* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download področja: posameznik in travma
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK PREVALENCA TRAVME In a recent review of studies investigating civilian-related trauma and PTSD, Resnick, Falsetti, Kilpatrick, and Freedy (1995) reported that lifetime exposure to a variety of traumatic events is relatively common (40-70%), with prevalence rates of PTSD ranging from 18% to 28% for individuals exposed to some type of civilian trauma. In the largest study on the prevalence of traumatic events and PTSD conducted to date (The National Cormorbidity Survey), Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, and Nelson (1995) surveyed a representative U.S. sample of 8,098 individuals 15 to 54 years of age and found that 60% of men and 51% of women reported experiencing at least one traumatic event in their lifetime, with 8% of the sample estimated to have a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD. Several studies have examined prevalence of traumatic events among college students. These studies report that exposure to traumatic events among college students also is relatively common. Vrana and Lauterbach (1994) found that 84% of college students reported experiencing at least one traumatic event during their lives, and over one third experienced four or more lifetime traumatic events. Green (1995) found a similarly high rate (>90%) of lifetime exposure to at least one traumatic event among members of a college sample. However, in both studies of college students, no attempts were made to examine rates of PTSD, and limited attempts were made to examine predictors of PTSD symptomatology. The current study is the largest to date to examine the prevalence of lifetime traumatic events and PTSD symptoms in a college sample. These results indicate that having experienced one or more traumatic events is common among college students. Consistent with previous data (e.g., Green, 1995; Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994), approximately 67% of participants reported experiencing at least one traumatic event during their lifetime. Differential reporting rates in the experience of traumatic events were evident, with men more likely to have been in more serious accidents, to have been physically assaulted, and to have witnessed more serious injury or death. Women reported a greater proportion than men of adolescent and adult experiences of sexual coercion and sexual assault. These results are consistent with the findings of Norris (1992), who studied the prevalence of traumatic events among a community sample of adults. She found that women were more likely to have been sexually assaulted, but men were more likely to have been in motor vehicle crashes, to have been nonsexually physically assaulted, or to have experienced some violent event. In the present study, the high prevalence rates of childhood sexual abuse and adolescent and adult sexual assault, particularly among women, are consistent with previous research with college (e.g., Gidycz, Coble, Latham, & Layman, 1993) and community samples (e.g., Finkelhor, Hoatling, Lewis, & Smith, 1990; Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993). The finding that a significant proportion of individuals (12% of traumatized respondents; 4% of the full sample) met PTSD criteria within the past week is also important given that this is a college sample. Although it might be argued that this estimate overrepresents the true prevalence of PTSD diagnosis because of the use of self-report methodology, this rate is very similar to estimates obtained in large-scale epidemiological studies that have assessed PTSD diagnosis with structured clinical interviews. For example, Resnick, Kilpatrick, Dansky, Saunders, and Best (1993) studied a national probability sample of 4,008 women, using a comprehensive traumatic event inventory (similar to the one used in this study) and PTSD interview format. The researchers estimated that 7% of women with a history of any type of traumatic event (5% of the full sample) met criteria for current PTSD diagnosis. In terms of exposure to violence, Jenkins (2001) found in a literature review of selected studies that between 26% and 70% of inner city children have been exposed to severe violence, such as witnessing a shooting. Bryant-Davis, T. (2005). Coping Strategies of African American Adult Survivors of Childhood Violence. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36(4), 409–414. In a study conducted 52 years after the Dresden bombing, we explored pathogenetic and salutogenetic aspects of the psychological consequences of the bombing night trauma in a population now aged between 57 and 95. The assumption guiding the investigation was that, although a traumatic incident like the Dresden bombing has pathological long-term aftereffects, the processing of the traumatic experiences may also contribute to personal growth (cf. Antonovsky, 1987; Frankl, 1973). Whereas pathogenetic factors contribute to the symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), salutogenetic factors are associated with the successful processing of the trauma and a nonsymptomatic and/or a positive outcome in terms of personal growth. Maercker, A. in Herrle, J. (2003). Long-Term Effects of the Dresden Bombing: Relationships to Control Beliefs, Religious Belief, and Personal Growth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(6), 579-587. Accidents, suicides, and homicides are the three leading causes of death among young people in the United States (U.S. Bureau of Census, 1999). (The Prevalence of PTSD Following the Violent Death of a Child and Predictors of Change 5 Years Later, Shirley A. Murphy,1;3 L. Clark Johnson,1 Ick-Joong Chung,2 and Randal D. Beaton1 Journal of Traumatic Stress, Vol. 16, No. 1, February 2003, pp. 17–25 ( C ° 2003)) PREVALENCA TRAVME – RAZLIKE MED SPOLOMA Gender is one potentially very powerful risk factor for victimization. In general, men are more likely to be exposed towar combat, nonsexual assaults between strangers, and to be victimized in public places (Craven, 1997; U.S. Census Bureau, 2003), whereas women are more likely to be sexually abused, injured by an intimate partner, and victimized in a private home (Craven, 1997; Finkelhor, 1994; Straus, 2001). A recent community survey revealed a number of gender differences in exposure to various kinds of trauma (Goldberg & Freyd, under review). Women were much more likely to report having been emotionally or psychologically mistreated by someone close as adults (approximately 40% compared to less than 12% of men) and as children (approximately 30% compared to less than 14%). Women also reported more sexual abuse in adulthood and in childhood than did men. However, men were much more likely to report having witnessed someone who they were not close to being killed, committing suicide, or being injured, in adulthood and childhood. Overall, women reported more events involving someone close to them, and men reported more events that did not involve other people, and events involving others who were not close to them. These data suggest that victims of betrayal-related events are more likely to be women than men, whereas victims of nonbetrayal events are more likely to be men. Exposure to different types of trauma may be one form of gender-based socialization that affects a range of psychological, social, and physical health outcomes (DePrince & Freyd, 2002; Freyd, 1999). Boys and girls reported anger as their primary reason for violence, however girls were more likely to report using violence as selfdefense, whereas boys reported using violence to exert control over their dating partner. DATING VIOLENCE AMONG ADOLESCENTS Prevalence, Gender Distribution, and Prevention Program Effectiveness LAURA J. HICKMAN, LISA H. JAYCOX, RAND Corporation. JESSICA ARONOFF, Break the Cycle TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 5, No. 2, April 2004 123-142 More boys reported perpetrating sexual abuse than girls (37% and 24%, respectively) and more girls reported perpetrating physical abuse than boys (28% and 11%, respectively). DATING VIOLENCE AMONG ADOLESCENTS Prevalence, Gender Distribution, and Prevention Program Effectiveness LAURA J. HICKMAN, LISA H. JAYCOX, RAND Corporation. JESSICA ARONOFF, Break the Cycle TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 5, No. 2, April 2004 123-142 PREVALENCA TRAVME – SPOLNA ZLORABA IN POSILSTVO Women in the CSA group experienced a first episode of abuse at a mean age of 8.8 years (SD = 3.9); Table 2 describes ages and types of abuse. More than one third of the women were victims of two or more perpetrators, and 58% were victims of incest at some point in their childhood. The majority of women were first abused by an unrelated male, a category that includes people who were familiar to the victim (e.g., mother’s boyfriend) and people who were unfamiliar to the victim (e.g., a stranger). Most victims were 15 or more years younger than their perpetrator. The duration of abuse was generally either a single incident or more than 2 years, and nearly one half of the women experienced penetration. Gorey and Leslie (1997) reviewed surveys involving North American community samples and estimated that 15% of women and 7% of men had experienced contact sexual abuse when they were children. Rates in Europe, Latin America, Africa, Australia, and New Zealand appear comparable to those in North America (Fergusson, Lynskey, &Horwood, 1996a; Finkelhor, 1994). More than one half of all respondents reported having been previously sexually assaulted (see Humphrey & White, 2000), with most victims being assaulted by people they knew, providing additional evidence to discount the stereotype of sexual assault being commonly committed by so called strangers (Crime Victims Research Treatment Center, 1992; Fieldhaus, Houry, & Kaminsky, 2000). Many women experience sexual assault at some time in their lives. Prevalence studies suggest that one third of women (32% to 34%) have experiencedchildhood sexual abuse (CSA; Vogeltanz et al., 1999; Wyatt, Loeb, Solis, Carmona, & Romero, 1999), whereas the prevalence of rape in adult women ranges from 14% to 25% (Koss, 1993). Humphrey and White (2000) found that 50% of college-bound women experience sexual assault prior to college, with 24% to 31% reporting sexual assault during each year of college. Women with victimization histories are at elevated risk of revictimization. Across several studies, 44% of survivors of CSA experienced adolescent or adult sexual assault (Breitenbecher, 2001). Merrill et al. (1999) found that survivors of CSA are 4.8 times as likely to experience adult victimization, and Humphrey and White (2000) found that women who experienced adolescent victimization were 14 times as likely to experience victimization during college. The Rape Abuse and Incest National Network (1998) reported that one woman is raped every 2 minutes in the United States. Estimates of the prevalence of child sexual abuse (CSA) in the general female population range from 15% to 33% (for review, see Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). Approximately 20% of women are victims of rape each year (Russell, 1983; Wyatt, 1985), and it is estimated that between 25% (Straus & Gelles, 1990) and 50% (Stark & Flitcraft, 1988) of women are physically abused by their husbands. The Revictimization of Child Sexual Abuse Survivors: An Examination of the Adjustment of College Women With Child Sexual Abuse, Adult Sexual Assault, and Adult Physical Abuse , CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2000) Partner abuse against women is a serious public health problem in the United States. Approximately 4.4 million women are estimated to suffer from partner abuse each year (Misra, 2001). Russell’s (1983, 1984) landmark study of community women in San Francisco revealed that 24% of women had experienced a completed rape and 44% had experienced a completed or attempted rape. Koss and her colleagues conducted a national random survey of college women and found that 1 in 4 women had experienced rape or attempted rape in their lifetimes and 84% of the women knew their attacker (Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski, 1987). Rape was not rare, and it was not primarily a stranger-in-the-bushes phenomenon. Itwas a violent crime committed against millions ofwomen by men they knewand trusted. (REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) Over the past two decades, the study of CSAwithin the family and its sequelae has been recognized as a significant issue for many women. Estimates vary as to its prevalence, ranging from about one third (31%) (Russell, 1983) (33%) (Wheeler & Walton, 1987) to 16% (Russell, 1983, 1984; Sedney & Brooks, 1984) Duration. The average duration of the CSA for the mutilators was almost double that of the nonmutilators. With a range of duration from less than 1 year to 22 years, the mutilators’ CSA began at the mean age of 6.06 years (SD = 3.91) and stopped at the mean age of 13.33 years (SD = 5.87). For the nonmutilators, the CSA started at the mean age of 7.64 years (SD = 4.30), and the mean age when it ended was 11.66 (SD = 6.36). Duration ranged from less than 1 year to 34 years. Duration of the CSAappeared to differentiate between the two groups and was included as a potential variable in the model-building phase of the analysis. Childhood sexual abuse (CSA) occurs in the lives of about 1 in 4 girls (Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis,&Smith, 1990; Russell, 1983). Thirteen percent of the 4,008 women surveyed in the National Women’s Study (National Victim Center and Crime Victims Research and Treatment Center, 1992) reported having experienced at least one completed, forcible rape in their lifetime. The investigators estimate that 638,000 American women were raped in the year before the survey was conducted. Rape was defined in the study’s preface as “an event that occurred without the woman’s consent, involved the use of force or threat of force, and involved sexual penetration of the victim’s vagina, mouth, or rectum.” Sexual assaults that did not involve force, threat of force, or penetration were not included in the prevalence and incidence estimates. (Women’s Responses to Sexual Violence by Male Intimates Claire Burke Draucker Phyllis Noerager Stern Western Journal of Nursing Research, 2000, 22(4), 385-406) Russell’s (1983, 1984) landmark study of community women in San Francisco revealed that 24% of women had experienced a completed rape and 44% had experienced a completed or attempted rape. Koss and her colleagues conducted a national random survey of college women and found that 1 in 4 women had experienced rape or attempted rape in their lifetimes and 84% of the women knew their attacker (Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski, 1987). Rape was not rare, and it was not primarily a stranger-in-the-bushes phenomenon. Itwas a violent crime committed against millions ofwomen by men they knewand trusted. Comparable prevalence rates have been obtained by multiple independent research teams, and 20 years later it is still clear that sexual assault is far too prevalent. (Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault 20 Years of Progress and Future Directions REBECCA CAMPBELL,SHARON M. WASCO JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) Despite prevalence estimates that 15% of males in the United States and Canada have a history of child sexual abuse (CSA) (Bagley,Wood,&Young, 1994; Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, & Smith, 1990), research on male victims of CSA continues to lag behind that research on female victims (KendallTackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). Definitions of masculinity tend not to allow expression of the fear, vulnerability, and helplessness that accompany the experience of sexual abuse (Dimock, 1988; Hunter, 1991; Lisak, 1994; Nasjleti, 1980). Agrowing body of literature has documented that the sequelae of CSA for male victims are much the same as for female victims (see reviews in Finkelhor, 1990; Urquiza & Capra, 1990). Data from the National Child Abuse and Neglect Data System indicate that 2.8 per 1,000women experienced childhood physical abuse and 2.3 per 1,000 experienced childhood sexual abuse in 1998 (United States Department of Health and Human Services, 2000). In terms of prevalence, national data reveal that approximately one and one half million children have experienced physical abuse (Straus & Gelles, 1990) and almost one half million children are sexually abused prior to age 18 (Sedlak, 1991). ALTHOUGHTHETRUEPREVALENCEof sexual abuse is unknown, research suggests that a significant proportion ofwomenin today’s society have experienced this trauma (e.g., Ganley, 1981, 1989; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1993; Pence & Paymar, 1986). In general medical practice, sex therapy, and institutional psychiatry, it is estimated that 25%, 50%, and 51% of women, respectively, report a history of sexual abuse (Craine, Hensen, Colliver, & MacLean, 1988). PREVALENCA TRAVME – DOMAČE NASILJE Domestic violence has reached epidemic proportions in the United States. Each year at least 4 million women are victimized, 2 million suffer serious injury, and 3,000 women suffer fatal injuries (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000). Underscoring the seriousness of domestic violence is that 30% of women killed in the United States die at the hands of a husband or boyfriend (Russell, 1995). Studies estimate that between 33% and 50% of women in the general population are physically abused by their husbands, ex-husbands, or live-in partners during their lifetime (Canadian Abilities Foundation, 2002; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000). Annually, approximately 1.5%, or 4.8 million, women are raped or physically assaulted by an intimate partner in the United States (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000). (MRUGAYA W. GORDE, CHRISTINE A. HELFRICH, MARCIA L. FINLAYSON Trauma Symptoms and Life Skill Needs of Domestic Violence Victims, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 19 No. 6, June 2004 691-708) Violence against women and children is a serious problem in this country. Estimates of the prevalence of child sexual abuse (CSA) in the general female population range from 15% to 33% (Kendall-Tackett,Williams,&Finkelhor, 1993). The national crime victim survey found that one out of eight women are victims of rape during their lifetime (National Victim Center, 1992), and Koss (1993) estimated rape and sexual assault prevalence among adult women to be between 15% and 25%. Approximately 25% (Straus & Gelles, 1990) to 50% (Stark & Flitcraft, 1988) of women are physically battered by their husbands. Women who have been raped or battered may experience a wide range of effects that may manifest in symptoms of depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and sexual dysfunction (Goodman, Koss,&Russo, 1993). The psychological sequelae ofCSAare similar and may persist into adulthood (Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993; Polusny&Follette, 1995). Moreover, in a recently studied community sample of close to 10,000 individuals, more than 26% reported having been victims of childhood physical violence, whereas only approximately 8% reported having experienced childhood sexual abuse (MacMillan et al., 1997). PREVALENCA TRAVME – ZANEMARJANJE However, as has been noted frequently, research on neglect is only a small fraction of research on child maltreatment (National Research Council, 1993). Cross-cultural comparative studies of neglect are even more rare. There are grounds for believing that neglect is as detrimental or more detrimental than physical or sexual abuse. Neglect, especially of the child’s emotional needs for love and support, may be the form of maltreatment with the greatest risk of serious social and psychological problems to children (Bowlby, 1982; Robbins, 1966; Spitz, 1959). PREVALENCA TRAVME – SMRT BLIŽNJEGA However, due to the limited availability of national data, it remains unclear how prevalent deaths of family members and friends are among adolescents. Perhaps most important, prevalence data for the current study oˇer information on experience of loss in a nationally representative sample of adolescents. Results of this study indicate that adolescents’ exposure to death is high, with one in three adolescents reporting the death of a family member in the past year and one in ˘ve adolescents reporting the past-year death of a close friend. Several demographic factorswere associated with higher prevalence of past-year death of a familymember.Girlsweremore likely thanboys to report experiencing the death of a family member in the past year. In addition, adolescents with lower household incomes were more likely than adolescents with higher household incomes to report the past-year death of a family member. PREDIKTORJI PREDIKTORJI – SPOLNA ZLORABA, POSILSTVO Rape victims with a history of child sexual abuse were found to have higher levels of trauma symptoms, made greater use of nervous and cognitive coping strategies, and were more likely to make attributions of blame towards themselves or society. Current symptoms were related to types of coping and attributions of blame, with history of child sexual abuse having an indirect relationship to these variables. The results suggest the importance of attributional and coping variables, as well as child sexual abuse history, as mediators of postrape adjustment. Sex role socialization theory proposes that there are developmental processes by which individuals learn what is appropriate for their gender. Through these processes, both women and men form expectations concerning acceptable behaviors in sexual interactions (Bridges, 1991). According to this theory, those who adhere to extremely traditional sex role socialization beliefs may viewacquaintance rape as an extreme and appropriate version of male-female sexual interactions. In general, the theory holds that the process of sex role socialization promotes the formation of rape-supportive beliefs, or false beliefs about rape, that can serve to mitigate the seriousness of rape. Rape-supportive beliefswould include beliefs such as rape is not psychologically damaging to victims as well as stereotypical beliefs about the victim’s role in her sexual assault (e.g., victim’s control over, enjoyment of, and responsibility for her assault). Results of the current study revealed that individuals made significantly less rape-supportive attributions when the victim-perpetrator relationship was dissolving than when the relationship was intact. Consistent with these findings, several states more fully support the prosecution of husband perpetrators if the couple is separated, living apart, seeking divorce, or divorced (Augustine, 1991). The legal community appears to consider sexual assaults that occur within dissolving marital relationships to more closely meet the nonconsensual criteria for rape. However, contrary to expectation, attributions regarding sex role stereotypical victim blame were not found to differ among the relationship conditions. This finding may be an indication that there has been a reduction in the societal tendency to blame the married victim. Adult rape victims with a history of child sexual abuse are reported to have longer recovery times (Burgess&Holmstrom, 1978) and poorer global social adjustment (Frank, Turner, & Stewart, 1980). In addition, repetitive victims have been found to have higher initial levels of distress and longer recovery times from the most recent episode when compared to first-time victims (Ruch, Arnedeo, Leon, & Gartrell, 1991). Research on adult women with histories of child sexual abuse have also documented the importance of self-blame and coping variables as mediators of adjustment. Why does childhood sexual abuse have a different impact on different individuals? One of the most supported explanations reported in etiological theories and studies of sexual abuse suggests that some variables may have an effect on the impact of childhood sexual abuse on victims’ development. Such variables could be organized along two principal axes: (a) “sexual abuse factors,” such as the variables related to the sexual contacts per se (nature, duration, frequency), those relative to the adult perpetrator (gender, age, tie to child), and those regarding the child victim (gender, age, emotions felt at time of contacts, dysfunctional, negligent, or violent family setting); (b) “later” or “iatrogenic factors,” such as the reaction of family and friends to the disclosure, whether the case is in the hands of the judicial or medical system, and number of interveners. According to the literature, the use of violence versus its absence during sexual abuse have an effect on victims’ feelings and perception with regard to the abuse. When sexual abuse is accompanied by physical force or verbal threats, victims of such contacts have reported feeling manipulated, betrayed, and humiliated. They have also indicated experiencing a profound sense of distress, relational difficulties (lack of trust in others and excessive wariness), poor self-esteem, feelings of guilt and helplessness (Doll et al., 1992; Gartner, 1999; Haugaard&Emery, 1989; Stein, Jacobs, Ferguson, Allen, & Fonagy, 1998; Urquiza & Capra, 1990), and sexual problems in adulthood (Mendel, 1995; T. G. M. Sandfort, 1992). In contrast, when sexual abuse occurred in a context of subtle manipulation and exempt of violence, certain victims have reported being consenting at the time of these experiences and even having initiated them (Okami, 1991; T. G. Sandfort, 1984). These victims qualified these events as positive experiences, discoveries, and sources of physical and sexual pleasure (Okami, 1991). In other words, it appears that the child’s consent at the time of such sexual contacts with an adult and the absence of force or threats are related to the victim’s positive perception. In this regard, T. G. M. Sandfort (1992) reported that victims who had nonconsensual contacts were more likely to present with poor adaptation and sexual dissatisfaction in adulthood than were those who reported consensual contacts. Where victims’ emotions at time of abuse are concerned, most studies have pointed out the existence of links between the presence of negative emotions associated with sexual abuse and the development of feelings of guilt, shame, and betrayal, which brought about depression and self-destructive behaviour over the medium and long term (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Gartner, 1999; Hauggard & Emery, 1989; Newberger & DeVos, 1988; Okami, 1991; Stein et al., 1998). Meanwhile, according to some etiological theories and studies, the symptoms observed among certain victims are more the result of the victim’s familial problems and dysfunctions (parental alcoholism, physical violence, negligence) than of the sexual contacts per se (Berliner, 1991; Hansen, Hecht, & Futa, 1998). In this regard, when families failed to respond to the needs and distress of victims, the latter have reported being as much disturbed by this absence of family support as by the sexual abuse per se (Bernard, 1981; Browne& Finkelhor, 1986; Faller, 1993; Finkelhor, 1990; Ingram, 1981; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993; Rind & Bauserman, 1993; Sauzier, 1989). Similarly, children who did not enjoy a reassuring family or structured social support (e.g., school, therapy) have been found to be at higher risk of developing psychological or behavioural problems in adulthood (Falshaw, Browne, &Hollin, 1996; Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993). However, it has also been reported that invasive interventions could be just as harmful as the absence of family and structured support to the victim’s development (Elwell & Ephross, 1987; Van Gijseghem, 1998). Furthermore, it is important to note that certain variables have not been studied much with male samples despite having proved crucial in the psychosocial adjustment ofwomen victims of childhood sexual abuse. Among these variables, the literature points out the importance of the family constellation and of the victim’s attachment style (Alexander, 1992; Alexander & Lupfer, 1987; Finkelhor, 1990; Harter, Alexander,&Neimeyer, 1988; Levang, 1989; Zeanah&Zeanah, 1989). The results of the present study suggest that the investigation of the impact of childhood sexual abuse requires that a multitude of variables be considered at the same time. These include variables related to the sexual abuse scenario, family context, attachment style, and possibly, to other types of victimization (negligence and verbal, physical, or psychological abuse) and to the circumstances of disclosure, which were not included in our analyses on account of their low frequency in our sample. Furthermore, the results suggest that similar childhood experiences do not elicit one and the same reaction in adulthood. Moreover, similar adjustment problems may arise from different sources. Finally, it does not seem possible to establish an exclusive causal link between childhood sexual abuse and an individual’s psychosocial adjustment in adulthood. In the majority of previous studies, abuse-related factors (e.g., proximity to the abuser, frequency and duration of the abuse, severity of the acts) were not found to be predictive of children’s or adolescents’adjustment following sexual abuse (Dubowitz, Black, Harrington, &Verschoore, 1993; Manion et al., 1998; Mannarino et al., 1991; Tebbutt et al., 1997). Some evidence was found for the predictive value of negative appraisals, symptomatology at initial assessment, and social support. In their study on 56 sexually abused children and adolescents, Manion et al. (1998) found that negative appraisals of guilt and blame predicted emotional functioning at 12 months postdisclosure. Also, and consistent with the results of Freedman, Brandes, Peri, and Shalev (1998), the authors show that internalizing symptoms, such as dissociation, depression and anxiety, and 3-month postdisclosure, are associated with more symptomatology at 12 months postdisclosure. They suggest that early internalizing symptoms and their concomitant negative appraisals of the traumatic event may reduce the victim’s ability to recover (Freedman et al., 1998). Social support also has a predictive value in recovery after sexual abuse (seeKendall-Tackett et al., 1993). Lynskey and Fergusson (1997) identified factors that discriminated sexually abused youngsters who developed a psychiatric disorder or adjustment difficulties from sexually abused youngsters who did not develop such problems. Results showed that parental support was an important factor protecting against the development of adjustment difficulties.With increasing reports of support, affection, and nurture, the occurrence of later adjustment difficulties decreased. This is consistent with Joseph (1999) who argued that crisis support immediately after the traumatic event is influential on later functioning. In his study on adolescent survivors of a ship disaster, greater direct crisis support was predictive of fewer feelings of depression and anxiety 18 months later. While numerous risk factors associated with the development of PTSD symptoms have been studied, there is still considerable confusion about why some individuals develop PTSD, and others remain relatively unscathed. Risk factors for the development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms include general vulnerability and severity of trauma exposure. Vulnerability factors related to PTSD include female gender, with women more likely to develop PTSD than men (Breslau et al., 1991; Norris, 1992), and history of exposure to traumatic events (Kilpatrick, Resnick, Saunders, & Best, 1998; Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994). Trauma severity indices associated with PTSD include threat to life, injury, and witnessing serious injury or death of another person. These factors indicate the degree of threat posed to an individual and are associated with the development of PTSD (e.g., Green, 1990; Kilpatrick et al., 1989; Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993). More recent research has suggested that individual response characteristics are also important predictors of PTSD symptoms. Specifically, peritraumatic reactions, immediate reactions experienced at the time of the trauma, such as dissociation, extreme anxiety, panic, and/or negative emotions, may be important predictors of subsequent PTSD symptoms. One reaction at the time of the trauma that has received considerable attention is dissociation. Marmar and his colleagues, have shown that peritraumatic dissociation is predictive of PTSD symptoms over and above the contribution of level of stress exposure and general dissociative tendencies in both male (Marmar et al., 1994) and female Vietnam veterans (Tichenor, Marmar, Weiss, Metzler, & Ronfeldt, 1996). The finding that peritraumatic dissociation is a robust predictor of PTSD symptoms has been found in other trauma populations as well. Weiss and colleagues (Weiss, Marmar, Metzler, & Ronfeldt, 1995) studied predictors of PTSD symptomatic responses among a group of emergency services personnel. After controlling for demographic variables, severity of exposure to a critical incident, adjustment, social support, locus of control, and general dissociative tendencies, peritraumatic dissociation remained strongly predictive of PTSD symptoms. In a study of the survivors of the Oakland/Berkeley, California firestorm, Koopman, Classen, and Spiegel (1994) found that individual dissociative symptoms occurring immediately after the fire predicted PTSD symptoms 7 to 9 months later. Moreover, in a recent prospective study of 51 injured trauma survivors, Shalev, Peri, Cannetti, and Schreiber (1996) found that peritraumatic dissociation reported 1 week after the trauma explained approximately 30% of the variance in PTSD symptoms at 6-month follow-up, above and beyond the contribution of demographic variables, event severity, and initial symptoms of intrusion, avoidance, depression, and anxiety. Despite the fact that immediate responses other than dissociation have received limited attention, several studies suggest that emotional and physical reactions experienced at the time of the trauma may be important predictors of PTSD symptomatic distress. Resick, Churchill, and Falsetti (1990) examined a range of immediate within-assault cognitive and emotional reactions experienced by rape victims. These authors found that emotional and dissociative reactions during the rape accounted for 46 to 79% of the variance in PTSD symptoms. In another study, Resnick, Falsetti, Kilpatrick, and Foy (1994) examined acute panic and emotional responses among a group of rape victims interviewed within 72 hr postrape during an emergency room postrape exam. Almost all (90%) of the women seen at the emergency room reported having a panic attack at the time of the rape. Longitudinal data further indicated that initial panic symptoms were predictive of PTSD intrusion symptoms at 3-month follow-up (Resnick, 1997). Additionally, Moleman, van der Hart, and van der Kolk (1992) reported a link between panic symptoms and dissociation in women who were undergoing extremely complicated childbirth. During childbirth, these women experienced a progression from initial panic symptoms to dissociation, and the majority of the women subsequently developed full-blown PTSD. From these studies, it appears that peritraumatic fear and attendant physiological arousal may lead to cognitive disruption in the form of peritraumatic dissociation and subsequent PTSD. The theoretical frameworks of early literature on battered women assumed pathology of the women and focused little or no attention on the consequences of experiencing violent acts (Gelles & Harrop, 1989; Walker & Browne, 1985). Yet research over the past decade indicates that the behaviors battered women demonstrate are primarily the result of the severity of threat and harm experienced (Follingstad, Brennan, Hause, Polek, & Rutledge, 1991; Ochberg, 1991; Walker & Browne, 1985). Although we did not find evidence that posttraumatic symptomatology is an underlying mechanism through which previous victimization leads to subsequent victimization (i.e., a mediator variable; Baron & Kenny, 1986), our data do highlight the importance of taking into account current levels of posttraumatic symptomatology when examining the link between child and/or adolescent sexual victimization and subsequent sexual victimization . Individuals with a history of previous sexual victimization who are experiencing PTSD symptoms are likely to have difficulty recognizing, attending to, or responding to danger cues appropriately (van der Kolk & McFarlane, 1996). They might have trouble discriminating threatening from nonthreatening information or make unwise decisions based on incomplete or inaccurate information. Moreover, perpetrators could identify these individuals as easy targets. As a result, they are at increased risk for sexual revictimization. ZAŠČITNI FAKTORJI Persistent unresolved anger or related negative feelings might serve to promote posttrauma symptoms, whereas forgiveness may be accompanied by less severe symptoms. Higher levels of anger were strongly associated with health status, emotional distress, and PTSD symptom severity. A growing body of evidence suggests that the personality trait of hardiness (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982) helps to buffer exposure to extreme stress. Hardiness consists of three dimensions: being committed to finding meaningful purpose in life, the belief that one can influence one's surroundings and the outcome of events, and the belief that one can learn and grow from both positive and negative life experiences. Armed with this set of beliefs, hardy individuals have been found to appraise potentially stressful situations as less threatening, thus minimizing the experience of distress. Hardy individuals are also more confident and better able to use active coping and social support, thus helping them deal with the distress they do experience (e. g. , Florian, Mikulincer, & Taubman, 1995). In an attempt to address this, they suggested that the negative sequelae of childhood sexual abuse could be viewed as a form of PTSD. From studies of adults who were sexually abused in childhood, it was suggested that individual differences in response to the abuse were related to three mediating variables including: severity of the abuse, availability of social support, and attributional styles regarding the cause of negative life events (Gold, 1986; Seidner and Calhoun, 1984; Silver et al., 1983). These three variables have also been shown to act as risk factors for onset of PTSD when exposed to negative life events such as rape or exposure to war (Baker and Peterson, 1977; Steketee and Foa, 1987; Cluss et al., 1983; Foy et al., 1984). Similar mediating factors in childhood sexual abuse have been presented by KendallTackett et al. (1993). Finkelhor and Berliner (1995) report that research has clearly demonstrated that certain elements are consistent predictors of the level of distress in children and the speed of their recovery. These influencing factors include parental support (Everson et al., 1991), maternal upset (Deblinger et al., 1990; Newberger et al., 1993), help-seeking in response to family crisis (Waterman, 1993) and also general elements of family functioning such as cohesion and healthy conflict management (Conte and Schuerman, 1987). The findings support the assumption that people who felt adequately informed would show better psychological adjustment. One should note that this aspect of cognitive coping at the first assessment (1989) had the strongest relationship with later symptoms. These findings suggest that assisting traumatized persons in their search for verifiable information may be important in early intervention efforts. The hypothesis that viewing the accident as a random incident would be associated with better psychological adjustment was supported. Whether antisocial youth become violent adults is determined by multiple risk and protective factors (Rutter, Giller, & Hagell, 1998). Such factors include individual strengths and vulnerabilities, family characteristics, and features of the wider community (including the peer group, school, and neighborhood). Learning difficulties, difficult temperament, and problems with regulating negative emotions are examples of individual risk factors, whereas easy temperament and good problem-solving skills are examples of individual protective factors. Examples of family-based risk factors include insecure attachment, family violence, and family disorganization, whereas examples of protective family factors include secure attachment, parental cooperation, and effective discipline practices. Membership of deviant peer groups and a low level of family support are examples of community-based risk factors. In contrast, involvement with nondeviant peers and a high level of family support are examples of protective factors. This study suggests that not all sexually abused children have the same perceived level of self-concept, social support, or traumatic symptoms. This indicates that there are variations to sexually abused childrens' response to the trauma and supports the examination of individual differences among this heterogeneous population. Perceived parental, classmate, and friend support were associated with higher global self-concept. This suggests that sexually abused children who felt better about themselves also perceived a higher level of social support. Children who reported high levels of posttraumatic stress indicated feeling worse about their behavior; children who reported high levels of dissociation reported more negative feelings about their social acceptance. The stability of these relationships will be reexamined with the larger sample of children when data are available. Sexually Abused Children's Perceptions: How They May Change Treatment Focus , By: Carla J. Reyes, Anna M. Kokotovic, Merith A. Cosden, Professional Psychology: Research And Practice, 0735-7028, December 1, 1996, Vol. 27, Issue 6 One important element of coping identified by Joseph et al. (1995) is crisis support. Crisis support has been defined by Joseph et al. (1995) as involving both the availability of others and their reactions to disclosures of trauma. The evidence suggests that, in general, individuals who receive higher levels of crisis support experience better psychological outcome (Joseph, Andrews, Williams, & Yule, 1992). Joseph et al.’s (1995) model indicates that crisis support is thought to influence appraisals, coping, and emotional states. A Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Post-Traumatic Stress for Sexually Abused Females SUZANNE L. BARKER-COLLO, WILLIAM T. MELNYK, LESLIE MCDONALD-MISZCZAK JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 15 No. 4, April 2000 375-392 In their discussion, the authors noted general agreement in the literature (Jones & Barlow, 1990; Joseph et al., 1996; S. D. Solomon, 1986) that increased availability of crisis support is predictive of external event appraisals (i.e., attribution of blame to others) and reduced PTSD symptomatology. However, some traumatic events (e.g., sexual abuse) can be stigmatizing and elicit shunning, avoidance, and blaming of the victim by crisis supports (Wortman & Lehman, 1985). Shunning, avoidance, and blaming by the support network and failure to engage the network may be particularly evident where the abuser is known to the survivor due to increased efforts to deny or hide the occurrence of the abuse (Meichenbaum, 1994). A Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Post-Traumatic Stress for Sexually Abused Females SUZANNE L. BARKER-COLLO, WILLIAM T. MELNYK, LESLIE MCDONALD-MISZCZAK JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 15 No. 4, April 2000 375-392 FAKTORJI TVEGANJA As expected, people with increased exposure and higher identification with the victims encountered higher stress (Schuster et al., 2001). In particular, evidence suggests that individuals who are vulnerable to faulty regulation of negative emotion may be at increased risk for aggressive and/or violent behavior (Davidson, Putnam, & Larson, 2000). Although age is clearly an important factor in placing girls and young women at risk for sexual assault, life course stage is also likely to shape the mental health consequences of those violent events. Adolescence is a period of self-exploration, a stage in which girls acquire a sense of self-worth and when self-esteem develops and becomes more stable (Steinberg & Sheffield Morris, 2001). Given that adolescence is a period in the life course that requires adjustment to a number of major social, cognitive, and psychological changes (Lerner & Galambos, 1999), exposure to violence may disrupt children’s progression through age-appropriate developmental tasks (Margolin & Gordis, 2000). Egan and Perry (1998) suggested that poor selfconceptions are risk factors and consequences of victimization during adolescence, diminishing self-regard over time and solidifying a child’s status as a victim.Violent victimization during this time period may, therefore, be particularly consequential. Experiences in adolescence will not only shape the direction the life course will take but also will, in large part, determine theavailability and stability of adult social roles (Clausen, 1991). Although Loeber and Hay (1997) pointed out that early stressful life events may lead to a variety of consequences including the development of negative behaviors, Recent nation wide studies established that the major predictors of post-traumatic stress disorder were the objective severity of the violence inflicted, the subjective fear of death or serious injury, and whether penetration of the body occurred (Epstein, Saunders, & Kilpatrick,1997). Also important were how much awoman blamed herself for what happened and how threatening the rape was to her worldview (Frazier,1990; Frazier & Schauben, 1994; Koss, Figueredo, Prince,&White, 2000; Norris&Kaniasty, 1991). A number of studies have shown that trauma symptoms are related to the severity of stressful life events. This has been found in research on various kinds of traumatic events, including among victims of motor vehicle accidents (Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 1998), Vietnam veterans (Green, Grace, Lindy, Gleser, & Leonard, 1990), Cambodian refugees (Carlson & RosserHogan, 1991), and survivors of a firestorm (Koopman, Classen, & Spiegel, 1994). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290) Tveganje za spolno zlorabo in navezanost Children with insecure patent-child relationships, aswehave seen, will have low self-esteem, poor relationship skills, and a desperate need for attention. As a consequence, they will readily respond to attention from any adult. Being unable to form relationships themselves and lacking confidence that anyone will like them, these children will be especially vulnerable to attention fromothers. They can be expected to be particularly responsive to physical attention. However, children who experienced anxious/ ambivalent relations with their parents will be more likely to be responsive to attention from others because they have a positive view of others and strongly desire closeness. Avoidant children do not trust others and may be repelled by adults who display physical affection. Thus, any adult who pays attention to an anxious/ ambivalent child can expect to get a strongly positive response. In fact, such a vulnerable child may bewilling to tolerate even sexual advances in exchange for feeling close to an adult. Thus, anxious/ambivalent children may be more likely to be sexually abused. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Specifically, engagement of crisis support has been linked to amount of force used by the perpetrator.As indicated byWyatt et al. (1991), increasing level of force used by a perpetrator is significantly related to increasingly negative reactions of others to the victim when sexual abuse is disclosed. A Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Post-Traumatic Stress for Sexually Abused Females SUZANNE L. BARKER-COLLO, WILLIAM T. MELNYK, LESLIE MCDONALD-MISZCZAK JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 15 No. 4, April 2000 375-392 clinical and research data suggest that prior traumatic experiences, such as prior sexual victimization, predict greater psychological distress following sexual assault (Burgess & Holmstrom, 1978; Frank, Turner, & Stewart, 1980). Adult rape victims with a history of child sexual abuse are reported to have longer recovery times (Burgess&Holmstrom, 1978) and poorer global social adjustment (Frank, Turner, & Stewart, 1980). In addition, repetitive victims have been found to have higher initial levels of distress and longer recovery times from the most recent episode when compared to first-time victims (Ruch, Arnedeo, Leon, & Gartrell, 1991). Coping With Rape The Roles of Prior Sexual Abuse and Attributions of Blame - CATALINA M. ARATA - University of South Alabama - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 1, January 1999 62-78 Kako izolacija od emocij in relacij privede do nasilja They propose that men are particularly socialized to suppress this emotion: the sense of being weak, inadequate, powerless, helpless, impotent, or incompetent. Rather than experience these painful feelings or let others see them undergoing them, men usually become blank or angry. Shame itself is harmless, indeed, necessary. Shame is a prime component of conscience, modesty, and morality. It becomes a problem only if covered. That is, one ingredient of violence, its incredible energy, is produced by masking shame with blankness or anger. Dva odziva na sram However, patients had two different, seemingly opposite responses in the shame context. In one, the patient seemed to be suffering psychological pain, but failed to identify it as shame. Lewis called this form overt, undifferentiated shame. A patient would usually refer to an emotion or feeling, but the reference misidentified the shame feeling (‘This is an awkward moment for me.’) In a second type of response, the patient seemed not to be in pain, revealing an emotional response only by rapid, obsessional speech on topics that seemed slightly removed from the dialogue. Lewis called this second response bypassed shame. Razlika moški ženske In her study of differences in the way men and women manage emotions, Lewis (1976) cites studies suggesting that the overt, undifferentiated form of unacknowledged shame is more characteristic of women than of men, and the bypassed form more characteristic of men than of women. She uses this difference in the management of shame to explain the higher rates of depression in women than in men, and the higher level of aggression in men. Trije vzroki za agresijo in nasilje The first is social: isolation, the absence of affectional attachments. The second is cognitive: obsessive preoccupation. The third is emotional: complete repression of shame in the form of shame/anger spirals. Zanka ponavljajočih se emocionalnih epizod Although not stated explicitly by Tomkins or Lewis, both seem to imply that emotions can form closed loops, a self-perpetuating emotional episode that refuses to subside. A familiar example are people who are ‘blushers.’ They are so self-conscious about their blushing that they are ashamed of it. But their shame about blushing increases the blush, and so on. This particular example suggests a loop that is not mentioned by either Tomkins or Lewis: shame/shame. But it is this loop, I believe, that gives rise to the most prevalent form of shame spirals, those that lead to blankness and withdrawal. The two kinds of shame spirals give rise to two different paths: withdrawal and silence (shame/shame) and anger, aggression and violence (shame/anger). »Emotional/relational« teorija nasilja The emotional/relational theory of violence outlined here would seem to be particularly applicable to instances involving long-term violence on a massive scale. suggest the three conditions for violence suggested by the theory outlined here: isolation from others, a single, overarching obsession, and complete repression of shame. MEDIATOR ZA POSLEDICE PRI SPOLNI ZLORABI – NAVEZANOST Oneway that attachment difficulties might be manifested in later relationships is through the development of the self. The development of the self can be seen to unfold in the context of attachment and the internalization of important others’ perceptions and expectations; sustained and early trauma arising from abuse can produce long-standing dysfunctions of self (Briere, 1992). Exactly how the “self” should be defined has not been clearly demonstrated, even by object relations and self psychology theorists for whom it is central (Briere, 1992). In general, the self can be understood as “the agent of actions, the experiencer of feelings, the maker of intentions, [and] the architect of plans” (Stern, 1985, p. 6), the development of which occurs in the context of attachment. Self-dysfunctions, or those related to this internal base, are purported to lead to difficulties such as identity confusion, boundary issues, and the inability to soothe oneself. Attachment theory (Ainsworth, 1985; Bowlby, 1973, 1980, 1982, 1988) suggests that early childhood experiences of parental support, nurturance, consistency, and responsiveness produce a secure attachment.Warm and responsive parenting, according to this model, is expected to result in positive models of both the self and others and hence to result in secure and fulfilling adult relationships. Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207 TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – RAZVOJ NEVARNE NAVEZANOSTI BOLJ POVEZAN Z ZLORABAMI IN ZANEMARJANJEM Insecure attachment has been observed to a much greater degree among children who have experienced physical abuse and neglect than in cases where abuse and neglect are absent (Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, & Braunwald, 1989; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981). It is estimated that between 70% and 100% of maltreated children exhibit insecure attachment (versus a base rate of about 30% in general population samples) and that these children are more likely to demonstrate an impaired sense of self and an impaired ability to share information about their thoughts, feelings, and intentions (Cicchetti, 1987). Similarly, insecure attachment has been noted via clinical observations of sexually abused children (Friedrich, 1990, 1996) and in research conducted with adult women. For example, a much higher proportion of insecurely attached women was found in a group of women who were sexually abused within their families than the proportion that would be expected according to Bartholomew and Horowitz’s (1991) normative sample (Alexander, 1993). Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207 TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – NAVEZANOST – +/- MODEL SEBE/DRUGEGA Patterns of adult attachment, or ways of being in relationships, can be organized in terms of Bowlby’s (1982) conception of internal working models (Bartholomew, 1990, 1993). As Bartholomew describes them, models of the self can be dichotomized as either positive (positive self-concept, the self as worthy of love and attention) or negative (negative self-concept, the self as unworthy of love and attention). Similarly, models of the other can be viewed as positive (the other as trustworthy, caring, and available) or negative (the other as rejecting, uncaring, and distant (see Figure 1). The degree of positivity of one’s self-model is associated with the degree of emotional dependence on others for self-validation; a positive self-model can be understood as an internalized sense of self-worth that is not dependent on others for validation. Apositive other-model is reflective of expectations of others’availability and supportiveness; a positive other-model facilitates actively seeking out intimacy and support in close relationships, whereas negative other-models lead to avoidance of intimacy and support (Bartholomew, 1990). Each working model of the self in combination with each working model of the other is hypothesized to define a particular adult attachment style (Secure, Fearful, Dismissing, and Preoccupied; see Figure 1). Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207 TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – NAVEZANOST – POJASNILO ODNOSA MED OTROŠKO SPOLNO ZLORABO IN ODRASLO PSIHOLOŠKO PRILAGODITVIJO In this examination of the relationship between child sexual abuse, adult attachment style, and adult psychological adjustment, we have demonstrated that attachment appears to mediate the relationship between CSA and psychological adjustment. Specifically, we found that CSA predicts both adult attachment style and psychological adjustment and that attachment also predicts psychological adjustment. In addition, attachment style continues to predict adjustment when the effects of CSA are controlled, whereas CSA no longer predicts adjustment when the effects of attachment are controlled, thus indicating that adult attachment style mediates the relationship between CSA and psychological adjustment. Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207 TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – OTROŠKA SPOLNA ZLORABA – OBLIKA NAVEZANOSTI NAPOVEDUJE SIMPTOME PTSM Because sexual abuse is strongly associated with adult attachment style, the role of attachment in adjustment is particularly salient for sexually abused women. Contrary to Alexander’s (1993) suggestion that attachment predicts basic personality structure but not symptoms associated with post-traumatic stress disorder (such as intrusive thoughts, avoidance, and depression), the present study indicates that attachment is of central importance in predicting these symptoms. In particular, the most important attachment dimension for predicting the severity of symptoms is one’s model-of-self. In addition, these results provide support for Bartholomew’s (1990) conceptualization of the two dimensional nature of attachment and the implication of Bowlby’s theory that the intersection of the underlying models of self and other is the basis for the four basic attachment styles. Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207 TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – OTROŠKA SPOLNA ZLORABA ZNOTRAJ DRUŽINE IMA TEŽJE POSLEDICE Although not all researchers agree about which characteristics of CSA are most likely to be associated with a poorer prognosis, one trend in the research suggests that abuse by fathers/stepfathers may lead to greater long-term effects (Beitchman et al., 1992). In the present study, when the degree of relatedness between victim and perpetrator is examined, it is apparent that the relationship between sexual abuse and adjustment is a function of the influence of intrafamilial sexual abuse; women who were abused within the family reported significantly more difficulties than women who were abused by someone outside the family. Women abused by a family member were especially likely to report problems in the following areas: depression, posttraumatic stress (i.e., intrusive thoughts and defensive use of avoidance), anxiety, and an inadequate sense of self and personal identity. Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207 VRSTE TRAVME IN TRAVMA Although there is no universally accepted definition of trauma, it is generally understood as a state of being negatively overwhelmed both physically and psychologically: it is the experience of terror, loss of control and utter helplessness during a stressful event that threatens one’s physical and/or psychological integrity. PTSD symptoms include reexperiencing the traumatic event in the form of flashbacks, nightmares, intense bodily or emotional sensations, obsessional preoccupations and behavioural re-enactments. Trauma victims inadvertently tend to re-enact the trauma compulsively by either acting selfdestructively, harming others or becoming revictimised. Re-experiencing traumatic events causes persons to alternate between persistent forms of emotional numbing and hyperarousal. Irritability, angry outbursts, restlessness, difficulty concentrating and difficulty sleeping are also common signs. In their attempts to ward off hyperarousal, sexual abuse victims experience withdrawal and detachment from emotions. Such post-traumatic stress symptoms can fragment one’s sense of self and agency and one’s ability to relate to others constructively. J. Beste; RECOVERY FROM SEXUAL VIOLENCE AND SOCIALLY MEDIATED DIMENSIONS OF GOD’S GRACE: IMPLICATIONS FOR CHRISTIAN COMMUNITIES; [SCE 18.2 (2005) 89–112] Neither clinicians nor researchers have had an effective system for assessing the extent of traumatic experience. Alarcon (1997) proposed a typology of PTSD consisting of six clinical types: depressive, dissociative, somatomorphic, psychotomorphic, organomorphic, and “neurotic-like.” In addition, he suggests that substance abuse and personality disorders need to be considered. This classification appears to be problematic due to extensive overlap; many clients could easily fit into several categories. (Eldra P. Solomon, Kathleen M. Heide. Type III Trauma: Toward a More Effective Conceptualization of Psychological Trauma, International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 43(2), 1999 202-210.) Terr (1991) proposed that there are two basic types of trauma, which she called Type I and Type II. Type I trauma results from a single event, such as a rape or witnessing a murder. Survivors of Type I trauma who are 3 years old or older at the time of the event generally retain complete memory of their experience. These individuals struggle to make sense out of what happened. They may experience perceptual errors such as visual hallucinations or time distortions. According to Terr (1991), Type II trauma results from “repeated exposure to extreme external events” (p. 15). Survivors of Type II trauma generally have at least some memory of their experience. Children who sustain Type II trauma use massive denial, repression, dissociation, identification with the perpetrator, and aggression against themselves as coping mechanisms. These children are often diagnosed as having Conduct Disorder, Attention Deficit Disorder, depression, or a dissociative disorder. We suggest this distinction because, based on our clinical experience and those of our colleagues, individuals who survive Type III trauma suffer more severe psychological effects requiring different treatment strategies. Examples of Type II trauma include such experiences as repeated fondling by a neighbor or uncle, or growing up with parents who engage in moderate psychological or physical abuse. Type III trauma is more extreme. It results from multiple and pervasive violent events beginning at an early age and continuing for years. Typically, the childwas the victim of multiple perpetrators, and one or more are close relatives. The abusive events were likely frequent, yet unpredictable. Generally, force is used and the abuse has a sadistic quality. The child may have been threatened with torture or death, or death of a loved one. Both sexual and physical abuse may have been perpetrated. Examples would include enduring sadistic ritual abuse by an organized group or repeated violent physical and sexual abuse by caretakers. (Eldra P. Solomon, Kathleen M. Heide. Type III Trauma: Toward a More Effective Conceptualization of Psychological Trauma, International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 43(2), 1999 202-210.) Very few studies have examined the interaction between emotional abuse or neglect and sexual abuse on PTSD symptomatology, although studies suggest that these types of child maltreatment frequently co-occur (Higgins & McCabe, 2000). Definicija travme in PTSD Although there is no universally accepted definition of trauma, it is generally understood as a state of being negatively overwhelmed both physically and psychologically: it is the experience of terror, loss of control and utter helplessness during a stressful event that threatens one’s physical and/or psychological integrity. PTSD symptoms include reexperiencing the traumatic event in the form of flashbacks, nightmares, intense bodily or emotional sensations, obsessional preoccupations and behavioural re-enactments. Trauma victims inadvertently tend to re-enact the trauma compulsively by either acting selfdestructively, harming others or becoming revictimised. Re-experiencing traumatic events causes persons to alternate between persistent forms of emotional numbing and hyperarousal. Irritability, angry outbursts, restlessness, difficulty concentrating and difficulty sleeping are also common signs. In their attempts to ward off hyperarousal, sexual abuse victims experience withdrawal and detachment from emotions. Such post-traumatic stress symptoms can fragment one’s sense of self and agency and one’s ability to relate to others constructively. Beste, J. (2005). Recovery from sexual violence and socially mediated dimensions of God's grace: Implications for Christian communities. Studies in Christian ethics, 18, 89-112. VRSTE TRAVME – TRAVME V NAVEZANOSTI VRSTE TRAVME – TRAVME OB IZGUBI (SMRTI) Theoretically, a number of authors have focused on loss as a defining feature of trauma. For example, Lifton (1988) referred to the “death imprint” (the “radical intrusion of an image or feeling of threat, or end, to life,” p. 18) as an important aspect of the (trauma) survivor syndrome. This imprint or intrusion makes it impossible for the survivor to deny the reality of death and brings him or her face-to-face with feelings of personal vulnerability and consequent anxiety. In the empirical literature on grief and bereavement, certain types or modes of death have been associated, at least in some studies, with bereavement that is prolonged or has more pathologic outcomes. These include (a) death that is unexpected, sudden, or untimely; (b) death that is horrific (grotesque) or painful; (c) death that is violent or stigmatized in some way (e.g., homicide, suicide, or AIDS); (d) death involving multiple losses; and (e) death of a child (e.g., Sanders, 1988, 1993). In a community study of individuals who experienced conjugal bereavement, Schut, DeKeijser,Van Den Bout, and Dijkhuis (1991) specifically examined PTSD symptoms (via self-report) and estimated the proportion of participants who had “probable” PTSD. They found that 20% to 31% of participants could be labeled as having probable PTSD during at least one follow-up point between 4 and 25 months following the death, although most spouses in this study had died of natural causes. Kaltman and Bonanno (2000) addressed the dimensions of traumatic loss that might be associated with elevated depression and PTSD symptoms in spousal survivors. Suicide of a family member or friend, more specifically, has been associated with elevated symptoms. Farberow, Gallagher-Thompson, Gilewski, and Thompson (1992) followed surviving spouses of individuals who committed suicide for several years and compared them to survivors of spouses who died of natural causes and to nonbereaved controls. To summarize, there is weak support for the hypothesis that unanticipated death alone meets a “traumatic” stressor criterion (i.e., sufficient to be associated with PTSD; Kaltman&Bonanno, 2000; Schut et al., 1991). There is stronger support, however, for the conclusion that violent death is associated with both PTSD symptoms and enduring distress, especially regarding grief and depression (Farberow et al., 1992; Kaltman & Bonanno, 2000; Zisook et al., 1998). Data gathered in a number of studies indicate that people with one traumatic event in their histories are likely to have experienced multiple prior and/or subsequent events (e.g., Green et al., 2000). Furthermore, studies have shown that multiple exposure to traumatic events, either within the same type of event or across event types, is associated with higher levels of symptoms than single exposure (e.g., Follette, Polusny, Bechtle, & Naugle, 1996; McCauley et al., 1997; Miranda, Green,&Krupnick, 1997). Njihova raziskava: In conclusion, traumatic loss, defined as loss of a close friend or familymember by suicide, homicide, or accident, was associated with high levels of intrusion and reexperiencing symptoms and high rates of acute stress disorder. These differences were evident in the absence of other life traumas that could account for these symptoms, in a relatively low-risk, high-resource group. Traumatic loss was also associated with high levels of subjective distress relative to other life events. Violent loss seems to precipitate traditional traumatic stress-response symptoms and to be appropriately classified as a traumatic stressor. Symptoms may continue for prolonged periods and could potentially be reduced by psychological interventions. VRSTE TRAVME – ZLORABA IN NADLEGOVANJE Sexual assault is any sexual act forced on a person against his or her will (Bowker, 1983). Child sexual abuse is a heterogeneous label, including single incident stranger assaults, in addition to cases of intrafamilial abuse lasting for years (Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993; Rowan & Foy, 1993). Sexual abuse may include a wide range of Criterion-A traumatic events including fondling, coercive sexual contact, and penetration (KendallTackett et al., 1993; Rowan & Foy, 1993). Sexual harassment has become a major social, legal, and mental health problem because of its high prevalence and its negative consequences for victims. These consequences can include decreased productivity, loss of job, decreased income, and impaired psychological and physical well-being. Despite evidence from empirical studies that victims often exhibit posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, some have argued that sexual harassment does not constitute legitimate trauma.We argue that many forms of sexual harassment meet the diagnostic Criteria A1 and A2 of PTSD. Finally, the DSM-IV trauma criterion is explicated, and its relationship with sexual harassment and its effects are discussed. Sexual harassment has become an increasingly important issue over the past two decades. Over 10,000 people made complaints of sexual harassment in 1992, and complaints bywomen have nearly more than doubled from 5,603 in 1989 to 14,420 in 1994 (Andrew&Andrew, 1997; Simon, 1996). Sexual harassment occurs in many different settings: 51% of family practice female resident physicians, 64% of females in the U.S. military, 70% of female officeworkers, and88%of female nurses report having experienced sexual harassment (Dan, Pinsof,&Riggs, 1995; Piotrkowski, 1998; Pryor, 1995; Vukovich, 1996). Definicije spolnega nadlegovanja: Legal and Regulatory Definitions The law proscribing sexual harassment derives from Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964.Title VII prohibits discrimination “with respect to : : : terms, conditions, or privileges of employment : : :” because of an individual’s sex, race, religion, and so forth. (Title VII, Civil Rights Act x2000-2(a). Although sexual harassment is not explicitly mentioned in the Act, courts later interpreted sexual harassment to be subsumed because it is gender-related. According to the U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (1980), sexual harassment is defined as Unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors, and other verbal or physical conduct of a sexual nature; when cooperation or submission was an implicit or explicit condition of employment; was used as a basis for the employment-related decisions; or when the conduct has the purpose or effect of unreasonably interfering with a person’s work performance or creating an intimidating, hostile or offensive working environment. (p. 74676) Psychological Definitions of Sexual Harassment A psychological definition does not focus on the incident itself but rather, attends to the victim’s evaluation of the situation such that the victim’s evaluation is influenced by factors like ambiguity, perceived threat, and loss (Fitzgerald, Swan, & Fischer, 1995). Fitzgerald, Swan, et al. (1997) defined sexual harassment psychologically as “unwanted sex-related behavior at work that is appraised by the recipient as offensive, exceeding her resources, or threatening her well-being” (p. 15). DSM-IV Criteria for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Harassment victims have been described as suffering from a “posttrauma syndrome” (Hamilton, Alagna, King, & Lloyd, 1987). A PTSD model of the sequelae of sexual harassment has been used to attempt to account for effects such as flashbacks, sleep disturbances, and emotional numbing (Gutek & Koss, 1993; Koss, 1990). Clinical researchers have reported that sexual harassment victims are frequently meeting the symptom criteria for PTSD (Dansky & Kilpatrick, 1997). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for PTSD are (A) The person has been exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following were present: (1) the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of self or others; (2) the person’s response involved intense fear, helplessness, or horror (pp. 427–428); (B) Reexperiencing the event and severe distress; (C) Avoidance of associated stimuli; and (D) Hyperarousal. VRSTE TRAVME – ZANEMARJANJE Definition of Neglect There is little agreement on the definition and measurement of neglect (Costin, Karger, & Stoesz, 1996; National Research Council, 1993). One of the most important points of disagreement concerns whether neglect should be defined and measured in a way that includes injury or harm to a child as compared to definition and measurement solely on the basis of the behavior of the caregiver (Straus & Kaufman Kantor, 2005). Another unresolved issue is whether the neglectful behavior must be intentional. VRSTE TRAVME – KOMPLEKSNA TRAVMA DEFINICIJA KOMPLEKSNE TRAVME IN SIMPTOMOV Complex trauma refers to a type of trauma that occurs repeatedly and cumulatively, usually over a period of time and within specific relationships and contexts. The term came into being over the past decade as researchers found that some forms of trauma were much more pervasive and complicated than others (Herman,1992a, 1992b). The diagnostic conceptualization of CPTSD/DESNOS as defined for the field trial consisted of seven different problem areas shown by research to be associated with early interpersonal trauma (Herman, 1992a, 1992b): 1. alterations in the regulation of affective impulses, including difficulty with modulation of anger and self-destructiveness. This category has come to include all methods used for emotional regulation and self-soothing, including addictions and self-harming behaviors that are, paradoxically, often life saving; 2. alterations in attention and consciousness leading to amnesias and dissociative episodes and depersonalization. This category includes emphasis on dissociative responses different than those found in the DSM criteria for PTSD. Its inclusion in the CPTSD conceptualization incorporates the findings regarding dissociation that were mentioned earlier, namely, that dissociation tends to be related to prolonged and severe interpersonal abuse occurring during childhood and, secondarily, that children are more prone to dissociation than are adults; 3. alterations in self perception, such as a chronic sense of guilt and responsibility, and ongoing feelings of intense shame. Chronically abused individuals often incorporate the lessons of abuse into their sense of self and self-worth (Courtois, 1979a, 1979b; Pearlman, 2001); 4. alterations in perception of the perpetrator, including incorporation of his or her belief system. This criterion addresses the complex relationships and belief systems that ensue following repetitive and premeditated abuse at the hands of primary caretakers; 5. alterations in relationship to others, such as not being able to trust and not being able to feel intimate with others. Another “lesson of abuse” internalized by victim/survivors is that people are venal and self-serving, out to get what they can by whatever means including using/abusing others; 6. somatization and/or medical problems. These somatic reactions and medical conditions may relate directly to the type of abuse suffered and any physical damage that was caused or they may be more diffuse. They have been found to involve all major body systems; 7. alterations in systems of meaning. Chronically abused individuals often feel hopeless about finding anyone to understand them or their suffering. They despair of ever being able to recover from their psychic anguish. OTROK in TRAVMA, MLADOSTNIK in TRAVMA, ODRASEL in TRAVMA Child hood trau mas include phys i cal mal treat ment (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995), sex ual assault or moles ta -tion (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995; Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996), life-threat ening acci dents (Winje & Ulvik, 1998), the unex pected death of close friends or fam ily mem bers (Appelbaum & Burns, 1991), life-threat ening ill ness (Stuber, Nader, Houskamp, & Pynoos, 1996), disas ter (Green et al.,1994; LaGreca, Silverman, Vernberg, & Prinstein, 1996), domes tic vio lence (Famularo, Fenton, Kinscherff, Ayoub, & Barnum 1994), and com mu nity vio lence (Cooley, Turner, & Beidel, 1995; Nader, Pynoos, Fair banks, & Fred er ick, 1990). Child Mal treatment, Other Trauma Ex po sure, and Posttraumatic Symptomatology Among Children With Oppositional De fi ant and At tention Def i cit Hy peractivity Dis or ders Julian D. Ford (University of Con necticut School of Med icine), Robert Racusin, Cynthia G. Ellis, Wil liam B. Daviss, Jessica Reiser, Amy Fleischer (Dartmouth Med i cal School), Julie Thomas (Youngs town State Uni ver sity). TRAUMA DURING CHILDHOOD To date, much of the research in the area of traumatic stress has focused specifically on events that arise during childhood. Although trauma can arise at any point in the life cycle (Janoff-Bulman, 1992), many investigators believe that exposure to trauma during childhood may be especially harmful. The importance of events that occur in childhood is suggested both by the work of Brown and Harris (1978) on early parental loss and Bowlby’s (1980) research on attachment theory. According to Brown and Harris (1978), early childhood is an exceptionally salient developmental period. In particular, they argued that during early life, a child’s parents are often the primary source for learning effective ways of exercising personal control. This is important because a strong sense of control helps people to anticipate potentially stressful experiences or conditions, take preventive steps to avoid them, and confront and deal with the adversity that does occur (Ross, Mirowsky, & Goldsteen, 1990). Consequently, the lessons learned during this early period of interaction between parents and children may have lasting effects on one’s ability to handle adversity. Bowlby (1980) also considered childhood a particularly important and vulnerable developmental stage. According to him, childhood is an important period for determining the nature of one’s social relationships later in life. Specifically, he maintained that a child’s relationship with his or her parents serves as a prototype for the development of social ties in adult years. Parent-child relationships that are intimate and caring tend to foster a sense of trust and security that facilitates the development of interpersonal closeness throughout the life course (for a recent reviewof this perspective, also see Reis&Patrick, 1996). Therefore, children may be particularly vulnerable to traumatic events, especially those of an interpersonal nature such as exposure to physical violence from one’s parents because they could have lasting effects on one’s ability to form and maintain meaningful and supportive social relationships throughout life. In studying the consequences of childhood trauma in general and childhood physical violence specifically,much of the current research has focused on immediate or short-term effects of exposure to violence on childhood behavior and psychosocial adjustment (e.g., Conaway & Hansen, 1989; Lamphear, 1985). Recently, however, investigators have become increasingly interested in the potential long-term consequences of childhood exposure to traumatic events. Profound loss covers a broad spectrum of childhood experiences. It can come from separation from parents or family through death, divorce, foster care placement, or the adverse political and economic circumstances that are associated with emigration.Recent data suggest that childhood loss of a parent from natural causes is as strongly associated with PTSD symptoms as children’s reactions to natural disasters (3). Profound loss can occur as an outcome of parental mental illness, sometimes quietly and without acknowledgment as when a parent is lost to depression, or with violent upheaval, as is often the case with alcohol or other substance use disorders. Today, abandonment and neglect due to these addictions are the most common reasons for foster care placement (4). Loss can include physical injury to the self, in which a part of the body is, or is believed to be, damaged or altered. The initial loss frequently sets off a series of events (e.g., hospitalization, relocation, foster care placement) which in turn lead to further loss. TRAVMA IN SPOMIN TRAVMA IN KONCENTRACIJA ZNAČILNOSTI STORILCA – SPOLNA ZLORABA Osebe, ki spolno zlorabljajo in navezanost In support of the contention that sexual offenders had poor parent-child attachments, there is a considerable body of literature indicating a variety of disruptive experiences in the childhood of these offenders. Langevin et al. (1984), for instance, found that rapists had quite inadequate parents with whom they failed to identify. Various other researchers (Awad, Saunders, & Levene, 1984; Bass & Levant, 1992; Finkelhor, 1984; Knight, Prentky, Schnieder, & Rosenberg, 1983; Lang & Langevin, 1991; Protter & Travin, 1987; Saunders, Awad, & White, 1986; Tingle, Barnard, Robbins, Newman, & Hutchinson, 1986) have reported disruptive and abusive family environments in the childhoods of sexual offenders. Drunkenness, physical and sexual abuse, inconsistency, emotional neglect or rejection, hostility, criminal activities, social isolation, and various other problematic circumstances were found to characterize the family backgrounds of sexual offenders. More specifically, Marshall, Serran, and Cortoni (2000) found poorer attachments to fathers than to mothers among child molesters, and Smallbone and Dadds (in press) showed that insecure attachments to fathers led to the enactment of coercive sexual behavior in adulthood. In a more recent study, Smallbone and Dadds (2000) again demonstrated that insecure childhood attachments were significantly associated with coercive sexual behavior. These results remained true even after the influences of antisociality and aggression were partialed out. Clearly, sexual offenders have experienced a significant incidence of problematic relations with their parents during their childhood, and these experiences appear to be causally significant in the development of their offensive behaviors. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Zlorabljeni zlorabljajo? It has certainly been demonstrated that sexual offenders report far higher rates of personal experience with being sexually victimized as children than do other males (Hanson & Slater, 1988). However, much of these data result from rather simple questions put to the offenders whomay have a vested interest in exaggerating childhood sexual abuse; for example, they see this as diminishing their responsibility for their ownoffending. Bentovim and Williams (1998) found that the majority of children who were sexually abused came from homes where they had been abused or neglected. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Although the majority of children who are subject to sexual abuse suffer both immediate and long-term consequences (Beitchman et al., 1992; Conte, 1988), these effects are not always seen by the victim as resulting from the abuse. Indeed, we might expect vulnerable children who lack a sense of self-worth to blame themselves for these effects and, as a consequence, to judge the abuse to not have harmed them.Many child molesters who were sexually abused as children claim that it did them no harm, and so, they see their own abusive behavior as not harmful to the victim. If sexual offenders have greater experience with being sexually molested as children, and if this satisfies their need for attention, they may construe the abuse as positive at least in some respects. Their low self-esteem may cause them to attribute any unfortunate consequences not to the abuse or the abuser but rather to some defect in their own character. Both their need for attention and their low self-esteem may allow these children to perceive the abuse in positive terms. Insofar as they perceive the abuse in this way, it may encourage them to see sex between an adult and a child as nonharmful and even beneficial, thereby removing one significant constraint against sexual offending later in their life. In addition, if they did derive pleasure from their own abuse, subsequently during masturbation they may fantasize about sexual contact between an adult and a child. In these fantasies, they may initially portray themselves in the role of the victim and then later see themselves as the offender. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Consistent with this line of reasoning, Smallbone and McCabe (2000) found that those sexual offenders whowere themselves sexually abused as children reported a significantly earlier onset of masturbation than did offenders who were not sexually abused. The results of this study by Smallbone and McCabe are consistent with some important links in our theory. Their data revealed that insecure parent-child attachments among sexual offenders led to a marked increase in the likelihood that these boys would be sexually abused, which in turn resulted in an early onset of masturbation among these boys. In addition, because Smallbone and Dadds (2000, in press) had shown that insecure attachments in childhood lead to adult sexual offending, it is reasonable to suggest that there is a pathway involving insecure attachments ® a greater risk to be sexually abused ® heightened sexualization (most particularly masturbation) ® which finally results in adult sexual offending. The next two sections attempt to fill in the gap between early masturbation (i.e., sexualization) and adult sexual offending. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Deviantna spolna aktivnost Abel and Rouleau (1990) found that between 40% and 50% of child molesters and 30% of rapists reported an interest in sexually deviant activities before the age of 18 years, and other reports are consistent with this early origin of deviant sexuality. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) SEX AS A COPING STRATEGY (Spolnost- kot strategija »soočanja« s težavami) Children who are deprived of love and affection, as most sexual offenders seem to have been, are quite likely to turn to self-stimulation as a way of making themselves feel better. Children learn quite early to masturbate and that this behavior is pleasurable (Goldman & Goldman, 1982, 1988; Masters, Johnson, & Kolodny, 1985). Anything that is pleasurable can readily serve as a distraction from problems. If deprived children find solace in masturbation, they will likely soon recognize that this is a way to avoid facing other problems; it will serve as an escape, albeit temporarily, from difficult issues. Because masturbation is a highly reliable source of pleasure guaranteed to divert attention away from difficulties, the criteria for defining an issue as a problem and using masturbation to relieve it can be expected to progressively expand to all manner of problems and to less and less intense problems. As we have seen, sexual offenders display an earlier onset of masturbation and a higher frequency during adolescence, and we consider this to be a result of them having learned to use sex as away of coping initially with their deprived experiences and later with awhole range of issues. Sex can, therefore, be expected to be used as a primary coping strategy by sexual offenders. If we are correct that vulnerable young males who are to become adult sexual offenders find, in masturbating, the comfort and relief they cannot otherwise obtain, then escape from dis-tress by masturbating should be negatively reinforced. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Negativno, pozitivno pogojevanje in spolnost Negative reinforcement, to remind readers, occurs when a behavior leads to the cessation of a noxious state (Skinner, 1969). When a person has a panic attack on an elevator and flees from the elevator at the next floor, the consequent reduction in panic reinforces avoiding elevators, and an elevator phobia may become entrenched. Each time a child or teenaged boy uses masturbation to avoid having to confront a difficult situation or to escape temporarily from distress, he is inadvertently engaging in a procedure that will negatively reinforce masturbation as a response to any and all upsetting events. As a result, masturbation, or for that matter any sexual activity, will become an established coping response. As we have seen, recent evidence suggests that sexual offenders do, indeed, use sex as a coping strategy. However, it is not just that masturbating negatively reinforces the use of sex as a coping strategy. Masturbation clearly induces a pleasurable state, and anything (e.g., the content of fantasies) that is consistently associated with this state is likely to acquire a positive valence. That is, masturbation also functions as a positively reinforcing experience just as the early conditioning theorists claimed. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Negativno, pozitivno pogojevanje, spolnost in vedenje Although the early conditioning theorists (Abel & Blanchard, 1974; McGuire et al., 1965) may have been wrong about the precise outcome of their theory (i.e., the entrenchment of a highly specific sexual preference), they were probably correct in suggesting that the content of masturbatory fantasies had some guiding influence on behavior. These early theorists, however, focused exclusively on the particular sexual elements of the fantasies because they appear to have believed that sexually deviant behaviors were motivated exclusively by sexual desire. Laws and Marshall (1990), on the other hand, describe a far more complex conditioning theory that allows a broader inclusion of stimuli (e.g., the exercise of power and control, the humiliation of the victim, the expression of aggression) into the content of masturbatory fantasies. Any repeated content (whether sexual or otherwise) of masturbatory fantasies is likely to become entrenched as a result of both the negatively reinforcing effects of escape from distress and the positively reinforcing properties of the pleasurable experience of sexual arousal. Very few, if any, human behaviors are motivated by a single desire; almost all behaviors have multiple motives. Sexual activities in particular seem to serve many purposes (Neubeck, 1974). Amongother things, sexmaybe sought to achieve feelings of intimacy or to obtain affection, to alleviate boredom or a sense of frustration (nonsexual), as a way to obtain self-affirmation, to achieve a sense of conquest, or as we have suggested, to escape from problems. Deviant sex may also be driven by or associated with a need to exercise power and control over another person, a chance to explore “forbidden” acts (e.g., anal sex occurs at an unusually high rate in sexual abuse), and as a chance to vent anger or to humiliate someone. Gratuitous physical abuse appears to be common in sexual assaults (Christie, Marshall, & Lanthier, 1979; Marshall & Christie, 1981), and rapists typically indicate that their primary motive in sexually assaulting a female is to degrade and humiliate her as a symbol of either all women or a particular woman who has offended them (Darke, 1990; Marshall & Darke, 1982). Groth (1979; Groth&Burgess, 1977a) has been an advocate of the view that rape is a pseudo sexual behavior. He believes it is the expression of power and the exercise of control over women, rather than the satisfaction of sexual desires, that drives a rapist. To support this account, Groth and Burgess (1977b) point to the fact that sexual dysfunctions occur in the offender quite frequently during rapes, and many of our clients have told us that they are rarely satisfied by the sexual release they obtain from raping a woman. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Doživljanje, čustvovanje Child molesters, on the other hand, appear to experience strong sexual satisfaction from molesting children. This does not mean, however, that other aspects of the abuse are irrelevant. Howells (1979), for example, found that child molesters feel emotionally congruent with children but afraid of potential adult partners and threatened by sex with an adult. Araji and Finklehor (1985) propose in their more general theory that emotional congruence with children is essential for child molesters to offend. One important consequence of this is that child molesters feel in control when they have sex with children, a feeling they do not experience in sex with adults, or for that matter, in most other aspects of their life. This feeling of control is emphasized by the fact that the molester has the power to direct the child to engage in whatever activities he wishes. This power derives both from the fact that he is an adult and children are trained to follow the orders of adults and from the likelihood that the child will feel threatened by amanwhois sexually assaulting him or her. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) We suggest that the higher frequency of masturbatory activities during adolescence increases the likelihood that sexual offenders will incorporate other elements (e.g., forbidden sexual acts, power and control, the expression of aggression) into their sexual fantasies. This may be done to avoid boredom with the same content of frequently repeated fantasies, but it is also likely to occur given their experience as a rejected or neglected child. Such children, as we have seen, lack a sense of self-worth and are deficient in interpersonal skills. They are unlikely, therefore, to find satisfaction in peer-aged appropriate relationships and will turn more and more to fantasy to fulfill their needs. Some of these children, as a result of their feelings of inadequacy, may feel capable of relating to weaker people, and they will thus feel emotionally congruent only with children. Therefore, the sexual fantasies of these adolescents may incorporate children over whom they exercise control and whom they portray in their fantasies as willing and eager to have sex with adults. Other vulnerable adolescents may feel angry about the way their parents have treated them, and they may blame women for their lack of intimate affection and sexuality. We (Garlick, Marshall, & Thornton, 1996) have, indeed, found that some sexual offenders blame women for the loneliness they experience. These adolescents will likely incorporate the expression of their anger into their sexual fantasies. This may take the form of excessive control over the sexual partner in their fantasies and may, over time, becomemore cruel and include elements of humiliation and degradation. It is easy to see how such a process and an associated severe lack of self-confidence might make such a male begin to interpret various behaviors by women as rejection, leading to the conclusion that all women are contemptuous of him. Rapists certainly hold negative and hostile views of women (Burt, 1980; Marshall & Hambley, 1996), and child molesters, aswehave seen, feel emotionally comfortable only with children (Howells, 1979). Wright and Schneider claim that sexual offenders progressively incorporate elements into their sexual fantasies that serve to bolster their self-esteem and justify their sexually offensive behaviors or desires. In an examination of this account, Wright and Schneider found that the fantasies of sexual offenders did, indeed, contain portrayals of victims as compliant, strongly sexually motivated, and as sexually provocative. Although Wright and Schneider did not specifically examine this, it could be that sexual offenders also include elements of violence, degradation, and the enactment of forbidden sexual acts into their fantasies. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) FROM A DISPOSITION TO ENACTMENT Two sets of factors seem to be important: disinhibiting influences and an opportunity to offend. Disinhibiting Factors Araji and Finkelhor (1985) propose that for a man to commit a sexual offense, he must overcome whatever inhibitions he has against these behaviors. Similarly, Barbaree (1990) suggests that sexual arousal to deviant acts occurs only in those men whose constraints against sexual arousal are disinhibited. Intoxication is the disinhibitor most familiar to the ordinary citizen. Alcohol ingestion disinhibits social constraints (Firestone, Keyes, & Korneluk, 1999), encourages the expression of aggression (Bushman & Cooper, 1990), and facilitates sexual arousal (Wilson, 1981). We (Barbaree, Marshall, Yates, & Lightfoot, 1983) demonstrated that alcohol intoxication increased sexual arousal to rape cues in nondeviant males, and it has been found that as many as 50% or more of the sexual offenders were intoxicated at the time of their offense (Amir 1967; Christie et al., 1979; Johnson, Gibson, & Linden, 1978). (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) Kognitivna distorzija in zloraba According to Wright and Schneider’s analysis, sexual fantasies are the primary source of the cognitive distortions (see Ward, Hudson, Johnston, & Marshall, 1997, for a description of these distortions) that clinicians and researchers report so commonly in sexual offenders and that some see as serving to justify and maintain sexual offending. These cognitive distortions, then, feed back into the processes that lead to sexual offending and are elaborated further during masturbatory fantasies. It is the conditioned enhancement of these elements of the fantasies of sexual offenders, rather than the strictly sexual elements, thatwebelieved entrench the disposition to offend. Abel and his colleagues (Abel, Becker, & Cunningham-Rathner, 1984; Abel et al., 1989) have described the typical cognitive distortions of child molesters. These involve a belief that children are sexually disposed, behave sexually provocatively toward adult males, initiate sexual acts with adults, enjoy such activities, and are not harmed by sex with an adult. Abel sees these distortions as serving to justify sexual abuse and as allowing the offender to continue his abusive behavior free of guilt and remorse. More specifically, Hartley (1998) describes how the cognitive distortions of incest offenders serve to overcome internal inhibitions about molesting. In other words, these distortions are thought to function as disinhibitors of child molesting tendencies. Similarly, rapists have been shown to hold views of women and their sexuality that can be expected to facilitate rape (Bumby, 1996; Burt, 1984; Dewhurst, Moore, & Alfano, 1992; Marshall & Hambly, 1996; Scott & Tetreault, 1987). Attitudes condoning violence toward women, feelings of hostility toward women, support for rape myths, and distorted perceptions of women have all been found to be common among rapists. Again, these distortions can be understood as facilitating sexual abuse and justifying continued offending. As such, the distortions of rapists serve to disinhibit constraints against rape. Sundberg, Barbaree, and Marshall (1991) demonstrated that blaming the victim in a rape led men to show significant increases in sexual arousal to rape depictions, thereby demonstrating that victim blame served to disinhibit responses to rape. Anger has been found to precede rapes (Pithers, Beal, Armstrong, & Petty, 1989), and we (Yates, Barbaree, & Marshall, 1984) showed that when angered by females, nondeviant males subsequently displayed very strong arousal to rape scenes. When not angered, these same nondeviant males showed little arousal to rape. Thus, anger toward females served to disinhibit sexual arousal to rape. Similarly, Proulx and his colleagues (McKibbon, Proulx,& Lusignan, 1994; Proulx, McKibbon,&Lusignan, 1996) have shown that various mood states trigger deviant sexual fantasies in sexual offenders whose fantasies are otherwise absent or normal. Looman (1999) essentially replicated Proulx’s findings. Priložnost in zloraba, zlorabljanje However, it is important to note that it is the probability of deviant, rather than normative fantasies and acts that increase in sexual offenders when they are experiencing stress or negative emotions. Once in a disinhibited state, a sexual offender will only offend when he has the opportunity to. Negative mood states appear to trigger deviant fantasies in sexual offenders, which in turn lead them to seize or seek to create an opportunity to offend. Pithers et al. (1989) showed that deviant fantasizing often preceded sexual offending, and Abel and Rouleau (1990) reported that sexual offenders typically developed their deviant sexual fantasies prior to offending. The confluence of the variety of factors we have outlined (i.e., a history of childhood neglect or abuse, the consequent lowered sense of self-worth, an incapacity to meet various needs in prosocial ways, along with the tendency to turn to sexwhenin distress, a conditioned desire for deviant sex, and the presence of a disinhibited state) will lead amale to seek or take advantage of an opportunity to sexually offend. It is clear that some and possibly most sexual offenders deliberately create opportunities to offend. Some do this with full awareness; they plan to offend, manipulate situations to get others out of the way and to get access to a victim alone, and groom victims or coerce them into complying. Others operate at a lower level of awareness, whereby they either actually develop an opportunity to offend or simply to allow events to unfold in a way that produces an opportunity. Ward, Hudson, and Marshall (1995) illustrate how this diminished awareness (called cognitive deconstruction) permits the generation of an opportunity while allowing the offender to maintain that he did not deliberately create the opportunity. We have also seen numerous sexual offenders who have stumbled on an unexpected opportunity to offend. For many of them, these unplanned opportunities oc occurred after their offending had already been established and they simply seized the chance to offend. In some but few, fortuitous opportunities presented themselves as the first chance to offend. In whatever way opportunities occur, the critical point is that however strongly disposed a man is to rape a woman or molest a child, he cannot do so unless an opportunity is present. When an opportunity occurs, the cognitive distortions developed during fantasizing (i.e., that children enjoy sex with adults, that all women secretly desire to be raped) facilitate, along with other disinhibiting influences, the decision to seize the change to offend. Once the man has offended, and particularly if he avoids detection, it is likely that the experience will feed subsequent masturbatory fantasies, further entrenching his disposition to engage in sexually offensive behaviors. No doubt, somemen are so shocked, fearful, or distressed over their initial offense that they do not offend again. This would be most likely to occur in those men for whom the disposition to offend is weakest. For those whose disposition is strong,wecan expect that subsequent to the initial offense, conditioning processes, occurring as a result of masturbating to fantasies of the offense, will further entrench the tendency to sexually offend. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) POSLEDICE TRAVME The psychological sequelae of CSA often persist into adulthood and include general psychological distress, anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and somatization. Difficulties in social and interpersonal functioning, fear and distrust of others, low self-esteem, and feelings of hostility are also common in CSA survivors (for review, see Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny & Follette, 1995). Reactions following sexual assault in adulthood are similar and may manifest in symptoms of depression, fear, anxiety, and PTSD as well as problems with social adjustment and self-esteem (Goodmanet al., 1993; Kilpatrick, Saunders,Veronen, Best,& Von, 1987; Koss, 1993). Women who have been physically assaulted exhibit similar symptomatology including high levels of depression, fear, anxiety, and PTSD-like symptoms including chronic fatigue and tension, intense startle reactions, disturbed sleeping and eating patterns, and nightmares (Goodman et al., 1993; Stark & Flitcraft, 1988). Physical violence also undermines a woman’s sense of trust and has a dramatic impact on her social interactions (Goodman et al., 1993) (The Revictimization of Child Sexual Abuse Survivors: An Examination of the Adjustment of College Women With Child Sexual Abuse, Adult Sexual Assault, and Adult Physical Abuse, CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2000) The available data have revealed that there is a dual effect of adversity including opportunities for growth apart from symptoms (Somerfield & McCrae, 2000). Correlational research has indicated a broad array of changes following negative events, including HIV/AIDS, cancer, marrow transplantation, newborn intensive care, impaired fertility, rape, and child sexual abuse (Affleck, Tennen, & Rowe, 1991; Armeli, Gunthert, & Cohen, 2001; Burt & Katz, 1987; Collins et al., 1990; Curbow, Somerfield, Baker, Wingard, & Legro, 1993; Ebersole & Flores, 1991; Lechman et al., 1993; McMillen, Zuravin, & Rideout, 1995; Mendola, Tennen, Affleck, McCann, & Fitzgerald, 1990; Park, Cohen, & Murch, 1996; Siegel & Schrimshaw, 2000; Thompson, 1985). Longitudinal studies have shown follow-up benefits of positive views following crisis, especially in patient outcomes (Affleck, Allen,Tennen, McGrade,&Ratzan, 1985; Affleck, Tennen, Croog, & Levine, 1987; Bower et al., 1998, 2003; Tennen, Affleck, Urrows, Higgins, & Mendola, 1992). Very fewstudies have examined the positive gains from violent events, in particular those involving killing and massive destruction. A survey found that, mediated by negative religious coping,war trauma contributed to diminished hope inKosovar refugees in the United States (Ai, Peterson,&Huang, 2003). Recent literature has suggested that many of the psychological effects of childhood sexual abuse, such as self-destructive behaviors, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), anxiety, interpersonal difficulties, and sexual dysfunction, may begin with cognitive distortions about the self and the world that become part of a child’s cognitive schema (Smucker, Dancu, Foa, & Niederee, 1995). Further research proposes that these schema distortions may contribute to the emotional distress experienced by many adult survivors of sexual abuse (Briere & Elliott, 1994). For more than two decades, research has consistently found that childhood sexual abuse (CSA) is associated with poor psychological outcome in adult populations. This is especially true of research that has investigated the relationship between mental illness and a history of CSA (e.g., Bryer, Nelson, Miller,&Krol, 1987; Chu&Dill, 1990; Fry, 1993; Mancini,Van Ameringen,& MacMillan, 1995; Ogata et al., 1990). Although no abuse-specific syndrome has been discovered, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomatology and sexualized behaviors are consistently shown to have a strong association with CSA (for literature reviews, see Beitchman et al., 1992; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). Finkelhor, 1986; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). To date, there has been a dearth of empirical investigation on the long-term psychological consequences associated with childhood physical abuse (CPA) (Malinosky-Rummell&Hansen, 1993), although research on the short-term effects of CPA has been extensive (see Ammerman, Cassisi, Hersen, & Van Hasselt, 1986). The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE Research over the past two decades supports the relationship between child abuse and a variety of long-term intrapersonal difficulties including depression, anxiety, substance abuse, anger, dissociation, low self-esteem, and suicidality (Jumper, 1995; Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996). Quite predictably, women with a history of IPV as compared to women who were nontraumatized had higher levels of psychopathology, reported more current relational aggression, and functioned more poorly. Childhood maltreatment also appears to have a significant impact on women’s current functioning. CSA was associated with increased anxiety sensitivity, which has been identified as a risk factor for the development of panic disorder (Schmidt, 1999). Emotional neglect during childhood was associated with more dissociative and depressive symptoms in this sample. Similar findings about emotional neglect have been reported in relation to major depression (Bernet & Stein, 1999), and psychological maltreatment has been linked to self-depreciation (Higgins & McCabe, 2000b). This group also showed a pattern of increasing difficulties with experience of more types of childhood maltreatment. Although childhood physical and sexual abuse have typically received more attention in the literature, these findings underscore the importance of attending to multiple types of childhood maltreatment and to the cumulative effect of such experiences. As with adults, traumatised children exhibit a spectrum of psychological consequences of the trauma, including altered attentional processes, deficits in cognitive systems necessary for learning, inefficient memory systems, deficits in affective responsiveness, and so on. However, there are very few research studies of generic memory in traumatised children. Research has demonstrated that exposure to traumatic events during childhood and adolescence is associated with severe and devastating emotional and behavioral outcomes. While the majority of trauma victims do not go on to develop chronic psychopathology, traumatic events such as child sexual abuse, physical abuse=assault, and witnessing violence increase the risk for the development of posttraumatic stress and other anxiety symptoms (Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003; McLeer, Deblinger, Atkins, Foa, & Ralphe, 1988; McLeer et al., 1998), depression (Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003; Tebbutt, Swanston, Oates, & O’Toole, 1997; Wozencraft, Wagner, & Pellegrin, 1991), suicide attempts (Oddone Paolucci, Genuis, & Violato, 2001), substance abuse (Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003), and delinquent behavior (Kilpatrick, Saunders, & Smith, 2003).These emotional and behavioral difficulties can be associated with significant and lasting disruption in children’s normal development, as well as dysfunction and distress well into adulthood (Beitchman et al., 1992; Briere & Elliott, 1994; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny & Follette,1995; Saunders et al.,1999). Abstract: There are very few re search stud ies that have eval u ated the re lationships be tween mul ti ple forms of child hood mal treat ment and psy chological ad justment in adult hood. This study eval u ates the in ter re la tion ships be tween five dif fer ent types of child mal treatment (sex ual abuse, phys ical abuse, psy cho log ical mal treat ment, ne glect, wit ness ing fam ily vi o lence) in a community sam ple of women and men (N = 175). The re la tion ships be tween the re ported ex perience of these forms of mal treat ment in child hood, fam ily char ac ter is tics dur ing child hood, and cur rent psy cho log i cal ad justment (trauma symptomatology and self-de preciation) were as sessed. As hy poth e sized, fam ily char ac ter is tics pre dicted mal treat ment scores and ad justment, and mal treat ment scores pre dicted adjustment af ter con trol ling for fam ily en vi ron ment. There were high cor re la tions be tween scores on the five mal treat ment scales. Re sults high light the need to as sess all forms of mal treatment when look ing at re lationships of mal treatment to adjustment and the im por tance of child hood fa mil ial en vi ronment for the long-term ad justment of adults. Multitype mal treat ment refers to the coex is tence of one or more mal treat ment types (Hig gins & McCabe, 1998, in press-b). A review of stud ies in which adult respon dents reported hav ing expe rienced mul tiple forms of child mal treatment showed that multitype mal treat ment is gen er ally asso ci ated with greater impair ment than sin gle forms of child mal treat ment (Higgins & McCabe, in press-b). Researchers have now begun to high light the impact of multitype mal treatment and the impor tance of assess ing more than one form of mal treat ment (e.g., Rorty, Yager, & Rossotto, 1994; Sanders & Becker-Lausen, 1995). Two major issues have been iden ti fied recently by research ers: (a) the comorbidity of mul tiple forms of child abuse and neglect and (b) the poten tial inde pendent or interactional con tri bu tion of fam ily fac tors. How ever, even when research ers acknowl edge the poten tial con tribution of other fac tors to the adjust ment prob lems observed in mal treated pop u la tions, they tend to look at only one of these two issues. The con tri bu tion of the cur rent study is its dual focus: the role of both fam ily fac tors and all forms of child maltreatment to the adjust ment prob lems of adults. A large pro por tion of the child mal treatment lit erature is focused on the del eterious effects of sex ual abuse. REZULTATI: The results of this study sup ported the hypoth eses that fam ily char ac ter is tics would pre dict both reports of child hood mal treatment and cur rent adjust ment; the degree of mal treating behav iors reported would pre dict adjust ment; mal treat ment would pre dict adjust ment after con trol ling for fam ily envi ron ment; and that there would be a high degree of over lap between reports of expe riencing sex ual abuse, phys i cal abuse, psy cho log i cal mal treat ment, neglect, and wit ness ing fam ily vio lence. Paren tal sex ual puni tiveness (the fre quency with which adults reported that their par ents had responded puni tively or neg a tively with regard to sex ual issues and behav iors) stood out as an impor tant pre dictor of all types of child mal treat ment except for wit ness ing fam ily vio lence. Expe ri ences of mal treat ing behav iors in child hood were asso ciated with both trauma symptomatology and self-depre ciation in adult hood. Sex ual abuse and psy cho log i cal mal treatment were the types of mal treatment most strongly related to trauma symp tomatology and self-depre ci a tion. These asso ci a tions were still pres ent even after allow ing for fam ily back ground fac tors. Con sis tent with the find ings of some research ers (e.g., Nash, Hulsey, Sex ton, Harralson, & Lambert, 1993; Nash, Neimeyer, Hulsey, & Lam bert, 1998), this sug gests that child mal treatment is not just an expres sion of a neg a tive fam ily envi ronment but is also an inde pendent source of trauma with long-term negative cor relates. The results indi cated that there was a high degree of over lap between adults’ reports of sex ual abuse, physical abuse, psy chological mal treat ment, neglect, and wit ness ing fam ily vio lence. Prob lems tend to occur together. Children who are rid iculed or sub jected to ver bal attacks are also likely to be phys i cally pun ished or harmed, have their phys i cal or emo tional needs neglected, and wit ness vio lence being directed toward other mem bers of the fam ily. Con sistent with the find ings of Bernstein et al. (1994), all mal treat ment types were strongly asso ciated with each other. Fam ilies in which chil dren expe rience mal treat ing behav ior are more likely to be char ac ter ized by paren tal sex ual punitiveness toward chil dren, low fam ily cohe sion and adapt abil ity, and poor qual ity interparental rela tion ships; how ever, paren tal divorce did not pre dict child mal treat ment. Studies from a variety of literature support the premise that avoidance and escape behaviors play fundamental roles in the development and maintenance of PTSD and trauma-related problems. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy in the Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder SUSAN M. ORSILLO, SONJA V. BATTEN - BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION, Vol. 29 No. 1, January 2005 95-129 Proces prilagoditve v normalni situaciji, ki je za nas stresna The restriction in the capacity for conscious information processing, and the greater prominence given to processing by an automatic, imaginal memory system, would appear to be advantageous. In normal circumstances, with the resumption of safety, flashbacks operate to transfer information from one memory system to the other, and rapidly decline in frequency after a few hours or days. Proces prilagoditve v situaciji, ko reagiramo s PTSD In PTSD, however, this process seems to be blocked, either because of too great a discrepancy between the contents of the VAM and SAM systems, or because the flashbacks are too aversive and have to be avoided. The absence of detailed verbally accessible memories prevents the inhibition of flashbacks, and high levels of negative emotions such as guilt and shame can, additionally or alternatively, prevent the inhibition of conscious thoughts and memories concerning the trauma. Further progress in developing what are already moderately effective treatments for PTSD is likely to depend on an integration of our knowledge about the phenomenology, underlying cognitive mechanisms, and neurobiology of the disorder. Although some individuals who have experienced traumatic life events do not seem to suffer from acute or chronic psychological distress, most individuals do report a range of psychological symptoms (e.g., anxiety, fear, phobic responses, and depression) following a traumatic experience (Classen, Koopman, & Spiegel, 1993; Neville & Heppner, 1999). Research has indicated that factors that influence an individual’s reaction to trauma include the nature and severity of the event and the individual’s ability to cope with stress and trauma in his or her life (e.g., Snyder & Ford, 1987). In fact, in the past 20 years, a substantial body of research indicates that applied problem solving and coping play a crucial adaptive role in dealing with stressful life events and often mediate or moderate the relationship between stress and both psychological and physical health (e.g., Heppner & Hillerbrand, 1991; Heppner& Lee, 2002; Heppner, Witty, & Dixon, 2004; Snyder & Ford, 1987; Summerfeldt & Endler, 1996; Zeidner & Endler, 1996). (P. Paul Heppner and Mary J. Heppner, Development and Validation of a Collectivist Coping Styles Inventory, Journal of Counseling Psychology, 2006, Vol. 53, No. 1, 107–125) POSLEDICE TRAVME – REVIKTIMIZACIJA IN KOMPULZIVNO PONAVLJANJE Recent studies have noted a relationship between the experience of CSA and an increased vulnerability for assaults in adulthood (for review, see Messman & Long, 1996; Polusny & Follette, 1995). Several factors appear to heighten a woman’s vulnerability to revictimization. Factors such as learning processes (Wheeler & Berliner, 1988), denial (Roth, Wayland, & Woolsey, 1990), substance abuse (Briere & Runtz, 1987), low self-esteem (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Jehu & Gazan, 1983), dissociation (Sandberg, Matorin, & Lynn, 1999), sexual attitudes (Smith, Whealin, Davies, & Jackson, 1996), learned helplessness (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Peterson & Seligman, 1983;Walker&Browne, 1985), and choices regarding relationships (Jehu & Gazan, 1983) may contribute to increased risk for adult assaults. Studies of revictimization with community women suggest rates of revictimization ranging from 37% to 68% (Gorcey, Santiago, & McCall-Perez, 1986; Russell, 1986; Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992). In one such study, CSA survivors were 2.4 times more likely than nonvictims to be revictimized as adults (Wyatt et al., 1992). Current findings do suggest that the revictimization of CSA survivors is associated with increased psychological distress. Sexual revictimization has been explained as resulting from poor risk recognition in women who have been previously victimized (Breitenbecher, 2001; Gold, Sinclair,&Balge, 1999). …studies suggest that women with a history of sexual assault may have difficulties responding effectively to sexual assault risk, rather than in recognizing it. studies suggest that revictimization is the result of inappropriate or ineffective behavioral responses to risk rather than risk recognition (Meadows et al., 1997; Naugle, 1999). VanZileTamsen et al. … women hold about sources of harm and their susceptibility to that harm determine how they interpret environmental stimuli and prepare to respond. In social interactions with male acquaintances (e.g., at parties or on dates), risk perception processes compete withwomen’s goals for entertainment, friendship, and intimacy, which dominate working cognitive processing. In these situations, sexual advances from a known perpetrator will likely be interpreted as sexual interest rather than aggression. Women may either miss or dismiss threat-related cues while focusing on having fun, finding a potential partner, or maintaining a current relationship (Livingston & Testa, 2000; Norris, Nurius, & Dimeff, 1996; Nurius, 2000). (The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005) …findings offer support for previous research suggesting thatwomen are less likely to appraise a situation as risky when the perpetrator is known (Hickman & Muehlenhard, 1997; Nurius, 2000). Women are more likely toperceive rape-related threat when the perpetrator is someone with whom they do not have an expectation of sexual intimacy. This effect of degree of intimacy on behavioral responses is largely mediated through appraisal. Hence, by failing to recognize advances from an acquaintance as sexual assault threat, women fail to engage in direct resistance, which is most likely to thwart the advances. The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005) - unexpected finding was that women are more likely to use indirect resistance when the perpetrator is a friend or date, regardless of the degree of risk perceived in the situation. The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005) …this finding is consistent with the fact that behavioral responses are guided not just by risk appraisal or assertiveness but by concerns for the relationship, as well. Women in such situations may be ambivalent. On one hand, they may not want to engage in sexual activity; however, they may be focused on preserving the relationship or saving the man’s feelings (Livingston & Testa, 2000; Norris et al., 1996) and, thus, respond nonassertively and less effectively. The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005) Another barrier to direct resistance involves the social embarrassment of accusing an acquaintance of sexual assault. Research has shown that people commonly blame the victim in situations of acquaintance sexual assault believing that the woman must have done something to encourage the man’s advances (Hammock & Richardson, 1997). The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005) … results suggest that women can resist unwanted sexual advances through the development of assertiveness skills and effective resistance strategies; however, responses to acquaintance sexual assault depend on the interpretation of the situation according to schemata that are heavily influenced by relation- ship to the perpetrator. Substantial empirical evidence suggests that women with a prior experience of sexual assault are at greater risk for sexual victimization than women without this experience. Across studies, rates of sexual victimization in adulthood range from 28% to 38% for women not sexually victimized in childhood, whereas childhood sexual assault survivors’ rates of adult victimization range from 48% to 66% (Banyard, Williams, & Siegel, 2001; Maker, Kemmelmeier, & Peterson, 2001). Approximately two thirds of adult victims of sexual assault report a history of earlier victimization (Arata, 2002; Stermac, Reist, Addison,&Millar, 2002; Urquiza & Goodlin-Jones, 1994). In a review of sexual revictimization literature, Arata (2002) concludes that girls who are sexually victimized in childhood are 1.5 to 2.5 times more likely to be sexually assaulted in adolescence or adulthood than their nonvictimized peers. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530 research suggests that revictimized women have poorer long-term psychological and emotional outcomes than their singly victimized or nonvictimized counterparts (Arata, 1999b; Banyard et al., 2001; Maker et al., 2001). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Mostprominently, adult victims of sexual assault who have histories of child sexual abuse have significantly higher levels of posttraumatic stress symptoms than nonsurvivors or than women with child-only or adult-only victimizations (Arata, 1999b; Gidycz et al., 1993; Maker et al., 2001). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Additionally, women victimized by different perpetrators at different time points have been shown to suffer greater levels of depression and anxiety than women victimized only in childhood or only in adulthood (Banyard et al., 2001; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001; Gidycz et al., 1993). Previously victimizedwomen take longer to recover froma subsequent assault, experience more postassault PTSD symptomatology, and use less effective coping methods to heal (Arata, 1999a; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Revictimization may also exacerbate long-term sequelae such as substance use and diminished physical health, although little literature to date has specifically examined the impact of multiple sexual assaults on these factors. Increased likelihood of alcohol and other drug abuse has been consistently linked with childhood sexual assault (Briere & Runtz, 1993). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Numerous researchers have suggested that the sheer accumulation of traumatic experiences is responsible for the increased psychological distress found among revictimized women. Women who experience sexual assault are at greater risk of experiencing nonsexual traumas both in childhood and adulthood (Banyard et al., 2001; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000; Stermac et al., 2002). Increasing numbers of experiences of child sexual abuse, adult sexual assault, and adult partner violence were accompanied by concomitant increases in anxiety, depression, and posttraumatic symptoms. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Alternatively, impaired psychological functioning resulting from trauma may impede women’s self-protective capacities in the face of later assault threats. Subsequent revictimization then further erodes psychological wellbeing. Forgetting or repressing a sexually assaultive experience may play a role in this cycle.Women victimized by multiple perpetrators were more likely to forget some or all of their initial assault experience, and forgetting was a marginal predictor of sexual revictimization. Forgetting may contribute to vulnerability by preventing a woman from actively processing her experience, from challenging self-blame or other self-defeating schemas, and from developing coping skills that reduce vulnerability. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) it appears that earlier experiences of sexual abuse create an initial vulnerability that is exacerbated by subsequent childhood or adolescent victimizations. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530 The age at which initial sexual assaults occur and their accompanying severity may increase women’s vulnerability by exacerbating the psychological impact of an early victimization experience. Evidence suggests that psychological distress, more generally, and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (PTSD), in particular, are likely mechanisms through which revictimization vulnerability builds. PTSD-related symptoms have been shown to moderate the relationship between early assault experiences and revictimization (Sandberg, Matorin, & Lynn, 1999) as well as the severity of childhood sexual abuse experiences and revictimization in adulthood (Arata, 2000). Thus, for women with a history of sexual victimization, high levels of current PTSD symptomatology can exacerbate vulnerability, decrease selfprotective capacity, or may constitute a vulnerability that potential perpetrators seek out and exploit (Messman-Moore & Long, 2003) (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) A complementary framework for understanding the link between experiences of child sexual abuse and vulnerability to revictimization is offered by Finkelhor and Browne (1985). These authors theorized that the experience of sexual abuse damages a young person’s self-concept and worldview through “trauma-genic dynamics,” which include a sense of betrayal, powerlessness, stigmatization, and traumatic sexualization. Vulnerability to reassault is posited to be exacerbated by psychological and emotional impact consistent with these dynamics. Aspects of an initial assault or its aftermath that intensify its psychological or traumatic effect may therefore increase risk for revictimization partially through the presence of traumagenic impact or previously mentioned posttraumatic symptoms. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Finally, the presence of nonsexual trauma during childhood can also increase young women’s risk of sexual revictimization. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530) Thus, young people whose early environments are characterized by risk of exposure to multiple types of trauma appear to be at greater risk of increased vulnerability. The nature of the environment’s response to disclosures of abuse can also impact young people’s vulnerability. Research has consistently demonstrated that supportive responses to a child’s disclosure of sexual abuse are associated with more positive mental health outcomes and more rapid healing (Everson, Hunter, Runyon, Edelson, & Coulter, 1989; Gries et al., 2000). Additionally, negative reactions fromformal or informal helping systems have been associated with poorer mental health outcomes following an assault (Filipas & Ullman, 2001). The nature of and reaction to help seeking by victims therefore appears to impact postassault functioning and, by extension, risk of exposure to repeated sexual victimization. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530). The results suggest that singly victimized women differ from women with repeat victimizations both in terms of the characteristics of their initial sexual assault and with respect to the long-term psychological and health consequences of their traumatic experiences. Both survivors of ongoing abuse and survivors of multiple assaults by different perpetrators were more likely to be younger at the time of their first assault and to experience more severe initial assaults (characterized by injury or degree of threat). Within these differences, a trend emerged in which victims of multiple perpetrators were even younger and experienced a higher number of severe aspects of victimization than women who were repeatedly victimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Additionally, women victimized by multiple perpetrators were more likely to have ever forgotten some or all of their initial assault experience and to find the response of informal supports unhelpful in response to disclosure compared to their singly victimized counterparts. Women victimized by multiple perpetrators experienced significantly more nonsexual traumas during their lifetime than either singly victimized women or survivors of ongoing abuse. Additionally, multiply victimized women experienced more types of sexually assaultive acts during the course of their lives than women revictimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Thus, although both groups of repeatedly victimized women may have more severe initial experiences than singly victimized women, women hurt by multiple offenders appear to face an added layer of exposure to both sexual and nonsexual trauma. This differential exposure to trauma is further reflected in the findings related to the long-term outcomes of women with different assault experiences. Consistent with expectations, women victimized by multiple perpetrators were struggling with significantly more current posttraumatic-stress-related symptoms, more depression symptoms, and poorer self-rated health than both the singly victimized group and the ongoing victimization group. Additionally, the multiple perpetrator victimization group was the only group of sexually assaulted women to have significantly higher past month drug use than nonsexually victimized respondents. Taken together, these results suggest that although repeated victimization by the same perpetrator is associated with greater psychological impact than a single assault experience, a new assault by a different perpetrator may be a more damaging form of revictimization than ongoing abuse by the same offender. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530). Rather than being independent traumatic events, a number of studies have found that women with a history of child sexual abuse are at increased risk for adult victimization compared to women who do not report histories of child sexual abuse (Ellis, Atkeson, & Calhoun, 1982; Fromuth, 1986; Miller et al., 1978; Urquiza & Goodlin-Jones, 1994). Women who have been sexually abused in childhood appear to be at greater risk for revictimization as adults. In a community sample of 930 women, Russell (1986) found that approximately 60% of the women who had been abused in childhood also reported being raped or having experienced attempted rape after the age of 14 compared with 35% ofwomen with no history of childhood abuse. A prospective study of 857 female college students conducted by Gidycz and colleagues (Gidycz, Coble, Latham, & Layman, 1993) examined the relationship between childhood sexual abuse and revictimization in adolescence and adulthood and showed that childhood sexual abuse predicts revictimization in adulthood. A recent metaanalysis of 38 studies confirmed that women with histories of childhood sexual abuse are more likely to be revictimized as adults (Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996). In addition to these problems, previous research has revealed a link between CSA and increased vulnerability for assaults in adulthood (Browne& Finkelhor, 1986; Messman & Long, 1996; Polusny & Follette, 1995). This phenomenon is called revictimization and may occur in the form of adult sexual assault, physical abuse, or psychological maltreatment. Several factors associated with CSA may increase a woman’s vulnerability to revictimization. CSA may result in learned maladaptive behaviors, beliefs, and attitudes and a failure to learn adaptive behaviors (Wheeler & Berliner, 1988). This may result in inappropriate dating and sexual behavior, acceptance of rape myths, and sex-role stereotypes in adulthood. Low self-esteem (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Jehu & Gazan, 1983), learned helplessness (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Peterson & Seligman, 1983), and relationship choices (Jehu &Gazan, 1983) may also contribute to revictimization. Other psychological difficulties associated with CSA may also be important. Both dissociation and substance abuse have been linked to CSA (Briere, 1992). It has been theorized that CSA survivors (CSAS) use substances to avoid unpleasant affect and memories of the childhood abuse that may place them at risk for subsequent assault (Briere, 1992; Polusny & Follette, 1995). Studies with college and clinical samples find that CSAS are more likely than nonvictims to experience sexual victimization as adults (Briere & Runtz, 1987; Gidycz, Hanson, & Latham, 1995; Koss & Dinero, 1989; Urquiza & Goodlin-Jones, 1994). One study found that 32.1% of CSAS experienced adult victimization compared with 13.6% of nonvictims (Gidycz, Coble, Latham,&Layman, 1993). Studies of revictimization with communitywomen reveal rates of revictimization ranging from 37% to 68% (Gorcey, Santiago,&McCall-Perez, 1986;Wyatt, Guthrie,&Notgrass, 1992). Fergusson, Horwood, andLynskey (1997) found that CSAS were 11 times more likely than nonvictims to experience rape or attempted rape. This studywas designed to overcome shortcomings of previous studies. First, a well-operationalized definition ofCSAsimilar to those used by other researchers and commonly used assessment measures to detect adult abuse experiences were employed. A large sample was obtained with an appropriate comparison group. The study of sexual revictimizationwas improved by examining unwanted sexual contact with acquaintances and strangers as well as by investigating the method of coercion involved. A dimensional measurement of unwanted sexual contact (i.e., fondling, oral-genital contact, and penetration by objects in addition to intercourse) also strengthened this investigation, as it is the first study to examine these experiences. This study extends previous findings by examining all three forms of revictimization simultaneously. Evidence here suggests that revictimization not only occurs as unwanted sexual contact but also as physical and psychological abuse. Research is needed to explain howand why revictimization occurs.With a few exceptions (e.g., Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Polusny & Follette, 1995; Walker & Browne, 1985; Wheeler & Berliner, 1988), theoretical conceptualizations of revictimization are lacking. Information regarding methods of coercion, especially verbal coercion and use of alcohol and drugs as well as physical force, may become important in developing such theories. Given the results from this study, it appears that CSAS are more vulnerable to verbal coercion or pressure from individuals in authority. This may be because the experience of CSA has instilled fear of authority figures. Previous CSA may have resulted in the development of certain coping strategies, such as “going along” with uncomfortable experiences, as the adoption of these strategies has in the past minimized physical harm. These strategies, however, may actually increase the woman’s risk for additional victimization. Research regarding the psychological adjustment of women who are revictimized is also needed. One may speculate that revictimized women will experience poorer psychological adjustment than nonvictimized women or women with only adult victimization experiences (see, e.g., Follette, Polusny, Bechtle, & Naugle, 1996). Revictimization further increases the risk of psychopathology for survivors of childhood abuse. Combat veterans with PTSD were more likely to have a history of CPA than those without PTSD (Bremner, Southwick, Johnson, Yehuda, & Charney, 1993). Battered women with PTSD had a higher rate of CSA(but not CPA) than batteredwomen without PTSD (Astin, Ogland-Hand, Coleman, & Foy, 1995). Survivors of combined CSA and CPA who were subsequently retraumatized (by rape, domestic violence, or criminal victimization) had significantly higher rates of PTSD than those without a history of childhood abuse or with CSA alone (Breslau, Chilcoat, Kessler, & Davis, 1999; Molnar, Buka, & Kessler, 2001; Rodriguez, Ryan, Rowan, & Foy, cited in Rodriguez et al., 1998). Retraumatization of those with a history of CSA as compared to traumatization only in adulthood has also been associated with more dissociative symptoms, alexithymia, lifetime suicide attempts, and interpersonal problems (Cloitre, Scarvalone,&Difede, 1997). Sexual abuse in childhood is a major risk factor for later sexual revictimization (Chu, 1992; Koss & Dinero, 1989) and may lead to greater sexual problems (Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992) as well as other trauma symptoms (Koverola, Proulx, Battle, & Hanna, 1996). These trauma symptoms among women sexually abused in childhood may be exacerbated by recent life stressors, such as problems at work or in the family (Koopman, Gore-Felton, & Spiegel, 1997). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290) For a substantial minority of women, however, revictimization occurs. Numerous studies have found that women with a history of sexual victimization are at greater risk for future victimization (Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman, 1995; Himelein, 1995; Humphrey & White, 2000; Nishith, Mechanic, & Resick, 2000). It has been speculated that such prior victimization may reduce a woman’s ability to appraise risk and to set appropriate boundaries (Nishith et al., 2000). Indeed,Wilson, Calhoun, and Bernat (1999) found that women with a victimization history exhibited poorer risk recognition (i.e., judging when a man’s sexual advances place her at risk).Women with a history of repeated sexual victimization took longer to indicate potential danger in an audiotaped date rape scenario. These women appear to experience a delay in recognizing sexually aggressive behaviors that may pose a threat. (The Role of Sexual Victimization in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64) Abstract: Lifetime trauma histories were ascertained for females with confirmed histories of childhood sexual abuse and comparison females participating in a longitudinal, prospective study. Abused participants reported twice as many subsequent rapes or sexual assaults (p = .07), 1.6 times as many physical affronts including domestic violence (p = .01), almost four times as many incidences of self-inflicted harm (p = .002), and more than 20% more subsequent, significant lifetime traumas (p = .04) than did comparison participants. Sexual revictimization was positively correlated with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (PTSD), peritraumatic dissociation, and sexual preoccupation. Physical revictimization was positively correlated with PTSD symptoms, pathological dissociation, and sexually permissive attitudes. Selfharm was positively correlated with both peritraumatic and pathological dissociation. Competing theoretical explanations for revictimization and self-harm are discussed and evaluated. Research over the past decade has documented a prospective link between rape and subsequent revictimization in short-term follow-up studies of adult victims (e.g., Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman, 1995; Kilpatrick, Acierno, Resnick, Saunders, &Best, 1997). The link between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent victimization that occurs later in adolescence or adulthood is less well understood. A growing body of research has documented associations between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent sexual victimization (see Messman&Long, 1996, and Briere&Runtz, 1987, for reviews; see also Arata & Lindman, 2002; Chu & Dill, 1990; Kessler & Bieschke, 1999; Koss & Dinero, 1989; Merrill et al., 1999; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000) and between childhood sexual abuse and laterphysical victimization including domestic violence (Arata, 1999; Collins, 1998; Gilbert, El-Bassel, Schilling, & Friedman, 1997; Krahe, Scheinberger-Olwig, Waizenhofer, & Kolpin, 1999; McClosky, 1997; Messman & Long, 1996). Otherstudies have documented higher rates of self-abuse or self-harm in childhood sexual abuse victims (Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Romans, Martin, Anderson, Herbison, & Mullen, 1995; Van der Kolk, Perry, & Herman, 1991; Winchel & Stanley, 1991; Yeo & Yeo, 1993). Further, it appears that the co-occurrence of multiple types of child maltreatment (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, child neglect) puts children at considerable risk for revictimization in adulthood (Briere, Woo, McRae, Foltz, & Sitzman, 1997; Dutton, Burghardt, Perrin, Chrestman, & Halle, 1994; Hillis, 2001). We operationally define victimization (either sexual or physical) as harm perpetrated by an outside source that serves as a reenactment of the initial abuse. Self-harm, on the other hand, implies a direct reenactment inflicted by the survivor herself and represents a certain internalization of the trauma. Therefore, self-harm is not considered a category of revictimization but will be studied as a separate and distinct phenomenon. REZULTATI: Analyses showed that, compared to nonabused participants, sexually abused participants were twice as likely to have been raped or sexually assaulted, almost fourtimes as likely to have inflicted subsequent self-harm (in the form of suicide attempts or self-mutilation), reported significantly higher rates of physical revictimization (including domestic violence), and reported a greater number of significant subsequent lifetime traumas than comparison participants. When alternative forms of childhood maltreatment were taken into account, childhood sexual abuse was a unique predictor of self-harm. Concurrent pathological dissociaton was shown to be predictive of physical victimization when in the company with variables from several theoretically distinct domains. These results indicate that a persistent reliance on dissociation as a coping mechanism can place participants at increased risk for physical harm. Thus, victims who adopt pathological dissociation as the primary defense strategy in adolescence or adulthood may be less able to engage in self-protection when physically threatened. Dissociation has been thought to be associated with suicide and self-injurious behaviors, and these results confirm this association (Brodsky, Cloitre, & Dulit, 1995). Self-harm may not be a direct response to sexual abuse but to the dissociative experiences that result from efforts to cope with the abuse. Results also indicate that being sexually active orbelie ving that sexual activity is permissible can increase one’s vulnerability for physical victimization. The incidence of self-harm in sexual abuse victims was quite dramatic. Being sexually abused was, by far, the strongest predictor of self-harm even when in company with other forms of child maltreatment. Abstract: This study investigated the relationship between the severity of childhood trauma and proneness to victimization in adulthood in a sample of 155 Australian women. A tendency for both violent and nonviolent revictimization was observed. The classical “repetition compulsion” theory of revictimization is less able to accommodate these findings than theories that implicate an effect of childhood abuse on self-concept. Additionally, the factors of peritraumatic dissociation, coping styles, and attachment styles were examined as possible intervening variables in the revictimization relationship. The findings of the study suggest that some coping styles mediate the relationship between childhood abuse and victimization in adulthood, whereas aspects of attachment styles may serve to moderate this relationship. Severe abuse and neglect in childhood can have diverse effects on functioning in adulthood, but one effect that has received the increasing attention of researchers in recent years is proneness to revictimization. That is, adults with a history of childhood trauma appear to have an enhanced likelihood of becoming victims of various types of violence such as rape and other sexual assaults, physical assault, battery, and domestic violence (Cloitre, Tardiff, Marzuk, Leon,&Portera, 1996; Messman&Long, 1996; Sappington, Pharr, Tunstall, & Rickert, 1997). The classical explanation of revictimization appeals to the psychodynamic notion of a “repetition compulsion” (Freud, 1920/1955). According to this account, people who fail to accommodate to a traumatic experience may be subconsciously driven to reenact that experience in an endeavor to achieve a sense of mastery over the original trauma. The theory can be criticized for its imputation that if abuse victims actually “want to be revictimized,” the perpetrators of violence against these people can be absolved from at least some personal responsibility for their actions. The issue of apportioning blame nevertheless has no bearing on the validity of the theory. Although the repetition compulsion theory continues to have its advocates (Chu, 1992; van der Kolk, 1989) contemporary commentators on revictimization tend to interpret the phenomenon as overdetermined, that is, as reflecting several distinct processes in varying degrees (Sandberg, Lynn, & Green, 1994). Among the processes that might be pivotal in this context are dissociative mechanisms, coping styles, and attachment styles. OBRAMBNI MEHANIZMI: Dissociation is one of the most primitive of defense mechanisms. As children develop, they tend to acquire more sophisticated coping styles by virtue of which they may eventually be able to accommodate to past traumas. Thus, child abuse survivors who have refused to dwell on their abusive experience or who have positively reframed their experience do tend to be more resilient (Himelein & McElrath, 1996) and thereby may be less liable to revictimization, although the reverse might be the case for those who rely on an escapist coping style (Proulx, Koverola, Fedorowicz, & Kral, 1995). A study by Myall and Gold (1995), however, failed to find support for coping style as a mediator between childhood sexual abuse and sexual victimization in adulthood. Nonetheless, there is scope for examining this issue in relation to childhood trauma and revictimization as more broadly conceived. REZULTATI: The sample of Australian women evidenced the revictimization phenomenon in a relatively broad context, namely, in relation to a range of childhood trauma and various instances of victimization in adulthood. Abstract: Recent investigations of risk factors for adult sexual assault have focused on a varietyof behavioral and cognitive variables, including victim risk-taking behaviors. In this study, cognitive appraisals of riskyactivities, behavioral intentions to engage in risk-taking behaviors, and alcohol use were examined in relation to future involvement in risk-taking behaviors and the incidence of sexual assault in a sample of college women. At Time 1, 50 (26%) participants reported a historyof sexual victimization and at Time 2, 16 (12.7%) reported new sexual victimizations. Discriminant function analysis indicated that alcohol use and expected involvement in risky activities at Time 1 were associated with new sexual victimizations at Time 2. Hierarchical regression analysis revealed that alcohol use and expected involvement in risky activities at Time 1 were predictive of frequencyof involvement in riskyse xual activities at Time 2. The implication of these findings for future research is discussed. Routine activities theory provides an overarching framework for howcertain behaviors may be associated with increased risk of assault. This theory proposes that activities involving greater exposure to potential assailants are associated with increased risk of victimization (Miethe & Meier, 1990; Mustaine & Tewksbury, 1998). Moreover, risk may be greater in certain social settings, such as bars, where alcohol is consumed and aggression is more likely (Parks & Miller, 1997). Research has shown that a combination of behavioral factors (e.g., alcohol use) and leisure activities (e.g., going to the mall, eating out) are associated with increased risk of criminal victimization among collegewomen (Mustaine&Tewksbury, 1998). Therefore, exposure to potential assailants serves as a risk factor for future sexual victimization, particularly in interpersonal contexts where a woman is likely to be perceived as a vulnerable target (e.g., Parks & Miller, 1997). Although routine activities theory predicts that exposure to “strangers” is salient, the application of this theory to acquaintances is particularly relevant in the case of sexual assault, given that the majority of sexual assaults are perpetrated by someone known to the victim (Koss, Dinero, Seibel, & Cox, 1988). In addition to risk of assault associated with exposure to potential perpetrators in specific contexts, research has demonstrated that certain behaviors and activities are associated with increased risk of victimization. Studies investigating the phenomenon of revictimization have examined sexual risktaking behaviors as potential risk factors. Theorists have suggested that sexual risk-taking behaviors, such as promiscuous and unprotected sexual activities, may increase risk for sexual assault (Koss & Dinero, 1989). Koss and Dinero (1989) hypothesized that Finkelhor and Browne’s (1985) concept of traumatic sexualization may explain increased sexualized behaviors among previously victimized women. More frequent sexual activity, an earlier age of onset of sexual intercourse, and a higher number of sexual partners have been found to strongly correlate with sexual assault (Alexander & Lupfer, 1987; Koss & Dinero, 1989; Mayall & Gold, 1995; Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992). Furthermore, victims report greater involvement in sexual risk-taking behaviors in comparison to nonvictims, including promiscuous and indiscriminate sexual activities as well as unsafe sexual behaviors that place them at risk for contracting sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) and human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) (see Polusny & Follette, 1995, for a review). Alcohol use has also received considerable attention as a risk factor for sexual victimization (Koss & Dinero, 1989; Muehlenhard & Linton, 1987; Testa & Parks, 1996). Alcohol use has been found to correlate with risk for sexual assault in retrospective studies (Koss & Dinero, 1989), to be more prevalent among sexual assault victims (Stewart, 1996; Testa & Dermen, 1999), and to predict future victimizations (Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman, 1995). REZULTATI: Of the study variables, alcohol use and behavioral intentions to engage in risk-taking behaviors were strongly related to new victimizations and engagement in risky sexual activities. Alcohol use consistently played a strong role in the predictive analyses andwas found to relate to both the occurrence of new victimizations and frequency of involvement in risky sexual activities. Moreover, the quantity of alcohol use was an important factor in that newly victimized women reported more than 3 times as many average binge-drinking days at Time 1 in comparison to women who were not victimized during the study. In addition, behavioral intentions to engage in risk-taking behaviors were strongly associated with future risk-taking behavior, providing support for the theory of reasoned action. Substantial empirical evidence suggests that women with a prior experience of sexual assault are at greater risk for sexual victimization than women without this experience. Across studies, rates of sexual victimization in adulthood range from 28% to 38% for women not sexually victimized in childhood, whereas childhood sexual assault survivors’ rates of adult victimization range from 48% to 66% (Banyard, Williams, & Siegel, 2001; Maker, Kemmelmeier, & Peterson, 2001). Approximately two thirds of adult victims of sexual assault report a history of earlier victimization (Arata, 2002; Stermac, Reist, Addison,&Millar, 2002; Urquiza & Goodlin-Jones, 1994). In a review of sexual revictimization literature, Arata (2002) concludes that girls who are sexually victimized in childhood are 1.5 to 2.5 times more likely to be sexually assaulted in adolescence or adulthood than their nonvictimized peers. Additionally, research suggests that revictimized women have poorer long-term psychological and emotional outcomes than their singly victimized or nonvictimized counterparts (Arata, 1999b; Banyard et al., 2001; Maker et al., 2001). FAKTORJI, KI POJASNJUJEJO RANLJIVOST: Zlorabe v otorštvu- začetna starost, težavnost napada, skupna teža psihološkega pritiska PREDICTORS OF SEXUAL REVICTIMIZATION Factors such as age, severity, and mental health consequences may linkwomen’s early victimization experiences to later vulnerability to new sexual assaults. Age at an initial sexual assault experience has received attention within the revictimization literature, with somewhat mixed results. Some research suggests that women who are first sexually assaulted during childhood are at greater risk of subsequent victimization than women first victimized during adolescence (Maker et al., 2001). Alternatively, child sexual abuse can increase vulnerability to new victimizations during adolescence, which, in turn, increases risk of exposure to sexual assault in adulthood (Gidycz, Coble, Latham, & Layman, 1993; Humphrey & White, 2000). Other research has found no effect for age at first assault (Jankowski, Leitenberg, Henning, & Coffey, 2002). On the whole, it appears that earlier experiences of sexual abuse create an initial vulnerability that is exacerbated by subsequent childhood or adolescent victimizations. Severity of initial assault experiences is also suggested to impact risk of revictimization. Early victimizations characterized by greater degrees of threat, force, and invasiveness may differentially predict revictimization above the experience of sexual abuse alone (Arata, 2000; Collins, 1998; Irwin, 1999). Furthermore, some evidence suggests that seriousness of initial experiences creates risk for more severe later assault experiences (Humphrey & White, 2000). In a prospective study of college women, Gidycz et al. (1993) found that severity of sexual assaults during childhood and adolescence predicted the severity of revictimization in early adulthood. Similarly, Mayall and Gold (1995) found that narrower definitions of child sexual abuse, including only physical contact forms of assault, were predictive of revictimization, whereas more broad conceptualizations of child sexual abuse were not. The age at which initial sexual assaults occur and their accompanying severity may increase women’s vulnerability by exacerbating the psychological impact of an early victimization experience. Evidence suggests that psychological distress, more generally, and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (PTSD), in particular, are likely mechanisms through which revictimization vulnerability builds. PTSD-related symptoms have been shown to moderate the relationship between early assault experiences and revictimization (Sandberg, Matorin, & Lynn, 1999) as well as the severity of childhood sexual abuse experiences and revictimization in adulthood (Arata, 2000). Thus, for women with a history of sexual victimization, high levels of current PTSD symptomatology can exacerbate vulnerability, decrease selfprotective capacity, or may constitute a vulnerability that potential perpetrators seek out and exploit (Messman-Moore & Long, 2003). RAZVOJ RANLJIVOSTI ZA PONOVNO SPOLNO TRAVMATIZACIJO Pojasnjevalni model za razvoj ranljivosti na podlagi zgodnje travme A complementary framework for understanding the link between experiences of child sexual abuse and vulnerability to revictimization is offered by Finkelhor and Browne (1985). These authors theorized that the experience of sexual abuse damages a young person’s self-concept and worldview through “traumagenic dynamics,” which include a sense of betrayal, powerlessness, stigmatization, and traumatic sexualization. Vulnerability to reassault is posited to be exacerbated by psychological and emotional impact consistent with these dynamics. Aspects of an initial assault or its aftermath that intensify its psychological or traumatic effect may therefore increase risk for revictimization partially through the presence of traumagenic impact or previously mentioned posttraumatic symptoms. FAKTORJI, KI POJASNJUJEJO RANLJIVOST: Prisotnost multiplih travm v otroštvu Finally, the presence of nonsexual trauma during childhood can also increase young women’s risk of sexual revictimization. Revictimized women are more likely to report neglect or physical abuse by caretakers in childhood, witnessing parental violence in childhood, and physical violence by a dating partner during adolescence than singly or never-victimized women (Banyard et al., 2001; Collins, 1998; Stermac et al., 2002). Thus, young people whose early environments are characterized by risk of exposure to multiple types of trauma appear to be at greater risk of increased vulnerability. The nature of the environment’s response to disclosures of abuse can also impact young people’s vulnerability. Research has consistently demonstrated that supportive responses to a child’s disclosure of sexual abuse are associated with more positive mental health outcomes and more rapid healing (Everson, Hunter, Runyon, Edelson, & Coulter, 1989; Gries et al., 2000). Additionally, negative reactions from formal or informal helping systems have been associated with poorer mental health outcomes following an assault (Filipas & Ullman, 2001). The nature of and reaction to help seeking by victims therefore appears to impact postassault functioning and, by extension, risk of exposure to repeated sexual victimization. LONG-TERM OUTCOMES OF REVICTIMIZATION Almost as consistent as the finding that previously victimized women are at greater risk of sexual assault is evidence that multiply victimized women have worse psychological outcomes than their nonvictimized or singly victimized counterparts. Most prominently, adult victims of sexual assault who have histories of child sexual abuse have significantly higher levels of posttraumatic stress symptoms than nonsurvivors or than women with child-only or adult-only victimizations (Arata, 1999b; Gidycz et al., 1993; Maker et al., 2001). Additionally, women victimized by different perpetrators at different time points have been shown to suffer greater levels of depression and anxiety than women victimized only in childhood or only in adulthood (Banyard et al., 2001; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001; Gidycz et al., 1993). Previously victimized women take longer to recover froma subsequent assault, experience more postassault PTSD symptomatology, and use less effective coping methods to heal (Arata, 1999a; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001). Diminished psychological health appears to be connected specifically to multiple interpersonal traumas, such as sexual assault; noninterpersonal traumas, such as serious illness or accidents, do not generate the level of psychological distress present for many sexually revictimized women (Green et al., 2000). Revictimization may also exacerbate long-term sequelae such as substance use and diminished physical health, although little literature to date has specifically examined the impact of multiple sexual assaults on these factors. Increased likelihood of alcohol and other drug abuse has been consistently linked with childhood sexual assault (Briere & Runtz, 1993). PRIMERJAVA ZNAČILNOSTI ENKRAT NASPROTI VEČKRAT SPOLNO NAPADENIM ŽENSKAM (s ponavljajočim ali z večimi različnimi napadalci) The results suggest that singly victimized women differ from women with repeat victimizations both in terms of the characteristics of their initial sexual assault and with respect to the long-term psychological and health consequences of their traumatic experiences. Both survivors of ongoing abuse and survivors of multiple assaults by different perpetrators were more likely to be younger at the time of their first assault and to experience more severe initial assaults (characterized by injury or degree of threat). These results echo the findings of previous studies that connect revictimization to initial assault severity and earlier victimizations (Arata, 2000; Humphrey & White, 2000; Maker et al., 2001). Within these differences, a trend emerged in which victims of multiple perpetrators were even younger and experienced a higher number of severe aspects of victimization than women who were repeatedly victimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Additionally, women victimized by multiple perpetrators were more likely to have ever forgotten some or all of their initial assault experience and to find the response of informal supports unhelpful in response to disclosure compared to their singly victimized Even more marked were the results related to the cumulative exposure to trauma throughout time.Women victimized by multiple perpetrators experienced significantly more nonsexual traumas during their lifetime than either singly victimized women or survivors of ongoing abuse. Additionally, multiply victimized women experienced more types of sexually assaultive acts during the course of their lives than women revictimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Thus, although both groups of repeatedly victimized women may have more severe initial experiences than singly victimized women, women hurt by multiple offenders appear to face an added layer of exposure to both sexual and nonsexual trauma. Within the multivariate framework in this analysis, only a younger age at the time of an initial sexual victimization and exposure to physical abuse in childhood emerged as significant predictors of sexual victimization by different perpetrators throughout time. Thus, women whose early environments are characterized by both physical and sexual trauma risks may be most vulnerable to subsequent sexual assaults. Additionally, the univariate relationship between membership in the multiple perpetrator victimization group and more unhelpful responses from informal sources of support to disclosures of an initial sexual assault experience suggests that these repeatedly victimized women’s early environments may be more likely to be generally unsupportive of young women’s safety These findings echo previous research in highlighting the importance of early remedial intervention and attention to the presence or lack of supports in a young person’s environment. Victimized youth in environments characterized by risk of exposure to multiple types of violence or trauma may be at elevated risk for revictimization and the compounding psychological impact of multiple traumas (Banyard et al., 2001; Follette et al., 1996; Green et al., 2000). Recent work points to the impairing effects of early violence exposure on victims’ self-concept and identity health, which, in turn, carry cognitive consequences for subsequent psychological well-being and life functioning (Kellogg, Hoffman, & Taylor, 1999; Nurius, Casey, Lindhorst, & Macy, 2004). Definicija ponavljanja For the purposes of this paper, repetition phenomena are defined as contemporaneously observed or reported reactions, manifested as behaviors, feelings, cognitions, memories, or physical sensations, expressed on their own or in combination, that involve some degree of reexperiencing of significant past events (e.g., intrusive reexperiencing of trauma, recreation of trauma, transference, recurrent dreams, and acting out). Kompulzivno ponavlanje Psychodynamic theory links contemporaneous reactions to formative influences, the origins of which may lie in the distant past and of which a person may have no conscious recall. Reactions observed in therapy, or reported by patients, may repeat in substantial detail the behaviors, cognitions, and affects associated with particular events. Some repetition phenomena are referred to as acting out, recreation, or reenactment. They may be manifest as simple momentary responses or highly elaborate sequences of reactions that recreate and reenact complex traumatic and developmentally disruptive experiences. Ponavljanje in PTSD These particular repetitions are recognised by DSM-IV (APA, 1994) and ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) symptom lists for PTSD but without reference to notions of acting out, recreation, and reenactment. For instance, a person may “: : : act or feel as if some aspect of the trauma were recurring : : :” (DSM-IV, B3), and may experience “: : : intense psychological distress at exposure to internal or external cues : : :” (DSM-IV, B4). These are valuable operational definitions for acting out, reexperiencing, and recreation under conditions where the precipitating trauma is known. Ponavljanje travme v sanjah In support of more individually tailored perspectives these authors refer to research indicating that the repetition of trauma in dreams takes at least three different and distinct forms: anxiety dreams with trauma-related content, traumatic nightmares, and traumatic reenactment. Each of these forms of dreaming is known to be mediated by different neurophysiological and psychophysiological processes (Mellman, Kulick-Bell, Ashlock, & Nolan, 1995; Shalev, Orr, & Pitman, 1993) Anksiozne sanje Patients with acute stress disorder or PTSD (APA, 1994) precipitated by a single recent traumatic event of moderate severity are likely to experience dreams containing images that repeat aspects of what happened. The phenomenology of these dreams is unlikely to differ significantly from anxiety dreams reported by individuals who do not have PTSD. Characteristically, such dreams do not waken up the dreamer; they occur during REM sleep phases; and dream episodes are recalled at the end of a major sleep period. Although dream images can be distressing, manifest dream content is amenable to interpretation, review, and discussion that promote insight and therapeutic progress as well as lead to a reduced frequency of repetitions over time. Travmatične nočne more Traumatic nightmares, on the other hand, share some of the phenomenological features of anxiety dreams but, in other respects, are quite different. These are terrifying repetitive dreams of reexposure to traumatic events. They are known to occur during both REM and non-REM sleep, and, in chronic PTSD, they are often persistent, intrusive, and treatmentresistant. Nonetheless, Schreuder (1996) argues that traumatic nightmares can contain symbolic representations of anxieties rooted in pretrauma and posttrauma phases of a person’s life. Typically, such anxieties concern existential issues, especially those involving threat to life, threat of abandonment and death. Podoživljanje travme med spanjem Posttraumatic reenactment during sleep defines an extreme end point of the continuum of dream repetitions. It is characterized by a subjective impression of reliving traumatic experiences. Schreuder (1996) describes these repetitions as exact and explicit recreations of traumatic incidents. Unlike other types of dreams, they constantly recur in a form that is largely unaltered and unelaborated, and they appear to be impervious to the effect of time. Anxieties and sensory perceptions experienced during posttraumatic reenactment repeat the most evocative reactions originally provoked by initial traumatic experiences. Triggers for posttraumatic reenactment are typically unrelated to anxieties associated with earlier developmental life phases, existential threats, or current reality-based conflicts or worries. The initial trauma that is reexperienced remains an isolated experience that has not been processed, and the reexperiencing has no symbolic or other relationship with anxieties and conflicts in later life (Schreuder, 1996). Dinamična psihoterapija, psihotravmatologija in ponavljanje As indicated above, the field of dynamic psychotherapy has a long and distinguished tradition of recognizing, both in theory and in clinical practice, the subtle, compelling, diverse, and compulsive nature of repetition phenomena. Psychotraumatology has, from a perspective of positivistic science, confirmed that significant past life experiences do evoke reactions that repeat some aspects of formative experiences. It is also clear that conscious recall of precipitating events is not a precondition for repetitions, but some representation of the experience in memory is. Manifestacije ponavljanja It must be accepted that repetitions manifest as a compulsion to acting out, recreation, transference, reenactment, and dreaming share some phenomenological features but, on closer examination, they are revealed to be much more diverse than generally realized. Any therapist treating patients with PTSD, or its related disorders, has to be clear about the conceptual implications of the differentiating phenomenological features of reported repetitions, and to plan treatment accordingly. Similarly, researchers will do well to recognize that all repetitions are not the same and should not be studied as if they were. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PRIMERJAVA RAZLIČNIH TRAVM During the past 20 years, we have learned how similarly harmful are experiences of terror, violence, and abuse, whether they occur on the combat field or at home. The field of family violence has gained much fromthe field of traumatic stress, and collaborations between these two previously separate fields have yielded important new answers, as well as new research questions. The field of traumatic stress is poised to integrate, more fully than in the past, a variety of aspects of trauma such as social betrayal, as well as outcomes of trauma such as depression, criminality, and physiological harm that go beyond posttraumatic stress. The field of family violence has much to offer in this process. The battered woman syndrome characterized for the first time the trauma symptoms of women in battered women’s shelters (Walker, 1983). As understanding of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among Vietnam veterans grew, it was becoming clear that women exposed to violence and terror in their homes responded in much the same asVietnam veterans exposed to the violence and terror of war. As Herman (1992) explained, Only after 1980, when the efforts of combat veterans had legitimized the concept of posttraumatic stress disorder, did it become clear that the psychological syndrome seen in survivors of rape, domestic battery, and incest was essentially the same as the syndrome seen in survivors of war. (p. 32) Multiple Abuse Experiences Several methodological limitations exist in empirical efforts directed at evaluating the impact of child abuse. The majority of studies target the correlates of childhood sexual abuse, with only moderate research interest in the outcome of physical abuse and psychological abuse (Claussen & Crittenden, 1991; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1996). Additionally, the failure to assess for the co-occurrence of various types of abuse or the impact of experiencing several types of abuse concurrently is even more problematic. Those studies that have assessed for the presence of multiple types of abuse (e.g., sexual, physical, and psychological) have found that they often co-occur and that different types of abuse are associated with unique patterns of adult symptomatology (Briere & Runtz, 1990; Claussen & Crittenden, 1991; Elliott & Briere, 1994; Moeller, Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993; Ney, Fung, &Wickett, 1994;Wind & Silvern, 1992). For example, when assessing the impact of childhood physical, sexual, and psychological abuse on adult functioning, Briere and Runtz (1990) found that psychological abuse was associated with low self-esteem, sexual abuse with dysfunctional sexual behavior, and physical abuse with anger/aggression. In another sample, Mullen et al. (1996) found trends for unique abuse–outcome relationships between sexual abuse and sexual difficulties, emotional abuse and low self-esteem, and physical abuse and marital difficulties. Additionally, all types of abuse were associated with increased psychopathology, interpersonal problems, sexual difficulties, and decreased self-esteem. Thus, both common and unique symptomatic responses to child abuse may occur. Investigators have also found that individuals who experienced multiple types of abuse report greater symptomatology than do individuals who experienced a single type of abuse (Mullen et al., 1996;Wind&Silvern, 1992). Although the results have not been consistent across studies with respect to the pattern and magnitude of symptomatology reported, it appears that different types of abuse, considered separately and in combination, have differential impact on adult psychological functioning. These studies support the necessity of concurrent assessment of multiple types of child abuse. POSLEDICE TRAVME – ZDRAVJE Childhood victimization not only is prevalent but also is associated with negative long-term psychological and physical health problems. Documented psychological problems include posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, anxiety, somatization, substance abuse, eating disorders, personality disorders, and suicidal behavior (Beitchman et al., 1992; Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Briere & Runtz, 1990; Brown & Anderson, 1991; McCauley et al., 1997; Miller,Downs, Gondoli,&Keil, 1987; Polusny&Follette, 1995; Romans, Martin, Anderson, Herbison,&Mullen, 1995; Rowan&Foy, 1993; Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, Kilpatrick, & Veronen, 1992; Silverman, Reinherz, & Giaconia, 1996; Thompson, Kaslow, Lane, & Kingree, 2000; Wyatt, 1985; Zlotnick, Zakriski, Shea, & Costello, 1996). Poor social and academic outcomes have also been documented among survivors of childhood victimization (Kaplan, Pelcovitz,&Labruna, 1999; Polusny&Follette, 1995). Twenty years following the abuse, female victims were significantly more likely than were nonvictims to evidence aggressive behaviors (Weiler & Widom, 1996), posttraumatic stress disorder (Widom, 1999b), substance abuse (Widom & White, 1997), poorer academic and intellectual outcomes (Perez & Widom, 1994), and personality disorders (Widom, 1999a). Research also has focused on the long-term physical health outcomes of childhood victimization. Most of these studies have been based on clinical samples and thus cannot be generalized to the population (Drossman et al., 1990; Harrop-Griffith et al., 1988; Kimerling & Calhoun, 1994; Lechner, Vogel, Garcia-Shelton, & Leichter, 1993; Leserman et al., 1996; Moeller, Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993). These studies found that women who had experienced childhood maltreatment (sexual abuse, physical abuse, emotional abuse, emotional neglect, or physical neglect) had significantly higher median annual health care costs, lower perceptions of their overall health, greater physical and emotional functional disability, a greater number of physical health symptoms, and a greater number of health risk behaviors than did women with no history of maltreatment. REZULTATI: Using data from a nationally representative sample of 8,000 women, we found that physical and sexual victimization experienced in childhood were associated with several health problems in adulthood. Physical and sexual childhood victimization showed similar associations to the health measures. Both physical and sexual victimization in childhood were significantly associated with perceived general health, serious injury, chronic mental health condition, and drug use, but neither physical nor sexual victimization was associated with chronic physical health conditions. Although physical victimizationwas associated with daily alcohol use but not miscarriages or stillbirths, sexual victimization was associated with miscarriages or stillbirths but not daily alcohol use. Several mediating variables may explain why childhood victimization is associated with health problems in adulthood. One possibility is that the association is mediated by psychological variables, such as depression. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA There is an increasing recognition that sexually abused adolescents show a heterogeneity of consequences (Bennett, Hughes,&Luke, 2000;Bal,VanOost, De Bourdeaudhuij, & Crombez, 2003). Anxiety, depression, dissociative complaints, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), anger, delinquency, and sexual problems are some of the most reported symptoms in these adolescents (Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). Not only the relationship of the abuser to the child but also the functioning of the family can contribute to symptom variety. Considerable evidence indicates that a cohesive, supportive family environment may serve as a buffer against the negative effect of sexual abuse (Ray&Jackson, 1997). Until now, little research focused on family functioning in families with an extrafamilial sexually abused adolescent. The aim of this studywas to look at differences in trauma-specific symptoms and family functioning in intra- and extrafamilial sexually abused adolescents. In a follow-up study of young adults abused or neglected as children, almost 80% of the sample failed to meet criteria for successful psychosocial functioning (McGloin &Widom, 2001). A longitudinal community study of young adult abuse survivors found approximately the same proportion meeting clinical criteria for one or more psychiatric disorders (Silverman, Reinherz, &Giaconia, 1996). Child abuse has been linked with some of the most severe and intractable psychiatric and social problems, including borderline personality disorder, dissociative identity (multiple personality) disorder, suicidality, substance abuse, sociopathy, and violence (Herman, Perry, &van der Kolk, 1989; Johnson, Cohen, Brown, Smailes, &Bernstein, 1999; Kluft, 1996; National Research Council, 1993). The inner protector model provides an elegant explanation for a range of problems experienced by abuse survivors. The phenomenon of dissociative identity disorder (DID) is particularly instructive. More than 90% of North Americans diagnosed with DID report histories of child abuse (see Kluft, 1996, for a review). Because of the severity of their symptoms and the extreme abuse they report, "clinicians in the dissociative disorder field, by consensus, regard DID as the paradigmatic example of the psychological response to severe, chronic childhood trauma" (Ross, 1996, p. 16). Patients with this disorder experience a fragmented identity in which distinct personality states (alters), often separated by amnesic barriers, take executive control of their behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The three most common alter types are inadequate or confused protectors, terrified children, and persecutors who act out violently and drive patients to injure themselves (Putnam, 1989; Ross, 1997). These alter types closely match the three internal roles proposed here. A common clinical symptom among abuse survivors is depression, characterized by low selfesteem, guilt, and intense shame (Ammerman et al. , 1986; Browne &Finkelhor, 1986). It is best understood, I believe, not as the expression of a single role but of interaction among all three. Child abuse is also associated with disorders of dissociation in later life (Briere, 1992). Generally speaking, theoretical, clinical, and empirical studies concur that men’s childhood sexual abuse may present varied long-term outcomes. The three major ones boil down to the following: (a) some of these men manifest in adulthood various symptoms or disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder, behavioural problems, or emotional problems (Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Fondacaro, Holt, & Powell, 1999; Gold, Lucenko, Elhai, Swingle,&Sellers, 1999; Holmes, Offen,& Waller, 1997); (b) others manifest specific pathologies of a sexual nature, such as pedophilia, adult sexual aggression, or other types of paraphilia (Lenderking et al., 1997; Lodico, Gruber, & Diclemente, 1996; McCellan, McCurry, & Ronnei, 1997); and (c) certain men manifest no major pathology in adulthood (Finkelhor, 1990; Laumann, Gagnon, Michael,&Michaels, 1994; Okami, 1991). It is important to note that the first two groups are not independent of one another and may overlap. Internalizing problems, such as anxiety, depression, dissociative complaints and problems related to posttraumatic stress disorder, and externalizing symptoms, such as sexual problems and anger, are among the most frequently reported symptoms (Bal, Crombez, Van Oost, & De Bourdeaudhuij, 2003; Wolfe & Birt, 1997). In their review of longitudinal and follow-up studies on child and adolescent sexual abuse, Kendall-Tackett et al. (1993) concluded that for one half to two thirds of all children and adolescents, postabuse symptoms decreased with time, whereas 10% to 24% of symptoms intensified. However, this pattern of recovery seemed to be different for different symptoms. In their follow-up study with sexually abused children and adolescents, Gomes- Schwartz, Horowitz, Cardarelli, and Sauzier (1990) found that anxiety problems tended to decrease, whereas problems of anger and sexual preoccupation seemed to persist or worsen. This is consistent with Mannarino, Cohen, Smith, and Moore-Motily (1991), who found that at 6- and 12-month followups, sexually abused children improved significantly on internalizing problems but not on externalizing problems. Other studies, however, did not find significant improvements in symptomatology with time. Sexual assault is associated with psychological morbidity including depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and anxiety (Ackerman, Newton, McPherson, Jones, & Dykman, 1998; Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995; Ellis, 1983; Roth & Lebowitz, 1988; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). Long-term effects of child sexual abuse include suicidal behavior, personality disturbances, substance abuse, eating disorders, and revictimization (Brier & Runtz, 1987; Chandy, Blum, & Resnick, 1996; Dube et al., 2001; McCauleyet al., 1997; Wonderlich et al., 2001). (The Experience of Sexual Assault. Findings From a Statewide Victim Needs Assessment) Although fewer than half of all chil dren exposed to sin gle-inci dent trau mas suf fer posttraumatic symptomatology (PTSD symp toms) suf fi cient to war -rant a clin ical diag nosis of PTSD (Green et al., 1994;LaGreca et al., 1996; Nader et al., 1990), chil dren sub jected to mal treatment (Cuffe et al., 1998; Famularo et al., 1994; Neumann et al., 1996; Steiner et al., 1997), dev as tat ing emo tional loss (Appelbaum & Burns, 1991; Green et al., 1994; Nader et al., 1990; Winje & Ulvik, 1998), or to the cumu la tive effect of mul tiple trau mas (Cuffe et al., 1998; Neumann, et al., 1996; Steiner et al., 1997) are at high risk for PTSD. Sev eral ODD etiologic fac tors, forexam ple, pov erty, fam ily con flict, and paren tal psychopathology (Biederman, Newcorn, & Sprich, 1991; Frick, Lahey, Loeber, Stouthamer-Loeber, 1992; Web ster-Stratton, 1996), also are risk fac tors for PTSD (Flisher et al., 1997; Green et al., 1994; Steiner et al., 1997). Our find ings indi cate that not only vic timization trauma in gen eral (Ford et al., 1999) but more specifically trau matic phys i cal and sex ual mal treatment are prev a lent among chil dren diag nosed with ODD and sec ond arily among chil dren diag nosed with ADHD. Children with a diagnosis of ADHD were never theless at risk for past exposure to mal treatment trauma but less so than children diagnosed with ODD: 25% had been exposed to physical mal treat ment, and one in nine had been exposed to sexual mal treatment. ADHD is heterogeneous with regard to psychiatric mor bid ity, with most severe impair ment associated with antisocial families (Faraone, Biederman, & Milberger, 1995). Child Mal treatment, Other Trauma Ex po sure, and Posttraumatic Symptomatology Among Children With Oppositional De fi ant and At tention Def i cit Hy peractivity Dis or ders Julian D. Ford (University of Con necticut School of Med icine), Robert Racusin, Cynthia G. Ellis, Wil liam B. Daviss, Jessica Reiser, Amy Fleischer (Dartmouth Med i cal School), Julie Thomas (Youngs town State Uni ver sity). Research on the impact of sexual violence, whether in childhood or later in the life course, suggests that sexual assault is associated with a number of short- and long-term mental health consequences including depression, PTSD, and substance abuse (Campbell & Soeken, 1998, 1999; Cascardi, Riggs, Hearst-Ikeda, & Foa, 1996; Jasinski,Williams, & Siegel, 2000; Ullman & Brecklin, 2002). Research by Boudreaux, Kilpatrick, Resnick, Best, and Saunders (1998) suggests that sexual assault is more strongly related to PTSD and major depressive episodes as compared to other types of violent victimization. Violence, particularly sexual violence, also increases the risk for substance abuse, alcohol dependency, problem drinking, and alcohol-related difficulties (Clark&Foy, 2000; Downs & Harrison, 1998; Jasinski et al., 2000; Lurigio, 1987). For example, Harrison, Fulkerson, and Beebe (1997) identified the earlier initiation of substance use among victims who were children and adolescents. Not only are adolescentswith a history of sexual abuse at increased risk formultiplesubstance use (Harrison et al., 1997) but also girls who are abused are significantly more likely to have alcohol or drug arrests in adulthood (National Institute of Justice, 1995). Alcohol and drugs may be used by victims in an attempt to cope with the trauma of violence, alleviating the symptoms and anxiety associated with victimization, thereby increasing feelings ofmastery and control (Banaji & Steele, 1989; Flannery, Singer, Williams, & Castro, 1998; Runtz & Schallow, 1997). Saunders et al. (1999) suggested that given the higher rates of depressive symptomatology associated with violent victimization alcohol abuse is potentially a coping strategy for those dealing with the symptoms associated with various mental health disorders. This suggests that victims may use alcohol and drugs as a form of self-medication (Jasinski et al., 2000; Spatz Widom, Ireland, & Glynn, 1995). POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PTSD Empirical findings indicate that the avoidance criterion of the DSM-IV diagnosis of PTSD (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994) may be too exclusive. Across study groups, the percentage of participants meeting the reexperiencing criterion or the hyperarousal criterion is much greater than the percentage meeting the avoidance criterion (e.g., Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993; Schutzwohl & Maercker, 1997). Thus, it appears likely that, by using the criteria of the DSM-IV, some people with otherwise genuine PTSD symptoms are excluded from the diagnosis because they do not exhibit the required three avoidant symptoms (Davidson& Foa, 1993b). Perhaps the most prominent definition of partial PTSD was proposed by Blanchard et al. (1994, 1995). According to this definition, partial PTSD is diagnosed if the minimum number of symptoms for the reexperiencing criterion, and either the avoidance criterion or the hyperarousal criterion are met. Giaconia et al. (1995) have maintained that PTSD interferes with and impairs adolescent psychosocial functioning and places them at a higher risk for other psychological disorders. Anger, rage, absence of feeling, impulsive behavior, or attention problems are associated with PTSD, but these symptoms can be misdiagnosed and seen as conduct disorders, attention deficit disorders, depression, or dissociative disorders (Terr, 1991). Failure to accurately diagnose PTSD could interfere with successful rehabilitation (Cauffman et al.). Pathological dissociation has not been examined adequately within the female juvenile offender population. Pathological dissociation in adolescents can interfere with the construction of a sense of self; it can also contribute to their failure to integrate their sense of self and their ability to integrate sexual, aggressive, and relational feelings (Armstrong, Putnam, Carlson, Libero, & Smith, 1997). A diagnosis of PTSD emphasizes the unusual nature of a stressor followed by a pattern of distressing physical and psychological responses. Characteristic features of PTSD include reexperiencing the traumatic event, emotional numbness or avoidance, and increased arousal (American Psychiatric Association, APA, 1994). Since the diagnosis of PTSD emphasizes both the stressor and patterns of response, PTSD theoretically can occur in any person placed in similar circumstances, thereby alleviating individual pathology or blame (Ochberg, 1991; Walker, 1991). Stressor Criterion PTSD may occur as a result of traumatic events that have either natural or human origins (APA, 1994). The traumatic event or sequence of events triggering a posttraumatic response is “... overwhelming, and dangerous to one’s self...” (Figley, Scrignar, & Smith, 1988, p. 113). Events arising from intentional human action tend to be more severe and destructive than those of natural origin. Events that are human in origin are characterized by deliberateness, negligence, or malice that destroys trust and security in human relationships (Green, 1990; Karl, 1989; Ochberg, 1991; Silvern & Kaersvang, 1989). Debate exists in the literature about classifying the ongoing, deliberate acts of battering of women by male partners as a traumatic event that may be responsible for the development of PTSD (Campbell, 1990; Figley, 1992; Kemp, Rawlings, & Green, 1991; Walker, 1991). Possible traumatic events noted for PTSD are a serious threat or harm to one’s life or physical integrity and prolonged physical or sexual abuse (APA, 1994). Yet, there is confusion with the stressor criterion in that the boundaries of a traumatic event are not always clear. Researchers have been interested in the phenomenon of human responses to traumatic events since the beginning of the century. Over the past several decades, posttraumatic stress research has extended beyond combat veterans to include victims of rape or other violent acts (Burgess & Holmstrom, 1974; Saunders, Arata, & Kilpatrick, 1990), incest (Goodwin, 1985; Herman, 1993; Lindberg & Distad, 1985), child abuse (Green, 1985; Terr, 1990), and natural disasters (Frederick, 1985; Murphy, 1986; Steinglass &Gerrity, 1989; Weinrich, Hardin, & Johnson, 1990). Goldberg and associates (1990) confirmed this assertion noting that PTSD was a normal response to abnormal stress and not a reflection of underlying pathology. Using a matched comparison group of 2092 identical twins who were combat veterans, Goldberg et al. found that the severity and length of PTSD symptoms, which ranged from 15 to 30 years in some instances, were related to the intensity of the conflict experienced. Furthermore, reexperiencing the trauma, through intrusive thoughts, nightmares, and flashbacks, comprised the most significant cluster of PTSD symptoms. The frequency and severity of traumatic experiences was cited as a major contributor to PTSD symptomatology across several trauma survivor groups (Foy & Card, 1987; Gallers, Foy, Donahoe, & Goldfarb, 1988; Greenwald & Leitenberg, 1990; Kilpatrick, Saunders, AmickMcMullen, Best, & Veronen, 1989; McLeer, Deblinger, Atkins, Foa, & Ralphe, 1988; Solkoff, Gray, & Keill, 1988; Wolfe, Gentile, & Wolfe, 1989). Many theoretical accounts of PTSD have emphasized the centrality of intense fear as the emotional experience associated with symptoms of intrusive memories, avoidance of eventrelated stimuli, and increased hyperarousal (Lee, Scragg, & Turner, 2001). Recent research on trauma has implicated shame in the development of PTSD symptoms for rape survivors, war veterans, and adult victims of violent crime with an abuse history (Andrews, Brewin, Rose, & Kirk, 2000; Jaycox, Zoellner, & Foa, 2002; Fontana & Rosenheck, 1994). Although there is considerable variation depending on gender, race, and the type of trauma, it has been estimated that 5–11% of trauma victims will develop posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (7). PTSD is an anxiety disorder including three symptom clusters: reexperiencing the trauma through nightmares, flashbacks, or intrusive memories; autonomic hyperactivity, such as exaggerated startle response, night sweats, and irritability; and avoidance symptoms, including social isolation, restricted range of emotion, and absence of intimacy in relationships (8). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a recognized psychiatric experience following an external traumatic event (Mezey & Robbins, 2001). This diagnostic category describes common symptoms experienced by individuals highly exposed to an event involving death or injury, with resultant fear and helplessness. PTSD occurs more often in women, people with more direct, traumatic disaster exposure, and those with a history of psychiatric illness. PTSD is highest in populations exposed to extreme violence, such as prisoners of war, concentration camp victims, and crime victims, suggesting that disasters caused by human aggression cause more psychological damage than natural disasters. Of the three categories of diagnostic PSTD symptoms, the numbness symptom cluster is particularly diagnostic of a more severe reaction: Feeling numb, experiencing emotional withdrawal, or avoiding all reminders of the event (McMillen, North, & Smith, 2000; North et al., 1999; Shariat, Mallonee, Kruger, Farmer, & North, 1999; Ursano, Fullerton, Kao,&Bhartiya, 1995). The remaining two symptom clusters include: (1) re-experiencing the traumatic event or uncontrollable intrusive thoughts about the event and (2) arousal symptoms, which describes a hyper-alert state exhibited by being easily startled, increased irritability, difficulty concentrating, or sleep disturbances. Both intrusionand hyper-arousal reactions have been found to be almost universal reactions to trauma, beginning in the hours and days following an event and often lasting months, particularly following disasters (McMillen, North, & Smith, 2000; North et al., 1999; Shariat et al., 1999 Areviewof the studies of PTSD symptomatology in children finds that about 25% to 40% of children of all ages exposed to traumatic events fit diagnostic criteria for PTSD, according to the DSM-IV (Fletcher, 1996). (ALYTIA A. LEVENDOSKY, ALISSA C. HUTH-BOCKS, MICHAEL A. SEMEL, DEBORAH L. SHAPIRO Trauma Symptoms in Preschool-Age Children Exposed to Domestic Violence, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 2, February 2002 150-164) Furthermore, in this study, symptoms of PTSD were associated with increased severity of the four types of violence. In particular, symptoms of reexperiencing were positively associated with all four types of violence and symptoms of hyperarousal were positively associated with all but the severe violence category. These findings mirror studies on older children (GrahamBermann & Levendosky, 1998b) and battered women (Kemp et al., 1995) finding that more abuse toward the mother is associated with higher levels of trauma symptoms in both children and their mothers. Thus, it is not merely the presence of violence in the parental relationship but also the extent and frequency of it. The lack of association of avoidant symptoms with any of the violence types lends further support to the hypothesis that the avoidant symptoms are not as reflective of trauma in this age group. (ALYTIA A. LEVENDOSKY, ALISSA C. HUTH-BOCKS, MICHAEL A. SEMEL, DEBORAH L. SHAPIRO Trauma Symptoms in Preschool-Age Children Exposed to Domestic Violence, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 2, February 2002 150-164) Following traumatic violence, about one third of individuals develop posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a syndrome characterized by reexperiencing the traumatic event, the use of avoidant coping strategies to cope with traumatic memories, emotional numbing, and hyperarousal (American Psychiatric Association, 2000;World Health Organization, 1992). Reexperiencing of the traumatic event may present as recurrent flashbacks or nightmares and intensification of distress when exposed to reminders of the event.With avoidance, cognitive and behavioral strategies are used to avoid thoughts, feelings, activities, or situations associated with the trauma. Attempts to avoid external situations associated with the trauma may lead to a constricted lifestyle. As understanding of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among Vietnam veterans grew, it was becoming clear that women exposed to violence and terror in their homes responded in much the same asVietnam veterans exposed to the violence and terror of war. As Herman (1992) explained, DEFINICIJA TRAVME Clarifying our definition of trauma should be one of our primary goals for the next 10 years. At present, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSMIV, American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for PTSD drew heavily on aspects of terror.We now know that few traumatic events that cause longlasting harm involve solely or even mostly terror. Sexual abuse can be highly terrifying, as in the case of most stranger rape, or involve virtually no immediate fear for life, as in the case of children who are groomed by perpetrators to view the abuse as acceptable. Social betrayal is a potent dimension of events that cause harm (see Figure 1) and very likely to be present in all forms of family violence (Freyd, 1996, 2001). DePrince (2001) found that the amount of betrayal in an event was more predictive of most negative symptoms than the amount of terror and fear. Evenwar combat may include significant elements of grief, shame, and betrayal (Shay, 1994). Overall, family violence is still highly stigmatized and difficult to prove in court (American Prosecutors Research Institute, 2003; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986). Survivors’ experiences of loss, betrayal, shame, stigma, and isolation have yet to be considered as rigorously as terror. Clarifying our definition of trauma will lead to a clarification and expansion of the effects of trauma. A whole class of difficulties following trauma have received relatively little research attention, including relationships with siblings, extended family, partners, and children. Sexual and sleep difficulties are common among survivors and may have implications for mental and physical well-being (e.g., Maltz, 2001; Matsakis, 1996). Finally, it is becoming very clear that victimizations are not unrelated, and multiple forms of victimization must be taken into account when assessing the impact of victimization (Finkelhor, Ormrod, Turner,&Hamby, 2004). In a report on the victimization experiences of a national representative sample of 2,030 children, 288 (14%) reported experiencing between four and six kinds of victimization, and 118 (9%) reported experiencing seven or more kinds of victimization (Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2004). Victimization kinds were defined as endorsing any of the items comprising the following scales: sexual victimization, physical assault, property victimization, maltreatment, peer or sibling victimization, and witnessing or indirect victimization. Controlling for several possible confounding variables, the number of different kinds of victimization predicted anger, depression, and anxiety better than chronic victimization of any one kind. Gender is one potentially very powerful risk factor for victimization. In general, men are more likely to be exposed towar combat, nonsexual assaults between strangers, and to be victimized in public places (Craven, 1997; U.S. Census Bureau, 2003), whereas women are more likely to be sexually abused, injured by an intimate partner, and victimized in a private home (Craven, 1997; Finkelhor, 1994; Straus, 2001). Thus, the scope of traumas assessed and the categories used to produce categories of experiences that are added together to form a measure of multiplicity of trauma experiences make a great deal of difference. A recent community survey revealed a number of gender differences in exposure to various kinds of trauma (Goldberg & Freyd, under review). Women were much more likely to report having been emotionally or psychologically mistreated by someone close as adults (approximately 40% compared to less than 12% of men) and as children (approximately 30% compared to less than 14%). Women also reported more sexual abuse in adulthood and in childhood than did men. However, men were much more likely to report having witnessed someone who they were not close to being killed, committing suicide, or being injured, in adulthood and childhood. Overall, women reported more events involving someone close to them, and men reported more events that did not involve other people, and events involving others who were not close to them. Different kinds of traumas are associated with particular outcomes. Traumas that involve high levels of threat are often associated with PTSD while secretive, family violence is more likely to be associated with dissociative symptoms (Freyd, 1996; Herman, 1992). Thus, although the number of kinds of traumas may predict general mental health outcomes, exposure to particular kinds of victimization may predict memory difficulty, dissociation, and PTSD. To the extent that exposure to violence is gendered, and outcomes differ by type of trauma, trauma-related psychological, social, and physical outcomes will be gender related. Understanding gender may be highly important for designing prevention and intervention strategies. UNLIKE MOST OTHER DSM-IV diagnoses, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) requires a specific, identifiable event to occur to qualify for the diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Of the characteristics of various traumatic events, one that has been described as important is the period of time over which traumatic events occur (Baum, O’Keefe, & Davidson, 1990). When compared to singleincident traumatic events, chronic traumatization has been associated with higher levels of PTSD symptoms (Herman 1992a). Chronic traumatization is characterized by repeated exposures to traumatic stressors within the same overall context over time. In the case of chronic traumatization, the environment contains an implied risk of danger even when there is no actual traumatic incident occurring (Smith, Smith,&Earp, 1999). Chronic traumatization may be damaging, not just because of the specific and repeated traumatic incidents but because of the effects of living in a state of constant danger (Baum et al., 1990; Herman, 1992b; Smith et al., 1999). PTSD is one of the most common negative outcomes associated with histories of child sexual abuse among child and adult survivors (Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996; Oddone, Genuis, & Violato, 2001). Rates of PTSD in clinical samples of sexually abused children have ranged from 21% to 74%,with the majority of studies reporting rates between 40% and 50% (Rodriguez, Van de Kemp, & Foy, 1998). The rate of current PTSD associated with childhood sexual abuse (CSA) ranges from 70% to 73% in help-seeking samples to 6% to 12% in the community (Rodriguez et al., 1998). Similarly, women with a history of childhood physical abuse (CPA) are 10 times as likely to currently have PTSD than those without such a history (Duncan, Saunders, Kilpatrick, Hanson, & Resnick, 1996). Combined CSA and CPA may be more likely than either alone to lead to PTSD (Schaaf & McCanne, 1998). Accordingly, the American Psychiatric Association (1987) defined traumatic events as incidents that are “outside the range of usual human experience” and are of such serious magnitude that they can be expected to be “markedly distressing to almost anyone.” Within this general definition, particular forms of trauma can be further distinguished. For instance, Wheaton (1996) suggested that traumatic life experiences can range from sudden events (e.g., parental loss and natural disasters) to events that are more chronic in nature (e.g., repeated physical and sexual abuse and war combat). Traumatic events can also be characterized by their scope of influence. For instance, certain traumatic events, such as war or natural disasters, affect entire groups of people simultaneously and thus can be considered macro-level traumas. Other forms of trauma, such as exposure to physical violence or parental loss, primarily affect individuals in isolation and thus can be thought of as micro-level traumas. Roughly a decade ago, the de˘nition of a traumatic event was expanded in the DSM-IV (APA, 1994) to include learning about unexpected death or threat of death experienced by a family member. Since this change in criteria, greater attention has been given in the empirical literature to psychopathology in relation to the death of a loved one. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PTSD – FAKTORJI TVEGANJA This study examined the prevalence of trauma, PTSD, and dissociation between genders within the same setting among juvenile offenders and referrals for a court-ordered psychological evaluation. The findings from this study are consistent with similar studies, which examined trauma and PTSD in an offender population and found high prevalence rates (Burton et al., 1994; Cauffman et al., 1998; Steiner et al., 1997). These findings are also consistent with research conducted within the general population. In a study of two samples of 2,000 women in each sample, Duncan, Saunders, Kilpatrick, Hanson, and Resnick (1996) found that victims of childhood physical assault were about 5 times more likely to have a profile of lifetime PTSD (53% vs. 11.2%). Furthermore, victims were 10 times more likely to be currently experiencing PTSD (32% vs. 3.8%) than were nonvictims. Breslau et al. (1998) studied 2,181 people in the Detroit area and found a lifetime prevalence rate of 89.6% of exposure to one or more traumatic events. The conditional probability of developing PTSD subsequent to the trauma was 9.2%, with PTSD being twice as high in women. Assaultive violence resulted in the greatest risk for developing PTSD, according to Breslau et al. (1998). In a similar study, Breslau, Chilcoat, Kessler, and Davis (1999) found that any previous exposure to trauma was associated with a greater risk for developing PTSD, and experiencing more than one traumatic event yielded a higher risk for PTSD. These authors also note that their data indicated that a trauma experienced in childhood did not place the individual at any greater risk for developing PTSD than did a trauma experienced later in life. Breslau et al. (1998) did suggest that trauma experienced in childhood could make one more vulnerable for PTSD if another trauma were experienced in adulthood. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PTSD – KRITIKA DIAGNOZE PTSM In DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), the PTSD diagnosis consists of Criterion A, which specifies a preceding traumatic event and an initial response; Criteria B, C, and D, which articulate clusters of “symptoms”—otherwise referred to as “the disturbance”; and Criteria E and F, which function to further delimit the use of the diagnostic category. Criterion A reads, The personwas exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following were present: the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of others. The person’s response involved intense fear, or helplessness, or horror. (p. 467) Bonnie Burstow 431 In this regard, note Kirk and Kutchins’s (1997) revelations: “There are 175 combinations of symptoms by which PTSD can be diagnosed” and “it is possible for two people who have no symptoms in common to receive a diagnosis of PTSD” (p. 124) Following the recognition that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may follow a variety of stressors, including combat, torture, incarceration, physical and sexual assault, and life-threatening illness and accidents, there has been considerable debate among researchers and clinicians about how best to characterize the kind of event that may reasonably attract a diagnosis of PTSD. In the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed.-revised (DSM-III-R, APA, 1987), it was suggested that such an event would be outside the range of usual human experience and would be markedly distressing to almost everyone. This formulation, known as the stressor criterion or Criterion A, was an integral part of the diagnosis of PTSD. Faced with criticisms that this formulation of Criterion A was insufficiently precise, and evidence that traumatic events involving threats to life and health were rather more usual than had hitherto been assumed (e.g., McFarlane&de Girolamo, 1996), the 4th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV, APA, 1994) changed Criterion A to require that a person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with events involving actual or threatened death, physical injury, or other threats to the physical integrity of the self or others (new Criterion A1). In addition, it attempted to specify the subjective impact of the trauma with greater precision in a new Criterion A2 by requiring that the person’s response had to involve intense fear, helplessness, or horror (March, 1993). The DSM-IV field trial confirmed a strong association between retrospective reports of subjective distress at the time of the trauma and subsequent PTSD (Kilpatrick et al., 1998). The addition of this subjective criterion implies that, during exposure to a traumatic event, individuals reliably experience certain intense emotions. This may seem reasonable, but it would not be surprising if some victims reported being numb or in a daze during the event, or reported absence of memory for the event, which might be expected to attenuate emotional intensity (see also Kilpatrick et al., 1998). Alternatively some events, such as motor vehicle accidents and some physical assaults, may happen too quickly for intense emotions to be registered at the time. A third factor to consider is the distinction between “primary” emotions occurring at the time of the trauma and “secondary” emotions arising out of subsequent cognitive appraisal, which could also act as potential risk factors for the development of PTSD (Brewin, Dalgleish, & Joseph, 1996). For example, there is recent evidence that both anger with others and shame, which may reflect secondary cognitive appraisal, are also strong predictors of PTSD symptoms longitudinally (Andrews, Brewin, Rose, & Kirk, 2000). In DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), the PTSD diagnosis consists of Criterion A, which specifies a preceding traumatic event and an initial response; Criteria B, C, and D, which articulate clusters of “symptoms”—otherwise referred to as “the disturbance”; and Criteria E and F, which function to further delimit the use of the diagnostic category. Criterion A reads, The personwas exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following were present: the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of others. The person’s response involved intense fear, or helplessness, or horror. (p. 467) Bonnie Burstow 431 In this regard, note Kirk and Kutchins’s (1997) revelations: “There are 175 combinations of symptoms by which PTSD can be diagnosed” and “it is possible for two people who have no symptoms in common to receive a diagnosis of PTSD” (p. 124) POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – BORDERLINE IN DRUGE OSEBN. MOTNJE Results indicate that BPD cannot be distinguished from other personality disorders on the basis of traumatic life events. The authors highlight the findings that individuals with BPD were no more likely to have reported childhood sexual abuse or adult victimization than were individuals with other personality disorders. Individuals with BPD were more likely to have PTSD than non-BPD participants, but PTSD rates were actually higher among individuals with paranoid personality disorder, suggesting no unique relationship between BPD and PTSD. Path analysis supported these results; direct and indirect relationships between BPD and PTSD were not significant in the models tested. Path analysis indicated that childhood abuse was significantly related to both BPD and PTSD directly and to PTSD indirectly through a history of assault in adulthood. In summary, the authors state that BPD has no unique claim among personality disorders as a trauma-spectrum disorder. (Trauma Is Not Specifically Related to Borderline Personality Disorder, Briefings in Behavioral Science Volume 22/Number 3 March 2004) The recent literature on the relationship between childhood abuse and personality disorders (PDs) appears relatively restricted to borderline and antisocial personality disorders and has focused mainly on clinical or incarcerated samples. Despite the importance of establishing the etiological association between childhood trauma and these disorders, broader investigations of PDs, particularly among more heterogeneous samples, are also needed. Research on the correlates of child abuse, as reviewed, for example, in Briere (1992) and Trickett and McBride-Chang (1995), has demonstrated a link between childhood abuse and a wide range of psychosocial difficulties. Against these findings we hypothesized that men with any history of childhood sexual or physical abuse would score higher on a measure of PD symptoms than men with no such history. Also, we hypothesized that men with more than one type of abuse would score higher than men with only one type of abuse. This would occur because of a greater frequency of abusive events, some sort of synergy between the types of abuse, or the possibility that families that create, or allow for, this level of abuse are more pathogenic in general. METODE, VPRAŠALNIKI In addition to a brief demographic information questionnaire, each packet included the Home Experiences History (HEH), a six-item checklist of typically disruptive family situations, including parental abuse of alcohol and drugs, physical and sexual abuse of a sibling, battery of a parent by another adult, and removal from the home by child protective services. The HEH was designed by Lisak and has been used in his research program (e.g., Altschuler, 1997; Hopper, 1997; Lisak et al., 1996; Smith, 1997). The Family Relationship Index (FRI) assesses perhaps more subtle aspects of family functioning and is composed of the Cohesion, Expressiveness, and (reverse-scored) Conflict subscales of the Family Environment Scale (FES), Form R (Moos & Moos, 1984). The FES is a widely used instrument, has demonstrated construct validity in a range of applications, and has strong internal consistency (Holahan & Moos, 1982; Moos & Moos, 1984). In what seems to be a relatively common revision (e.g., Sines, 1984), the items were recast in the past tense and participants were asked to respond in terms of their family while they were “growing up.” The Abuse-Perpetration Inventory (API) assesses histories of sexual and physical abuse. The abuse items of the API list a series of potentially abusive situations and ask subjects to respond regarding whether these happened to them before age 16. For each item, if subjects answer positively, they are then asked a short series of questions that assess the characteristics of the experience( s). The situations are stated in strictly behavioral terms (e.g., “someone had you fondle them,” “someone beat you with an object”); participants are not asked whether they were “abused,” “molested,” and so forth. The API has been used in six studies and has demonstrated validity with college male samples (Lisak, Conklin, Hopper, Miller, & Smith, 1997). Personality disorder symptoms were measured using the Schizoid, Antisocial, Borderline, Narcissistic, Avoidant, Dependent, and PassiveAggressive subscales from the Personality Diagnostic QuestionnaireRevised (PDQ-R) (Hyler & Rieder, 1987), an instrument designed to assess personality disorders as defined by DSM-III-R criteria. The full PDQ-R was not used primarily because of concerns about the length of the packet. In addition to using Hyler and Rieder’s subscales, we constructed a composite scale (PDQTOT), which is composed of the total number of responses indicating pathology across the seven subscales. Note that results for PDQTOT cannot be compared directly with other reports of PDQ-R composite scores because of the deletion of several subscales. KRITERIJI The criteria for physical abuse consider the nature of the relationship, the presence of injury, the threat of death, and the chronicity and frequency of the experience(s). First, with rare exception, physical abuse is restricted to intrafamilial relationships. Exceptions include relationships in which the other party appears to have chronic proximity to, and power over, the subject, beyond that afforded by the violent acts themselves. Moreover, the subject must have apparently fewmeans of escape or recourse from the abusive situation (e.g., a student-teacher relationship). Second, if the subject reports physical injuries greater than mild bruises or scratches or thought, or was told, that he was going to die, then the item is coded as abuse. Third, if these injury criteria were not met but the event occurred at least 10 times and for at least 1 year, then the item is coded as abuse as well. Note that the coding procedure for physical abuse allows for aggregation of data across items. That is, if a participant does not meet the duration or frequency criteria for an item but if his responses would meet criteria if this item were restated to include subtypes of physical abuse (e.g., kicking, hitting, stabbing) from another item(s) as well, then these combined itemswould be coded as abuse for the last of the items aggregated (i.e., representing a total of one “item’s worth” of physical abuse). This procedure was initiated to avoid categorizing as “not abused” individuals whose physical abuse characteristics were not well represented by the distribution of behaviors over the abuse items. In practice, however, abuse designations based on aggregated items were infrequent compared to those based on the more straightforward criteria. Both hypotheses were supported. Compared to nonabused men, men with any history of sexual or physical abuse indicated a greater degree of personality psychopathology associated withAvoidant, Borderline, Dependent, and general PD (i.e., PDQTOT) symptoms. Moreover, men with both types of abuse indicated greater symptomatology than men with one type of abuse on PDQTOT, Borderline, and Dependent. In addition, Schizoid scores exhibited trends consistent with both hypotheses. No differences were found on the Antisocial, Narcissistic, or Passive-Aggressive subscales. One interesting feature of these data is the absence of an association between childhood abuse and symptoms of antisocial personality disorder (APD). Although clinical levels of this disorder are less likely to be found in a relatively high functioning college sample, this lack of any association whatsoever was unexpected, given reports such as Luntz and Widom (1994) in which histories of abuse and/or neglect predicted APD symptoms and diagnostic status in adults when controlling for sex, race, age, socioeconomic status (SES), and criminal history. The reason for this lack of significant findings is unclear. One possible explanation is that DSM-III-R criteria (and therefore the PDQ-R’s criteria) for APD are weighted in the direction of criminality per se and away from personality characteristics such as callousness, low frustration tolerance, lack of moral development, and so forth, which are also associated with APD (Hare, Hart,&Harpur, 1991;Widiger& Corbitt, 1995). Thus, this emphasis on antisocial behavior may be achieved at the expense of sensitivity toward personality attributes, attributes that may be of greater relevance when assessing a college sample. Yet this explanation is weakened by findings that sexual or physical abuse histories predict antisocial behaviors in community and college samples (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1997; Malinosky-Rummell & Hansen, 1993; Trickett & McBride-Chang, 1995). POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – SAMOMOR Stressful events have long been acknowledged as important risk factors for suicidal behavior. Although suicide research has generally focused on less severe stressful events, a long-standing vulnerability for suicidal behavior may be a sequela of prolonged traumatic stressors. The present paper discusses the relationship between prolonged traumatic stress and subsequent suicidality by reviewing studies that have examined suicidal behavior in relationship to child abuse and combat trauma. Traumatic stress is conceptualized according to a person-environment interactional paradigm, and this paradigm is used to discuss the characteristics of traumatic events, recovery environments, and individuals that may contribute to subsequent suicidality. Research examining the relationship between traumatic stress and suicide has primarily come from two areas of study: child abuse trauma and combat trauma. Both child abuse and combat trauma appear related to suicidal behavior, with the risk for suicidal behavior often persisting for many years following the traumatic experience. Stressful events have long been acknowledged as important risk factors for suicidal behavior (Paykel, Prusoff, & Myers, 1974; Rich, Warsradt, Nemiroff, Fowler, & Young, 1991). Both acute and chronic stressors are related to suicidal behavior, and stressful events have an additive effect, with the risk of suicidal behavior increasing as the number of stressful events experienced by an individual increases (Adams, Overholser, & Spirito, 1994). Child abuse trauma and combat trauma differ greatly in populations affected, events involved and adjustment demands placed on the individual. However, they are similar in that both frequently represent Type II trauma (Terr, 1992), a form of trauma that is prolonged and repeated (Herman, 1992). Traumatic stress is a difficult construct to operationalize (Escobar, 1987; Lindy, Green, & Grace, 1987). Previous definitions of trauma focused on the traumatic event as being "outside the range of usual human experience" (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1987), implying a categorical difference between traumatic events and less severe stressful events (Breslau & Davis, 1987). Recent definitions of trauma are more consistent with theoretical models of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) in emphasizing the interaction between person and environment. Currently (APA, 1994), the essence of a traumatic stressor is in the threatening nature of the event and in its ability to overwhelm normal human adaptive capacities. Physical and sexual abuse are acknowledged childhood traumatic events. The potential for suicidal behavior has been observed among child abuse survivors in clinical settings, with survivors often reporting a chronic preoccupation with death and dying that accompanies feelings of helplessness, hopelessness, and anger at self and others (Briere, 1989; Courtois, 1988). In research, a relationship between a history of child abuse trauma and subsequent suicidal behavior has been observed in a variety of populations. In a study with 1,040 psychiatric inpatients (Brown & Anderson, 1991), suicidality was the most common admitting symptom for those patients who had experienced child abuse trauma. Seventy five percent of the child abuse survivors presented with suicidal behavior, compared with 57% of the nonabused patients. A study with female psychiatric inpatients (Bryer, Nelson, Miller, & Kroll, 1987) indicated a similar relationship between child abuse trauma and suicidality. Patients presenting with suicidal ideation, gestures or attempts were three times as likely to have a history of child abuse as nonsuicidal patients. The rate of suicidal behavior is also high among psychiatric outpatients with a history of child abuse. One study (Anderson, Yasenik, & Ross, 1993) reported that 49% of women seeking outpatient therapy for sexual abuse reported having made one or more suicide attempts. In other studies, the rate of suicide attempts among sexually abused women was 55% at a crisis counseling center (Briere & Runtz, 1986) and 66% at a psychiatric emergency room (Briere & Zaidi, 1989). In contrast, for depressed patients, a 23% to 35% rate of suicide attempts has been found (Adams & Overholser, 1992; Tanney, 1992). Among college students, both suicidal ideation and suicide attempts have been shown to be related to a history of child abuse. Suicidal ideation was a coping mechanism self-reported by college students who experienced sexual abuse as children (Runtz, 1993), and suicide attempts were more likely given a history of physical abuse (Briere & Runtz, 1987) and sexual abuse (Sedney & Brooks, 1984). "Chronic suicidality" refers to recurrent suicidal episodes rather than a persistent suicidal state (Motto, 1992). Chronic suicidality among trauma survivors is described in clinical reports, with some survivors tracing their suicidal preoccupation to their first incident of child abuse or to their combat experience (Briere, 1989; Hendin & Haas, 1984). The chronicity of suicidality following child abuse or combat trauma has not been directly examined in empirical studies, but indirect research evidence suggests a long-term vulnerability to suicidal behavior following child abuse or combat trauma. Interactional paradigms of stress emphasize both environmental and individual factors in defining stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Applying this paradigm to traumatic stress, the factors defining a traumatic stressor would include characteristics of: (1) the event, (2) the recovery environment following the event, and (3) the individual (Wilson, 1989). Considering these components separately could help clarify the relationship between traumatic stress and suicidal behavior (i.e., which characteristics of the event and recovery environment contribute to increased suicide risk). An increased number of traumatic stressors is related to a greater likelihood of negative outcomes (Bryer et al., 1987; Solkoff, Gray, & Keill, 1986). As stress theory is being applied to traumatic stress, the recovery environment is increasingly being recognized as a key element in the traumatic stress process. Stress theory incorporates the role of environmental social support in the stress process and posits a stress buffering effect for social support (Cohen & Wills, 1985). According to stress theory, social support buffers the effects of a stressful event in two ways: (1) by intervening between the event and the stress reaction, thus reducing the appraised threat or (2) by intervening between the experienced stress and a potential pathological process by providing solutions or facilitating adaptive behaviors. Studies of less severe stressful events have shown that environmental social support reduces the likelihood of subsequent psychopathology and suicidal behavior (Kessler & McLeod, 1985; Overholser, Norman, & Miller, 1990). …9 stran se ne vidi! …suggested that an individual's response to a traumatic event occurs in three stages: initial reactions, ongoing accommodation, and long-term elaboration. At each stage, the individual interacts with various dimensions of the traumatic event and the recovery environment (Wilson, 1989). Dissociation may be one mechanism that underlies the relationship between traumatic stress and suicidality. Dissociation as a coping mechanism often originates with traumatic experiences (Spiegel, 1993). Guilt may also be a mechanism underlying the relationship between trauma and suicidality. For some individuals, feelings of guilt, shame, responsibility and complicity may originate with a traumatic event and become integrated into the experience of self (Briere, 1989; Courtois, 1988). Both child abuse trauma and combat trauma are related to multiple psychiatric diagnoses, with psychiatric symptoms often becoming chronic or recurrent (Green et al., 1989; Newman, Orsillo, Herman, Niles, & Litz, 1995). Among the diagnoses most frequently named in relation to suicidal behavior are depressive disorders, substance abuse disorders, PTSD, generalized anxiety disorder, and panic disorder. Psychiatric co-morbidity often typifies the clinical presentation of child abuse and combat trauma survivors (Beitchman et al., 1992; Green et al., 1989) which may further increase the risk of suicidal behavior (Beautrais et al., 1996). Manetta (1999) examined the relationship between different types of abuse (partner abuse, childhood physical and sexual abuse, and rape) and suicidal tendencies (as a symptom of depression) among African American women seen at medical and psychiatric facilities (N= 91). The most frequent type of abuse reported among those who were suicidal was partner abuse (24.2%). In several studies, suicidal behavior has been found to relate to trauma exposure. For example, Lundin (1984), in a study of familial morbidity following 50 cases of sudden and unexpected death, found 10% of the surviving bereaved to have committed suicide. Kilpatrick, Best, and Veronen (1985) found the rate of attempted suicide to be 8.7 times higher among victims of completed rape than among nonvictims. In a study among former WW II prisoners of war (POWs), it was found that 57% of POWs imprisoned by the Japanese harboured suicidal thoughts and that 7% of POWs under the Germans had attempted suicide (Miller, Martin, & Spiro, 1989). Somasundaram (1993) reported that suicidal thoughts were present in 38% of a group of 160 former POWs subjected to torture in Sri Lanka. The principal aim of the present study was to assess (a) the prevalence of PTSD and psychiatric comorbidity, (b) the incidence suicidal behavior among refugees with history of exposure to severe trauma, and c) the possible difference between the different diagnoses with respect to modes of suicidal behavior. In this study of 149 traumatized refugees, the prevalence of PTSD among all cases in which a principal psychiatric diagnosis was established was 83%. A significant overrepresentation of suicidal behavior was found in the group of refugees with PTSD diagnoses in comparison with the no PTSD cases. Among the traumatized refugees in our study (all diagnoses included) assessed with suicidal behavior, 40% (30/74) had made at least one previous suicide attempt. Besides paramount ethnic and cultural factors which have been reported as potential risk for suicide among refugees (Ferrada-Noli, 1994; Ferrada-Noli, Asberg, Ormstad, & Nordstrom, 1995; Ferrada-Noli, Asberg, & Ormstad, 1996; Ferrada-Noli & Sundbom, 1996), new epidemiological findings have disclosed the highly significant nation-wide overrepresentation of immigrants in the Swedish suicide statistics, x2(1, N = 10,225) = 44.7, p = .0001). The idea of a principal impact of PTSD (and also of reactive posttraumatic depression) in the pathogenesis of suicidal behavior among tor- tured victims may find indirect support in investigations describing the nature and prevalence of PTSD symptoms, since some of these symptoms have been reported earlier as being clinically associated with suicide risk factors. In the study of Basoglu et al. (1994) 'restricted expectations' was found to be three times more frequent among tortured than among nontortured subjects. Ginsberg (1989) regarded the level of the patient's feelings of hopelessness the most frequent association with attempted suicide in patients suffering from depression, and the previous early findings in suicidology on a high correlation between sense of hopelessness and inability to see into the future and future suicide (Cavanough, 1986), are also to be related with the very formulation of PTSD diagnostic criterion C-7 (sense of a foreshortened future) in DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). A preliminary finding on an association between the suicide method contained in the patient's suicidal ideation and the torture method to which he or she recounted have being subjected was reported by Ferrada-Noli (1993) at a CTD symposium at the Karolinska Hospital. The principal aim of the present study was to ascertain whether relationships existed between the type of torture stressors and suicidal ideation, the hypothesis being that the nature of the torture methods would be reflected in the content of posttraumatic self-destructive ideation. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – DEPRESIJA Research has demonstrated that youth who are sexually orphysically abused are at greater risk for developing depression. Although the association between depression and child maltreatment has been well documented, much less is known about the potential differences in the clinical presentation of depressive symptomatology among these victims. The current study examines differences in symptoms of depression in adolescents based on differing histories of abuse (i.e., sexual abuse only, physical abuse only, sexual and physical abuse, and no history of sexual or physical abuse), abuse incident characteristics, and gender. The presence of depression in adolescence is a significant risk predictor for major depression in adulthood (Harrington, Fudge, Rutter, Pickles, & Hill, 1990). In addition, depressed youth are at a significantly higher risk for suicide, which is the third leading cause of death in adolescents (Brent, 1995; Brent, Bridge, Johnson, & Connolly, 1996; Centers for Disease Control [CDC], 2002). Research has demonstrated that children and adolescents who are sexually or physically abused are at greater risk for developing depression (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1996; Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003). Research has emphasized that CSA is a risk factor for depression (e.g., Hill, 2003; Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003; Mannarino & Cohen, 1996). CPA also recently has been recognized as a risk factor for mood disorders (e.g., Brown&Kolko, 1999; Clark, De Bellis, Lynch, Cornelius, & Martin, 2003; Johnson, Kotch, et al., 2002; Runyon & Kenny, 2002). Despite the consistent findings that there is a strong link between depression and child maltreatment, little is known about the specific role of child abuse in the development and course of depression or how differences in abuse history may be associated with the clinical presentation of depressive symptomatology. In another study comparing symptom differences in children who had been physically abused, neglected, or who had no abuse history, researchers found that children who were physically abused were more likely to be suicidal than children who were neglected as well as the children who were nonmaltreated (Finzi, Har-Even, Shnit, & Weizman, 2002). Several studies have examined the perpetrator-victim relationship with regard to sequelae of the abuse, often finding that intrafamilial victims suffer greater physical and emotional injury (e.g., Faust, Runyon, & Kenny, 1995; Fischer&McDonald, 1998; Ruggiero et al., 2000). It is thought that the violation of a trusting intrafamilial relationship contributes to negative sequelae as a result of the abuse, particularly PTSD. Thus, related depressive symptoms (i.e., problems with sleep, appetite, and energy) may be especially prevalent in children who experience abuse by the hand of a caretaker or in the home. In addition, endorsements of several specific symptoms differed among the groups. Particularly noteworthy is that guilt and thoughts of hurting oneself were endorsed most by female adolescents in either group (abuse or no abuse history), indicating that these are particularly prevalent symptoms for female adolescents who are depressed. Some researchers have begun to examine how differences in normal adolescent development between boys and girls may affect such differences in depression, such as differences in coping styles (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994), which may help to explain why suicidal ideation is higher among girls. Child Maltreatment in Depressed Adolescents:Differences in Symptomatology Based on History of Abuse Carla Kmett Danielson, Michael A. de Arellano, Dean G. Kilpatrick, Benjamin E. Saunders, Heidi S. Resnick Medical University of South Carolina CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 10, No. 1, February 2005 37-48 Depression is often a co-morbid condition that comes with PTSD (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and not surprisingly this is common in many of the cases with sexual problems following trauma. Depression is a common correlate of childhood maltreatment as well. A number of studies have shown increased rates of depression, suicidality, and lowself-esteem inwomen with a history of CSA (for reviews, see Beitchman et al., 1992;Weiss, Longhurst, &Mazure, 1999). Duncan et al. (1996) found thatwomen reporting a history of CPA were two times as likely to have had a major depressive episode in their lifetime and four times as likely to be currently experiencing a major depressive episode. Bernet and Stein (1999) found that histories of childhood abuse among patients who were depressed were associated with earlier onset of a first depressive episode, more lifetime depressive episodes, and greater comorbidity. Self-depreciation has been linked to CSA and childhood psychological maltreatment (Higgins & McCabe, 2000b). In addition, childhood abuse has been associated with anxiety, borderline personality disorder, somatization, sleep problems (McCauley et al., 1997), dissociative symptoms (Chu & Dill, 1990), and interpersonal and sexual difficulties (Rumstein-McKean&Hunsley, 2001). POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PSIHOZA Several lines of evidence suggest an association between trauma and psychosis (1). First, studies have demonstrated a high incidence of trauma in the lifetimes of patients with psychosis. Ross et al. (2) found that positive symptoms of schizophrenia are related to a history of childhood trauma (2, 3). Abused patients are particularly likely to experience positive symptoms, such as hallucinations (4–6) paranoid ideation, thought insertion, visual hallucinations, ideas of reading someone else’s mind, ideas of reference and hearing voices making comments. In a recent study, childhood abuse was a significant predictor of hallucinations, even in the absence of adult abuse (7). Secondly, in patients with other diagnoses, a history of child abuse has also been found to _co-occur_ with a high frequency of auditory hallucinations and delusions. Childhood sexual abuse has an impact on the later symptom profile of patients with bipolar affective disorder, increasing their vulnerability to experience hallucinations (8). Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder manifest increased levels of positive psychotic symptoms (9). Dissociative identity disorder, which is assumed to be a disturbance resulting from severe childhood abuse (10–12) may present with a great number of Schneiderian first rank symptoms, particularly in the form of auditory hallucinations (11, 12). It has even been suggested that of all diagnostic categories, psychosis displays the strongest associations with child abuse (13, 14). Thirdly, according to Briere (15) childhood sexual abuse is the most powerful predictor of later psychiatric symptoms and disorders after controlling for significant demographic variables. A study of adult outpatients found child abuse to be a more powerful predictor of suicidality than a current diagnosis of depression (16). The more severe the abuse, the greater the probability of psychiatric disorder in adulthood (17, 18). It has been suggested that the experience of abuse may create a biological (1) or psychological (19) vulnerability for the development of psychotic symptoms, including sub-clinical psychotic experiences such as low-grade delusional ideation and isolated auditory hallucinations (20). In the general population, childhood sexual abuse is related to schizotypy, including perceptual aberrations (21) which are 10 times more common in adults who were maltreated as children (22). In both clinical and non-clinical populations, the diagnostic group with the highest rate of childhood abuse consistently reported the most Schneiderian symptoms (23). Thus, two previous studies found evidence for an association between abuse and psychotic experiences in non-clinical samples (21, 24). For example, subjects who reported abuse in the highest frequency category had an estimated 30 times greater chance to develop a needs-based diagnosis of psychosis compared to those not exposed to childhood abuse. Less frequent abuse was associated with an estimated five times greater risk to develop a need-based diagnosis of psychosis compared to those without any exposure to childhood abuse, whereas the risk was not increased in subjects who reported abuse in the lowest frequency category. The results of this study suggest that reported childhood abuse predicts psychotic symptoms in adulthood in a dose–response fashion. The association between childhood abuse and psychotic symptoms was robust and remained significant after adjustment for possible confounders. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – ADHD I want to include some necessarily rather brief thinking about Gary’s ADHD diagnosis and its relevance to his early history. In ‘Wrestling with the whirlwind: an approach to the understanding of ADHD’ (Orford, 1998), the author quotes research done by Perry and his coworkers in 1995.Their approach was neuro-biological, and it has now been proved that early experience does indeed have an effect on the development of neural pathways and later brain functioning. They note that the symptoms of ADHD are very similar to those that occur during trauma: the hyper-alertness, the need to act quickly, to live in constant expectation of danger to the exclusion of other thoughts. What they suggest is that in a critical period in infancy some children experience trauma which initiates an habitual automatic response, as though to external threat. As they grow up, these children are hyper-sensitive to threat and revert to ‘action stations’ in time of crisis. Babies may be exposed to frightening experiences which cause them to become habituated to feelings of threat. Perry’s . nding has been that children with ADHD have established neural pathways on the basis of response to threat and trauma. In other words, some traumatized children may develop ADHD. As Schore (1998) also explains, early traumatic events which result in excessive use of projective identi. cation and then dissociation become imprinted in the right brain as primitive defence mechanisms which can potentially affect the regulation (or dysregulation) of feelings throughout the lifespan. (On being dropped and picked up: adopted children and their internal objects, JUDITH EDWA RDS ) POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – ODVISNOST POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – SAMOPOHABLJANJE In examining the possible connections, researchers have noticed that incest is often present in the history of selfmutilators. However, conclusions often go no further than reporting the correlation. Although the correlation is noteworthy, it overlooks an important observation: Not all incest survivors mutilate themselves.Nostudies to date have explored the interrelation of variables that lead to an understanding ofwhysomeCSA survivors self-mutilate and others do not. Of those with sexual abuse histories, 17% had selfmutilated. None of thewomenwithout a sexual abuse history had self-mutilated. Astudy of 45 incest survivors by de Young (1982b) found that 58% had engaged in self-injurious behaviors, all beginning after the CSA.Of hospitalized adolescent self-mutilators, 56% reported sexual abuse in a study by C. Simpson and Porter (1981). Studies by Goldney and Simpson (1975) and Grunebaum and Klerman (1967) noted the connection between self-mutilation and sexual abuse perpetrated by parents. In the sexual abuse accommodation syndrome described by Summit (1983), self-mutilation was considered one of the adaptive methods developed to survive and accommodate the secrecy, helplessness, and entrapment of the abuse. More recently, self-mutilation by CSA survivors has been conceptualized as a symptom of post-traumatic stress disorder. In one study, 25% of CSA survivors meeting the criteria for post-traumatic stress disorder also self-mutilated (Albach & Everaerd, 1992). Depression is one emotion often associated with the emotional sequelae of CSA (Beitchman et al., 1992). Brodsky, Cloitre, and Dulit (1995) found a strong correlation between depression, selfmutilation, and CSA histories. Incorporating many of the above features, the diagnosis of borderline personality disorder is commonly given to patients who self-mutilate (Briere&Zaidi, 1989; Grunebaum& Klerman, 1967; Shapiro, 1987; Walsh & Rosen, 1988). Parental perpetrator(s). Self-mutilators, in comparison to the nonmutilators, were sexually abused more often by their fathers (51.3% to 25.0%) and mothers (10.3% to 2.8%). A parental perpetrator appeared to substantially differentiate between the two groups and therefore was included in the log-linear analysis. Frequency of abuse. Participants were askedhowoften the sexual experience(s) occurred. Therewas space provided to answer both a standardized response and a respondent-generated estimate. Results of this question supported that there is a relationship between increased frequency and self-mutilation. On average, self-mutilators remembered 22 nonfamilial sexual incidents and 45 familial ones.Womenwho did not self-injure remembered 10.8 and 38.44 incidents, respectively. Penetration. The group of self-mutilators experienced less vaginal intercourse than the nonmutilators in both familial (12.8% to 13.9%) and nonfamilial (37.9% to 47.8%) sexual abuse. However, slightly more self-mutilators experienced anal (12.8% to 8.3%) or attempted vaginal (30.8% to 19.4%) intercourse. The presence of vaginal intercourse did not seem to contribute to the distinction between self-mutilators and nonmutilators. Therewas some suggestion, however, that the presence of anal or attempted vaginal intercourse may indicate a proclivity toward self-mutilation. However,more of the fathers of the nonmutilators had died compared to fathers of the mutilators (14.3% to 9.5%). These results were the opposite of the suggested findings; therefore, the loss of one’s father was not included in further analyses. The loss of one’s mother, however, yielded a different pattern. During the participants’ childhoods, more mothers of selfmutilators died compared to mothers of nonmutilators (7.1% to 2.4%, n = 4, combined). Also, the mothers of mutilators were seriously ill or suffered injuries more often in a comparison to mothers of nonmutilators (38.1% to 31%, n = 16 and n = 13). In examining the possible connections, researchers have noticed that incest is often present in the history of selfmutilators. However, conclusions often go no further than reporting the correlation. Although the correlation is noteworthy, it overlooks an important observation: Not all incest survivors mutilate themselves.Nostudies to date have explored the interrelation of variables that lead to an understanding ofwhysomeCSA survivors self-mutilate and others do not. Of those with sexual abuse histories, 17% had selfmutilated. None of thewomenwithout a sexual abuse history had self-mutilated. Astudy of 45 incest survivors by de Young (1982b) found that 58% had engaged in self-injurious behaviors, all beginning after the CSA.Of hospitalized adolescent self-mutilators, 56% reported sexual abuse in a study by C. Simpson and Porter (1981). Studies by Goldney and Simpson (1975) and Grunebaum and Klerman (1967) noted the connection between self-mutilation and sexual abuse perpetrated by parents. In the sexual abuse accommodation syndrome described by Summit (1983), self-mutilation was considered one of the adaptive methods developed to survive and accommodate the secrecy, helplessness, and entrapment of the abuse. More recently, self-mutilation by CSA survivors has been conceptualized as a symptom of post-traumatic stress disorder. In one study, 25% of CSA survivors meeting the criteria for post-traumatic stress disorder also self-mutilated (Albach & Everaerd, 1992). Depression is one emotion often associated with the emotional sequelae of CSA (Beitchman et al., 1992). Brodsky, Cloitre, and Dulit (1995) found a strong correlation between depression, selfmutilation, and CSA histories. Incorporating many of the above features, the diagnosis of borderline personality disorder is commonly given to patients who self-mutilate (Briere&Zaidi, 1989; Grunebaum& Klerman, 1967; Shapiro, 1987; Walsh & Rosen, 1988). Over the past two decades, the study of CSAwithin the family and its sequelae has been recognized as a significant issue for many women. Estimates vary as to its prevalence, ranging from about one third (31%) (Russell, 1983) (33%) (Wheeler & Walton, 1987) to 16% (Russell, 1983, 1984; Sedney & Brooks, 1984) REZULTATI Duration. The average duration of the CSA for the mutilators was almost double that of the nonmutilators. With a range of duration from less than 1 year to 22 years, the mutilators’ CSA began at the mean age of 6.06 years (SD = 3.91) and stopped at the mean age of 13.33 years (SD = 5.87). For the nonmutilators, the CSA started at the mean age of 7.64 years (SD = 4.30), and the mean age when it ended was 11.66 (SD = 6.36). Duration ranged from less than 1 year to 34 years. Duration of the CSAappeared to differentiate between the two groups and was included as a potential variable in the model-building phase of the analysis. Parental perpetrator(s). Self-mutilators, in comparison to the nonmutilators, were sexually abused more often by their fathers (51.3% to 25.0%) and mothers (10.3% to 2.8%). A parental perpetrator appeared to substantially differentiate between the two groups and therefore was included in the log-linear analysis. Frequency of abuse. Participants were askedhowoften the sexual experience(s) occurred. Therewas space provided to answer both a standardized response and a respondent-generated estimate. Results of this question supported that there is a relationship between increased frequency and self-mutilation. On average, self-mutilators remembered 22 nonfamilial sexual incidents and 45 familial ones.Womenwho did not self-injure remembered 10.8 and 38.44 incidents, respectively. Penetration. The group of self-mutilators experienced less vaginal intercourse than the nonmutilators in both familial (12.8% to 13.9%) and nonfamilial (37.9% to 47.8%) sexual abuse. However, slightly more self-mutilators experienced anal (12.8% to 8.3%) or attempted vaginal (30.8% to 19.4%) intercourse. The presence of vaginal intercourse did not seem to contribute to the distinction between self-mutilators and nonmutilators. Therewas some suggestion, however, that the presence of anal or attempted vaginal intercourse may indicate a proclivity toward self-mutilation. However,more of the fathers of the nonmutilators had died compared to fathers of the mutilators (14.3% to 9.5%). These results were the opposite of the suggested findings; therefore, the loss of one’s father was not included in further analyses. The loss of one’s mother, however, yielded a different pattern. During the participants’ childhoods, more mothers of selfmutilators died compared to mothers of nonmutilators (7.1% to 2.4%, n = 4, combined). Also, the mothers of mutilators were seriously ill or suffered injuries more often in a comparison to mothers of nonmutilators (38.1% to 31%, n = 16 and n = 13). The nature of the abuse, the relationship of the perpetrator, the age when the abuse occurred, and the duration of the abuse are variables of the abuse that further influence the likelihood of self-harm. Little has been written about the psychological determinants that lead to self-harm as an expression of the internal distress associated with a history of abuse. Themore extreme forms of self-harm are defined as purposeful actions that harm the body and that are outside the bounds of social acceptability. These include cutting, burning, abrading, or hitting oneself, inserting sharp objects in the anus or vagina, pulling out body hair, or other self-attacking behaviors that are idiosyncratic to the survivor and his or her abuse history. However, cutting has been found to be the most common form of self-harm (Babicker & Arnold). Self-harm is often an attempt to communicate and relieve pain and maintain discourse. Women who self-harm often hate their bodies and consider their bodies to be representations of their internalized badness and ugliness. Cutting the external body symbolically attacks the internal badness and, because of boundary confusion, may represent an attack on the abuser. Cutting, unlike childhood abuse, is within the control of the trauma survivor. Of course, the relief it brings is short-lived and often leads to shame and guilt, and the cycle of pain, relief, and shame starts again. Abstract: Lifetime trauma histories were ascertained for females with confirmed histories of childhood sexual abuse and comparison females participating in a longitudinal, prospective study. Abused participants reported twice as many subsequent rapes or sexual assaults (p = .07), 1.6 times as many physical affronts including domestic violence (p = .01), almost four times as many incidences of self-inflicted harm (p = .002), and more than 20% more subsequent, significant lifetime traumas (p = .04) than did comparison participants. Sexual revictimization was positively correlated with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (PTSD), peritraumatic dissociation, and sexual preoccupation. Physical revictimization was positively correlated with PTSD symptoms, pathological dissociation, and sexually permissive attitudes. Selfharm was positively correlated with both peritraumatic and pathological dissociation. Competing theoretical explanations for revictimization and self-harm are discussed and evaluated. Research over the past decade has documented a prospective link between rape and subsequent revictimization in short-term follow-up studies of adult victims (e.g., Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman, 1995; Kilpatrick, Acierno, Resnick, Saunders, &Best, 1997). The link between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent victimization that occurs later in adolescence or adulthood is less well understood. A growing body of research has documented associations between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent sexual victimization (see Messman&Long, 1996, and Briere&Runtz, 1987, for reviews; see also Arata & Lindman, 2002; Chu & Dill, 1990; Kessler & Bieschke, 1999; Koss & Dinero, 1989; Merrill et al., 1999; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000) and between childhood sexual abuse and laterphysical victimization including domestic violence (Arata, 1999; Collins, 1998; Gilbert, El-Bassel, Schilling, & Friedman, 1997; Krahe, Scheinberger-Olwig, Waizenhofer, & Kolpin, 1999; McClosky, 1997; Messman & Long, 1996). Otherstudies have documented higher rates of self-abuse or self-harm in childhood sexual abuse victims (Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Romans, Martin, Anderson, Herbison, & Mullen, 1995; Van der Kolk, Perry, & Herman, 1991; Winchel & Stanley, 1991; Yeo & Yeo, 1993). Further, it appears that the co-occurrence of multiple types of child maltreatment (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, child neglect) puts children at considerable risk for revictimization in adulthood (Briere, Woo, McRae, Foltz, & Sitzman, 1997; Dutton, Burghardt, Perrin, Chrestman, & Halle, 1994; Hillis, 2001). We operationally define victimization (either sexual or physical) as harm perpetrated by an outside source that serves as a reenactment of the initial abuse. Self-harm, on the other hand, implies a direct reenactment inflicted by the survivor herself and represents a certain internalization of the trauma. Therefore, self-harm is not considered a category of revictimization but will be studied as a separate and distinct phenomenon. REZULTATI: Analyses showed that, compared to nonabused participants, sexually abused participants were twice as likely to have been raped or sexually assaulted, almost fourtimes as likely to have inflicted subsequent self-harm (in the form of suicide attempts or self-mutilation), reported significantly higher rates of physical revictimization (including domestic violence), and reported a greater number of significant subsequent lifetime traumas than comparison participants. When alternative forms of childhood maltreatment were taken into account, childhood sexual abuse was a unique predictor of self-harm. Concurrent pathological dissociaton was shown to be predictive of physical victimization when in the company with variables from several theoretically distinct domains. These results indicate that a persistent reliance on dissociation as a coping mechanism can place participants at increased risk for physical harm. Thus, victims who adopt pathological dissociation as the primary defense strategy in adolescence or adulthood may be less able to engage in self-protection when physically threatened. Dissociation has been thought to be associated with suicide and self-injurious behaviors, and these results confirm this association (Brodsky, Cloitre, & Dulit, 1995). Self-harm may not be a direct response to sexual abuse but to the dissociative experiences that result from efforts to cope with the abuse. Results also indicate that being sexually active orbelie ving that sexual activity is permissible can increase one’s vulnerability for physical victimization. The incidence of self-harm in sexual abuse victims was quite dramatic. Being sexually abused was, by far, the strongest predictor of self-harm even when in company with other forms of child maltreatment. POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – SOMATIZACIJE, FIZIČNI PROBLEMI In addition to posing psychological risks, child abuse can have serious medical consequences (Berkowitz, 2000; Wharton, Rosenberg, Sheridan, &Ryan, 2000). Many physically abused children suffer neurological or neuropsychological impairment, severe injuries, or even death (Ammerman, Cassisi, Hersen, &Van Hasselt, 1986). Teicher et al. (1997) found evidence suggestive of abnormal cortical development in nearly three quarters of their sample of sexually and physically abused children. There are also data about such reactivity among women who have PTSD due to childhood sexual abuse (PTSD–CSA). In a recent study, Orr, Lasko, et al. (1998) compared women with histories of CSA who had full PTSD, partial PTSD, and no PTSD. All three groups reacted to the trauma-related stimuli, but women with full PTSD showed significantly greater increases in heart rate (HR) and forehead muscle tension than did women without PTSD. In a related study, this group found that the CSAexposed women with current PTSD or lifetime PTSD had greater HR responses and slower habituation of skin conductance responses to auditory startle stimuli compared to the CSA-exposedwomen without PTSD (Metzger et al., 1999). These results suggest thatwomen with PTSD–CSA exhibit patterns of psychophysiological responding similar to those observed in male combat veterans with PTSD when confronted either with reminders of their childhood trauma or with startling tones. In summary, our results with female survivors of CSA are consistent with the findings in other PTSD subpopulations, that heightened psychophysiological reactivity is associated with reminders of the trauma and with PTSD symptom severity. In addition, our finding of a negative association between PTSD symptom severity and psychophysiological reactivity for the mental arithmetic task is consistent with other studies of PTSD. Together, these results point to the need to examine how appraisal processes and other individual differences mediate psychophysiological responses to laboratory stress tasks. The physiological response to trauma-related stimuli of up to one third of participants with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) cannot be discriminated from that of controls. Psychophysiological measures (heart rate and blood pressure) of 22 PTSD and 23 control civilian participants, all exposed to missile attacks during the Gulf War, were recorded while listening to five scripts. The physiological response of PTSD subjects with high image control (IC) was lower than that of PTSD participants with low IC and similar to that of non-PTSD subjects. The physiological response poorly discriminated high IC PTSD participants from controls, but was successful in discriminating low IC PTSD subjects from controls with 91% specificity and 92% sensitivity. Image control is proposed as a function influencing physiological response in PTSD. POSLEDICE TRAVME – SPOLNOST, INCEST, SPOLNO NADLEGOVANJE, POSILSTVO Victims of sexual assault report loss of interest and satisfaction with daily life and impairment of functioning (Ellis, Atkeson, & Calhoun, 1981). Other reactions to sexual assault include altered sleep patterns, sexual dysfunction, eating irregularities, posttraumatic symptoms, and somatic difficulties (Foa & Riggs, 1994; Nadelson, Notman, Zackson, & Gornick, 1982). Victims of sexual assault also report intense fear of revictimization and feelings of violation, vulnerability, hopelessness, loss of control, shame, anxiety, concentration difficulties, lethargy, and irritability (Becker & Kaplan, 1991; Moscarello, 1991; Nadelson et al., 1982). Sexual assault impairs a victim’s social functioning by provoking fear of strangers, social interaction, and people in close proximity, thus promoting avoidance or intense anxiety in situations similar to the sexual assault event (Steketee & Foa, 1987). Accord ing to Rind et al. (1998), CSA gen er ally is believed to cause severe and last ing harm to the major ity of chil dren who expe ri ence it, and males and females are believed to be sim i larly affected. Their anal ysis does not sup port these beliefs for the col lege stu dent pop u la tion. Jumper (1995) con ducted a meta-analysis of 26 stud ies of CSA with par tic i pants from com mu nity, stu dent, and clin i cal pop u la tions and con cluded that stu dents gen er ally emerge from CSA expe ri ences with less psy cho log i cal adjust ment difficulties than do CSA sur vivors in clin i cal or com mu nity pop u la tions. Com mu nity-Clinical Studies In study ing women in a com mu nity set ting, Coffey, Leitenberg, Henning, Turner, and Bennett (1996) report that the group who reported sex ual abuse in child hood showed greater psy cho log i cal symptomatology, includ ing higher clin i cal lev els, than did the com par i son group, who reported no his tory of CSA. In another com mu nity study, Mullen, Mar tin, Ander son, Romans, and Herbison (1995) inter viewed 298 women youn ger than 65 who indi cated per sonal his to ries of sex abuse in child hood. The authors stated that fam i lies with low socio eco nomic sta tuses have more dis rup tion than do fam i lies with higher socio eco nomic back grounds; there fore, abuse is higher in these fam ilies. Mullen et al. (1995) con cluded that child sex ual abuse is not as influ en tial on adult psychopathology as pre vi ously thought. It is an indi ca tor of other types of abuse, how ever. Women who reported more than one form of abuse gen er ally had more adult sex ual abuse in child hood have been gen er al ized inac cu rately. prob lems—sex ual prob lems, addic tions, low self-esteem, eat ing dis or ders, depres sion, and psy chi at ric hos pi tal iza tion. There are fac tors mod er at ing the abuse that were asso ci ated with lower neg a tive out comes, such as the fam ily stay ing intact, con fid ing with one’s mother, and hav ing a close per sonal friend. An exten sion of this find ing is that a child may suf fer abuse, but close, sup port ive rela tion ships with friends or fam ily mem bers may alle vi ate some of the long-range prob lems asso ci ated with abuse. Pistorello and Follette (1998) exam ined vid eo taped ses sions of CSA sur vivors in group ther apy to develop five cat e go ries of prob lems in inti mate rela tion ships reported dur ing group ther apy. The five cat e go ries are sex-, sur vivor-, part ner-, rela tion ship-, and atti tude-specific state ments. Survivors of childhood sexual abuse are more likely to experience a variety of trauma symptoms in adulthood than adults who have not experienced childhood sexual abuse (reviewed in Beitchman et al., 1992; Polusny & Follette, 1995). These symptoms include depression (Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992), suicidality (Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, & Kilpatrick 1992), anxiety disorders (Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992), dissociative experiences (Briere & Runtz, 1987), sexual problems (Davis, Petretic-Jackson, & Ting, 2001), relationship problems (Davis et al., 2001), problems with sleep (Briere&Runtz, 1987), and borderline personality disorder (Herman, Perry, & van der Kolk, 1989). Because only some adult survivors experience these symptoms to a troubling degree in adulthood (Fromoth, 1986), it is possible that etiological factors such as differences in life stress are associated with these symptoms. A number of studies have shown that trauma symptoms are related to the severity of stressful life events. This has been found in research on various kinds of traumatic events, including among victims of motor vehicle accidents (Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 1998), Vietnam veterans (Green, Grace, Lindy, Gleser, & Leonard, 1990), Cambodian refugees (Carlson & Rosser- Hogan, 1991), and survivors of a firestorm (Koopman, Classen, & Spiegel, 1994). It has been noted that a history of exposure to extreme psychological stress appears to make a person more vulnerable to experiencing psychological distress when stressors occur later in life (Bremner, Southwick, & Charney, 1995). Sexual abuse in childhood is a major risk factor for later sexual revictimization (Chu, 1992; Koss & Dinero, 1989) and may lead to greater sexual problems (Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992) as well as other trauma symptoms (Koverola, Proulx, Battle, & Hanna, 1996). One model that can explain the vulnerability of some individuals to trauma symptoms is “sensitization” (Post,Weiss,&Smith, 1995). The idea is that the original trauma initiates the first episode of symptoms, but as repeated stressful life events occur, there is a progressive sensitivity to becoming symptomatic (McFarlane&Yehuda, 1996). Sensitivitywould also be influenced by the severity of the life events. Our findings provide evidence that recent life stressors and sexual revictimization during adulthood are associated with severity of traumatic stress symptoms in women who have been sexually abused in childhood and who meet the DSM-IV criteria for current PTSD. Our findings provide some evidence for the sensitization model that an initial traumatic event renders an individual vulnerable to becoming symptomatic following subsequent stressful life events. Within the past 20 years, we have learned that the mental health effects of this crime are devastating as rape survivors are the largest group of persons with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Foa & Rothbaum, 1998). (REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) …emerging research suggests that rape survivors experience more acute and chronic physical health problems than do women who are not victimized (Golding, 1994; Koss, Koss, & Woodruff, 1991). Sexual assault also affects women’s sexual health risk-taking behaviors and places some at greater risk for contracting HIV (Campbell, Sefl,&Ahrens, 2004). (REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) Research indicates that coping with the aftermath of rape can cause significant stress for the family, friends, and significant others of sexual assault survivors (Ahrens & Campbell, 2000; Burge, 1983; Remer & Elliott, 1988). (REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) Accordingly, we suggest that when a rape is committed by a known and trusted other, it is probably schema discrepant, and assimilation rather than accommodation should be the more likely outcome. Some support for this notion comes from prior work showing that women raped by known offenders, compared with those raped by strangers, were more likely to show evidence of assimilation by 3 months postrape (Mechanic, Resick, & Griffin, 1994). Assimilation should decrease the likelihood of emotional or information processing of the trauma experience, increasing the likelihood of recall deficits. Thus, we hypothesized that women raped by men they knew would be more likely to suffer from memory failure for parts of the rape compared with women raped by strangers. More recently, CPA has been tested as a predictor of sexual/social outcomes. For example, Widom and Kuhns (1996) investigated the relationship between early childhood maltreatment and subsequent promiscuity, prostitution, and teenage pregnancy. Although CPAwas not associated with a higher risk of promiscuity or teenage pregnancy, it was significantly associated with an increased risk of prostitution. The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE Compound abuse, where a victim suffers two or more forms of abuse either concurrently or separately, is more likely to lead to negative psychological outcomes than any single form of childhood abuse (Bryer et al., 1987; Fox & Gilbert, 1994; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1996). For example, Fox and Gilbert (1994) found that women who reported experiencing more than one form of abuse in childhood also reported they were significantly more depressed than those who suffered one form of abuse in childhood. The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE In an important finding, the current study indicated that a history of CPA can contribute to a poor psychological outcome in adult life. Results showed trauma-related symptomatology to be associated with either CPA or CSA. This suggests that victimization through the use of violence may result in numbed responsiveness, withdrawal, reexperience of traumatic events, and other symptoms consonant with PTSD. Thus, psychological damage is likely to be as severe among CPA victims as it is among CSA victims, resulting in similar patterns of post-traumatic symptomatology for both groups. The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE A wide array of social, psychological, and somatic problems has been connected with childhood sexual abuse (CSA). These problems include sleep disorders, eating disorders, selfmutilation, social withdrawal, antisocial behavior, sexual dysfunction, injured sense of self, and disorders of attachment ( Bagley & Ramsay, 1985 ; Briere & Runtz, 1989 Browne & Finkelhor, 1986 ; Cohen & Mannarino, 1988; Finkelhor, 1987 ; Herman, 1981; Roth & Lebowitz, 1988; Young, 1992 ) . The consequences of undetected abuse compound the immediate trauma in child victims and are associated with grave developmental outcomes typically characterized by impaired capacities for trust, intimacy, and sexuality, and by a variety of chronic mental health problems. (Title: Variables in Delayed Disclosure of Childhood Sexual Abuse , By: Eli Somer, Sharona Szwarcberg, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 0002-9432, July 1, 2001, Vol. 71, Issue 3) Using a definition of rape that includes forced vaginal, oral, and anal sex, the National Violence AgainstWomen Survey found that one of six U.S. women and one of 33 U.S. men has experienced an attempted or completed rape as a child and/or adult (Tjaden&Thoennes, 1998). (The Experience of Sexual Assault Findings From a Statewide Victim Needs Assessment, LAURA M. MONROE, LINDA M. KINNEY, MARK D. WEIST, DENISE SPRIGGS DAFEAMEKPOR, JOYCE DANTZLER, MATTHEW W. REYNOLDS, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 7, July 2005 767-776) Sexual assault is associated with psychological morbidity including depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and anxiety (Ackerman, Newton, McPherson, Jones, & Dykman, 1998; Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995; Ellis, 1983; Roth & Lebowitz, 1988; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). Long-term effects of child sexual abuse include suicidal behavior, personality disturbances, substance abuse, eating disorders, and revictimization (Brier & Runtz, 1987; Chandy, Blum, & Resnick, 1996; Dube et al., 2001; McCauley et al., 1997; Wonderlich et al., 2001). (The Experience of Sexual Assault Findings From a Statewide Victim Needs Assessment, LAURA M. MONROE, LINDA M. KINNEY, MARK D. WEIST, DENISE SPRIGGS DAFEAMEKPOR, JOYCE DANTZLER, MATTHEW W. REYNOLDS, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 7, July 2005 767-776) Posledice CHILDHOOD SEXUAL ABUSE is readily understood to be a traumatic event both at time of occurrence and for years subsequent to the actual abuse. Specifically, a history of childhood sexual abuse has been associated with consequent mood alteration such as depression and anxiety; hindered or impaired cognitive functioning; negative self-evaluation; difficulty in trusting others/interpersonal relationships; and behavioral difficulties such as substance abuse, suicidality, self-harm, delinquent activity and learning disabilities (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Beitchman, Zucker, Hood, daCosta, & Akman, 1991; Briere & Elliott, 1994; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Gomes-Schwartz, Horowitz, & Cardarelli, 1990; Green, 1993; Kluft, 1990; Trickett & Putnam, 1993). Some research reports that exposure to traumatic events such as sexual abuse in childhood, is associated with the leading causes of death in adulthood, including heart disease, cancer, chronic lung disease, skeletal fractures and liver disease (Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards, Koss, & Marks, 1998). Literature thus far indicates that individuals with histories of childhood sexual abuse also report using health services at significantly higher rates than non-sexually abused individuals (Arnow, Hart, Scott, Dea, O’Connell, & Taylor, 1999; Leserman, Li, Drossman, & Hu, 1998; Walker, Gelfand, Katon, Koss, Von-Korff, Bernstein, & Russo, 1999). Physical sequelae to childhood sexual abuse Different types of physical conditions appear to be particularly common in individuals with histories of childhood sexual abuse. Review of the literature indicates that documented physical sequelae to childhood sexual abuse can be broken down into at least three categories, including gastrointestinal and gynecological problems, obesity and somatic complaints. Gastrointestinal and gynecological problems A number of studies identify a relationship between childhood sexual abuse and the presence of either or both gastrointestinal disorders and gynecologic problems. Drossman, Leserman, Nachman, Zhiming, Gluck, Toomey, and Mitchell (1990) studied female gastroenterology patients to identify prevalence of sexual or physical abuse history. Patients with a functional gastrointestinal disorder (e.g. irritable bowel syndrome, non-ulcer dyspepsia, chronic abdominal pain), chronic or recurrent pelvic pain were more likely to have an abuse history (Drossman et al., 1990; Drossman, Talley, Leserman, Olden, & Barreiro, 1995; Lechner, Vogel, Garcia-Shelton, Leichter, & Steibel, 1993; Leserman, Drossman, Li, Toomey, Nachman, & Glogau, 1996; Leserman, Li, Hu, & Drossman, 1998; Walker, Katon, HarropGriffiths, Holm, Russo, & Hickok, 1988). This finding is corroborated by studies solely examining abuse effects, where irritable bowel syndrome and dyspepsia have been associated with sexual abuse history (Longstreth & WoldeTsadik, 1993; Tally, Boyce, & Jones, 1998; Talley, Helgeson, & Zinsmeister, 1992). Research also consistently supports a relationship between gynecological disorders and childhood sexual abuse. Samples from the general population indicate a relationship between childhood abuse (physical or sexual) and ‘severe menstrual problems’ and urinary tract infections (Plichta & Carmella, 1996). Other gynecologic problems often associated with sexual assault history include: sexually transmitted diseases, pelvic inflammatory disease, multiple yeast infections, premenstrual syndrome, early hysterectomy, excessive menstrual bleeding, genital burning, painful intercourse, dysmenorrhea, menstrual irregularity, lack of sexual pleasure and non-specific gynecologic problems (Bachmann, Moeller, & Benett, 1988; Golding, 1996). Obesity Another line of research indicates a relationship between childhood sexual abuse and the physical conditions of obesity and weight gain. This relationship is reported in various medical patients (Felitti, 1991; Felitti et al., 1998; Moeller, Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993; Sansone, Sansone, & Fine, 1995); females enrolled in weight loss programs (King, Clark, & Pera, 1996); and in studies of both obese and non-obese females (Wiederman, Sansone, & Sansone, 1999). Regrettably, none of these studies specifically sampled females with a history of sexual abuse. While obesity may also be secondary to events other than sexual abuse (e.g. depression), research consistently supports the sexual abuse–obesity relationship. Somatic complaints Lastly, review of the findings of physical health problems and childhood sexual abuse would be incomplete without mention of the more broad-based category— ‘Somatic Complaints’. ‘Somatic Complaints’ is a categorization of physical symptoms, including any ‘physical complaints without known biological cause’ (Briere, 1992) as opposed to a symptom-related syndrome or disorder as in the case of gastrointestinal and gynecological problems. This category is typically used to indicate an array of physical complaints often associated with sexual abuse yet differentiated from gastrointestinal and gynecological disorders, including: headaches, sleep disturbance, anorexia, asthma, shortness of breath, chronic muscle tension, muscle spasms and elevated blood pressure (Briere, 1992; Felitti, 1991; Lechner et al., 1993). Patients with a sexual abuse history report: greater fatigue; more headaches; increased back, breast and face pain; increased skin and respiratory problems; increased shortness of breath and choking sensations; decreased appetite; decreased sleep; less satisfaction with their overall health status; a greater number of actual somatic symptoms; and increased engagement in health risk behaviors versus non-abused patients (Leserman, Li, Drossman, & Hu, 1998; McCauley, Kern, Kolodner, Dill, Schroeder, DeChant, Ryden, Derogatis, & Bass, 1997; Moeller et al., 1993; Walker et al., 1988). Sexual abuse has also been associated with reports of poorer overall health; greater functional limitation; increased chronic disease; increased medically explained and medically unexplained complaints; greater general sleep problems and nightmares; and more pain and muscular tension including headaches (Bendixen, Muus & Schei, 1994; Briere & Runtz, 1987; Golding, 1994, 1999; Golding, Cooper, & George, 1997; Lechner et al., 1993; Springs & Friedrich, 1992). In some cases where controlled for, this association remained regardless of individual characteristics and level of depression (Golding et al., 1997). Zdravstvo Research with HMO enrollees suggests that females with a history of abuse spend significantly more money on medical healthcare costs than females without an abuse history, even when mental health costs were controlled. Furthermore, females with an abuse history have been significantly more likely to use the emergency room for treatment than females with a history of physical abuse or neglect (Arnow et al., 1999; Walker, Gelfand, Katon, Koss, Von-Korff, Bernstein, & Russo, 1999). Among general medical, gastrointestinal and gynecological patients, patients with a history of childhood abuse (sexual and physical) report higher rates of hospitalization; more days in bed due to disability; more doctor visits; and a greater number of lifetime surgeries than nonabused patients (Drossman et al., 1990, 1995; Felitti, 1991; Leserman et al., 1996; Leserman, Li, Drossman, & Hu, 1998; Leserman, Li, Hu, & Drossman, 1998; Moeller et al., 1993). In addition, data from samples more specific to sexual abuse indicate a higher incidence of medical healthcare in abused patients. Studies of victims of adult sexual assault indicate that females who experience sexual assault crimes report significantly poorer general and mental health, endorse more physical symptoms and report more outpatient visits than did nonassaulted females (Golding, Stein, Siegel, Burnam, & Sorenson, 1988; Koss, Koss, & Woodruf, 1991). Leserman et al.’s (1996) study found that the severity of sexual and physical abuse history (injury during abuse, having multiple perpetrators, being raped) explained adult health status. Subsequently, Leserman, Li, Drossman and Hu (1998) found that females with more severe sexual abuse (penetration versus other contact) reported more: physical symptoms, functional disability and healthcare visits than less severely abused or non-abused females. Similarly, Leserman, Li, Hu and Drossman (1998) found that level of severity, as defined by the type of sexual contact (penetration versus other contact), was the strongest predictor of current health status in gastrointestinal patients with childhood sexual abuse history. In summary, a consistent relationship has been found between history of childhood sexual abuse and the presence of certain adult physical problems. It appears that females with a history of childhood sexual abuse are more likely: to experience increased physical health problems, including gastrointestinal and gynecological disorders, obesity and miscellaneous somatic complaints; to report decreased satisfaction with overall physical health; and to require increased health services utilization relative to non-abused females. Nevropsihološke posledice Several studies of maltreated individuals have found significant dysregulation of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, most prominently increased cortisol levels, ACTHblunting to corticotrophin releasing hormone and flattening of the normal circadian rhythm for cortisol (DeBellis, Baum, Birmaher, Heshaven, Eccard, Boring, Jenkins, & Ryan, 1999; DeBellis, Chrousos, Dorn, Burke, Helmers, Kling, Trickett, & Putnam, 1994; Gunnar, Morrison, Chisholm, & Schuder, 2001; Kaufman, Birmaher, Perel, Dahl, Moreci, Nelson, Wells, & Ryan, 1997). Heightened sympathetic nervous system activity manifested by increased 24-hour urinary catecholamines has been noted in two samples of traumatized children (DeBellis, Baum, Birmaher, Heshaven, Eccard, Boring, Jenkins, & Ryan, 1999; DeBellis, Lefter, Trickett, & Putnam, 1994). There is also preliminary evidence for immune system compromise in sexually abused girls (De Bellis, Burke, Trickett, & Putnam, 1996). Most recently, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has identified structural abnormalities in the brains of traumatized children that correlate with post-traumatic stress disorder and dissociative symptoms (DeBellis, Keshavan, Clark, Casey, Giedd, Boring, Frustaci, & Ryan, 1999). (The Long-term Physical Health and Healthcare Utilization of Women Who Were Sexually Abused as Children, JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 7(5)) Negative environmental factors such as family conflict and stress can contribute to the development of psychopathology in insecurely attached individuals, whereas securely attached individuals who experience stress fail to develop psychopathology or pedophilia (Finkelhor, 1990; Lewis, Feiring, McGuffog, & Jaskir, 1984; Rind et al., 1998). From this perspective, an individual can develop a vulnerability to environmental problems depending on the nature of their early attachment experiences. Disturbances in the developmental continuity of attachment may limit an adult’s ability to have their needs met appropriately, regulate their emotional well-being, respond empathically to the needs of others, and seek assistance to ameliorate abusive behaviours. The above personal characteristics have been found to be lacking in individuals who sexually abuse children (Pithers, Kashima, Cummings, Beal, & Buell, 1988). Early deviant sexual experiences may serve as a template for later deviant sexual behaviours, and inconsistent caregiving serves to undermine the security of attachment; all this contributes to disturbances in an individual’s ability to form intimate relationships (Hudson&Ward, 1997; Marshall, 1989). An adult who has not developed a secure attachment system may construe early abusive sexual experiences as enticing because they represent a form of intimacy regardless of their deviant nature. Consequently, males who have not experienced secure attachment may be vulnerable to placating intimacy needs by engaging in pedophilic behaviour(Ward, Hudson,&Marshall, 1996), particularly when interpersonal stressors such as relationship dissolution, rejection, and separation threaten their intimacy needs (Marshall, 1989). In contrast, those who have developed a secure attachment in childhood may be more resilient to interpersonal trauma and intimacy problems (Masten & O’Connor, 1989). A securely attached sexual abuse victim may not become a child sexual abuser because resilience in the form of secure attachment may contribute to an individual’s ability to deal with relationships and stressors. The victims and the pedophiles came from similar, self-reported, abusive backgrounds (see Table 3) but the results of this study indicated that the controls and victims were both more securely attached than the pedophiles, who were found to be insecurely attached on two constructs, namely, high on Relationships as Secondary and lowon Confidence (lowsecure attachment). The finding for the controls and pedophiles is in line with the research of Ward et al. (1996). Currently, there are no published results relating to the quality of attachment for male nonoffending victims of sexual abuse. The findings reported here provide some support for the ASQ as a measure of attachment and as a clinical tool to measure differences in attachment style. These results support the notion that an insecure attachment style may make a man vulnerable to developing pedophilic behaviours. Conversely, those men with a secure attachment style are more resilient to participating in pedophilic behaviour, even if they have experienced neglect and/or abuse as children. The current findings are consistent with the suggestion (Hudson & Ward, 1997) that insecure attachment represents a vulnerability with intimate relationships. It isworth considering, however, that there are likely to be individual differences between insecure styles of attachment, and these variations may not be congruent with specific offending behavior. This has implications for treatment, as attachment style may provide a better basis for understanding the psychological processes associated with offenders and victims rather than categories of psychopathology and criminality. These findings may be underscored by the finding that both groups experienced significant childhood victimisation, with 64% of pedophiles and 68% of victims reporting a “severely stressful” childhood. In contrast, controls indicated that they had not experienced childhood sexual activity with an adult and none reported that their childhood was “severely stressful.” Both victims and pedophiles reported that prior to the age of 14 they had engaged “very often” in sexual activity with an adult (43% and 43%, respectively). This finding may bring into question the contribution of sexual abuse as a discrete factor in the development of pedophilic behavior (Finkelhor, 1986, 1990). Pedophiles reported more sexual abuse experiences with a relative (82%) in comparison with victims (37%). This “stranger” factor is thought to mitigate against the adverse effects of sexual abuse (Briggs & Hawkins, 1996; Finklehor, 1990). In this study, both the controls and victims were found to have secure attachments, even though the pedophiles and the nonoffending victims had experienced similar levels of neglect and abuse. “children with sexual behavior problems” Further support of the relationship between childhood sexual abuse and SBP is found in the work of Johnson (1988, 1989). The relationship between sexual victimization and demonstrating SBP may be stronger in preschool-age children than in school-age children for boys. In a study of 47 boys with SBP, Johnson (1988) found that 72% of the 4- to 6-year-olds had a history of being sexually abused, whereas 42% of the 7- to 10-year-olds and 35% of the 11- and 12-year-olds had such a history. Girls with SBP may be more likely to have a history of child sexual abuse than boys. In a sample of 13 female children with SBP (ages 4 to 12 years), Johnson (1989) found that 100% of the children had a history of child sexual abuse. The intense and wide range of problems that these young children exhibited was quite striking. In addition to having a mean level of SBP at the 99th percentile on the CSBI, the children had a complex array of other behavior and emotional symptoms and experienced multiple stressful events, including changes in caregivers and home placements. As hypothesized, on the PSI, the caregivers reported significant stress associated with raising the children and during interviews also reported distress specifically associated with observing and responding to the sexual behaviors. Furthermore, the rate of depressive symptoms was quite remarkable, with 6 children reaching full criteria for MDD. Symptoms of depression and anxiety in childhood are often overlapping and interrelated. The relationship between internalizing symptoms and SBP remains unclear. Furthermore, assessment and diagnosis of PTSD in children is complicated by developmental factors, social factors, and comorbid conditions (March, 1999). Caregivers may not be aware of the thoughts and internal reactions young children are experiencing and thus have difficulty responding to standard questions about PTSD symptoms. A subgroup of these children demonstrated verbal delays, which would further inhibit the identification of internal distress. In addition, the avoidance symptoms may be reduced in many of children who were no longer living in the homes in which they experienced the trauma. Furthermore, experiencing physical abuse may increase the likelihood of demonstrating interpersonal SBP in young children who have been sexually abused, perhaps by the impact on feelings of anger and shame and beliefs about use of control with others (Hall et al., 1998). Another potentially critical factor that was not assessed in the current study is child neglect. Childhood neglect has been found to be associated with significant behavior problems, including increased risk of sex crimes as an adult (Widom & Ames, 1994). Poor impulse-control skills, other aggressive behaviors, and inaccurate perceptions of social stimuli in some children with SBP further hinder social relationships and cause problems at school (Araji, 1997; Friedrich & Luecke, 1988; Gil & Johnson, 1993; Horton, 1996). In addition, poor boundaries and indiscriminate friendliness often found in young children with SBP may place them at increased risk of being victimized. Raising children with SBP is often stressful for the caregiver and may lead to dysfunctional adult-child interactions and disruptions in the child’s residential placement. Indeed, in the present study, caregivers reported stress associated with raising these young children with SBP, and many of the children had already experienced changes in their residential placements. In particular, parents of asymptomatic sexually abused children express concerns about whether their children need to get out their feelings to prevent future difficulties or that their children will grow up to be molesters themselves. Mannarino, A. P., Cohen, J. A., Smith, J. A., & MooreMotily, S. (1991). Six and twelve-month follow-up of sexually abused girls. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6, 494-511. Animportant issue with regard to this topic is whether existing assessment measures are adequately detecting all of the psychological problems that sexually abused children may exhibit. Some examples are Friedrich’s Child Sexual Behavior Inventory and Briere’s Trauma Symptom Checklist for Children, which assess abuse-related sequelae, and Mannarino and Cohen’s Children’s Attributions and Perceptions Scale, which measures cognitive variables that are correlated with psychological symptomatology. One study (Mannarino, Cohen, Smith, & Moore-Motily, 1991) found during a 1-year follow-up that sexually abused girls who had been subjected to intercourse had significantly more emotional and behavioral symptoms than thosewhohad experienced fondling only. This finding suggests that there may be a sleeper effect related to the type of sexual abuse that manifests itself over time. Unfortunately, the length of the follow-up in this study was relatively brief and may not have been adequate to assess more long-term problems. It should be noted that although the child maltreatment field has come a long way in terms of the development of more sophisticated assessment measures that tap abuse-related sequelae, we still know very little about whether traumatic experiences cause subtle vulnerabilities in children that do not reach the level of symptoms but which, in combination with other factors, may have an adverse impact in later developmental periods. Despite this lack of empirical data, many clinicians provide short-term interventions for asymptomatic sexually abused children, including body awareness training and safety education. In addition, some sessions with par192 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / April 2000 ents of asymptomatic children can be invaluable in terms of normalizing common parental reactions to disclosure, providing reassurance, and discussing what potential problems parents can be looking out for in the future. The extensive literature on the sequelae of rape provides a wealth of evidence that the experience of rape often leads to frank sexual dysfunction as well as general intimacy dif. culties in the victims (e.g. Becker et al., 1986; Feldman-Summers et al., 1979). Loss of libido, anorgasmia and sexual aversions and phobias are common among the sexual effects. There is an extensive literature which shows that sexual abuse in childhood can have serious negative consequences for a person’s later sexual functioning (e.g. Courtois, 1979; Finkelhor, 1990; Jehu, 1988). Further, there is strong evidence that those who have been sexually tortured (e.g. forced penetration with objects; infliction of pain on the genital organs) can develop sexual dysfunction (e.g. Lunde & Ortmann, 1990). Further, there is strong evidence that those who have been sexually tortured (e.g. forced penetration with objects; in• iction of pain on the genital organs) can develop sexual dysfunction (e.g. Lunde & Ortmann, 1990). Recent research has implicated emotional abuse as a strong, possibly stronger, predictor than physical abuse of internalizing disorders, externalizing disorders, social impairment, low selfesteem, suicidal behavior, psychiatric diagnoses, psychiatric hospitalizations, and long-term psychological functioning (Kaplan et al., 1999; McGee, Wolfe, & Wilson, 1997; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1996; Vissing, Straus, Gelles, & Harrop, 1991). Emotional Abuse in Children: Variations in Legal Definitions and Rates Across the United States Stephanie Hamarman, New Jersey Medical School, Kayla H. Pope, American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Sally J. Czaja, New Jersey Medical School CHILDMALTREATMENT,Vol. 7, No. 4, November 2002 303-311 DOI: 10.1177/107755902237261 Women in the study who had been raped were more likely to experience posttraumatic stress disorder, major depression, attempted suicide, and drug and alcohol problems than were women who had not been victims of violent crimes. Other studies have shown that women who have been raped also report more physical problems, such as chronic pelvic pain, gastrointestinal disorders, headaches, general pain, psychogenic seizures, and premenstrual symptoms (Koss & Heslet, 1992). (Women’s Responses to Sexual Violence by Male Intimates Claire Burke Draucker Phyllis Noerager Stern Western Journal of Nursing Research, 2000, 22(4), 385-406) Survivors of childhood sexual abuse are more likely to experience a variety of trauma symptoms in adulthood than adults who have not experienced childhood sexual abuse (reviewed in Beitchman et al., 1992; Polusny & Follette, 1995). These symptoms include depression (Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992), suicidality (Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, & Kilpatrick 1992), anxiety disorders (Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992), dissociative experiences (Briere & Runtz, 1987), sexual problems (Davis, Petretic-Jackson, & Ting, 2001), relationship problems (Davis et al., 2001), problems with sleep (Briere&Runtz, 1987), and borderline personality disorder (Herman, Perry, & van der Kolk, 1989). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290) Approximately one in four women are raped in their adult lifetime, which causes severe psychological distress and long-term physical health problems. The impact of sexual assault extends far beyond rape survivors as their family, friends, and significant others are also negatively affected. Moreover, those who help rape victims, such as rape victim advocates, therapists, as well as sexual assault researchers, can experience vicarious trauma. Future research and advocacy should focus on improving the community response to rape and the prevention of sexual assault. (Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault 20 Years of Progress and Future Directions REBECCA CAMPBELL,SHARON M. WASCO JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) From that founding research on the prevalence of rape, other researchers began to document the widespread deleterious effects that sexual assault has on women’s lives.Within the past 20 years, we have learned that the mental health effects of this crime are devastating as rape survivors are the largest group of persons with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Foa & Rothbaum, 1998). The inclusion of PTSD into the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) in 1980was a major conceptual development in the study of trauma associated with sexual violence. Although this framework may be limited in its ability to capture fully the nature of sexual assault (see Wasco, 2003), it has spawned a proliferation of research documenting the psychological injury caused by rape. Beyond this focus on psychological impact, emerging research suggests that rape survivors experience more acute and chronic physical health problems than do women who are not victimized (Golding, 1994; Koss, Koss, & Woodruff, 1991). Sexual assault also affects women’s sexual health risk-taking behaviors and places some at greater risk for contracting HIV (Campbell, Sefl,&Ahrens, 2004). (Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault 20 Years of Progress and Future Directions REBECCA CAMPBELL,SHARON M. WASCO JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131) Establishing appropriate role boundaries with sexually abused children and their families is a challenging ethical issue for all practicing clinicians who provide services to this population. This can best be accomplished prior to any clinical contact with a family. For example, a parent may call with concerns that his or her child has been sexually abused and about possible associated behaviors/symptoms such as sexually inappropriate behaviors, separation anxiety, or sleep problems. This parent may want to know whether the child has been abused, how to deal with visitation and custody issues, and also how to deal with the presenting behavioral difficulties. (TREATING SEXUALLY ABUSED CHILDREN AND THEIR FAMILIES Identifying and Avoiding Professional Role Conflicts ANTHONY P. MANNARINO,JUDITH A. COHEN TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 2, No. 4, October 2001 331-342) Women who have been raped experience a range of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral symptoms. They are more likely to meet the diagnostic criteria for a number of mental disorders than women who have not experienced violence, including posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety disorders, depression, and drug and alcohol abuse (Burnam et al., 1988; Kilpatrick et al., 1985; Winfield, George, Swartz, & Blazer, 1990). Victims of a sexual assault also are at increased risk for suicide attempts (Bridgeland, Duane, & Stewart, 2001; Kilpatrick et al., 1985). In addition to acute medical symptoms (Resnick, Acierno, Holmes, Dammeyer, & Kilpatrick, 2000), victimization also can affect long-term health and sexual functioning (Becker, Skinner, Abel, & Treacy, 1982; Ellis, Calhoun, & Atkeson, 1980; Golding, 1994; Koss, Koss, & Woodruff, 1991). Rape can shatter awoman’s feelings of safety and security, leaving her feeling vulnerable and helpless (Janoff-Bulman, 1985). The woman may find herself in a state of disequilibrium and struggle to come to terms with her victimization, to define her experience, and to reconstruct her beliefs about the world (Janoff-Bulman, 1985; Lebowitz & Roth, 1994). (The Role of Sexual Victimization in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64) Women who have been sexually victimized through force or threat of force differ from nonvictimized women when rating women’s target behavior. Women who have been sexually victimized perceived less sexual interest when rating women’s target behaviors than women who have not been victimized. The lower ratings of sexual interest for women’s behaviors may be the result of self-protective attributions by women who have been assaulted and have experienced victim blame following their own sexual assault. In addition, it may be that victimized women perceive a wider range of behaviors as not indicating sexual interest. Thus, victimized women may not believe that their behavior is communicating an interest in sexual activities even when others would interpret the behavior in this manner. As a result, sexually victimizedwomen may underestimate the sexual connotativeness of their behavior. (The Role of Sexual Victimization in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64) Women who have experienced sexual aggression may be more alert for signs of sexual interest so they can avoid unwanted sexual advances. Because sexually victimized women report more experiences with having their sexual intent misperceived (Abbey et al., 1996), they may be alert for potentialmisperceptions. Sexually victimized women may view men’s behavior more sexually as a protective measure. Women’s increased vigilance may be a way that victims assert agency. Through increased awareness,women may feel better able to protect themselves and to make decisions about their sexual safety. (The Role of Sexual Victimization in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64) The finding that women who have been victimized through force or threat of force are more vigilant appears to contradict the finding that victimization increases the risk of future victimization. Although women who have been victimized appear to be more vigilant of signs of men’s sexual interest, they may not be able to effectively detect risk at a stage early enough in the interaction to permit escape from a sexually aggressive man. Women who have experienced repeated victimization did respond more slowly to sexually threatening situations in an analogue study (Wilson et al., 1999) and also may be slower to respond in circumstances that pose a danger of revictimization. (The Role of Sexual Victimization in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64) Past research has found that, on average, victimized women have a history of a greater number of sexual partners and sexual experiences (Himelein, Vogel, &Wachowiak, 1994; Koss, 1985; Koss & Dinero, 1989). Generalizing these results, we might predict that victimized women with a history of sexual experience may be better able to accurately recognize sexual interest in a partner than women with little sexual experience. Indeed, in this study, women who reported sexual victimization experiences also reported a greater number of sexual partners. This finding of increased sexual activity, however, may be a consequence of prior victimization (Koss & Cleveland, 1997). In addition, increased sexual activity may place a woman at greater risk for encountering a sexually aggressive man. This finding is compatible with other research that has found that sexually conservative women (Himelein, 1995) and women who regularly attend religious services (Mynatt&Allgeier, 1990) are less likely to be sexually victimized. In Himelein’s study, sexually conservative women also scored higher on scales assessing adversarial sexual beliefs and acceptance of rape myths. In addition, these women reported fewer consensual sexual experiences and less assertiveness. Thus, it may be that sexual conservativism reduces women’s risk through their greater wariness and mistrust of men’s sexual motives and through their decreased involvement in sexual activity. (The Role of Sexual Victimization in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64) Depression is the symptom most commonly reported by adult survivors of CSA (Beitchman et al., 1992; Browne &Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny&Follette, 1995). Indeed, numerous studies find a relationship between CSA and depression or depressive symptoms (Braver, Bumberry, Green, & Rawson, 1992; Briere & Runtz, 1988; Hunter, 1991; Jackson, Calhoun, Amick, Maddever, & Habif, 1990; Roland, Zelhart, & Dubes, 1989;Yama, Tovey,&Fogas, 1993).A majority of investigations also report a higher prevalence of major depressive disorder among sexually abused than nonabused participants (Polusny & Follette, 1995). CSA survivors also report chronic and recurrent depression during adulthood (Andrews, 1995) and longer depressive episodes in comparison to nonabused participants (Zlotnick, Mattia,&Zimmerman, 2001). Thus, the accumulated evidence points to depression and depressive symptoms as significant long-term correlates of CSA. Researchers have commonly conceptualized CSA as a major risk factor for the development of depression and other difficulties in adulthood (Briere, 1992; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny & Follette, 1995). Given these difficulties, it is not surprising that adult CSA survivors also frequently report social isolation (Harter, Alexander, & Neimeyer, 1988), poor social adjustment (Follette, Alexander, & Follette, 1991; Harter et al., 1988; Jackson et al., 1990), and considerable distress and dissatisfaction in their relationships (e.g., Briere, 1988; DiLillo& Long, 1999; Feinauer et al., 1996; Herman, 1992; Hunter, 1991). CSA survivors report difficulties in forming trusting, intimate relationships (Gorcey, Santiago,&McCall-Perez, 1986; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, &Herbison, 1994), being distant and controlling in relationships (Whiffen et al., 2000), and avoiding the development of close adult relationships due to fears of rejection (Alexander, 1993). Thus, the first generation of research has shown that CSA is a risk factor for various forms of emotional distress, particularly depression, anxiety, PTSD, and dissociation. Having documented this association, researchers have moved onto a second generation of research, the aim of which is to understand the causal mechanisms underlying this association; that is, why are CSA survivors at risk for emotional distress? Children who are sexually abused may be at risk for feelings of shame and self-blame, especially when the abuse was prolonged or when the perpetrator or significant others blamed the child for the abuse. Individuals with a history ofCSAare likely to abuse alcohol as adults; researchers conceptualize alcohol use in this population as a form of coping with the distress generated by the CSA or as an attempt to self-medicate (Briere, 1988; Ireland & Widom, 1994; Lindberg & Distad, 1985; Moeller, Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993). History of childhood sexual abuse (CSA) has been linked to increased risk for long-term consequences on the lives of survivors (e.g., Adams-Tucker, 1982; Briere & Runtz, 1988; Brooks, 1983; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Peters, 1988; Spaccarelli, 1994; Trickett & Putnam, 1993). For example, CSA survivors are more likely to experience symptoms related to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), such as moments of increased arousal and extreme affective reactivity, and alternatively, a general predisposition toward numbing of responsiveness, and emotional experiencing (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). There has been considerable focus in recent years on developing therapeutic interventions that target the cognitions and emotional experiences associated with traumatic symptoms experienced by CSA survivors. Treatments of choice typically focus on helping CSA survivors gain a greater sense of emotional stability around the traumatic episode through a variety of interventions, such as support and corrective processing of the cognitions around the event, imaginary or actual exposure (with the goal of increased habituation or decreased anxiety), and stress management, among others (e.g., Deblinger, McLeer, & Henry, 1990; Foa et al., 1999; Rothbaum, Meadows, Resick, & Foy, 2000). Although there is a growing consensus by scholars and practitioners in the identification and treatment of PTSD symptoms in cases of CSA, until lately, little attention had been given to investigating emotional coherence among this group. Specifically, there is an imperative need for further research to clarify how physiological, experiential, and expressive response domains in emotional experiencing (e.g., Ekman, 1992; Levenson, 1994) may inform psychological functioning and response to treatment. Womenwho are exposed to childhood sexual abuse (CSA)may experience a number of negative outcomes, some of which are evident in high rates of CSA histories in women accessing inpatient and outpatient psychiatric services (Goodman, Rosenberg, Mueser, & Drake, 1997; Mitchell, Grindel, & Laurenzano, 1996; Read, 1997). In terms of general measures of psychological disturbance, psychiatric patients who have experienced abuse are symptomatic and receive psychiatric care at an earlier age (Briere & Zaidi, 1989; DarvesBornoz, Lemperiere, Degiovanni, & Gaillard, 1995; Goff, Brotman, Kindlon, Waites, & Amico, 1991; Read, 1998), experience more frequent/longer hospitalizations and more frequent relapses (Darves- Bornoz et al., 1995; Goff et al., 1991; Read, 1998), and are more likely to be prescribed psychotropic medications (Sansonnet-Hayden, Haley, Marriage, & Fine, 1987). In addition, childhood abuse has been specifically linked to a number of clinical problems in adulthood including suicidal ideation and attempts (Read, Agar, Barker-Collo, Davies, & Moskowitz, 2001; Resnick & Newton, 1992); eating disorders and self-mutilation (Briere&Runtz, 1989); addictive behaviors (Cameron, 1994); poor social adjustment, depression, and anxiety (Briere & Runtz, 1989); low self-esteem (Jehu, 1989); somatization (Gelinas, 1983); psychosis (Neria, Bromet, Sievers, Lavalle, & Fochtman, 2002; Read, Agar, Argyle, & Aderhold, in press; Read &Argyle, 1999; Read, Perry,Moskowitz,&Connolly, 2001; Ross, Anderson, & Clark, 1994); and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Kiser, Heston, Millsap, & Pruitt, 1991). In light of the huge range of difficulties that have been linked to CSA, it is apparent that there exist large, unexplained individual differences in the presence and presentation of mental health problems following CSA. Outcome assessment Povezanost med spolno zlorabo in PTSD For example, given the prevalence of CSA, abuse survivors may constitute the largest single group of PTSD sufferers (Foa, Steketee, & Rothbaum, 1989). However, the proportion of sexual abuse survivors developing clinical symptoms of PTSD is estimated at only 50% (Kiser et al., 1991). In populations of sexual abuse survivors referred for psychiatric examination, estimated prevalence of PTSD rises to 73% (O’Neil & Gupta, 1991). Thus, although exposure to a traumatic stressor such as sexual abuse is by definition necessary in the etiology of PTSD, the evidence suggests that experiencing sexual abuse is not sufficient to cause PTSD symptomatology. There exist large unexplained individual differences in the presence, severity, and persistence of PTSD symptomatology following abuse. POSLEDICE TRAVME – FIZIČNA ZLORABA Among the various types of child maltreatment, physical abuse has been studied most frequently and is defined by the World Health Organization (1999) as “all forms of physical ill-treatment . . . resulting in actual or potential harm to the child’s health, survival, development or dignity in the context of a relationship of responsibility, trust, or power” (p. 15). In addition to physical injuries, physical child abuse often leads to serious cognitive and socioemotional problems, including cognitive impairments and poor school performance (Eckenrode, Laird, & Doris, 1993), aggression, impulsiveness (Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997), poor peer relations, delinquency, later substance abuse (e.g., Azar & Wolfe, 1998; Erickson & Egeland, 2002), and greater likelihood of continuing the cycle of abuse as a parent POSLEDICE TRAVME – SMRT BLIŽNJEGA Specifically, children and adolescents who have experienced the death of a close relative are at greater risk for sleep problems, depressive symptoms, isolation, school performance problems, and suicidality (Harris, 1991; Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1996; Silverman & Worden, 1992; Valente, Saunders, & Street, 1988; Weller, Weller, Fristad, & Bowes, 1991; Worden, 1996;Worden, Davies, & McCown, 1999). In addition, death of a loved one during adolescence increases the risk of depression and suicidal behavior during adulthood (Adams, Overholser, & Lehnert, 1994; Fikelstein, 1988; Parker & Manicavasagar, 1986). Familial loss research has focused primarily on the deaths of parents, with relatively less attention being given to deaths of other close relatives and friends. Only a few studies have examined adolescent bereavement after the death of a close friend. Unfortunately, the majority of bereavement studies with adolescents have examined the effects of parental death, with few investigating peer death. Among the studies that have examined peer death, findings typically have been based on small community samples. Bereavement reactions including shock, numbness, sadness, anger, insomnia, survivor guilt, nightmares, loneliness, fear of own death, substance abuse, suicidal ideation, and school problems have been associated with the death of a peer during adolescence (McNeil, Silliman, & Swinhart, 1991; Sklar & Hartley, 1990; Ringler & Hayden, 2000). POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIONALNA ZLORABA (1992)Herman stated that the main dialectic of emotional trauma is the conflict between the need to deny unbearable experiences and the need to give testimony. (Title: Variables in Delayed Disclosure of Childhood Sexual Abuse , By: Eli Somer, Sharona Szwarcberg, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 0002-9432, July 1, 2001, Vol. 71, Issue 3) POSLEDICE TRAVME – MEDOSEBNI ODNOSI If, as Finkelhor and Browne (1985) suggested, early sexual trauma can profoundly “alter a child’s cognitive and emotional orientation toward the world and cause trauma by distorting the child’s self concept,world view, or affective capacities” (p. 531), then there is reason to suspect thatCSAmay disrupt survivors’ long-term interpersonal adjustment as well. These theories suggest that one of the consequences of having been sexually abused in childhood is the development of aberrant relationship models that in turn lead to interpersonal difficulties. More recently, research has begun to examine the impact of childhood abuse within an interpersonal context (Polusny&Follette, 1995). Although such studies typically focus on subsequent adult social adjustment, parenting, sexual functioning, high risk sexual behavior, and revictimization experiences, a limited number of studies (Ducharme, Koverola & Battle, 1997; Roche, Runtz, & Hunter, 1999; Whiffen, Judd, & Aube, 1999) have investigated the partner relationships (e.g., intimacy functioning) of childhood abuse survivors. Interpersonal Functioning Research suggests that survivors of childhood sexual abuse may experience difficulties in a number of interpersonal relationship contexts. Such findings were incorporated into recent theoretical models of the dynamics of child sexual abuse and its long-term correlates (Briere, 1992; Cole & Putnam, 1992; Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Polusny&Follette, 1995;Westerlund, 1992). For example, Polusny and Follette (1995) developed a theoretical model that highlights the function of emotional avoidance (Hayes, 1987) in determining the long-term correlates of child sexual abuse. This model suggests that individuals with histories of child sexual abuse attempt to diminish negative thoughts, affective states, and memories of abuse through various coping behaviors including dissociation, substance abuse, casual sexual relationships, and avoidance of intimate relationships. Although these behaviors are hypothesized to initially relieve pain by reducing or suppressing intense emotional responses associated with abuse, their use may result in longterm negative effects, such as feelings of social isolation, dissatisfaction with relationships, sexual dysfunctions, and revictimization. The few investigations that have examined the relationship between child physical abuse and adult interpersonal relationships indicate that physically abused children may be more aggressive (Alessandri, 1991; Kolko, 1992) or more interpersonally sensitive than are nonabused children (Briere & Runtz, 1988; Bryer, Nelson, Miller, & Drol, 1987; Chu & Dill, 1990). Disturbances in intimacy and inability to trust also arise and in turn cause, and contribute to, relationship difficulties. This is partly a secondary effect of frank sexual dysfunction, and partly the result of other trauma-induced factors such as irritability, inability to trust, fear of getting close to someone, etc. This lack of sexual interest or reduced ability to function, however, further erodes the man’s already impaired self-con. dence and sense of masculinity. Many get confused, leading to confusion in the wife, too. Traumatic experiences can lead to various changes in the emotional relationship of the couple, with factors such as over-protectiveness, holding back, impaired trust, guilt, feelings of being let down and of letting the partner down, fears for the future, over-sensitivity and fear of rejection coming into play (see Matsakis, 1998). Young adults who were victims of childhood bullying have also been found to be more introverted than nonvictims (Swain, 1996), which may play a role in their heightened experience of loneliness. This loneliness is also reflected in the results found by Gilmartin (1987) in his study of “love shy” adult men (persons whose shyness with the opposite sex leaves them unable to date or marry) Maltreatment by Parents and Peers: The Relationship Between Child Abuse, Bully Victimization, and Psychological Distress, Renae D. Duncan, Murray State University CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 4, No. 1, February 1999 45-55 CSA is thought to have a negative impact on interpersonal relations because it occurs in the context of an interpersonal relationship, typically one where a degree of safety and trust has developed. Thus, the experience of CSA may impede the development of trusting relationships subsequently, particularly with romantic partners. Furthermore, the experience of CSA may compromise the development of a positive sense of the self, which will inherently influence social relationships (Cole & Putnam, 1992). Abstract: Traumatic events can have a major impact on attachment behavior and interpersonal relationships. In addition to the detrimental effects of post-trauma symptomatology, the traumatic experience can become embedded in the memory structure of the individual causing a progressive avoidance of interpersonal triggers. The traumatic experience may also have detrimental effects on self-awareness, intimacy, sexuality and communication all of which are key elements to the maintenance of healthy interpersonal relationships. Investigations into the effect of PTSD on interpersonal relationships should focus on a longitudinal model of attachment. Pre-traumatic coping mechanisms may be altered by the traumatic experience, and the relationship between pre-traumatic, epi-traumatic and post-traumatic attachments should be addressed. The impact of a major stressor on an individual’s relationships needs to be considered from a longitudinal perspective. There are three windows during which attachment patterns need to be considered when analysing the impact of disasters on relationships: pre-traumatic, epi-traumatic and post-traumatic. Every individual has a mode of dealing with relationships which will pre date their traumatic exposure and is indicative of developmental experiences. Ultimately, changes that traumatic events bring will be superimposed on these pre-traumatic ways of managing relationships. Particularly in those who develop post-traumatic stream disorder (PTSD) the emerging symptoms come to have a highly detrimental effect on their personal relationships. If the individual has developed PTSD con• ict at these times will spark the irritability which is one of the most disruptive symptoms in terms of family relationships. In this regard, embedded in the relationship can be a frequent re-enactment of the fears of a recurrence of the trauma compounded by the individual’s irritability. Paradoxically, the detrimental effects of this pattern of reaction can be further exacerbated by the numbing and attachment disruption which are also recognized as part of the symptomatology of PTSD. The numbing is often experienced by individuals as a loss of a sense of empathy and as hardening. Thus, while at one level the individual will be behaving in an increasingly agitated and anxious way, at other times this will be mirrored by a state of apparent detachment and affectlessness. The effects of this increasing con• ict on the family will become reinforced because of the individual’s progressive loss of social contact and decreasing social circle. Often these external relationships serve to mitigate against the disruptions caused by post-traumatic consequences so that, if they are lost, it further compounds the disruption of the homeostasis within the family. In this way, traumatic memories have the capacity to disrupt attachments and lead to the progressive distancing and avoidance of the interpersonal triggers that are the stimulus for the traumatic re-enactments. If some element of the traumatic event has involved being let down by a colleague, this can similarly evoke the involvement of issues of trust in the traumatic memory structure. Moments of dependence and reliance on a partner can become in. ltrated and similarly corrupted by these experiences. Following disasters it is recognized that there is an increased incidence of domestic violence and the abuse of children (Goenjian, 1993). This is a practical demonstration of the capacity for these events to have signi. cant detrimental effects on the lives of those exposed. The rami. cations of trauma extend through the family and can have multi-generational effects (Forman & Havas, 1990; Yehuda et al., 1998) through the way in which trauma ruptures attachment bonds (Lifton, 1983; Allen & Bloom, 1994). Although clinical experience attests to this view, it is surprising to . nd that few research studies have directly examined the impact of trauma on intimacy and sexuality. Although scant, the research literature supports the notion that trauma markedly affects relations with partners. Studies of war veterans report that they have serious dif. culties maintaining intimate relationships and a high degree of negative emotionality directed at spouses (Escobar et al., 1983; Carroll et al., 1985; Jordan et al., 1992; Solomon et al., 1987; Johnson et al., 1996; Riggs et al., 1998). Similar, . ndings are reported in studies of people who develop PTSD following motor vehicle accidents (Blanchard et al., 1995). POSLEDICE TRAVME – MEDOSEBNI ODNOSI - INTIMNOST Two characteristic features of PTSD speci. cally damage intimacy. These are avoidance and hyperarousal (criteria C and D in DSM-IV). Avoidance includes: · markedly diminished interest or participation in signi. cant activities; · feelings of detachment or estrangement from others; · restricted range of affect; · sense of a foreshortened future. All of these features of avoidance are the results of loss of intrapsychic intimacy and will inhibit recovery through interpersonal intimacy. Hyperarousal damages intimacy because of increased irritability and outbursts of anger. Intimacy There are two broad categories of intimacy. Intrapsychic intimacy (Sheehan, 1994) is the result of an individual achieving adequate self-knowledge and self-acceptance which in turn foster the willingness to share these thoughts and feelings with another. It measures the extent to which an individual knows himself or herself and will undoubtedly in• uence the degree of impact of a traumatic event. Resilience and vulnerability can be seen as aspects of intrapsychic intimacy. Intrapsychic intimacy represents the capacity to develop intimate relationships with others. Interpersonal intimacy is seen as the result of interaction and can occur only between people who share something meaningful with each other. It will predictably POSLEDICE TRAVME – NAVEZANOST Ward, Hudson in McCormack so opisali tri različne stile navezanosti in predstavili hipoteze ki vodijo do pomanjkanja intimnosti v odraslem obdobju. Zaskrbljen stil opisujejo kot negativni pogled samega sebe in pozitiven na druge. Pri omenjeni navezanosti bo pri posamezniku moč opaziti visoko stopnjo osamljenosti ter spolno zaskrbljenost. Pri navezanosti strahu ima posameznik negativen pogled nase in na druge. Pri njem bo prisotna želja po socialnem stiku in intimnosti kot tudi strah pred zavrnitvijo ki ga vodi v izogibanje bližini in odnosom. Zadnji stil, odpuščujoč vsebuje pozitiven pogled nase in negativen na druge. V varno navezanost lahko uvrstimo nenasilni in nespolni napad ki vključuje pozitiven poged nase in druge. Terapevt bo v terapevtski obravnavi spodbudil klienta da sprejme odgovornost ter se osredotočil na žrtev in pri tem skušal povečati samozavest. Early experiences with a traumatizing caregiver are well known to impact negatively the child’s attachment security, stress coping strategies, and sense of self (Crittenden and Ainsworth, 1989; Erickson, Egeland, and Pianta, 1989). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.) With increasing interest in adult attachment, researchers began to develop and examine various models of adult attachment styles (Bartholomew, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Main et al., 1985). Hazan and Shaver (1987) fashioned their model of adult attachment in romantic relationships on Ainsworth’s childhood attachment styles of “secure,” “avoidant,” and “ambivalent.” Main and colleagues developed a model of adult attachment that described four categories of attachment style including “secure-autonomous,” “dismissing,” “preoccupied,” and “unresolveddisorganized” (Main et al., 1985). Bartholomew has proposed a four-category model of attachment that empirically validates Bowlby’s theory of internalworking models of self and other among adults (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). The self model and the other model can each be viewed as either positive or negative. Bartholomew’s attachment measure, the Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ), allows for the measurement of the four attachment categories, as well as the two continuous underlying attachment poles, view of self and view of other. The four attachment categories include the secure pattern, which reflects a positive self and other model. Someone with a secure attachment pattern has an integrated sense of self-worth and is comfortable forming intimate relationships. The preoccupied pattern reflects a negative self and a positive other model. A preoccupied person seeks a sense of safety by gaining the acceptance and approval of others. The dismissing pattern reflects a positive self and a negative other model. Someone with a dismissing attachment style dismisses dependency needs and emphasizes independence as a means of maintaining positive self-regard. The fearful pattern reflects a negative self and a negative other model. Fearful individuals avoid intimacy to avoid the pain of rejection or loss. Attachment theorists (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Bowlby, 1980) postulate that insecure attachments result from interactions that cause individuals to doubt the trustworthiness, responsivity, and accessability of others, and to question the integrity of the self. Similarly, PTSD comprises feelings of distrust of others, and reflects a state of anxious apprehension that impedes an individual’s ability to have satisfying interpersonal relationships (Stewart, 1996). Another reason to suggest a link between attachment style and posttraumatic stress symptomatology is that both are related to problems of affect regulation. Alexander (1992) asserted that, in infancy, individuals develop specific affect regulation strategies as a result of interactions with their primary caregivers. These strategies represent the infant’s attempts to cope with anxieties arising from their initial attachment relationships, and these coping strategies subsequently continue into adulthood. Because PTSD can be conceptualized as a disorder of affect regulation that results from an inability to cope with a stressful event, it is plausible that certain attachment styles may create a vulnerability for the development of PTSD, whereas others may act as a protective factor to guard against the development of PTSD. As such, Alexander (1992) predicted that individuals who have a fearful attachment style will be most at risk for the development of the more profound disorders of affect, including PTSD. Some empirical investigations have been conducted examining the relationship between attachment and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (Alexander et al., 1998; Mikulincer, Florian, &Weller, 1993). Results of these studies suggest that individuals who possess an insecure attachment style endorse more symptoms of posttraumatic stress than individuals with secure attachments. REZULTATI: This study examined the relationship between adult attachment style and posttraumatic stress symptoms among high-risk adults who reported experiencing childhood abuse. Results indicated that 76% of individuals in this sample had an insecure attachment style. This finding is consistent with studies suggesting that an abused population is more likely to have insecure attachments (Alexander et al., 1998). According to attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969), experiences in close relationships can profoundly influence perceptions of the social world. In particular, individual differences in attachment quality have been proposed as an important moderator of the extent to which attachmentrelated information is attended to and processed (e.g., Fraley, Garner, & Shaver, 2000; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). Avoidant individuals, who are uncomfortable with closeness and intimacy, are theorized to limit the processing of potentially distressing information, with the goal of preventing activation of the attachment system (e.g., Edelstein & Shaver, 2004; Fraley, Davis, & Shaver, 1998). Anxious individuals, on the other hand, who are preoccupied with relationship partners and attachment-related concerns, are theorized to be hypervigilant to information that could result in attachmentsystem activation (e.g., Cassidy, 1994, 2000). Findings from the few studies of attachment-related differences in attention and memory are consistent with these theoretical ideas, suggesting that avoidant compared to nonavoidant individuals are less attentive to material with emotional, attachment-related themes (e.g., pictures depicting close relationships; Kirsh & Cassidy, 1997; Main et al., 1985) and, perhaps as a result, have greater difficulty recalling such material (Edelstein, 2005; Fraley et al., 2000; Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995). In addition, although evidence is somewhat mixed, anxious individuals appear to be particularly vigilant to emotional, attachment-related information (e.g., Mikulincer, Gillath,&Shaver, 2002), which may enhance later recall (Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995). Individual differences in adult attachment are generally assessed by a person’s placement on two relatively independent continuous dimensions, avoidance and anxiety (Fraley & Waller, 1998). Individuals with high scores on the avoidance dimension are characterized by chronic attempts to “deactivate” or minimize activation of the attachment system (Cassidy, 2000; Edelstein & Shaver, 2004): In stressful situations, avoidant individuals tend to minimize expressions of distress (Fraley & Shaver, 1997) and are unlikely to turn to or provide support for others (e.g., Edelstein et al., 2004; Fraley & Shaver, 1998; Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992). They dislike physical and emotional intimacy (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998; Fraley et al., 1998) and grieve less following a breakup compared to nonavoidant adults (Fraley & Shaver, 1999). Attachment anxiety, in contrast, appears to reflect “hyperactivation” of the attachment system (Cassidy, 2000): Individuals scoring high on the anxiety dimension report fears of being alone and are preoccupied with intimacy and relationship partners. They are hypervigilant to attachment figures and attachment-related concerns (e.g., Mikulincer, Birnbaum, Woddis, & Nachmias, 2000; Mikulincer et al., 2002) and are easily distressed by even brief separations from attachment figures (Feeney & Noller, 1992; Fraley & Shaver, 1998). In this two-dimensional framework, individuals who score low on both dimensions are considered secure. Although any kind of unwanted sexual experience is likely to be emotional and possibly traumatic, the most severe cases are presumably experienced most negatively and, due to their potentially threatening nature, should be most likely to activate attachment-related concerns and defenses (e.g., Mikulincer, Florian, & Weller, 1993). Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1980, 1988) proposed that the quality of attachment between an infant and caregiver formed a template for the development of relationships in later life. Specifically, he proposed that secure early attachment to caregivers, associated with an attuned responsive parenting style, leads in later life to positive peer relationships, fulfilling romantic relationships, and productive parental relationships. In contrast, insecure or disorganized early attachment, associated with unresponsive, neglectful, or abusive parenting, leads in adulthood to problematic, neglectful, abusive, or violent romantic and parental relationships, and to problematic relationships with peers. Abusive experiences during childhood are thought to disrupt the attachment process. As a result, the interpersonal schemas that those with a history of abuse bring with them to adulthood tend to be negative and unwavering across different relationships. Such schemas (e.g., abuse is a way of connecting with another person) may motivate behavior that increases the likelihood of subsequent victimization (Cloitre, Cohen, & Scarvalone, 2002). Revictimization, including intimate partner violence (IPV), is well documented among survivors of childhood abuse (Rodriguez et al., 1998; Schaaf & McCanne, 1998). In some early work, Briere and Runtz (1987) found that women who were sexually abused were more likely than their counterparts who were nonabused to be victims of physical abuse by a partner. Coid et al. (2001) found two to three times the risk of domestic violence among women who reported CPA or CSA. Whitfield, Anda, Dube and Felitti (2003) found approximately three times the likelihood of current IPV in women who reported CPA and approximately two times the likelihood in women who reported CSA or witnessed their mother being battered. Risk of victimization increased with the number of types of childhood maltreatment. This study was limited, however, because the measure of IPV consisted of a single screening question. Another way of looking at interpersonal difficulties is through the lens of attachment theory, which proposes that early relationships between children and their caregivers shape the development of children’s internal working models of the self and others. Working models are cognitive schemas that reflect a child’s sense of self-worth and his or her expectations about the emotional responsiveness of significant others. For instance, children who experience warmth and consistency in their relations with their caregivers will develop a working model of the self as lovable and a working model of others as loving and reliable. Children who are sexually exploited may develop negative working models of both the self and others. Specifically, these children may form a working model of the self as shameful and a working model of others as untrustworthy, unresponsive to their emotional needs, and abusive. Navezanost (vrste, kako se kaže) Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1980) pointed to the important role of parent-child bonds for the healthful development of the child. He observed that when parent-child relationships were poor, children suffered. Such children often become anxious, angry, or depressed. Subsequent research has demonstrated quite clearly that all manner of problems result from inade-quate parenting (Bretherton, 1985; Kolvin, Miller, Fletting, & Kolvin, 1988; Loeber, 1990; Paterson & Moran, 1988). Ainswoth and her colleagues (Aisworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) distinguished three types of parent-child attachments: secure, anxious/ambivalent, and avoidant.Whenthese early attachment bonds are secure, the child develops the self-confidence and skills necessary to form effective relationships with children outside the family. Bowlby (1969) suggested that parent-child relationships provided the developing child with a template for all future relationships. In secure bonds, the child learns that he or she has the qualities to be loved and that other people can be loving. This gives the child not only self-confidence but also confidence in others. These secure bonds instill a resiliency in the child such that he or she can cope adequately with the ups and downs of life. Insecure parent-child attachments, on the other hand, typically fail to produce these features of resilience. Where parents offer little or no support to their children and are inconsistent in their responses, they are said to form anxious ambivalent bonds with their children. Children who are products of this type of relationship have negative views of themselves and are desperate to be close to others, but at the same time, they are afraid of closeness for fear they will be rejected. When parents are cold and distant and lack emotional expressiveness, children will develop an avoidant style. Avoidant children see others as untrustworthy and will attempt to maintain a safe distance from other people to protect themselves. Both anxious/ambivalent and avoidant children are likely to have low selfesteem, poor relationship skills, and be unable to handle life’s daily problems. These children are vulnerable to stress because they have no confidence in their capacity to deal with problems and because they feel they have no one to rely on whom they can trust. This incapacity to deal with problems leads to the development of an inadequate coping style by which the child, and later the adult, either avoids facing difficulties or simply responds by being emotionally overwrought and withdrawn. Both these inadequate coping styles lead to self-indulgence as a way to deal with problems. In addition, these children will be readily responsive to the attention of others, even though they are wary, if not afraid, of closeness. (THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263) POSLEDICE TRAVME – OSEBNOST, SAMOPODOBA, VIDENJE DRUGIH If, as Finkelhor and Browne (1985) suggested, early sexual trauma can profoundly “alter a child’s cognitive and emotional orientation toward the world and cause trauma by distorting the child’s self concept,world view, or affective capacities” (p. 531), then there is reason to suspect thatCSAmay disrupt survivors’ long-term interpersonal adjustment as well. Abused children do not receive protection when they need it. As noted by Herman (1997), "At the moment of trauma the victim is utterly helpless. Unable to defend herself, she cries for help, but no one comes to her aid. She feels totally abandoned. The memory of this experience pervades all subsequent relationships" (p. 137). For child abuse survivors, problems with protection persist into adulthood. They often have difficulty setting limits in interpersonal relationships, defending themselves in conflict situations, and guarding against repeated victimizations (Briere, 1992). In addition, many have trouble protecting their own children from abuse (Goodwin, McCarthy, &DiVasto, 1981; McCloskey &Bailey, 2000; Oates, Tebbutt, Swanston, Lynch, &O'Toole, 1998; Spieker, Bensley, McMahon, Fung, &Ossiander, 1996). Many trauma researchers have recognized a connection between receiving protection in childhood and feeling protected in later life. For example, according to van der Kolk, van der Hart, and Marmar (1996). Theories of the long-term impact of child abuse typically stress concepts such as affect regulation, identity, cognitive schemas, and interpersonal trust (e. g. , Alexander, 1992; Briere, 1996; Cole &Putnam, 1992; Horowitz, 1997; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Linehan, 1993; van der Kolk &Fisler, 1994). I know of no theories of child abuse that emphasize the development of self-protection. This article presents a case for interpersonal protection as an organizing construct in abuse research and treatment. Sexual assault upsets perceptions of safety and may significantly affect a victim’s future ability to feel safe. Marital sexual assault poses a greater threat to a victim’s sense of security and safety in the world than do other forms of sexual assault because it violates one’s safety expectations (Foa & Riggs,1994) and may reduce one’s confidence to judge others and to form safe relationships (Finkelhor&Yllö, 1982; Goodman, Koss,& Russo, 1993; Kilpatrick, Best, Saunders,&Veronen, 1988; Whatley, 1993). Lachmann and Beebe (1997) point out that an event becomes traumatic when it ruptures the individual’s selfobject tie, without opportunity for repair, thereby dramatically altering her self-state. With trauma, personal responsibility is distorted due to the lack of control one feels when the trauma is happening. This confronts the patient with the disparity between what he wanted to happen and the tragic events that actually occurred. In the comprehensive review carried out by Kendall-Tackett et al. (1993), the authors conclude that the impact of sexual abuse upon children does not necessarily yield a distinct identifiable syndrome. Instead, the experience manifests itself in a variety of symptomatic and pathological behaviours. These problems may not remit but may result in lifelong impairments that include fear, depression, substance abuse, dissociative disorders and sexual dysfunction (Browne and Finkelhor, 1986). Furthermore, it has been suggested that childhood abuse often disrupts children’s development by stimulating primitive coping strategies and by creating cognitive distortions of self, others and the future (Briere, 1992). The self-loathing that many traumatized women experience may generalize into thinking of both the self and the body as bad and ugly (Hyman, 1999; Miller, 1994). The body, the site of the original abuse, was violated, and this abuse included both emotional and physical boundary violations. These boundary violations may lead to self-harm, which temporarily helps define the body boundaries. Self-image and self-esteem also appear to be key factors in some. The overall negative self-image thus caused can lead to predictions of failure in all matters, including sex, and to a sense that one is not attractive even when there is no physical dis. gurement. PROCESI, KI RAZLAGAJO POSLEDICE –SPOMIN IN OBLIKOVANJE SHEM O SEBI, SVETU vpliv travme na razvoj shem, motnje spomina, pomankljiva integracija izkušenj, manjkajoč občutek konstantnosti When considering the psychological impact of trauma, much has been written about possible effects on memory and self representation. Clinical case studies and theoretical literature describe disturbances such as amnesia for early autobiographical memories, instability of self image, disturbed cognitions relating to self; lack of a sense of a self; identity confusion; and poor or negative internal self-representations (Briere 1989, 1992; Herman and van der Kolk, 1987; McCann and Pearlman, 1990; Parkin, 1987; Putnam, 1990; Reviere, 1996; Schetky, 1990; Schultz, 1990; van der Kolk, 1987; Zelikovsky and Lynn, 1994). In general, the presence of trauma in childhood is presumed to create a disruption in the continuity and stability of experience theoretically necessary for normative development of schematic representations of self and the world, at least in part, through autobiographical memories (McCann and Pearlman, 1990; Stern, 1985). More specifically, the sustained attention that traumatized children devote to physical and/or emotional survival is thought to interfere (through any of various mechanisms in the memory processes) with encoding or retention of early autobiographical memories. Such a narrowing of attention has been theorized to result in distortions in memory and thus, impairment in development of a coherent, well developed self schema. It has been posited that the memory impairment often observed in trauma may be linked to an underlying motivation to preserve the integrity of nascent schemas and psychological wellbeing in a child. Specifically, the inclusion of trauma in an autobiographical narrative may threaten basic schematic structures and assumptions (e.g. of safety); thus, one adaptation may be to leave the traumatic material unassimilated, thereby disrupting memory and basic schema formation (Bartlett, 1932; Fine, 1990; Horowitz, 1991; Janoff-Bulman, 1989; Piaget, 1967; Schachtel, 1959; Singer and Salovey, 1991). Further, if trauma is encountered during child hood, at a time when schemas and beliefs about the self and world are forming, traumatic experience may interfere with development of supraordinate, general schemas that create a sense of continuity in self, memory, and meaning (Fine, 1990; Horowitz, 1991). For a child who has not yet achieved the internal stability afforded by adequate schematic development, such disruption could have significant affects. The attempt to resolve this disruption may result in significant distortions in self-experience as the child attempts to organize experience and self in a way that allows some degree of perceived control. Since schemas are likely not available for assimilation of traumatic information, and accommodation of developing schemas to include traumatic material precludes a basic sense of mastery of self and eorld, dissociation of traumatic events and resulting memories may provide for the preservation safety needed by traumatized child (Horowitz, 1991; van der Kolk et al., 1989). As such, the consolidation of memories related to self may be disrupted. This may lead to a self schema that remains unintegrated with subsequent experience, or the trauma can become an organizing frame frame for potentially impaired self development (Barclay, 1986; Barclay and DeCooke, 1988; Barsalou, 1988; Brewer, 1986; Bruhn, 1990; Schachter et al., 1989). If large segments of autobiographical memory are separated from personal identity, in part due to schematic disruptions, the usual frame of reference for continual calibration and definition of self is lost. Thud trough the a process of mutual influence, the disruption of consistency in life experience may create a propensity for disruption in development of schemas and a disruption in a development of a coherent autobiographical memory system (Barclay, 1986, 1988; Bartlett, 1932; Bruhn, 1990; Neisser, 1988). POSLEDICE TRAVME – SOOČANJE S KASNEJŠIM STRESOM It has been noted that a history of exposure to extreme psychological stress appears to make a person more vulnerable to experiencing psychological distress when stressors occur later in life (Bremner, Southwick, & Charney, 1995). This was demonstrated in a study of female and male Vietnam veterans that found that stressful life events occurring after the war were associated with greater PTSD (King, King, Fairbank, Keane, & Adams, 1998). Among Holocaust survivors, both cumulative and recent stressful life events were significantly related to PTSD (Yehuda et al., 1995). Similarly, among Israeli soldiers who suffered combat stress reactions during the 1982 Lebanon War, additional negative life events were found to be associated with greater PTSD and recovery from PTSD (Solomon, Mikulincer, & Flum, 1989). Furthermore, among survivors of a firestorm, stressful life events occurring prior to, during, and after the firestorm were each found to have independent relationships in predicting later PTSD symptoms (Koopman et al., 1994). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290) POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI Attempts to suppress negative affect associated with the trauma may lead to generalized constricted affect, an inability to have tender or loving feelings, and problems with making and maintaining relationships. Drug and alcohol abuse may also occur as a way of avoiding trauma-related affect. With hyperarousal, a variety of difficulties may occur including an exaggerated startle response, hypervigilance, poor concentration, irritability or outbursts of anger, and difficulty falling or staying asleep. Being alone invites the overwhelming pain and affect associated with the trauma and self-badness to be experienced. Abused women, many of whom have been victims of male perpetrators as children, report fear of being alone (Lobel, 1992). The fear of being alone is related to the isolation in which the abuse took place—no one saw or heard and certainly no one stopped the assault. Lobel found that 63% of women who had been sexually abused as children reported feelings of self-hatred, worthlessness, and guilt when they were alone. That is, numbing may involve suppression of the expression of emotions (at least some components of emotional responding), which would lead to an increase in emotional experiencing (in other components). Since theories propose numbing to occur in response to trauma cues (e.g., Foa, Zinbarg, & Rothbaum, 1992; Litz, 1992), emotional suppression and incongruities in emotional experiencing would be most likely to occur subsequent to exposure to trauma cues. This pattern may therefore account for the difficulties in processing of traumatic experiences in individuals with PTSD as well as the maintenance of PTSD symptomatology. The theories proposed to account for the change in self-report of emotional experiencing and increased physiological arousal that accompany emotional inexpressivity and suppression emphasize the physical and psychological effort required to inhibit emotions (e.g, Cacioppo et al., 1992; Notarius & Levenson, 1979). Trauma leaves in its wake a loss of trust, of faith, of safety, of connection. It is often a frightening confrontation with helplessness, with the fear and rage that helplessness induces, and with a numbness that mutes these painful states of mind. It is perhaps for these reasons that successful treatment of people who have been traumatized often uses multiple modalities, occurs in stages, and extends over a lengthy period of time (1). (THE TRAUMA OF PROFOUND CHILDHOOD LOSS: A PERSONAL AND PROFESSIONAL PERSPECTIVE Francine Cournos, M.D., Psychiatric Quarterly, Vol. 73, No. 2, Summer 2002) Symptoms of emotional numbing are a core feature of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in the nomenclature of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed. [DSM–IV]; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In DSM–IV, emotional numbing is assessed in symptom cluster C. Criteria for cluster C include symptoms of avoidance and emotional numbing, whereas cluster B assesses symptoms of re-experiencing and cluster D symptoms of hyperarousal. Emotional numbing involves diminished interest in activities, feelings of detachment from others, and the restriction of affect (Foa, Davidson,&Frances, 1999). Such features of emotional numbing have salient clinical impact, as they are likely to produce personal distress and impairment in interpersonal functioning (Carrion & Steiner, 2000). Moreover, the symptoms of emotional numbing are closely associated with features of major depression and dysthymia. A Prospective Test of the Association Between Hyperarousal and Emotional Numbing in Youth With a History of Traumatic Stress - Carl F. Weems, Kasey M. Saltzman, Allan L. Reiss, and Victor G. Carrion - Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003, Vol. 32, No. 1, 166–171 Research employing adult samples has shown that symptoms of emotional numbing are strongly associated with symptoms of hyperarousal (e.g., Flack, Litz, Hsieh, Kaloupek,&Keane, 2000; Litz et al., 1997), and theorists (Litz, 1992; Litz et al., 1997) have hypothesized that emotional numbing may result from emotional exhaustion produced by prolonged periods of arousal. More specifically, symptoms of emotional numbing are thought to be caused by the depletion of cognitive and emotional resources due to prolonged hyperarousal. Support for this theory comes from a variety of sources, including animal models of inescapable shock and humans exposed to traumatic events (see Flack et al., 2000; Litz, 1992; Litz et al., 1997; Van der Kolk, Boyd, Krystal, & Greenberg, 1984). A Prospective Test of the Association Between Hyperarousal and Emotional Numbing in Youth With a History of Traumatic Stress - Carl F. Weems, Kasey M. Saltzman, Allan L. Reiss, and Victor G. Carrion - Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003, Vol. 32, No. 1, 166–171 Results in this sample provided support for the theory that emotional numbing may develop as a result of chronic hyperarousal in youth. The prospective design used in this study added to an understanding of the association between hyperarousal and emotional numbing by showing that emotional numbing was not a robust prospective predictor of hyperarousal. Such findings suggest the direction of the association. That is, hyperarousal is associated with later emotional numbing, but emotional numbing does not robustly predict later hyperarousal. Taken together, these results provide preliminary support for the theory that emotional numbing may result from emotional exhaustion or the depletion of cognitive and emotional resources due to prolonged hyperarousal in youth. A Prospective Test of the Association Between Hyperarousal and Emotional Numbing in Youth With a History of Traumatic Stress - Carl F. Weems, Kasey M. Saltzman, Allan L. Reiss, and Victor G. Carrion - Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003, Vol. 32, No. 1, 166–171 POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – KRIVDA IN SRAM Self-blame following sexual assault has been studied extensively, particularly in relation to Janoff-Bulman’s theory (1979). In general, this research has found that many victims of sexual assault use both characterological selfblame and behavioral self-blame; however, both types of attributions are associated with higher distress both immediately postrape and over time (Arata, 1994; Frazier, 1990; Frazier&Schauben, 1994; Hill&Zautra, 1989; Katz & Burt, 1988; Mandoki & Burkhart, 1989). Furthermore, engaging in self-blame is associated with greater use of maladaptive coping strategies (Arata & Burkhart, 1998; Arata, 1994) and higher rates of posttraumatic stress disorder (Arata & Burkhart, 1996). The research on self-blame following adult sexual assault has focused primarily on the distinction between behavioral and characterological selfblame and, more recently, whether there are differences in feelings of blame versus responsibility or avoidability (Abbey, 1987). Women with a history of child sexual abuse were more likely to engage in self-blaming attributions regarding the rape. That is, they were more likely to blame themselves for the rape, including seeing themselves as having deserved the rape, being a victim type, being a bad person, or not being able to take care of one’s self. At the same time, women with a history of child sexual abuse also reported more societal blame. They tended to feel that theworld is unsafe forwomen, that there are many emotionally disturbed individuals around, and that no one is ever available to help when it is needed. The fact that thesewomen also report greater use of cognitive strategies is more difficult to interpret. Presumably, cognitive strategies are more adaptive, however, this finding is consistent with Burt and Katz’s (1987) description of rape victims alternating between emotion-focused (such as nervous) coping and problem-focused (such as cognitive) coping, with use of all coping strategies decreasing as symptoms decrease. Additionally, the results suggest that women may continue to use the same maladaptive coping strategies for dealing with adult assault as were employed for their childhood abuse. Coffey et al. (1996b) and Leitenberg et al. (1992) found that disengagement or avoidant coping strategies were the types of coping most frequently employed bywomen abused as children. One issue to consider is whether these coping strategies might also serve as vulnerability factors to revictimization. Shame is a negative and disturbing emotional experience involving feelings of selfcondemnation and the desire to hide the damaged self from others (Lewis, 1992; Tangney, 1995). It is a state in which the whole self feels defective, often as a result of a perceived failure to meet social and self-imposed standards. Clinical, theoretical, and empirical work have emphasized shame as a common consequence of child sexual abuse (Feiring, Taska, & Lewis, 1996, 2002; Finkelhor & Browne, 1987; Nathanson, 1989). Previous results from this study showed that shame for the abuse is common at the time of discovery and generally decreases during 1 year’s time (Feiring et al., 2002). It is a self-conscious emotion that requires the cognitive ability to have a sense of self and evaluate one’s behavior against a standard (Lewis, 2000). Shame, as measured by body posture and facial expression, can be observed in children as young as 3 years of age (Lewis, Alessandri, & Sullivan, 1992). It is not until later, around the age of 8 years, that children are able to generate appropriate examples of shame experiences (Ferguson & Stegge,1995). Guilt, like shame, is a self-conscious emotion. Both emotions focus on the self and involve negative feelings. However, guilt focuses on specific aspects of the self that are perceived as causing failure, whereas shame focuses on the whole self. Guilt concerns one’s actions. Shame concerns one’s entire being. Guilt motivates taking action to repair the perceived failure (e.g., being on time rather than late for dinner dates with a close friend), whereas shame motivates hiding the self from exposure and inaction (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). A consistent finding in shame research is that this self-conscious emotion often motivates an avoidance response such that the individual wants to hide the exposed self (Barrett, ZahnWaxler, & Cole, 1993; Tangney, 1995). Strong negative emotions associated with traumatic events are aversive. They promote cognitive and behavioral avoidance, which, in turn, prolong PTSD symptoms (Berliner & Wheeler, 1987; Foa &Riggs, 1994). Earlier abuserelated shame was expected to show longterm consequences for the experience of PTSD symptoms. Individuals with high abuse-related shame during the 1st year following abuse discovery were expected to be most at risk for experiencing clinically significant levels of PTSD symptoms 6 years following abuse discovery. Findings for individual patterns of persistence in shame across time showed that high levels of shame are likely to abate. This suggests that although abuse-related shame was common at abuse discovery, there was a good chance that such high levels of shame would not become characteristic of the individual across several years. For individuals low in shame at abuse discovery and for those low in shame 1 year later, the prognosis was very good. Feedback from significant others about one’s failure to be good and lovable should be a primary contributor to shame and its persistence across time. Higher levels of shame in children are associated with parental hostility, rejection, negative affective displays and comments during learning, and minimal recognition for good behavior (Alessandri & Lewis, 1996; Ferguson & Stegge, 1995; Stuewig & McCloskey, 2005). It has been suggested that the more the child hears and internalizes deficiency messages that he or she is not and never will be good enough or lovable, the greater the likelihood of shame (Potter-Efron, 1989). It is not just fear that motivates avoidance of thinking about traumatic events. Our findings suggest that to cognitively and emotionally process traumatic experiences, therapists must help clients to confront both shame and fear. Although clients may not spontaneously volunteer feelings of shame, shame can readily be observed in a client’s use of language, reluctance to disclose, and nonverbal behavior (e.g., avoidance of eye contact, covering the face, head down, body collapsed, body hidden with a pillow or coat). (The Persistence of Shame Following Sexual Abuse: A Longitudinal Look at Risk and Recovery) - vsi odstavki ABSTRACT:Guilt about surviving a traumatic event is thought to be an associated feature of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Shame is an emotion closely related to guilt but is a distinct affective state. Little is known regarding the role of shame in PTSD and there are no studies of PTSD where shame and guilt are examined simultaneously. We used a measure of shame- and guilt-proneness in 107 community residing former prisoner of war veterans all of whom had been exposed to trauma. The measure of shame-proneness was positively correlated with PTSD symptom severity whereas guilt-proneness was not. This study provides the first empirical data regarding a possible role for shame in PTSD and may have important therapeutic and theoretical implications. Many combat veterans experience profound feelings of guilt following the survival of a trauma and guilt can be related to painful wartime memories (Glover, 1984; Kubany, 1994). Although guilt and shame are terms that are often used interchangeably, current theoretical and empirical literature underscores that these are distinct affective experiences (Lewis, 1971; Lindsay-Hartz, 1984; Tangney, 1990, 1991). Lewis (1971) defined guilt as the self’s negative evaluation of particular behaviors while shame involves the self’s negative evaluation of the entire self. In contrast, shame is conceived of as a more devastating and painful emotion in which the entire self, not just the behavior, is negatively evaluated (Tangney, 1991). Shame theoretically involves painful self-scrutiny, and feelings of worthlessness and powerlessness (Lindsay-Hartz, 1984; Tangney, 1990). Also hypothesized is an associated sense of sudden and unexpected exposure, which renders the individual feeling diminished or defective (Lewis, 1971). Shame conceptually, therefore, may lead to a global and debilitating painful affective reaction with a desire to hide or escape from others (Gramzow & Tangney, 1992). Stone (1992) hypothesized that traumatized individuals with PTSD suffer from symptoms of both guilt and shame. In a largely conceptual article, he points out that in combat an example of guilt is the troubling feeling that one survived when others did not, whereas shame is the feeling of doubting the right to exist. Theoretically, therefore, guilt may be related to actions performed and shame to perception of oneself (Janoff-Bulman, 1979).Wong and Cook (1992) conducted the only empirical study on shame in those with PTSD. Veterans with PTSD scored higher on measures of shame than veterans with substance abuse or depression. Neither trauma exposure nor severity of PTSD symptoms was ascertained and, therefore, limited conclusions can be drawn from this study. REZULTATI: Contrary to our initial hypothesis, only shameproneness and not guilt-proneness, as measured by the TOSCA, correlated positively with severity of PTSD symptoms. Those withPTSDhad higher shame-proneness scores than those without. Self-blame as a consequence of sexual abuse has received a great deal of attention in the literature, although much of this research has focused on self-blame following adult sexual assault. In studies of women with histories of child sexual abuse, self-blame and self-denigratory beliefs have been found as frequent effects (Courtois, 1988; Herman, 1992; Jehu, 1988). Females who blame themselves for their child victimization may have feeling of guilt, shame, and low self-worth with subsequent increases in sexual behavior because the victim perceives herself as only being worthy of relationships if sex is offered. Cutting, unlike childhood abuse, is within the control of the trauma survivor. Of course, the relief it brings is short-lived and often leads to shame and guilt, and the cycle of pain, relief, and shame starts again. The negative reactions caused by a traumatic even often lead to shame, self-loathing and self-blame, even when the individual had no responsibility for the event. Feelings of guilt also seem to be a relevant factor. Guilt about the accident itself, however irrational, is common. There is also guilt, commonly felt by these trauma victims, about letting down one’s family or partner, being a burden on the partner or family, and being ‘worthless’. Children who are sexually abused may be at risk for feelings of shame and self-blame, especially when the abuse was prolonged or when the perpetrator or significant others blamed the child for the abuse. POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – SRAM IN GNUS V raziskavi so Dumbn in Marshall ter Langton predvidevali, da izkušnja sramu ki je posledica spolnega napada v otroškem obdobju v otroku poveča osebno stisko, občutje krivde, poveča kognitivne motnje s tem pa poraste tveganje ponovne obnovitve. Krivda in sram sta opisana kot samega sebe zavedajoče občutje. Skrivanje pred drugimi je dejanje, ki je bilo v empiričnih raziskavah opisano s strani Barlowa in drugih zopet drugi pa so sram opisali kot pozunanjenost graje. ž Priznavanje oziroma izpoved sta Mascolo in Fischer opisala kot krivdo in pripravljenost obnoviti prvotno stanje ki vključuje opravičilo. Pri krivdi je v ospredju dejanje samo medtem ko je pri sramu na prvem mestu oseba. Zato je razumljivo, da ob krivdi pride do izraza empatija do misli in dejanj bolj kot do osebe. Razkritje samega sebe je mejnik v katerem razlikujemo med krivdo in sramom. Izkušnja sramu vključuje obsojanje samega sebe kot nesposobnega ali slabega ali da razmišlja o sebi na način kako ga drugi obsojajo. Zunanji sram odseva skrb kako je posameznik viden s strani drugega. Od slednjega je tudi odvisno kako pomembna je druga oseba v našem življenju. SHAME AND GUILT Shame and guilt are often cited as two different but related moral emotions that regulate social behavior. Although the terms are often used interchangeably, there are important conceptual differences (Tangney, 1990, 1991; Tangney, Wagner, & Gramzow, 1992). Both shame and guilt are “negative” or uncomfortable emotions and as such are usually correlated. Both also deal with self-evaluative judgments, in that we judge ourselves, and our actions, according to internal standards. H. B. Lewis (1971) theorized, however, that the key difference between shame and guilt concerns the distinction between “the self” and “behavior.” Shame focuses less on specific behaviors and more on the evaluation of the entire self against internalized standards. Guilt, on the other hand, reflects feelings about actions that are inconsistent with internalized standards. The two emotions have been shown to lead to different “action tendencies” (Lindsay-Hartz, 1984; Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow, 1996). When guilt is experienced, people are motivated to make reparations for the behavior. When shame is felt, people feel awful about themselves; they want to hide or disappear. Although guilt is an uncomfortable emotion, shame can be more debilitating. Phenomenological reports of shame describe people feeling powerless and insignificant (Wicker, Payne, & Morgan, 1983). The self-loathing that many traumatized women experience may generalize into thinking of both the self and the body as bad and ugly (Hyman, 1999; Miller, 1994). The body, the site of the original abuse, was violated, and this abuse included both emotional and physical boundary violations. These boundary violations may lead to self-harm, which temporarily helps define the body boundaries. Marital violence, for instance, is related to more depression and anxiety in school-age children (Hughes & Luke, 1998; McCloskey et al., 1995). Harsh punitive parenting and low parental warmth predict adolescent delinquency (Loeber & Dishion, 1983; McCord, 1997; Sampson & Laub, 1993; Simons, Wu, Johnson, & Conger, 1995), as does sexual abuse, especially among girls (Herrera &McCloskey, 2003; Paolucci et al., 2001; Siegel &Williams, 2003). Retrospective reports of parental emotional abusiveness, but not physical abusiveness, were related to shameproneness, whereas neither was related to guiltproneness (Hoglund & Nicholas, 1995). In the past 20 years, the majority of research examining shame and guilt using the self- versus behavior distinction has focused on psychological adjustment. Researchers have found that after controlling for guilt, shame tends to be highly related to a variety of psychopathology, including PTSD, obsessive- compulsiveness, psychoticism, anxiety, and depression (Andrews et al., 2000; Ferguson, Stegge, Eyre, Vollmer, & Ashbaker, 2000; Harder, Cutler, & Rockart, 1992; Quiles & Bybee, 1997; Tangney et al., 1992). Shame-proneness showed no association with criminal behavior but was linked to ensuing depression. Guilt-proneness, on the other hand, showed little relation to depression but seemed to inhibit engagement in criminal activities. Shame-proneness in early adolescence was associated with symptoms of depression in late adolescence even when controlling for childhood symptoms of depression. Although researchers have often found a concurrent relationship between shame-proneness and depression (Tangney et al., 1992), very few studies have looked at this prospectively. The Relation of Child Maltreatment to Shame and Guilt Among Adolescents: Psychological Routes to Depression and Delinquency Jeffrey Stuewig, George Mason University, Laura A. McCloskey, University of Pennsylvania CHILDMALTREATMENT,Vol. 10, No. 4,November 2005 324-336 A common assumption is that shame arises from thoughts that one has done something bad or shameful, and the relationship between abuse-specific internal attributions and shame is indeed significant, both at the time of initial disclosure and 1 year later (C. Feiring, personal communication, July 2001). However, it is also possible for shame to arise from the belief that one simply is bad or unworthy, independent of any actions one has or has not taken. For example, abuserelated shame could occur even in the absence of selfblame, if children believe that the mere fact of having been abused makes them bad, dirty, or shameful. Addressing Attributions in Treating Abused Children - Judith A. Cohen, Anthony P. Mannarino - CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 7, No. 1, February 2002 81-84 The articles in this special section on child maltreatment and shame are efforts to elucidate the role of shame as a contributor to child abuse consequences. As many of the authors mention, most of the research attention on emotions in child maltreatment— especially with reference to posttraumatic stress—has focused on fear as the negative emotion that produces psychological distress in the aftermath of trauma. More recently, researchers have begun to explore other negative emotions that might arise during or after abuse and that might factor into understanding outcomes. Shame has emerged as a leading candidate. Although shame is the specific target of the investigations, the primary contribution of these articles may be in confirming that strong negative emotional responses to abuse experiences beyond fear are important in explaining outcomes. For example, why does child sexual abuse produce such high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) when most situations do not involve fear-inducing events? While part of the explanation may lie in the perception of life threat as opposed to objective danger (a known predictor for PTSD), perhaps there is something about the nature of sexual trauma that is more likely to produce other intense negative emotions. Because shame and anger are associated with PTSD in adult samples experiencing a variety of traumas (Andrews, Brewin, Rose, & Kirk, 2000), more attention to these emotions in child research is warranted. In addition, of course, clinicians have long observed that strong negative emotional responses that are not fear related seem to be just as troubling for children who are traumatized. POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – REGULACIJA EMOCIJ IN AFEKTOV All key participants talked about their use of alcohol and drugs to numb their emotional pain from difficult and often painful life experiences and practices. All of the key participants shared that they had discovered early on that alcohol and drugs helped them to numb painful feelings from a past they had desperately tried to escape. The Mediating Effect of Emotion Regulation Coinciding with the manifestation of other temperamental characteristics, emotion regulation comprises a set of competencies to modulate affective states (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998). Examples of emotion regulatory strategies include self-soothing, reframing upsetting events and provocative stimuli (Schwartz & Proctor, 2000), and inhibiting or initiating emotionally driven behavior (Eisenberg et al., 2001). These abilities are formed in the family context and transferred to the peer realm (Fabes, Eisenberg, & Miller, 1990). Parents shape children’s acquisition of regulation skills through parent– child interactions (Parke et al., 1992) or by coaching and modeling (Carson & Parke, 1996; Davies & Cummings, 1994). As noted by Eisenberg et al. (1999), “parental coaching helps children to develop the ability to inhibit negative affect, to self-sooth, and to focus attention (including attention in social contexts)” (p. 514). “Parents who exhibit hurtful and hostile negative emotions frequently may model dysregulated behavior for children to imitate” (Eisenberg et al., 2001, p. 488). A number of empirical studies support the link between the emotion regulatory abilities of parents and their children. The available evidence suggests that there is a clear link between parenting styles and children’s capacities for emotion regulation. Variously described as harsh, overreactive, emotionally negative, coercive, and controlling and authoritarian (Arnold, O’Leary, Wolff, & Acker, 1993; Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997), the specific acts comprising a cluster of harsh parenting behaviors include yelling, frequent negative commands, name calling, overt expressions of anger, and physical threats and aggression. These harsh parenting descriptions can be summarized into categories of coercive acts and negative emotion expressions. In other words, sometimes parents hit their children when they are angry or emotionally out of control (Patterson, 1982). Studies by emotion researchers also suggest the mediating effect of children’s emotion regulation in channeling the effect of emotion-related negative parenting practices on children’s social adjustment. A large number of studies suggest that coercion and harshness from mothers’ parenting behaviors have a stronger effect on children than do fathers’ behaviors (e.g., Denham et al., 2000). The social learning theory (e.g., Bandura & Walters, 1959) postulates that the role modeling effect is facilitated by gender identification. Thus, parenting behaviors should have stronger effects on same-sex than opposite-sex children. A socialization theory on gender role differentiations also predicts that parents in general feel greater responsibility for the socialization of same-sex children (Huston, 1983) and thus exert closer control over them (Power & Shanks, 1989). However, because most children spend more time with their mothers than fathers (Russell & Russell, 1987) and because girls are less rigid in gender stereotyping than boys (Ruble & Martin, 1998), the potential gender identification effect is expected to be more evident with fathers and sons than with mothers and daughters (Lytton & Romney, 1991). In relation to harsh parenting and child aggression, a biological approach would also predict different arousals and responses from same-sex than opposite-sex parent–child pairings (Fabes, 1994). Other mechanisms may also affect parent–child relations. One that is also pertinent to the emotional channeling of harsh parenting is that of attachment. Existing research suggests that attachment security does not vary as a function of a child’s gender (Ainsworth, 1973), especially in early childhood involving child–mother attachment (Rosen & Burke, 1999). Gender differences in attachment to fathers and in related child–father relationships also do not seem to appear until late childhood to adolescence (Lieberman, Doyle, & Markiewicz, 1999). This attachment research is also consistent with Davies and Cummings’s (1994) child emotional security hypothesis. The emotional security theory does not postulate gender differences in young children’s emotional responses to inter-adult conflict (Davies & Cummings, 1994). Cognitive differences in boys’ and girls’ coping with parental conflict have been observed only in older children and adolescents (Davies & Cummings, 1994). One of the most enduring problems associated with childhood abuse is difficulty in affect regulation. Under ideal circumstances, the emergence of emotion regulation skills is guided in development by caretakers through activities, such as labeling and interpreting emotional experiences, soothing activities, and role modeling of effective mood regulation (e.g., Malatesta & Haviland, 1982). Unfortunately, these important socializing experiences are disturbed in caretaking environments characterized by sustained physical and/or sexual abuse. Substantial research has shown that maltreated children, compared with nonmaltreated children, have more difficulty managing their emotions adaptively throughout childhood (e.g., Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Shipman & Zeman, 2001). Similarly, as adults, victims of childhood abuse show difficulties in emotion regulation, especially in the context of interpersonal relationships. Several studies have demonstrated that, compared with women who have suffered first-time traumas as adults (e.g., rape, physical assault), childhood abuse victims have been found to have more difficulty managing anger, hostility, anxiety, and depression (Browne & Finkelhor, 1986) and report significantly more problems in interpersonal functioning in work, home, and social domains (Zlotnick, Zakriski, Shea, & Costello, 1996). Survivors often experience uncontrolled emotion as the most salient indicator that something is deeply wrong. Van der Kolk et al. (1996) confirmed that affect dysregulation is a central symptom for survivors. Feelings are enemies that arrive unannounced, create havoc in the survivor’s internal and external world, and then leave behind a dark hole of nothingness that can feel even worse than the storm that preceded it. The intense stress of the early trauma can lead to overstimulation of the central nervous system. In turn, this stimulation can cause permanent neural changes that negatively affect learning and stimulus discrimination (van der Kolk, 1994). Alterations in biological functioning (easy to startle, chronic hyperarousal, hypervigilance, and diffuse physical complaints) are the hallmarks of many trauma survivors (van der Kolk, 1994). Researchers have shown that individuals with a history of trauma react to stimuli with conditioned autonomic responses, such as increased heart rate and increased blood pressure (Bremner, 1999). These responses mimic the responses at the time of trauma. POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – OBRAMBNI MEHANIZMI The findings described thus far are consistent with Bowlby’s (1980, 1987) notion of defensive exclusion. Bowlby proposed that some individuals may selectively and defensively regulate the processing of material that could result in attachment-system activation. Such defensive behavior may serve to prevent the negative affect associated with reminders of attachment-related loss. Avoidant individuals are thought to rely on these kinds of defensive strategies to regulate attention to attachment-related information: If potentially upsetting information is not fully processed, the attachment system is less likely to be activated, thus preventing further rejection and distress and, of relevance to the present study, impairing memory. POSLEDICE TRAVME – VEDENJE (ACTING OUT), DELINKVENTNOST, NASILJE Studies have found a possible link between an individual experiencing trauma and later developing delinquent behaviors. Burton, Foy, Bwanausi, Johnson, and Moore (1994) reported a significant level of PTSD in male juvenile delinquents adjudicated for felony crimes. Similarly, Steiner et al. (1997) reported a significantly high prevalence rate for PTSD within an incarcerated group of male juvenile delinquents when compared with males from a local high school who were not incarcerated. PTSD in male criminal youth was found to be associated with exposure to a malevolent environment such us domestic violence, living in unsafe neighborhoods, substance abuse, and criminal behavior among family members (Erwin et al., 2000). There is strong evidence that child maltreatment (physical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect) can increase risk for a range of negative adolescent behaviors, including delinquency, substance use, and violence (Fergusson, Horwood, & Lynskey, 1996; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1997; Hawkins et al., 1998; R. C. Herrenkohl, Egolf, & Herrenkohl, 1997; Smith & Thornberry, 1995;Widom, 1989a, 2000;Wolfe, 1999). Violence as an outcome of physical child abuse is, perhaps, most well studied, although results are inconsistent (Hawkins et al., 1998; Widom, 1989a, 1989b). In their comprehensive review of the research literature, Hawkins et al. (1998) found that effect sizes vary notably across studies linking maltreatment to official and self-reported violence in youths. Widom (1989a, 1989b) suggested that research design and measurement problems are common in studies on child maltreatment and that these problems explain, in part, why results differ. In several longitudinal studies, the relation between physical child abuse and later violence does indeed appear to hold (Widom, 1989a). For example, in Widom’s (1989a, 1998a, 2000) longitudinal cohorts design study, results show a strong link between physical child abuse and youth (and adult) violent crime arrests. In that well-designed studies have shown a developmental link between physical child abuse and youth violence, a next step is to determine why. Several explanations for why abuse places children at risk for violence have been offered. One hypothesis is that youths simply reenact some version of the behavior to which they fell victim at the hands of an abusive parent, having learned the uses of violence (Widom, 2000). Dodge and colleagues (Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990; Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997; Dodge, Price, Bachorowski,&Newman, 1990) hypothesized instead that children who areabused suffer deficits in social information processing, which make them prone to violence. Their research suggests that abused children, through repeated exposure to severe punishment, develop a tendency to overattribute hostile intentions to others and to misread social cues in instances in which threats are unclear or even unintended. In this process, a youth assumes the role of an aggressor against a perceived hostile target. Results of the prospective abuse model showed that the parental attachment and school commitment variables played lesser a role in the mediation of abuse on later violence than did violent attitudes and peer involvement. In this model, abuse predicted violent attitudes, which, in turn, predicted involvement with antisocial peers, which, in turn, predicted violent behavior in youths. The link between earlier abuse and violent attitudes in youths is indeed suggestive of a cognitive/social learning perspective on the transmission of violence, in which youths incorporate into their own cognitive and social interactional styles ways of relating that resemble those used by their parents toward them. POSLEDICE TRAVME – NEVROPSIHOLOŠKE POSLEDICE Early experiences with a traumatizing caregiver are well known to impact negatively the child’s attachment security, stress coping strategies, and sense of self (Crittenden and Ainsworth, 1989; Erickson, Egeland, and Pianta, 1989). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.) Current studies in developmental traumatology conclude that “the overwhelming stress of maltreatment in childhood is associated with adverse influences on brain development” (de Bellis et al., 1999, p. 1281). And so it is now thought that specifically a dysfunctional and traumatized early relationship is the stressor that leads to PTSD, that severe trauma of interpersonal origin may override any genetic, constitutional, social, or psychological resilience factor, and that the ensuing adverse effects on brain development and alterations of the biological stress systems may be regarded as “an environmentally induced complex developmental disorder” (de Bellis, 2001). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.) During the first two years of life, chronic and cumulative states of overwhelming, hyperaroused affective states, as well as hypoaroused dissociation have devastating effects on the growth of psychic structure. The survival mode of conservation-withdrawal induces an extreme alteration of the bioenergetics of the developing brain. In critical periods of regional synaptogenesis this would have growth-inhibiting effects, especially in the right brain, which specializes in withdrawal. This is because the biosynthetic processes that mediate the proliferation of synaptic connections in the postnatally developing brain demand, in addition to sufficient quantities of essental nutrients, massive amounts of energy. An infant brain that is chronically shifting into hypometabolic survival modes has little energy available for growth (see Schore, 1994, 1997a, 2001b). Recent neurobiological studies in developmental traumatology indicate that the infant’s psychobiological response to trauma is comprised of two separate response patterns, hyperarousal and dissociation (Perry et al., 1995; Schore, 1998d, 1999b, c, 2001b, e, f, 2002e). In the initial stage of threat, an alarm reaction is initiated, in which the sympathetic component of the ANS is suddenly and significantly activated, resulting in increased heart rate, blood pressure, and respiration. Distress is expressed in crying and then screaming. This state of fear-terror is mediated by sympathetic hyperarousal, and it reflects increased levels of the major stress hormone corticotropin releasing factor, which in turn regulates noradrenaline and adrenaline activity (see Schore, 1997a, 2001b, 2002e). But a second later-forming, longer-lasting traumatic reaction is seen in dissociation, in which the child disengages from stimuli in the external world and attends to an “internal” world. The child’s dissociation in the midst of terror involves numbing, avoidance, compliance, and restricted affect. Traumatized infants are observed to be “staring off into space with a glazed look.” This parasympathetic dominant state of conservation-withdrawal occurs in helpless and hopeless stressful situations in which the individual becomes inhibited and strives to avoid attention in order to become “unseen” (Kaufman and Rosenblum, 1967; Schore, 1994). This primary regulatory process for maintaining organismic homeostasis (Engel and Schmale, 1972) is characterized by a metabolic shutdown (Schore, 2001b, in press a) and low levels of activity (McCabe and Schneiderman, 1985). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.) Very recent basic research indicates that maternal deprivation increases cell death in the infant brain (Zhang et al., 2002). Is this the death instinct? Recall, the state of conservationwithdrawal, a primary regulatory process of decreased metabolic energy is accessed when active coping (flight or fight) is not possible, occurs in hopeless and helpless contexts, and is behaviorally manifest as feigning death (Engel and Schmale, 1972; Powles, 1992). (Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.) Functions of the mind that spoil what we call ‘intimacy’ will deepen and darken the ‘black hole of trauma’ (van der Kolk & McFarlane, 1996). It is also known that secondary stressors play an increasingly in• uential role in the development of a trauma reaction progressively over time and damaged capacity for intimacy represents an important potential secondary stressor. C Posttraumatic stress effects on child witnesses have also been studied but, again, not as frequently as one might expect. Witnessing domestic violence between parents has been said to qualify as a traumatic stressor insofar as it entails the deliberate harm or threat of serious harm to a loved one and a profound sense of helplessness for child witnesses. CHILDREN EXPOSED TO INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE Research Findings and Implications for Intervention BONNIE E. CARLSON State University of New York at Albany TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 4, October 2000 321-342 Research findings do not support hippocampal shrinkage in children, although there is some evidence of generalized lower brain volumes. DeBellis and colleagues (DeBellis, Baum, et al., 1999; DeBellis,Keshavan, et al., 2002) failed to find hippocampal volume loss in maltreated children and adolescents compared with controls but did find other brain differences such as lower overall cerebral volume, lower corpus callosum volume, and greater ventricular and cerebral fluid volumes in the maltreated group. Similar results were found by Carrion, Weems, Eliez, et al. (2001) who observed significantly smaller overall brain and cerebral volumes in children with a history of repeated exposure to trauma compared with controls. Trauma-exposed children had smaller intracranial, cerebral, and prefrontal cortex, and prefrontal cortical white matter, as well as greater frontal lobe fluid volume than controls. Advances and Future Directions in the Study of Children’s Neurobiological Responses to Trauma and Violence Exposure KATHERINE BEVANS, Tulane University, ARLEEN B. CERBONE, Louisiana State University, STACY OVERSTREET, Tulane University JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 4, April 2005 418-425 Stress response is largely mediated by two interrelated systems, the locus coeruleusnorepinephrine (LC/NE) system and the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis (Lupien & McEwen, 1997). However, most neurobiological research on child exposure to violence and trauma concerns HPA-axis stress response (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001; Hart, Gunnar, & Cicchetti, 1996). The HPA axis is a complex system of interrelated, multiple structural regions and neuromodulators in the brain designed to mediate the fight-flight response. Basal activity of the HPA axis, which is essential for normal brain growth and metabolic activity, follows a circadian rhythm with high earlymorning cortisol levels declining to low levels around the onset of sleep (McEwen, 1998). Neuroimaging is another promising area of research into the effects of violence and trauma on the developing brain. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) can be used to identify gross pathological abnormalities as well as to quantify sizes of various brain regions. MRI is superior to computed tomographic (CT) scanning because of the relatively inferior soft tissue resolution seen in CT. Advances and Future Directions in the Study of Children’s Neurobiological Responses to Trauma and Violence Exposure KATHERINE BEVANS, Tulane University, ARLEEN B. CERBONE, Louisiana State University, STACY OVERSTREET, Tulane University JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 4, April 2005 418-425 One of the most exciting developments to emerge from the field in the past 20 years is the increasing attention to neurobiological responses to violence and trauma exposure. Although researchers have yet to identify a consensual pattern of neurobiological response to violence and trauma exposure, it does appear that some type of alteration in the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis is likely. This article briefly reviews the multiple moderating factors that help account for the divergent patterns in HPA function as well as methodological advances that will continue to improve the assessment of HPA function in youth exposed to violence and trauma. Advances and Future Directions in the Study of Children’s Neurobiological Responses to Trauma and Violence Exposure KATHERINE BEVANS, Tulane University, ARLEEN B. CERBONE, Louisiana State University, STACY OVERSTREET, Tulane University JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 4, April 2005 418-425 A number of researchers maintain that these two criteria preclude PTSD and amnesia from coexisting, and some empirical studies support this position [7± 10]. Sbordone [7] noted that the typical PTSD patient cannot erase the traumatic event from his/her mind and suffers recurrent intrusions and re-experiencing of the event, symptoms that are incompatible with amnesia. He further argued that amnesia, commonly resulting from cerebral contusion, is an impairment of the ability to process and record ongoing events. Therefore, in the aftermath of the trauma, there are no memories of the event for the amnesic patient to re-experience. Despite the theoretical arguments against co-occurring PTSD and amnesia, a number of case reports and empirical studies have indicated that amnesic victims can develop PTSD [1, 11± 13]. The present study prospectively examined the incidence of PTSD in amnesic and non-amnesic MVA victims. BIOLOŠKE ZNAČILNOSTI Recent studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients differ in basal neuroendocrine levels from trauma victims without PTSD. For example, PTSD patients have exhibited greater levels of 24-hour urinary catecholamine excretion than controls [14± 16], and a majority of studies (for exceptions see [17, 18]) have found lower 24-hour urinary cortisol excretion in patients with PTSD compared to trauma victims without PTSD and controls [14, 19± 23]. Heightened basal levels of catecholamines suggest a general hyperarousal of sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activity, while lower cortisol levels indicate an accompanying down-regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. In addition, combining SNS and HPA hormone levels into a NE/cortisol ratio has demonstrated greater diagnostic specificity for PTSD than either hormone alone [24]. These findings have led researchers to suggest that greatly increased catecholamine levels without the compensatory effect of accompanying cortisol increases at the time of a traumatic event could lead to the formation of `superconditioned’ memories that may become the basis of intrusive thoughts and the re-experiencing symptoms of PTSD [25]. The majority of these studies have examined neuroendocrine abnormalities in chronic PTSD patients who have often been symptomatic for over 20 years and suffered comorbid drug or alcohol abuse. More recent research has reported that MVA victims who met PTSD criteria 1 month following the accident excreted significantly lower levels of cortisol in 15-hour urine samples collected upon admission to the trauma unit than non-PTSD victims [2]. This suggests that initial physiological responses to trauma may contribute, in part, to the development of PTSD following a traumatic event. If amnesic patients experience the traumatic event differently from non-amnesics, this may provide a mechanism through which amnesia could buffer post-traumatic stress. The present study examined initial neuroendocrine response and the development of PTSD symptomatology in amnesic and non-amnesic MVA victims. It was hypothesized that amnesic patients would be less likely to meet PTSD diagnostic criteria 1 month after their accident and would display lower catecholamine levels and higher basal cortisol than non-amnesic victims. Further, it was hypothesized that initial hormone levels would mediate the relationship between amnesia and PTSD. REZULTATI Of the 53 participants who were assessed at the 1-month follow-up, nine individuals (17%) met full PTSD diagnostic criteria. Chi-square analyses revealed that non-amnesics were more likely to meet diagnostic criteria than amnesics (À2…1; n ˆ 53† ˆ 4:85; p < 0:05). None of the amnesic subjects met criteria for PTSD. The present study provides partial support for the hypothesis that amnesia for a traumatic event can serve as a buffering function in the development of subsequent PTSD among MVA victims. None of the amnesic patients in the sample met PTSD criteria due to their accident, and amnesic patients reported fewer symptoms of PTSD on the IES and on the SCID. Additionally, amnesics and non-amnesics differed in their initial physiological responses to the MVA, with amnesics having lower NE/cortisol ratios than non-amnesics. These results suggest that amnesics may physiologically experience the accident differently from non-amnesics, and that amnesics have lower subsequent PTSD incidence. However, urinary hormones did not mediate the effects of amnesia on PTSD, although this may have been due to the lack of power afforded by the small sample size. The findings of lower NE/cortisol ratio in amnesics compared to non-amnesics may provide some insight into the protectiveness of amnesia in subsequent PTSD development. Prior research has found that heightened catecholamine levels are associated with greater memory retention of stressful stimuli in animal and human studies [38, 39]. In addition, other researchers have hypothesized that high levels of catecholamines without the compensatory effects of cortisol could lead to aberrant memory formation or `supermemories’ [25]. These results support this hypothesis, as it was found that amnesics who had a combination of low NE and high cortisol (without differences in either alone) also had a lower incidence of PTSD, and fewer PTSD symptoms, than non-amnesics. Traumatic experiences cause traumatic stress, which disrupts homeostasis. During the past few years, we have dramatically increased our understanding of the effects of traumatic stress on the brain, sympathetic nervous system, and endocrine system. Through a physiological domino effect, these changes affect many other body systems, including the cardiovascular system, respiratory system, and muscular system. The endocrine system works closely with the nervous system to regulate the body’s physiology. Traumatic experience causes both immediate and long-term endocrine changes that affect metabolism and neurophysiology. The sympathetic nervous system is immediately affected by any perception of danger and signals the adrenal medulla to greatly increase its output of epinephrine and norepinephrine. These hormones rapidly affect many body systems leading to a fight-or-flight response. If neither response is possible, the person freezes. During stress, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system is also activated, leading to increased levels of cortisol, a glucocorticoid released by the adrenal cortex that modulates the physiologic response to stress and helps activate effective coping strategies. Cortisol concentration has been the focus of several recent studies because abnormally high levels of cortisol, associated with stress, can damage neurons in the hippocampus. Abnormal concentrations of adrenal hormones depress the immune system and contribute to the physiological hyperarousal (e.g., exaggerated startle response, hypervigilance) characteristic of PTSD. Chronic physiological hyperarousal makes it very difficult to regulate autonomic responses to internal or external signals and decreases the ability to respond appropriately to emotional signals (van der Kolk, 1996). In chronic stress and in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), cortisol concentrations are lower than would be expected, and exposure to new stressors elicits lower levels of cortisol secretion. To understand the problems presented by traumatic memories, we will first review how nontraumatic memories are processed. Memories of ordinary experiences are temporarily stored in the limbic system as episodic memories, memories of personal experience and events. Episodic memories are autobiographical; they include a sense of time and self. Cognitive aspects are stored in the hippocampus and the associated emotion is stored in the amygdala. As the brain processes these memories over time, aspects of them are abstracted and transferred to the neocortex, particularly the association areas of the frontal lobes, for long-term storage. These memories are semantic, or factual, memories. (Episodic and semantic memories are two types of explicit memory.) The memories of moderately disturbing experiences apparently remain in the right limbic system for a longer period of time than the memories of neutral events.We process disturbing memories by thinking, talking, and sometimes dreaming about the experience. As the brain slowly processes the memory, it is abstracted and transferred into the left neocortex where it is filed away along with other memories and becomes part of the narrative of one’s life. The stored information can be retrieved when needed to understand future events. Traumatic events overwhelm the brain’s capacity to process information. The episodic memory of the experience may be dysfunctionally stored in the right limbic system indefinitely and may generate vivid images of the traumatic experience, terrifying thoughts, feelings, body sensations, sounds, and smells. Such unprocessed traumatic memories can cause cognitive and emotional looping, anxiety, PTSD, maladaptive coping strategies, depression, and many other psychological symptoms of distress. Because the episodic memory is not processed, a relevant semantic memory is not stored and the individual has difficulty using knowledge from the experience to guide future action. Because traumatic experiences are terrifying, the survivor avoids thinking and talking about what happened. This avoidance prevents processing. Trauma alters physiology and gives rise to images, feelings, sensations, and beliefs that may persist throughout life. Only after the traumatic memory is fully processed and integrated can homeostasis be restored. Traumatic memories can be triggered by stimuli that are in some way associated with the traumatic event. Terrifying memories, including the affect associated with them, may be reexperienced with their original intensity. Survivors feel the terror and may lose their sense of time and place. One client stated, “Part of me knows it’s not really happening now, but it feels so real that I get mixed up.” Brain scan technology enables us to study the brain in action. Using PET scans, researchers have demonstrated some of the neurophysiological effects that take place when traumatic memory is triggered. In one study, participants were asked to write detailed narratives of their traumatic experience (Rauch et al., 1996). Then, each participant was asked to read the narrative during brain scanning. The results were dramatic. Activity increased in the right brain, primarily in the limbic system and in the visual cortex (the site of vivid images of the event). Activity decreased in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which normally modulates the limbic system. Activity also decreased in Broca’s area, an area of the brain important in semantic processing and articulation of language. This decrease in activity may be the neurophysiological basis for the “speechless terror” that many individuals experience both during a traumatic event and when processing trauma in therapy. In the United States, child abuse and neglect are the most common causes of Type III trauma, extreme trauma characterized by multiple traumatic experiences that typically begin at an early age (Solomon & Heide, 1999). An infant’s relationship with its primary caregiver has a direct effect on the hard wiring of neural circuits in the developing brain. Many of the neural circuits affected by early experience connect areas of the brain critical for emotional, physiological, psychological, and social development. Some of these circuits are necessary for adaptive coping in emotional and stressful situations (Schore, 2003). The orbitofrontal cortex helps regulate emotional states and responses. By way of its connections with the hypothalamus and limbic system, it regulates autonomic responses to social stimuli and mediates emotionally “attuned communication.” This part of the cortex helps us understand other people’s emotional experience, enabling us to respond empathically, a capacity necessary for moral judgment. Normal development of the right brain and later emotional and social development depend on healthy attachment between infant and caregiver (Schore, 1994, 1996, 2003). Children who are severely neglected experience chronic traumatic stress that compromises right brain development, resulting in neuron damage and atrophy. Impairment of the orbitofrontal cortex and the circuits connecting it with subcortical areas can diminish the child’s sense of self, leading to disconnection from other people. Severely neglected children do not deal well with stress and do not develop the ability to regulate the intensity and duration of their affect (Schore, 2002; van der Kolk & Fisler, 1994). Because these children have difficulty understanding emotion expressed by other people, they may not develop empathy. Many studies conclude that impaired development of the orbitofrontal cortex and its neural connections with the limbic system decreases capacity to regulate affect. For example, the orbitofrontal cortex normally inhibits areas in the hypothalamus that are associated with aggression and thus is central in the regulation of aggressive impulses. Abnormal development of the neural circuits linking the orbitofrontal cortex and ACC with the amygdala interferes with normal inhibition of rage responses.Without the normal corSolomon, Heide / THE BIOLOGY OF TRAUMA 55 tical modulating effect, the amygdala’s responses are exaggerated. When aggressive impulses are not inhibited, an individual may act out violently. This lack of inhibition is part of the pattern of sociopathy (Best,Williams,& Coccaro, 2002; Schore, 2003). Studies suggest that trauma caused by neglect and abuse can lead to antisocial behavior (Heide, 1992, 1999). Many long-term changes in the brain have been associated with Type III trauma, including abnormal concentrations of certain neurotransmitters, changes in EEG patterns, and a decrease in integration between right and left hemispheres. Measurable size decreases have been found in the cerebral volume, the corpus callosum, amygdala, and hippocampus. Whether or not these changes are reversible with treatment is an important question for future study. NEVROPSIHOLOŠKE RAZLIKE MED SPOLOMA Fig. 1. The amygdala (red region), a small almond-shaped structure located deep in the anterior temporal lobe, plays a critical role in a variety of emotional processes including emotional memory and adaptive responses to emotional stimuli. Recent work suggests that several differences between men and women in emotional responses arise in part from sex differences in amygdala responses. Reprinted with permission by Digitial Anatomist Project, Department of Biological Structure, University of Washington. In addition to functional differences in amygdala response, such as in emotional memory and in responses to sexually arousing stimuli, the amygdala in men and women differs in terms of structure and in aspects of brain development. These structural and developmental differences likely contribute to the functional differences observed in neuroimaging studies. One major difference between the sexes is the size of the amygdala. In the adult human brain, the male amygdala is significantly larger than the female amygdala, even when total brain size is taken into account (Goldstein and others 2001). Although the specific consequences of this sex difference in amygdala size are not known, structural differences in brain anatomy often are associated with differences in brain function and response. For example, one recent study found a relation between the size of the amygdala in patients with epilepsy and sexual drive; patients with greater residual amygdala size after undergoing neurosurgery reported greater sexual drive and motivation (Baird and others 2004). Interestingly, the brain regions that differ in size between men and women tend also to be the same regions that contain high concentrations of sex hormone receptors, suggesting that male and female hormones play a role in determining the size of specific brain regions such as the amygdala during brain development (Goldstein and others 2001). Consistent with this idea, neuroimaging studies have found that amygdala, which contains relatively high concentrations of sex hormone receptors, develops structurally at different rates in human males and females. Other structural differences in areas that receive strong neuronal connections from the amygdala, such as the hypothalamus, which is larger in men than women, may also contribute to sex differences in brain response that involve the amygdala. Circulating levels of sex hormones in the bloodstream constitute an additional influence on amygdala response through their action on receptor sites. Future work will be necessary to elucidate the complex relationship between structural, developmental, and functional aspects of amygdala sex differences. Memory for emotional events is generally better than memory for emotionally neutral events (Hamann 2001). Several psychological studies have reported that men and women differ substantially with respect to emotional memory (Hamann and Canli 2004). For example, women can recall emotional memories more quickly, can recall more emotional memories in a given period of time, and report that the emotional memories they recall are richer, more vivid, and more intense. In general, then, women tend to experience greater enhancement of their memory by emotion (Seidlitz and Diener 1998). The stronger effect of emotion on women’s memories is not entirely beneficial, however. As described below, emotion can also impair memory in some situations, and this impairment is accentuated in women. In addition, the fact that emotional memories tend to be stronger for women may be linked to the greater prevalence of depression and some types of anxiety disorders in women (Davidson and others 2002). The three neuroimaging studies that have examined the brain correlates of these differences in emotional memories have found a remarkably consistent pattern of sex differences in the role of the left and right amygdala in emotional memory. These studies have focused on the effects of emotional arousal on declarative memory, memory for facts or events that can be brought to mind through a conscious, voluntary effort to retrieve the memory (Squire and Zola 1996). Each of these studies examined differences in brain activity occurring during memory encoding (i.e., memory formation) that were predictive of subsequent successful emotional memory retrieval. That is, one can examine which items are successfully retrieved on a later test and then go back to determine which brain areas were more active when those items were originally encoded in the brain scanner. Cahill and others (2001) used PET to image brain activity while men and women watched either highly aversive films or neutral films. The level of amygdala activity at encoding predicted later emotional memory performance for both males and females. However, for females, this relation was found in the left amygdala whereas for males it was in the right amygdala. A later study by Canli and others (2002) examined brain activity in men and women during the encoding of emotional and neutral scenes in photographs, using fMRI. Consistent with the prior PET study, amygdala activity during the encoding of the most emotionally arousing photographs was strongly related to later recognition memory for the emotional pictures, but again this relationship was seen in the left amygdala for women and the right amygdala for men. The strength of an emotional experience, referred to as emotional arousal, is currently thought to be the most important factor that determines the degree of memory enhancement associated with an emotional event (Hamann and others 1999; Canli and others 2000). In this study, participants’ ratings of emotional arousal correlated with left amygdala activity in both men and women. Thus, in females the brain regions involved in emotional reactions coincide with those involved in encoding memory for the experience, whereas in males these processes occur in different hemispheres. The authors suggested the greater overlap between the neural correlates of emotional experience and emotional memory in women as a possible explanation for the greater vividness and accuracy of their emotional memories. In the third study, Cahill and others (2004) examined memory for emotionally arousing photographs in men and women using a similar fMRI task as was used by Canli and others (2002) and again found that later levels of emotional memory were strongly correlated with left amygdala activity in women but right amygdala activity in men. In summary, these studies have found a consistent sex difference between the role of the left and right amygdalas (Fig. 2). Speculations regarding the origin of these sex differences have included sexually dimorphic brain development (Goldstein and others 2001), the influence of sex hormones both during development and during adulthood, and possible differences in the cognitive style used by men and women in encoding emotional experiences. Strange and others (2003) examined the effect of inserting an emotional event (an emotionally arousing word) into a sequence of neutral events (a list of neutral words). As expected, memory for the arousing word was better than memory for the neutral words. Interestingly, however, memory for words presented just before the emotional word was also affected, but instead of enhancement, memory was impaired. This impairment was not found in a patient with bilateral amygdala lesions, strongly suggesting that the effect is amygdaladependent. In addition, the size of the emotion-induced memory impairment was found to be twice as large for women than for men. The specific mechanisms responsible for this memory impairment are yet unknown, but these findings are noteworthy in that they suggest that both the enhancing and the impairing effects of emotion are magnified in women. MERJENJE Life Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ) The LEQ is a self-report instrument developed by the second author (Long, 1999), which includes questions regarding demographics and childhood sexual experiences. Participants were instructed to report all sexual experiences occurring before the age of 17. Modified Sexual Experiences Survey (MSES) The MSES is a modified version of the 10-item Sexual Experiences Survey (SES) (Koss&Gidycz, 1985) andwas used to assess adult unwanted sexual contact. The MSES asks a series of yes/no questions assessing whether specific types of sexual activities had been attempted or completed with the participant since the age of 17. Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS) The CTS Form N developed by Straus (1979) was used to assess the presence and extent of intimate violence among dating partners and spouses. Psychological Maltreatment of Women Inventory (PMWI) Tolman’s (1989) PMWI was used to assess psychological abuse and contains two subscales. Abuse questionnaire The next questions are about forms of childhood abuse to which you may have been exposed to before 16 years of age. 1. Do you think that there was any kind of emotional neglect? (This means for example that people at home didn’t listen to you, that your problems were ignored, that you had the feeling of not being able to find any attention or support from the people in your house). 2. Do you think there was any kind of psychological abuse? (This means for example; being sworn at, lesser treatment compared to brothers or sisters, unjustified punishment, blackmail). 3. Do you think there was any kind of physical abuse? (That is, were you ever beaten, kicked, punched or did you experience any other kind of physical abuse? 4. Were you ever approached sexually against your will? (This means: were you ever been touched sexually by anyone against your will or forced to touch anybody, were you ever pressured into sexual contact against your will?) INSTRUMENTI ZA MERJENJE SIMPTOMOV KOMPLEKSNE TRAVME Posttraumatic symptoms, PTSD, and CPTSD. The following instruments are recommended at this time: Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al., 1996), Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES–R; Weiss & Marmar, 1997), Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic States (DAPS; Briere, 2001), and Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa, 1995). Perhaps the two most useful in the identification of CPTSD are the Trauma Symptom Inventory (TSI), an instrument developed to assess trauma symptoms proper but that assesses domains of the self and relations with others (Briere, 1995; Briere, Elliot, Harris, & Cotman, 1995), and the Structured Interview for Disorders of Extreme Stress (SIDES), developed for the DSM–IV field trial (Pelcovitz et al., 1997; van der Kolk, 1999; Zlotnick & Pearlstein, 1997). Additionally, the Inventory of Altered Self Capacities (IASC; Briere, 2000b) assesses difficulties in relatedness, identity, and affect regulation and is therefore very pertinent to this population, as do the Cognitive Distortion Scales (CDS; Briere, 2000a) and the Trauma and Attachment Belief Scale (Pearlman, 2003), measures of trauma-related beliefs and cognitive distortions. NEOPREDELJENO