Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics LOT Summer School 2006 Issues in the biology and evolution of language Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini University of Arizona Session 1 (June 12) The birth of a paradigm: Innatism versus selectivism Plan of this course Today (Monday): The birth of selectivism and the idea of parameters Tuesday: Towards a genetics of language Wednesday: Loss of speech Thursday: The return of the laws of form Friday: Contemporary biology and the minimalist program LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 2 Little guide to the readings General position papers on biolinguistics: Chomsky’s Three factors My paper with Cedric Boeckx Freidin and Vergnaud Tutorials Boeckx Chapter 5 on minimalism My handout on the Hauser, Chomsky and Fitch versus Pinker and Jackendoff on evolution Christiansen and Kirby on language evolution NEW Simon Fisher The tangled web in Cognition June 6, 06 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 3 Little guide to the readings (2) “Representative” pieces Turing on morphogenesis Davidson and Erwin on Gene Networks Hill and Walsh on brain evolution Marcus and Fisher (on FOXP2) Gibbs on epigenetics Punctual papers Somerville et al. on Williams syndrome Fisher on genes and language Scharf and White on Foxp2 in birds Uriagereka and me on the immune syntax (unreadable) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 4 Some caveats The biology of language is a huge field 750 papers just on brain imaging and language About 150 references (papers and books) on the evolution of language, just in the last 10 years or so About 25 genes (tentatively) identified already as being language-related Many other fields are relevant (molecular genetics, evo-devo, neuroscience of cognition, various pathologies, comparative cognitive ethology etc.) Not to mention, of course, linguistics, language acquisition and psycholinguistics LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 5 Some caveats Our strategy here: Explore with a critical eye the “possibility” of a biology of language Its “logic” and its possible import Privileging what we know (rather than what we would like to know, but we don’t) Concentrating on the strong points Singling out the best cases (breakthroughs) And plausible avenues of future development With (yes!) some “fine” details that may, at first blush, seem of scant interest to linguists But they are not (I hope I will persuade you that they are really very interesting) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 6 Three factors in language design (1) genetic endowment, which sets limits on the attainable languages, thereby making language acquisition possible; (2) external data, converted to the experience that selects one or another language within a narrow range; (3) principles not specific to FL. Some of the third factor principles have the flavor of the constraints that enter into all facets of growth and evolution, and that are now being explored intensively in the “evo-devo” revolution. There are other third factor elements as well, among them properties of the human brain that determine what cognitive systems can exist. It also might turn out that general cognitive principles that enter into language acquisition pose conditions on FL design. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 7 Two varieties of pessimism Max Planck: A new scientific truth does not triumph by convincing its opponents and making them see the light, but rather because its opponents eventually die, and a new generation grow up that is familiar with it. Noam Chomsky: New ideas circulate only because, eventually, professors are embarrassed by their students for confessing they do not know about them. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 8 The innatist-selectivist explanatory strategy: Enters linguistics via the Poverty of the Stimulus (POS) Explicit references (in earlier work by Chomsky) to Luria and Delbruck, to Hubel and Wiesel and to Monod and Jacob. In continuity with the powerfully emerging trend in molecular biology Later reinforced by Fodor’s modularity By data on language acquisition And by the principles-and-parameters framework LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 9 Charles Darwin (1809 -1882) selection LOT Summer 2006 Jean-Baptiste de Lamarck (1744 - 1829) instruction Birth of a paradigm 10 A long-standing debate Instructive versus selective change and adaptation Revamped in immunology (around 1890) Revamped in microbiology (Pasteur and Koch, from 1880 onwards) Koch’s postulate: one disease = one microbial agent (cholera, typhus, tuberculosis etc.) Doubts that bacteria could “have a genetics” until about 1935 Frederick Griffith (1928): the “transforming agent” of pneumococcus from harmless to pathogenic LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 11 A long-standing debate (continued) Avery, McLeod and McCarty (1944): the transforming agent is DNA Quite a shock to everyone (Nobel Prize 1983) Further revamped by the discovery of the healing power of antibiotics in the late Thirties and Forties (penicillin, streptomycin, chloramphenicol) In particular, by the appearance of resistant microbial strains A debate about what? LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 12 Two positions: The first The “inductivists” (Felix D’Hérelle et al.): Adaptive mutations are induced by the external agent (temperature, antibiotics, viruses, metabolites etc.) There are “directed adaptive heritable changes” (induced adaptations) The reference conceptual model: spontaneous radioactive decay (The probability of decaying is constant across all atoms of a given isotope of that element) And catalysis (the dominant conceptual model) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 13 Two positions: The second The “selectivists” (A. Gratia, F. M. Burnet et al.): Mutations are spontaneous, with a stable fixed average probability of occurrence (about 10-8 per locus per generation) BUT They occur independently of, in the absence of, and prior to, any exposure to the environmental factor. No “directionality”. At the 3rd Congress of Microbiology in New York, in 1939, Andre’ Gratia declared: "Adaptation by passive selection of pre-existing variants is the only fact to be proven beyond any doubt" (GRATIA 1939) Selection acts post hoc and “adaptation” is a result of it LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 14 Why do we care? Reference to these phenomena, and to selectivist explanations, is ubiquitous in Chomsky’s work (see his debate with Piaget) Luria and Chomsky and Eric Lenneberg at MIT created a bio-linguistics group meeting regularly The 1974 meeting at Endicott House Fodor’s innatism and the pre-existence of all concepts Principles and parameters (ever since the late Seventies) Parameter-based language acquisition LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 15 No learning: Rather the fixation of a handful of linguistic parameters Each having only two possible values + or A “cascade” of switches One language? LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 16 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 17 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 18 Mark Baker 2001, 2003 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 19 A simple “knockdown” experiment Salvador E. Luria and Max Delbrück (1943) “Mutation of bacteria from virus sensitivity to virus resistance”, Genetics, Vol. 28, pp 491-511 Nobel Prize in 1969 with Alfred D. Hershey Hall of fame of elegant experiments in biology Inspiration from a slot-machine in a Country Club in Bloomington Indiana The very idea: Grow different cultures of bacteria sensitive to a virus (a phage) Make successive dilutions of samples from the various cultures (successive generations) Add the virus, then see how many resistant colonies you obtain LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 20 A simple “knockdown” experiment If the inductivists are right, then You get an average constant percentage of resistant mutants at each generation If and only if, they have been exposed to the virus. If the selectivists are right, you get “a distribution with an abnormally high variance” All (or most) of the descendants of a mutant are resistant All (or most) of the descendants of a sensitive wild type are wiped out The presence of the virus allows us to make a selection, but it is not the “inducing agent” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 21 A technical challenge: In order to ascertain the existence of resistant mutants You have to add the virus to the culture But then it’s hard to decide whether the mutants pre-existed or are “induced” by the virus Luria’s and Delbrück’s solution Fluctuations across generations. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 22 The Luria-Delbrück dilution experiment Bacteria sensitive to the virus (a bacteriophage) in black. Resistant mutants in red. Culture 1 harbors a 3rd generation mutant. Culture 3 harbors a 1st generation mutant. The probability of observing mutants varies very strongly. In fact, it is 1 or 0, depending on whether the ancestor is or is not a mutant. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 23 The Luria-Delbrück dilution experiment Had the mutation been “induced” by the exposure, we would Expect a uniform probability of finding mutant colonies (an average constant fraction of all later cultures would be mutants) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 24 Conclusion: “We consider the above results as proof that in our case the resistance to virus is due to a heritable change of the bacterial cell which occurs independently of the action of the virus.” (emphasis added) Do we need successive dilutions? Not necessarily Same results with a different technique: Replica Plating LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 25 Replica plating (Joshua and Esther Lederberg 1952) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 26 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 27 “The procedure at no time exposes the indirectly selected populations to the specific agent [streptomycin]. These observations, therefore, are cited as confirmation of previous evidence for the participation of spontaneous mutation and population selection in the heritable adaptation of bacteria to new agents.” (emphasis added) Joshua and Esther Lederberg (then at Madison Wisconsin) Journal of Bacteriology, 1952 Joshua Lederberg, Nobel Prize 1959 “for studies on genetic recombination and organization of the genetic material in bacteria” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 28 1st important lesson: The selective agent does not induce the mutation It selects pre-existing mutants Specific mutants pre-exist, regardless of all encounters with the selective agent LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 29 Next classic experiment Preceded, over many years, by a puzzle (enzymatic adaptation) Something you expected to happen but doesn’t. Imagine the following cases: (1) A new kind of combustion engine Outputs 200 HPs when burning fuel A Outputs 300 HPs when burning fuel B What do you expect with a mixture of the two fuels? (2) Most patients recover in 30 days under treatment with antibiotic A Most patients recover in 60 days under treatment with antibiotic B What do you expect with a mixed treatment? LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 30 Jacques Monod and the “double growth” (diauxia) (1940) Log n Log n 2 Glucose 1 Xylose 1 t Log n t Glucose + Xylose Expected t LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 31 Jacques Monod and the “double growth” (diauxia) (1940) Log n Log n 2 Glucose 1 Xylose 1 t t Log n Glucose + Xylose Actually observed t LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 32 Monod’s original (non-logarithmic) graphs LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 33 In Monod’s doctoral dissertation (1940) “Microbiology will not make much progress until we have solved this puzzle”. It took 20 years to solve it: Genetic regulation as a switching process (not a “catalytic” one) There are DNA sequences (genes) whose exclusive function is the activation-inactivation of adjacent genes. Nobel Prize with François Jacob and André Lwoff in 1965 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 34 Monod’s and Jacob’s explanation The regulating mechanism and the final result have been associated and fine-tuned by natural selection (the “inductor” is the very metabolite that the enzyme - expressed by the activated gene “digests”) But the process is totally “mechanical” The regulator and “its” gene can be separately disassembled and re-assembled at leisure LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 35 QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 36 In the absence of lactose repressor blocks promoter QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 37 In the presence of lactose repressor cannot bind QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 38 Central points: Seeing clearly that a puzzle in a class of phenomena stonewalls the discipline as a whole Even in the absence of the faintest idea on how to solve the puzzle Seeing clearly that the extant conceptualizations (catalysis) cannot begin to solve the puzzle An educated guess that the solution of the puzzle will reverberate much beyond that class of phenomena LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 39 The case of antibodies Selectivism, then 50 years of instructivism Then, finally, selectivism LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 40 How everything began: Paul Ehrlich LOT Summer 2006 The hypothesis Ehrlich developed to explain immunological phenomena was the side-chain theory, which described how antibodies - the protective proteins produced by the immune system - are formed and how they react with other substances. This theory was based on an understanding of the way in which a cell was thought to absorb and assimilate nutrients. Birth of a paradigm 41 Ehrlich’s side-chain theory of antibody production LOT Summer 2006 Each cell has on its surface a series of side chains, or receptors, that function by attaching to certain food molecules. While each side chain interacts with a specific nutrient - in the same manner as a key fits into a lock - it can also interact with disease-causing toxins produced by an infectious agent. When a toxin binds to a side chain, the interaction is irreversible and blocks subsequent binding and uptake of nutrients. The body then tries to overwhelm the obstruction by producing a great number of replacement side chains — so many that they cannot fit on the surface of the cell and instead are secreted into the circulation. Birth of a paradigm 42 Ehrlich’s side-chain theory of antibody production LOT Summer 2006 According to Ehrlich's theory, the circulating side chains are the antibodies, which are all gauged to and able to neutralize the disease-causing toxin and then remain in the circulation, thus immunizing the individual against subsequent invasions by the infectious agent. Antibodies pre-exist. Birth of a paradigm 43 Karl Landsteiner and the dawn of biochemistry LOT Summer 2006 Small organic molecules of simple structure, such as phenyl arsonates and nitrophenyls, are not natural danger signals, and do not provoke antibodies when injected by themselves. However, antibodies can be raised against them if the molecule is attached covalently, by simple chemical reactions, to a protein carrier. Such small molecules were termed haptens (from the Greek haptein, to fasten) by the immunologist Karl Landsteiner, who first studied them in the early 1900s. Birth of a paradigm 44 Karl Landsteiner and the dawn of biochemistry LOT Summer 2006 Landsteiner found that animals immunized with a hapten-carrier conjugate respond by producing distinct sets of antibodies. No lock-and-key, but a more or less good fit. Antibodies drape themselves over the charge outline of their target antigen (instructivist model). Birth of a paradigm 45 Felix Haurowitz and the Template Theory of Antibody Formation. Selectivism is unvorstellbar LOT Summer 2006 Haurowitz and Landsteiner collaborated to define the chemical nature of antibodies. "I concluded that the antibody must be serum globulin and suggested therefore that the antigen interferes with the process of globulin biosynthesis in such a way that globulins complementarily adjusted to the antigen are formed." Antibody formation takes place by the assembly of the antibody molecule on the antigen (instructivist model). Birth of a paradigm 46 The 1984 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine: Niels Jerne LOT Summer 2006 Niels Jerne’s natural selection theory for the immune system was published in 1955 (!). Lederberg and Nossall: one lymphocyte clone = one antibody Jerne proposed that the capacity of the immune system to recognize millions of foreign molecules was predetermined, already existing in the body when the very first contact with a foreign structure was made. What then happened was merely a selection amongst the naturally occurring antibody population resulting in an increase in production of exactly those antibodies which happened to have a good fit for the structure. Birth of a paradigm 47 The 1984 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine: Niels Jerne LOT Summer 2006 Jerne's theory stood in great contrast to prevailing theories at that time (the unimaginable wastefulness of selection), but was rapidly confirmed and extended. Natural selection applies to the cells of the immune system. Those cells which happen to have received the property to produce a wanted antibody type will upon vaccination be rewarded with proliferative capacity and survival. Birth of a paradigm 48 The adaptive immune response The molecules of adaptive immunity (e.g., antibodies): Are generated by random DNA rearrangements Pre-exist to the encounter with danger signals (innate) Are selected by specific stimuli Repertoire is virtually unlimited (3D recognition of molecular shapes) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 49 Grammar is a science that is more than 2000 years old, whereas immunology has become a respectable part of biology only during the past hundred years. Though both sciences still face exasperating problems, this lecture attempts to establish an analogy between linguistics and immunology, between the descriptions of language and of the immune system. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 50 An immunologist quotes a linguist At this point, I (Jerne) shall make a quotation from Noam Chomsky concerning linguistics: “The central fact to which any significant linguistic theory must address itself is this: a mature speaker can produce a new sentence of his language on the appropriate occasion, and other speakers can understand it immediately, though it is equally new to them … Grammar is a device that specifies the infinite set of wellformed sentences and assigns to each of these one or more structural descriptions. Perhaps we should call such a device a generative grammar … which should, ideally, contain a central syntactic component…, a phonological component and a semantic component.” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 51 Jerne’s conclusion The inheritable “deep” structure of the immune system is now known: certain chromosomes of all vertebrate animals contain DNA segments which encode the variable regions of antibody polypeptides. Furthermore, experiments in recent years have demonstrated the generative capacities of this innate system. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 52 A remarkable insight: “It seems a miracle that young children easily learn the language of any environment into which they were born. The generative approach to grammar, pioneered by Chomsky, argues that this is only explicable if certain deep, universal features of this competence are innate characteristics of the human brain. Biologically speaking, this hypothesis of an inheritable capability to learn any language means that it must somehow be encoded in the DNA of our chromosomes. Should this hypothesis one day be verified, then linguistics would become a branch of biology”. (emphasis added) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 53 Chomsky’s “Review of Skinner’s ‘Verbal Behavior’” (1959) “The magnitude of the failure of this [the behaviorist’s] attempt to account for verbal behavior serves as a kind of measure of the importance of the factors omitted from consideration, and an indication of how little is really known about this remarkably complex phenomenon”. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 54 A Review of Skinner’s “Verbal Behavior” (1959) “Study of the actual observed ability of a speaker to distinguish sentences from non-sentences, detect ambiguities, etc., apparently forces us to the conclusion that this grammar is of an extremely complex and abstract character, and that the young child has succeeded in carrying out what from the formal point of view, at least, seems to be a remarkable type of theory construction. Furthermore, this task is accomplished in an astonishingly short time, to a large extent independently of intelligence, and in a comparable way by all children. Any theory of learning must cope with these facts.” (my emphasis) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 55 Jerry Fodor in the debate with Piaget (1976) “…there must be some notion of learning that is so incredibly different from the one we have imagined that we don’t even know what it would be like, as things now stand.” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 56 Another window of opportunity: Selective visual deprivation Extreme specificity of the sensitivity of individual neurons; Modularity of the organization of the primary visual cortex Strong innate components The “selective” (not instructive) nature of the visual inputs The effects of selective deprivation (shift of allegiance) The crucial importance of critical periods. The crucial importance of competitive mechanisms LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 57 In essence: a whole new paradigm The specificity and fine-graininess of the innate endowment (pre-wired selective sensitivity to shapes, modes of motion, edges, contrasts, etc.) Strong modularity The role of specific data (from experience) as selectors (activators / suppressors) The crucial role of critical periods The crucial role of competition mechanisms (winner-take-all) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 58 Our two protagonists David Hubel Torsten Wiesel At Johns Hopkins and then at Harvard 1959-1962 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 59 Our two protagonists David Hubel Torsten Wiesel Nobel Prize in 1981 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 60 The role of experience From their Nobel lectures “Innate mechanisms endow the visual system with highly specific connections, but visual experience early in life is necessary for their maintenance and full development.” “Deprivation experiments demonstrate that neural connections can be modulated by environmental influences during a critical period of postnatal development.” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 61 The role of experience (continuation) “Such sensitivity of the nervous system to the effects of experience may represent the fundamental mechanism by which the organism adapts to its environment during the period of growth and development.” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 62 A central reflection, an afterthought “Visual experience seems to have the power of validating or vetoing not only the outcomes of the process of differentiation but the process itself”. (my emphasis) Wiesel, T.N. 1982 Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. Nature, 299, 583-591. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 63 Blakemore, C., and Cooper, G. F. (1970). Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature , 228:477-478. The result is that the “vertical” cells multiply, while the “horizontal” cells shrink and degenerate. Neither eye was ever closed. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 64 Optic nerve Optic chasm Optic tract Lateral geniculate body Primary visual cortex LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 65 (Drawing by Jeff Stripling) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm Area 17 Area V1 Striate cortex 66 QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. Area 17 http://webvision.med.utah.edu/imageswv/capas-cortex.jpg LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 67 Quick Time™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 68 QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 69 QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 70 The columnar organization of the cortex In the years 1955-1959 Vernon B. Mountcastle (at Johns Hopkins University) discovered the columnar organization of the cortex Basically, this means that, as we proceed “vertically”, from the outside inwards, we encounter groups of cells (of about 100 cells each) that are very similar in their specialization (they are sensitive to the same stimuli) If, instead, we proceed “horizontally” (parallel to the surface of the cortex), we encounter groups of cells that have different specializations. With “abrupt transitions in functional properties which separate one column from the next”. See http://cercor.oupjournals.org/cgi/content/full/13/1/2 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 71 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 72 The “meaning” of a column In Hubel’s words: A column = “a little machine that takes care of contours in a certain orientation in a certain part of the visual field”. If the cells of one set are to be interconnected, and to some extent isolated from neighboring sets, it makes obvious sense to gather them together. The function of the visual cortex is the transformation of information from circularly symmetric form to orientation-specific form, and the stepwise increase in complexity. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 73 Binocular cells A high proportion of cells in the primary visual (striate) cortex receive inputs from both eyes. BUT In the lateral geniculate body, cells receive input from one eye only. Hubel and Wiesel discovered that there is a striking similarity of the corresponding cells’ receptive fields in the two eyes, in size, complexity, orientation and position. Presumably this forms the basis of the fusion of the images in the two eyes. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 74 A wonderful online treatise on eye and vision, with great images: http://webvision.med.utah.edu/index.html LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 75 The first “processing” station: The retinal ganglion cells are the “output” of the retina They act largely independently one from the other to encode information see Niremberg S. et al (2001) Nature Vol. 411, pp. 698-701 They gather and integrate impulses from several cells in the retina Haldan Keffer Hartline (Nobel with Ragnar Granit and George Wald in 1967) discovered in 1935 that there are basically three kinds of such cells: The ON, the OFF and the ON-OFF ganglion cells LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 76 A subtle concept: The receptive field http://psych.hanover.edu/Krantz/receptive/ The region of the retina within which a local change of brightness would cause the ganglion cell to discharge (Hartline, 1935-38) The interactive “annulus” (center and ring) that causes a ganglion cell to discharge (Kuffler, 1953) Includes the structure of the effective stimulus (Hubel and Wiesel, 1956) area of the visual field in which stimulation leads to response of a particular sensory neuron (Levine and Shefner, 1991) Notice: The definition went from the retina to the “outside world” LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 77 See David Hubel’s online book Eye, Brain and Vision http://neuro.med.harvard.edu/site/dh/bcontex.htm No stimulus On-center retinal ganglion cell No stimulus Off-center retinal ganglion cell Stephen Kuffler (Johns Hopkins, ever since 1952) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 78 QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see thi s picture. This cell not only responds exclusively to a moving slit in an eleven o'clock orientation but also responds to movement right and up, but hardly at all to movement left and down. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 79 Responses to a long, narrow slit of light QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. Orientation is crucial LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 80 How narrow is the optimum angle? Typically it’s 10-20 degrees Notice that the degree between two successive hours on a clock dial is 30 degrees LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 81 A typical “vertical” cell LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 82 A typical (vertically) directionally sensitive complex cell, more sensitive to the top-down than to the bottom-up displacement LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 83 A remarkable fact (Pasko Rakic 1972) By the 26th gestational week the human neocortex is already composed of a large number of minicolumns in parallel vertical arrays. This remarkable regularity is revealed in histological sections closely aligned with the vertical axes of minicolumns. At least at the level of the cortex, “modularity” is quite precocious. Columns vary between 300 and 500 µm in transverse diameter, and do not differ significantly in size between brains that vary in size over three orders of magnitude (Bugbee and Goldman-Rakic, 1983) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 84 Species-specificity of the critical period The length of the critical period varies between species. In cats it is 3 to 4 months And from clinical observations in humans (in ophthalmology clinics) it may extend up to 5 10 years, though the susceptibility to deprivation appears to be most pronounced during the first year and declines with age. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 85 The organization Cells of different complexities, whose receptive fields are in the same part of the visual field and which have the same optimal orientation, are likely to be interconnected, whereas cells with different optimal orientations are far less likely to be interconnected. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 86 QuickTime™ and a TIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. S. M. Kosslyn, A. Pascual-Leone, et al Science Vol 284, pp. 167-170,1999 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 87 References Pathways of the Brain, chapters 16-17 and Vernon Mountcastle Perceptual Neuroscience: The Cerebral Cortex, Harvard University Press, 1998. See also Yves Burnod, An Adaptive Neural Nework: The Cerebral Cortex (1990) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 88 The forming of maps and associations Cortical columns in sensory areas (auditory, visual, somatosensory) form maps. Regions of cortex adjacent to these maps are associative, with the associations becoming progressively higher level and more abstract with greater distance from the sensory map. For instance, the intensities of different frequencies of sound waves are mapped on the planum temporale, while cortical areas in more inferior areas of the temporal lobe process higher level information, starting with sounds and moving to word concepts. http://www.ruf.rice.edu/~lngbrain/Farh/cc.html LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 89 The famous deprivation experiments “In an animal that has undergone monocular deprivation, the geniculate terminals with input from the non-deprived eye take over much of the space that would normally have been occupied by terminals from the deprived eye”. “The deprived eye input has shrunken down to occupy the small strips lying between the terminals of the non-deprived eye input”. “Tangential electrode penetrations through cortical layers reveal long expanses of cells driven by the non-deprived eye interrupted by small patches of cells that are either unresponsive or driven by the deprived eye.” From the Nobel lectures (emphasis added) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 90 Shift of allegiance, not un-responsiveness “Cells at later stages have shifted their allegiance from the deprived to the nondeprived eye, rather than becoming unresponsive”. (my - MPP - emphasis) “This conclusion is supported by the physiological findings that the large majority of cells in superficial and deep layers respond only to the stimulation of the normal eye”. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 91 Innateness It still seems remarkable that a cell should not only be wired with the precision necessary to produce complex or hyper-complex properties, but should have a duplicate set of such connections, one from each eye. That this is hard wired at birth forms some of the material of Torsten Wiesel’s lecture. In vertical penetrations the preference remains the same all the way through the cortex. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 92 Projections from one eye only (the ipsilateral one) in the adult macaque’s striate cortex LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 93 From Wiesel’s Nobel Lecture Monocularly deprived at 2 weeks for 18 months 1,256 cells 100 cells ipsilateral Ocular dominance histograms (Rhesus macaque) LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 94 Ocular dominance histograms Right eye closed at 2 weeks for 18 months At 1 year for 1 year LOT Summer 2006 At 10 weeks for 4 months At 6 years for 1and 1/2 years Birth of a paradigm 95 The sooner, the worse, and no recovery 20-day-old monkey whose right eye had been closed since 8 days of age. LOT Summer 2006 Adult monkey whose right eye had been closed from 21 to 30 days of age. Tested after 4 years of normal vision Birth of a paradigm 96 A more recent validation “Innate mechanisms endow the visual system with highly specific connections, although the specificity is initially blurred by a high degree of exuberant growth”. Pascal D. Zufferey, Fuzi Jin, Hiroyuki Nakamura, Laurent Tettoni and Giorgio M. Innocenti European Journal of Neuroscience Volume 1, Page 2669 August 1999 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 97 A later generalization: The connective organization of an evolving neuronal network is related to the effects of the environment on this organization by stabilization or degeneration of labile synapses associated with functioning. Learning, or the acquisition of an associative property, is related to a characteristic variability of the connective organization: the interaction of the environment with the genetic program is printed as a particular pattern of such organization through neuronal functioning. A Theory of the Epigenesis of Neuronal Networks by Selective Stabilization of Synapses, by Jean-Pierre Changeux, Philippe Courrege and Antoine Danchin PNAS (1973) vol. 70; pp 2974-2978 LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 98 Another Nobel Prizewinner (but for a very different kind of work) The theory of neuronal group selection (Neural Darwinism) by Gerald Edelman (Basic Books, 1987) Focus on perceptual categorization as it relates to memory and learning. He proposes that these functions could be understood in terms of "neural Darwinism" — the idea that higher brain functions are mediated by developmental and somatic selection upon anatomical and functional variance occurring in each individual animal. LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 99 “Overflow” of the paradigm onto linguistics Essentially, detailed in my 1989 Cognition paper “Evolution, selection and cognition: From “learning” to parameter-setting in biology and in the study of language” In two parts, for electronic viability Downloadable in pdf from my web-page LOT Summer 2006 Birth of a paradigm 100