. (2004). Male caregiver wins sex discrimination case. Vancouver Sun. June 15, 2004. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://rapereliefshelter.bc.ca/learn/resources/male-caregiverwins-sex-discrimination-case. Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1–27. Kasen, S., Chen, H., Sneed, J., Crawford, T., & Cohen, P. (2006). Social role and birth cohort influences on gender-linked personality traits in women: A 20-year longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 944–958. doi:10.1037/0022– 3514.91.5.944. Katz-Wise, S. L., Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender-role attitudes and behaviour across the transition to parenthood. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 18–28. doi:10.1037/a0017820. Women and Men in Conflicting 225 Legal Eagle Eye Newsletter for the Nursing Profession. (2008, September). Gender-based shift assignments: Federal court upholds male CNA’s sex-discrimination lawsuit. Retrieved April 30, 2011 from http://www.nursinglaw.com/firesafety2.pdf. Lupton, B. (2006). Explaining men’s entry into female-concentrated occupations: Issues of masculinity and social class. Gender, Work and Organization, 13(2), 103–128. Marshall, K. (2008). Fathers’ use of paid parental leave. Perspectives. Statistics Canada, 75–001-X. Retrieved April 30, 2011 from http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/75–001-x/2008106/pdf/10639eng.pdf. Meadus, R. J. (2000). Men in nursing: Barriers to recruitment. Nursing Forum, 35(3), 5–12. doi:10.1111/j.1744–6198.2000.tb00998.x. Newell-Withrow, C., & Slusher, I.L. (2001). Diversity: An answer to the nursing shortage. Nursing Outlook, 49(6), 270–271. O’Lynn, C. E. (2004). Gender-based barriers for male students in nursing education programs: Prevalence and perceived importance. Journal of Nursing Education, 43, 229–237. O’Lynn, C. E. & Tranbarger, R. E. (2007). Men in nursing: History, challenges, and opportunities. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company. Oakhill, J., Garnham, A., & Reynolds, D. (2005). Immediate activation of stereotypical gender information. Memory & Cognition, 33(6), 972–983. Oregon Center for Nursing. (2010). Posters & more. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://www.oregoncenterfornursing. org/index.php?mode = cms&pageId = postersandmore. Petersen, T., & Morgan, L. (2005). Separate and unequal: Occupational-establishment sex segregation and the gendered wage gap. American Journal of Sociology, 101(2), 329–365. doi:10.1086/230727. Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins. (1989). 490 U.S. 228. Retrieved from Cornell University Law School at: http://www.law.cornell.edu/supct/html/historics/USSC_CR_0490_0228_ZS.html. Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American Community. NY: Touchstone Books. Roth, J. E., & Coleman, C. L. (2008). Perceived and real barriers for men entering nursing: Implications for gender diversity. Journal of Cultural Diversity, 15(3), 148–152. Rudman, L. A., & Fairchild, K. (2004). Reactions to counterstereotypic behavior: The role of backlash in cultural stereotype maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 157–176. Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle-managers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1004–1010. Rudyk, A. (2010). A rising tide: The transformation of sex discrimination into gender discrimination and its impact on law enforcement. The International Journal of Human Rights, 14(2), 189–214. doi:10.1080/13642980802535625. Sherrod, B., Sherrod, D., & Rasch, R. (2006). From the bedside to the boardroom, nursing needs to increase the number of men in its ranks. Men in Nursing, 1, 34–39. Sibley, C. G., & Wilson, M. S. (2004). Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexist attitudes toward positive and negative sexual female subtypes. Sex Roles, 51(11–12), 687–696. doi:10.1007/s11199– 004-0718-x. Society for Canadian Women in Science and Technology. (2011). Scholarship links. Retrieved October 18, 2011 from http://www.scwist.ca/index.php/main/scholarship-links/. Statistics Canada. (2009). Culture, tourism and the centre for education statistics. 81–595-M, number 74. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/81–595-m/81–595m2009074-eng.htm. Statistics Canada. (2010). The Daily. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://www.statcan.gc.ca/dailyquotidien/101220/ dq101220b-eng.htm. Sullivan, E. J. (2000). Men in nursing: The importance of gender diversity. Journal of Professional Nursing, 16(5), 253–254. doi:10.1053/jpnu.2000.9455. Takabayashi, K. (2007). The effect of threat to self on prejudice toward women: An examination from the perspective of ambivalent sexism theory. The Journal of Social Psychology, 23(2), 119–129. 226 Clow and Ricciardelli United Nations. (2009). World population ageing. Department of Economic and Social Affairs. Retrieved August 30, 2010 from http://www.un.org/esa/population/ publications/WPA2009/WPA2009_ WorkingPaper.pdf. Viki, G. T., Abrams, D., & Hutchison, P. (2003). The “true” romantic: Benevolent sexism and paternalistic chivalry. Sex Roles, 49(9–10), 533–537. Whittock, M., & Leonard, L. (2003). Stepping outside the stereotype. A pilot study of the motivations and experiences of males in the nursing profession. Journal of Nursing Management, 11(4), 242–249. doi:10.1046/j.1365–2834.2003.00379.x. Williams, C. L. (1992). The glass escalator: Hidden advantages for men in the “female” professions. Social Problems, 39(3), 253–267. doi:10.1525/sp.1992.39.3.03×0034h. Williams, M. J., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2008). Biological conceptions of race and the motivation to cross racial boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 1033–1047. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.94.6.1033. Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 699–727. doi:10.1037//0033–2909.128.5.699. Yang, C. -I., Gau, M. -L., Shiau, S. -J., Hu, W. -H., & Shih, F. -J. (2004). Professional career development for male nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 48(6), 642–650. doi:10.1111/j.1365– 2648.2004.03252.x. Zernike, K. (2011). Gains, and drawbacks, for female professors. New York Times, March 21, 2011, pp. 1–2. Retrieved April 30, 2011 from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/ 03/21/us/21mit.html?_r=1&src= me&ref=homepage. Zysberg, L., & Berry, D. M. (2005). Gender and students’ vocational choices in entering the field of nursing. Nursing Outlook, 53(4), 193–198. doi:10.1016/j.outlook.2005.05.001. KIMBERLEY A. CLOW is an Associate Professor in the Faculty of Social Science and Humanities at the University of Ontario Institute of Technology. She received her PhD in Psychology from the University of Western Ontario. Her research focuses on stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination. Current research projects investigate stigma and wrongful conviction, stereotypes of minority defendants within the context of pre-trial publicity, as well as the stigma encountered by men and women in social roles that conflict with traditional gender roles. ROSEMARY RICCIARDELLI is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Sociology, Faculty of Liberal Arts and Professional Studies at York University. She received her Ph.D. in Sociology from McMaster University. Her research looks at wrongful conviction, as well as conceptualizations of masculinity and risk in the criminal justice system. Her other research interests include gender, identity construction, and male corporeality. She is particularly interested in research combining qualitative and quantitative methodologies.