* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download running head: the rejected and the bullied
Self-categorization theory wikipedia , lookup
Social loafing wikipedia , lookup
False consensus effect wikipedia , lookup
Communication in small groups wikipedia , lookup
Social tuning wikipedia , lookup
Social dilemma wikipedia , lookup
Albert Bandura wikipedia , lookup
Group dynamics wikipedia , lookup
Belongingness wikipedia , lookup
Social perception wikipedia , lookup
Peer pressure wikipedia , lookup
Vladimir J. Konečni wikipedia , lookup
Relational aggression wikipedia , lookup
The Rejected and The Bullied RUNNING HEAD: THE REJECTED AND THE BULLIED The Rejected and the Bullied: Lessons about Social Misfits from Developmental Psychology Jaana Juvonen and Elisheva F. Gross University of California, Los Angeles Draft of Presentation at the 7th Annual Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology: “The Social Outcast: Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection, and Bullying” To be included as a chapter in: Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., & von Hippel, W. (Eds.) under contract). The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying. New York: Psychology Press. Address Correspondence to: Jaana Juvonen Department of Psychology University of California, Los Angeles Los Angeles, CA 90095-1521 (310) 825-8293 e-mail: [email protected] 1 The Rejected and The Bullied 2 The Rejected and the Bullied: Lessons about Social Misfits from Developmental Psychology The study of social outcasts among children has a long tradition in developmental psychology. This topic has played a prominent role in social developmental research in part because of the surprisingly potent power of the consequences of being rejected or bullied. Rejected and bullied children are at risk for a range of subsequent problems, including dropping out of school, compromised mental health, and criminality (Parker & Asher, 1987; Kupersmidt, Dodge & Coie, 1990). More recently, this topic has received renewed attention in light of media accounts of infamous school shootings in the US. Many of the youngsters who hurt and killed their schoolmates and teachers were allegedly bullied and rejected by their peers. The goal of this chapter is to provide insights from developmental research on the complex array of intrapersonal and interpersonal difficulties that both lead to and result from peer rejection. To highlight the distinctive contributions of developmental psychology to our understanding of social outcasts, we begin by comparing the last decade of relevant research published in developmental and social psychology’s leading journals. Following this brief analysis, we review the developmental research in light of three guiding questions: 1) who is most at risk for rejection; 2) what are the consequences of rejection and bullying, and 3) who is immune from these consequences? We then propose an integrative model of the intrapersonal and group-level processes by which peer rejection leads to long-term maladaptive outcomes. We conclude by exploring the implications of our review and proposed model for the social psychological study of rejection. Throughout our review, we highlight the role of the social, and specifically the peer group, contexts of rejection. In our developmental analysis of rejection, we focus on peers for both theoretical and The Rejected and The Bullied 3 practical reasons. From a theoretical perspective, same or similar age peers provide a unique developmental context. In contrast to relationships with adults, peer relationships are presumably more symmetrical or balanced in terms of power. Among peers, children gain important insights about equality, reciprocity, loyalty, and trustworthiness (Berndt, 1996). Furthermore, peer relationships, and friendships in particular, provide opportunities for “social practice” in interpersonal behaviors that are critical to both current and future relationship development and maintenance. These behaviors include cooperation, negotiation, compromise, conflict resolution, and the provision and seeking of social support (Hartup, 1996). In light of the developmental functions served by healthy peer relationships, it is therefore important to understand what happens to children who are social outcasts deprived of normative opportunities for social practice. In the present review, we therefore include only studies that examine rejection and bullying by same- or similar-age peers, (to the exclusion of rejection by parents and romantic partners, for example). A second reason to focus on peers in reviewing the developmental research on rejection and bullying is practical: Most developmental investigators conceptualize rejection in terms of perceptions, preferences and behaviors directed toward an individual by familiar peers, as opposed to strangers or romantic partners. In so doing, developmentalists have forged a path toward understanding social outcasts that is often distinct, both methodologically and conceptually, from that taken by social psychologists. A note on terms: The many faces of peer rejection. As anyone who has been picked last for kickball, endured ridicule or nasty rumors from ostensible friends, or sprinted home to avoid the neighborhood bully will attest, there are numerous ways to experience peer rejection. Following Asher, Rose and Gabriel (2001), who undertook the onerous task of counting those The Rejected and The Bullied 4 ways (and counted 32), we employ the term “peer rejection” inclusively. It should be noted, however, that this is not typical of developmental psychologists who normally differentiate bullying or victimization by peers from peer rejection. In developmental research, rejection is commonly defined as peers’ social avoidance of, dislike of, or reluctance to affiliate with an individual child. In contrast, bullying is conceptualized as an active form of hostility toward a target (rather than mere avoidance or dislike) that is characterized by an imbalance of power (Olweus, 1978), such as a strong person intimidating a weaker one. Bullying can take many forms including physical aggression, exclusion, and spreading nasty rumors. Bullying is typically carried out by one or a few children, although bullied children are often also rejected by the larger peer group (Graham & Juvonen, 1998; Juvonen, Graham, & Schuster, 2003). In the present review, we include bullying within the broad category of peer rejection because we presume that from the perspective of the target of rejection or bullying the experiences are more similar than different. We also propose that although the manifestations of rejection (avoidance) and bullying (hostility) vary, the action of non-inclusion or exclusion serves similar functions for the group (i.e., the rejectors). We therefore review developmental research on rejection and bullying as two conceptually and empirically related phenomena relevant to understanding the social outcast. But how do developmental psychologists study peer rejection and bullying? We now turn to a brief comparison between developmental and social psychological research. A Decade of Studying the Outcast: Comparing Developmental and Social Studies We conducted an analysis of relevant studies published between 1993 and 2003 in the two leading journals in developmental psychology (Child Development and Developmental Psychology) and social psychology (Journal of Personality and Social Psychology and The Rejected and The Bullied 5 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin), respectively (see Appendix). Based on a search using six key words (rejection, exclusion, ostracism, harassment, victimization, and bullying), a total of 145 articles were identified (49% were published in the social psychology journals). This method is a short-hand way to represent the two fields and, as such, has limitations. Nevertheless, this quick comparison reveals striking differences in conceptualizations and methodological approaches across the two areas of psychology. What Constitutes Rejection? Chronic versus discrete experiences. As shown in the Appendix, most developmentalists examine rejection and bullying experiences as chronic social problems rather than isolated interpersonal experiences. As shown in Table 1, 89% of the developmental publications, compared to 45% of the social psychology articles published within the last decade investigated rejection as a repeated experience or chronic problem. The chronicity of the social experience is reflected in developmental measures of rejection that indicate sociometric status or social standing. Children are typically asked to name the individuals (e.g., classmates) whom they do not like or with whom they do not want to affiliate (i.e., play or “hang out”). An individual’s rejected status is based on the number or proportion of such nominations within the group (see Cillessen & Bukowski, 2000, for a comprehensive review of these methods). Rejection is thus operationalized as the consensus or overall attitude of the group toward one of its members. The operational prerequisite for group consensus means that rejected status is likely to be based on recurring rejection experiences, as opposed to fleeting or isolated incidents. Developmental studies that rely on such indicators of social status stand in sharp contrast to social psychological studies in which participants often describe or undergo a discrete incident of rejection. Three quarters of the social psychological studies and only 26% of the developmental articles examined The Rejected and The Bullied 6 discrete social rejection, ostracism, or bullying incidents. Notably, some studies (24% in social and 13% in developmental) investigated both short-term and chronic forms of rejection. Self-reported versus other-reported rejection. Who determines whether an individual is rejected? Given the aforementioned popularity of peer nomination procedures in developmental research, it is not surprising that 78% of the last decade’s developmental studies assessed rejection based on others’ perceptions. In addition to peer ratings or nominations assessing group sentiment toward a target child (e.g., dislike or avoidance), measures of behavioral or social reputation (e.g., “Who bullies others?” or “Who gets bullied?”) are also used. Social psychological studies assessed others’ perceptions of social outcasts much less frequently (18%). Instead, 97% of non-experimental investigations in social psychology journals relied on selfreports of rejection (as compared to 23% of non-experimental developmental studies). Who is doing the rejecting? One of the most notable differences between the social and developmental studies pertains to the source of rejection: 96% of the developmental studies versus 54% of the social psychological studies examined perceptions or acts of rejection by familiar others (e.g., classmates), as opposed to strangers, who were the source of rejection in 48% of social and just 4% of developmental investigations. In addition, only 16% of the developmental studies, compared to 75% of the social psychological studies, involved rejection by one individual (as opposed to a group). The relatively few developmental studies of rejection by an individual mainly focused on maternal rejection (a topic excluded from our review). Experimental, Correlational, and Longitudinal Designs As shown in Table 1, the vast majority (81%) of the relevant studies on social outcasts published in the developmental journals were non-experimental, whereas those published in social psychology journals are fairly evenly split between experimental (51%) and non- The Rejected and The Bullied 7 experimental (49%). As we will discuss later, the correlational nature of developmental studies hinders inferences about whether developmental outcomes associated with rejection (e.g., criminality) are better explained by the antecedents of rejection (e.g., aggressive tendencies) rather than by rejection per se. In 70% of the social psychology experiments, the rejection experience was experimentally manipulated. In 42% of these studies, however, rejection did not involve actual interpersonal interaction with the source of rejection (either in person or via phone, video or computer). In these cases, rejection was communicated on paper only or by the experimenter. This form of rejection manipulation was used in only one developmental study. When developmental psychologists make use of experimental methods, they often employ contrived play group procedures in which participants are assigned to play in groups of unfamiliar peers for a series of brief sessions. This method allows investigators to study the behaviors and emerging social status of previously unfamiliar children in a relatively controlled setting. In past developmental studies, this approach has been used to examine both antecedents and consequences of rejection and bullying (Rabiner & Coie, 1989; Schwartz, Dodge, & Coie, 1993), as well as the stability of rejection across groups (Coie & Kupersmidt, 1983). Among the non-experimental studies, about half of the developmental investigations were longitudinal, in contrast to 21% of the social investigations. Although not equally prevalent in the two disciplines, the mean length of the longitudinal studies in developmental and social psychological studies was similar (Ms = 40 and 35 months, respectively). In sum, in comparison to social psychological research, developmental research on social outcasts more often 1) employs correlational and longitudinal designs, 2) focuses on chronic rather than discrete experiences of rejection, and 3) assesses rejected or bullied status based on the consensus of the peer group. Although each approach has its limitations, the developmental The Rejected and The Bullied 8 research on antecedents and correlates of persistent rejection by familiar peers can provide a more complete picture of the evolution or unfolding of rejection and the group functions rejection may serve. We now begin our review of the developmental body of research by asking: what causes an individual to become a social outcast? Rather than provide an exhaustive review of the hundreds of studies that have examined the antecedents of rejection and bullying (see Hawker & Bolder, 2001; Kupersmidt et al., 1990; McDougall, Hymel, Vaillancourt, & Mercer, 2001, for comprehensive reviews), we propose that the most parsimonious account for the cause of peer rejection is person-group dissimilarity. The Social Outcast as Group Misfit Although virtually everyone experiences peer rejection or bullying at some point in their childhood or adolescence, repeated rejection and chronic bullying are neither random nor universal experiences. Numerous studies indicate that aggressive and socially withdrawn youth are most likely to be cast out from the group (e.g, Boivin, Hymel, & Bukowski, 1995; Rubin, LeMare, & Lollis, 1990). Associations between peer rejection and aggression as well as between rejection and social withdrawal have also been observed cross-culturally, among Italian (Tomada & Schneider, 1997), Chinese (Schwartz, Chang & Farver, 2001), and Indonesian children and adolescents (French, Jansen, & Pidada, 2002). What can account for children and adolescents’ intolerance for aggression and social withdrawal among their peers? When Coie and Pennington (1976) asked children to describe someone who is “different from other kids,” aggression and shyness were among the behavioral descriptions most frequently mentioned by first- through eleventh-grade students. Similar results were obtained in a study with 12-year-old Finnish children (Juvonen, 1991). Younger, Gentile, The Rejected and The Bullied 9 and Burgess (1993) have likewise shown that both aggression and social withdrawal are perceived as deviant in middle childhood. Hence, the associations between peer rejection and both aggression and social withdrawal may in part reflect that these behaviors are among the most salient deviations from group behavioral norms across childhood and adolescence. There is a small body of developmental research specifically testing the deviancerejection hypothesis. These studies suggest that determinants of rejection are not necessarily invariant across all groups; rather, they differ depending on the prevailing group norms. The Person-Group Misfit Model Wright et al. (1986) were the first to formally propose and test a norm-based model of rejection. They examined associations between individual behavior and peer status in groups of 10-year-old boys at a summer camp for children with behavioral difficulties. They found that aggression and withdrawal each predicted peer rejection only in groups in which the behavior was non-normative. In groups displaying low levels of aggressive behavior (e.g., verbal threats, hitting), aggressive individuals were rejected. In contrast, in high-aggression groups, social withdrawal was the non-normative and less accepted behavior, whereas aggressive behavior was unrelated to peer status. This landmark study provided strong initial evidence for the importance of group composition in determining who gets rejected. Aggression, commonly conceived as an invariant cause of peer rejection, was instead shown to cause rejection only when it represented deviance from group norms. More recent investigations have found similar group effects. Using an experimental paradigm, Boivin, Dodge, and Coie (1995) found relations among aggressive, prosocial, and withdrawn behaviors, group behavioral composition, and social preference that were similar to those reported by Wright and colleagues. Moreover, their findings are even more The Rejected and The Bullied 10 consistent with the person-group misfit hypothesis than Wright et al.’s. Among novel play groups composed of African American boys recruited from first and third grades of a local elementary school, both withdrawn and aggressive behaviors predicted low status in groups in which the behavior was infrequent. A more recent large-scale naturalistic study provides further support for the person-group misfit model (Stormshak, Bierman, Bruschi, Dodge, & Coie, 1999). Using multilevel modeling techniques to simultaneously test group- and individual-level patterns of associations across 2895 children in 134 first-grade classrooms, Stormshak and colleagues (1999) found strong evidence that the relation between certain individual behaviors and peer preference varied by classroom context. Interestingly, they found distinct contextual effects for aggression, social withdrawal, pro-social behavior and inattention/hyperactivity. Whereas aggression and social withdrawal varied in their acceptability across classrooms in a manner consistent with Wright et al. (1986) and Boivin et al. (1995), pro-social behavior and inattention/hyperactivity were invariant in their associations with social preference, with pro-social positively related and inattention/hyperactivity negatively related to social preference. Although most studies of person-group misfit share a focus on aggressive, withdrawn and pro-social behaviors as correlates of acceptance and rejection, there are exceptions. A recent study of 9-, 13-, and 16-year-old Chinese youth showed that relations between academic performance and social acceptance varied according to group academic performance (Chen, Chang & He, 2003). For example, in high achieving groups, low achievers were less accepted than high achievers. This investigation is also notable for defining the group not simply as the classroom, but rather in terms of self- and peer-defined social networks within the larger school setting (e.g., Cairns, Cairns, & Neckerman, 1989; Cairns & Cairns, 1994). This approach The Rejected and The Bullied 11 provides a more precise definition of the peer group and lends itself to the study of older youth, for whom a single classroom no longer necessarily represents the sole or even primary source of peer affiliations. Development as a Dynamic Normative Context The studies reviewed thus far suggest that the violation of social norms predicts peer rejection. Indeed, social norm violation has been shown to predict rejection among children as young as four years old (Clifford, 1963). However, what constitutes non-normative or deviant behavior might vary according to age. Accordingly, the correlates of rejection are expected to differ. For example, the types of aggression that predict rejection vary by age. Across the elementary school years, the strength of association between rejection and overt forms of aggression (e.g., hitting) wanes as children practice more indirect forms of aggression, such as ostracism or spreading rumors (see Coie, Dodge, & Kupersmidt, 1990, for a review). In one multi-age group study, patterns of association among peer ratings of popularity, unpopularity, and specific interpersonal behaviors reversed when children entered adolescence (LaFontana & Cillessen, 2002, Study 1). Physical and relational aggression were positively correlated with unpopularity among younger participants, but positively correlated with popularity among adolescents. Withdrawal, in turn, was not associated with unpopularity until seventh grade. Similarly, it has been shown that the correlations between peer status and both isolated and overemotional (cf. manifestations of social withdrawal) behaviors become stronger by fourth and fifth grades compared to earlier grades (Coie et al., 1990; Wood, Cowan, & Baker, 2002). What can account for the observed age-based shifts in the social norms governing rejection? Cognitive developmental influences and constraints. The changing social evaluations of The Rejected and The Bullied 12 behavior across age may be at least partially accounted for by changes in the cognitive capacities of the evaluators. Younger et al. (1993) have shown that whereas aggression is a salient behavioral dimension in first-graders’ peer assessments, withdrawal behaviors are less welldefined but become progressively more well-defined—and less acceptable—in children’s perceptions across the elementary school years. Younger and colleagues discuss multiple explanations for this relatively late development, including the greater cognitive complexity of withdrawn versus aggressive schemas (the former relying more than the latter on an understanding of inner states, which are not commonly incorporated into young children’s interpersonal perceptions; e.g., Peevers & Secord, 1973), the fact that withdrawn behavior is not as easily categorized as morally “bad” as aggressive behavior, and, most relevant to the present review, the gradual normative increases in children’s social interactivity across the elementary school years. Normative shifts in adolescence: Dissociation between peer and adult values. An examination of adolescent social norms or values offers a promising explanation for the startling reversals in the social acceptability of aggression in adolescence. By adolescence, peers may not only tolerate, but even admire and ascribe dominance and popularity to aggressive and assertive individuals (Luthar & McMahon, 1996; Parkhurst & Hopmeyer, 1998; LaFontana & Cillessen, 2002). This shift may be part of a broader trend toward increased normativeness of adult rule violations in adolescence (Allen, Weissberg, & Hawkins, 1989; Coie, Terry, Lenox, & Lochman, 1995; Moffitt, 1993). The individual who deviates from adult norms may be an outcast as a child but a hero as a teenager. Among the possible explanations for the observed dissociation between adult and adolescent norms is that adolescents, uncomfortable with the gap between their newfound physical maturity and restricted social status, perceive aggressive and anti-social The Rejected and The Bullied 13 peers to have the mature social status they seek (Moffitt, 1993). Another explanation for the popularity of aggressive youth may be that the need to fortify one’s developing social identity results in a polarization of the values associated with the ingroup (i.e., peer society) versus the outgroup (i.e., adult society; Juvonen & Cadigan, 2002). In sum, a growing body of evidence suggests that aggressive and withdrawn behaviors that are often considered invariant or universal behavioral predictors of peer rejection may be more accurately and parsimoniously conceptualized as behaviors that invite exclusion and hostility from the group to the extent that they deviate from group norms. Group effects on the predictors of rejection and victimization have been found for groups defined by age, social network, and assignment (e.g., classroom or experimental play group). Although this evidence indicates that one of the group functions of rejection is to mark, and presumably limit, group deviance, a critical question remains to be addressed: what is the effect of peer rejection on the deviant individual? If, as we propose, rejection and bullying serve to deter deviance from group norms, then these group behaviors should be experienced as aversive by targets, in a manner analogous to the painful burn experienced by a child who is learning that hot stoves should be avoided. The Aversive Consequences of Being Rejected or Bullied We now turn to the question of rejection’s consequences. As indicated by our review of the last decade’s research in the two leading developmental journals, it is often debatable whether what are conceptualized as “consequences” of exclusion are in fact mere correlates or antecedents of rejection and bullying (see Parker & Asher, 1987). Tests of causal models that span relatively long time periods may be most effective in distinguishing consequences from correlates and antecedents; we therefore focus here on a few key longitudinal studies. It should The Rejected and The Bullied 14 be noted, however, that even longitudinal studies (cf. Parker and Asher, 1987) often failed to control for behavioral correlates of rejection in predictions of adjustment outcomes. Hence, few studies can conclusively identify whether rejection precedes or exerts an independent impact on manifestations of distress, as opposed to serving merely as a marker or indicator of concurrent or pre-existing difficulties. Direct Effects of Rejection on Social Distress In a study capitalizing on transition to a new school, Vernberg, Abwender, Ewell, and Beery (1992) examined the longitudinal associations between bullying experiences and social anxiety. They found that socially anxious young teens were no more likely than their nonanxious peers to experience bullying in the new school, but teens who were bullied were at increased risk for social avoidance and distress about new situations one month into the schoolyear. These findings are consistent with a four-year prospective study of kindergartners (Kochenderfer-Ladd & Wardrop, 2001) showing that children who became victims of bullying in subsequent grades did not initially differ from their non-victimized peers but later reported feeling more lonely. In a study of Italian sixth- and seventh-graders, Kiesner (2002) reported that peer preference (computed as the difference between the number of “liked most” and “liked least” nominations a child receives) predicted depressive symptoms two years later, even after controlling for prior levels of depression. In sum, earlier experiences of bullying and rejection do predict subsequent emotional pain. If social withdrawal is conceptualized as a behavioral manifestation of distress, then there is additional evidence for the prediction that rejection produces social pain. In a study examining the emergence of bullying in novel play groups, Schwartz et al. (1993) found that non-assertive boys, who emerged as victims of peer maltreatment across multiple play sessions, became The Rejected and The Bullied 15 increasing withdrawn as the group’s behavior became more targeted, negative, and coercive toward them. The most victimized boys spent less and less time in social play as they received increasing negative responses from their peers. Similarly, in one of the first studies of contrived play groups, Dodge (1983) found that boys who were neglected (i.e., not liked or disliked) by their peers displayed high levels of withdrawn behavior only after receiving repeated rebuffs from their peers. To return to the analogy of the child at a hot stove, these findings convey that the child who experiences pain in response to the burn of the stove’s heat is likely to subsequently avoid the stove altogether. Evidence that rejection can lead to further withdrawal, coupled with the fact that rejection appears to be most strongly associated with social distress among withdrawn youth (e.g., Crrick & Ladd, 1993; Rubin et al., 1993), suggests a plausible alternative account for increases in social distress: might social withdrawal be a more direct predictor than rejection per se? Not according to at least one longitudinal study, in which the relation between withdrawal and distress was fully mediated by peer rejection: that is, children who withdrew further from peers across a one-year period became more lonely to the degree that their withdrawn behavior led them to be more rejected by classmates. The research reviewed above indicates that rejection and bullying place youth at increased risk for emotional as well as behavioral difficulties. However, in real life, these associations are decidedly more complex, inasmuch as the consequences of rejection in turn increase the risk for subsequent maltreatment by peers. We next review evidence from longitudinal research that takes into account the bidirectional nature of associations between rejection and distress. Bidirectional Links Between Rejection and Distress The Rejected and The Bullied 16 In a one-year longitudinal study of bullying among young teens, the fit of two path models were compared: one model in which emotional distress triggers peer victimization and the one in which victimization triggers distress (Nishina, Witkow, & Juvonen, 2003). The findings revealed that psychological indicators of social pain—depression, social anxiety, and loneliness—may both cause and be caused by bullying experiences. Similarly, Egan and Perry (1998) found that low self-regard predicted increased risk of being bullied and that bullying experiences also predicted negative self-views across a one-year time interval. Bidirectional associations were also reported by Hodges and colleagues (1999): teacher-reported internalizing and externalizing problems increased the likelihood of being perceived a victim by peers, and peer reputation as a victim itself increased the likelihood of experiencing subsequent internalizing and externalizing problems. These data suggest that psychologically vulnerable youth are easy targets and when they are bullied, they become even more vulnerable. Effects of Chronicity of Rejection Experiences Research distinguishing chronic from temporary or transient peer rejection has indicated that the presence and intensity of aversive outcomes may depend on the chronicity or duration of rejection or victimization experiences (e.g., DeRosier, Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 1994). For example, Vernberg et al. (1992) found repeated self-reported bullying experiences to predict social avoidance and distress at the end of the school year. Burks, Dodge and Price (1995) also identified long-term but not temporary rejection as a risk factor for later internalizing problems such as depression, loneliness, and social withdrawal. In their study, boys rejected by their peers for two consecutive years displayed significantly more internalizing problems in the short and long term than boys rejected for only one year, who did not differ from never-rejected peers in terms of internalizing problems (Burks et al., 1995). The Rejected and The Bullied 17 In further support of the claim that the stability of rejection experiences matters, recent data document that temporary experiences of bullying do not necessarily have long-lasting emotional impact. For example, Kochenderfer-Ladd and Wardrop (2001) observed no significant differences between third-grade children bullied only in kindergarten (but not subsequently) and non-victimized peers. Similarly, Juvonen, Nishina, & Graham (2000) demonstrated that young teens bullied in the beginning of the study but not a year later did not differ from non-victimized peers in terms of feelings of depression, loneliness, and self-esteem. Hence, the negative effects of bullying or rejection can be short-lived; however, prolonged or continued experience increases the probability of social and emotional pain. What contributes to the stability (or instability) of rejection? Kochenderfer and Ladd (1997) found that among 5- and 6-year-olds, aggressive retaliation (fighting back) increased the likelihood of continued victimization. In contrast, having a friend was associated with decreased bullying among boys. Hence, the victim’s behavioral response and his or her social affiliations are likely to play a role in maintaining or ending peer maltreatment. These is also some evidence that also the expectations of the rejected youth play a role in maintaining (or changing) their social experiences. By relying on contrived peer group methodology, Rabiner and Coie (1989) showed that the mere manipulation of expectations of the sociometrically rejected children improved their social status within the novel play groups consisting of non-rejected peers. The sociometrically rejected children, who were told that their new playmates liked them after their initial play group meeting, were better liked than the randomized control group not receiving such positive expectation induction. In this study, no behavioral difference between the experimental and control groups were identified. Hence, it remains unclear how positive expectations “worked” to produce more favorable reactions. The Rejected and The Bullied 18 Nevertheless, these findings indicate that expectations on the part of the rejected children play a part in maintaining or changing others’ reactions toward them. Expectations and attributional biases of peers (or rejectors) also contribute to the perpetuation of peer rejection. For example, Hymel (1986) found that rejected children acquire social reputations that function as cognitive schemas biasing the reactions toward the rejected. She showed that children interpret identical actions by liked and disliked peers in strikingly different ways. Whereas liked peers were given the benefit of doubt for negative actions, disliked peers were perceived to have intentionally performed negative behaviors and were rarely credited for positive actions (see also Hymel, Wagner, & Butler, 1990 for a review). Such expectations or what Hymel calls “affective biases” make it more likely that the same children continue to get excluded and ostracized. Taken together, Rabiner and Coie’s and Hymel and colleagues’ findings suggest that social cognitive processes of both the rejected and rejectors may conspire to perpetuate a child’s status as outcast. The Moderating Effects of Social Context If both social and individual factors moderate the stability of rejection, might they also moderate the effects of rejection? Developmental researchers have begun to examine a variety of social moderators of the impact of rejection and bullying on feelings of distress. For example, having even just one friend appears to mitigate at least some social pain of peer victimization: bullied youth with a friend tend to display lower levels of distress than those who are victimized and friendless (Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997; Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999). A longitudinal study of Canadian third- through seventh-graders by Hodges et al. (1999) demonstrated that children who experienced bullying but had a friend reported neither elevated behavioral problems nor increased distress. In contrast, the behavioral problems of bullied The Rejected and The Bullied 19 children with no protective friendships worsened over time. Our recent research on daily encounters with bullying illustrates more subtle social moderators of children’s responses to victimization (Nishina & Juvonen, 2003). We proposed that youth interpret their experience of being bullied not in a social vacuum but in comparison to others. Hence, in addition to obtaining data on personal experiences of bullying, we also asked youth to describe bullying incidents that happened to others. We then examined fluctuations in daily affect (humiliation, anxiety) among young teens as a function of their reported incidents of bullying. As expected, we found that witnessing others being bullied ameliorated some of the acute effects of being bullied oneself. On days when youth reported both experiencing and witnessing bullying, their feelings of anxiety were increased, whereas their reports of humiliation were reduced, compared to days when they experienced but did not witness bullying. Hence, social comparison may simultaneously ease and exacerbate different manifestations of social pain. In our most recent work on bullying, we examined the moderating role of classroom environment on the psychological responses of victims (Bellmore, Witkow, Graham, & Juvonen, 2003). Extending the person-group misfit hypothesis (e.g., Stormshak et al., 1999), we hypothesized that victimization is most strongly associated with emotional and social pain when the classroom is orderly as opposed to disorderly. In other words, the plights of victims are worse when the local norms do not legitimize disruption and peer maltreatment (e.g., Leadbeater et al., 2003). Indeed, we found that in disorderly classrooms, all students (bullied or not) experienced relatively high levels of social anxiety, whereas in the orderly classrooms, it was only bullied students who reported higher rates of social anxiety. Paradoxically, this may mean that classroom environments that are most protective for the majority of students (i.e., when The Rejected and The Bullied 20 bullying is rare) may be most risky for the few who do experience bullying. Underlying Social Cognitive Processes To better understand the moderating effects of social comparisons (e.g., witnessing victimization) and contexts (e.g., classroom level of disorder), it is helpful to examine the social cognitive processes that may be involved in these episodes. That is, how do victims of bullying construe their plights? Our research shows that compared to their non-bullied peers, youth who view themselves as frequently bullied are more likely to blame themselves for incidents of peer intimidation (Graham & Juvonen, 2000). Furthermore, our research shows that characterological self-blame partly mediates the relation between self-perceived victim status and emotional distress (i.e., social anxiety and loneliness). In light of our findings regarding classroom social context and on witnessing others being bullied, we suspect that youth are most likely to blame themselves when there is little bullying in their classroom and when they do not notice others being bullied. Furthermore, we presume that such self-blaming tendencies account in part for bullied children’s reported distress and depression. Research on children’s self-perceptions or their awareness of their plight further helps us understand the rejection –rejection linkage. Research by Panak & Garber (1992) has shown that sociometrically rejected children (i.e., those whose peers avoid or dislike them) who were aware of their low status were more depressed than sociometrically rejected children who were unaware of their low status. Consistent with these findings, our research research also suggests that children who view themselves as victimized but without a victim reputation (“paranoids”) are more likely to feel lonely, socially anxious, and have lower self-esteem than others (including those who have a reputation of a victim but who do not perceive themselves as victimized; Graham & Juvonen, 2000; Graham, Bellmore & Juvonen, 2003). Hence, self-perceptions of The Rejected and The Bullied 21 one’s social plight are critical in determining whether rejected youth experience social distress. Immunity from Social Pain: The Case of Aggressive Outcasts Social-cognitive processes may advance our understanding of not only what accounts for the social pain experienced by rejected youth, but also why aggressive children appear not to be hurt by peer rejection. It is to this distinctive subgroup of social outcasts that we now turn. The case of aggressive children and adolescents is of theoretical interest and practical concern, given that unlike most rejected children (and withdrawn-rejected children in particular), aggressiverejected children and adolescents do not report social pain following rejection (e.g., Parkhurst & Asher, 1992; Renshaw & Brown, 1993). This finding implies that the group function of rejection might not “work” for aggressive youth, although evidence from longitudinal studies suggests that aggressive children are at risk for subsequent maladaptive functioning, albeit in the form of criminality rather than internalizing problems (e.g., Kupersmidt & Coie, 1990; Tremblay, Masse, Vitaro, & Dobkin, 1995). A number of explanations have been advanced to account for the absence of direct adverse psychological effects of peer rejection on aggressive children and adolescents. One set of explanations receiving considerable attention is that aggressive-rejected youth display a variety of self-protective (although often interpersonally problematic) social cognitive biases. Numerous studies have documented aggressive children and adolescents’ tendencies to display self-serving biases in evaluations of their own competencies and in their responsibility (or lack thereof) for problematic peer experiences (Dodge & Crick, 1990; Orobio de Castro, Veerman, Koops, Bosch, & Monshouwer, 2002; White et al., 2002). In a recent meta-analysis of research on aggressive behavior and hostile attributions, Orobio de Castro and colleagues (2002) found strong evidence for the tendency of aggressive children to perceive a peer’s ambiguously threatening behavior as The Rejected and The Bullied 22 intentionally provocative. This attributional bias may help to explain the aggressive child’s lack of emotional distress, inasmuch as blame directed at others is associated with anger and hostility (see Weiner, 1995) rather than with social anxiety or depression (Graham, Hudley, & Williams, 1992). Other types of biases in social information processing may also account for the lack of distress following peer rejection. Schippell, Vasey, Cravens-Brown and Bretveld (2003) identified attentional biases among adolescents identified as reactively aggressive: such youth displayed suppressed attention to rejection, ridicule and failure cues. Hence, the apparent protection enjoyed by aggressive-rejected youth may be in part explained by individual differences in social cognitive processing, including but not limited to attributional and attentional biases (see Dodge & Crick, 1990, for a discussion of additional processing biases and deficiencies in aggressive youth). Given our chief concern in the present review is to examine the role of the peer group in the individual’s experience of rejection, we now move from the well-charted realm of individual difference explanations to more recent explorations of social-contextual factors that may protect the feelings of aggressive youth. The first social contextual explanation we address is rooted in the fact that a peer rating of rejection is an indicator of peer sentiment, but not necessarily peer treatment of the individual. The harsh treatment and ostracism one would expect disliked individuals to experience may be reserved for targets not expected to retaliate. A rejected child’s aggressiveness appears to be a critical factor in the degree to which he or she receives negative peer feedback regarding his or her unacceptable behavior. When 10- to 13-year-old children of differing sociometric status documented their acute, everyday peer experiences (Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003), peer rejection, withdrawal and aggression each predicted more frequent daily The Rejected and The Bullied 23 negative peer experiences. However, withdrawn-rejected boys were significantly more likely than their aggressive-rejected counterparts to report negative peer treatment. According to Sandstrom and Cillessen, the peer context experienced by aggressive-rejected children may be a “kinder” one than might be expected on the basis of their sociometric status. If peer rejection serves to enforce group norms regarding interpersonal behavior, then the absence of negative, rejecting behaviors from peers toward aggressive children may constitute a breakdown of the norm enforcement process. The absence of negative feedback from peers may protect aggressive-rejected children from social distress but at the same time, they may also have little impetus to modify their behavior. Another protective factor in aggressive-rejected children’s social experience has been observed upon closer examination of their peer relationships. Recall that peer rejection is a measure of consensus of the general peer collective; the rejected individual may still experience belonging and social validation of their behavior by other deviant peers. Patterson, Capaldi and Bank (1991) suggested that rejection limits aggressive children’s options for healthy peer relationships, permitting them only to associate with and befriend similarly aggressive and rejected peers. Several studies of aggressive children’s friendships and social networks demonstrate the compensatory social functions of deviant friends. Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Gest, and Gariepy (1988) reported that aggressive fourth graders and seventh graders were no less likely than their non-aggressive peers to be socially connected. They were equally likely to be nominated as a best friend, to have reciprocal friendships, and to be perceived as a central member of a social cluster. An analysis of the aggressive children’s peers indicated, however, that they were also likely to be aggressive. These patterns have been observed in more recent research as well (e.g., The Rejected and The Bullied 24 Bagwell, Coie, Terry, & Lochman, 2000). Moreover, affiliation among deviant peers is known to facilitate “deviance training,” whereby anti-social youth encourage one another’s problematic behavior (Dishion, Spracklen, Andrews, & Patterson, 1996). It is therefore not surprising that anti-social (including aggressive) youth who repeatedly affiliate with similar others are at greater risk for subsequent criminal behavior (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999). As indicated earlier, research documents that aggressive youth and bullies are not only socially connected, but also enjoy popularity within their social networks (Juvonen, Graham & Schuster, 2003; Luthar & McMahon, 1996; Parkhurst & Hopmeyer, 1998; Rodkin, Farmer, Pearl, & Van Acken, 2000). For example, Juvonen et al. (2003) found that early adolescent groups identified as bullies based on peer nominations (of physical, verbal, and relational aggression) were considered “cooler” than other groups, including groups identified as socially well-adjusted. Although bullies were popular, they were also moderately rejected. At this point, we do not know whether teen bullies are rejected and perceived as “cool” by the same or different individuals. Given the work of Patterson and colleagues (1991), bullies’ rejected status may reflect the views of non-deviant peers, whereas their high social status may reflect the views of similarly aggressive individuals. Experimental work by White, Rubin and Graczyk (2002) provides support for this account. In their research, aggressive children and adolescents were less likely than their withdrawn and non-deviant peers to report negative affect toward or exclude an aggressive peer. It is also possible, however, that the same individuals who avoid the company of bullies may also view them as “cool.” In that case, coolness might indicate being a bully is a successful strategy to exert dominance. As argued by Hogg (in this volume), group leaders are not necessarily prototypical and “deviates” are often highly functional leaders who further engage in The Rejected and The Bullied 25 strategic marginalization of others. Hence, aggressive youth initially rejected from the normative (non-aggressive) peer group might nevertheless ultimately exert considerable leadership within the normative group because they are unaffected by the avoidance or dislike of their peers. This possibility is especially likely in adolescence. As discussed earlier, adolescence represents a unique phase in the lifespan in which aggressive and even criminal deviance elevates one’s social status (Moffitt, 1993). The question of why this occurs remains to be further investigated. Finally, we want to return to the question posed in the beginning of this chapter regarding school shooters who were allegedly bullied and rejected by their peers. The evidence reviewed thus far does not suggest that rejected children or victims of bullying lash out at their tormentors. Nor does our review imply that aggression results from rejection or bullying experiences. However, there is a growing body of research on yet another special group: aggressive or provocative victims (Olweus, 1993, 1993; Perry, Kusel, & Perry, 1988). This particular group of bully-victims shows a distinct profile of difficulties in school functioning and social-emotional problems. For example, Juvonen et al. (2003) found that among all identifiable sociometric groups, aggressive victims were rated by their teachers as most disruptive and disengaged from school. They were also by far the most rejected among peers, and showed somewhat elevated signs of emotional distress. Hence, they seemed to have the worst of both worlds: all the problems and little of the immunity. In their review of the existing research on aggressive victims, Schwartz, Proctor and Chien (2001) suggest that the particular behavioral profile of aggressive victims may be indicative of other underlying problems, such as emotion regulation problems typical of hyperactive children who have attention deficit disorders. Hence, the fact these youth are rejected by the group not only signals that they are deviant or that they do not fit in, but that there The Rejected and The Bullied 26 is an underlying problem that can explain their reactions to rejection and bullying experiences. This portrayal is probably most consistent with the depiction of the alleged school shooters in a recent federal report (Vossekuil, Fein, Reddy, Borum, & Modzeleski, 2002). This in-depth case analysis suggests that most of these young males displayed signs of aggression or violent ideation and two-thirds of them had been bullied or rejected by their peers. In addition, in most cases, the final impetus for the violent act involved a social failure, such as a rejection experience. Hence, the combination of aggressive tendencies and aversive social experiences appears to be especially volatile and problematic. Lessons Learned The critical issue in the case of rejected and bullied children is how repeated and aversive social experiences affect development. We began this paper by suggesting that repeated rejection and bullying experiences deprive children of normative peer interaction opportunities that adversely affect their development. To summarize some of the critical mediating mechanisms discussed above that might explain the links between rejection and subsequent adjustment, we now propose a general model of rejection that includes both person-level and group-level mediating mechanisms in Figure 1. Consistent with the present review, we propose that rejection is initially triggered by a lack of fit between the child and the group (path # 1). The subsequent processes involve both the target (the individual child) and source (the normative peer group) of the initial rejection. The upper section of path model depicts the personal or intrapsychological processes following the experiences of rejection, where as the lower part depicts the group level processes that capture the thoughts and actions of the rejectors. The Rejected and The Bullied 27 Consistent with social-cognitive or information processing models (e.g., Dodge & Crick, 1990; Crick & Dodge, 1999), we propose that rejection elicits negative expectancies and socialcognitive (attentional as well as attributional) biases in both the rejected and their rejectors. These social-cognitive biases in turn account for the subsequent behavioral reactions. Focusing first on the lower level of the figure, rejected children acquire social reputations that function as cognitive schemas biasing the reactions against the rejected (path # 2). The best demonstrations of such biases was provided by Hymel (1986) who showed how children interpret the identical actions by liked and disliked peers in strikingly different ways. Such affective biases then in turn make it more likely that these same children continue to get excluded and ostracized (path # 3). At the person or intrapsychological level, rejection experiences can foster different type of social-cognitive biases (path # 4) depending on individual differences that promote the very behaviors that elicited rejection in the first place. In the case of most children, and submissive and socially withdrawn children in particular, tendency to blame oneself, specifically characterological self-blame, increases feelings of depression, anxiety, and depression, which in turn promote avoidance or withdrawal from peer interactions. In the case of aggressive children, inferences of hostile intent of the actions by others further promote their anger and retaliation. Hence, the social-cognitive biases are likely to promote the very behavior patterns (e.g., social withdrawal and aggression) that elicited the rejection in the first place (path # 5). As depicted in Figure 1, we propose that such perceptions mediate the relation between the initial eliciting behavior and accentuation of these same behavior patterns. Research on the experimental manipulation of interpersonal expectations of rejected children (Rabiner & Coie, 1989) suggest that negative expectations might also maintain their rejected status. This pathway is not necessarily mediated by changes in overt behaviors, The Rejected and The Bullied 28 inasmuch as Rabiner and Coie could not detect any behavioral indicators of improved social status when rejected youth were led to believe that others liked them. (Alternatively, they may be very subtle behaviors that they are difficult to detect.) Nevertheless, in light of the available data, we propose that the person-level construct tapping expectations predicts group-level processes by increasing the chances that peers reject the target repeatedly (path # 6). Repeated rejection on the part of the peer group, in turn, is likely to accentuate the initial behavioral responses that elicited rejection in the first place. However, such a pathway is likely to be bi-directional (path # 7), inasmuch as increased social withdrawal and aggression in turn are associated with prolonged experiences of rejection. Repeated rejection by normative peer group, in turn, restricts children’s opportunities for normative peer interactions (path # 8). Also, accentuated eliciting behaviors are bidirectionally linked to restricted opportunities to affiliate with normative peer group (path # 9). We reviewed evidence showing how increased withdrawal means that children end up playing alone and ceasing to interact therefore restricting their opportunities to engage in normative peer interactions. In the case of aggressive youth, we documented that their opportunities are restricted in the sense that they seek the company of other aggressive children. Such affiliations are likely to promote “deviance training” (Dishion, et a., 1996) among the aggressive youth that is likely to result in increased behavior problems (Dishion et al., 1999). Restricted opportunities to affiliate with the normative peer group result in lack of “social practice” for the rejected children (path # 10), which in turn predicts maladaptive functioning of these youth (path #11). In the case of socially withdrawn youth, they are at risk for internalizing problems, such as depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem, whereas aggressive youth are at risk for antisocial behaviors (McDougall et al., 2001). The Rejected and The Bullied In summary, the proposed developmental model depicts an array of interpersonal and intrapersonal processes that appear to conspire to propel the social outcast toward increased deviance and maladaptive functioning among normative peers, although not necessarily away from all relationships. Our goal was to present a general model that applies to most rejected or bullied children, regardless of individual differences that have received the most attention in developmental research. Finally, the set of predictions and associations depicted in this model reflect our review of the literature. Thus, it is possible that other set of linkages and more complex relations can added to this basic model. How can such a model or the developmental research in general then help us understand some of the issues currently debated among social psychologists? Contributions of the Developmental to the Social Research To be added in response to other chapters in the volume. 29 The Rejected and The Bullied 30 References Allen, J.P., Weissberg, R.P., & Hawkins, J.A. (1989). The relation between values and social competence in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 25, 458-464. Asher, S.R., Rose, A.J., & Gabriel, S.W. (2001). Peer rejection in everyday life. In Leary, Mark R. (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 105-142). London: Oxford Press. Asher, S. R., & Wheeler, V. A. (1985). Children's loneliness: A comparison of rejected and neglected peer status. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 53, 500-505. Bagwell, C.L., Coie, J.D., Terry, R.A., & Lochman, J.E. (2000). Peer clique participation and social status in preadolescence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 280-305. Bellmore, A., D., Witkow, M. R., Graham, S. & Juvonen, J. (in press). From beliefs to behavior: The mediating role of hostile response selection in predicting aggression. Aggressive Behavior. Bellmore, A., Witkow, M., Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (in press). Beyond the individual: The impact of ethnic context and classroom behavioral norms on victims' adjustment. Berndt, T.J. (1996). Friendships in adolescence. In Vanzetti, Nelly & Duck, Steve (Eds.), A lifetime of relationships (pp. 181-212). Belmont, CA, US: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co. Boivin, M., Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1995). Individual-group behavioral similarity and peer status in experimental play groups of boys: The social misfit revisited. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 69, 269-279. Boivin, M., Hymel, S., & Bukowski, W.M. (1995). The roles of social withdrawal, peer rejection, and victimization by peers in predicting loneliness and depressed mood in childhood. Development & Psychopathology. Special Issue: Developmental processes in peer relations and psychopathology, 7, 765-785 The Rejected and The Bullied 31 Bryk, A.S., & Raudenbush, S.W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Burks, V.S., Dodge, K.A., & Price, J.M. (1995). Models of internalizing outcomes of early rejection. Development & Psychopathology. Special Issue: Developmental processes in peer relations and psychopathology, 7, 683-695 Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of youth in our time. New York: Cambridge University Press. Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., & Neckerman, H. J. (1989). Early school dropout: Configurations and determinants. Child Development, 60, 1437-1452. Cairns, R.B., Cairns, B.D., Neckerman, H.J., Gest, S.D., & Gariepy (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer support or peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24, 815-823. Chen, X., Chang, L., & He, Y. (2003). The peer group as a context: Mediating and moderating effects on relations between academic achievement and social functioning in Chinese children. Child Development, 74, 710-727. Cillessen, A.H.N. & Bukowski, W.M., Eds. (2000). Recent advances in the measurement of acceptance and rejection in the peer system. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 88. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Cillessen, A.H.N., Bukowski, W.M., & Haselager, G.J.T. (2000). Stability of sociometric categories. In Cillessen, A.H.N. & Bukowski, W.M. (Eds.), Recent advances in the measurement of acceptance and rejection in the peer system. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 88 (pp. 75-94). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Clifford, E. (1963). Social visibility. Child Development, 34, 799-808. The Rejected and The Bullied 32 Coie, J. D., & Cillessen, A. H. (1993). "Peer rejection: Origins and effects on children's development." Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 89-92. Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990). Peer group behavior and social status. In S. R. Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood. Cambridge studies in social and emotional development (pp. 17-59). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Coie, J.D., & Kupersmidt, J.B. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerging social status in boys’ groups. Child Development, 54, 1400-1416. Coie, J.D., & Pennington, B.F. (1976). Children's perceptions of deviance and disorder. Child Development, 47, 407-413 Coie, J.D., Terry, R., Lenox, K., & Lochman, J. (1995). Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable patterns of adolescent disorder. Development & Psychopathology. Special Issue: Developmental processes in peer relations and psychopathology, 7, 697-713. Crick, N. & Dodge, K. (1994). A Review and Reformulation of Social InformationProcessing Mechanisms in Children’s Social Adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101. Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and socialpsychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710-722. Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1990). Children's perceptions of the outcomes of social strategies: Do the ends justify being mean? Developmental Psychology, 26, 612-620. Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Children's perceptions of their peer experiences: Attributions, loneliness, social anxiety, and social avoidance. Developmental Psychology, 29, 244-254. The Rejected and The Bullied 33 Deater-Deckard, K. (2001). Annotation: Recent Research Examining the Role of Peer Relationships in the Development of Psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 565-579. DeRosier, M. E., Kupersmidt, J.B., & Patterson, C.J. (1994). Children's academic and behavioral adjustment as a function of the chronicity and proximity of peer rejection. Child Development, 65, 1799-1813. Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 755-764. Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D. W., & Patterson, G. R. (1996). Deviancy training in male adolescent friendships. Behavior Therapy, 27, 373-390. Dodge, K. A., & Crick, N. R. (1990). Social information-processing bases of aggressive behavior in children. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. Special Issue: Illustrating the value of basic research, 16, 8-22. Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162-170. Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Pettit, G. S., & Price, J. M. (1990). Peer status and aggression in boys' groups: Developmental and contextual analyses. Child Development, 61, 1289-1309. Eder, D.(1995). School talk: Gender and adolescent school culture. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. g. (1998). Does low self-regard invite victimization? Developmental Psychology, 34, 299-309. The Rejected and The Bullied 34 French, D.C., Jansen, E.A., & Pidada, S. (2002). United States and Indonesian children's and adolescents' reports of relational aggression by disliked peers. Child Development, 73, 1143-1150. Graham, S., Hydley, C, & William, E (1992). Attributional and emotional determinants of aggression among African American and Latino early adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 31, 28, 731-740. Hallinan, M. T. (1979). Structural effects on children's friendships and cliques. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 43-54. Hartup, W.W. (1996). The company they keep: Friendships and their developmental significance. Child Development, 67, 1-13. Hawker, D. S. J., & Boulton, M.J. (2000). Twenty years’ research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 441-455. Hawkins, J. D., & Lam, T. (1987). Teacher practices, social development, and delinquency. In J. D. Burchard & S. N. Burchard (Eds.), Prevention of delinquent behavior (pp. 241-247). Newbury Park,CA: Sage. Hodges, E. V. E. & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and consequences of victimization by peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677685. Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999). The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94-101. The Rejected and The Bullied 35 Hodges, E. V. E., Malone, M. J., & Perry, D. G. (1997). Individual risk and social risk as interacting determinants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1032-1039. Hogg, M. (this volume). All animals are equal but some animals are more equal than others: Social identity and marginal membership. Hope, T. L., & Bierman, K. L. (1998). Patterns of home and school behavior problems in rural and urban settings. Journal of School Psychology. Special Issue: School Violence, 36, 4558. Hymel, S. (1986). Interpretations of peer behavior: Affective bias in childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 57, 431-445. Hymel, S., Vaillancourt, T., McDougall, P., & Renshaw, P. D. (2002). Peer acceptance and rejection in childhood. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development. Blackwell handbooks of developmental psychology (pp. 265-284). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Hymel, S., Wagner, E., & Butler, L. J. (1990). Reputational bias: View from the peer group. Cambridge studies in social and emotional development [Peer rejection in childhood], 156-186. Janes, L.M, & Olson, J.M. (2000). Jeer pressure: Effects of observing ridicule of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 474-485. Juvonen, J. (1991). Deviance, perceived responsibility, and negative peer reactions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 672-681. Juvonen, J. & Cadigan, J. (2002). Social determinants of public behavior of middle school youth: Perceived peer norms and need to be accepted. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan, (Eds.), The Rejected and The Bullied 36 Adolescence and Education, Vol. 2: Academic motivation of adolescents (pp.277-297). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2000). Peer harassment, psychological adjustment, and school functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 349-359. Killen, M., Crystal, D. S., & Watanabe, H. (2002). Japanese and American children's evaluations of peer exclusion, tolerance of differences, and prescriptions for conformity. Child Development, 73, 1788-1802. Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). Victimized children's responses to peers' aggression: Behaviors associated with reduced versus continued victimization. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 59-73. Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Variations in peer victimization: Relations to children's maladjustment. In J. Juvonen, & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 25-48). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kochenderfer-Ladd, B. & Wardrop, J.L. (2001). Chronicity and instability of children's peer victimization experiences as predictors of loneliness and social satisfaction trajectories. Child Development, 72, 134-151. LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2002). Children's perceptions of popular and unpopular peers: A multimethod assessment. Developmental Psychology, 38, 635-647. Leadbeater, B., Hoglund, W., & Woods, T. (2003). Changing contents? The effects of a primary prevention program on classroom levels of peer relational and physical victimization. Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 397-418. The Rejected and The Bullied 37 Lease, A. M., & Axelrod, J. L. (2001). Position of the peer group's perceived organizational structure: Relation to social status and friendship. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 377-404. Luthar, S. S., & McMahon, T. J. (1996). Peer reputation among inner-city adolescents: Structure and correlates. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 581-603. McDougall, P., Hymel, S., Vaillancourt, T., & Mercer, L. (2002). The consequences of childhood peer rejection. In M. Leary (Ed), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 213-247). London: Oxford University Press. Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674-701. Newcomb, A.F., Bukowski, W.M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children's peer relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 99-128. Nishina, A., & Juvonen, J. (submitted for publication). Daily Reports of Witnessing and Experiencing Peer Harassment in Middle School. Nishina, A., Juvonen, J., & Witkow, M. (submitted for publication). Sticks and Stones May Break My Bones, But Names Will Make Me Sick: The Consequences of Peer Harassment. Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73, 916-934. Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the schools: Bullies and whipping boys. Oxford, England: Hemisphere. The Rejected and The Bullied 38 Panak, W. F., & Garber, J. (1992). Role of aggression, rejection, and attributions in the prediction of depression in children. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 145-165. Parker, J.G., & Asher, S.R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357-389. Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle school: Subgroup differences in behavior, loneliness, and interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 231-241. Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and peer-perceived popularity: Two distinct dimensions of peer status. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 125-144. Patterson, G.R., Capaldi, D., & Bank, L. (1991). An early starter model for predicting delinquency. In Pepler, Debra J. & Rubin, Kenneth H. (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 139-168). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Peevers, B. H., & Secord, P. F. (1973). Developmental changes in attribution of descriptive concepts to persons. Pepler, D. J., Craig, W. M., & Roberts, W. L. (1998). Observations of aggressive and nonaggressive children on the school playground. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 55-76. Rabiner, D., & Coie, J. (1989). Effect of expectancy inductions on rejected children's acceptance by unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 450-457. Rodkin, P.C., Farmer, T.W., Pearl, R., & Van Acken, R. (2000). Heterogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial configurations. Developmental Psychology, 36, 14-24. Rubin, K.H., LeMare, L.J., & Lollis, S. (1990). Social withdrawal in childhood: Developmental pathways to peer rejection. In Asher, Steven R. & Coie, John D. (Eds.), Peer The Rejected and The Bullied 39 rejection in childhood. Cambridge studies in social and emotional development (pp. 217-249). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. Sandstrom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2003). Sociometric Status and Children's Peer Experiences: Use of the Daily Diary Method. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 427-452. Sandstrom, M. J., & Cramer, P. (2003). Girls use of defense mechanisms following peer rejection. Journal of Personality, 71, 605-627. Schippell, P.L., Vasey, M.W., Cravens-Brown, L.M., and Bretveld, R.A. (2003). Suppressed attention to rejection, ridicule, and failure cues: A unique correlate of reactive but not proactive aggression in youth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 32, 40-55. Schmitt, M. T., Silvia, P. J., & Branscombe, N. R. (2000). The intersection of selfevaluation maintenance and social identity theories: Intragroup judgment in interpersonal and intergroup contexts. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1598-1606. Schwartz, D., Dodge, K.A., & Coie, J.D. (1993). The emergence of chronic peer vicitimization in boys' play groups. Child Development, 64, 1755-1772. Schwartz, D., Farver, J., & Chang, L. (2000). Correlates of victimization in Chinese children’s peer groups. Harassment Across Diverse Contexts. Stormshak, E. A., Bierman, K. L., Bruschi, C., Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1999). The relation between behavior problems and peer preference in different classroom contexts. Child Development, 70, 169-182. Tomada, G., & Schneider, B.H., (1997). Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psychology, 33, 601-609. The Rejected and The Bullied 40 Tremblay, R.E., Masse, L.C., Vitaro, F. & Dobkin, P.L. (1995). The impact of friends' deviant behavior on early onset of deliquency: Longitudinal data from 6 to 13 years of age. Development & Psychopathology. Special Issue: Developmental processes in peer relations and psychopathology, 7, 649-667. Vernberg, E. M., Abwender, D.A., Ewell, K.K., & Beery, S. H. (1992). Social anxiety and peer relationships in early adolescence: A prospective analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 189-196. Vossekuil, B., Fein, R., Reddy, M., Borum, R., & Modzeleski, W. (2002). The final report and findings of the safe school initiative: Implications for prevention of school attacks in the United States. US Department of Education, Office of Elementary and Secondary Educations, Safe and Drug-Free Schools Program, and US Secret Service. Washington D.C. Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. White, K.J., Rubin, E.C., & Graczyk, P.A. (2002). Aggressive children's perceptions of behaviorally similar peers: The influence of one's own behavioral characteristics on perceptions of deviant peers. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships, 19, 755-775. White, K. J., Sherman, M. D., & Jones, K. (1996). Children's perceptions of behavior problem peers: Effects of teacher feedback and peer-reputed status. Journal of School Psychology, 34, 53-72. Wood, J. J., Cowan, P. A., & Baker, B. L. (2002). Behavior problems and peer rejection in preschool boys and girls. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163, 72-88. The Rejected and The Bullied 41 Wright, J. C., Giammarino, M., & Parad, H. W. (1986). Social status in small groups: Individual-group similarity and the social "misfit." Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50, 523-536. Younger, A., Gentile, C., & Burgess, K. (1993). Children's perceptions of social withdrawal: Changes across age. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 215-235). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Zeller, M., Vannatta, K., Schafer, J., & Noll, R. B. (2003). Behavioral reputation: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 39, 129-139. The Rejected and The Bullied 42 Appendix Our review was based on the 145 studies published between 1993 and 2003 in Child Development, Developmental Psychology, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology and Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin that were found in a PsycINFO search on the following six keywords: rejection, ostracism, exclusion, harassment, victimization and bullying. Each of thirteen characteristics were coded independent of the others; that is, an article could include both experimental and non-experimental data, or rejection by both strangers and familiar others. Table 1A. Comparison of Characteristics of Social and Developmental Investigations, 1993-2003 % of Articles in Developmental Psychology Journals (n = 74) % of Articles in Social Psychology Journals (n = 71) χ2 89% 26% 45% 75% 29.56** 36.76** 78% 19% 1% 18% 51% 18% 52.32** 16.20** 12.33** 12% 44% 16.24** Source of Rejection Strangers Familiar others Individual Group 4% 96% 16% 89% 48% 55% 73% 34% 33.81** 33.29**‡ 52.59**‡ 47.38**‡ Research Design Experimental Non-experimental Longitudinal 20% 81% 45% 62% 49% 21% 26.11** 16.20** 8.89* Characteristic Chronicity of Rejection Chronic Discrete Determination of Rejected Status Other-report Self-report Rejection involving no direct interpersonal interaction with source Experimental manipulation or behavioral coding only Notes: Unless otherwise noted, χ2 statistics reflect Fisher’s Exact test comparing articles with and without each characteristic in the two fields; comparisons that include a third category “unspecified,” used a standard two-tailed χ2 test, and are noted by “‡”; * = p <.01; ** = p < .001. The Rejected and The Bullied Figure Caption Figure 1. Proposed developmental model of intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of peer rejection. 43 The Rejected and The Bullied 44 PERSON-LEVEL PROCESSES Negative Expectancies & Attributional Biases 4 5 Accentuation of Eliciting Behaviors Lack of social “practice” 1 Lack of PersonGroup Fit 6 Peer Rejection 2 GROUP-LEVEL PROCESSES Negative Reputation Among Peers: Expectancies & Biases 7 3 9 Repeated Rejection Across Situations 8 10 Restricted Affiliation with Normative Peer Group 11 Maladaptive Functioning