Download The english language - the WAC Clearinghouse

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Ukrainian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Untranslatability wikipedia , lookup

Arabic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Agglutination wikipedia , lookup

Old Norse morphology wikipedia , lookup

Compound (linguistics) wikipedia , lookup

Morphology (linguistics) wikipedia , lookup

Modern Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Kannada grammar wikipedia , lookup

Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup

Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Inflection wikipedia , lookup

Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup

English clause syntax wikipedia , lookup

Preposition and postposition wikipedia , lookup

Japanese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Lithuanian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Chinese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Pleonasm wikipedia , lookup

Romanian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup

Turkish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup

French grammar wikipedia , lookup

Russian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Esperanto grammar wikipedia , lookup

Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup

Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Malay grammar wikipedia , lookup

Pipil grammar wikipedia , lookup

English grammar wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
The English Language
Written in a clear style, it guides its readers on topics including basic assumptions about
language and discourse, pronunciation, word-formation strategies, parts of speech, clause
elements and patterns, how clauses may be combined into sentences, and how clauses
and sentences are modified to suit speakers’ and writers’ discourse purposes.
The English Language avoids presenting the language as set of arbitrary facts by grounding its conclusions in the analytic methods that have characterized the best grammatical
and linguistic practices for hundreds of years. Although its perspectives derive from
modern-traditional and generative grammar, its goal is to provide its readers with a broad
spectrum of basic knowledge about English. Its stance is rigorously descriptive, but the
object of its description is the standard variety of the language, thus making it an invaluable resource compatible with a wide range of purposes, including educated engagement
with the language issues that periodically convulse the media and educational institutions.
Each chapter contains a glossary of terms, a list of readings, and numerous exercises
(many using authentic texts).
Gerald P. Delahunty is Associate Professor of Linguistics and English and Assistant
Chair of the Colorado State University Department of English, where he teaches courses
on all aspects of linguistics and occasional courses on Irish literature. He has published
on syntactic theory, English syntax, sociolinguistics, and Irish archaeology.
James J. Garvey taught linguistics and literature courses in the English Department at
Colorado State University. He died tragically in 2006.
Perspectives on Writing
Series Editor, Mike Palmquist
The English Language
Grounded in linguistic research and argumentation, The English Language: From Sound
to Sense offers readers who have little or no analytic understanding of English a thorough
treatment of the various components of the language. Its goal is to help readers become
independent language analysts capable of critically evaluating claims about the language
and the people who use it.
Delahunty
Garvey
The
English
Language
From Sound
to Sense
The WAC Clearinghouse
http://wac.colostate.edu/
816 Robinson Street
West Lafayette, IN 47906
w w w.parlorpress.com
S A N: 2 5 4 - 8 8 7 9
ISBN 978-1-60235-180-6
WAC
Clearinghouse
Parlor
Press
Gerald P. Delahunty
James J. Garvey
PERSPECTIVES ON WRITING
Series Editor, Mike Palmquist
PERSPECTIVES ON WRITING
Series Editor, Mike Palmquist
The Perspectives on Writing series addresses writing studies in a broad sense.
Consistent with the wide ranging approaches characteristic of teaching
and scholarship in writing across the curriculum, the series presents works
that take divergent perspectives on working as a writer, teaching writing,
administering writing programs, and studying writing in its various forms.
The WAC Clearinghouse and Parlor Press are collaborating so that these
books will be widely available through free digital distribution and lowcost print editions. The publishers and the series editor are teachers and
researchers of writing, committed to the principle that knowledge should
freely circulate. We see the opportunities that new technologies have for
further democratizing knowledge. And we see that to share the power of
writing is to share the means for all to articulate their needs, interest, and
learning into the great experiment of literacy.
Existing Books in the Series
Charles Bazerman and David R. Russell, Writing Selves/Writing Societies (2003)
Charles Bazerman, Adair Bonini, and Débora Figueiredo (Eds.), Genre in a
Changing World (2009)
David Franke, Alex Reid and Anthony DiRenzo (Eds.), Design Discourse: Composing
and Revising the Professional and Technical Writing Curriculum (2010)
Gerald P. Delahunty and James Garvey, The English Language: from Sound to Sense
(2010)
The English Language
From Sound to Sense
Gerald P. Delahunty
James J. Garvey
The WAC Clearinghouse
wac.colostate.edu
Fort Collins, Colorado
Parlor Press
www.parlorpress.com
West Lafayette, Indiana
The WAC Clearinghouse, Fort Collins, Colorado 80523-1052
© 2010 Gerald P. Delahunty
Copyeditor, Designer: David Doran
Series Editor: Mike Palmquist
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Delahunty, Gerald Patrick.
The English language : from sound to sense / Gerald P. Delahunty, James J. Garvey.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-1-60235-180-6 (pbk. : alk. paper) -- ISBN 978-1-60235-181-3 (adobe ebook)
1. Linguistics. 2. Language and languages. 3. English language--Study and teaching.
I. Garvey, James J. II. Title.
P121.D384 2010
425--dc22
2010011194
The WAC Clearinghouse supports teachers of writing across the disciplines. Hosted by
Colorado State University, it brings together scholarly journals and book series as well as
resources for teachers who use writing in their courses. This book is available in digital
format for free download at http://wac.colostate.edu.
Parlor Press, LLC is an independent publisher of scholarly and trade titles in print and
multimedia formats. This book is available in paperback and Adobe eBook formats
from Parlor Press on the World Wide Web at http://www.parlorpress.com. For submission information or to find out about Parlor Press publications, write to Parlor Press,
816 Robinson St., West Lafayette, Indiana, 47906, or e-mail [email protected].
For Marna and Cian
To the memory of James J. Garvey
Contents
1 Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language �������������������������������� 3
key concepts��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������3
who these books are for������������������������������������������������������������������������������������3
how to use these books�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������3
what these books are about������������������������������������������������������������������������������3
communication ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������4
language��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������5
discourse ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������5
text����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������6
genre��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������6
ideology��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������7
language in education ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������7
thinking critically about language��������������������������������������������������������������10
standard english����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������11
grammar������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������12
other reasons for studying and teaching about language������������������������14
the organization of these books��������������������������������������������������������������������15
hints for success����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������16
references and resources��������������������������������������������������������������������������������18
glossary ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������19
2
Conceptions of Language and Grammar ���������������������������������������������� 23
the study of language ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������23
the roles of the english teacher������������������������������������������������������������������25
what is a language?������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������27
competence and performance ������������������������������������������������������������������������38
approaches to the study of language������������������������������������������������������������43
references and resources��������������������������������������������������������������������������������49
glossary ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������51
3 A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar���������������������������������������� 57
introduction����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������57
clauses ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������58
sentences����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������68
phrases ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������72
complementation and modification ��������������������������������������������������������������74
words ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������75
morphemes��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������76
parts of speech��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������77
vii
regular and irregular forms��������������������������������������������������������������������������79
grammatical categories ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������79
sounds and spelling ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������81
form, function, and meaning��������������������������������������������������������������������������82
references and resources��������������������������������������������������������������������������������87
glossary ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������87
4 Phonetics and Phonology ���������������������������������������������������������������������� 89
introduction����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������89
articulatory phonetics������������������������������������������������������������������������������������89
consonants��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������91
vowels����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������98
syllables and feet������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������105
phonology ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������107
phonemes ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������108
allophones������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������108
phonological rules����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������113
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������116
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������117
5 Morphology and Word Formation������������������������������������������������������121
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������121
words and morphemes������������������������������������������������������������������������������������122
morphemes, allomorphs, and morphs����������������������������������������������������������124
words ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������125
registers and words ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������137
the internal structure of complex words��������������������������������������������������138
classifying words by their morphological properties������������������������������140
references and resources ����������������������������������������������������������������������������141
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������141
appendix a: some english derivational morphemes�������������������������������������142
6 The Major Parts of Speech��������������������������������������������������������������������147
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������147
the major parts of speech: nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs��������������������148
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������184
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������185
Appendix to major parts of speech����������������������������������������������������������������187
references and resources for appendix ������������������������������������������������������192
7
viii
The Minor Parts of Speech ������������������������������������������������������������������193
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������193
pronouns ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������194
articles������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������208
auxiliary verbs������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������213
prepositions����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������216
intensifiers������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������221
conjunctions��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������222
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������230
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������230
8 Word Meaning��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������235
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������235
dictionary entries������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������236
mental dictionaries����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������261
concluding remarks��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������267
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������268
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������269
9
Phrases��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������273
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������273
what is a phrase? ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������274
modification and complementation ������������������������������������������������������������277
the adverb phrase (advp)�������������������������������������������������������������������������������279
the prepositional phrase (pp)������������������������������������������������������������������������282
the adjective phrase (ap) ������������������������������������������������������������������������������285
the noun phrase (np)��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������288
the verb phrase (vp)����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������306
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������318
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������319
10 Basic Clause Patterns����������������������������������������������������������������������������321
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������321
elements of the clause����������������������������������������������������������������������������������322
subjects������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������324
semantic roles������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������330
auxiliary verbs������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������336
tense and aspect ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������338
basic clause patterns��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������347
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������364
glossary��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 364
appendix: time and tense��������������������������������������������������������������������������������365
references and resources for appendix ������������������������������������������������������382
ix
11 Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns ����������������������������������������������383
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������383
movement��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������383
assignment of semantic roles������������������������������������������������������������������������388
deletion����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������397
insertion����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������402
passive��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������403
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������407
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������407
12 Multi-Clause Sentences ����������������������������������������������������������������������411
introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������411
multi-clause sentences����������������������������������������������������������������������������������411
recursion��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������412
complex sentences������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������413
finite vs. non-finite clauses��������������������������������������������������������������������������428
coordination��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������435
miscellaneous information-restructuring sentence types ����������������������436
concluding remarks��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������442
references and resources������������������������������������������������������������������������������442
glossary ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������442
Index������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������445
x
The English Language
From Sound to Sense
1 Introduction to the Linguistic Study of
Language
key concepts
Who these books are for
How to use these books
What these books are about
Communication
Language
Discourse
Text
Genre
Ideology
Language in education
Thinking critically about language
Standard English
Grammar
Other reasons for studying and teaching about language
The organization of these books
Hints for success
w h o t h e s e b o o k s a re f o r
This is the first of two books for teachers about the English language. We believe that all teachers, not just English teachers, share the responsibility for
helping students develop their abilities to speak, read, and write. Students
must learn to communicate appropriately about math, chemistry, history,
and every other school subject. Teaching students these skills necessarily extends across the curriculum. Thus, while one part of our intended audience
is English K-12 teachers, we have prepared this book and its companion
with teachers (and student teachers) from all disciplines in mind.
h ow t o u s e t h e s e b o o k s
In these books, we use certain typographical marks to help you focus on key
points. Important terms are bolded. You can find their definitions in the
text and in the glossary. Examples are noted in italics or are separated from
the text.
w h a t t h e s e b o o k s a re a b o u t
These books are about language, but specifically about the English language
3
Delahunty and Garvey
and its uses. The first book is about the grammar of English; the second is
about related topics, including language variation (e.g., dialects), language
learning, English spelling, and the history of the English language.
Generally, when people hear the word “grammar,” they immediately
think of “correct” or “incorrect” and “good” or “bad” language. Thinking
about language in this way is said to be prescriptive. English has a long
tradition of judging some expressions as “correct” and others as “incorrect.”
For example, expressions such as We was are viewed as “incorrect,” even
though a great many people use them. The “correct” version is said to be We
were.
Counter-posed to the prescriptive tradition is the descriptive one, which
developed in linguistics, anthropology, and sociology. This approach is
concerned with describing and understanding the linguistic behavior of a
community, without judging it. From a descriptive point of view, We was
is unobjectionable when used by a member of a community of speakers
who characteristically use this expression. However, it is unacceptable to the
wider English speaking community in, for example, formal speaking and
writing.
The point of view presented in these books is essentially descriptive.
However, except where the topic is explicitly about linguistic variation, we
describe the form of English used in relatively formal public speaking and
writing. We recognize that language changes, and that consequently even
the prescriptive rules have to change. We believe that these rules should be
descriptions of the best accepted practices of the day rather than impositions (often irrelevant) on the language and its use.
communication
Communication occurs when one person acts with the intention of influencing the mind of another, for example, by getting him/her to entertain
some idea, and when that other person recognizes the first person’s intention to influence his/her mind. Clearly, it is possible to influence another
person’s mind unintentionally; for instance, if I (unintentionally) sneeze,
I might prompt you to think that I might have a cold. However, this is a
rather different kind of event than one in which I intentionally sneeze and
you recognize that my sneeze was intentional. From my first (unintentional)
sneeze, you cannot infer that I am trying to get you to think I have a cold;
from my second (intentional) sneeze, you can infer that I am trying to get
you to think something or another, perhaps that I have a cold.
Imagine that we have gone to a party together and that we want to coordinate our leaving. So, before we get to the party I say to you, “I’ll pre4
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
tend to sneeze when I’m ready to go home,” and you agree to interpret my
sneeze in this way. When I sneeze at the party you can infer that I sneezed
intentionally and interpret my sneeze as indicating my desire to leave.
For this communication to succeed two elements must be in place:
first, the assumption that I intend to influence you in some way, and second, our agreement about the meaning of my intentional sneeze. There is
nothing in the nature of a sneeze that requires it to mean “Let’s go home.”
We could have agreed that it was to mean, “It’s safe to slip upstairs to steal
the host’s jewelry.” By specifying a meaning for a sneeze, we have created
a little code, a sort of miniscule language.
language
Fortunately, we cannot read each others’ minds. So, if we want to allow someone access to what we are thinking, we must provide them with clues that
they can perceive. Language is a system that connects thoughts, which can
not be heard, seen, or touched, with sounds, letters, manual signs, or tactile
symbols (e.g., Braille) which can. In this way, one person’s private ideas may
be communicated to another person. For example, imagine that I want to
communicate to you my idea that my study needs to be tidied up. You can’t
see, hear, touch, taste, or otherwise perceive that idea; it’s locked away in my
mind. To communicate it to you I have to cast it in a form that you can
perceive—typically in spoken, visual, or tactile form—that is systematically
connected to the idea, for example, the sentence, My study needs to be tidied
up. Without this perceivable expression, you cannot know that I have an idea
to communicate; without the systematic connection between the idea and the
form of the expression, you cannot know which idea I want to communicate.
So, language is a code that systematically connects private thoughts with public expressions. These books are about the systems we use to connect private
ideas to public activities.
Language has been a major topic of research for well over two centuries.
Linguistic research intersects with anthropology, biology, computer science,
history, human development, literature, philosophy, politics, psychology, as
well as reading and writing.
discourse
When we communicate we engage in discourse; that is, we deploy language
with the purpose of providing our audiences with clues about how we want
to influence them.
All discourse takes place in context; that is, the producer of a piece of
discourse (speaker/writer) purposefully deploys, at some time and in some
5
Delahunty and Garvey
place, clues about his or her intention which are to be interpreted by their
intended recipient(s) (audience). The clues have, generally, been selected
with that audience, in that time and place, and with those purposes in mind.
Some scholars argue that because different discourse situations require
different patterns of communicative practice, we must speak of discourses rather than of discourse (Gee 1992, 1996). We have, for instance, the
discourse in which we are currently engaged—the discourse of linguistics,
which differs from the discourse of literary study, which differs from the discourse of chemical engineering, which differs from the discourse of history,
and so on. A student who aims to be a practitioner in a field must master the
ways in which practitioners in that field communicate with each other about
topics in the field. Recognizing these specialized communicative practices has
given rise to the Writing Across the Curriculum (WAC) movement.
text
When people communicate, they produce texts. Texts always occur in
some medium, which may be auditory, visual, tactile, or some combination of these. Texts also always occur in some channel, that is, the environment through which the medium travels from the text’s producer(s) to its
receiver(s). For ordinary face-to-face conversation, the medium is the air,
which is set in motion by the producer and whose motions affect the ears
of the receiver(s). Communication by telephone involves at least two channels—the air between the speaker’s mouth and the phone, the mechanical
and electronic devices that connect the speaker’s and receiver’s phones, and
the air between the receiver’s phone and his/her ear. Texts may incorporate
non-linguistic elements such as pictures, diagrams, music, and the like.
g e n re
A genre is a communicative category. Genres differ from each other in participants, forms, and purposes. Texts come in genres; for example, a Shakespearean sonnet is a different type of text from a business letter, which is a different
type of text from a casual conversation.
Communicative acts come in genres, too. The sales pitch of a car salesman
differs from an end-of-term class presentation, which differs from texting a
party invitation to a friend.
The various discourses require their own specific genres. For example, the
discourse of creative writing in English includes the genres of the short story,
the novel, and poetry (which includes such sub-genres as the lyric and the
dramatic monologue). The discourse of business includes the annual report,
various kinds of advertisements, and business letters.
6
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
ideology
Many scholars stress the power of discourse and language to influence speakers’ perceptions and conceptualizations of their worlds, and to create and
maintain the structures of their societies. Educators interested in language
emphasize its power to create and maintain ideologies, i.e., beliefs about the
ways in which goods are distributed in society. Goods are “anything that the
people in the society generally believe are beneficial to have or harmful not
to have, whether this be life, space, time, ‘good’ schools, ‘good’ jobs, wealth,
status, power, control, or whatever” (Gee, 1996: 21).
language in education
Language is central to education: it is the means by which educational content is communicated; it is an object of study; it is an object of beliefs that
are important in education; it is a key element of students’ identities; it poses
potential problems in education, largely because of the beliefs we have about
it; and it is a valuable resource for those who know how to make use of it.
Language is a means of education in that it is the primary medium of
communication between students and teachers and between students and
textbooks.
Language is an object of education because it is the material out of which
texts are woven, and because language itself is the object of study in writing and speaking courses. We focus on language as we learn to edit our
essays and speeches. We develop our vocabularies and learn the meanings,
uses, and conventional spellings of words. We learn to control the genres
required for various disciplines and the specific characteristics expected in
those genres, for example, personal essays, academic papers of various sorts,
business letters, reports, and magazine articles. Language is also an object
of study in so far as we develop our skills in using it to communicate, to
acquire knowledge from lectures and books, to integrate new information
with old, to replace false beliefs with new true ones, and to increase or decrease our estimates of the likelihood that some belief we hold is true.
It is important to note here that students who are learning English as a second language labor under a double burden, because English is simultaneously
both the means and an object of their education.
Exercise
When asked what she thought was the most important aspect of learning English as a second language, a Japanese student replied: “Knowing
7
Delahunty and Garvey
many vocabularies.” What do you think she meant? Is her expression an
acceptable piece of English? How would you change it so it retains her
apparent meaning and is acceptable? Why would you make that particular change? Is (your understanding of) her assertion true?
Language is also an object of our beliefs. Many people believe that some
forms of English are good and others bad; that some languages are beautiful
and others ugly; that some languages are limited in what they can express
when compared to languages such as English; that people who speak certain
varieties are uneducated, perhaps stupid, and unworthy of certain types of
work. Beliefs like these constitute ideologies about language. Some ideologies are liberating and others quite oppressive. Whether liberating or oppressive, they must become objects of critical awareness for teachers and of
critical discussion for students (Kress 1985; Fairclough 1989, 1992).
Language also represents one of the key elements of our students’ social,
cultural, and personal identities. Writing explores values our students may
not be able to explore otherwise. As their writing improves, the range and
sophistication of these identities increases.
Teachers have potentially powerful effects on students’ lives. Our response to our students’ language will influence their attitudes. Young children have a fascination with language and almost no inhibitions about
it. Adults, in contrast, typically display considerable anxiety about their
language. They often have “strongly negative attitudes towards their native
speech pattern” (Labov 1972: 117). This anxiety is known as linguistic insecurity. This insecurity does not develop naturally; it is the consequence
of repeated experiences in which their native speech patterns are disparaged, often by teachers (who should know better). This problem is particularly acute for students who are not native speakers of English, or who
do not speak the variety of the language regarded as “correct.”
Exercise
1. How do you feel about your ability as a singer? Would you be willing to sing Madonna’s “Love Profusion” in front of your class? (It’s on
her American Life album, if you want to practice beforehand.) What
experiences with singing have formed your attitude? What attitudes
about singing do children have? What light does this shed on linguistic
insecurity?
8
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
2. How many words do you have in your vocabulary? Consider first your
active vocabulary, i.e., words you use regularly in speaking and writing, such as often. Then estimate your passive vocabulary, i.e., words
that you recognize and understand, but which don’t come readily to
mind when you want them, for example, prestidigitation. Estimates
based on objective study appear at the end of this chapter.
Language is a potential problem to the extent that it—or our beliefs about
it—impedes students’ learning. If we believe that students who speak English
with a Latino accent, or who speak Black English (a.k.a. “Ebonics”), will be
unable to keep up in our classes, then very likely they will not, because teachers’ expectations strongly affect students’ success in school. Because teachers
respond to students’ language on many levels, they must develop a critical
awareness of their own linguistic preferences, prejudices, and beliefs—everyone has these beliefs, even linguists. They must also be able to critically
evaluate textbooks, dictionaries, style manuals, computerized style analyzers,
and newspaper articles on language, because these also embody assumptions
about language, many of them just plain wrong, often destructively so.
Language is a potential resource for teaching critical thinking. We can evaluate our attitudes about other languages and other dialects and their speakers; we can collect linguistic data, observe its patterns, and articulate those
patterns as hypotheses which we can then test; we can evaluate the ways we
talk about language for their precision, and come to appreciate the value of
precision in language use generally. Language data for analysis is very readily
available. Students can collect their own data from bumper stickers, license
plates, ads, poems—whatever. Schools (or the internet) can provide computerized collections of authentic spoken and written texts (corpora) along with
computer programs to analyze them (concordancers). Because the linguistic
study of language is fundamentally scientific, studying language in this way
can provide us and our students with an understanding and appreciation of
scientific methods.
Exercise
1. Write a brief essay on at least two of the ways in which language is
an element in education.
2. In your college library, consult the journals Linguistics and Literature,
9
Delahunty and Garvey
Style, and Linguistics and Education. Report back to the class on (a) the
types of topics covered in each journal and (b) one article that interested you.
3. What do you understand by the term “grammar”?
thinking critically about language
Clearly, teachers must know about reading and writing, as well as about
teaching their disciplines. But why should they learn about language? One
answer is that teachers should have a well-developed critical understanding of at least some modern thinking about the nature of language and its
roles in education because reading, writing, and all subject matters crucially
depend on language. Good craftspeople always understand their materials,
and as language is the raw material of the discourses of all disciplines, teachers should understand its nature.
Second, all modern approaches to reading and writing—cultural, feminist, Marxist, post-modernist, psychological—accord language a central place.
Third, because the linguistic study of language is quite different in its approaches, goals, and methods from the approaches to the study of reading or
writing, it complements those approaches. Fourth, societal attitudes to language (teachers’, students’, and parents’) can profoundly affect students’ learning and performance.
One of our goals is to enable you to think critically about language and
the claims of those who write about it (including ours). Critical thinking has
many facets, including creating and evaluating arguments, reasoning from
premises to conclusions, and detecting covert claims in arguments. In language study, we think critically when we determine whether a grammar, style
manual, or dictionary is appropriate for our students, or whether a linguistic
claim (e.g., “double negatives make a positive”) has any validity.
Exercise
Is it valid to say that double negatives make a positive in English? What
evidence can you muster for your decision? How valid is your evidence?
Critical thinking is important in any discipline, but it is of particular importance in reading and writing. To be able to read in any discipline, students
10
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
must know how to accurately interpret the language of texts in that discipline
and to be able to recreate their authors’ meanings. Both of these tasks require,
at a minimum, knowing the discipline’s technical terms. Some disciplines may
require readers to be knowledgeable about further aspects of the language.
Literature students, for instance, must be able to understand language made
difficult by archaisms, rhetorical figures, complex grammar, and willful grammatical and semantic violations (Dillon, 1978).
When writing, students think critically when they analyze their personal
preconceptions and biases, when they assess the relevance and effectiveness
of their ideas, and when they decide on the best linguistic formulation of
those ideas for their intended audiences.
The ability to think critically about language is particularly needed now,
because the school grammar tradition has generally become quite uninformed about research into current English discourse practices. The responsibility for this situation lies partly with linguists themselves. We have not
been successful in our efforts to educate the public about language. However, the greatest share of the responsibility lies with institutions, journalists,
and teachers who have vigorously defended an ultra-conservative status quo,
who know little if anything about language, and who often misconstrue
what linguists have to say about it. Many believe, for instance, that linguists
claim that “anything goes in English these days.” Nothing could be farther
from the truth, as we will show in our chapter on Conceptions of Language.
s t a n d a rd e n g l i s h
Learning to read and write is partly a matter of linguistic development, i.e.,
the growth in a student’s ability to communicate appropriately in an increasingly broad range of circumstances. Teachers who concern themselves
with the linguistic development of their students typically view their role
as twofold: (a) to promote their students’ ability to speak, read, and write
in their disciplines, and (b) to develop their students’ ability to write in
Standard English (SE), the variety of English generally expected in formal
communication in various disciplines.
Exercise
1. Where around the world is English spoken? In what kinds of circumstances? For what kinds of purposes? Make lists from your own general
knowledge before you consult sources such as Bernard Comrie’s The
World’s Major Languages; David Crystal’s Cambridge Encyclopedia of
Language; Peter Trudgill and Jean Hannah’s International English: A
11
Delahunty and Garvey
Guide to the Varieties of Standard English; and the Summer Institute
of Linguistics (SIL) website at http://www.ethnologue.com/show_language.asp?code=ENG (SIL is a Christian Bible translation organization.)
2. Why are things standardized? What would the consequences be if
electrical outlets were not standardized throughout the US?
3. Consider the expressions We was and We were. Which is Standard
English and which is not? How do you think that one became standard
while the other did not? What do YOU think about expressions such as
I ain’t never been there, We was waiting for the ambulance, and the
speakers who use them? Be honest.
4. Select a technical expression (from any discipline) that you believe
all of your students should know and know how to use properly. Paraphrase that expression in non-technical English. Do the technical expression and its non-technical paraphrase have exactly the same meanings?
grammar
You probably answered exercise 3 on page 10 by saying that “grammar” tells
us which expressions are correct. You would, of course, have meant “prescriptive grammar.” However, linguists add at least two other interpretations to the
word. First, they use it to refer to the knowledge that a speaker or writer of
a language must have in order to be able to use the language at all. Second,
they use it to refer to any attempt to describe that knowledge. We will return
to these issues in the next chapter when we discuss prescriptive and descriptive approaches to language study more thoroughly. It is important, when
we speak about “grammar,” that we are clear, to ourselves and our audiences,
which meaning of “grammar” we intend.
This first book is about the grammar of English. Some of our readers will
be required to teach grammar classes per se; others will use information about
English grammar while teaching composition; and still others will use it while
teaching writing-intensive classes across the curriculum. It is important to
note that grammar refers only to a part of language, and that these books deal
with language, not just grammar. We believe that a teacher’s knowledge of
language is far more broadly relevant than just knowledge of “grammar.”
It is also important to recognize that teaching “grammar” is highly controversial. To get a sense of the arguments, we recommend that you read
12
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
the relevant articles in English Journal 1996: 85.7 and 2003: 92.3, as well
as other NCTE publications such as Grammar Alive: A Guide for Teachers
(Haussamen et al 2003) and Code Switching: Teaching Standard English in
Urban Classrooms (Wheeler and Swords 2006). You might also browse Freeman and Freeman (2004) and Honegger (2005). Neither you nor we can
predict what you will believe about language, grammar, and the teaching
of either by the time you have read these books. However, we do know
that in discussions about how to teach writing, you will hear arguments
that teaching grammar “out of context” does not improve students’ writing.
(Generally what is meant by “grammar” in those discussions is the set of prescriptive conventions for speaking and writing Standard English.) Certainly
there is a large body of research going back more than a century purporting
to support this position. However, we repeat, these books are not just about
grammar; they are about language, including how grammar fits into language. It is as important for teachers to know about language as it is to know
about their subject matter. A teacher who knows nothing about language is
a cyclist without wheels. Worse, a teacher who knows nothing about language is a chemist who knows (and cares) nothing about the environmental
consequences of the substances he or she creates.
Our approach to the study of language is heavily influenced by the results
of recent linguistic research and methods. This allows us to tie our discussion
to critical thinking, literature, Writing Across the Curriculum, and composition studies, as well as to philosophy and the social, psychological, neurological, and computer sciences (see Traugott and Pratt 1980 as well as journals like
English Journal and Style).
Most of this book deals with English grammar. Aside from the fact that
the general public expects teachers to have a mastery of grammar (by which is
usually meant prescriptive grammar), you will probably be expected to teach
the subject in one way or another. We do not suggest that you use this book
as a syllabus. It contains too much material and is not geared to a junior or
senior high-school audience. Nonetheless, in spite of the amount of material
it covers, it’s merely a good basis for continuing your study of language. We
hope that you will find the analytic and critical methods of exploring language
used in the books to be more productive and interesting than the more conventional handbook approach—exposition plus drill-and-practice.
More importantly, we hope that you will present to your students the
broader conceptions about language that are expressed in these books.
These conceptions are presented initially in our chapter on Conceptions
of Language, but are developed in various ways in other chapters.
13
Delahunty and Garvey
o t h e r re a sons for studying an d t e a c h i n g a b o u t
language
Besides its importance in the development of critical thinking skills, there
are many other reasons for studying language. You might want to know
about language variation (“dialects” of various sorts), about how languages
change over time, about the history of English, about the standardization
of languages, about how languages are learned, about language disorders,
about the relationships between language and culture or society, or about
how computers are programmed to understand or produce language. These
are all to one degree or another relevant to teachers and we deal with many
of them in these books.
Deciding what should be included in books like these is remarkably difficult. We have followed the guidelines of the National Council of Teachers
of English (NCTE) and the National Council for Accreditation for Teacher
Education (NCATE) about what English teachers should know, and we
depended on the research on Writing Across the Curriculum (WAC). Nonetheless, because such a huge amount is known about language generally,
and about English in particular, and because (as in any area of vigorous
intellectual activity) there are many competing approaches to these topics, it
would be impossible to synopsize them all here. In the first book, we present a grammar of English which addresses traditional topics and concerns,
but which is influenced considerably by current grammatical and discourse
research. In the second book we present a range of topics that we hope will
be of interest and value to teachers across the disciplines.
Fulfilling the goals of instruction becomes particularly important in a world
growing in technological complexity, social diversity, and multiple “Englishes.” (See the essays in Kachru 1992 and Kachru and Nelson 1996, as well
as Crystal 2003; Jenkins 2003; McArthur 1998; Melchers and Shaw 2003.)
Many students are passionate about their studies in literature, the physical
and social sciences, business, or in other intellectual pursuits; unfortunately,
however, many students and teachers see the study of language as merely the
study of “correct grammar.” We have already begun to sift through the various
meanings of “grammar” and will develop this discussion in later chapters.
Teachers face a complex set of responsibilities. Parents, boards of education, and legislators look increasingly to school systems to prepare students
for the demands of the future. Worries that American students lag behind
those of other developed countries translate directly into concerns about public funding (i.e., taxes) and accountability in education, as the No Child Left
Behind Act requires. These pressures appear in the form of demands for success on standardized tests, for “getting back to basics,” for public funding of
14
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
private education (“vouchers” and “charter schools”), for longer school years,
for ongoing competency testing for teachers, and for the assessment and ranking of schools.
From our perspective, teachers’ responsibility is to their students. We
must meet the needs of the learner rather than simply present material on
a take-it-or-leave-it basis. As a result, the learnability of classroom material becomes more important than its teachability (how easily it can be
taught), or its assessability (how easily it can be assessed).
Moreover, in the coming generation, the diversity of its students in US
classrooms will change dramatically. The 2008 US Census Bureau projected
percentages for the major racial/ethnic groups are displayed in the following
chart:
White
Hispanic
Black
Asian
2008
66
15
14
5.1
2050
46
30
15
9.2
(See U.S. Census Bureau National Population Projections.) In 2005 the
percentage of the US population born abroad was 12.4.
These projections suggest that in addition to knowing their disciplines
and how to teach them (ideally by incorporating lots of writing), teachers
will also have to know about how to teach ESL (English as a Second Language).
In the rest of this introduction we will explain the organization of these
books and provide some hints for working successfully with them.
the organization of these books
The next chapter of this book (Conceptions of Language) is on the nature
of language. The remaining chapters are on various aspects of the English
language, including its sound system, its vocabulary, its parts of speech,
word meanings, and the ways in which words are combined into phrases,
clauses, and sentences—essentially the grammar of English.
Book I gives you some basic information about English grammar, about
how to do simple linguistic analyses, and about thinking critically about
language. Because it is impossible to remember the analysis of every expression you might be asked about by a student (there are far too many), our
main concern is to help you become independent by providing you with the
means to do linguistic analysis as you need it.
15
Delahunty and Garvey
We regularly use an analytic method, in which we formulate criteria for
determining how to categorize words, phrases, or sentences. For example,
whenever we want to know the part of speech of a particular word, we use
these criteria to test a hypothesis about the word’s part of speech. We will
ask you to do similar activities in the exercises, sometimes by gathering data,
sometimes by analyzing material that lies just a step beyond what is covered
in the text.
Book II deals with selected topics of particular importance to teachers: spoken and written language; spelling; variation in language; usage; punctuation;
history of the English language; and language acquisition. Our presentation
brings together current studies in each of these areas and prepares you to read
applied studies that you will encounter in your career. These chapters can be
read independently of Book I, though on occasion you will find some crossreferences to chapters in Book I, along with some phonetic notation that may
send you back to our chapter on Phonetics and Phonology.
In many instances we will mention a topic, briefly discuss it, and return to
it in greater depth later. Our hope is that this cycling will provide you with an
opportunity to get an initial familiarity with a topic and then build on that
familiarity later.
These books are far from covering the wealth of information on topics that
you might be interested in as a teacher. To survey all of these would require
several more books. We encourage you to consult your instructor for further
references and bibliographical resources, for example on linguistics and literature, composition, or reading.
hints for success
The study of the English language is demanding. First, you will find yourself
confronting challenges to linguistic assumptions (and even prejudices) that
have become ingrained in you through your education and that are widely
accepted without critical examination by the majority of educated English
speakers. Second, you may find yourself in a mode of analysis quite different
from that of your own studies. Third, you will confront considerable linguistic detail and the large number of new terms required to conceptualize
and describe it. While we cannot guarantee that these new ideas will be easy
to master (although many people do find them so), we believe that they are
worth your effort and will serve you well in your career.
Because what you will learn in these pages is as much skill as information, do not expect to master this material in a single reading. The best strategy
for most people is to do a preliminary reading, do the exercises, and then reread (and reread again). Research on learning and remembering shows what
16
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
is perhaps obvious—the more frequently and deeply you review material
the better you will remember and understand it. If you are a student, attend
class and ask questions; if you are having difficulties, the chances are good
that many of your classmates are too and will benefit from the instructor’s
answers to your questions.
Exercises are scattered throughout the book. We encourage you to tackle
as many as possible. This need not involve working them out in full detail
(unless your instructor requires it); you might just work out the outline of
a solution. However, you cannot learn the analytic skills required to study
linguistics or grammar without doing lots of hands-on work. If you have
difficulties with a problem, try to identify them as specifically as possible.
If you get an incorrect answer, make sure also to get an explanation of the
correct one. Try to retrace the thinking that led you to miss the question.
Sometimes you can learn more from mistakes than from perfection. One of
our goals is to help you develop your skills in independent language analysis.
Teachers are regularly called upon to answer questions whose answers cannot be found in textbooks or reference works. Doing exercises is essential for
independence.
Terminology is plentiful in linguistics, just as in every other discipline.
Remember that technical terms usually have specific meanings so you may
not be able to substitute ordinary words for them. We have provided glossaries to help you identify definitions; you should consult them often. Be
particularly careful with terms (e.g., semantics) that may have a familiar
meaning in ordinary language, but a significantly different one in technical
usage.
Definitions should be supplemented with explanations, elaborations of
the minimal statements in the glossary. Remember, though, to use precise
language in defining terms. Linguists like to think of themselves as scientists
and so value precision and accuracy. Explanations may be taken from the
text (or from class notes, if you are a student). Try to have a specific, prototypical example of each term. Select an example that is clear to you and is
uncontroversial, and be sure to understand just why your example exemplifies the concept.
Memorization has had a bad press, but it is necessary more often than we
think (and not only for exams). We do not encourage memorization for its
own sake, but rather to make your passive knowledge more active. The best
time to memorize is after you have become familiar with a concept through
exposure. For long lists (e.g., the prepositions of English), do not try to remember every item; select a small number, and then only to illustrate a concept, and use the criteria given in the book for deciding which other items
17
Delahunty and Garvey
belong to the list. As you gain experience, try to add gradually to that short
list. We do not recommend that you require your students to learn lists of
items, unless they are learning English as a second or foreign language (and
even then only sparingly).
Second opinions aren’t just good for your health. Reading other authors on
the topics of these books will greatly help you to learn and remember what
you study. We encourage you to seek out other books on these topics, beginning with those we’ve listed in the References and Resources at the end of each
chapter.
At the head of each chapter, we list the chapter’s key concepts. For teachers,
we hope these will help you find topics you want to read about. For students,
we imagine these topics serving as the focus for essays that might form a part
of your course work. The internal parts of the chapters are clearly indicated by
headings to allow for easy access.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Aitchison, Jean. 1994. Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental
Lexicon. (2nd ed.) Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
Comrie, Bernard. (ed.) 1990. The World’s Major Languages. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Crystal, David. 1987. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____ 1995. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the English Language. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____ 2003. English as a Global Language. (2nd ed.) Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Dillon, George. 1978. Language Processing and the Reading of Literature.
Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Fairclough, Norman. 1989. Language and Power. London: Longman.
_____ 1992. Critical Language Awareness. London: Longman.
Freeman, David E. and Yvonne E. Freeman. 2004. Essential Linguistics: What
you Need to Know to Teach Reading, ESL, Spelling, Phonics, Grammar.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Gee, James Paul. 1992. The Social Mind: Language, Ideology, and Social
Practice. New York: Bergin and Garvey.
_____ 1996. Social Linguistics and Literacies. London UK: Taylor and Francis.
Haussamen, Brock, Amy Benjamin, Martha Kolln, and Rebecca S. Wheeler.
2003. Grammar Alive! A Guide for Teachers. Urbana, IL: NCTE.
Honegger, Mark. 2005. English Grammar for Writing. Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
18
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
Jenkins, Jennifer. 2003. World Englishes: A Resource Book for Students. London,
UK: Routledge.
Kachru, Braj B. (ed.) 1992. The Other Tongue: English Across Cultures. (2nd
ed.) Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Kachru, Braj B. and Cecil L. Nelson. 1996: 71-102. World Englishes. In
Sandra L. McKay and N. H. Hornberger. (eds.) Sociolinguistics and
Language Teaching. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kress, Gunther. 1985. Linguistic Processes in Sociocultural Practice. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
McArthur, Tom. 1998. The English Languages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
_____ (ed.) 1992. The Oxford Companion to the English Language. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
Melchers, Gunnel, and Philip Shaw. 2003. World Englishes. London, UK:
Arnold.
Traugott, Elizabeth, and Mary Louise Pratt. 1980. Linguistics for Students of
Literature. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
Trudgill, Peter and Jean Hannah. 1994. International English: A Guide to the
Varieties of Standard English. London: Arnold.
U.S. Census Bureau National Population Projections website: http://www.
census.gov/population/www/projections/
Wheeler, Rebecca S. and Rachel Swords. 2006. Code Switching: Teaching
Standard English in Urban Classrooms. Urbana, IL: NCTE.
How many words are in your vocabulary? According to Aitchison (1994:
6), “the average college student turned out to know approximately 58,000
common ‘basic words,’ 1,700 rare ‘basic words,’ and 96,000 derivatives and
compounds. The total comes to over 150,000. The highest student score
was almost 200,000, while even the lowest was over 100,000.” Are you surprised by these numbers? Other scholars suggest even higher ones.
glossary
active vocabulary:
and writing.
those words that we have ready access to for speaking
assessability:
the ease or difficulty with which knowledge can be assessed.
the environment through which the communicative medium
travels from the text’s producer to its receiver(s).
channel:
19
Delahunty and Garvey
communication:
activities by which one person intends to influence the
mind of another person.
concordancer: a computer program that allows you to search through
computerized collections of linguistic data for specified expressions along
with some of their context and to perform statistical operations on the data.
corpus/corpora: collection(s) of linguistic data, spoken or written, which
may or may not be computerized.
critical thinking: the process of evaluating the validity of assertions and
arguments.
descriptive grammar: any attempt to describe the linguistic knowledge and
behavior of individuals or communities without judging or evaluating them
as “correct/incorrect” or “good/bad.”
discourse: communicative activities, typically involving language, in particular contexts, whose purpose is to provide audiences with clues about
how we want to influence them.
genre: communicative categories differing from each other in participants,
forms, and purposes.
grammar: the word has several meanings. (1) conventions that judge which
of several expressions belongs to Standard English (see prescriptive grammar); (2) the knowledge that a speaker or writer of a language must have
in order to be able to use that language at all (see descriptive grammar);
(3) any attempt to describe that knowledge; (4) publications in which the
prescriptions and descriptions are expressed (e.g., a Spanish/English/etc.
grammar).
hypothesis: a prediction derived from a theory that may be tested to see if it
is true or false. If it is true, the theory is strengthened; if it is false the theory
is weakened, perhaps disproved.
ideology: “a social theory which involves generalizations (beliefs, claims)
about the way(s) in which goods are distributed in society... By ‘goods’ I
mean anything that the people in the society generally believe are beneficial
to have or harmful not to have, whether this be life, space, time, ‘good’
schools, ‘good’ jobs, wealth, status, power, control, or whatever. By ‘society’
I mean any and all groupings of people who share beliefs about what counts
as ‘goods’ (and since probably all humans share some of these, all humanity
counts as one sort of society). In this sense we all belong to many societies.”
(Gee, 1996: 21)
language: a system that connects private thoughts with public symbols.
learnability: the ease with which material can be learned by students.
linguistic insecurity: the feeling or belief that one’s language is in some
way deficient, for example that one’s accent is not as good as other accents.
20
Introduction to the Linguistic Study of Language
medium:
the sense(s) involved in communication—hearing, seeing, touching, or some combination of these.
ordinary language philosophy: an early to mid twentieth-century philosophy concerned with the meanings and uses of language.
passive vocabulary: those words whose meanings we recognize when we
hear them spoken or see them written but cannot easily bring to mind in
speaking or writing.
pragmatics: the study of contextually situated meanings.
prescriptive grammar: the set of conventions that define the standard variety of a language; generally couched in evaluative and judgmental terms
such as “correct/incorrect” and “good/bad.”
semantics: (the study of ) the literal meanings of linguistic expressions.
standard english: the variety of English expected in formal writing and
speaking, which is codified in dictionaries and style manuals, and taught in
composition classes.
teachability: the ease with which material can be organized for presentation in classrooms.
text: the auditory, visual, and/or tactile artifacts produced by communicators.
21
2 Conceptions of Language and Grammar
key concepts
The study of language
The roles of the English teacher
What is a language?
Competence and performance
Approaches to the study of language
the study of language
The study of spoken and written language occupies a significant part of
contemporary primary and secondary school and university curricula. The
grammars, handbooks of style, and composition texts used in these curricula are based on various assumptions about language and about why it
should be studied. It is important that teachers have a critical understanding
of these assumptions, which in many instances are either indirectly stated or
omitted entirely. These books are designed to help you to:
•
•
•
develop the critical resources you need as a teacher to respond to
many language-related issues;
understand the many concepts needed to talk appropriately and
accurately about language;
develop skills that you will use in everyday teaching of language,
literature, reading, and writing.
In the pages to follow you will encounter ideas about language that may be
new to you and which may contradict ideas you’ve been taught. We cannot
guarantee that these new concepts will be easy to master, but we do believe
that they are worth your best efforts. We will, as we said earlier, try to begin
with what you know about language. For example, you have probably been
taught to avoid non-standard expressions such as seen or seed instead of saw,
to avoid multiple nouns as modifiers, to make sure that your subjects and
verbs agree, to use parallel structures where possible, and the like. These are
usage rules. They have at least two jobs to do. First, they help define the
standard variety of English—recall our question in our introductory chapter
that asked you to consider why anything, e.g., electrical outlets, might be
standardized. You probably answered by saying that standardization allows
the greatest number of people to use it for the greatest number of purposes.
You might also have added that if something is standardized, then it can be
maintained in that form for a long period of time. Standardizing a language
23
Delahunty and Garvey
has the same goals: to allow as many people as possible to communicate effectively with each other, and to allow people at any time to read texts that
were written perhaps hundreds of years before they were born, much as we
read the novels of Jane Austen now. And standardization allows us to write
texts that will be understood by many generations to come.
The usage rules help ensure that standard English is used in formal writing and speaking so as to make our writings and speeches clear, efficient,
and effective, given our purposes in communicating and the characteristics
of our audiences. Rules that tell us which forms to choose (saw not seen
or seed as past tense of see), or what syntactic patterns to avoid (multiple
noun modifiers), or to use (parallel structures) are prescriptive. Ideally they
prescribe what are taken to be the most generally used formal writing and
speaking practices at a particular time.
Usage rules are extremely important. Speakers and writers who violate
them are likely to be judged harshly. It is a major part of any teacher’s job to
ensure that students can write in accordance with these rules. They can be
found in composition textbooks, which often devote entire sections to them;
they can also be found in writers’ handbooks of usage rules, in usage dictionaries, or in selected entries in desk dictionaries. Unfortunately, these handbooks
do not always agree with each other and do not always keep up with the accepted writing practices in important genres. Moreover, the conventions differ
from one discipline to another.
However, for teachers to be able to teach the usage rules, they must understand the concepts that underlie them and the terminology in which
they are expressed. For example, they must know what nouns are, be able
to recognize them in texts and to produce examples of them on demand;
what “past tense” means and how it is formed; what “agreement” means and
how it is expressed; which structures are parallel and which are not; and
what participles are so that they will be able to recognize them when they
“dangle,” or to teach them in order to expand the range of structures their
students can use in their writing. And they must be aware of current usage
controversies.
You may know about some of these things. For example, you may know
about the traditional parts of speech, about subjects and predicates, about
direct and indirect objects. In this book we will develop all these and related ideas by making use of the findings of modern linguistic and discourse
studies. Our point of view will be descriptive rather than prescriptive. That
is, rather than prescribing how someone thinks the language should be, we
will attempt to describe as objectively as we can as much of modern standard English as space allows. Our descriptive stance is that of linguistics in
24
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
general, which tends to think of itself as scientific. We include a chapter on
Usage in Book II.
Exercise
Many people think of dictionaries as the final arbiters of usage issues,
particularly regarding words. Read the front matter (i.e., all the text
before the list of words) of your dictionary and find out how its editors
view usage issues. Then look up some words whose usage is controversial, such as hopefully as a sentence adverb, e.g., Hopefully, a solution
will be found for the problems in the Middle East; unique as a gradable
adjective, e.g., His writing style is very unique; demagogue as a verb,
e.g., He demagogued his way into the White House; and lifestyle to
mean culture, e.g., The San people of Southwest Africa enjoy a hunter/gatherer lifestyle. How does your dictionary treat these controversies? Is the treatment consistent with the editors’ front matter claims?
When was your dictionary published? Do you think that the publication
date might have an effect on these controversies? Our Usage chapter
explores these issues in more detail.
NOTE: For a fascinating story about the OED, you might read Simon
Winchester’s The Professor and the Madman: A Tale of Murder, Insanity,
and the Making of the Oxford English Dictionary. For an excellent history of the development of the dictionary see Winchester’s The Meaning of Everything: The Story of the Oxford English Dictionary.
t h e ro l e s o f t h e e n g l i s h t e a c h e r
Standard English
We recognize that teachers are caught between apparently irreconcilable
forces. They must ensure that their students master the forms of English
that are regarded as acceptable, correct, educated, and expected in formal
communication, i.e., as standard. However, educational linguistic research
demonstrates that students will not learn the conventions of standard English unless teachers respect their native ethnic, regional, and social varieties.
So how might this impasse be resolved?
First, we must know what is and what is not currently acceptable. Second, we must have a framework of concepts and terminology that will allow
us to understand and teach about language. Third, we should adopt the be25
Delahunty and Garvey
lief that our only legitimate role is to add control of standard English to our
students’ linguistic repertoire, not to eliminate our students’ native varieties
on such unsupportable grounds as that they indicate laziness or stupidity.
They don’t! These books are designed to help teachers fulfill these roles.
In addition, teachers should make use of their students’ natural language
learning abilities and what is known from fields such as linguistics and applied
linguistics about teaching language. For example, rather than overwhelming
students by red-lining every error, teachers should select those “errors” which
seem amenable to correction at the time and bring the students’ attention
to the similarities and differences between their own practices and the target
ones. They should then focus on the target until it is well controlled. (See the
work of Rebecca S. Wheeler and her collaborators, e.g., Wheeler and Swords
2004: 470-480; Wheeler 2005: 108-112.)
Linguistic variation and bilingualism
All languages vary. That is, there is no language whose speakers all speak in
the same way in all circumstances. Groups of people may speak differently
from each other and still be speaking the same language; that is, a language
may exhibit dialect variation. A simple demonstration of this is to conduct an
informal survey about the words people use for soft drinks, such as soda, pop,
and the like, and then identify where in the country the various expressions
are used. Languages vary by nation, region, ethnicity, gender, age, and almost
every other grouping of people that one can imagine.
Languages also vary according to their uses. An individual speaker will vary
his or her style of speech according to contextual factors such as the formality
of the occasion. For example, on relatively informal occasions we are likely
to use abbreviations such as can’t and should’ve in our speech and writing; on
more formal occasions we will use the unabbreviated forms cannot and should
have.
The mode or channel by which language is transmitted can affect it also.
The language of a personal phone call differs from that of a face-to-face conversation and from a radio or TV call-in program. Spoken language differs
from written language, though in rather complex ways (Biber et al. 2002).
Occupations may have their own special varieties of a language, that is,
they differ in register. For example, the technical terms you know or will
learn about linguistics and grammar belong to the linguistics register, whereas
corner kick and throw-in belong to the soccer register.
In addition, individuals and groups make use of various genres or text
types. These are extended stretches of language, written or spoken, which
have relatively stable and identifiable characteristics. Genre is a well-estab26
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
lished notion in literature; it refers to novels, shorts stories, poems, and such
sub-genres as sonnets and lyrics. More generally, text types include such categories as business letters, term papers, newspaper reports, opinion pieces, and
many others, which are characterized by their content, their purposes, their
textual structure, their form of argumentation, and level of formality (Crystal
2003: 200-1). These are often divided into descriptive texts, which have to do
with the location of entities in space; narrative texts, which have to do with
situations and events in time; directive texts, which are concerned with future
activity; expository texts, which explain phenomena; and argumentative texts,
which attempt to confirm or change the beliefs of their readers (Gramley and
Pätzold 2004: 152-5).
Most communities and many individuals around the world are bi- or
multi-lingual; that is, they make use of more than one language. People in
the United States make use of many languages. Some languages, like Navajo
and Hawaiian, are native to the US; others, like Spanish, French, German,
and English, are longtime residents but were brought by colonists; and still
others, such as Thai and Hmong, were brought by recent immigrants.
In all communities, some varieties and languages are favored and others
denigrated. Children whose native language is not respected in the community or the school are at great risk of failing in school. Because language is
such an important component, not just of education, but of an individual’s
personal, ethnic, and social identities, teachers must tread a fine line between
their responsibility to teach the standard variety required for social mobility
and respecting students’ native varieties as manifestations of their identities.
Just as every child has a right to expect teachers to respect their sex, ethnicity,
social class, color, and creed, so every child has the right to expect teachers
to respect their language. It is a lot easier to accept linguistic variation if we
understand it and understand our own attitudes toward it. We deal with this
issue in more depth in our chapters on Variation and Usage in Book II.
In the rest of this chapter, we will consider some of the basic ideas about
language that inform this book.
what is a language?
As teachers of language (which we are, whether we teach linguistics, literature, ESL, or physics), we need to have a clear notion of what it is that we
teach. Surprisingly, few people have even the most rudimentary conception
of what a language is, even though they use (at least) one in nearly every
waking moment of their lives. Generally we can lead perfectly adequate lives
without conceptions based on serious reflection on important topics. For
instance, we do not need a precise understanding of physical notions such
27
Delahunty and Garvey
as force, work, or energy to hit home runs or drive cars. But education aims
to help us understand things that we take for granted. Language is a prime
example. It is a device of mind-boggling complexity, but few people have a
clear conception of its nature and use.
So, what is a language? What we have in mind here is a natural (i.e., not
an artificial or computer-based) system for human communication, such as
English, Chinese, Swahili, or American Sign Language (ASL).
In this book, we’ll assume that
A language is a set of rules, unconsciously present in the mind, which
enables human beings to represent and communicate meanings by
producing audible, visible, or tactile symbols that these rules systematically relate to those meanings.
This definition may seem forbidding and abstract, so let’s look at it piece
by piece.
A language enables its users to communicate meanings by
systematically relating perceptible actions and meanings.
Meanings are mental states or activities, and as such cannot be directly observed. If we want to communicate our meanings to someone else, we must
use something they can perceive with their senses—for example, noises,
gestures, flag waving, or marks on paper. For any of these to communicate
successfully, there must be a system that consistently relates the observable
signals with the private meanings. For lots of good reasons, sound evolved
as the primary mode of human communication. This issue is discussed in
the next section.
Most people conceive of meaning in terms of information—ideas
about the external world or about our thoughts and beliefs. This is called
referential (experiential, ideational) meaning. Referential meanings represent events such as The US women’s soccer team won the World Cup or
states such as The sun is a small star. They are descriptions of states of
affairs, real or imagined. Referential meaning is probably the most commonly communicated type of meaning. However, there are other kinds:
• Expressive meaning reflects the emotional state of a speaker. Ouch!
has no referential status but expresses pain.
• Persuasive (conative) meaning refers to the intended effect of an utterance on its hearer; it attempts to get an audience to perform an action or to believe something. Get out! is an attempt to get
28
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
•
•
•
•
someone to leave; I love you. Honest, I really do! is an attempt to get
someone to believe that “I” loves them.
Social (phatic, interpersonal) meaning, as in expressions such as
Hi! and How are you?, establishes and maintains social contact between communicators.
Textual meaning is communicated by utterances that constitute
(part of ) a text, e.g., The dogs were very noisy. The German shepherds were the worst. Without very noisy in the first of these two
sentences, it would be impossible to interpret the worst as noisiest.
This meaning derives from the assumption that the two sentences
are to be interpreted as a text, that is, one or more sentences or
utterances intended to be taken as a coherent whole. Some expressions have only textual meanings. For example, in some of its uses
so indicates that the expression it introduces is to be interpreted as
a conclusion drawn from a prior expression or from the context.
The retort So what? is a demand to know what conclusion to draw
from what a speaker has just said.
Metalinguistic meaning addresses matters concerning the language itself. Definitions and word puzzles are metalinguistic, e.g.,
What I meant to say was . . ., or What English word has three double
letters in a row? (See the end of this chapter for an answer.)
Poetic meaning reflects nuances of interpretation created by the
manner in which information is expressed. It is the aesthetic dimension of language and language use. Advertisers make good use of
language’s poetic possibilities. They use puns as well as rhythm and
rhyme: Wendy’s restaurants advertised their extended business hours
with the pun, See ya later!; a Cheyenne, WY store advertised tires
with the rhyme, Great deals / On tires and wheels; local authorities
attempt to draw drivers’ attention to road work with the pun Give
’em a brake! and the rhyme Cone Zone.
Exercise
1. Explain how each expression below illustrates one (or more) of the
meaning types just discussed:
a. Don’t touch me!
b. There is a bull in that field.
c. Hello. Are you there? (phone conversation)
d. No pun intended.
e. Jeanne is wearing jeans.
29
Delahunty and Garvey
f. I hate broccoli.
g. Gag me with a spoon.
h. I’ve typed teh and langauge again.
2. For each of the following types of meaning, give a brief text that
illustrates it:
a. persuasive (conative) meaning
b. referential meaning
c. social (phatic) meaning
d. textual meaning
e. poetic meaning
f. expressive meaning
g. metalinguistic meaning
h. referential and conative meaning
A language uses sound as its primary mode of expression
In saying that sound is the “primary” mode of linguistic expression we mean
that it is the principal, earliest, and most fundamental mode. Literate people
who are not linguists tend to assume that writing is the most important
form of language. In fact, they tend to assume that the spoken language
should be modeled on its written form. For example, many people will use a
word’s spelling to resolve a dispute over its pronunciation, and the pronunciation of some words has changed to be more consistent with their spellings. For instance, often has historically been pronounced offen. However,
many people nowadays think that because it is written with the letter <t>, it
should be pronounced with a [t] sound.
Linguists, in general, believe that sound is the primary medium of language, because it precedes writing in evolutionary and individual development; because letters represent sounds, not vice versa; and because we use
spoken language more frequently in our lives, so it is arguably more important to us. To support their claim, linguists point out facts such as the
following:
•
•
•
•
30
children learn to talk before they learn to read and write
children learn to talk naturally, that is, without being expressly
taught; reading and writing must be taught
there are many languages that have no writing systems
writing is a comparatively recent historical development (it has
been around for only a few thousand years); spoken language is at
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
•
least 60,000 years old (see Aitchison 1996, 1997 ch. 2)
all writing systems are attempts to represent aspects of spoken language, generally individual consonants and vowels, less frequently
syllables, less frequently still, words
We do not deny the importance of other modes of expression. Written
language is extremely important in modern societies, and we all spend many
years mastering it. The sounds speakers produce and which are (partially)
processed by hearers’ ears fade away very rapidly. Writing attempts to overcome this rapid fading.
There is always a dynamic relation between spoken and written language.
Each influences the other to various degrees. For example, currently we tend
to allow more speech-like forms into our writing than our grandparents did,
e.g., contractions such as can’t, I’ve, and she’s.
Sign languages of the deaf, which use the hands to express meanings,
are another important language type. But while they can express whatever
a signer wishes to communicate, just as a spoken language can, they are a
relatively uncommon form of language.
If we group together sounds, written symbols, and manual gestures as linguistic forms, then we can think of a language as a system for relating forms
to meanings.
Exercise
1. Find and discuss three differences between spoken and written English (or any other language that you are familiar with). For example,
you might consider “tone of voice.”
2. What advantages or disadvantages do you think spoken language has
over other forms of communication (such as written language, manual
language, waving flags, scratching signs in dirt or rocks, etc.)? Think
both in terms of our distant ancestors and of practical contemporary
needs. As a concrete example, you might consider how to explain,
without speaking or writing, how to bake bread, wash a car, upgrade a
computer, or use a phone keypad to respond to commands from a company’s computerized answering system. (Consult actual texts, such as
recipe books, labels on bottles, or users’ manuals). Refer to your personal experience wherever possible. (You might also look at software
that turns speech into typed text, e.g., Dragon Naturally Speaking.)
31
Delahunty and Garvey
The relation between meaning and sound is conventional
and arbitrary
According to Ferdinand de Saussure (1983 [1916]) and accepted by the vast
majority of linguists, the relation between a word’s sound and its meaning is
conventional. That is, the speakers of a language tacitly agree on which meanings to associate with which sounds. For example, the fruit we make apple
jelly from is called apple in English, pomme in French, manzana in Spanish,
úll in Irish, and other names in other languages.
This conventional relationship is arbitrary. That is, speakers of a language,
as a group, are free to associate any sounds with any meaning. It doesn’t matter which sounds they associate with which meanings. Thus the sound of the
word I is arbitrarily (though not randomly, i.e., without purpose) chosen by
English speakers to represent the speaker of an utterance; we could equally
designate the speaker by the sounds je as in French, or yo as in Spanish, or ich
as in German, or wo as in Chinese, or any other sound(s) we agreed on.
From the claim that sound/meaning relationships are both conventional
and arbitrary, it follows that there need not be any similarity between sound
and what it refers to. The word pigeon bears no resemblance to the birds it
refers to. Similarly, the words yell and whisper can be said either loudly or
softly, even though they refer to loud and soft sounds. Giant and dwarf have
the same number of sounds, despite the different sizes of the things they refer
to. Lilliputian is a big word meaning “small,” but big is a small word meaning
“large.”
Finally, there is no natural or causal connection between words and their
meanings. That is, words and their meanings are not connected in the way
that smoke and fire, or explosions and noise, or cars and air pollution, are. We
know that fires cause smoke, and so when we see smoke we can assume that
there is also fire. The particular sounds of a word do not cause its meanings in
this way, nor do the meanings cause the choice of word sounds.
However, all languages have some expressions that are non-arbitrary. They
are said to be motivated by some factor other than convention. One motivation is similarity between the word sounds and sounds associated with
the things the words refer to. Common examples of these are onomatopoeic
words for animal noises, e.g., moo, bow-wow, and quack-quack. Note that the
last two of these suggest that dogs and ducks normally make noises in pairs
and that English speakers can distinguish a dog’s bow from its wow. Note
however, that an Irish dog goes amh-amh and a Serbo-Croatian one goes avav. This suggests that onomatopoeic words are not perfect imitations; at least
some conventionality is at work in them. To appreciate the range of ways in
which languages represent animal sounds, go to http://www.eleceng.adelaide.
32
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
edu.au/personal/dabbott/animal.html
Another type of motivation is sound symbolism, the relatively consistent
association of certain sounds with certain meanings. For example, the [ee]
vowel sounds of teeny suggests something small. We find similar uses of similar
vowels in other languages. Spanish, for example, uses the suffix -ito/a to designate small things and children. However, it can hardly be said that this vowel
always carries this diminutive meaning. For example, no hint of smallness
appears in words like beefy, treaty, keep, or heal. And similarities with other
languages may be purely accidental: -chen is a diminutive suffix of German, a
language more closely related to English than Spanish is.
While it is true that the vast majority of words that consist of just a single
meaningful part, e.g., lamp and post are arbitrarily related to their meanings,
combinations of such words frequently are not. That lamppost means “lamppost” is motivated by the fact that it consists of lamp and post.
Exercise
What apparent motivation occurs in the following English words? What
words can you think of that don’t fit the patterns?
a. slop, slime, slush
b. itsy-bitsy, tinkle, twinkle
c. slip, slink, slide, slither
d. moo, meow, cuckoo
Duality of patterning
This strange phrase means that meaningful linguistic units such as words are
composed of discrete units that have no meaning. For example, the word
book clearly has a meaning; but just as clearly, each of its individual sounds,
[b], [oo], and [k], has no meaning. Individual sounds like these can be used
to create other words. So languages take one or a combination of meaningless sounds and then assign meanings to them. The expressions of nonhuman animals, even those with relatively large numbers of expressions,
seem not to be designed like this, with the result that their call repertoires
cannot be readily expanded.
Displacement
Human language allows human beings to talk about anything, regardless of
whether what they talk about is in the immediate context, occurred in the
past, will occur in the future, or, indeed, did not, may not, or will not ever occur. This freedom from the here and now is called displacement. Non-human
33
Delahunty and Garvey
communication is typically tied to the time and place at which it occurs. As a
result, we are far better liars than other animals.
A language is distinctively human
This is a remarkably controversial topic. When we speak of language in this
book what we have in mind are systems such as English, French, Swahili,
or Navajo. However, the word language is often used loosely to indicate any
means of conveying meaning—e.g., the language of dance, the language of
flowers, animal languages. The discipline of semiotics developed to study the
language-like characteristics of various forms of communication. The range of
semiotic (meaningful) systems is great, encompassing natural languages, gestures, spatial relations, animal communication, film, advertising logos, traffic
signals, clothing, and many other modes of communication. Much semiotic
research draws on linguistic concepts.
Semiotic and other linguistic studies have demonstrated the richness of
human communication, but have never uncovered any means of communication superior to human language in the complexity, range, or precision of
its meanings. This is not surprising. One could hardly imagine translating the
Constitution of the United States into body language or the language of clothing. While semiotics has dramatically enlarged our awareness of the scope
of meaningful systems, it has produced no challengers to language either on
quantitative or qualitative grounds.
Likewise, research into animal communication has vastly improved our appreciation of the natural communication systems of primates, dolphins, birds,
and frogs. But it has presented no rivals to human communication, again either on qualitative or quantitative grounds. A few primates have learned, usually with intensive training, to communicate in language-like ways, through
manual signs, plastic symbols, or computers. Their success tells us a good deal
about their intelligence (especially of bonobo chimps), but their communicative systems are not equivalent to English or any other human language.
For some people it is not at all surprising that humans have language and
animals don’t. According to many religions, language was given to humans
by a god. For others this topic is intensely controversial. Some claim that our
closest animal relatives share some of our linguistic capacities; others insist that
there is no continuity between whatever cognition and communication other
primates are capable of and human language. (The following items should
give you a roller-coaster ride on the research; not all are easy reads: CarstairsMcCarthy, 1999; Gardner, Gardner, and Van Cantfort 1989; Greenfield and
Savage-Rumbaugh 1990; Hauser, 1996; Hawkins and Gell-Mann 1992;
Hockett, 1960; Lieberman 1984, 1991; Savage-Rumbaugh 1986; Savage34
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
Rumbaugh and Lewin 1994; Sebeok, 1981; Sebeok and Rosenthal, 1981;
Terrace, 1981; Wallman 1992.)
We must add here that all normal human beings can be both producers
and receivers of human language, a characteristic known as reciprocity or
interchangeability. In many animal communication systems one sex, usually the male, produces signals while the other merely receives them.
Another perspective on this issue is the relationship between intelligence
and language. Assuming (controversially) that IQ provides a reliable index of
intelligence, Lenneberg claimed that language abilities are not significantly
absent even at dramatically low levels (though he may have overstated the case
[Jackendoff 2002: 95 n.13]):
Children whose I.Q. is 50 at age 12 and about 30 at age 20 are completely in possession of language though their articulation may be
poor and an occasional grammatical mistake may occur. (Lenneberg
1964: 41-42)
A language is culturally transmitted
No child comes into the world capable of learning only a specific language
or set of languages. All normal children can learn any human language. All
they need is appropriate learning situations. Languages are transmitted from
one generation to the next by cultural transmission, not by genetic transmission, as is the case with many animal communication systems.
Having said that, we must add that without the physiological and neurological bases that language depends on, children would be unable to learn
any language. So learning a language depends upon having both the right
biological bases and the right learning environment.
Knowledge of a language is unconsciously present in the
mind
Consider the following questions:
(1) a. Do you like duckling?
b. Do you like snorkeling?
c. Do you like Kipling?
Without the slightest bit of thought, you know which one of these questions can be answered Yes, I like to
. You know that snorkel but
not duckle or Kiple can occur after I like to ______. You can tell all of this
without any knowledge of grammatical analysis such as that snorkel is a
verb. And though you may not know terms such as morpheme and di35
Delahunty and Garvey
minutive (-ling in (1a)), you know that duckling and snorkeling have two
meaningful parts but that Kipling has only one. You also know the grammatical form and function of snorkeling in (1b), though you might not be
able to provide a technical description. (See our chapters on Phrases, Basic
Clause Patterns, and Multi-clause Sentences.) Knowing a language, then,
is not the same as knowing terminology or being able to articulate grammatical descriptions. Your knowledge of language is unconscious knowledge. No amount of introspection, meditation, psychotherapy, or brain
surgery will allow you to access it directly.
The clearest sign of unconscious knowledge is the presence of linguistic
intuitions—gut feelings about language that we could not have without unconscious linguistic knowledge. These intuitions are not the product of education; totally illiterate people have them. They derive from genetic capacities
specific to humans and from having acquired a language. One’s unconscious
knowledge of language is called linguistic competence. We will have more to
say about linguistic competence below.
A language consists of rules
Unfortunately, the word rule conjures up exactly the wrong image of linguistic knowledge, suggesting the prescriptions of right and wrong that we
find in handbooks. Linguists, however, use the word to mean two related
ideas. First, A rule is a part of our unconscious knowledge of our language (our
linguistic competence). It is a mental pattern about a limited part of a language,
e.g., pronunciation, sentence structure, or what a word means. For instance,
English has a basic subject-verb-object word order:
(2) a. [SubjectPatti] [Verbplays] [Objectthe cello].
b. [SubjectMichael] [Verbwrote] [Objectsome fine poetry].
When we produce sentences of this sort, we are acting as if we were following
a rule that says: Put subjects before verbs and verbs before objects. If we were not
following rules, our speech would be chaotic and unintelligible, not the highly
patterned, communicative activity it is.
Second, linguists also use the word rule to refer to their attempts to formulate these linguistic patterns in words, that is, to the model we build of
an unconscious mental rule. Our model is not the rule itself, which remains
forever inaccessible.
36
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
Exercise
1. What rule would you formulate that would allow English speakers to
say that sentences a, b, and c are OK, but that d is not? (The symbol *
means ungrammatical, i.e., not in conformity with the rules of competence.) Feel free to make use of grammatical terminology and also of
terms for meanings.
a. John looked the address up.
b. John looked it up.
c. John looked up the address.
d. *John looked up it.
2. And what rule would you formulate to explain why (a-c) below are
grammatical, but (d) is not?
a. Harry sent a present to Mary.
b. Harry sent Mary a present.
c. Harry sent a package to Boston.
d. *Harry sent Boston a package. (Can you think of a context or a
meaning in which this sentence can be grammatical?)
A language is a system
Rules are not distributed randomly in the mind like potatoes in a sack. Rather,
they are systematically related to one another. It is easiest to envision this conception with an analogy. A computer system has a set of components (central
processing unit, monitor, keyboard, speakers, drives of various types) whose
overall function is to process information. The components interact with each
other; you can, for instance, play a CD while reading your email. The components also contain smaller parts, all of which interact in precise, though
limited, ways with each other and with parts of other components.
Language systems likewise have components. The most commonly cited
ones are:
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
phonetics/phonology
morphology
vocabulary
orthography/spelling/writing
syntax
semantics
pragmatics
discourse
37
Delahunty and Garvey
Phonetics and phonology are concerned with the sounds of language, morphology with the structure of words, vocabulary with our store of words,
orthography with the spelling system, syntax with the principles of sentence
structure, semantics with the literal meanings of words and sentences, pragmatics with the meanings that arise when expressions are used in specific
contexts, and discourse with the linguistic and rhetorical patterns in texts of
various kinds. As we proceed, you will learn the intricate ways in which the
system operates.
For the moment, let us look at one concrete example of how the system
creates interdependencies among its rules and components. The syntactic rule
for yes/no questions is connected to the rules of pronunciation, specifically
the rules for intonation, the musical pattern of speech. Listen to the rise and
fall of your voice as you say (3a) as a statement of fact and (3b) as a question:
(3) a. They’re leaving at 6:00.
b. They’re leaving at 6:00?
The order of words stays the same, but the intonation pattern indicates
whether the sentence is to be interpreted as a statement or as a question.
In this book, we will begin our discussion with a skeletal overview of
English grammar, beginning with the largest grammatical units (sentences)
and working down to the smallest (sounds and letters). This is the opposite
of our presentation of the grammar in the following chapters. There we begin with the smallest units and work our way up to the largest. We hope that
by spiraling in this way, readers who have no background in language study
will get an initial orientation, and those who have had some background
will get a quick refresher before venturing into greater depth.
c o m p e t e n c e a n d p e r f o rm a n c e
As we mentioned, modern linguists distinguish between the knowledge that
speakers of a language must have in order to be able to use that language, and
the actual use they make of that knowledge to speak, understand, read, or
write. Linguists call our unconscious knowledge of the rules that constitute
the language competence, and our linguistic activities that make use of that
knowledge, performance.
Performance provides ample evidence of competence. We can use our ability to specify what is and what is not grammatical (i.e., consistent with the
unconscious rules of our language). Consider the following:
(4) *The blocking the entrance protester was arrested.
38
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
Though we can certainly make sense of the sentence, we know that it isn’t
natural English. (The German translation would be grammatical with this
word order.) Of course, we may not be able to articulate exactly what makes
the sentence unnatural; nor is it likely that we have been taught anything explicitly about sentences like this.
Likewise, you can determine hidden grammatical relations, that is, implicit subjects, objects, and the like:
(5) a. Joan is eager to please.
b. Joan is easy to please.
In (5a), Joan will do the pleasing; in (5b) someone else will please Joan. Such
“understood” relations are very common in language.
Finally, you can also perceive ambiguity (two or more distinct interpretations):
(6) Molly told Angela about herself.
Here Molly is talking either about Molly or about Angela.
Exercise
Advertisers often make use of ambiguity, for example, GE’s We bring
good things to life. Find 4-5 other examples of ambiguity in advertising. Express their ambiguous meanings in non-ambiguous sentences.
Why do you think advertisers might like ambiguity? How about poets?
You might mull over the last line of Dylan Thomas’ poem “A refusal to
mourn the death, by fire, of a child in London”: After the first death,
there is no other.
The idea of competence depends on certain idealizations. Many linguists,
though by no means all, assume that all speakers of a language have the same
set of rules in their competence. This is a deliberate simplification, made
with full awareness of the variety inherent in natural language. It is done to
allow linguists to develop models of competence without being distracted
by phenomena that do not appear to affect the model’s basic principles.
This assumption is not uncontroversial. It has been viewed as an attempt
to ignore the social, discourse, and textual functions of language, which
some linguists believe to be crucial in understanding language structure. It
39
Delahunty and Garvey
has also been viewed, because the majority of linguists are white, male, and
middle class, as a thinly disguised attempt to define their variety of English
as the basis for the grammatical theory for all languages and all varieties,
much as Latin grammar was until recently (and in many situations still is)
the model for the grammars of many European languages.
While neither criticism is justified in its extreme version, both point to
limitations of the language‑as‑competence approach. They also point to the
need to understand language as a social artifact used by social beings in
social contexts for social purposes. We deal with such considerations in our
chapters on Variation and Usage in Book II.
A language is acquired
Because many modern theoretical linguists begin from the assumption that
what they are modeling is knowledge, it follows that their theories have
implications for psychology and ultimately for biology. Many believe that
language is a very specialized, perhaps unique, kind of knowledge. They
believe that an individual’s primary form of language is not acquired in the
ways that other kinds of knowledge are acquired, such as writing or arithmetic. In support of this belief, they point out that children learn the language
(or languages) of their environments without any instruction or correction
from parents or peers. All they need to acquire language is someone to communicate with them. Moreover, they learn a vastly complex system in a very
short time, and all create very similar grammars of a given language regardless of the differences in what they hear about them, and (up to a point)
regardless of their differences in intelligence.
Most tellingly, linguists point out that when we know a language we
know far more than we could have gleaned just from the language we heard
around us. Our linguistic competence is far richer in its “depth, variety, and
intricacy” (Smith 1999: 41) than the evidence that we used to acquire our
languages. For example, English speakers know that sentences like (7a) are
ungrammatical while (7b) is fine:
(7) a. *She sang beautifully the song.
b. She sang the song beautifully.
No child learning English (as opposed to French or Italian) as their native language has to be taught (indeed, no child can be taught) that sentences such as
(7a) are ungrammatical. (How would you articulate the rule that (7a) violates
and then explain it to a child?) The idea that we know more than we have
evidence for is called the poverty of the stimulus argument. The difference
40
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
between what we know and what we have linguistic evidence for must have
come from somewhere. It can only have come from cognitive and brain structures specialized for language acquisition. The linguists who are persuaded by
arguments like these hold that human beings do not enter the world as “blank
slates,” rather, they bring with them innate ideas that guide them in acquiring
their language(s).
On the basis of these observations, and the similarities between languages,
many linguists argue that human beings are genetically endowed with a capacity to acquire languages with particular kinds of rule systems. They argue that
linguistic knowledge is of a different type than other knowledge, because it is
based on specialized cognitive structures, which in turn appear to be based on
specialized brain structures.
In support of this neurobiological claim, linguists point out that first language learning must be accomplished within a “critical period” in a person’s
life (before the teenage years) if it is to be successful. Moreover, damage to certain parts of the brain, mainly in the left hemisphere, affects people’s linguistic
abilities, whereas damage to corresponding areas in the right hemisphere need
not. Thus the language capacity appears to be (at least partially) localized in
the left side of the brain. If this point of view is correct, it explains why, even
with intense and specific training, no non‑humans (even the most intelligent
ones) have ever learned a human language.
In contrast, we must be taught how to write, though we may begin learning it at any age—but generally only after we have already learned a primary
form of language.
Exercise
1. What kinds of errors have you observed people make as they learn a
second language? Or children as they learn their first language?
2. Find a description of one of the many attempts to teach an ape a
human language. Describe the teaching methods and the results. How
do they compare to the ways in which children acquire languages? How
do these animals’ linguistic skills compare with the linguistic skills of
ordinary people?
A language is infinite
Our view of language and grammar makes some very important claims about
the nature of knowledge, at least of linguistic knowledge. It used to be thought
41
Delahunty and Garvey
that all knowledge, whether acquired by a human or a bird, was essentially a
matter of habit. One learned to respond in specific ways to particular events,
and the strength of the habit was a function of the number of times a particular stimulus and response were associated by the learner. From this point of
view, understanding a sentence would be a matter of associating a particular
response with it. And learning a language would be a matter of learning just
which responses go with which sentences. The process was viewed by behaviorist psychologists as in principle identical to the process by which a laboratory pigeon learns to peck at different colors or shapes.
In 1957 Noam Chomsky published a remarkable little book, Syntactic
Structures, in which he pointed out that the behaviorist approach to language
cannot in principle account for language, its acquisition, or its use. This is
because language is vast. In fact, the number of sentences in any language is
infinite. So no theory that assumes that language learning is habit-formation
can, in principle, explain it. You can demonstrate the vastness of language for
yourself in a number of ways.
Select what you believe to be the longest sentence of the language. Once
you have your candidate, put the words I believe that before it. Now you have
created a sentence even longer than the first. This must now be the longest
sentence of the language. But even to this we can add Fred thinks that to create an even longer sentence. To make an (infinitely) long story short, there
is no longest sentence in English or any other natural language. (Although
recent reports on the Amazonian language, Pirahã, call this into question
[Colapinto 2007: 118-137].) Language allows us, in principle, if not in actuality, to create infinitely long sentences, and consequently to create an infinite
number of sentences. We do this by inserting one sentence within another,
within another, within another . . . ad infinitum. This property of inserting
a sentence within a sentence is called recursion. It is because natural languages are recursive that they allow for the creation of an infinite number of
sentences. All natural human languages have this property. So do all varieties
and dialects of all human languages. It follows that all languages and varieties
are equal. From a linguist’s point of view, the creativity of language is based
on its recursiveness.
Exercise
Can you think of a different set of sentences that demonstrates the infinity of language? For example, start with the sentence The book that
I read was interesting because . . . Expand the bolded parts.
42
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
Now, while our sentences may be infinite, our memories are not. Consequently, our knowledge of our language, our competence, cannot be just
a set of sentences. It must be a finite set of devices that allow us to create
or understand sentences as we need to. Thus we can produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences that we have never heard or uttered
before. We do this, partially, by matching what we hear with the rules of
language that we keep in our heads.
Exercise
1. Reread several pages of this chapter and list the sentences that you
had read or heard before reading it the first time. We are confident
that your list will be either empty or very short. What does this fact
tell you about how you made sense of the sentences that you had never
encountered before?
2. Briefly explain and illustrate with at least one appropriate example
each of the following concepts:
a. Metalinguistic meaning
b. The arbitrariness of the relation between words and their meanings
c. Linguistic competence
d. Linguistic performance
e. The infinity of language
a p p ro a c h e s t o t h e s t u d y o f l a n g u a g e
Prescriptive and descriptive viewpoints
Prescriptive grammarians are mainly concerned with the conventions that
govern formal, written communication. Their goal is to maintain a standardized variety of a language so that it can function as the variety used
for communication by the major domains of a state (such as education,
government, commerce, and law), as well as among people separated by
great distances, by great cultural differences, and by considerable spans of
time. This requires a set of widely accepted conventions that are codified in
grammars, dictionaries, and style manuals. These conventions are designed
with the goal of ensuring that people using the standard variety will use the
same forms in the same ways and with the same meanings, thus presumably
43
Delahunty and Garvey
facilitating clear and unambiguous communication. Our Usage chapter addresses prescriptive grammar in more detail.
Descriptive linguists are primarily interested in people’s actual linguistic
knowledge and behavior—in what they say and how they say it—regardless
of whether it conforms to the standard prescriptions. They do not judge it to
be correct or incorrect. Generally, they believe that if a community of native
speakers of a language consistently speaks or writes in such and such a way,
then so be it. That, for the descriptive linguist, is correct, regardless of how
prescriptive grammarians view the behavior. Descriptive linguists attempt to
put aside their own linguistic prejudices (yes, we all have them) and accept
and describe what they observe.
Exercise
Using a usage or style manual, find three expressions (e.g., We was)
that English speakers frequently use but which the manual claims we
shouldn’t. What, if any, reasons does the manual give for preferring one
expression over others? How valid do you think the reasons are?
To make the differences between these two approaches more concrete, let’s
consider an example. Consider who and whom. Handbooks claim that who
and whom should be used as illustrated in (8a-c):
(8) a. Who phoned?
b. To whom did you speak?
c. Whom did you speak to?
However, in ordinary conversation we are unlikely to use whom in sentences
like (8c) and far more likely to use who:
d. Who did you speak to?
Descriptive linguists comparing (8c) with (8d) would note the different forms
and that (8c) with whom is used in very formal contexts whereas (8d) with
who is used in less formal ones. (See Baron 1994: 27-8.) They would also note
that the to associated with who(m) is to its left in (8b) but not in (8c) or (8d).
From this they would infer that to stays to the left of who(m) in formal contexts, and in those circumstances, whom is required.
44
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
Exercise
Many people use seen as the past tense form of see; for instance, I seen
him yesterday. (i) What would (a) a prescriptive grammarian, and (b) a
descriptive linguist say about this form/usage? (ii) What would each say
about the people who use this form? (iii) What would each say about
the contextual circumstances in which it is used?
Descriptive viewpoints: theoretical, analytic, and applied
The descriptive approach to language encompasses a much wider range of
inquiry than just grammar. In the following sections we sketch the spectrum
of interests that descriptive linguists have pursued. In so doing, we hope to
stimulate your curiosity about topics that will one day inform your own teaching.
Theoretical linguistics
Theoretical linguists take a descriptive attitude, but they want to go beyond
merely describing language. Their goals are to understand what they observe
and to explain why human languages are as they are. To do this they construct
models or theories of language. Models are portrayals (verbal and visual) of
the design of languages. For instance, a model might sketch out a syntactic
component that includes several different types of rules, as we’ll see in our
chapter on Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns. In so doing, theoretical
linguists try to formulate general statements about what is possible and what
is not possible in the syntax of human languages. For example, a theoretical
linguist might try to determine why no human language asks questions by
simply reversing the word order of statements.
Theoretical linguists thus can identify some very general principles that
govern language. In science, as in many other fields, if a statement follows logically from general principles, then it is regarded as explained. In linguistics,
if the rules proposed for a language follow logically from general assumptions
about the nature of human language, they are regarded as explanations, and
the general hypotheses are supported.
Theoretical linguists tend to think of themselves as scientists and of their
activities as following the methodologies of science. They observe phenomena, make general statements to describe their observations, hypothesize what
else should be true if their generalizations are true, and test whether they were
correct. If they are correct, they create more hypotheses and test again. If they
are incorrect, they revise their generalizations and hypotheses, and test again.
In a sense they want to be wrong. When they find where they are wrong, they
can improve their original formulation and account for a wider range of data
45
Delahunty and Garvey
than before. Linguistic study, from this point of view, is not a hunt for errors,
but rather a dynamic, ongoing, creative task, subject to constant criticism
and revision. It is important to understand this, because not to do so leads to
several misconceptions.
Language rules from this perspective are not a body of immutable laws or
conventions discovered or imposed by scholars. They are reflections of our
current understanding of the phenomena of natural language.
It is also important not to see this as an endorsement of the view that
“anything goes in English these days.” In the last two centuries, linguists have
discovered an enormous amount about many individual languages and much
about natural language in general. Consequently, particularly since the late
1950s, new understandings about the nature of human beings and the human mind have emerged. These have provided far richer models of how human beings learn, remember, and solve problems than were available before.
Furthermore, we have discovered a great deal about the relationships between
language, culture, and society. It turns out that we (and other creatures) are far
more complex than we had given ourselves (or them) credit for.
Exercise
What implications do you think the scientific study of language might
have for psychology, computer science, education, and law?
As we noted, we do not believe that “anything goes” in English, or in any
language. Languages and language varieties are rule‑governed; if they weren’t,
we wouldn’t be able to understand each other. Some forms are meaningful,
grammatical, or acceptable; others are meaningless, ungrammatical, or unacceptable. The status of an expression is judged against the rules that constitute
the grammar of the language or variety and the rules of appropriateness of
utterances to specific situations. If the grammar cannot assign a meaning to
the utterance then it will be either completely or partially meaningless. If the
utterance is not in accord with the structural rules of the language then it will
be ungrammatical. If the utterance is inappropriate in a given situation or
context, then it is unacceptable.
Exercise
Consider these sentences:
a. Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
46
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
b. Rusty old cars deteriorate rapidly.
c. We ate quickly our lunches.
d. Hi Dubya! (To President Bush.)
Are these sentences meaningful? Grammatical? Appropriate (in some
context)? Explain your reasoning. (Regarding (a), you might track down
Sister Mary Jonathan’s poem, “You, Noam Chomsky,” when you answer
this question.)
Analytic linguistics
Language theorists do not work in a vacuum. Rather, they base their hypotheses on the careful examination of language done either by themselves or by
others, including philosophers, psychologists, and sociologists. Linguistic
analysis draws upon various theories and their analytic tools to provide a description of the facts and rules of entire languages or of portions of a language.
For example, modern traditional grammars, such as the series developed by
Randolph Quirk and his colleagues (e.g., Quirk et al. 1985), and the recently
published comprehensive Cambridge Grammar of the English Language (Huddleston and Pullum 2002) are based primarily on traditional grammatical
concepts, but they are influenced by developments in more recent grammatical theories and methods. Many modern grammars (and dictionaries), such
as Cambridge Grammar of English: A Comprehensive Guide: Spoken and Written English Grammar and Usage (Carter and McCarthy 2006) and Longman
Student Grammar of Spoken and Written English (Biber, Conrad, and Leech
2002), make use of huge databases of spoken and written language (corpora)
that can be searched with special computer programs (concordancers); the
sentences and other forms found by these searches can then be used as both
data for analysis and for illustration (see Biber et al. 1992; Carter and McCarthy 2006). Authentic data are particularly important for materials designed
for second language students of English, as well as for teaching the conventions of various genres to native speakers.
Linguistic analysis extends into many fields. The study of regional variation (dialectology) and of social variation (sociolinguistics) has contributed
much to our awareness of the diversity of English (see our chapter on Variation in Book II). Corpus research has broadened our understanding of first
and second language acquisition, as well as of the role of language in psychological, legal, and computer contexts. In education, analytic linguistics has
contributed to areas such as syntax, lexicography, usage, reading, writing, and
literature. These accomplishments mostly concern the present state of the language, a perspective called synchronic linguistics. Analysis also extends to
47
Delahunty and Garvey
the historical study of languages, a perspective called diachronic linguistics.
Applied linguistics
Applied linguists draw upon theoretical models and analytic work for practical purposes. Computer parsers, artificial intelligence (e.g., speech recognition
and synthesis), and machine translation form the computational side of the
applied linguistics family. Linguists have been hired as consultants to help in
the simplification of legal documents and in documenting the identities of
tape-recorded human voices in trials (forensic linguistics). They have helped
the governments of emerging nations devise writing systems and establish
public policies on language (language planning). In education, they have
provided the bases for methods of language teaching (e.g., foreign languages,
including English as a foreign or second language, and bilingual education).
And there are linguistic underpinnings to designs for English curricula, ranging from phonics to Whole Language.
In spite of its many contributions, linguistics has not had its full impact on
education. Perhaps teachers fear the technicalities of a discipline that claims
English study to be a science. Some may see linguistics as a threat to traditional values in teaching. The reason may be that mainstream linguistics is an
independent discipline with its own objectives and methods and which has
not embraced the poststructuralist and postmodernist approaches adopted in
literature and composition studies. This is an ironic turn, because the roots
of all of these approaches lie in early twentieth century linguistics. In spite of
this divergence of interests, we are convinced that teachers are best served by
an understanding of the nature of language and the ways it is approached by
linguists.
One of the aims of this book is to initiate you into the linguistic point of
view and to provide you with the linguistic literacy that you will need in the
contemporary classroom. As we have tried to show in this section, applied
work grows out of theoretical and analytical frameworks. Moreover, theoretical and analytic notions can provide us and our students with intellectually
stimulating and rewarding classroom activities. Many of the exercises in this
book exemplify such activities.
Exercise
1. Go to your college library and visit the language section. Identify
three areas that interest you (e.g., child language acquisition, regional
dialects) and report on the books available on these subjects. Try to
find journals on the topic too and note the types of articles that appear
48
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
in them. Consult the ERIC (Educational Resources Information Clearing
House) system; you may do so either with hard copy or on a computer.
See the ERIC thesaurus of descriptors under the heading “language.”
For a broader spectrum of research on language, you can consult the
LLBA (Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts), also on computer
or in hard copy. (Note: this exercise is a good way to get started on a
course project or paper.)
2. Search the Web for sites dealing with English and other languages. A
good starting point is Richard Lederer’s website. Just enter “verbivore”
on your search engine. The Linguistic Society of America (LSA) website
(www.lsadc.org) contains essays on many aspects of language and fields
of linguistics. The TESOL (Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages) site provides lots of information useful to ESL or EFL teachers.
The LINGUIST List provides information on a very broad range of linguistic topics and links to many valuable sites. Report on what you find.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Aitchison, Jean. 1996. The Seeds of Speech: Language Origin and Evolution.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____1997. The Language Web. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Baron, D. 1994. Guide to Home Language Repair. Urbana: NCTE.
Biber, Douglas, Susan Conrad, and Geoffrey Leech. 2002. Longman Student
Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, UK: Longman.
_____Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad, and Edward Finegan.
1999. Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, UK:
Longman.
Carstairs-McCarthy, Andrew. 1999. The Origins of Complex Language. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
Carter, Ronald and Michael McCarthy. 2006. Cambridge Grammar of English:
A Comprehensive Guide to Spoken and Written English Grammar and Usage.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.
Colapinto, John. 2007. The interpreter. In The New Yorker, April 16, 118-137.
Crystal, David. 2003. A Dictionary of Linguistics. (5th ed.) Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
de Saussure, Ferdinand. 1983 [1916]. Course in General Linguistics. Translated
by R. Harris. London: Duckworth.
Gardner, R.A., B.T. Gardner, and T.T. Van Cantfort. 1989. Teaching Sign
49
Delahunty and Garvey
Language to Chimpanzees. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Gramley, Stephan and Kurt-Michael Pätzold. 2004. A Survey of Modern
English. (2nd ed.) London: Routledge.
Greenfield, P.M. and E.S. Savage-Rumbaugh. 1990: Grammatical
combination in Pan paniscus: processes of learning and invention in the
evolution and development of languages. In S. Taylor Parker and K.R.
Gibson (eds.) Language and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hauser, M.D. 1996. The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Hawkins, John A. and Murray Gell-Mann. (eds.) 1992. The Evolution of
Human Languages: Proceedings of the Workshop on the Evolution of Human
Languages, August 1984, Santa Fé, NM. Proceedings Vol XI, Santa
Fé Institute, Studies in the Science of Complexity. Redwood City, CA:
Addison-Wesley.
Hockett, Charles. 1960, 1981. The origin of speech. Scientific American,
203:3, 88-95. Also in William S-Y. Yang, (ed.), Human Communication:
Language and its Psychobiological Bases. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and
Company, 4-12.
Huddleston, R. and G. K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar of the
English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar,
Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Lenneberg, E. 1964. A biological perspective on language. In R.C. Oldfield
and J.C. Marshall, (eds.) Language. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin
Books, 32-47.
Lieberman, P. 1984. The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
_____ 1991. Uniquely Human: The Evolution of Speech, Thought, and Selfless
Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Quirk, R., S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svartvik. 1985. A Comprehensive
Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S. 1986. Ape Language: From Conditioned Response to
Symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S. and R. Lewin. 1994. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of
the Human Mind. New York: Doubleday.
Sebeok, T. A. 1981. The ultimate enigma of “Clever Hans”: The union of
nature and culture. In Sebeok and Rosenthal, 199-205.
_____, and R. Rosenthal (eds.), 1981. The Clever Hans Phenomenon:
Communication with Horses, Whales, Apes, and People. Annals of the 50
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
New York Academy of Sciences 364. New York: The New York Academy
of Sciences.
Smith, Neil. 1999. Chomsky: Ideas and Ideals. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Terrace, Herbert. 1981. A report to the Academy. In Sebeok and Rosenthal,
94-114.
Wallman, J. 1992. Aping Language. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wheeler, Rebecca S. 2005. Code-switching to teach standard English. English
Journal, 94, 5: 108-112.
_____ and Rachel Swords. 2004. Codeswitching: Tools of language and
culture transform the dialectally diverse classroom. Language Arts, 81, 6:
470-480.
Winchester, Simon. 1998. The Professor and the Madman. New York: HarperCollins.
_____ 2003. The Meaning of Everything: The Story of the Oxford English
Dictionary. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
glossary
acceptable:
in accord with both descriptive and prescriptive rules. See
grammatical, unacceptable.
acquisition of language: a process by which children develop the rules of
competence in their native language; based on genetic predisposition and
exposure to language rather than on formal teaching.
ambiguity, ambiguous: having two or more clearly distinct interpretations.
analytic (also called descriptive) linguistics: that branch of language study that
attempts to analyze whole languages or parts of languages, proposing descriptive
rules.
applied linguistics: that branch of language study that (ideally) employs
theory and description for practical purposes—e.g., first language teaching, especially composition; second language teaching; translation; language
policy; etc.
arbitrary, arbitrariness: the idea that languages may associate any meaning with any sounds.
bilingual (also multilingual): personal or societal use of two or more languages.
causal: one thing causes another, e.g., exams cause stress.
competence: (native) speakers’ unconscious knowledge of the rules of their language.
components of language: the interrelated sub-parts of a model of language, specifically phonology, morphology, orthography, syntax, semantics,
51
Delahunty and Garvey
and pragmatics. Each component is made up of rules.
composition text: a book designed for teaching the various skills of writing
at the junior high, high school and college level; may combine features of
prescriptive grammar and conventional rules. Also offers suggestions about
the process of writing.
convention, conventional, conventionality: the idea that the speakers
of a language agree on which meanings are associated with which sounds.
corpus linguistics: linguistic analysis based on collections of language
data, usually stored as computerized data bases and analyzed by computer
programs.
creativity of language: the capacity of language to express an infinite
number of sentences.
cultural transmission: the idea that human beings learn their native
language(s) from speakers around them, rather than by being genetically
preprogrammed with a language, as is the case with some animals.
descriptive linguistics: concerned with actual patterns of language and language use.
diachronic linguistics: the study of historical change in languages.
dialect, dialectal, dialectology: (the study of ) regional variation in a
language.
diminutive: a part of a word indicating smallness or youth, e.g., Billy.
duality of patterning: the idea that the smallest meaningful linguistic
units are composed of reusable, meaningless sounds.
explanation: linguistic rules that follow logically from general assumptions
about the nature of human language are regarded as explanations of the
phenomena they describe.
expressive meaning: meaning that indicates the emotional state of a speaker.
grammar (descriptive): (1) an overall systematic description of a language,
written by a linguist or some other person; (2) the syntactic part (component) of the overall description, describing the systematic rules of sentence
structure; (3) linguistic competence, i.e., the unconscious but systematic
knowledge of the rules of one’s native language (also called “internalized
grammar”); (4) the systematic rules in one’s linguistic competence that apply to sentence structure.
grammar (prescriptive): an unsystematic list of language variations with
the claim that one of the variants is right/correct/proper and the others are
not.
grammar book: summary of the syntactic structures of a language, including part of speech, word order, sentence structure, and sometimes rules of
usage.
52
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
grammatical (descriptive): (1) in accord with rules of competence; (2) per-
taining to linguistic structure.
in accord with rules of linguistic correctness.
See grammar (prescriptive).
grammatical relations: relationships such as subject, object, or predicate
of a sentence.
handbook of style: a resource that provides information such as rules of
grammatical usage, hints for clarity of expression, and bibliographical formatting.
infinity of language: the capacity of language to express an indefinite
number of sentences, as well as an endlessly long sentence. See creativity
of language.
internalized grammar: See competence.
interchangeability (reciprocity): the idea that human beings can both
produce and receive/understand their language(s).
language: a system of rules, unconsciously present in the mind, that enables
humans to relate sounds (also gestures or graphic symbols) and meanings.
linguistic competence: See competence.
linguistic intuition: the natural sense of grammaticality, ambiguity, and
structure in one’s native language.
linguistic meaning: meaning that arises from semantic and pragmatic factors of an utterance, as a result of a hearer’s perceiving a speaker’s intention.
meaningless (descriptive): making no sense.
metalinguistic meaning: meaning focusing on items of the language system.
model of language: a linguist’s schematic representation of a rule, of a
component of language, or of an entire language.
morpheme: minimal, meaningful linguistic form.
morphology: linguistic component dealing with the units (morphemes)
that can be combined to make up words. See chapter on Morphology and
Word Structure.
motivated: having non-arbitrary connections between a sign (e.g., a sequence of sounds) and its meaning.
natural: one thing is associated with another by nature rather than by convention.
objects of sentences: parts of the sentence representing the thing(s) affected (direct object) or who receive something or benefit in some way from
the situation (indirect object).
onomatopoeia: a word or phrase whose sound appears to imitate the
object(s) it refers to.
grammatical (prescriptive):
53
Delahunty and Garvey
paradox:
tion.
a poetic device employing a contradiction that may allow resolu-
parts of speech:
categories of words such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives.
See chapter on Parts of Speech.
performance: the use of language in specific situations (speaking, writing,
listening, reading), subject to interferences such as slips of the tongue, etc.
persuasive (conative) meaning: the intention to get someone to perform an
action or believe something.
phonetics: study of speech sounds as sounds.
phonology: study of speech sounds that distinguish meaningful units in a
language.
poetic meaning: meanings conveyed through the manner in which a piece
of information is expressed.
poverty of the stimulus argument: the idea that we know more about our
languages than the situations in which we learned them gave us evidence for.
This is an important argument for believing that language learning requires
substantial help from specialized cognitive and brain structures.
pragmatics: linguistic component dealing with the system of non literal
word and sentence meanings in a language. See semantics.
predicate of a sentence: the phrase that completes a clause or sentence
when added to a sentence subject. See chapter on Basic Clause Patterns.
preposition stranding: ending a sentence with a preposition.
prescriptive: ideally, directions for the most generally used formal writing
and speaking practices, which help define standard English.
rapid fading: the swift disappearance characteristic of speech sounds and
manual gestures.
reciprocity: See interchangeability.
recursion: a property of competence and of rules by which they repeat
themselves, resulting in an infinity of structures.
referential meaning: meaning concerned with information about the external world or about internal thoughts or beliefs.
register: words and expressions particular to occupations, hobbies, etc.
rule of language (descriptive): (1) the mental representation in competence of some specific regularity in the language; (2) a statement that attempts to describe that representation—i.e., a model of a part of competence.
rule of language (prescriptive): a statement that specifies a correct or an
incorrect usage.
semantics: linguistic component dealing with the system of literal meanings
of words and sentences. See pragmatics.
54
Conceptions of Language and Grammar
semiotics: the study of communicative (sign) systems, including language but
also such systems as gestures, spatial relations, animal communication, film,
advertising logos, traffic signals, clothing, etc.
social (phatic) meaning: meaning that creates and/or maintains social contact between communicators.
sociolinguistics: the study of language variation according to social class,
ethnicity, gender, and formality.
sound symbolism: aspects of the pronunciation of words that suggest aspects
of their meaning; e.g., the vowel sounds of teeny as suggestive of smallness.
style: the choice of expression that reflects contextual factors such as the formality of the situation.
subject of a sentence: The phrase that when integrated with a predicate completes a basic clause. See chapter on Basic Clause Patterns.
synchronic linguistics: the study of a language at a particular time, i.e., as
abstracted from historical change.
syntax: linguistic component dealing with the system of sentence structure.
text: one or more spoken or written utterances that form a coherent whole.
textual meaning: meaning that derives from utterances put together to form
(part of) a text.
theoretical linguistics: that branch of language study that attempts to specify (1) the nature of language, its acquisition, and its use; and (2) appropriate
models and other technical devices used to describe language.
theory: general statements based on observation that describe the nature of
some domain such as language, partially verified by testing hypotheses that
derive logically from the statements, and which explain phenomena in the
domain.
unacceptable: evoking a negative response for any reason whatever; the broadest category of disapproval of language.
ungrammatical (descriptive): not in accord with linguistic competence, i.e.,
not natural, normal, or in agreement with the intuitions of the native speaker.
ungrammatical (prescriptive): not in accord with rules of correctness.
usage: rules designed to ensure that standard English is used in formal writing and speaking and to make our writings and speeches clear, efficient, and
effective, given our purposes in communicating and the characteristics of our
audiences.
Answer: The word bookkeeper has three double letters in a row.
55
Delahunty and Garvey
56
3 A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
key concepts
Clauses
Sentences
Phrases
Complementation and modification
Words
Morphemes
Parts of speech
Regular and irregular forms
Grammatical categories
Sounds and spelling
Form, function, meaning
i n t ro d u c t i o n
This chapter has a number of purposes. First, it aims to place the study
of grammar within the larger study of discourse and text. Second, it aims
to provide a review of traditional grammatical concepts and terminology for those who have studied grammar before. If you have not studied
grammar before, it will give a very simple introduction to grammatical
ideas and plenty of practice in using those ideas in straightforward exercises. Third, it provides a basis for the deeper and more critical study of
language that we present in the remainder of this book and in Book II.
In this chapter we only provide examples of concepts; we do not justify
those concepts or their application (though we will sketch how to justify
analyses in our section on Form, Function, and Meaning). Our goal is
to help you get up to speed in recognizing basic grammatical elements.
Because the presentation in the following chapters progresses from the
smallest units (sounds), and works upward to the largest (sentences), we
begin this chapter with the largest units, clauses and sentences, and work
downward to the smallest.
We use language to communicate with each other. This communication may occur in conversation, which can be either directly face-to-face
or mediated by some technology such as the telephone. We may also communicate by writing to each other. Communication in which language
is used is called discourse and the products of discourse are called texts,
which can be recordings of spoken communication or the writings that
constitute a written discourse. Discourse is coherent, cohesive, contextualized, and purposeful communicative activity. We will have more to
57
Delahunty and Garvey
say about discourse and texts in our chapter on Discourse. The texts that
realize a discourse are composed of various kinds of linguistic units, the
most important of which is the sentence. The sentence is the largest unit
to which the grammatical rules of a language apply. Sentences are composed of one or more clauses; a clause is composed of at least two phrases,
one of which plays the role of subject, the other, of predicate; phrases
are composed of one or more words functioning as a single grammatical
unit; words are composed of morphemes; morphemes are meaningful
sequences of phonemes (units of sound); phonemes are the units of sound
that distinguish words or morphemes from each other. They are represented more or less adequately by letters of the alphabet.
clauses
Because sentences are composed of one or more clauses, we begin with a
discussion of the latter. Clauses represent simple states of affairs. These may
be states or events. States are unchanging situations; events are situations in
which something changes.
The simplest type of complete clause must have two phrases, one functioning as the subject and the other as the predicate. In the following examples the subject is italicized and the predicate is bolded. Note that both
subject and predicate can consist of one or more words:
58
Squirrels jump.
Dogs chase cats.
Jack and Jill ran up the hill.
A clause is composed of a subject and a predicate.
You should have been there.
The students left the room quietly.
the little lame baloonman whistled far and wee (e.e. cummings)
Reality TV is a crashing bore.
He tried to avoid the freighter.
The author was born in 1943.
FDR was a great leader.
Their voices make up the story.
Husbands give wives flowers.
Linguistics will expand knowledge of language.
The book deals with contemporary issues in political ethics.
Four people were killed today in a small plane crash in San Diego.
It is raining.
You know the answers to most of these questions.
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
Exercise
1. Put brackets around the complete subject and complete predicate in
each of the following clauses. For example, [All politicians] [give speeches].
(Subject) (Predicate)
a. This section has a number of purposes.
b. We provide copious exercises for you.
c. Sentences stand alone.
d. We begin with easy exercises.
e. Beginning writers leave out parts of sentences.
f. I need a cup of tea.
g. You should try yoga for your nerves.
h. Karate is excellent exercise.
i. The queen may move any distance in any direction.
j. Alice and the March Hare went to the Mad Hatter’s tea party.
k. Politicians lie.
l. Politicians tell lies.
m. Our library is rather small.
n. Its collection consists of old books.
o. It is raining heavily.
p. The astronauts sent greetings to the earth-bound.
2. A very simple question: What is the typical order of subject and
predicate in English clauses?
Besides occurring before the predicate, subjects are often defined as the
phrase that represents something about which something is said by the
predicate, or about which the predicate answers a question. For instance,
we can view the subjects (italicized) in the following clauses as representing
something about which the predicates (bolded) say something or answer a
question about the subject. For instance, What can we say about dogs? They
bark:
Dogs bark.
Flannery O’Connor wrote several books.
His mother sent him a care package.
He paid an unscrupulous dealer $400,000 for a vintage DB 6.
Subjects are traditionally viewed as expressions that represent the doer
59
Delahunty and Garvey
of the action represented by the verb of the clause. They can often answer
the question, Who did what the predicate describes? For example, Who wrote
many great plays? Oscar did. This is true in the following examples:
Donald ate a pizza.
Dick walked the dog.
Peggy went to the concert.
Notice that the subject is the first word or phrase in each of the clauses
above. This is often the case, but not always. In the following examples,
another phrase precedes the subject. Again, the subjects are italicized:
In the stormy seas, Jim O’Neill is doing his best to win a yacht race.
Manipulated by MI6, O’Neill is dragged into a bloody confrontation.
Exercise
Identify the subject and predicate in the following clauses.
a. Fortunately, I still have time to prepare for the semester’s classes.
b. According to the great majority of atmospheric scientists, the
earth is warming alarmingly.
c. Evidently, the globe is in for increasingly extreme weather.
d. Surprisingly, people are not doing very much about this.
e. Hopefully, they will begin adapting soon.
An essential grammatical element in a predicate is its main verb. Every
complete clause must have one. The italicized words below are the main
verbs in their respective clauses:
Tom Foote went to sea as a young man.
Welch chronicles the family history of the Condons.
The plot grew frightening.
Martha resembles her mother.
Donald owns a Testarossa.
The tank weighs over 6,000 pounds.
Traditionally, main verbs are defined as words that denote actions or
states of being. The verbs italicized just above fit this definition. Note,
60
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
though, that most of them end in -s and occur after the subject.
Exercise
Put brackets around the main verb in each of the following clauses. For
example, The cat [escaped] from the house.
a. This chapter has several purposes.
b. The book provides copious exercises for you.
c. The student appears calm.
d. We begin with easy exercises.
e. Unskilled writers omit parts of sentences.
f. Sentences stand alone.
g. Government mismanagement caused huge losses.
h. She tried yoga for her nerves.
i. The queen moves for any distance in any direction.
j. Alice and the March Hare went to the Mad Hatter’s tea party.
k. It rains heavily here.
Some clauses contain phrases that play the role of object. Object phrases
generally follow the main verb and are part of the predicate. They are italicized in the following examples. Note that object phrases may consist of one
or more words:
Dogs chase cats.
Tigers eat meat.
His military service took him around the world.
This collection of photographs captures the beauty of California’s northern
coastline.
Objects are traditionally viewed as the expression denoting the “receiver”
of, or the entity directly affected by, the situation represented by the verb.
Thus, if the subject represents the agent of the action denoted by the verb,
then the subject does whatever is denoted by the verb to whatever is represented by the object. This definition fits all of the examples immediately
above.
Exercise
1. Put brackets around the entire object in each of the following sen61
Delahunty and Garvey
tences. For example, John Lennon wrote [many great songs].
a. Oscar ate the hamburger.
b. Akim read his book.
c. The CEO told several lies.
d. Some sentences contain modifiers.
e. Cats like catnip.
2. Very easy question: What is the typical order of subjects, verbs, and
objects in English clauses?
Some sentences contain two objects, separated by brackets in the following examples:
a. Frederick sent [his mother] [flowers] for her birthday.
b. Mathilda gave [the children] [a pumpkin].
c. The book shows [its readers] [all of the most important monuments].
d. Dad baked [the children] [a birthday cake].
Both objects are part of the predicate and follow the main verb in these
clauses. The first is the indirect object; the second is the direct object.
Again, note that each can consist of one or more words.
Traditionally, the indirect object is viewed as the phrase that represents
the “recipient” of whatever is denoted by the direct object, or the “beneficiary” of whatever situation is denoted by the clause. Thus, his mother is the
recipient of flowers according to (a) above, and the children represents the
beneficiary of dad’s baking in (d).
Exercise
1. Put brackets around both complete objects in each of the following clauses, and indicate which phrase is the direct and which is the
indirect object. For example, She gave [the dog](Indirect) [a bone]
(Direct).
a. We offer all our customers unparalleled values.
b. The store rented them the video.
c. Show me your etchings!
d. Bring my friend another beer!
e. Send the senator a protest letter!
62
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
2. Pronouns are words like I, you, he, she, it, they, and their relatives
me, your, him, her, its, them, and the like. Substitute an appropriate
pronoun for the italicized phrases in the clauses below. For example,
James Joyce wrote Ulysses and Finnegan’s Wake > He wrote them.
a. Hemingway admired Faulkner.
b. Navratilova beat Evert.
c. The women outnumber the men.
Your response to the last exercise should have been:
He admired him.
She beat her.
They outnumber them.
We want you to notice that you chose different pronoun forms to replace
subject and object phrases. By form we mean the observable grammatical
characteristics of expressions, such as their pronunciation (e.g., compáct,
cómpact), what endings they have (e.g., -ize on verbs such as realize), what
endings they may add (e.g., -s indicating plural on nouns), where they appear in sentences (e.g., him occurs as an object), and what kinds of expressions can substitute for them (e.g., pronouns for noun phrases, do (so) for
predicates).
Clauses may contain one or more modifiers, also called adverbials or
adjuncts, which may take several different grammatical forms and which
may occur in several different positions:
Proceed cautiously/with caution. [Adverbial at end]
Linguists write slowly. [Adverbial at end]
Unfortunately, grammarians frequently disagree. [Adverbial at beginning
and in middle]
One can easily find biographies of all the presidents. [Adverbial in middle]
The papers have been carefully selected and edited/with taste. [Adverbial
in middle and at end]
[In the nineteen twenties,] [at the age of fourteen,] Seamus Murphy began
his apprenticeship. [Adverbials at beginning]
Exercise
1. Put brackets around each entire modifier in each of the following
63
Delahunty and Garvey
clauses. Where a clause contains more than one modifier, identify
the individual ones separately. For example, [Fortunately], they will
[eventually] get well.
a. Move quickly!
b. At the end of your studies, you’ll get a job overseas.
c. Guglielmo Marconi created the radio transmitter despite widespread skepticism.
d. In 1894, Marconi sent a wireless signal more than 1.5 miles.
e. By the end of his life, Marconi had acquired great wealth.
2. In each of the following clauses put brackets around the complete
subject, complete object(s), main verb, and the complete expression
of any modifiers. Do not identify modifiers within subjects, objects,
or other modifiers. For example, [The astronauts] (Subject) [landed]
(Main verb) [their spaceship] (Direct object) [safely] (Modifier).
a. The Insectoids landed yesterday.
b. These space travelers reached the planet Armeron.
c. Below the surface, they get a real shock.
d. In this strange world, a powerful new energy source pulses.
e. A few jolts from these Volt Stones supply all their power.
f. Giant insects prowl the planet.
g. To protect themselves, the Insectoids disguise their spaceships.
h. They build a bug-zapping trap. (Adapted from Lego web site)
Besides their main verbs, clauses may also contain auxiliary or “helping”
verbs: be, have, do, can, could, may, might, shall, should, will, would, must.
These occur before, and in addition to, the main verb:
Sentences may contain auxiliary verbs.
Books are written by obsessive compulsives.
Do all tigers eat meat?
Would you please close the door?
We can offer these books at the special price of $19.95.
A clause may contain several auxiliary verbs (up to four, in fact):
She [could] [be] lost in the forest.
He [has] [been] waiting all his life to meet his soul mate.
He [should] [have] [been] spending his time on more important things.
64
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
Exercise
1. Identify any auxiliary verb(s) in each of the following clauses. For
example, We [are] expecting rain.
a. Oscar is waiting for a train.
b. Frieda has finished all her homework.
c. The Insectoids have built a protective shield.
d. He must be staying for dinner.
e. She could have been arrested by the TSA officers.
2. Easy question: Where do(es) the auxiliary verb(s) occur relative to
the subject and the main verb?
3. Turn each of the clauses (a) through (e) in Exercise (1) just above
into a question. For example, Is Oscar waiting for a train?
4. What happens to the relative order of subject and auxiliary verb in
the creation of these questions?
5. When a clause contains more than one auxiliary verb, which one is
affected by the creation of a question? (NB (d) and (e) in Exercise (1).)
The verbs be, have, and do may be either auxiliary or main verbs. If one of
them is the only or the last verb in a clause, then it is a main verb:
The concert was a great success.
The Insectoids have a new power source.
John did the dishes.
The reading series has been wonderful.
Frieda has already had her lunch.
Oscar is doing his homework.
The kid may be doing poorly at school.
She should still be in the library.
Clause types
At this point we will distinguish seven types of clauses. We begin with the
voice distinction between active and passive versions of sentences. Sentence
(a) is active and (b) is its passive version:
a. Richard Dawkins wrote A Devil’s Chaplain.
65
Delahunty and Garvey
b. A Devil’s Chaplain was written by Richard Dawkins.
Note that the passive version has a form of be (was) as an auxiliary verb and a
phrase beginning with by. Note also that the subject of the passive (A Devil’s
Chaplain) corresponds to the object of the active, and that the object of by,
(Richard Dawkins) corresponds to the subject of the active.
The by phrase is optional; it can often be omitted without reducing the
sentence to a fragment:
c. Someone has abandoned this car.
d. This car has been abandoned.
We distinguish three sentence types according to different moods: the
declarative, the interrogative, and the imperative. Sentences (a) through (f )
are declarative (a.k.a. indicative):
e. John should be here soon.
f. Fred left early.
Note that the subject of (e), John, is before the auxiliary verb should, and in
(f ) the subject Fred is before the main verb left. Declarative sentences are
typically used to make assertions, although like all sentence types, they can
be used for several purposes.
Imperative sentences contain neither a subject nor an auxiliary verb:
g. Get up!
h. Stop that!
Imperative sentences are typically used to give orders, directions, and instructions. For instance, the instructions in cookbooks are typically written
in the imperative: Mix the chopped apple with the other ingredients and pour
into greased pan.
There are two main types of interrogative sentences, Yes/No interrogatives (so called because they are used when a yes or no answer is expected),
and Wh- or Information interrogatives, which are used when a more elaborate answer than just yes or no is expected. Sentence (i) is a Yes/No interrogative. Note that its auxiliary verb will comes before its subject, John:
i. Will John be here soon?
66
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
Remember that when a sentence contains more than one auxiliary verb, it is
the first of these that moves to the left of the subject:
j. Will John be leaving soon?
(k) is a Wh-interrogative. It begins with what and its auxiliary verb, should,
occurs before its subject, you:
k. What should you be doing now?
A clause’s polarity has to do with whether it is positive or negative. Examples (a-k) are all positive. Negative clauses are created by placing not after
the first auxiliary, should in (l):
l. You should not be sleeping here.
The first auxiliary and not can be contracted into a single word:
m. You shouldn’t be sleeping here.
Negative sentences may be declarative, like (l) and (m). They can also be
interrogative:
n. Shouldn’t you be sleeping over there?
And imperative:
o. Don’t touch me!
Note the appearance of don’t in the negative imperative.
And (p) is both negative and passive:
p. Rome wasn’t built in a day.
Exercise
For each of the following sentences say whether it is (i) active or passive; (ii) declarative, interrogative, or imperative; and (iii) positive or
negative. For example, Couldn’t it have been left behind? Passive, in-
67
Delahunty and Garvey
terrogative, negative.
a. House Republicans have a new leader.
b. Can I borrow your mulcher?
c. Call us at 555-3232.
d. Heroic leadership is not required.
e. Children from Jamaica have been barred from the National Spelling
Bee.
f. Jody-Anne was the first Jamaican winner.
g. Buy more, save more.
h. Was the case made well enough by House managers for a conviction?
i. Area hospitals have been warned of mislabeled drugs.
sentences
In written English and other familiar languages, sentences are quite easy to
recognize: they typically begin with a capital letter and end with a period or its
equivalent. From a linguistic point of view, sentences are the largest grammatical units. They are also intuitively complete in two senses: first, they may stand
alone as informative units, and second, and more importantly, they include all
required grammatical elements. This paragraph is composed of sentences, and
each of the following is a sentence:
Cats purr.
Tigers eat meat.
Some birds cannot fly.
Journalists think that they do important work.
Bill Gates is fighting for the rights of all capitalists.
If wishes were cars, I’d drive a Ferrari.
Language learners and beginning writers commonly leave out parts of
sentences, creating fragments, like:
68
Cats.
Meat.
Fly.
That they do important work.
For the rights of all capitalists.
If wishes were cars.
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
Some fragments may be perfectly appropriate in certain contexts, for example, as answers to questions:
What animals purr? Cats.
What do tigers eat? Meat.
What is Bill Gates fighting for? For the rights of all capitalists.
Exercise
For each expression below, say whether it is a sentence or a fragment.
Explain how you decided. For example, Lipitor reduces the risk of
stroke. (Sentence because it includes both subject [Lipitor] and predicate [reduces the risk of stroke]). About Lipitor. (Fragment because it
has no main verb and therefore no subject and predicate).
a. The Jacket has a fully adjustable hood.
b. All seams are tape-sealed.
c. Fully waterproof, full of features.
d. Breathable Nylon Rainwear—lightweight, packable, affordable.
e. That is Land’s End Rainwear.
f. Both the Jacket and Pants come with a stuff sack.
g. Sized to fit over light layers.
h. The Pants have a full mesh lining, an elastic waist with drawcord,
two side pockets, and side ankle zips.
i. Imported.
(Adapted from Land’s End Catalog The Real Stuff February 1999, p.
49.)
Sentences come in a variety of shapes and sizes. Some consist of only a
single clause. That is, they are simple sentences and have only one main verb,
one subject, and one predicate, though they may include modifiers of various
sorts. A useful way to begin identifying clauses in sentences is to count main
verbs. For each main verb there will be a clause.
Linguists write slowly.
Dogs chase cats.
Peter O’Neill is missing.
Cyclists should always wear bike helmets.
69
Delahunty and Garvey
Other sentences are coordinate (a.k.a. compound) because they combine two or more clauses or smaller sentences within them by connecting
them with and, but, or or:
[Dogs chase cats] but [cats chase mice].
[Elizabeth Bowen lived between England and Ireland all her life] and
[her death marked the end of the Anglo-Irish literary tradition].
Complex sentences also contain two or more clauses, but at least one
of them is subordinate to another in the sense that it plays a grammatical
role such as subject, object, or modifier in the larger sentence. Clauses that
function as objects are often referred to as complement clauses. In the following examples the subordinate sentence is italicized and its role is given in
parentheses:
That this is a witty and entertaining book does not justify its high price.
(Subject)
Oscar thinks that Lady Bracknell is a fine creation. (Object/Complement)
To improve your stamina, jog five miles every day. (Modifier)
I read your short story, although you asked me not to. (Modifier)
While researching the history of the castle, Robertson unearthed one of the
great scandals of twentieth century Scotland. (Modifier)
If you think carefully about it, language is extraordinarily complex. (Modifier)
When you leave, shut the door behind you. (Modifier)
Exercise
1. For each of the following sentences say whether it is simple,
compound/coordinate, or complex. For example, Tony was eating dinner when the police barged in. Complex because it contains a main
clause (Tony was eating dinner) and a subordinate clause (when the
police barged in).
a. A nice seasonal note emerged from the Vatican.
b. Three of the curia’s computers are known as Raphael, Michael, and
Gabriel.
c. Raphael maintains what is known as a firewall and it protects the
Pope’s website from hackers.
d. We thank you for your custom throughout 2002 and we look forward to your patronage again in 2003.
70
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
2. Put brackets around the entire subordinate clause in each of the
following sentences. For example, [When he died in 2005,] Wilson had
just finished his play “Radio Golf.”
a. Journalists think that they do important work.
b. John has read two books since he woke up.
c. That Fred would do such a thing is very surprising.
d. If wishes were cars, I would drive a Ferrari.
3. Put brackets around the entire subordinate clause(s) in each of
the following sentences, and for each one you identify, say whether
it functions as a subject, object/complement, or modifier. For example, You might think [that they had other things on their minds]
(Object/complement).
a. When you fly long distances, you get jet lag.
b. Many people believe that politicians are dishonest.
c. That she is late is extremely distressing.
d. If I had enough money, I would buy myself the latest David Brown
automobile.
The term clause is used to refer to simple sentences. It is also used to refer to sentences within larger sentences. We refer to the clause that all other
clauses are subordinate to as the main clause. So we can speak of main, subordinate, and coordinate clauses in sentences. Would you marry me anyway
is the main clause of the sentence If I were a carpenter and you were a lady,
would you marry me anyway?
Exercise
In each of the following examples, put brackets around each clause
and then say for each whether it is a main clause, a subordinate clause,
or a coordinate clause. For example, [Linguists believe] (Main clause)
[that they understand the nature of language pretty well] (Subordinate clause).
a. Journalists think that they do important work.
b. If wishes were cars, I’d drive a Ferrari.
c. Cats purr and tigers eat meat.
d. When you arrive, call us.
71
Delahunty and Garvey
Some sentences include both coordinate and subordinate clauses. These
are called compound/complex sentences:
[S1 When he’s good, he’s very, very good] but [S2 when he’s bad, he’s horrid]
In this example, but coordinates S1 and S2, however, S1 and S2 each contain a modifying subordinate clause beginning with when (italicized).
Exercise
In each of the following examples, put brackets around each clause and
then say whether it is a main clause, a subordinate clause, or a coordinate clause. For example, [Mahogony Gamble says] (Main clause) [that
membership has exploded] (Subordinate clause) and [that many new
members are from non-Buddhist families] (Subordinate clause)—the
two subordinate clauses are coordinated/conjoined by and.
a. Biologists believe that tigers can swim and that dodoes are extinct.
b. If it rains and the weather gets cold, we won’t go for our hike.
c. Susan felt pretty bad, but when she went to the doctor, he
couldn’t find anything wrong with her.
d. Although the house is Wi-Fi, the laptop can’t access the internet
unless the main PC is also logged on.
phrases
Phrases are grammatical units composed of one or more words that function as unified parts of clauses. They are traditionally viewed as not containing both a subject and a predicate, e.g., this old house, the Sopranos, crime
scene investigator, should do important work, in a balloon, very wicked, are all
phrases. But this old and should do important are not complete phrases.
Exercise
For each of the following items say whether it is a phrase, a sentence,
or a clause. For example, Wherever you go, you can now email (Sentence); At your service (Phrase); that the clock isn’t correct (Clause).
a. A Christmas story from Belfast.
b. At 30,000 feet.
c. Extraordinarily beautiful.
72
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
d.
e.
f.
g.
Come out with your hands up!
The administration should have performed better.
Question:
What do you call a group of chess grand masters who are bragging
to each other in a hotel lobby?
h. Answer:
i. Chess nuts boasting in an open foyer.
Phrases are constructed around a head word, that is, the main word of
the phrase. This may be the only word in the phrase, like Cats and purr in
Cats purr. Phrases may also include modifiers, objects/complements.
the café (Head: café, Modifier: the)
the all-night café (Head: café, Modifiers: the, all-night)
in the all-night café (Head: in, Object/Complement: the all-night café)
the old man in the all-night café (Head: man, Modifiers: the, old, in the
all-night café)
important work (Head: work, Modifier: important)
do important work (Head: do, Object/Complement: important work)
the wood-shed (Head: wood-shed, Modifier: the)
to the wood-shed (Head: to, Object/Complement: the wood-shed)
Exercise
For each of the following phrases, mark the head word with H, any
modifiers with M, and any objects/complements with O. Make sure to
identify the entire expression in each instance.
For example, [The] [cat] [in the hat].
(M) (H)
(M)
a. Happy Christmas
b. a steep hill
c. the experts in the government
d. a meeting in the new conference room
e. free appraisal
f. the only word in the phrase
g. celebrating our 20th anniversary
h. to the mountains
73
Delahunty and Garvey
complementation and modification
Words, phrases, and sentences play several roles in language, two of which
are complementation and modification. We have already seen these roles in
examples, but have not discussed them per se. We will do that here.
Complementation
When one element in an expression creates the expectation that another expression will also occur, the expected element complements (i.e.,
completes) the expecting element. (Note the spelling—complements not
compliments.) For example, a preposition like on requires a phrase denoting notions such as location or time, as in on the pavement, on time, on
your mark; and a verb such as buy requires a phrase denoting something
bought, as in buy lunch.
Exercise
Identify the entire complement phrase in each of the following expressions. For example, to [the store]:
a. for better or worse
b. keep the change
c. during the storm
d. sell your car
e. at an important juncture
Modification
A word alone denotes an entire class of things, for example, motorcycle
denotes the entire class of things we call motorcycles. When one expression is modified by another, then, generally the two expressions together
denote only a subset of the things denoted by the unmodified expression.
For example, in the phrase, Harley-Davidson motorcycles, the head, motorcycles, is modified by Harley-Davidson, and consequently the whole phrase
denotes just those motorcycles that are Harley-Davidsons.
Phrases may include many modifiers, as in tall, black, neutered, male,
domestic, short-haired cat. Here we have six modifiers each restricting the
potential reference of the word cat. The result of piling on these modifiers is
that the referent of the phrase must satisfy all of them—it must be a cat that
is tall, black, neutered, male, domestic, and short-haired. Each modifier acts
like a criterion that the ultimate referent(s) of the phrase must satisfy.
Notice that modifiers are not required or implied by the expressions
74
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
they modify. These expressions would be grammatically complete without
the modifiers—though of course adding or removing modifiers affects the
meaning and consequently the referents of the modified expressions.
Exercise
Identify the modifier(s) in each of the following expressions. For example, [an] [excellent] wine:
a. expensive tastes
b. barely awake
c. drive quickly
d. someone special
e. little lame baloonman (e.e. cummings)
w o rd s
Words are the units from which phrases are constructed. In ordinary written English they are generally separated from each other by spaces. All the
items separated by spaces in this paragraph are words.
Words can be created in a number of ways. Some, like cat, are internally
quite simple. Others are created by combining two or more words together
to create another word. For example, rainfall is composed of rain and fall; all
three are separate words. Words created in this way are called compounds
or compound words.
Exercise
The following are compound words. Note that some are spelled without
internal spaces, some with hyphens, and some with internal spaces,
separating their constituent words. Separate them into their component
words. For example, Peace Corps consists of Peace and Corps. (This is an
extremely easy exercise. It is designed to get you accustomed to thinking about the various spelling formats for compounds words.)
boy-friend, fishing rod, holding pattern, pickpocket, kill-joy, nosedive, make-believe, fast-food, software, now generation, put-down,
drawback, son-in-law, forget-me-not, carbon-date, color-code, testmarket, free-associate, double-book, overbook, overeducate, badmouth, childproof, leadfree, fail-safe, ready-made, over-qualified. (L.
Bauer 1983 pp. 202-213).
75
Delahunty and Garvey
morphemes
Words may be constructed from one or more morphemes. Morphemes are
the smallest forms (i.e., spoken and/or written units) in a language that have
meanings or grammatical functions. (Note: they are not the smallest units of
meaning.) Cat is a word consisting of one morpheme, cat. Cats consists of
two morphemes, cat and -s. Inactive contains three: in-, act, and -ive.
From the point of view of their functions in words, morphemes may be
divided into three classes: derivational, inflectional, and root. Adding a
derivational morpheme to a word or to another morpheme creates a separate, though related, word. For example, adding the derivational morpheme
­–er to the word read creates the word reader. In the following examples,
the derivational morphemes are in bold: man-hood, king-dom, act-or, antithet-ic-al, act-ive, re-act-or, act-iv-ate, wise-ly. Notice that there may be
several derivational morphemes in a word.
While adding derivational morphemes to a root or word creates a separate word, adding an inflectional morpheme merely creates a modified
version of the word to which it is added. Inflections are added to words to
indicate such things as plural, past tense, or comparison. They are bolded
in the following examples: paint-ed, book-s, small-er. Modern English uses
only eight inflectional morphemes:
-s plural of nouns: coats
-’s genitive of noun phrases: Harry’s, the kid next door’s
-er comparative of short adjectives and adverbs: faster
-est superlative of short adjectives and adverbs: fastest
-s third person, singular, present tense of verbs: sleeps
-ed regular past tense of verbs: pointed
-ing progressive marker on verbs (occurs with be): is eating
-ed/-en past participle marker of verbs (occurs with have and passive be):
has eaten, has asked, was challenged
The root morpheme of a word is the morpheme left over when all derivational and inflectional morphemes have been removed. Thus, seem is what
remains when we remove the derivational morphemes {-ing} and {-ly} from
seemingly, and must therefore be its root.
If an inflectional morpheme occurs in an English word, it will always
follow the root and any derivational morphemes, as in:
tele-phon-ist-s
D R D I
76
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
A morpheme attached before the root of a word is said to be prefixed; a
morpheme attached after the root of a word is said to be suffixed.
“Word” is ambiguous. On one meaning, phone and phones would be two
separate words, but on another meaning, they would be different versions of
the same word. We can eliminate the ambiguity with a little lexical ingenuity. We’ll say that the inflected forms of a word are word forms, and we’ll
call the word that they are inflected forms of a lexeme. This implies that
derivationally related words are different lexemes.
Exercise
1. The expressions listed below are derived words. Identify their roots
with R and the derivational morphemes with D. For example, [fail] (R)
[ure] (D); [de] (D) [act] (R) [iv] (D) [ate] (D).
leadership, heroic, national, statement, music, musical, legal, legality, legislator, customizing, setting, enable, disable, disability.
2. Identify the individual morphemes in the following words. Mark roots
with R, derivational morphemes with D, and inflectional morphemes
with I. For example, [person] (R) [al] (D) [s] (I).
telephonic, dusted, repainted, leaders, expectations, surprises, believers, waiting.
parts of speech
Words can be grouped into parts of speech or word classes. Traditional
grammars of English and other western European languages usually recognize eight such classes: nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs, prepositions,
pronouns, articles, and conjunctions. Some add a ninth, interjections,
though strictly speaking, this is a use made of many different types of expressions, and not a part of speech at all.
The parts of speech may be gathered into two groups, the lexical (a.k.a.
major) word classes (nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs) and the grammatical (a.k.a. minor) word classes (prepositions, pronouns, articles, conjunctions). Lexical words are an open class in the sense that new words may be
added fairly freely. The grammatical classes are closed, meaning that it is
harder to add new members to them. The lexical class words also convey
the main meaning elements of sentences, whereas the grammatical classes
tend to perform grammatical functions such as relating expressions to each
other: for example, of relates the class to top in top of the class. The meanings
77
Delahunty and Garvey
associated with grammatical words are often referred to as grammatical
meanings to distinguish them from the lexical meanings associated with
lexical words.
The noun class includes such words as book, coat, dog, human, milk, freedom. Nouns are traditionally viewed as words that denote persons, places,
ideas, and things.
Verbs include words such as eat, leave, know, be, have, own, cough. They
are traditionally viewed as words that denote actions and states of being.
Adjectives include words such as old, wise, red, attractive, friendly. They
denote qualities and are traditionally defined as words that modify nouns.
Adverbs include wisely, attractively, regretfully, rapidly, wildly, knowledgeably, frequently. They are traditionally defined as words that modify verbs,
though they also modify adjectives, adverbs, and sentences.
Prepositions include up, to, toward, along, by, with, onto. These denote
direction, instrumentality, and a number of other such notions.
The conjunctions divide into two classes: coordinating (and, but, or)
and subordinating (that, if). Conjunctions join expressions to each other
in various ways.
Pronouns divide into several subtypes, of which we’ll mention only one
here: personal pronouns (I, you, he, she, it, they and their variant forms).
Wh-words include who, whom, whose, what, when, where, why, how.
English uses two types of article: definite (the) and indefinite (a). Other
languages may use one (Irish) or none (Latin).
Auxiliary verbs include be, have, do and the modal auxiliaries: will,
would, can, could, shall, should, may, might, must.
We will critically review the traditional definitions of these word classes
in our chapters on Major and Minor Parts of speech.
Exercise
Using the descriptions and lists just above, identify the part of speech
of each of the following words. For example, songbird (noun).
may, an, whose, her, but, if, from, strangely, strange, write, writer,
octopus, the, their, must, indefinitely, because, mysterious, wander,
how.
re g u l a r a n d i r re g u l a r f o rm s
Regular forms are those that follow the general patterns of the language;
irregular forms are those that do not. Regular English nouns are marked
78
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
for plural by the addition of -(e)s: book, books; class, classes. Irregular nouns
have numerous plural forms: child, children; mouse, mice; man, men; alumna,
alumnae; cherub, cherubim. Some of these irregulars derive from historically
earlier regular patterns: ox, oxen; others are due to the effects of borrowing:
corpus, corpora; datum, data.
Regular verbs form their past tense and past participle forms by adding
-(e)d: kiss, kissed; owe, owed. Irregular verb forms generally derive from earlier English patterns: ring, rang, rung; buy, bought; dive, dove.
Exercise
Give five more examples of irregular nouns and verbs.
grammatical categories
We have already mentioned the grammatical categories of plural, tense, person, voice, mood, and polarity. A fuller list of grammatical categories, their
meanings, and further examples includes:
Number (whether one or more than one entity is being referred to):
singular (book) and plural (books)
(relevant to nouns, pronouns, verbs)
Gender (whether the entity or entities being referred to are male, female,
or neither): masculine (he), feminine (she), neuter (it)
(relevant to pronouns, nouns)
Person (whether the speaker, addressee, or some other entity is being
referred to): first (I), second (you), third (they)
(relevant to pronouns and verbs)
Case (whether the pronoun or noun is the subject, object, or modifier in
its clause): nominative (I), objective (me), genitive (my, mine)
(relevant to pronouns, nouns, and noun phrases)
Tense (whether the past, the present, or the future is being referred to):
present (kiss/es, drive/s), past (kissed, drove), future (will kiss; may drive)
(relevant to verbs)
Aspect (the name given to linguistic forms that indicate characteristics of
situations such as whether they are spread out over a period of time,
i.e., progressive, or completed but still relevant, i.e., perfect):
progressive (is kissing)
perfect (has eaten)
(relevant to verbs)
79
Delahunty and Garvey
Voice (whether the subject of a clause represents the entity responsible
for the event, i.e., active, or affected by it, i.e., passive):
active (We have eliminated polio.)
passive (Polio has been eliminated.)
(relevant to whole clauses)
Mood (having to do with the kinds of clauses typically used to make
statements, ask questions, or give orders/directions):
indicative (The snows have melted.)
interrogative (Have you ever been to Europe?)
imperative (Take out the garbage!)
(relevant to whole sentences)
Polarity (whether a clause is positive or negative):
positive (The package arrived.)
negative (The package didn’t arrive.)
(relevant to whole clauses)
Exercise
1. For each of the following words identify its number, gender, person,
and case, where relevant. For example, he: singular, masculine, third
person, nominative/subject case.
its, Mary’s, she, him.
2. For each of the following clauses identify its tense, aspect, voice,
mood, and polarity, where relevant. For instance, She hasn’t been seen
by anyone since last Friday: present, perfect, passive, indicative, negative.
a. We will be arriving late tonight.
b. Ahmed is staying with us till Sunday.
c. Fritz overstayed his welcome.
d. Is James in his study?
e. Enjoy yourselves!
f. Don’t leave home without it!
g. Hasn’t Oscar been arrested yet?
sounds and spelling
Sounds and spelling must be kept conceptually distinct. We should always
be clear about whether we are discussing sounds or letters. Long before anyone wrote, people spoke to each other. When we learned our first language,
80
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
we learned the spoken version before we learned the written one. When
we learned to read and write, we probably learned to associate letters with
sounds, first one letter to a sound (and vice versa). For example, we learned
to use the letter <s> to represent its most typical hissing sound, [s]. Later we
learned that many letters are associated with several sounds (and vice versa).
For example, <c> sometimes represents the sound typically associated with
<s>, as in city, and sometimes the sound typically associated with <k>, as in
cat, and sometimes both, as in electric [k] and electricity [s].
Sounds are more basic to language than letters; letters represent sounds,
not the other way around. Having said that, however, we must also acknowledge that in highly literate societies such as ours, the written version
of language has more prestige than the spoken version—because it is generally associated with more prestigious functions. We are so literate that even
when we are required to pay attention to the sounds of our languages, we are
strongly influenced by how they are spelled; we tend to “see” sounds rather
than to hear them. We hope that our chapter on Phonetics and Phonology
will redress that imbalance.
Letters, as we mentioned, are visual symbols for sounds. English has approximately 40 sounds that it uses to distinguish words from each other. If
we were to design an ideal writing system for a language like English, we
might consider associating each significant sound with a single letter. However, our letters derive from Latin, which got them from Etruscan, which
got them from Greek, which got them from Phoenician, so they’ve been
around the block quite a bit and don’t fit the language very consistently.
There are only 26 of them to represent the 40 or so sounds. This disparity
is particularly acute among the vowels, for which we have just 6 symbols
(counting <y>) to represent about a dozen different vowel sounds. <i> does
at least double duty; it represents different sounds in bit and bite, (the “silent
<e>” alerts us to the difference). And many sounds are represented in several
ways: the sound [f ], as in father, may be represented by the letters <f> in
frankly, <ph> in physics, and <gh> in enough. While the letters themselves came to English (and other European languages) from Latin, English “borrowed” lots of words from lots of other languages, often keeping a version of the original language’s spelling. The result is
that the way we spell particular sounds often depends on the word the sound
is in and where that word came from. For example, the sound [f ] is written as
the letter <f> in native English words such as feather and finger. However, the
same sound is spelled <ph> in many words that were borrowed into English
from Greek, such as phone or Philadelphia.
The sounds used to distinguish words from each other in a language are
81
Delahunty and Garvey
called phonemes: [p] and [b] distinguish pat and bat so they are English
phonemes.
But phonemes are pronounced differently in different contexts. If you
listen carefully, you will notice that the [p] of pat differs from the [p] of spat.
The former [p] is considerably breathier than the latter. The different pronunciations of a phoneme are its allophones. The pronunciation differences
among the allophones of a phoneme do not distinguish among words in a
language. For instance, there is no pair of separate English words which are
identical except that where one member of the pair has breathy [p] where
the other has non-breathy [p].
Exercise
Identify three more instances in which English spells a single sound in
more than one way and three more instances in which an English letter
or group of letters represents more than one sound.
f o rm , f u n c t i o n , a n d m e a n i n g
Our discussion has depended on concepts that we must now discuss somewhat more fully. These are the notions of form (observable grammatical
characteristics), function (role or relation in an expression), and meaning
(what an expression denotes or refers to; the information encoded by an
expression).
Form
The notion of form refers to the observable elements that make an object
what it is. A toothbrush, for instance, has the formal features of a 5”–6” long
narrow handle with a bunch of bristles at one end. Individual toothbrushes
might have other formal characteristics such as a particular color, particular
stiffness of bristles, devices for massaging the gums, etc., but only the first two
formal features must be present for something to be identified as a toothbrush.
In grammar, we make use of formal features to identify the category that an
expression belongs to. In the study of parts of speech, for example, the form
of a word comprises its observable linguistic properties. Besides whether it is
spoken, gestured, or written, these formal features include:
a. Actual and potential inflectional elements, e.g., looks, looked, looking
b. Actually occurring derivational elements, e.g., goodness, goalkeeper
82
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
c. Stress (emphasis), e.g., cómpact, compáct
d. Potential position in grammatical structures, e.g., adjectives may occur before the nouns they modify
e. Potential for grammatical operations such as substitution, e.g., the woman
may be “replaced” by the pronoun she in a sentence.
These features serve as the basic criteria for identifying parts of speech. For
instance, consider how we might assign the word realize to the class of verbs.
First, it has the potential to accept inflectional endings typical of verbs: realizes, realized, realizing. It also ends in the verb-creating ending -ize. The word
can occur after other words commonly associated with verbs: will realize, has
realized, to realize. Finally, it can be replaced (along with its associated elements) by the form do (so)(too), as in She realized that “The Simpsons” was more
than just a cartoon, and so did he.
Note that in this example not all of the criteria are useful. Stress tells us
little about the part of speech of realize. But in words such as convért (verb)
and cónvert (noun), stress provides a useful clue. Much of the skill involved in
linguistic analysis requires the use of appropriate formal tests. Applying them
shows that:
a. Any word is potentially a member of several different parts of speech,
e.g., hit can be both a noun and a verb.
b. The meaning of a word provides no reliable clue to its part of speech,
e.g., verbs are often characterized as words that denote actions or
states, but the words actions and states are both nouns.
c. Distinguishing the formal properties of words is a necessary preliminary to assigning them to a class.
d. Formal criteria apply in different ways to different words. In the case of
realize, inflectional and derivational clues were available; in the case of
convert, stress information was available. But for a word such as since,
we would have had to rely on its potential positions in sentences.
Function
The functional view of language asks, “What is an expression’s role in its
sentence?” For an analogy, let’s return to our toothbrush. Generally, one
uses a toothbrush to clean one’s teeth. In language, function designates
the role that an expression plays in a larger unit, in particular, the relationship of the expression in question to other expressions in the larger
unit. Table 1 identifies the main functions used in this text.
83
Delahunty and Garvey
Subject
Direct Object
Object of Preposition
Modifier
Predicate
Indirect Object
Complement
Head
table 1. major grammatical functions
The modifier-head relation may exist between many different pairs of
parts of speech. For example (the head word appears in italics):
a. reliable source (Adjective—Noun)
b. something strange (Noun—Adjective)
c. ran swiftly (Verb—Adverb)
d. cautiously approached (Adverb—Verb)
e. very reliable (Intensifier—Adjective)
f. very cautiously (Intensifier—Adverb)
A head word serves as the main element of the construction that contains
it; modifiers qualify a head. The head is thus the “syntactic center” of the
construction. If the head changes, other parts of a sentence containing the
construction may be affected:
a. A reliable source is hard to find.
b. Reliable sources are hard to find.
The use of the singular source calls for an article (a or the) and a singular verb,
is; the plural sources is incompatible with a and requires a plural verb, are.
Reliable has no effect on these changes. In some cases, e.g., (c,d) above, modifiers can appear either before or after the head. Finally, some words (such as
very and quite) nearly always function as modifiers. As in much grammatical
analysis, there is no single formal characteristic for a modifier-head relation.
Each instance calls for the use of different principles.
Functions can also occur within functions, as in, [[very reliable] source].
In this case, we have two headwords, reliable and source. Reliable serves as the
headword of the construction very reliable and source serves as the headword
of very reliable source, in which very reliable is analyzed as a unified modifier.
Our analysis can be represented clearly in a tree diagram:
84
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
CONSTRUCTION
very reliable source
MODIFIER
very reliable
MODIFIER
HEAD
very
reliable
HEAD
source
As we will observe later, tree diagrams are used also to represent layers of
grammatical structure based on formal grounds. This mode of representation
is more common in modern linguistics than the Reed-Kellogg (functional)
diagrams you may be familiar with from school grammar lessons. Thus the
phrase above might be formally labeled as in:
NP
very reliable source
AP
very reliable
INTENS
very
N
ADJ
reliable
source
(NP = noun phrase; AP = adjective phrase; N = noun; ADJ = adjective;
INTENS = intensifier)
85
Delahunty and Garvey
The headword will typically determine the grammatical category of the
phrase that it heads. Thus a phrase with a noun head will be a noun phrase;
one with a verb head will be a verb phrase; and so on.
Meaning
The third perspective on grammar is that of meaning, i.e., the definition of an
expression or the information potentially communicated by it. In this topic,
we include the meaning of words, phrases, and whole sentences. A simple
example of lexical (word) meaning used in grammatical analysis is the familiar
definition of a verb as a word that denotes “action or state of being.” Another
familiar example is the traditional definition of the subject of a clause as the
“doer of an action.” Several chapters in this book will say more about such
constructional meanings in regard to phrases, clauses, and entire sentences.
Our position in this book is that the study of grammar can take place most
successfully when built on a solid base of formal analysis. Thus our chapters
on parts of speech identify parts of speech primarily by analyzing their forms.
Our emphasis on form does not at all imply that function or meaning is
unimportant to language. Exactly the opposite is true, and we do include
functional and semantic commentary on parts of speech and other grammatical forms. However, we will not rely solely or even primarily on function or
meaning as criteria for identification. As we proceed to phrases and clauses,
we will be able to make a more extensive use of the functional and meaningful properties of grammar, precisely because of our strong formal foundation.
Our goal is to help you make appropriate use of all these facets in your thinking and teaching about language.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Bauer, Laurie. 1983. English Word-formation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Biber, Douglas, Susan Conrad, and Geoffrey Leech. 2002. Longman Student
Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, UK: Pearson Education
Limited.
Fromkin, Victoria, Robert Rodman, and Nina Hyams. 2007. An Introduction
to Language. (8th ed.) Boston, MA: Thomson Wadworth.
Huddleston, Rodney and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2005. A Students’ Introduction
to English Grammar. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
O’Grady, William, John Archibald, Mark Aronoff, and Janie Rees-Miller.
2005. Contemporary Linguistics: An Introduction. (5th ed.) Boston, MA:
Bedford/St. Martin’s.
86
A Skeletal Introduction to English Grammar
glossary
clause:
grammatical unit composed of two phrases—subject and predicate.
the phrase traditionally viewed as representing the “recipient” of the action denoted by the main verb, or as representing the entity/
ies directly affected by the event denoted by the main verb. Typically occurs
after the main verb.
discourse: coherent, cohesive, contextualized, and purposeful communicative activity.
form: an expression’s observable characteristics, including actual and potential inflections, actual derivational endings, stress, potential position in
grammatical structures, and potential for grammatical operations.
function: the roles an expression plays in a sentence. Functions include
Subject, Predicate, Direct Object, Indirect Object, Object of a Preposition,
Complement, Adjunct, Modifier, Head.
head: main element in a grammatical construction.
letter: graphic unit, which in an alphabet, approximately represents a phoneme.
lexeme: the basic form of a word; the form that you would look up in a
dictionary (see word form).
meaning: definition of an expression or the information potentially communicated by an expression, studied in semantics and pragmatics.
modifier: an expression qualifying the head of a grammatical construction.
morpheme: the smallest linguistic unit that has a meaning or grammatical
function; composed of one or more phonemes.
phoneme: a contrastive sound unit, more or less adequately represented by
one or more letters of the alphabet.
phrase: a grammatical unit composed of one or more words.
relationship: the role or function of a word or phrase in a sentence.
sentence: the largest unit to which the grammatical rules of a language apply; may be composed of one or more clauses.
text: language produced during discourse; can be produced as written documents or as recordings of spoken communication.
word: a grammatical unit composed of one or more morphemes.
word form: an inflected form of a word (see lexeme).
direct object:
87
4 Phonetics and Phonology
key concepts
Articulatory phonetics, phonetic symbols
Consonants, approximants, vowels
Syllables, feet
Phonology, phonemes, allophones, phonological rules
i n t ro d u c t i o n
In this chapter we sketch the pronunciation system of English. We begin
with phonetics, a system for describing and recording the sounds of language objectively. Phonetics provides a valuable way of opening our ears to
facets of language that we tend to understand by reference to their written
rather than their actual spoken forms. Phonology concerns itself with the
ways in which languages make use of sounds to distinguish words from each
other.
Teachers should be knowledgeable about the phonetics and phonology
of English because (1) the sound system is primary and the basis for the
spelling system; (2) they may have to teach English pronunciation to students who are not native speakers of English; (3) they may have to teach
poetry, which requires that they teach about rhyme, alliteration, assonance,
and other poetic devices that manipulate sound; (4) it is important to understand accents and language variation and to react appropriately to them
and to teach appropriate language attitudes about them to students (see our
chapters on Language and Society and Usage in Book II); (5) we are so literate that we tend to “hear” the sounds of our language through its spelling
system, and phonetics/phonology provides a corrective to that; and (6) phonetics and phonology provide systematic and well-founded understandings
of the sound patterns of English.
articulatory phonetics
We have three goals in this section. First, we introduce you to the ways in
which the sounds of English are produced. Second, we develop a system for
classifying speech sounds on the basis of how they are produced. Simultaneously we introduce an alphabet approximating that developed by the International Phonetics Association (IPA), which will allow us to refer to sounds
quite precisely. When we want to indicate that letters are to be interpreted
as phonetic symbols, we enclose them in square brackets, [ ], and when we
want to indicate that letters are to be interpreted as letters from an ordinary
spelling system, we enclose them in angled brackets, < >.
89
Delahunty and Garvey
The phonetic alphabet uses many of the letters of the English alphabet, but
their pronunciations are very restricted and are not always the ones you might
expect. In this system, there are no “silent” letters—every phonetic symbol
represents an actual sound. Every letter always has the same pronunciation
regardless of its context, no letter has more than one pronunciation, and no
sounds are represented by more than one letter. To make fine distinctions,
phoneticians add special symbols, called diacritics, to the basic letters. For
some English sounds and for languages other than English, symbols not from
the English alphabet have been devised. (You might visit the IPA web site for
a full listing of the symbols.)
In the sections to follow, we describe the sounds represented by these
symbols and how these sounds are made. As we go through these sections,
pay attention to the ways in which individual sounds are ordinarily spelled
in English, as well as to the phonetic spellings.
To produce speech, air must flow from the lungs through the vocal tract,
which includes the vocal folds (popularly called the vocal cords, though
they are more like thick elastic bands than strings), the nose or nasal cavity, and the mouth or oral cavity (See Figure 1). The vocal folds vibrate
for some sounds but not for others. Air flows through the nose for certain
sounds but not others. But the main creator of speech sounds is the mouth.
We will describe the roles that each of these elements plays in the following
paragraphs.
figure 1: vocal apparatus
90
Phonetics and Phonolog y
consonants
Consonants include the sounds we represent as <p, b, t, d, m, n, f, v, s, z,
l, r, h> in the ordinary alphabet. All consonants are produced by entirely
or almost entirely stopping the airstream coming from the lungs. When
we almost entirely stop the airstream we force it through such a narrow
opening that the airflow at that point is turbulent and noisy.
We classify consonants according to the following characteristics: (a)
whether or not the vocal folds are vibrating (voicing); (b) whether the
sound is made with a fully stopped or merely constricted airstream (its manner of articulation); (c) where in the mouth the stoppage or constriction is
made (its place of articulation); (d) whether or not air is flowing through
the nasal cavity (nasality); and (e) whether or not the lips are pursed (liprounding).
Voicing
As a warm-up exercise, make the sound fffff, and keep it going for a count of
five. Now make the sound vvvvv, and keep it going for a count of five. Now
alternate these two: fffffvvvvvfffffvvvvv. You probably noticed that vvvvv
had a “buzz” that fffff did not have. That “buzz” is caused by the vibrating
of your vocal folds—which you can check by putting your fingers on your
throat or by covering your ears as you alternate fffff and vvvvv. Now try the
same exercises with the first sounds of the following words: thigh, thy; sip,
zip. You should be able to feel the vocal folds vibrate as you make the second
sound of each pair.
Sounds produced with vibrating vocal folds (see Figure 1) are said to be
voiced; those produced without vocal cord vibration are voiceless. Table 1
lists the voiced and voiceless consonants of English. The letters in [ ] are the
phonetic symbols for the sounds.
voiced
by [b]
my [m]
wet [w]
vie [v]
thy []
die [d]
nigh [n]
zip [z]
lie [l]
rye [r]
voiceless
pie [p]
fie [f]
thigh [T]
tie [t]
sip [s]
91
Delahunty and Garvey
beige [Z]
bash [S]
jive [dZ]
chive [tS]
yet [j]
guide [g]
kite [k]
gong [N]
hive [h]
table 1: voiced and voiceless consonants
Exercise
1. Collect a set of words in which each of the voiced and voiceless
sounds listed in the two columns above occurs as the first sound of a
word, in the middle of a word (specifically between two vowels), and at
the end of a word, as in: [b] bird, rubbing, rub; [p] pan, tapping, tap.
How are each of these sounds ordinarily spelled? (Note: English single
and double consonants, as in rub and rubbing, tap and tapping, represent the same sound. The doubled consonants tell us how the vowel
before them is to be pronounced; cf. tapping, taping.)
2. Identify the sound represented by each of the following phonetic
symbols and for each sound collect five words in which it occurs: [p,
b, f, v, T, D, S, Z, tS, dZ, s, z]. How is each of these sounds ordinarily
spelled?
Nasality
Make the sound represented by <m> in the word Pam and continue it
for some seconds. As you continue it, pinch your nose and observe what
happens to the sound. It should stop immediately. This shows that air was
flowing through your nose as you produced this sound. Now try the same
little experiment with the <n> of pan and the <ng> of pang. You should find
that the air flows through the nose in these two cases also. Sounds in which
air flows through the nose are called nasal sounds. The air is allowed into
the nose by lowering the velum, the soft palate at the back of the mouth (see
Figure 1). English has three main nasal sounds:
92
[m] Pam
[n] pan
[N] pang
clammy clannish clingy
mat
Nat
----
Phonetics and Phonolog y
Exercise
Using the data just above, say where [N] cannot occur in a word. How
are each of these nasal sounds ordinarily spelled?
Manner of articulation
By manner of articulation we mean the kind of closure or constriction
used in making the sound. We classify English consonants according to
three manners of articulation: stops (full stoppage of the airstream somewhere in the oral cavity between the vocal folds and the lips, as in [p], [b],
[m]); fricatives (constriction of the airstream in the oral cavity producing turbulence and noise, as in [f], [v]); affricates (full stoppage of the
airstream followed immediately by constriction, as in [tS], [dZ]). Table 2
summarizes the different manners of articulation.
Stops
[p] pad
[t] tad
[k] cad
Fricatives
[f] fie
[T] thigh
[s] Sue
[S] shoe
[h] how
[b]
[d]
[g]
bad
dad
gad
[v]
[D]
[z]
[Z]
vie
thy
zoo
jus (au jus)
[m]
[n]
[N]
mat
Nat
tang
Affricates
[tS] chin
[dZ] gin
table 2: manners of articulation
Exercise
For each of the sounds listed in Table 2, collect five words in which the
sound occurs as the last sound of the word and another five in which
the sound occurs in the middle of the word (specifically, between two
93
Delahunty and Garvey
vowels), as [N] is in ring, ringing. How is each sound ordinarily spelled?
Place of articulation
By place of articulation we mean the area in the mouth at which the consonantal closure or constriction occurs. English uses only seven places of
articulation (see Figure 1) which we describe and illustrate below.
Bilabial sounds are made by bringing both lips together to stop the airstream:
[p] pie
[b] by
[m] my
cupping
clubbing
coming
cup
cub
come
Labiodental sounds are made by bringing the top teeth into contact with
the bottom lip and forcing air between the two to create the fricatives:
[f]
[v]
feel veal raffle ravel
tough
dove
Interdental sounds are made by placing the tip of the tongue between
the top and bottom teeth and forcing air through. Again, these are both
fricatives:
[T] thigh ether
mouth bath (noun)
[] thy
either mouth bathe (verb)
Alveolar sounds are made by bringing the tongue and the alveolar ridge
(the bony ridge just behind the top teeth) together to create either a stop or
fricative:
[t] tub
[d] dub
[n] knit
boating
boding
boning
boat [s] sip
bode [z] zip
bone [r] rip
fussy
fuzzy
terror
grace
graze
tear
(Alveo-)palatal sounds are made by bringing the blade of the tongue to,
or close to, the alveo-palatal area of the roof of the mouth to create fricatives
and affricates:
94
Phonetics and Phonolog y
[S] sure vicious
rush
[Z] genre vision
rouge
[tS] chin catcher
etch
[dZ] gin
edger
edge
Velar sounds are created by stopping the airstream by bringing the back
of the tongue into contact with the velum:
[k] could backer
[g] good bagger
[N] ------ banger
tuck
tug
tongue
Glottal sounds are created by either narrowing the vocal folds sufficiently to create a fricative or closing them to create a stop:
[h] hat cahoots
[?] butter (some varieties of English)
Exercise
For each of the sounds listed under Place of Articulation, find five
words in which the sound occurs. How are each of these sounds ordinarily spelled?
Approximants
Approximants are sounds made by narrowing the oral cavity but not enough
to cause turbulence in the airstream; the airstream is said to be smooth. The
beginning sounds of lye and rye are approximants. The narrowest point in
the airstream is wider in approximants than in fricatives, but is not as wide
as it is in vowels. Approximants are more sonorant (resonant, i.e., naturally
loud) than consonants, but less so than vowels. They are like consonants in
that they typically occur before or after the vowels of syllables (see below).
English has three kinds of approximants.
Lateral approximants are made by touching the tongue to the alveolar
ridge while allowing the air to pass along one or both sides, as in [l]—in
lack, call, and callow.
Central approximants are made by raising the sides of the tongue so that
the air flows along the center of the tongue, as in [r]—in rock, roll, and Rory.
[r] is regarded as an alveolar sound.
Glides (semivowels) come in two kinds: palatal and labio-velar. Palatal
95
Delahunty and Garvey
glides are made by raising the tongue toward the hard palate, close to where
the vowel in eat is made. The first sound of yet, yolk, and y’all is a palatal glide,
represented phonetically as [j]. Labio-velar glides are made by rounding the
lips and simultaneously raising the back of the tongue toward the velum,
close to where the vowel sound of ooze is made. Labio-velar glides thus have
two places of articulation—they are both labial and velar. The first sound of
wet, wall, and wink is a labio-velar glide, represented phonetically as [w].
Lateral [l]
let
Central
[r]
Rhett
Glides
Labio-velar
[w]
wet
Palatal
[j]
yet
Articulatory descriptions
An articulatory description of any consonant or approximant must specify
(at least) its place and manner of articulation, whether it is voiced or voiceless, and whether it is nasal or oral. For example, [m] is made at the lips by
stopping the airstream, is voiced, and is nasal. These features are represented
as:
[m] [w]
[l]
Voicing voiced
voiced
voiced
Place bilabial
labio-velar
alveolar
Manner stop
glide
lateral approximant
Nasality nasal
oral
oral
Example word
mime
wow
low
We can gather all of the consonants that we have described into a single
chart:
labio‑ inter‑
(alveo-)
bilabial dental dental alveolar palatal velar glottal
stop pb
td
kg
(?)
nasal stop
m
n
N
fricative
f v
T
sz
S Z
h
affricate
tS dZ
approximants
glides
(w)
j
(w)
lateral
l
central
r
table 3: english consonants and approximants
96
Phonetics and Phonolog y
Exercise
You should now be able to provide an articulator description for each of
the following sounds. Consult Tables 1-3.
[t]
[k]
[b] [d]
[g]
Voicing
Place
Manner
Nasality
Example
word
[n]
[N]
[f]
[v]
[T]
Voicing
Place
Manner
Nasality
Example
word
[ð]
[s]
[z]
[S]
[Z]
Voicing
Place
Manner
Nasality
Example
word
[tS]
[dZ]
[l]
[r]
[h]
Voicing
Place
Manner
Nasality
Example
word
[w]
[j]
Voicing
Place
Manner
Nasality
Example
word
97
Delahunty and Garvey
vowe l s
Vowels include the sounds we ordinarily represent as the letters <a, e, i, o,
u>, as well as a number of other sounds for which the ordinary alphabet has
no unique symbols.
Vowels are distinguished from consonants in several ways. As we have
seen, consonants are produced by constricting the airstream to various degrees as it flows through the oral tract. Vowels are produced with a smooth,
unobstructed airflow through the oral tract.
Differences in vowel quality are produced by different shapes of the oral
cavity. Characteristic vowel qualities are determined by (a) the height of the
tongue in the mouth; (b) the part of the tongue raised (front, middle, or
back); (c) the configuration of the lips; and (d) the tension of the muscles
of the oral tract. An articulatory description of a vowel must include all of
these features.
Tongue height
Pronounce the words eat and at. Now pronounce just the vowels of these
two words. Notice that as you go from the vowel of eat to the vowel of at,
your mouth opens. If this is not obvious to you just by playing with these
two vowels, look in a mirror as you produce them. Alternate the words, and
then just the two vowels.
Once you’ve become accustomed to the different degrees of openness of
these two vowels, pronounce ate between eat and at. The degree of openness of its vowel falls between those of eat and at, so there is a continuous
increase in mouth openness as you go from one vowel to another. These
degrees distinguish high, mid, and low vowels. We will use the following
symbols for this sequence of vowels:
(1)
eat
ate
at
[i] [e]
[]
High
Mid
Low
Exercise
For each of the three vowels above, find five words in which the vowel
occurs. Be clear about which symbol most accurately applies to each
vowel. How is each of these vowels ordinarily spelled?
98
Phonetics and Phonolog y
Front and back vowels
Now compare the vowel of beat with that of boot. Alternate the words, and
then just the vowels. It will be more difficult this time to monitor the activities of your tongue as you shift from one of these to the other, but try
anyway.
You produce the [i] of beat with the front (blade) of your tongue raised
toward your palate. If you draw in your breath as you make this vowel, you
will feel the cold air against your palate. As you shift from [i] to [u], the
vowel of boot, you will find yourself raising the back of your tongue. (You
will also find yourself pursing (rounding) your lips, but disregard this for
the moment.) Because of the relative positions at which these vowels are
made in the mouth, phoneticians call [i] and the other vowels in (1) front
vowels, and [u] a back vowel.
The back vowels, like the front ones, descend from high, through mid,
to low, in a continuous sequence. You can observe this by pronouncing the
words coot, coat, and cot, and then just their vowels. As you produce this
series of vowels you’ll find your mouth opening (monitor your lower jaw) as
you go from coot to coat to cot. We use the following symbols for these back
vowels:
(2)
coot
coat
cot
[u]
[o]
[A]
High
Mid
Low
Exercise
For each of the three vowels just above, find five words in which the vowel
occurs. Be clear about which symbol most accurately applies to each vowel. How is each of these vowels ordinarily spelled?
We combine these two series of vowels in Table 4:
front
back
high
i
u
mid
e
o
low 
A
table 4: front and back vowels
99
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
For each of the vowels in Table 4, find five more words in which the vowel
occurs. Be clear about which symbol most accurately applies to each vowel. How is each of these vowels ordinarily spelled?
Lip rounding
As you compared [i] and [u] you probably noticed that your lips changed
shape as you shifted from the front vowel to the back one. Your lips were
rounded as you produced [u]. They were unrounded (spread or neutral)
as you produced [i]. As you moved through the series of back vowels you
may also have noticed that lip rounding decreased as you moved from high
to low. In fact the lips are unrounded during the pronunciation of [A]. In
English, the only rounded vowels are back, though many languages, such as
French and German, have rounded front vowels.
Exercise
Find five pairs of words to illustrate lip rounding. The first member of
each pair of words must include a rounded vowel; the second member
should be as similar as possible to the first, but must include a corresponding vowel that is not rounded. Assign a phonetic symbol to each
vowel, e.g., heat [i], hoot [u]. As always, note how each vowel is ordinarily spelled.
Intermediate vowels
First, pronounce the words meat, mitt, mate, met, and mat. Then pronounce
just their vowels:
meat me
mitt
mate may
met
mat
[i]
[I]
[e]
[E]
[{]
The vowels we’ve just added, [I] and [E], are intermediate in height between
[i] and [e], and [e] and [{], respectively.
100
Phonetics and Phonolog y
Exercise
For each of the vowels we’ve just discussed, find 5 more words in which
they occur. Note how they are ordinarily spelled.
Now pronounce the series of words suit, soot, sowed, sought, sot. Then
pronounce just their vowels:
suit
soot sowed sought sot cooed
flew
could
code
flow
fraught
caw
cot
spa
[u]
[U]
[o]
[O]
[A]
We’ve added two more intermediate vowels to the back series, [U] and [O].
Exercise
For each of the vowels we’ve just discussed, find 5 more words in which
they occur. Note how they are ordinarily spelled.
Now say the following words, paying attention to their vowels, and especially to the movement of your tongue as you go from one vowel to the
next: ate, up, oat. The vowel in ate is [e] and that in oat is [o]. The vowel in
up is represented by [V], called “wedge” or “caret.”
We hope that you noticed your tongue pull back as you went from [e] to
[V], and back farther as you went from [V] to [o]. [e] is a mid, front vowel,
and [o] is a mid back vowel. As [V] is between these two and at about the
same height, it is a mid central vowel.
We’ve now added five intermediate vowels: [I] as in mitt, hid, rip; [E] as
in wept, bed, flex; [U] as in hood, could; [O] as in caw; and [V] as in mutt. Of
these, [I] and [E] are front and unrounded, while [U] and [O] are back and
rounded, and [V] is central and unrounded. These new vowels differ from
the ones we introduced earlier in several ways:
1. In length: [i], [e], [u], [o], [O], and [A] are longer than [I], [E], [{],
[U], and [V], when they occur in the same contexts.
2. In position in the mouth: [i] and [e] are higher and farther front than
[I] and [E], respectively; [u] and [o] are higher and farther back than
[U] and [O], respectively.
101
Delahunty and Garvey
3. All vowels can occur in syllables (see below) that end in at least one
consonant (closed syllables); [i], [e], [u], [o], [O], and [A] can occur as
the final sound in a syllable (open syllables).
4. Muscle tension: [i], [e], [u], [o], [O], and [A] are produced with greater muscle tension in the articulators than [I], [E], [{], [U], and [V]
are. The former are tense vowels; the latter are lax. The greater tension
in [i], [e], [u], [o], [O], and [A] may explain why they are longer and
more peripheral, i.e., closer to the boundary of the mouth, than the
other vowels.
Even though there are several differences separating these two sets of
vowels, we will refer to them as tense and lax vowels. Table 5 lists all of
them:
beat, bee
boot, boo
bait, bay
boat, beau
bought, paw
pot, spa
tense
[i]
hit
[u]
hood
[e]
head
[o]
hat
[O]
hut
[A]
lax
[I]
[U]
[E]
[{]
[V]
table 5: tense and lax vowels
You may have noticed that all of the example words we have used to exemplify the vowels we have distinguished consist of a single syllable. This is
because vowels in multi-syllabic words can differ from those in monosyllables, and we wanted to compare vowels in similar contexts. We have now
distinguished the following vowels:
front central i

e
E
V
{
O
A
back
u
U
o
table 6: english vowels in monosyllabic words
Not all American English speakers distinguish [O] and [A] in all contexts.
102
Phonetics and Phonolog y
In some dialects of American English (California, Midwest), the vowels [O]
and [A] in pairs of words such as sought and sot, caught and cot, and wrought
and rot are pronounced identically, though the vowel used is neither [O] nor
[A], but one intermediate between them.
Vowels in multi-syllabic words
Pronounce the words above, soda, sofa, comma, arena, patina, photograph,
paying particular attention to the vowel represented by the bold letters.
Then pronounce this vowel in isolation. This vowel is called schwa and
written [@]. Schwa is made at approximately the same place as [V], that is,
farther forward than the back vowels and farther back than the front ones.
Hence, it is central. In addition, [@] is mid, lax, and unrounded. It is heard
primarily in unstressed syllables, as in the words above. It is the vowel we
produce if we vocalize as we prepare to speak—uh. The tongue is said to
be in its neutral position as we pronounce this vowel.
Exercise
Find five words to illustrate the vowel [@]. Can you estimate how common
this vowel is in English? What letters of the alphabet ordinarily indicate this
sound?
We can present the vowels as we presented the consonants, on a chart
indicating their articulatory properties.
front
central
Unrounded
back
Upper high
i
Lower high I
Upper mid
e
@
Lower mid
E
V
Low

A
table 7: english vowels
Rounded
u
U
o
O
103
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. Find five words to illustrate each of the vowels we distinguish in
Table 7. Be clear about which symbol most accurately applies to each
vowel.
2. Provide an articulatory description for each of the following vowels;
that is, indicate its height, position (front or back), tension, and lip configuration.
[]
[]
[]
[]
[]
[]
Height
Position
Tension
Rounding
Example
word
[]
[{]
[o]
[i]
[@]
[O]
Height
Position
Tension
Rounding
Example
word
Diphthongs
We have approached vowels as if they were articulated by a specific configuration of the tongue, lips, and oral cavity, which is held constant throughout
their pronunciation. Vowels made like this are called monophthongs; others, called diphthongs, involve a change in the configuration of the mouth.
The vowel sounds in the words boy, by, and how involve a change in
the shape of the mouth as the vowel is being produced. The vowel of boy
begins with approximately the mid back vowel [O] and finishes with approximately the high front lax vowel [I] (or the palatal glide [j]). The vowel
of by begins with approximately the low back vowel [a] (a low back vowel
slightly more forward than [A], but not as forward as [{]) and also finishes
with approximately [I] (or [j]). The vowel of how begins with approximately
[a] and finishes with approximately the high lax rounded vowel [U] (or the
labio-velar glide [w]). We represent these diphthongs as [OI], [aI], and [aU],
respectively (though many linguists use [Oj], [aj], and [aw]).
104
Phonetics and Phonolog y
Exercise
1. For each of the three diphthongs symbolized below provide four
more example words. In two of these words the diphthong should appear in a closed syllable (i.e., before a consonant, e.g., Boyd) and in
the other two words it should appear in an open syllable (i.e., not followed by a consonant, e.g., boy).
[] _________
[] _________
[] _________
_________
_________
_________
_________
_________
_________
_________
_________
_________
2. Are the English diphthongs tense or lax? (Hint: they can occur in open
syllables.)
A second set of English diphthongs is not as clearly distinguished as the
first, primarily because we tend to perceive them as simple vowels. However,
in a precise (narrow) phonetic transcription they must be represented as
diphthongs. The tense front vowel [e] is diphthongized. If you listen carefully you will notice that the vowel of bate is actually pronounced [eI]. The
tense back vowel [o] is also diphthongized: if you listen carefully you will
notice that the vowel of boat is actually pronounced [oU]. So, the front tense
vowel is diphthongized by the addition of a front vowel and the back tense
vowel is diphthongized by the addition of a back vowel. We can express this
pattern as a rule: Mid and high tense vowels are diphthongized by the addition
of a high lax vowel that matches the original vowel in frontness or backness.
Diphthongization of these vowels is a feature of English rather than a
universal feature of natural language. Other languages, notably Spanish and
German, do not diphthongize their corresponding vowels. The tendency to
diphthongize these vowels is one characteristic of the “foreign accent” that
betrays English speakers when they begin to learn these languages.
syllables and feet
It’s a lot easier to count syllables than to give them a satisfactory definition.
If the entire class were to count the syllables in this paragraph, there would
be considerable agreement about the number, but probably not about where
each syllable begins and ends. The fact that syllabic writing systems developed before alphabetic systems (see our Spelling chapter in Book II) suggests that syllables are very salient linguistic units. That children seem to be
able to associate symbols with syllables before they can associate symbols
105
Delahunty and Garvey
with phonetic segments also points to the importance of the syllable.
Every syllable (symbolized as $) consists of at least a nucleus (symbolized
as N), which is typically a vowel. The nucleus may be preceded by an onset
(symbolized as O), consisting of one or more consonants, and followed by
a coda (symbolized as C), again consisting of one or more consonants. The
nucleus and the coda together make up a unit called the rhyme (R). The
diagram (3) illustrates the constituent of the single-syllable word then.
(3)
$
O
N
ð
E
R
C
n
Because vowels are high in sonority, a syllable nucleus is usually a vowel.
However, a consonant with high sonority, such as [l,r,m,n,N] may also be
a nucleus. The sonority level of a syllable thus rises from the onset (if there
is one) up to a peak in the nucleus and falls off again in the coda. In this
respect, the onset and coda are (almost) mirror images of each other.
Parts of syllables may be repeated for poetic effects. Of these repetitions,
rhyme is the most important: it involves repeating the rhyme of syllables,
usually at the ends of lines, as the rhyming words in the following stanza
show:
(4)
Piping down the valleys wild,
Piping songs of pleasant glee,
On a cloud I saw a child,
And he laughing said to me:
(William Blake, Introduction to Songs of Innocence)
The syllable onsets, [w] of wild, [tS] of child, [gl] of glee, and [m] of me are
not part of Blake’s rhymes.
Repeating onsets, or first sounds in onsets, as in then and there, creates
alliteration. Repeating nuclei, as in Mikey likes it, or the incredible edible egg
creates assonance.
In speech, syllables are combined into rhythmic units called feet, which
are also of considerable importance in scanning lines of poetry. Each foot
consists of at least one stressed syllable (its energy peak) and one or two
106
Phonetics and Phonolog y
unstressed syllables. Feet are differentiated from each other by the number
of stressed syllables they contain and by the position of the stressed (S)
syllable(s) relative to other syllables in the foot. In (5), S represents a stressed
syllable and U an unstressed one; the stressed syllable of each example word
is bolded.
(5)
Iambic:
Trochaic:
Anapestic:
Dactylic:
Spondaic:
[U S]
[S U]
[U U S]
[S U U]
[S S]
today
trochee
intervene
personal
good news
In English, stressed syllables tend to be approximately equally far apart in
time; as a result unstressed syllables may be articulated slower or faster, depending on the type of foot. (See Beers (2003: 339) Appendix I: the 175
most common syllables (as ordinarily spelled) in the 5,000 most frequently
occurring English words.)
Exercise
1. In the stanza given in (4) above, identify each stressed syllable,
determine the feet, and identify the kind of meter (iambic, trochaic,
etc.) used.
2. How does your dictionary identify syllables and the stressed
syllable(s) in words? Why does your dictionary indicate syllabication of
words? (You’ll probably have to read the relevant section of your dictionary’s front matter for this.) Would your dictionary and our system
always give the same syllabic analysis of words?
3. Compare the phonetic alphabet we introduced here with the system
used in your dictionary to indicate pronunciation. Which is simpler to
learn? Which is simpler to use? For whom? What other pros and cons can
you think of for each?
phonology
While phonetics is the study of the ways in which speech sounds are produced, phonology is the study of (1) how the speech sounds of a language
are used in that language to distinguish meaningful units (such as words)
107
Delahunty and Garvey
from each other, and (2) how sounds are patterned in a language. Consequently, the study of phonology requires us to take meaning into consideration, while phonetics does not. In this section we explore phonology and
the basic unit of phonological analysis, the phoneme.
phonemes
You might reasonably have assumed that whenever speakers distinguish between a pair of sounds, they will use that difference to distinguish between
words. For example, we know that English speakers distinguish between [s]
and [z], and we use this difference to signal the difference between the words
sip and zip. We will say that [s] and [z] contrast with each other in English.
In fact, all of the sounds we have described so far contrast with each other in
English and so are used by English speakers to distinguish words from each
other. You can test this out by taking any pair of sounds (as we took [s] and
[z]) and creating a pair of words (like sip and zip) which are identical, except
that where one has one sound, the other has the other sound, just as where
sip has [s], zip has [z]. Pairs of words like this are called minimal pairs,
and are used to demonstrate that pairs of sounds are used in a language to
distinguish words from each other. Sound units that distinguish words from
each other are called phonemes. We enclose phonemes in / / (e.g., /s/, /z/)
to distinguish them from sounds ([s], [z]) and ordinary letters (<s>, <z>).
Exercise
Phonemes are most easily identified through minimal pairs. Thus Pete
[pit] and beat [bit] differ only in that where [pit] has [p], [bit] has [b].
These two words make a minimal pair that shows that [p] and [b] represent separate phonemes in English, which we symbolize as /p/, /b/.
For each pair of sounds below, identify a minimal pair that shows that
they represent different phonemes.
[k]—[g]
[n]—[N]
[f]—[s]
[s]—[S]
[T]—[ð]
[w]—[j]
[i]—[I]
[E]—[]
[]—[]
[aI]—[aU]
[aI]—[oI]
[tS]—[s]
[l]—[r]
[f]—[dZ]
[tS]—[dZ]
[k]—[N]
allophones
Now listen to the vowels in the words cat and cad. Are they identical or different? We hope you said “different.” Can you now say how they differ? We
108
Phonetics and Phonolog y
hope you said that one was longer than the other. Now listen to the consonants after the vowels. Are these the same or different? Again, we hope you
said different, and that you know that [t] is voiceless and [d] is voiced. Now,
which vowel, the longer or the shorter, precedes [d] and which precedes [t]?
We hope you said that the longer vowel precedes the voiced consonant.
Are the two vowels similar in any way? Again, we hope you said that they
seem to be longer and shorter versions of the same vowel, [{]. Let’s use [:]
to indicate extra length. So, the vowel before voiceless [t] is just [{], but the
one before voiced [d] is [{:].
Now let’s listen to some more word pairs like cat and cad:
root moat leaf
gape
rood
mode
leave
Gabe
Listen to the vowels in each pair. You should hear that the vowel in the second word in each pair is a little longer than the vowel in the first.
Now determine the similarities and differences between the consonants
after the vowels in each word pair. You should find that the consonant in the
first word is the voiceless version of the consonant in the second word.
Turning our attention again to the vowels in each word pair: how are
they related? We hope you said that they were very similar vowels, specifically, short and long versions of the same vowel.
You should now be able to determine a very general rule of English.
When are vowels lengthened and when are they not lengthened?
Your answer should be something along the lines of: English vowels are
lengthened when they occur before a voiced consonant; otherwise they are not
lengthened.
So far we’ve seen [{] and [{:], [u] and [u:], [o] and [o:], [i] and [i:], and
[e] and [e:]; in each case the longer vowel occurs before a voiced consonant.
We’ve also noted that the vowels are otherwise virtually identical—they differ only in length. So it makes good sense to regard these pairs of vowel
sounds as slightly different pronunciations of the same vowel, and that
whether the vowel is lengthened or not depends on whether the consonant
that follows it is voiced or not.
Importantly, the long and short pairs of vowels do not contrast with
each other: English contains no pairs of words that are identical except that
where one contains a short version of a vowel, the other contains the longer
version of the same vowel. Consequently, the long and short versions of
109
Delahunty and Garvey
vowels do not represent separate phonemes.
Let’s now turn our attention to some consonants. For example, English
speakers pronounce the [t] in toll differently from that in stole. The [t] of toll
is breathier than the [t] of stole. The former is said to be aspirated, and the
latter unaspirated. We represent the aspirated [t] as [th], with the diacritic
[h] indicating aspiration. We represent the unaspirated [t] as [t] with no diacritic. The important point here is that English speakers do not signal any
difference in meaning with the difference between [th] and [t]. They treat
the two sounds as variant ways of pronouncing the “the same sound.” Substituting one of these sounds for the other would not affect the meaning of
a word, but it would create an odd and perhaps non-native pronunciation of
the word. No pair of English words is distinguished solely by the difference
between [t] and [th]. You can satisfy yourself that this is so by trying to find
a minimal pair of English words differentiated solely by the fact that where
one has an aspirated consonant the other has an unaspirated version of that
same consonant. (Don’t spend too long trying!)
Let’s now look at a different pair of English sounds. If we replace the
[t] in [rt] (rot) with [d], then we get the sequence of sounds [rd] (rod),
which, of course, is quite distinct in meaning from rot. Clearly, English
speakers treat the difference between [d] and [t] differently from the way
they treat the difference between [th] and [t] and between longer and shorter
versions of vowels. In the case of [t] and [d], the difference can signal a
difference in meaning; in the other cases it cannot. Differences in sound
that signal differences in meaning are said to be phonemic, distinctive, or
contrastive. Differences in sound that do not signal meaning differences
are non‑distinctive or non‑contrastive. One objective of phonology is to
identify which sound differences are contrastive and which are not. As we
have seen, the contrastive sound units are called phonemes.
Phonemes and allophones
A good way to think about a phoneme is as a group of phonetically similar
sounds that are treated as members of the same sound category. Because the
members of a sound category are treated as “the same sound” in a language,
they cannot be used for communicating differences in meaning. English
speakers treat [th] and [t] as belonging to the same sound category, so they
cannot be used to distinguish one word from another. Different phonemes
are different categories of sounds and the differences among these categories can signal differences in meaning. English speakers treat [t] and [d] as
belonging to different sound categories—/t/ and /d/, respectively—and so
110
Phonetics and Phonolog y
these can be used to differentiate one word from another.
Sound categories are abstractions. We can only perceive them when one
of their members is pronounced. The sounds that make up the category are
called the allophones of that phoneme. Thus [t] and [th] are allophones of
the English phoneme /t/. Notice that the individual sound symbols are the
same as those we used for phonetics, but to distinguish phonology from
phonetics, we enclose phonemes in slanted brackets / / and use square
brackets [ ] for phonetic notation. Perhaps the following diagram will help.
It represents the phoneme /u/ and two of its allophones:
/u/
[u]
[u:]
That is, the phoneme /u/ is pronounced in (at least) two ways, [u] and [u:],
depending upon its context. Table 8 lists the phonemes of English.
Consonants: /p, t, k, b, d, g, m, n, N/
/f, T, s, S, h, v, ð, z, Z/
/tS, dZ/
/r, l, w, j/
Vowels:
/i, I, e, E, , A, O, o, U, u, (@)/
Diphthongs: /OI, aU, aI/
table 8: english phonemes
As you have no doubt noticed, there are nearly 40 phonemes of English
(the number varies somewhat from dialect to dialect), while there are only
26 letters in the English alphabet. This is one of the reasons why the alphabet appears to fit the language so poorly. (For more on English spelling see
our chapter on Spelling in Book II.)
Exercise
1. What phoneme is represented by the bolded letter(s) in the following words? Make sure to enclose the symbols you choose in the phoneme
slashes //.
ton, bump, dip, comb, chin, zoom, shave, mango, thing, lame,
read, sleep, red, mat, good, caught, kite, bid, coy.
111
Delahunty and Garvey
2. Transcribe the following words in a phonemic (broad) transcription.
That is, just represent the phonemes that each word is composed of
and ignore the allophonic detail.
thin, then, cheese, rouge, June, shin, fling, heave, yak, cow.
Allophones and their contexts
We have already noted that if we substitute the aspirated allophone of /t/
for its unaspirated relative, then we create an odd pronunciation of a word.
[tIl] is the typical American English pronunciation of till, but [tIl] is not.
What, if any, patterns can we observe in where allophones of a phoneme can
and cannot occur?
Some allophones of a phoneme are in complementary distribution,
that is, they occupy different positions (contexts or environments) in
words—where one can occur the other cannot. As we have seen, English
has a very general pattern of lengthening vowels before voiced consonants.
That is, the allophone of a vowel phoneme before a voiced consonant will
be appreciably longer (up to three times longer) than the allophone of the
same vowel phoneme before a voiceless consonant. For example, listen to
the pronunciation of /E/ in bet and bed. You should have little difficulty in
hearing the difference in vowel length. We can represent the pattern of occurrence (distribution) of these two allophones of the phoneme /E/ as the
following phonological rule: When the phoneme /E/ occurs before a voiceless
consonant it is pronounced as its allophone [E]; when it occurs before a voiced
consonant it is pronounced as its allophone [E:]. (Remember: [:] is a diacritic
indicating a lengthened sound.)
In fact, the rule is much more general than this. Because it applies to all
vowels, we can write it as: In English a vowel is longer before a voiced consonant
than it is before a voiceless one. One of our objectives in studying a language
is to be able to describe these sound patterns, i.e., to be able to specify in
the most general terms possible the phonetic environments in which each
allophone occurs.
Let’s look at another very systematic set of English vowel allophones.
The vowels of cap and can differ phonetically: that of cap is a plain []; that
of can is nasalized, represented by [{~~]. (If you have trouble hearing the
difference, try starting to say each word normally and then omit the final
consonant.) The phoneme // thus has the allophones, [] and [{~]. In fact,
all English vowels have both nasalized and non-nasalized allophones. We
can represent this as the rule: Whenever an English vowel occurs before a nasal
consonant, it becomes nasalized; otherwise it is non-nasalized.
112
Phonetics and Phonolog y
In fact, the situation is a bit more intricate that this. Because nasals are
voiced, we should expect a vowel before them to be lengthened relative to
the same vowel before a voiceless sound. And, indeed, this is what we find.
Listen to the vowels in cat, cad, and can. You should notice that the first
vowel is unlengthened, [{]; the second one is lengthened, [{:]; and the
third one is both lengthened (in fact, probably even more than the second
one) and nasalized, [{~:].
Exercise
1. What sounds are presented by the bolded letter(s) in the following
words? Provide an allophonic (narrow) transcription.
mad, back, spill, cat, tang
2. Try your hand at distinguishing allophones of phonemes. Using the
discussion above as a guide, see if you can describe the phonetic differences between the allophones of the designated phoneme in the
example words.
a. /k/: kin, skin
b. /E/: bet, Ben
c. /e/: rate, raid
d. //: bat, bad
e. /l/: lead, pull
f. /k/: cool, keel
phonological rules
As we saw above, a phonological rule is a general statement about the
distribution of a phoneme’s allophones, e.g., those of /t/. There are several
types of phonological rules to represent the several patterns of distribution
of sounds in a language.
The rule for the [th] allophone of /t/ can be seen as adding extra breathiness after the release of a voiceless stop. This rule adds the aspiration feature
to the consonant. Such rules are referred to as feature addition rules.
Exercise
Listen carefully to the sounds represented by the bolded letters in each
of the following pairs of words: steal, teal; spin, pin; skate, Kate.
What phonetic difference can you hear between the [t] of steal and the
113
Delahunty and Garvey
[t] of teal? Write the two sounds in narrow (allophonic) phonetic transcription. Where does each of these two sounds occur in the example
words? Answer the same questions for the [p] of spin and pin and the
[k] of skate and Kate. What general pattern applies to all three pairs of
sounds? Express this general pattern as a phonological rule.
Feature changing rules change the value of a component feature of a
sound, for instance, from non-nasal to nasal or from short to long. The
nasal pronunciation of the vowel of can is due, as we’ve seen, to the influence of the nasal consonant /n/ that comes immediately after it. In this
case, the rule changes an oral (non-nasal) sound to a nasal one.
Segment deletion rules remove sound segments. For instance, in informal speech, a segment deletion rule removes the second of a pair of consonants at the end of one word when the next word begins with a consonant.
Thus words such as frost and ask are pronounced as [fras] and [s] when
they occur before consonants (e.g., Ask Katie). This effect is especially likely
when the last consonant of the first word is phonetically similar to the first
consonant of the next word, as in used to [jus t@], instead of [just t@], (which
leads to the incorrect spelling use to). French adjectives which end in consonants routinely lose those consonants if the following word begins with a consonant: ‘small friend’ petit ami [p@tit ami] vs. ‘small book’ petit livre [p@ti livr].
Phonological rules may also reverse the order of segments in words. In
some dialects of English the verb ask is pronounced as [ks], reversing [s]
and [k]. Several hundred years ago, the word bird, now pronounced as [b@rd]
was pronounced [brId]. The vowel and the [r] switched places. Rules that
reverse a sequence of segments are called metathesis rules.
Some rules, such as the vowel nasalization rule, make a segment and its
neighbor more alike. Such rules are called assimilation rules.
Exercise
1. (a) Identify the rapid, natural pronunciation of the sound represented by the letter <n> in the words input, intake, and inquest. (b) Identify the sound immediately after the sound represented by <n> in each
word. (c) In what ways are the sound represented by <n> and the sound
immediately following it in each word similar? (d) Express the similarity
between the members of the pairs of sounds in all three words in one
general rule. (e) What kind of phonological rule is this?
114
Phonetics and Phonolog y
2. Examine the rapid, natural pronunciation of <n> in the phrases below. Write each entire phrase phonemically. Then try to state a rule
that accounts for the different pronunciations. What type of rule did you
discover?
a. In Bill’s house
b. In Ted’s house
c. In Greg’s house
3. Describe the phonetic difference between the allophones of /k/
(written as <c> and <k> in ordinary spelling) in the two columns of
words:
coop
keep
could
kid
coat
Kate
cot
cat
Express the difference and the distribution of the allophones as a phonological rule. What kind of rule did you come up with?
Assimilation can be so thoroughgoing that two sounds can merge into
one. For example, [t,d,s,z] are palatalized—i.e., pronounced [tS, dZ, , Z] respectively—when they occur at the ends of words and the next word begins
with the palatal glide [j]. For example, Did you? is typically pronounced as
[dIdZ@] or even [dZ@]; the [dZ] results from the coalescence of [dj]. The study of phonology shows that languages make use of unpredictable units (phonemes) to differentiate words from each other. It also shows
that languages employ very general patterns of sounds. By representing the
general, predictable patterns as phonological rules, we leave only that information which is unpredictable and idiosyncratic to be listed in the set of
phonemes. This way we minimize the number of basic phonemic units we
need to posit; we also minimize the number of times any given piece of information is mentioned, thus simplifying the overall grammar or description of the language. For example, English has two series of vowels, those
with and those without nasalization. The nasalized vowels occur only before nasal consonants; the non-nasalized ones occur everywhere else. If we
merely listed all these vowels as belonging to the language, then we would
have postulated far more basic units than we—or native speakers—really
need. And we would have missed the generalization that the two series of
vowels are really quite alike, one series being merely a predictable positional
115
Delahunty and Garvey
variant of the other. We capture this generalization by eliminating the series
of nasalized vowels from our inventory of basic units and replacing it with
the nasalization rule.
Exercise
1. Arabic speakers learning English often produce [b] where English requires [p], e.g., saying “bark” instead of “park.” Describe the phonetic
difference between [b] and [p].
2. We noted that English has approximately 40 phonemes but only 26
letters of the ordinary alphabet to represent them. Illustrate with appropriate examples at least three ways in which the English spelling
system uses those 26 letters to represent its almost 40 phonemes.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Ashby, Patricia. 1995. Speech Sounds. London, UK: Routledge.
Attridge, Derek. 1995. Poetic Rhythm: an Introduction. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Beers, Kylene. 2003. When Kids Can’t Read: What Teachers Can Do.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Celce-Murcia, Marianne, D.M. Brinton, and J.M. Goodwin. 1996. Teaching
Pronunciation: A Reference for Teachers of English to Speakers of Other
Languages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Crane, B.L., E. Yeager, and R.L. Whitman. 1993. Phonetics. In L.M. Cleary
and M.D. Linn (eds.). Linguistics for Teachers. pp. 397-410. New York:
McGraw Hill.
Hogg, R. and C.B. McCully. 1987. Metrical Phonology: A Coursebook.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kreidler, Charles W. 1997. Describing Spoken English. London, UK:
Routledge.
Ladefoged, Peter. 2001. A Course in Phonetics. (4th ed.) Ft. Worth, TX:
Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
_____2005. Vowels and Consonants. (2nd ed.) Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Lass, Roger. 1984. Phonology: An Introduction to Basic Concepts. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
McMahon, April. 2001. An Introduction to English Phonology. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Pennington, Martha C. 1996. Phonology in English Language Teaching.
116
Phonetics and Phonolog y
London, UK: Longman.
Yavaş, Mehmet. 2006. Applied English Phonology. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
glossary
affricate:
sound produced with full stoppage of the airstream followed immediately by constriction.
allophone: non-distinctive phonetic variant of a phoneme.
alveo-palatal: sound produced at the hard palate just behind the alveolar
ridge.
alveolar: sound produced at the alveolar ridge, the bony ridge behind the
teeth.
approximants: sounds produced when the articulators approach each other
but not so closely as to cause turbulence in the airstream; they include laterals (the tongue touches the top of the mouth but the air is allowed to pass
along one or both sides, as in [l]); central (the sides of the tongue are raised
so that air flows along the center of the mouth, as in [r]); as well as the labiovelar [w] and palatal [j].
aspirated: consonant sound released with a puff of air.
assimilation rule: phonological rule that makes a sound similar to a nearby
sound. e.g., palatalization.
back vowel: vowel produced with the back of the tongue raised toward the
soft palate.
bilabial: sound produced with constriction or closure of the lips.
broad transcription: the attempt to record pronunciation without regard
to non-contrastive details. See narrow transcription.
central: vowel—e.g., [@]—produced with the tongue raised at the center
of the mouth rather than at the front or back.
coda: last part of a syllable; follows the nucleus.
complementary distribution: when the allophones of a phoneme occupy
different positions in words.
consonant: sound produced with complete or partial obstruction of the air
flow through the mouth. See vowel.
contrastive (also distinctive): sounds used in a language to signal differences of meaning.
diacritic: phonetic symbols used to represent fine differences in pronunciation, e.g., the [h] that indicates aspiration.
diphthong: vowel unit that begins with one oral configuration and ends
with another. See monophthong.
distinctive: See contrastive.
distribution: specific circumstances (environments) in which a sound oc117
Delahunty and Garvey
curs, e.g., at the beginning, middle, or end of a word.
environment: See distribution.
feature changing rule: rule that changes the value of a component feature
of a sound, e.g., from stop to fricative, from non-nasal to nasal, or from lax
to tense.
foot: a rhythmic unit consisting of at least one stressed syllable and 1-2
other syllables, typically unstressed.
fricative: sound produced with constriction of the airstream, producing
friction.
front vowel: vowel produced with the front of the tongue raised toward
the hard palate.
glides: sounds, e.g., [j] and [w], that are intermediate in openness and sonority between consonants and vowels. Also called semivowels.
glottal: sound produced by constricting or stopping the airstream at the
vocal folds.
high vowel: vowel pronounced with the mouth in the least degree of openness. See mid vowel and low vowel.
interdental: sound produced with the tongue protruding between the
teeth.
labiodental: sound produced with constriction between the bottom lip
and top teeth.
labiovelar: sound produced by raising the back of the tongue to or toward
the velum and rounding the lips, e.g., [w].
lateral: sound produced with the tongue touching the top of the mouth
with air allowed to pass along one or both sides, as in [l].
lax: sound produced with musculature of the mouth relatively relaxed. See
tense.
low vowel: vowel pronounced with the mouth in the greatest degree of
openness. See high vowel and mid vowel.
manner of articulation: the kind of closure or constriction used in making a consonant sound.
metathesis rule: phonological rule that reverses the order of segments in
words.
mid vowel: vowel pronounced with the mouth in an intermediate degree of
openness. See high vowel and low vowel.
minimal pair: two words of different meaning that are phonetically the same
except for one sound, e.g., pit and bit (used to demonstrate that [p] and [b]
contrast with each other).
monophthong: vowel unit consisting of a single segment held constant
during its pronunciation. See diphthong.
118
Phonetics and Phonolog y
narrow transcription:
attempt to record non-contrastive details of pronunciation. See broad transcription.
nasal, nasalized: sounds articulated with air flowing through the nasal cavity.
non-contrastive (also non-distinctive): sounds not used in a language to
signal different meanings.
nucleus: central part of a syllable, i.e., the segment with the highest sonority.
onset: initial part of a syllable; precedes the nucleus.
phoneme: contrastive or distinctive sound category; distinguishes words
from each other.
phonetics (articulatory): the study of how speech sounds are produced.
phonological rule: a general statement about the distribution of a phoneme’s allophones and about other phonological processes.
phonology: the study of the ways in which a given language shapes sounds
into distinctive categories of perception and of its rules of pronunciation.
place of articulation: the area in the mouth at which the consonantal
closure or constriction occurs.
rhyme: the nucleus and coda of a syllable.
rounded: vowel sound produced with the lips pursed. See unrounded.
schwa: a mid central unrounded vowel, represented as [@].
segment deletion rule: phonological rule that eliminates a sound from
pronunciation in a word or phrase.
semivowel: see glide.
sonorant: sounds produced with a smooth airflow, allowing for a high degree of resonance.
stop: sound produced with full stoppage of the airstream anywhere in the
oral cavity from the vocal folds to the lips.
tense: sound produced with musculature of the mouth relatively tight. See
lax.
unrounded: vowel produced without lip rounding. See rounded.
velar: sound produced with constriction at the soft palate.
voiced: sound produced with the vocal folds vibrating.
voiceless: sound produced with the vocal folds not vibrating.
vowel: sound produced with smooth, unobstruction air stream through the
mouth. See consonant.
119
Delahunty and Garvey
120
5 Morphology and Word Formation
key concepts
Words and morphemes
Root, derivational, inflectional morphemes
Morphemes, allomorphs, morphs
Words
English inflectional morphology
English derivational morphology
Compounding
Other sources of words
Registers and words
Internal structure of complex words
Classifying words by their morphology
i n t ro d u c t i o n
This chapter is about words—their relationships, their constituent parts,
and their internal organization. We believe that this information will be of
value to anyone interested in words, for whatever reason; to anyone interested in dictionaries and how they represent the aspects of words we deal
with here; to anyone involved in developing the vocabularies of native and
non-native speakers of English; to anyone teaching writing across the curriculum who must teach the characteristics of words specific to their discipline;
to anyone teaching writing who must deal with the usage issues created by
the fact that different communities of English speakers use different word
forms, only one of which may be regarded as standard.
Exercise
1. Divide each of the following words into their smallest meaningful
parts:landholder, smoke-jumper, demagnetizability.
2. Each of the following sentences contains an error made by a nonnative speaker of English. In each, identify and correct the incorrect
word.
a. I am very relax here.
b. I am very boring with this game.
c. I am very satisfactory with my life.
d. Some flowers are very attracting to some insects.
e. Many people have very strong believes.
121
Delahunty and Garvey
f. My culture is very difference from yours.
g. His grades proof that he is a hard worker.
h. The T-shirt that China drawing. (from a T-shirt package from
China)
In general terms, briefly discuss what English language learners must
learn in order to avoid such errors.
3. Some native speakers of English use forms such as seen instead
of saw, come instead of came, aks instead of ask, clumb instead of
climbed, drug instead of dragged, growed instead of grew. Are these
errors? If they are, are they the same kinds of errors made by the nonnative speakers of English listed in Exercise 2? If not, what are they?
w o rd s a n d m o r p h e m e s
In traditional grammar, words are the basic units of analysis. Grammarians
classify words according to their parts of speech and identify and list the
forms that words can show up in. Although the matter is really very complex, for the sake of simplicity we will begin with the assumption that we are
all generally able to distinguish words from other linguistic units. It will be
sufficient for our initial purposes if we assume that words are the main units
used for entries in dictionaries. In a later section, we will briefly describe
some of their distinctive characteristics.
Words are potentially complex units, composed of even more basic units,
called morphemes. A morpheme is the smallest part of a word that has
grammatical function or meaning (NB not the smallest unit of meaning);
we will designate them in braces—{ }. For example, sawed, sawn, sawing,
and saws can all be analyzed into the morphemes {saw} + {‑ed}, {‑n}, {‑ing},
and {‑s}, respectively. None of these last four can be further divided into
meaningful units and each occurs in many other words, such as looked,
mown, coughing, bakes.
{Saw} can occur on its own as a word; it does not have to be attached
to another morpheme. It is a free morpheme. However, none of the other
morphemes listed just above is free. Each must be affixed (attached) to some
other unit; each can only occur as a part of a word. Morphemes that must
be attached as word parts are said to be bound.
Exercise
1. Identify the free morphemes in the following words:
122
Morpholog y and Word Formation
kissed, freedom, stronger, follow, awe, goodness, talkative, teacher,
actor.
2. Use the words above (and any other words that you think are relevant) to answer the following questions:
a. Can a morpheme be represented by a single phoneme? Give examples. By more than one phoneme? Give examples.
b. Can a free morpheme be more than one syllable in length? Give
examples. Can a bound morpheme? Give examples.
c. Does the same letter or phoneme—or sequence of letters or phonemes—always represent the same morpheme? Why or why not?
(Hint: you must refer to the definition of morpheme to be able to
answer this.)
d. Can the same morpheme be spelled differently? Give examples.
e. Can different morphemes be pronounced identically? Give examples.
f. A morpheme is basically the same as:
i. a letter
ii. a sound
iii. a group of sounds
iv. none of the above
3. The words district and discipline show that the sequence of letters
d-i-s does not always constitute a morpheme. (Analogous examples are
mission, missile, begin, and retrofit.) List five more sequences of letters that are sometimes a morpheme and sometimes not.
4. Just for fun, find some other pairs like disgruntled / *gruntled and
disgusted / *gusted, where one member of the pair is an actual English
word and the other should be a word, but isn’t.
Affixes are classified according to whether they are attached before or
after the form to which they are added. Prefixes are attached before and
suffixes after. The bound morphemes listed earlier are all suffixes; the {re‑}
of resaw is a prefix. Further examples of prefixes and suffixes are presented in
Appendix A at the end of this chapter.
Root, derivational, and inflectional morphemes
Besides being bound or free, morphemes can also be classified as root, derivational, or inflectional. A root morpheme is the basic form to which other
123
Delahunty and Garvey
morphemes are attached. It provides the basic meaning of the word.The
morpheme {saw} is the root of sawers. Derivational morphemes are added
to forms to create separate words: {‑er} is a derivational suffix whose addition turns a verb into a noun, usually meaning the person or thing that
performs the action denoted by the verb. For example, {paint}+{-er} creates
painter, one of whose meanings is “someone who paints.”
Inflectional morphemes do not create separate words. They merely
modify the word in which they occur in order to indicate grammatical properties such as plurality, as the {-s} of magazines does, or past tense, as the {ed}
of babecued does. English has eight inflectional morphemes, which we will
describe below.
We can regard the root of a word as the morpheme left over when all
the derivational and inflectional morphemes have been removed. For example,
in immovability, {im-}, {-abil}, and {-ity} are all derivational morphemes, and
when we remove them we are left with {move}, which cannot be further divided into meaningful pieces, and so must be the word’s root.
We must distinguish between a word’s root and the forms to which affixes are attached. In moveable, {-able} is attached to {move}, which we’ve
determined is the word’s root. However, {im-} is attached to moveable, not
to {move} (there is no word immove), but moveable is not a root. Expressions
to which affixes are attached are called bases. While roots may be bases,
bases are not always roots.
Exercise
1. Can an English word have more than one prefix? Give examples. More
than one suffix? For example? More than one of each? Give examples.
Divide the examples you collected into their root, derivational, and
inflectional morphemes.
2. Check your dictionary to see how it deals with inflected and derived
word forms. Does it list all the inflections of regular inflected words?
Just irregular ones? Does it accord derived forms their own entries or
include them in the entries of the forms from which they are derived?
3. Does your dictionary list bound morphemes? Which kinds?
morphemes, allomorphs, and morphs
The English plural morpheme {-s} can be expressed by three different but
124
Morpholog y and Word Formation
clearly related phonemic forms /z/ or /z/, /z/, and /s/. These three have
in common not only their meaning, but also the fact that each contains an
alveolar fricative phoneme, either /s/ or /z/. The three forms are in complementary distribution, because each occurs where the others cannot, and it is
possible to predict just where each occurs: /z/ after sibilants (/s, z, , , t,
d/), /z/ after voiced segments, and /s/ everywhere else. Given the semantic
and phonological similarities between the three forms and the fact that they
are in complementary distribution, it is reasonable to view them as contextual pronunciation variants of a single entity. In parallel with phonology,
we will refer to the entity of which the three are variant representations as a
morpheme, and the variant forms of a given morpheme as its allomorphs.
When we wish to refer to a minimal grammatical form merely as a form,
we will use the term morph. Compare these terms and the concepts behind
them with phoneme, allophone, and phone. (Hint: note the use of / /, [ ],
and { }.)
Exercise
Consult the glossary in the chapter on Phonetics and Phonology and
try to determine the meanings of the morphemes {phone}, {allo-}, and
{-eme}.
(1)
[allophone]
(2)
/allomorph/
/phoneme/
[allophone]
[allophone] etc.
{morpheme}
/allomorph/
/allomorph/ etc.
w o rd s
Words are notoriously difficult entities to define, both in universal and in
language specific terms. Like most linguistic entities, they look in two directions—upward toward larger units of which they are parts (toward phrases),
and downward toward their constituent morphemes. This, however, only
helps us understand words if we already understand how they are combined
into larger units or divided into smaller ones, so we will briefly discuss sev125
Delahunty and Garvey
eral other criteria that have been proposed for identifying them.
One possible criterion is spelling: in written English text, we tend to
regard as a word any expression that has no spaces within it and is separated
by spaces from other expressions. While this is a very useful criterion, it
does sometimes lead to inconsistent and unsatisfactory results. For instance,
cannot is spelled as one word but might not as two; compounds (words composed of two or more words; see below) are inconsistently divided (cf. influx,
in-laws, goose flesh, low income vs. low-income).
Words tend to resist interruption; we cannot freely insert pieces into
words as we do into sentences. For example, we cannot separate the root of
a word from its inflectional ending by inserting another word, as in *sockblue‑s for blue socks. Sentences, in contrast, can be interrupted. We can insert adverbials between subjects and predicates: John quickly erased his fingerprints. By definition, we can also insert the traditional interjections: We
will, I believe, have rain later today.
In English, though by no means in all languages, the order of elements
in words is quite fixed. English inflections, for example, are suffixes and
are added after any derivational morphemes in a word. At higher levels in
the language, different orders of elements can differ in meaning: compare
John kissed Mary with Mary kissed John. But we do not contrast words with
prefixed inflections with words with suffixed inflections. English does not
contrast, for example, piece + s with s + piece.
In English, too, it is specific individual words that select for certain inflections. Thus the word child is pluralized by adding {‑ren}, ox by adding
{‑en}. So if a form takes the {-en} plural, it must be a word.
So words are units composed of one or more morphemes; they are also
the units of which phrases are composed.
English inflectional morphology
Inflectional morphemes, as we noted earlier, alter the form of a word in order to indicate certain grammatical properties. English has only eight inflectional morphemes, listed in Table 1, along with the properties they indicate.
Except for {-en}, the forms we list in Table 1 are the regular English inflections. They are regular because they are the inflections added to the vast
majority of verbs, nouns, adjectives, and adverbs to indicate grammatical
properties such as tense, number, and degree.
They are also the inflections we typically add to new words coming into
the language, for example, we add {-s} to the noun throughput to make it
plural. When we borrow words from other languages, in most cases we add
the regular English inflections to them rather than borrow the inflections
126
Morpholog y and Word Formation
they had in their home languages; for example, we pluralize operetta as operettas rather than as operette as Italian does; similarly, we sing oratorios rather
than oratori. [Thanks to Paula Malpezzi-Price for help with these examples.]
The regular inflections are the default inflections that learners tend to use
when they don’t know the correct ones (for example, growed rather than
grew).
nouns:
noun phrases:
{-s}
{-s}
plural
genitive/possessive
(the birds)
(the bird’s song)
adjectives/adverbs:
{-er}
{-est}
comparative
superlative
(faster)
(fastest)
verbs: {-s}
3rd person singular present tense
{-ed} past tense
{-ing} progressive/present participle
{-en} past participle
(proves)
(proved)
(is proving)
(has proven)
(was proven)
table 1: the eight english inflectional morphemes
[Note: the regular past participle morpheme is {-ed}, identical to the
past tense form {-ed}. We use the irregular past participle form {-en} to
distinguish the two.]
However, because of its long and complex history, English (like all languages) has many irregular forms, which may be irregular in a variety of
ways. First, irregular words may use different inflections than regular ones:
for example, the modern past participle inflection of a regular verb is {-ed},
but the past participle of freeze is frozen and the past participle of break
is broken. Second, irregular forms may involve internal vowel changes, as
in man/men, woman/women, grow/grew, ring/rang/rung. Third, some forms
derive from historically unrelated forms: went, the past tense of go, historically was the past tense of a different verb, wend. This sort of realignment
is known as suppletion. Other examples of suppletion include good, better,
and best, and bad, worse, and worst. (As an exercise, you might look up be,
am, and is in a dictionary that provides etymological information, such as
the American Heritage.) Fourth, some words show no inflectional change:
sheep is both singular and plural; hit is both present and past tense, as well
as past participle. Fifth, many borrowed words, especially nouns, have irregular inflected forms: alumnae and cherubim are the plurals of alumna and
127
Delahunty and Garvey
cherub, respectively.
Irregular forms demonstrate the abstract status of morphemes. Thus the
word men realizes (represents, makes real) the two morphemes {man} and
{plural}; women realizes {woman} and {plural}; went realizes {go} and {past
tense}. Most grammar and writing textbooks contain long lists of these exceptions.
As a final issue here we must note that different groups of English speakers use different inflected forms of words, especially of verbs. When this is
the case, the standard variety of the language typically selects one and rejects
the others as non-standard, or, illogically, as “not English,” or worse. For
example, many English speakers use a single form of be in the past tense
(was) regardless of what the subject of its clause is. So they will say, We was
there yesterday. This is an uncontroversial issue: was in instances like this is
universally regarded as non-standard. Other forms are more controversial.
For example, what is the past tense of dive—dived or dove? How are lie and
lay to be used? How does your dictionary deal with such usage issues?
Exercise
1. Can you think of a reliable way to distinguish the past tense and past
participle of a verb, regardless of whether it is regular or irregular?
(Hint: think of words or classes of words that often occur with these
forms.)
2. Check a reference grammar for further examples of irregular inflections. Also, for an excellent discussion of this and related issues, read
Pinker (1999).
3. From the following words, determine the three distinct pronunciations or allomorphs of the past tense morpheme {-ed}: towed, sighed,
tapped, tabbed, tossed, buzzed, raided. Specify the phonological environment in which each allomorph occurs. (Hints: look at the last sound
of the word to which the morpheme is added and think of the allomorphs of the plural morpheme discussed earlier.)
4. Pinker (1999) notes that children learning English as their native
language sometimes produce forms like goed and readed. Why do you
think they do this?
5. Would you expect adult non-native learners of English to produce
128
Morpholog y and Word Formation
forms similar to those of native speaking children? What further difficulties might non-native speakers have that native English-speaking
children might not have? (Hints: think of the frequency of irregular
forms in English and think of your own experience in learning a second
language.)
English derivational morphology
Derivation is the process of creating separate but morphologically related
words. Typically, but not always, it involves one or more changes in form. It
can involve prefixing, as in resaw, and suffixing, as in sawing, sawer, sawable.
Another type of derivation, while not visible, is at least audible. It involves a change in the position of the primary stress in a word. Compare:
(3)
permit (noun)
contact (noun)
perfect (adj.)
convert (noun)
permit (verb)
contact (verb)
perfect (verb)
convert (verb)
In some derivationally related word pairs, only a feature of the final consonant changes, usually its voicing:
(4)
advice
belief mouth
breath advise
believe
mouthe
breathe
/s/  /z/
/f/  /v/
//  //
//  //
In some cases adding a derivational morpheme induces a change in a
stressed vowel:
(5) divine
profane
serene divinity
profanity
serenity
/a/  //
/e/  //
/i/  //
In other cases, the addition of a suffix triggers a change in the final consonant of the root. For example, an alveolar consonant becomes palatal with
the same voicing value:
(6) part
face
partial
facial
/t/  //
/s/  //
129
Delahunty and Garvey
seize
remit
seizure
remission
/z/  //
/t/  //
In a multi-syllabic word with a stressed tense vowel, the palatalization
may be accompanied by a laxing of that vowel:
(7) collide
collision
/d/  //
/a/  //
elide
elision
/d/  //
/a/  //
Sometimes the addition of a derivational affix requires a change in the
stress pattern, with consequential changes in the pronunciations of the vowels. In most cases an unstressed vowel is pronounced as schwa:
(8) telegraph
regal
tutor
telegraphy
regalia
tutorial
In still other cases we find suffixing, stress migration with change of vowel quality, and change of consonant:
(9) approve
approbation
/u/  //
/v/  /b/
Additionally, English allows us to change a word’s part of speech without
any change of form. As a result, identical forms may belong to different
parts of speech, e.g., saw the noun and saw the verb:
(10) a. This saw is too dull. (noun)
b. Don’t saw that board. (verb)
Other examples include hit, buy, dust, autograph, brown-bag, which can all
be both verbs and nouns. Change of part of speech without any corresponding formal change is called conversion (also functional shift or zero derivation). There is more on this topic in our chapter on Major Parts of Speech.
Exercise
1. Write each of the example words in (3)-(9) in a phonemic notation.
130
Morpholog y and Word Formation
2. True or False?
a. Every English word contains at least one root.
b. In English, derivational morphemes occur before inflectional morphemes.
c. In English, derivational suffixes regularly occur before inflectional suffixes.
d. In English, a few inflectional morphemes can occur as prefixes.
e. Every root in English is a free morpheme (i.e., there is no such
thing as a bound root.) (Hint: consider receive, deceive, conceive, perceive.)
f. In English, some morphemes have both a free and a bound allomorph. (Hint: consider able, ability; France, Franco-.)
3. a. In a broad phonetic (phonemic) transcription, transcribe the
sounds represented by the bolded letters in impossible, inedible, illegible, irresponsible.
b. What meaning do these pairs of letters have in common?
c. What is the first sound in all four pairs of sounds?
d. What are the second sounds in the pairs of sounds?
e. Why does the second sound vary as it does?
f. How would you analyze this variation in terms of morphemes and
allomorphs?
4. As English readily allows conversion, you should have no trouble compiling a list of ten pairs of words with identical forms but different
parts of speech. For each pair of words, create a pair of short sentences
that show that the words do belong to different parts of speech.
As we’ll see in more detail in the next chapter, words belonging to different parts of speech take different inflections—e.g., {Nsaw} + {pl-s}; {Vsaw} +
{-ed}. Because derivationally related forms often belong to different parts of
speech and consequently allow different inflections, and because the meanings of derivationally related pairs are not always as parallel as their forms
are, derived forms may be given their own entries in dictionaries. Webster’s
New World Dictionary, for instance, has separate entries for generate and
generation and for compete and competence. Look up these words in your own
dictionary and note how the meanings of generation and competence are not
entirely predictable from those of generate + {-ion} and compete + {-ence},
131
Delahunty and Garvey
respectively.
The term word family is often used for a set of words that are related to
each other derivationally or inflectionally, though the term is also used to
refer to any set of words that rhyme with each other.
Compounding
The italicized words in (11) are created by combining saw with some other
word, rather than with a bound morpheme.
(11) a. A sawmill is a noisy place.
b. Every workshop should have a chain saw, a table saw, a jig-saw, a
hack saw, and a bucksaw.
c. Sawdust is always a problem in a woodworker’s workshop.
d. Sawing horses are useful and easily made.
Such words are called compounds. They contain two or more words
(or more accurately, two or more roots, all, one, or none of which may
be bound; cf. blueberry with two free morphemes, and astronaut with two
bound morphemes). Generally, one of the words is the head of the compound and the other(s) its modifier(s). In bucksaw, saw is the head, which
is modified by buck. The order is significant: compare pack rat with rat
pack. Generally, the modifier comes before the head.
In ordinary English spelling, compounds are sometimes spelled as single
words, as in sawmill, sawdust; sometimes the parts are connected by a hyphen, as in jig‑saw; and sometimes they are spelled as two words, as in chain
saw, oil well. (Dictionaries may differ in their spellings.) Nonetheless, we are
justified in classifying all such cases as compound words regardless of their
conventional spelling for a variety of reasons.
First, the stress pattern of the compound word is usually different from
the stress pattern in the phrase composed of the same words in the same
order. Compare:
(12) compound White House
funny farm
blackbird
flatcar
132
phrase
white house
funny farm
black bird
flat car
Morpholog y and Word Formation
Exercise
Very bad teenager joke:
Q: How do you make a cat drink?
A: Put it in a blender.
What are the verbal tricks here?
In the compounds the main stress is on the first word; in the phrases the
main stress is on the last word. While this pattern does not apply to all compounds, it is so generally true that it provides a very useful test.
Second, the meaning of the compound may differ to a greater or lesser
degree from that of the corresponding phrase. A blackbird is a species of
bird, regardless of its color; a black bird is a bird which is black, regardless
of its species. A trotting-horse is a kind of horse, regardless of its current activity; a trotting horse must be a horse that is currently trotting. So, because
the meanings of compounds are not always predictable from the meanings
of their constituents, dictionaries often provide individual entries for them.
They do not do this for phrases, unless the meaning of the phrase is idiomatic and therefore not derivable from the meanings of its parts and how
they are put together, e.g., raining cats and dogs. Generally the meaning of a
phrase is predictable from the meanings of its constituents, and so phrases
need not be listed individually. (Indeed, because the number of possible
phrases in a language is infinite, it is in principle impossible to list them all.)
Third, in many compounds, the order of the constituent words is different from that in the corresponding phrase:
(13) compound sawmill sawing horse
sawdust
phrase
mill for sawing
horse for sawing
dust from sawing
Fourth, compound nouns allow no modification to the first element.
This contrasts with noun phrases, which do allow modification to the modifier: compare *a really-blackbird and a really black bird.
There are a number of ways of approaching the study and classification of
compound words, the most accessible of which is to classify them according
to the part of speech of the compound and then sub-classify them according to the
parts of speech of its constituents. Table 2 is based on discussion in Bauer (1983).
133
Delahunty and Garvey
1. Compound nouns
a. Noun + noun: bath towel; boy-friend; death blow
b. Verb + noun: pickpocket; breakfast
c. Noun +verb: nosebleed; sunshine
d. Verb +verb: make-believe
e. Adjective + noun: deep structure; fast-food
f. Particle + noun: in-crowd; down-town
g. Adverb + noun: now generation
h. Verb + particle: cop-out; drop-out
i. Phrase compounds: son-in-law
2. Compound verbs
a. Noun + verb: sky-dive
b. Adjective + verb: fine-tune
c. Particle + verb: overbook
d. Adjective + noun: brown-bag
3. Compound adjectives
a. Noun + adjective: card-carrying; childproof
b. Verb + adjective: fail safe
c. Adjective + adjective: open-ended
d. Adverb + adjective: cross-modal
e. Particle + adjective: over-qualified
f. Noun + noun: coffee-table
g. Verb + noun: roll-neck
h. Adjective + noun: red-brick; blue-collar
i. Particle + noun: in-depth
j. Verb + verb: go-go; make-believe
k. Adjective/Adverb + verb: high-rise;
l. Verb + particle: see-through; tow-away
4. Compound adverbs
uptightly
cross-modally
5. Neo-classical compounds
astro-naut
hydro-electric
mechano-phobe
table 2: english compounds (bauer, 1983)
An alternative approach is to classify compounds in terms of the semantic relationship between the compound and its head. The head of a com134
Morpholog y and Word Formation
pound is the constituent modified by the compound’s other constituents.
In English, heads of compounds are typically the rightmost constituent (excluding any derivational and inflectional suffixes). For example, in traffic-cop
the head is cop, which is modified by traffic; in line-backer the head is backer,
which is modified by line. Linguists distinguish at least three different semantic relations between the head and modifier(s) of compounds.
First, the compound represents a subtype of whatever the head represents. For instance, a traffic-cop is a kind of cop; a teapot is a kind of pot; a
fog-lamp is a kind of lamp; a blue-jay is a kind of jay. That is, the head names
the type, and the compound names the subtype. These are called endocentric compounds.
Second, the compound names a subtype, but the type is not represented
by either the head or the modifier in the compound. For example, Deadhead, redhead, and pickpocket represent types of people by denoting some
distinguishing characteristic. There is typically another word, not included
in the compound, that represents the type of which the compound represents the subtype. In the case of Deadhead, redhead, and pickpocket this other
word is person, so a Deadhead is a person who is an enthusiastic fan of the
band The Grateful Dead. These are called exocentric compounds.
Third, there are compounds in which both elements are heads; each contributes equally to the meaning of the whole and neither is subordinate
to the other, for instance, bitter-sweet. Compounds like these can be paraphrased as both X and Y, e.g., “bitter and sweet.” Other examples include
teacher-researcher and producer-director. These can be called coordinative
compounds.
Exercise
For each set of words below, say whether the words are endocentric,
exocentric, or coordinative compounds. Justify your identification.
a. redneck, yellowjacket, cocktail, blackhead
b. armchair, breathtest, rockopera
c. secretary-treasurer, scholar-administrator
As a third (and final) possible mode of analyzing compounds we briefly
consider that used in the series of modern traditional grammars prepared
by Quirk, Greenbaum, Leech and Svartvik (1972, 1985). In this method,
the compounds are analyzed and classified according to the relationships
among their constituents when the meaning of the compound is expressed
135
Delahunty and Garvey
as a phrase or clause. For example:
phrases
bee-sting
blood-test
swimming pool
adding machine
girlfriend
killer shark
windmill
motorcycle
self-control
clauses
sunrise
a sting by a bee
a test of blood
a pool for swimming
a machine for adding
a friend who is a girl
a shark which is a killer
a mill powered by wind
a cycle powered by a motor
someone able to control self
when the sun rises
table 3: underlying syntactic/semantic analysis of english
compounds
Exercise
Paraphrase each of the following compounds according to at least one
of the patterns in Table 3.
babysitter, catfish, cry-baby, story-teller, dancing girl, darkroom,
doorknob, taxpayer, security officer, sleepwalking
Other sources of words
Besides derivation and compounding, languages make use of coining, abbreviating, blending, and borrowing to create new words.
Coining is the creation of new words without reference to the existing
morphological resources of the language, that is, solely out of the sounds
of the language. Coining is very rare, but googol [note the spelling] is an
attested example, meaning 10100. This word was invented in 1940 by the
nine-year-old nephew of a mathematician (see Compact Edition of the Oxford English Dictionary Vol. III Supplement to the OED Vols. I-IV: 1987
p. 317).
Abbreviation involves the shortening of existing words to create other
words, usually informal versions of the originals. There are several ways to
abbreviate. We may simply lop off one or more syllables, as in prof for professor, doc for doctor. Usually the syllable left over provides enough information
136
Morpholog y and Word Formation
to allow us to identify the word it’s an abbreviation of, though occasionally this is not the case: United Airlines’s low cost carrier is called Ted. (Go
figure!) Alternatively, we may use the first letter of each word in a phrase
to create a new expression, an acronym, as in UN, US, or SUV. In these
instances the acronym is pronounced as a sequence of letter names. In other
instances, such as UNICEF from United Nations International Children’s
Emergency Fund, the acronym can be pronounced as an ordinary English
word. Advertisers make prolific use of acronyms and often try to make them
pronounceable as ordinary words.
Blending involves taking two or more words, removing parts of each,
and joining the residues together to create a new word whose form and
meaning are taken from the source words. Smog derives from smoke and
fog and means a combination of these two substances (and probably lots of
others); motel derives from motor and hotel and refers to hotels that are convenient in various ways to motorists; Prevacid derives from prevent acid; eracism derives from erase and racism and means erase racism or, if read against
the grain, electronic racism (cf. email, ecommerce, E-trade); webinar derives
from (worldwide) web and seminar. In November 2007, an interviewee on
an NPR news item created the blend snolo to refer to playing bike polo in
the snow.
Borrowing involves copying a word that originally belonged in one language into another language. For instance, many terms from Mexican cuisine, like taco and burrito, have become current in American English and are
spreading to other English dialects. Borrowing requires that the borrowing
language and the source language come in contact with each other. Speakers of the borrowing language must learn at least some minimum of the
source language for the borrowing to take place. Over its 1500 year history
English has borrowed from hundreds of languages, though the main ones
are Latin (homicide), Greek (chorus), French (mutton), Italian (aria), Spanish (ranch), German (semester), and the Scandinavian languages (law). From
Native American languages, American English has borrowed place names
(Chicago), river names (Mississippi), animal names (opossum), and plant
names (hickory).
The borrowed word never remains a perfect copy of its original. It is
made to fit the phonological, morphological, and syntactic patterns of its
new language. For example, the Spanish pronunciation of burritos is very
different from the English pronunciation. At the very least, the two languages use different /r/s and /t/s, and the plural marker {-s} is voiced in English
but voiceless in Spanish.
See our chapter on the History of the English Language in Book II for
137
Delahunty and Garvey
more on borrowing.
re g i s t e r s a n d w o rd s
Although most of the words we use every day can be used in almost any
context, many words of the language are restricted to uses in certain fields,
disciplines, professions, or activities, i.e., registers. For example, the word
phoneme is restricted to the linguistic domain. Interestingly, some words
may be used in several domains with a different meaning in each, though
these meanings may be a specific version of a more general meaning. For example, the word morphology is used in linguistics to refer to the study of the
internal structure of words and their derivational relationships; in botany
to refer to the forms of plants; in geology to refer to rock formations. The
general, abstract meaning underlying these specific meanings is the study
of form.
Besides words that may be used in almost any context and those that are
technical or discipline specific, there are words that play important roles
in academic discourses generally, for example, accuracy; basis; concept and
its related forms, conception, conceptual, conceptualize; decrease; effect; factor; indicate and its related forms, indication, indicative; and result. As such
words are used across disciplines, generally without local idiosyncrasies of
meaning, they are important words for English learners, both native and
non-native speakers. For a useful overview of the attempts to create lists of
such academic (or subtechnical) words and a new list of them, see Coxhead (2000) and the references therein (another academic word).
t h e i n t e rn a l s t r u c t u re o f c o m p l e x w o rd s
Complex words (those composed of more than one morpheme) are not
merely unstructured sequences of morphemes. For example, the plural {‑s}
suffix on dropouts must be added to the entire compound dropout, not to out
to which drop is then added. The reason for this is that the plural suffix may
be attached to nouns, but not to verbs or particles. Drop and out constitute
a noun only after they have been brought together in the compound.
We can use brackets with subscripts to represent these relations:
[N[N[Vdrop][Prtout]]s]. Alternatively, and equivalently, we can use tree diagrams to indicate the parts (constituents) of complex words and their structural relations:
138
Morpholog y and Word Formation
(14)
N
N
Pl
V
Prt
drop
out
s
Consider another example: unreadability. We analyze this word as
[N[Adjun1[Adj[Vread]abil]]ity], represented by the following tree:
(15)
Prefix
un1
N
Adj
Suffix
Adj
V
Suffix
read
abil
ity
Let’s consider this analysis more closely. The suffix {‑able} attaches to verbs
to create adjectives. Besides readable we have the adjectives doable, manageable, and attachable, which are derived from the verbs read, do, manage, and
attach, respectively. We can represent this part of the word as: [Adj[Vread]
able].
The prefix {un1-} attaches to adjectives, meaning “not” or “the converse
of.” Compare unwise, unfair, ungrateful, uncomfortable, unmanageable with
unreadable. All can be glossed as not having the quality denoted by the
adjective to which they are attached: “not comfortable,” “not fair,” etc. This
morpheme must be distinguished from the prefix {un2-} meaning “to reverse
the action,” which can be attached only to verbs (e.g., untie).
{Un1‑} cannot attach to the verb read; although there is the word unread,
pronounced [@nrEd], not [@nrid], an adjective meaning “not read” and derived from the past participle of read. Consequently, in unreadable, {-able}
must be attached to {read} to create the adjective readable. {Un1‑} may then
be attached to readable to create unreadable. We will represent this part of
the word as: [Adjun1[Adj[Vread]able]].
The suffix {‑ity} attaches to adjectives to create abstract nouns. Consequently it must be attached to the adjective unreadable. The structure of
139
Delahunty and Garvey
the entire word therefore must be: [N[Adjun1[Adj[Vread]able]]ity], as specified
above. In pronunciation the morpheme {-able} will be assigned its allomorph /@bIl/ (spelled <abil>, the same allomorph that appears in ability).
Exercise
Provide an analysis tree for each of the following words: retry, sinkable, thoughtless, meaningfulness, microorganisms.
classifying words by their morphological
properties
Once the morphemes of a language have been identified, their allomorphs
determined, and their distributions specified, we can use our analysis to assign the words of a language to parts of speech. For many words, inflections
provide the main basis of this assignment. Refer to Table 1 for the list of
English inflections.
Nouns can be identified as those words that can be inflected for plural.
Verbs are words that can be inflected for 3rd person singular present
tense, past tense, past participle, and progressive. These forms are often referred to as the principal parts of the verb.
Short adjectives and adverbs are words that can be inflected for comparative and superlative.
Derivational regularities can also be used to classify words. We can, for
example, classify as adverbs words derived from adjectives by the addition of
the suffix {‑ly}, e.g., quickly.
Classifying words on the basis of their internal morphological structure
works only up to a point. There are lots of words that are not internally complex and so cannot be classified without recourse to other types of criteria.
For example, the preposition to has no internal morphological structure and
so cannot be assigned to a grammatical class on that basis. Likewise, adverbs
such as hard or fast lack the characteristic {‑ly} ending. It becomes necessary
to use other criteria to classify these and many other words. We consider in
detail the principles which have been proposed for assigning words to parts
of speech in the chapters on Major and Minor Parts of Speech in this book.
Exercise
1. Discuss two relatively reliable criteria (don’t use spelling) for distinguishing words from morphemes and phrases. Illustrate your discussion
140
Morpholog y and Word Formation
with appropriate examples.
2. Derivation displays a range of patterns in English. Discuss three different derivational patterns, illustrating your description with appropriate examples.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Beers, Kylene. 2003. When Kids can’t Read. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Bauer, Laurie. 1983. English Word-formation. London, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
_____ 1988. Introducing Linguistic Morphology. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh
University Press.
Booij, Geert. 2005. The Grammar of Words. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Coxhead, Averil. 2000. A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly 34, 2:
213-238.
Haspelmath, Martin. 2002. Understanding Morphology. London: Arnold.
Huddleston, Rodney and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar
of the English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Matthews, P.H. 1974. Morphology: An Introduction to the Theory of Word
Structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Pinker, Steven, 1999. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York:
Basic Books.
Quirk, R., S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svartvik. 1972. A Grammar of
Contemporary English. New York: Seminar Press.
_____ 1985. Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London, UK:
Longman.
Spencer, A. 1991. Morphological Theory: An Introduction to Word Structure in
Generative Grammar. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
glossary
affix:
an inflectional or derivational morpheme; to attach an inflectional or
derivational morpheme to an expression.
allomorph: variant phonological representation of a morpheme.
auxiliary verb: a verb other than the main verb of a clause.
base: part of word to which an affix may be attached; may but need not be
a root morpheme.
bound morpheme: a morpheme that must be attached to another morpheme.
141
Delahunty and Garvey
constituent:
sentence).
a unified part of a construction (e.g., of a word, phrase, or
conversion:
derivational relationship between two words of different parts
of speech but without any formal marking of the difference.
coordinative compound: a compound word that denotes an entity or
property to which both constituents contribute equally; e.g., bittersweet refers to a quality which is both bitter and sweet.
derivation: process of changing a word from one part of speech to another
or from one subclass to another, typically by making some change in form.
endocentric compound: a compound word that denotes a subtype of
whatever is denoted by the head. Armchair represents a type of chair; breathtest represents a kind of test.
exocentric compound: a compound word that denotes a subtype of a category that is not mentioned within the compound; e.g., pickpocket represents
a kind of person, not a kind of pocket nor a kind of pick.
free morpheme: a morpheme that need not be attached to another morpheme, but can constitute a word on its own.
head: the main constituent of a compound, which may be modified by the
compound’s other constituents.
inflectional morpheme: a bound morpheme that signals a grammatical
function and meaning in a specific sentence, e.g., plural {-s}, past tense
{-ed}, comparative {-er}, superlative {-est}.
morph: a minimal meaningful form, regardless of whether it is a morpheme
or allomorph.
morpheme: the smallest part of a word that has meaning or grammatical
function.
prefix: a bound morpheme attached before a root.
realization: the representation of one or more abstract elements (e.g., morphemes) by concrete elements (e.g., sounds); e.g., women represents the
morphemes {woman} + {plural}.
root: the basic constituent of a word, to which other morphemes are attached.
suffix: a bound morpheme attached after a root.
suppletion: irregular inflectional forms of a word resulting from the combination of historically different sources; e.g., go/went.
a p p e n d i x a : s o m e e n g l i s h d e r i va t i o n a l
morphemes
(See Beers 2003: Appendixes D and E for other lists of roots and derivational affixes.)
142
Morpholog y and Word Formation
Prefixes
Class/category changing
a‑blaze
be‑calm
be‑friend
en‑tomb
Adj < V
V < Adj
V<N
V<N
Class maintaining
Nouns
arch‑monetarist
mal‑nutrition
micro-dot
mini‑dress
step‑father
Verbs
de‑escalate
Adjectives
a‑typical
cis‑lunar
extra‑sensory
Noun or Verb
fore‑ground
bacl-ground
mis‑fortune
mis‑lead
re‑arrangement
Noun or Adjective
ex‑President
ex‑orbital
in‑definite
mid‑morning
mid‑Victorian
Verb or Adjective
143
Delahunty and Garvey
circum‑navigate
circum‑polar
Noun, Verb, or Adjective
co‑author
counter‑culture
counter‑demonstrate
counter‑intuitive
dis‑ambiguate
dis‑bound
dis‑information
inter‑mix
sub‑conscious
sub‑let
Suffixes
Creating Nouns
from Nouns
‑dom
‑er
‑ess
‑ette
‑iana
‑hood
‑ism
‑let
‑ling
‑scape
‑ship
king‑dom
Birch‑er
lion‑ess
kitchen‑ette
Victor‑iana
man‑hood
absentee‑ism
stream‑let
duck‑ling
sea‑scape
kin‑ship
from Verbs
‑al
‑ary
‑ation (esp. with ‑ize)
‑ee
‑er
‑ment
‑ure
144
arriv‑al
dispens‑ary
categor‑iz‑ation
blackmail‑ee
kill‑er
manage‑ment
clos‑ure
Morpholog y and Word Formation
from Adjectives
‑ce
‑cy
‑dom
‑er
‑hood
‑ist
‑ity
‑ness
‑th
dependen‑ce
excellen‑cy
free‑dom
six‑er
false‑hood
social‑ist
divin‑ity
good‑ness
warm‑th
Derived Verbs
from Nouns
‑fy
‑ize
metr‑ify
Cambodian‑ize
from Adjectives
‑en
short‑en
Derived Adjectives
from Nouns
‑al
‑ate
‑en
‑ese
‑esque
‑ful
‑ic
‑less
‑ly
‑ous
‑y
from Verbs
‑able
‑less
education‑al (allomorphs/allographs: ‑ial, ‑ual:
presidential, habitual)
passion‑ate
wood‑en
Peking‑ese
pictur‑esque
doubt‑ful
algebra-ic
clue‑less
friend‑ly
venom‑ous
catt‑y
unbeliev‑able
count‑less
145
Delahunty and Garvey
‑ant/‑ent
‑atory
‑ful
‑ive
absorb‑ent
affirm‑atory
resent‑ful
generat‑ive
from Adjectives
‑ish
‑ly
green‑ish
good‑ly
Derived Adverbs
‑ly
‑ward(s)
‑wise
slow‑ly
in‑ward(s)
length‑wise
Miscellaneous
146
down‑er
iff‑y, upp‑itty
in‑ness, much‑ness, such‑ness,
there‑ness, why‑ness
thus-ly
6 The Major Parts of Speech
key concepts
Parts of Speech
Major Parts of Speech
Nouns
Verbs
Adjectives
Adverbs
Appendix: prototypes
i n t ro d u c t i o n
In every language we find groups of words that share grammatical characteristics. These groups are called “parts of speech,” and we examine them in
this chapter and the next. Though many writers on language refer to “the
eight parts of speech” (e.g., Weaver 1996: 254), the actual number of parts
of speech we need to recognize in a language is determined by how finegrained our analysis of the language is—the more fine-grained, the greater
the number of parts of speech that will be distinguished. In this book we
distinguish nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs (the major parts of speech),
and pronouns, wh-words, articles, auxiliary verbs, prepositions, intensifiers,
conjunctions, and particles (the minor parts of speech).
Every literate person needs at least a minimal understanding of parts
of speech in order to be able to use such commonplace items as dictionaries and thesauruses, which classify words according to their parts (and
sub-parts) of speech. For example, the American Heritage Dictionary (4th
edition, p. xxxi) distinguishes adjectives, adverbs, conjunctions, definite articles, indefinite articles, interjections, nouns, prepositions, pronouns, and
verbs. It also distinguishes transitive, intransitive, and auxiliary verbs. Writers and writing teachers need to know about parts of speech in order to be
able to use and teach about style manuals and school grammars. Regardless
of their discipline, teachers need this information to be able to help students
expand the contexts in which they can effectively communicate.
A part of speech is a set of words with some grammatical characteristic(s)
in common and each part of speech differs in grammatical characteristics
from every other part of speech, e.g., nouns have different properties from
verbs, which have different properties from adjectives, and so on. Part of
speech analysis depends on knowing (or discovering) the distinguishing
properties of the various word sets. This chapter describes several kinds of
properties that separate the major parts of speech from each other and de147
Delahunty and Garvey
scribes several ways in which to identify a word’s part of speech.
t h e m a j o r p a r t s o f s p e e c h : n o u n s , ve r b s ,
a d j e c t i ve s , a d ve r b s
The major parts of speech contribute the major “content” to a message, and
hence are sometimes called content words, as opposed to other parts of speech
known as function or structure words. The content words are the ones that
we see in newspaper headlines where space is at a premium and they are the
words we tend to keep in text messaging where costs per word can be high.
However, in most types of discourse, function words significantly outnumber
content words.
We begin our discussion of each part of speech by examining its traditional definition, which is generally either semantic or functional. We evaluate the traditional treatment and suggest more effective means of classifying the word type by referring to its formal characteristics. These include a
word’s potential inflectional morphology, its actual derivational morphology, and the positions in phrases and clauses in which it may occur. For
example, the word kingdom is a noun because it can be inflected for plural
(kingdoms); it ends in the noun creating suffix -dom; and it can occur after
the (the kingdom). We also examine some of the major functions of each
part of speech. Each section concludes with a discussion of subclasses of the
larger class.
Nouns
Traditionally, a noun is defined as a word that names “a person, place, thing,
or idea” (Weaver 1996: 252). This defines the noun category according to
what its members are assumed to typically denote, so it is a meaning-based
or semantic definition. (Occasionally this definition gets abbreviated to “a
noun is a person, place, or thing,” which makes no sense at all!) By Weaver’s
definition, Madonna, Pittsburgh, and Godzilla are all nouns, which is correct, so the definition provides a useful start. However, if we apply it precisely (and to be worth keeping, definitions should be precisely applicable),
then the word desk is not a noun because it denotes, not a thing, but a whole
class of things. Most nouns are like desk in this regard—peacock denotes not
a peacock but all the peacocks living now, as well as all those that existed
before, all those that will ever exist, and all the peacocks that we merely
imagine. If we want to refer to one peacock, we have to add a modifier such
as a—a peacock, cf. a desk, a book, a hard drive. We might revise our definition to take such nouns into account—“nouns name classes of persons,
places, things, and ideas.” But now we require Pittsburgh to refer not to one
148
The Major Parts of Speech
Pittsburgh, but to a whole set of them, which doesn’t seem quite right.
So, there is something right about saying that nouns name classes of
things, but there also seem to be nouns that name individual things. The
nouns that name classes of things are common nouns; the nouns (and other
types of expression) that name individual things are proper nouns: printer
is a common noun; Denver is a proper noun. In English, we conventionally
capitalize the initial letter of proper nouns. A common noun can be turned
into a proper noun, in which case it should be capitalized; for instance, we
have a friend whose dog’s name is Dog. Similarly, we can distinguish god (of
which there may be many) from God (which is presumed to be unique—at
least in some contexts).
Proper nouns name individual things. But these things are many and varied. They include individual people (Madonna), individual animals (Lassie),
individual places (Addis Ababa); individual things (Earth). We’ll have a lot
more to say about proper names in our chapter on Phrases.
We’ve said that common nouns name classes of things, but this needs
development. Certainly, books are things, but is grease a thing? Thing seems
to us to denote only things that can be individuated and counted—thing
one, thing two; one potato, two potatoes, and so on. But grease doesn’t seem to
allow this; we don’t (at least not typically) say two greases, or even just one
grease. Grease is like milk and information and lots of other similar words
in that it seems to denote stuff (physical or mental) rather than individual
things. So, we might revise our definition of noun again, and say that “common nouns name classes of things and stuff,” or if you prefer to go uptown,
“nouns name classes of entities and substances.” We’ll return to this issue below when we distinguish more fully between count and non-count nouns.
Unfortunately, characterizing nouns as names of things and stuff only
works if we limit our interpretation of “things and stuff” to just what nouns
name, which makes it utterly circular. Moreover, if we answer “yes” when
asked whether events, actions, states, characteristics, and relationships are
things, then we must allow that verbs, adjectives and other parts of speech
also name things. But surely that’s a bit of a problem, because verbs have
traditionally been assumed to name actions and states of being, adjectives to
name characteristics, and prepositions to name relationships. Our focus on
the typical meanings of nouns is what has gotten us into this fix. So we must
look at other characteristics of nouns if we are to have any success in finding
ways to identify them.
We’ve worked through the definition of noun as thoroughly as we did
because we take definitions seriously. We think they should be accurate
(remember our discussion of critical thinking in our Introduction to this
149
Delahunty and Garvey
book): imagine looking up the word dugong in a dictionary and finding it
defined merely as “a kind of animal.” Such a definition won’t help us use the
word accurately. Likewise, if we are to use the word noun accurately, then we
need to define it accurately. We need accurate definitions of parts of speech
to allow us to accurately determine which categories words belong to. And
this is important because a word’s part of speech determines whether and
how it can be inflected as well as its roles in phrases and sentences. We
want our definitions to provide us with criteria by which we can accurately
determine the part of speech of any word we choose to examine. For better
analyses we must consider the forms of words.
Formal characteristics of nouns
We approach the classification of nouns, and of the other major parts of
speech, through a series of simple formal tests. However, because no single test
will always lead to reliable results, part-of-speech identification requires multiple criteria and tests of different types. We cannot rely on a single test because
our tests are like any scientific tests—sometimes they give false positive results
(e.g., they tell us that we are ill when we aren’t) and sometimes false negatives
(e.g., they tell us that we are well when we are ill). This is primarily due to the
fact that each part of speech includes many sub-categories, each of which has
slightly different properties from the other sub-categories and which therefore
respond somewhat differently to our tests. As a result, we have to interpret our
test results cautiously. We say that a word belongs to a particular part of speech
to the extent that it passes the various tests for that part of speech.
analytic test 1. A word may be a noun if it ends or can end in the plural
inflection.
Table 1 shows the spoken and written versions of the regular noun inflection:
Plural: morphophonemically /s/, /z/, and /Iz/ or /@z/
spelled -s or -es (e.g., printers)
table 1: the regular noun inflections
The majority of English nouns accept the {-s} plural. The exceptions are
the small subclass of nouns that refer to animals (deer, fish, etc.), nouns that
denote stuffs (grease, oatmeal, ice), and nouns that mark the plural in idiosyncratic ways (child/children, man/men, woman/women, cherub/cherubim,
alumnus/a, alumni/ae). (A general principle of language is that irregularity
150
The Major Parts of Speech
tends to occur in the most frequently used or most over-learned items. As
a result, teachers can assume that native English speaking students know
many of the most frequently used irregular forms, although the irregularities
may vary from dialect to dialect.)
Exercise
1. Provide the inflected plural forms of the following nouns (i.e., apply
Analytic Test 1): insect, email, hinge, solo, calf, disease, coil, promise,
daisy. Pay attention to the words’ spelling and consult a dictionary if
you are uncertain.
2. The following words have undergone zero derivation/conversion:
rip-off, snap, wipeout, update. To each, apply Analytic Test 1 to show
that it is (or can be) a noun.
analytic test 2. A word may be a noun if it actually ends in a nominal
derivational suffix.
In English, the last derivational suffix on a word gives a strong clue to the
word’s grammatical class. If the last suffix is one of those listed in Table 2, then
that is a good indication that the word is a noun.
suffix -age
-ance/-ence
-ard
-cy
-dom
-er/or
-ess
-hood
-ism
-ist
-ity
-ment
-ness
-th
-(a)tion
example
acreage, mileage
tolerance, adherence
drunkard
decency
freedom
teacher, actor
actress
knighthood
existentialism
existentialist
activity
amusement
truthfulness
truth
adulation, fruition
151
Delahunty and Garvey
-ude
gratitude
table 2: common noun-forming derivational suffixes
Exercise
Why do you think English has so many different noun-forming derivational suffixes? (Hint: look up several of them in a large dictionary.)
A common role of derivational morphemes in a language is to change
words of one part of speech into related words of another part of speech. Thus
the verb tolerate becomes the noun toleration; likewise, the verb act becomes
the adjective active, which becomes the noun activity, by the addition of their
respective suffixes. Sometimes derivation will change a word to a different
subclass of the same part of speech, with a different, though related meaning. For example, the suffix {-hood} turns the noun knight into the noun
knighthood, just as {-dom} turns the noun king into the noun kingdom. As we
mentioned, only the final derivational suffix on a word determines its part of
speech: disestablishmentarianism contains four suffixes; the last, {-ism}, makes
it a noun. (Plural and genitive inflections may follow the derivational suffix
without affecting Test 2.)
Derivational suffixes are less useful than inflections as clues to nouns because of their limited productivity, that is, how freely they may be added
to words: {-er}, {-ness} and {-ity} are relatively productive noun-forming suffixes; we could, for example, add {-er} to a newly minted verb, e.g., to iPod
to create the noun iPodder. On the other hand, the {-th} suffix in depth can
no longer be used to derive nouns from adjectives; *lowth from low + th, cf.
height from high + th (the spelling is misleading here). (See Bauer 1983, 1988;
Huddleston and Pullum 2002; Quirk, Greenbaum Leech and Svartvik 1972
for discussions of productivity.)
Moreover, as you know, English allows zero derivation (conversion, category change, and functional shift), by which a word’s grammatical category
may be changed without any change of form, such as the addition of a derivational suffix. Thus the verb trade has been converted to the noun trade, as
illustrated by the ability of the latter to accept the plural inflection (trades). As
a result of zero derivation, there will be many derived nouns that have no derivational endings. Such forms may appear to students to possess the semantic
characteristics associated with their original class. For instance, the noun kick
will (accurately) seem to name an action rather than a person, place, or thing.
152
The Major Parts of Speech
This fact further illustrates the danger of semantic definitions.
Exercise
1. Using the derivational affixes in Table 2, apply Test 2 to determine
whether the following words are nouns: certitude, probity, wealth,
goodness, defilement, recency, boredom, editor, fragrance, characterization, transcendentalist, motherhood.
2. Check a dictionary for the meanings and other properties of the
noun-creating suffixes in Table 2.
analytic test 3. A word may be a noun if it can occur alone after a word
that typically precedes nouns and together they constitute a complete phrase.
Nouns can be identified by the company they can keep. Words that can
occur immediately before nouns and together with a noun create a potentially complete noun phrase are:
a. articles:
b. genitives:
c.
d.
e.
a, an (indefinite) (e.g., a bulldog)
the (definite) (e.g., the building)
my, our, your, his, her, its, our, genitive noun
phrases (e.g., my novel, our class, Sheila’s desk, the
man’s car)
demonstratives: this, that, these, those (e.g., that cup)
quantifiers: some, any, all, no, every, numerals (e.g., every
time, two pots)
ordinals (first, second, etc.) (e.g., first place)
most adjectives: good, subtle, etc. (e.g., good work)
Some of these forms—particularly demonstratives, quantifiers, and adjectives—can occur alone as phrases. It is their potential to combine with a
noun to constitute a noun phrase that is relevant here.
The possibilities listed above form the basis for frames. Frames consist
of context items, such as articles or demonstratives in the case of nouns,
and a test position where we put the word whose part of speech we want
to identify. For example, from the fact that an article and a following noun
can constitute a complete noun phrase, we can create the frame [the____]
to test for nouns. Here the is the context item and _____ is where we put
153
Delahunty and Garvey
the item to be tested. These tests operate simply. Just put the word to be
tested (e.g., defense, kitchen) into the test position, and if the combination of
context item and test item forms a grammatical noun phrase, the test word
is very probably a noun. For example, the fact that [the cattle], [the fish],
[the furniture] are all grammatical expressions shows that cattle, fish, and
furniture may all be nouns.
Exercise
Apply the frame [the_____] to show that apple, grievance, bellows,
invitation, and implement can all be nouns.
Words that cannot grammatically fill this test position are probably not
nouns, for example, *[the defend], *[the the], *[the this], *[the never], *[the
correctly]. (Remember, a * before an expression indicates that the expression
is ungrammatical.)
Exercise
Using the frame [the_____], show that increased, there, also, as, and
generate are not nouns.
From the remaining context items in Analytic Test 3, we can create other
frames for nouns, for example, [a(n)_____], [your_____], [my friend’s_____].
Exercise
1. Using the frames just above, determine whether defense, kitchen,
activity, active, certainty, certain, beating, demanding, limousine, depend, and luxurious can be nouns.
2. Create five more frames to test for nounhood using the context items
in Analytic Test 3. Then use your frames to determine whether any of
the following words can be nouns: force, graciousness, amplitude, vaporize, colossal, quietly. Check your analysis by applying Analytic Tests
1 and 2 to these words.
154
The Major Parts of Speech
Functional characteristics of nouns
Nouns have two main functions. The first, and perhaps less important one,
is that of modifier of other nouns, e.g., metal door, linguistics class. The
more dominant function is that of being the head of a noun phrase. Many
functions traditionally associated with nouns (e.g., subject, direct and indirect object of clauses, object of a preposition, subject and object complement) are really the functions of noun phrases. Hence we will postpone
discussion of these functions to our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns.
Subclasses of nouns
There are lots of different kinds of nouns, and in spite of our reservations
about using meaning as a criterion to determine parts and subparts of speech,
we will use aspects of meaning to distinguish the traditional subclasses of
nouns, but we will back up the semantic distinctions by pointing out formal
patterns that correlate with them. In fact, we can only be certain that meaning
distinctions really exist in the language if they correspond to distinct formal
patterns.
Proper nouns, as we have seen, are the words that best fit the traditional
definition of a noun—i.e., a word that names a person, place, or thing. Thus
your personal name names you (though it may also name other people); Denver names the capital city of Colorado, and Colorado names the state that Denver is the capital of—both of which are places and things. Note that proper
nouns are spelled with an initial capital letter, and if the proper name consists
of more than one word, e.g., the Statue of Liberty, then all the major words are
spelled with initial capitals. We will deal with complex proper names like this
in our chapter on Phrases. Some texts may vary in their treatment of nouns.
For example, McBeth (2001) sometimes capitalizes Gay and sometimes does
not, and Gee (1996) distinguishes discourse from Discourse.
Common nouns name classes of things. Individual physical objects are the
most straightforward instances of things, and the words that name classes of
such things are indeed generally nouns, e.g., book names the class of books and
hard drive names the class of hard drives. Nouns that name classes of physical
things are called concrete nouns. Other examples include sneeze, floor, and
paper.
Not all things are physical; some, like ideas, exist only in our minds. Words
for classes of things that exist only in minds, e.g., goodness, truth, beauty, and
reason, are called abstract nouns. Abstract nouns tend to be non-count (see
below) and to end in certain derivational suffixes, e.g., {-ness}, {-ity}, {-th},
{-ude}.
Common nouns may also name classes of collections of things; for exam155
Delahunty and Garvey
ple, platoon names the class of a particular type of collection of soldiers; team
names the class of collections of people gathered together for some common
purpose. Nouns that name classes of collections are called collective nouns.
Other examples include army and congress. In American English, collective
nouns normally take a singular verb (e.g., The jury is out), while in British
English they take a plural (e.g., The jury are out). The American variety sometimes uses the plural to suggest lack of unity within the group (e.g., The jury
are divided). Pronoun substitutes for collectives are also normally singular in
American and plural in British English.
An important subdivision of nouns, particularly for people learning English as a second language, is between count and non-count (also called mass)
nouns. Some non-count nouns are thought of as representing things as if they
were undifferentiated masses whose parts are not identified as discrete units
(rice, sugar, milk, news). Count nouns represent entities that can be individuated and counted (typewriter, diskette, page). It follows that a piece of news is
still news, but a piece of a diskette is not a diskette.
Formally, count nouns may be singular and plural (cup, cups); non-count
nouns are typically singular (information, *informations). Count nouns may
be preceded by the indefinite article (a day); non-count nouns may not (*a
furniture). Count nouns may be preceded by many (many bikes), while noncount nouns may not (*many dust). Count nouns may be preceded by not
many (not many kittens); non-count nouns may not be (*not many wealth).
Non-count nouns may be preceded by not much (not much rice/wealth); count
nouns may not be (*not much books).
If the head of the subject of a sentence is a non-count noun, then the verb
will be in the singular (The milk is in the fridge), but if the head is a count
noun, the number of the verb will depend on the number of the noun (cf. The
bottle is in the fridge; The bottles are in the fridge).
Zero derivation can recategorize count and non-count nouns. Non-count
nouns may become count nouns, and as a result may be pluralized. However,
they undergo a semantic shift—for example, to either type of something (e.g.,
the cheeses made in Wisconsin) or unit of something (e.g., three coffees). Analogously, count nouns may be recategorized as non-count nouns, but they also
undergo a semantic shift, for example, from individuals of the count noun
category (He caught a fish), to stuff derived from the individuals (He likes to eat
fish).
Exercise
1. Using the grammatical characteristics just discussed, say whether
156
The Major Parts of Speech
each noun in the following list is count, non-count, common, proper,
concrete, abstract, or collective. Some may belong to more than one
of these subclasses. For each one that does, say which subclasses it
belongs to: aluminum, class, college, couple, criterion, excellence,
information, member, Michigan, nomination, patience, platoon, tranquility, troop, Yosemite.
2. Some non-count nouns denote substances made up of small discrete
particles, and we can speak of individual particles or numbers of them
by modifying the noun with an expression of the form X of Noun, e.g.,
grain of wheat, kernel of corn. Identify ten more such nouns and the
expressions that denote their particles.
3. Other non-count nouns denote what Huddleston and Pullum (2002:
336) refer to as “aggregates,” that is, instead of denoting masses composed of very similar particles, they denote aggregates of miscellaneous things that typically share some function. These words also have
special individualizing words, e.g., piece of furniture, item of apparel.
Identify ten more such nouns and the expressions that denote their
individuals.
4. We also have special expressions for the portions we typically divide
some stuffs into, e.g., slice of cake, loaf of bread, wedge of pie. Identify ten more such nouns and the expressions that denote their typical
portions.
5. Using a selection of count and non-count nouns, determine which
subclass the following expressions may directly modify: enough, little,
each, neither, all. For example, sufficient can modify non-count but
not (singular) count nouns—sufficient money but *sufficient dollar.
6. The count/non-count distinction poses difficulties for non-native
speakers of English, at least in part because languages do not all make
the distinction in the same way. As a result, nouns that are translation
equivalents may belong to different subcategories. Thus information
is non-count in English, but its translation equivalents in French and
Italian are count. Check a piece of text written by a learner of English
to see whether the writer has full control over the count/non-count
distinction.
157
Delahunty and Garvey
Some remarks on the genitive case
The genitive case is typically indicated by the suffix {-’s}, e.g., Maria’s success.
The genitive expression modifies a following noun: Maria’s modifies success.
Its spelling is generally written as -’s (the book’s cover) if the expression it is
attached to is singular or is an irregular plural (the children’s toys), and either just an apostrophe if the expression it’s attached to ends in s (the Jones’
house), though editorial practice varies on this and some editors use -’s (the
Jones’s house).
While the genitive is generally spelled as -’s or ­-s’, there are some specific
exceptions. Several of the personal pronouns, as we’ll see in our chapter on
Minor Parts of Speech, have genitive forms ending in -s: ours, yours, theirs.
Note that there is no apostrophe in the spelling of these forms. This is particularly noteworthy in its, the genitive of it, which is frequently misspelled
as it’s. This error is due to two factors: first, its violates the general pattern
of spelling the genitive with an apostrophe (though it is consistent with the
sub-pattern that pronouns do not include the apostrophe); second, its may
be confused with the word it’s, which is a contraction of it is. Note that
whose, without the apostrophe, is the genitive of the pronoun who, while
who’s, the contraction of who is, is parallel to it’s.
Like the regular plural {-s}, the genitive has three allomorphs: [s] after
voiceless segments (Dick’s), [z] after voiced segments (Toni’s, Tom’s), and [@z]
or [Iz] after sibilants (Francis’s).
The genitive is often referred to as the possessive case. However, the genitive denotes far more than just possession. For example, Bill’s in Bill’s TV
show is in the genitive, but besides possessing the TV show, Bill might also
have been its producer, director, star, gaffer, key-grip, fan, or occasional
viewer. To avoid too narrowly characterizing the meaning of the genitive,
we prefer this term to possessive.
Exercise
Briefly discuss the possible meaning relations between the genitive expression and the noun it modifies in:
a. the candidate’s advisors
b. the book’s author
c. Andy’s pottery
d. the bird’s egg
e. the company’s CEO
f. the country’s attackers
158
The Major Parts of Speech
The genitive case has generally been regarded as an inflection suffixed
to nouns and pronouns. And while it is true that pronouns may take the
genitive inflection, it is more accurate to say that noun phrases, not nouns,
may take it. Note where the genitive inflection is attached in Oscar’s plays,
Humpty Dumpty’s fall, the kid’s skateboard, the kid next door’s dog, the guy
you broke up with’s car. Unlike the plural inflection, which is suffixed to the
head noun of an NP, the genitive inflection is suffixed to the end of the NP.
Although s-genitives occur on nearly all noun phrases, sometimes the alternative
of-phrase sounds stylistically more natural; cf. the cause of the accident vs. the accident’s
cause. In English, the inflected genitive is most comfortable with animate entities.
Verbs
Verbs can be subdivided into main and auxiliary verbs. We will treat the
various types of auxiliaries, such as may, might, and should, in our chapter on
Minor Parts of Speech and will concentrate here on main verbs, i.e., those
which may occur alone in a clause. Traditional grammars define verbs semantically, e.g., as words that represent activities (grow, kiss, freeze, run) and states
of being (be, have, resemble). States are unchanging situations while activities
are situations in which change occurs. (Activity verbs are also called dynamic
verbs, though the terminology is far from consistent.) State verbs typically
have to do with existence and static relationships. Just as nouns denote classes
of entities and stuff, verbs denote classes of states and activities.
As with most meaning-based definitions, this one is a tad simplistic. For
instance, nouns derived from verbs through zero derivation (e.g., strike,
kick, throw) maintain their sense of action, as nouns derived from verbs
by derivational affixing do (e.g., action). Likewise, verbs derived from
nouns—e.g., pot, as in to pot plants—may appear to retain some of the
entity-naming sense they had as nouns. In addition, students occasionally
classify certain adjectives as verbs, especially those adjectives that suggest
activity (e.g., vigorous, playful, cruel), and we’ve had a student who classified the preposition as as a verb because it denoted a relationship, as verbs
often do. Additionally, adjectives and other types of expressions may name
states, cf. to sleep and asleep. Nonetheless the semantic division of verbs is
a good place to start our discussion, though we’ll refine the activity/state
division in the exercises in this section.
As we noted in our discussion of nouns, it is important to correlate a semantic distinction with distinct formal patterns. The distinction between activity and state verbs correlates with whether or not a verb can occur in the
progressive aspect: activity verbs can (Oscar is growing tall); state verbs cannot
(*Oscar is resembling his father).
159
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. Using the progressive aspect test, determine for each of the following verbs whether it is an activity or a state verb: read, examine,
email, own, buy, know, dry, love, be, become. Did you run into any
problems? How did you solve them?
2. State verbs are particularly common in academic writing. Confirm
this by examining a selection of texts from that genre. Why do you
think state verbs are so common in this genre?
We saw that nouns may shift between subcategories, so it should be no
surprise to find verbs shifting between the state and activity subcategories. For
example, be is a state verb in Oscar is weird but an action verb in Oscar is being
weird, as its occurrence in the progressive shows. The former sentence means
that Oscar is generally or characteristically weird; the latter means that he is
acting weird, though we can expect him to snap out of it eventually.
Exercise
1. The distinction between permanent and temporary characteristics is
an important one. Compare Tigers are fierce with Tigers are tired. The
former sentence is grammatical and unremarkable. It represents a general characteristic of the class of tigers. The latter sentence is odd in
that it seems to attribute a characteristic we would normally assume to
be temporary as if it were a permanent characteristic of tigers in general. The permanent/temporary distinction is exploited in dictionaries.
Look up tiger and four other words in a dictionary and discuss how this
distinction is reflected in how they are defined.
2. For students who know Spanish. Spanish has two verbs that correspond to English be, ser and estar. What is the rule usually given for
when to use each of these verbs? Check your answer in a Spanish dictionary, grammar, or textbook.
Like all words, individual verbs may have more than one meaning. Consequently, we might expect one meaning of a verb to represent an activity and another meaning of the verb to represent a state. Smell is such a
verb. The sentence John is smelling the roses is grammatical in the progressive
160
The Major Parts of Speech
and therefore has an activity interpretation. Now compare The roses smell
musty, which is non-progressive and grammatical, with *The roses are smelling musty, which is progressive but ungrammatical. From data like this we
must conclude that smell also allows a state interpretation.
There are many more subclasses of verbs and we present some of them in
the following exercises to allow you to develop your understanding of verbs
to the degree you need or want.
Exercise
1. Just on the basis of your intuitions, classify the following verbs as
state verbs or activity verbs: cost, depart, approve, approve of, remember, remain. What difficulties did you experience in classifying
these verbs semantically? Check your classification by using the progressive test.
2. Look up the word appear in a good desk dictionary. How many different meanings does it have? Identify which of these meanings indicate
states, changes of state, or actions.
3. Clean and tidy are activity verbs. What change of state does each
describe? Paraphrase the verbs using the adjectives clean and tidy.
Think of several more such verbs and their associated adjectives.
4. Which kind of verb (state or activity) can be used to answer the
question, What did X do? Supply example sentences, both grammatical
and ungrammatical (e.g., She learned American Sign Language vs. *She
knew American Sign Language) to support your answer.
5. A distinction related to change of state is between telic and atelic
verbs. Telic verbs represent events that have a natural end point, the
accomplishment of some purpose, or a change of state, e.g., make,
evaporate. (These are also called accomplishment or resultative
verbs.) Once something is made or has evaporated, the making and
evaporating processes must stop for that thing. As with change of state
verbs, telic verbs in the progressive represent processes before their
completion. The water is evaporating implies that the water has not
yet fully evaporated. Atelic verbs represent events as having no natural
end-state or product, e.g., golf, as in The CEOs are golfing. A verb may
be telic on some occasions, e.g., The children are playing a game of
161
Delahunty and Garvey
chess, but atelic on other occasions, e.g., The children are playing. For
each of the following sentences decide whether it represents a telic or
an atelic situation and justify your decision.
a. She wrote a poem.
b. She writes poetry.
c. The water froze.
d. The water is freezing.
e. The plane arrived.
6. The count/non-count distinction in nouns is similar to the telic/
atelic distinction in verbs. Count nouns represent classes of bounded
entities; telic verbs represent classes of bounded situations. Non-count
nouns represent unbounded classes of things or substances; atelic verbs
represent unbounded classes of events. Thus, a piece of a chair is not
a chair, but a piece of paper is still paper. Similarly, writing a piece of
a poem is not the same as writing a poem, though a piece of writing is
still writing. What conclusions might you derive about human cognition
from this similarity between nouns and verbs?
7. Identify all the main verbs in Exercise 6 just above. Then classify
each main verb as state or activity. Which kind of verb predominates in
that piece of text? Can you divide the text into two sections, each with
a different rhetorical purpose? Do the verbs in the two sections differ?
Why do you think that might be?
8. We can divide the category of activity verbs into those that represent events that take just a point of time (punctual verbs), e.g., tap,
and those that take a period of time (durative verbs), e.g., read. In the
progressive, punctual verbs strongly suggest repeated action. Oscar is
tapping his fingers impatiently describes multiple finger taps. But Oscar is reading strongly suggests a single, continuous episode of reading.
Put each of the following verbs into the progressive, then determine
whether the resulting expression denotes multiple, repeated events,
or a single, continuous activity: punch, beat, nap, flap, wink, close,
run, work.
9. Change of state (process) verbs are yet another subclass of activity
verb. As their name suggests, they describe change from one state to
another, e.g., melt. If something melts then it changes from a solid to
a liquid state. But notice how such verbs are interpreted when they
162
The Major Parts of Speech
are in the simple past tense (The ice melted describes the completed
change) and in the progressive aspect (The ice is/was melting describes
the melting in progress but not yet complete). By comparing their interpretations in the past tense and the progressive, show that the following verbs are also change of state verbs: freeze, evaporate, arrive,
ignite, die.
10. A change of state verb simply represents some entity as undergoing or having undergone a change from one state to another. However,
sentences may also include information on what caused the change of
state; compare The cook thickened the sauce with The sauce thickened. The former sentence tells us who caused the sauce to thicken;
the latter does not. Let’s call clauses that include information on the
cause of a change of state action clauses, and their verbs action verbs.
(Remember, activity and action verbs are different.) Generally, simple
change of state clauses are intransitive with the entity undergoing the
change of state represented as the subject, while action clauses are
transitive with the subject representing the cause of the change of
state and the object representing the entity changed. Here are some
instructions from recipes whose verbs are action verbs:
a. Preheat oven to moderate.
b. In a saucepan melt the butter.
c. Boil the milk.
d. Thicken the sauce.
e. Brown the meat.
f. Dissolve the sugar in the boiling water.
Why do you think that the instructions in recipes use so many action
verbs?
11. Here are some sets of verbs from the instructions in other recipes:
(a) peel (potatoes), core (apples), bone (meat); (b) cube (meat); (c)
cut (meat), chop (vegetables); (d) place (ingredient in pot), layer (ingredients in pot); (e) add (ingredient to pot); (f) fry, sauté, broil; (g)
boil, simmer. What information, other than simply “do something to
something,” do the verbs in each group convey?
12. Think of ten verbs that include the meaning “movement to/from
somewhere,” for example, go, come, run, and bicycle. What information other than just movement to/from somewhere do these verbs include? If you know Spanish, translate float (e.g., The log floated into
163
Delahunty and Garvey
the lake) and roll (e.g., We rolled the barrel off the truck) into that
language and note how that other information is expressed. How would
you describe the difference between English and Spanish in how they
express that other information?
13. Begin, start, keep, continue, stop, finish, and quit denote parts,
facets, or aspects of events (which is why they are called aspect verbs).
Which aspect of events does each of the verbs in the list denote?
14. Say, tell, announce, ask, answer, argue, deny, sing, yell, and whisper all have to do with communication. What information, other than
just “communicate,” does each verb convey? Communication verbs like
these are particularly common in conversation, news, and fiction. You
can check this for yourself by taking samples of each genre. Why do you
think communication verbs are common in these genres?
15. Know, think, feel, want, and mean represent mental states and
activities. These are particularly common in conversation and fiction.
Collect some samples of these two genres, identify the main verbs in
your samples, and determine how many are mental verbs. Why do you
think conversation and fiction are particularly rich in these verbs?
The semantic divisions of verbs represented in many of the exercises just
above is only one of many. (See Biber et al. 1999, Cruse 2004, Frawley
1992, and Gregory 2000, Quirk et al. 1972, for various categorizations.)
Clearly, because their meaning is so extraordinarily complex, the semantics
of verbs may confuse students. It is simpler to use formal characteristics to
identify verbs.
Formal characteristics of verbs
We must first distinguish formally between main verbs and auxiliary verbs.
The main verb can appear by itself in a verb phrase; an auxiliary verb regularly appears only before a main verb. Consider sentence (1):
(1) The building collapsed onto the streets.
In this sentence, collapse is the main verb; it cannot be removed from the
sentence without producing an ungrammatical result (e.g., *The building
onto the streets). The main verb will always be the farthest to the right in any
164
The Major Parts of Speech
series of English verbs (e.g., The building may have collapsed). In addition,
auxiliaries can be inverted in questions (e.g., Will the building collapse?),
while main verbs cannot (e.g., *Collapsed the building?).
Just as we did with nouns, we use formal analytic tests to determine
which words are verbs. English verbs potentially allow four inflections:
a. 3rd person singular present tense (spelled -s or -es and pronounced /s/,
/z/, and /Iz/ or /@z/).
We symbolize verbs with this inflection as Vs.
For example, Harris bakes strudel regularly.
b. Past tense (in regular verbs, spelled -d or -ed and pronounced /t/, /d/,
and /Id/ or /@d/).
We symbolize verbs with this inflection as Ved.
For example, Harris baked strudel last night.
c. Ing-form (spelled -ing and pronounced /IN/).
We symbolize verbs with this inflection as Ving; it normally occurs
with a form of the auxiliary verb be, or with a similar verb.
For example, Harris is baking strudel.
d. En-form (in regular verbs, spelled and pronounced identically to the
past tense).
We symbolize verbs with this inflection as Ven; it normally occurs
with the auxiliary verb have to create the perfect aspect, or with forms
of be to create passive sentences.
For example, Harris has baked/eaten strudel. (Perfect aspect sentence.)
For example, Harris was pursued/eaten by a lion. (Passive sentence.)
table 3: verb inflections
Using these inflectional possibilities, we can create a test for verbhood:
analytic test 4. A word may be a verb if it can take some or all of the four
types of verb inflections: Vs, Ved, Ving, Ven.
Traditionally, Ving is called the present participle. When this form occurs with a form of the auxiliary be, it is part of the progressive aspect,
which typically denotes an activity in progress, as in Harris is baking. As a
marker of the progressive, {-ing} is usually regarded as an inflection. However, Ving also occurs in structures traditionally known as gerunds, e.g.,
Parting is such sweet sorrow. When {-ing} is part of a gerund, it is regarded
as a derivational morpheme because its addition causes a change in part
165
Delahunty and Garvey
of speech, namely from verb to noun. Ving forms may also be adjectives
derived from verbs, for example, a crying baby, a peeping tom. Note that
a “present” participle, that is, the Ving form itself, whether as the progressive or as an adjective, tells us nothing about when the activity occurred.
Whether the situation represented by Ving took place in the past, is occurring at the time of speaking, or may occur in the future, is indicated by other
parts of the clause, such as by tense marking on an auxiliary verb. Gerunds
are similar: the events are not related to the time of speaking.
Traditionally, Ven has been called the past participle form of the verb.
When the Ven form occurs with the auxiliary have to create the perfect aspect
or with the auxiliary be to create the passive, it is usually regarded as an inflection because it does not cause a change in part of speech. However, there are
adjectives in English that are formally identical to past participles, e.g., broken
heart. In cases like this the affix is to be regarded as a derivational morpheme
because it does cause a change of part of speech. For some verbs, the past participle form and the derived adjective form differ, e.g., A drunken sailor; She
has drunk all the martini. Again, although the Ven form is traditionally called
a “past” participle, like the “present” participle, the situations it represents are
not related to the time of speaking.
Exercise
By adding the Vs, Ved, Ving, and Ven inflections show that each of the
following words can be a verb: provide, help, demonstrate, outline,
promote.
Tests based on inflections are so valuable that some traditional grammar
books, especially those used to teach non-native speakers, provide extensive
lists of the principal parts of verbs. Rather than using inflections to identify
verbs, however, these grammars often suggest that students memorize these
vast lists. A sample of such a list appears below as Table 4:
166
The Major Parts of Speech
V
Vs
Base
Infinitive
Present
Ved
start
clean
try
run
bring
see
throw
shrink
hit
started cleaned
tried
ran
brought
saw
threw
shrank
hit
starts
cleans
tries
runs
brings
sees
throws
shrinks
hits
Past
Ving
Ing-form
Progressive
starting
cleaning
trying
running
bringing
seeing
throwing
shrinking
hitting
Ven
En-form
Past Participle
started
cleaned
tried
run
brought
seen
thrown
shrunk
hit
table 4: principal parts of selected verbs
Table 4 also shows some important properties of verb inflections. First,
certain inflections sometimes require changes in the spelling of the root (e.g.,
tries, hitting). Phonologically, these words follow the regular patterns noted
above. Second, for regular verbs—e.g., start, clean, try—the past tense and
Ven-forms are identical in sound and spelling. Irregular verbs—those which,
for historical reasons, differ from the general pattern—display a variety of inflectional differences, but almost exclusively in the Ved and Ven-forms. With
very few exceptions (e.g., is, has, does), Vs and Ving forms fit the general pattern. Moreover, some irregular verbs, such as bring and hit, follow the same
pattern as regular ones by having identical Ved and Ven forms, though these
are not formed by adding {-ed}. Besides varying among themselves in the formation of Ved and Ven forms, irregular verbs sometimes have dialect variants.
For instance, shrink has the alternate past shrunk. Although shrank resembles
the historically older form of the verb, shrunk reflects a pressure in modern
English to make irregulars more regular, especially by making the Ved and
Ven forms identical. Indeed, the archaic shrunken is used now only as an adjective, e.g., shrunken head. Only a few modern English verbs, such as broken
and frozen, actually still use {-en} for the past participle form, though dialects
vary—British English uses mown as past participle, whereas American English uses mowed. Finally, irregular verbs often have regional and social variants
that may be stigmatized, particularly in academic settings. Students are often,
unfortunately and inaccurately, considered slow or ignorant if they use dove,
drunk, and seen as the past tense forms of dive, drink, and see. We discuss these
issues in our chapters on Usage and Language Variation in Book II.
167
Delahunty and Garvey
Enormous lists of principal parts of verbs may be useful for reference purposes or for non-native speakers, but they are not a generally useful pedagogical tool. For native speakers, rote memorization of such lists is a mindnumbing pursuit of trivia. Knowledge of the basic patterns discussed above,
along with normal native-speaker intuition will allow any native English
speaking student to produce the principal parts of all common English verbs.
For instance, the Ved form can be determined by placing the verb in a simple
sentence beginning with the word yesterday (e.g., Yesterday I drank a gallon of
grapefruit juice). The Ven form can be obtained by placing the verb in a simple
sentence following the auxiliary have or has, for example, I have run two miles.
Exercise
1. Identify which of the words below are verbs, using as many of the inflectional criteria above as possible. Do not be surprised if, with a little
ingenuity, you can turn almost any word into a verb. Such potential innovations attest to the power of zero derivation and to the limitations
of semantic definitions:eraser, elbow, sense, fork, several, even, easy,
always, up
2. Because of historical changes in English, formerly inflectional morphemes have come to be derivational morphemes that are pronounced
the same as their inflectional counterparts. This change affects two
forms. First, {-ing} has come to occur on nouns formed from verbs, as
in the grumblings of the sailors. Note that the {-ing} word may be pluralized and/or preceded by the. Second, both {-ing} and {-en} have become adjective endings in some words, as in more interesting remarks
and very frozen pipes. If a word can be modified by very, quite, and
rather, it is an adjective or an adverb, not a verb. Remember also the
kinds of words that precede nouns. Consider the sentences below and
argue that the italicized expression is or is not a verb.
a. Frederick’s constant working out in the gym
b. The inner workings of a computer
c. A shrunken head
d. Juan’s penetrating observations
analytic test 5. A word may be a verb if it actually ends in a verbal derivational affix.
168
The Major Parts of Speech
Table 5 lists some typical verbal derivational affixes:
suffixes
-ify
-ize/-ise
-en
-ate
examples
magnify
canonize, advertise
lighten
participate, fluctuate
table 5: typical derivational verb suffixes
Exercise
1. Using the list of affixes in Table 5, show that the following words
are verbs: traumatize, customize, sanitize, stupefy, electrify, brighten,
darken, anticipate, punctuate.
2. Check a dictionary for the meanings and grammatical characteristics
of each of the suffixes in Table 5.
3. For each italicized word below, add, remove, or change one or more
derivational affixes to make it into a verb. Double-check your answer
by using inflectional criteria. Assassin (noun), tight (adjective), critical (adjective), alive (adjective), fat (noun, adjective), extermination
(noun), harmony (noun).
analytic test 6. A word may be a verb if it can be immediately preceded by
words that typically precede verbs.
Verbs have the potential to occur immediately after several classes of other
words:
a. auxiliaries (be, have, and do)
b. modals (will, would, can, could, may, might, shall, should, and must)
c. to (infinitival) (e.g., to have and to hold)
We can create test frames based on these patterns, for example, [to____]
(e.g., to be), [VPdo_____] (e.g., did endure).
169
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
Using the frames [VPwill_____] and [VPdo_____], show that recommend,
find, know, assume, and inquire can all be verbs.
It is important to use as many such frames and contexts as possible. The
reason for this is that many of the context words can be ambiguous (e.g., to
may also be a preposition) and they may appear before other parts of speech.
As an example, let us ask whether apply can be a verb.
(2)
a.
b.
c.
d.
I am applying.
I have applied.
I will/can/should apply.
I want to apply.
These examples show that apply can be a verb. Also, to apply test frames, we
must allow the inflections of the word under scrutiny to vary to fit the specific
test, for instance, the test word in [have____] must be in past participle form,
e.g., have sown. Finally, we must make certain that the test sentences are appropriate. For instance, to can have a directional meaning, as in We drove to
Paris. Confusing the preposition to with the infinitive to will lead to wrong
results. The infinitival to in (2d) has no meaning; it serves only to mark a verb.
(To make matters more complex, there is a third to that indicates purpose, as
in I drove to relieve tension. When it is used in this way, to can be followed by
a verb.)
Exercise
1. Create five more frames based on the context items in Analytic Test
6 and apply them to attend, participate, give, conclude to show that
they can be verbs.
2. Using Analytic Test 6, determine which of the words tall, stretch,
underestimate, replace, and playful can be verbs.
3. Using the verb frames in the text and those you created yourself,
identify the verbs and non-verbs among the following words: individual, learn, engage, various, actually, accomplish.
170
The Major Parts of Speech
Functional characteristics of verbs
Main verbs have one function, to be the head of a verb phrase (VP). As
such, they may be preceded by auxiliaries, followed by objects and complements, and modified in various ways. As in the case of noun phrases, a VP
may consist of a single word (e.g., Harris left). We will deal with VPs in our
chapter on Phrases.
Formal subclasses of verbs
Verbs may be (and in dictionaries for native speakers of English, usually are)
subdivided into transitive, intransitive, and linking. Transitive verbs (e.g.,
see, arrest) require a direct object, which typically takes the form of a following noun phrase (e.g., The police arrested Steve Biko). Intransitive verbs (e.g.,
die) do not take a direct object (e.g., He died). Some verbs may be transitive
in some sentences and intransitive in others. For example, run is intransitive
in She runs regularly but transitive in She runs the company. Linking verbs (be,
become, seem) must be followed by a subject complement, which may appear
as either a noun phrase (e.g., He is a nurse) or an adjective phrase (e.g., She is
aware of the situation). Traditional grammars often refer to the former as a
predicate nominal and to the latter as a predicate adjective. In either case,
with a linking verb, the subject and the complement both refer or apply to the
same individual (he—a nurse; she—aware of the situation).
Exercise
1. Check a dictionary to see which of the following is regarded as transitive, which intransitive, and which both: lift, anticipate, arrive, endure.
2. Check a dictionary to see if it distinguishes linking verbs from other
types. Look up the verbs be, become, and seem and report on whether
and how they are categorized as linking verbs by your dictionary.
3. Compare the part of speech and subcategory information given about
give and send in a dictionary for native speakers and in one for English
learners.
Adjectives
While traditional grammars usually define nouns and verbs semantically, they
171
Delahunty and Garvey
often shift to functional criteria to characterize adjectives. A typical definition
of adjective is “a word that modifies a noun or pronoun.” (Occasionally you
will see adjectives defined as “words that describe nouns,” which makes no
more sense than saying that “nouns are persons, places, and things.” If adjectives describe anything, it is whatever the nouns they modify denote.) While
we might criticize the traditional definition for changing from meaning to
function, it is more appropriate to determine whether it leads to reasonably
successful identification of adjectives.
The definition holds good in simple cases, such as old shoes, offensive remark, and matters inconsequential, though in the last case, students may have
trouble recognizing the second word, rather than the first, as an adjective. But
in each case, the adjective does modify a noun, which serves as the head of
the phrase. However, words that are clearly not adjectives may modify nouns;
for instance, stone in stone wall is, by formal criteria, a noun and not an adjective (cf. stones and stone’s). Likewise, the in the wall shows none of the formal
characteristics of adjectives, though it clearly modifies the noun, wall. In a
nutshell, the fact that a word modifies a noun is not a sufficient reason to call
it an adjective.
The traditional, functional definition suffers also because it claims that adjectives may modify pronouns. Clearly an adjective cannot modify a pronoun
in any of the examples below:
(3) a. *old them
b. *offensive it
c. *they inconsequential
To justify the inclusion of pronouns, grammarians refer to a different
use of adjectives, as in sentences (4) and (5):
(4) The judge was late.
(5) She was ill.
In (4) and (5), the adjectives late and ill are predicate adjectives or subject
complements. But the use of these separate labels suggests—correctly—that
such uses of adjectives are really not instances of modification at all, but rather
of complementation. Any student who tries to relate such examples to clear
cases of modification will become befuddled.
Notice also the difference in the meaning of late in (4) and in the late judge.
While most adjectives can occur as noun modifiers and as predicate adjectives, there are some that are specialized for only one of these two roles. For
172
The Major Parts of Speech
example, former can only occur as a noun modifier (The former senator vs. *The
senator is former) and alive, asleep, askew can occur only predicatively (*The
alive/asleep cat vs. The cat is alive/asleep).
Exercise
Check the words former, asleep, alive, askew in your dictionary to see
if their limitations are mentioned. Then check them in a dictionary
designed for learners of English as a second language, such as the Cambridge International Dictionary of English, to see how they are treated
there.
Clearly, adjectives may modify nouns. But because the ability to modify a
noun is neither a necessary nor a sufficient basis for adjectivehood, we must
augment the functional criteria with more reliable formal ones.
Formal characteristics of adjectives
The major formal characteristic of an adjective is its ability to be compared:
analytic test 7a. A word may be an adjective if it can be made comparative
and/or superlative by the addition of the inflectional suffixes {-er} and {-est}. (Applies to short words.)
analytic test 7b. A word may be an adjective if it can be made comparative and/or superlative by being modified by more and most. (Applies to longer
words.)
Exercise
Create four test frames for adjectives based on Analytic Tests 7a and
7b. Apply your frames to show that traditional, perfect, unfavorable,
similar, and subordinate may be adjectives.
Comparison is a semantic change in adjectives that is regularly signaled by
formal means. The uninflected forms are in the positive degree; the inflected
forms are in the comparative and the superlative.
173
Delahunty and Garvey
positive
old
beautiful
comparative
older
more beautiful
superlative
oldest
most beautiful
A short adjective (one of 1-2 syllables) takes the {-er} and {-est} inflectional
endings. Longer adjectives, including some 2-syllable words such as alone,
may be modified by more and most, a class of words called intensifiers which
includes very and quite.
Exercise
Using Analytic Tests 7a and 7b, identify which of the following words
can be made comparative and/or superlative: strong, honest, retaliate, harsh, uncommon, local, intensify.
While these criteria are very powerful, they do not work for all adjectives, especially scientific adjectives such as nuclear and barometric. Fortunately, other
types of tests are available:
analytic test 8. A word may be an adjective if it actually ends in an adjectival derivational suffix.
Table 6 lists some of the major adjectival suffixes in English.
-ish
-al
-ar
-ful
-some
-y
-ic
-able/ible
-ing
-ed
boorish, skittish
comical, alphabetical
nuclear, circular
cheerful, careful
winsome, awesome
funny, uncanny
choleric, atmospheric
debatable, sensible
interesting, amusing
disputed, concerned
table 6: adjectival derivational endings
174
The Major Parts of Speech
Exercise
1. Extend the list of adjectival derivational endings. If you are in doubt
about an ending consult a good desk dictionary or a reference grammar.
2. Which of the italicized words in the sentences below are adjectives?
Justify your answers solely by Tests 7a, 7b, and 8.
a. Your tie is outlandish.
b. I have no particular doubts about your proposal.
c. The chamber contains more particular matter than that one.
d. Zubin is quite unstable.
e. Some dogs are mean.
3. The last two derivational suffixes in Table 6 are superficially identical
to verb forms in the present participle (Ving) and the regular forms of
the past tense (Ved) and past participle (Ven). Think of example sentences that the adjectives appear in; think of sentences in which the
verb forms appear. How can you differentiate the two? (We have noted
that there is an ongoing historical process through which participles
shift to adjectives. The process apparently occurs word by word. Can
you identify any other verbs that are currently in the course of becoming adjectives?)
4. In a dictionary, look up the meanings and other grammatical characteristics of the adjective-creating suffixes in Table 6.
The tests we have provided eliminate from the list of adjectives many sorts of
words that have been traditionally included with this class, to the confusion of
many people. For instance, cardinal numerals such as five and ordinal numerals such as fifth cannot be called adjectives, since we do not say *fiver or *fivest
or *fifther or *fifthest. Similarly we exclude (a) the articles the and a/an; (b) the
demonstratives this, that, these, and those; (c) indefinites, including quantifiers
such as all, no, every; (d) possessive pronouns such as my, your, their; and (e)
interrogative pronouns such as what and which. All of these forms regularly
modify nouns. None of them are adjectives.
To Tests 7 and 8, we can add one further formal feature of adjectives, their
position in a sentence. Adjectives occur in a very limited set of positions.
The two most common are (a) between a determiner (an article or article-like
word) and a noun, and (b) after a linking verb (i.e., of the be-become-seem
175
Delahunty and Garvey
type). We will discuss the words that function as determiners in our chapter
on Minor Parts of Speech. Briefly, this class includes words such as a/an, the,
this, that, some, every, and many. Thus the italicized words in sentences (6a-c)
are adjectives:
(6) a. The recent discovery of HG 116 . . .
b. This remarkable discovery . . .
c. Some unnerving developments . . .
Examples of adjectives that appear after be-become-seem verbs are:
(7) a. I am steadfast.
b. She grew stubborn.
c. He appears pig-headed.
Using these patterns we can create another analytic test for adjectives:
analytic test 9: A word may be an adjective if it can occur (a) between a
determiner and a noun, or (b) after verbs of the be-become-seem class.
These tendencies are not as strong as the earlier analytic tests that we proposed, since non-adjectives can appear in both positions; however, they may
support the tests in doubtful cases.
Exercise
Create two test frames for adjectives based on Analytic Test 9, and apply them to prominent, certain, seasonal, different, and next to show
that they may be adjectives.
As we mentioned earlier, adjectives and adverbs may occur after words such as
very, rather, and quite:
(8) a. Very tired
b. Quite bored
c. Rather inflexible
analytic test 10: A word may be an adjective if it can be modified by very,
quite, or rather.
176
The Major Parts of Speech
Exercise
Create a test frame based on Analytic test 10 and apply it to book, desperate, print, readable, squishy, and depend to determine which may
be adjectives.
We must make a rather important distinction here between gradable and
non-gradable adjectives. Gradable adjectives are those that can be inflected for
comparative and superlative and modified by more, most, very, somewhat, and
similar words. Thus smart is a gradable adjective: smarter, smartest, very smart.
Most of the adjectives we’ve discussed in this section are gradable. Total, on
the other hand, is non-gradable: *She’s a totaler/very total freak. Other nongradable adjectives include absolute, atomic, electronic, entire, single, as you can
demonstrate for yourself by trying to inflect or modify them. Non-gradable
adjectives may over time become gradable, and this process may cause usage
controversy, as the recent history of unique shows. For more on this word,
we suggest that you check unique in a dictionary, preferably one such as the
American Heritage, which includes thoughtful usage notes.
Our final test for adjectivehood relies on the fact that only adjectives can
occur in the frame [as ___ as a(n) X], where X is an appropriate noun. For
example, in as fearless as a lion, fearless is in the test position and lion is a noun
filling the X position. Because the expression is grammatical we can infer that
fearless may be an adjective.
Exercise
Apply the test frame [as ___ as a(n) X], with appropriate nouns in place
of X, to the following words to determine which are adjectives: reasonable, round, table, give, quickly, homeward, reddish.
Functions of adjectives
While adjectives often appear to directly modify nouns, in fact they primarily function as heads of adjective phrases (AP), which we discuss in our
chapter on Phrases.
Subclasses of adjectives
Traditional grammarians employ a set of distinctions that we will not adopt
because some of the subcategories are based on a confusion of form and function. However, for purposes of completeness, we list these distinctions here,
177
Delahunty and Garvey
but treat the forms elsewhere. Descriptive adjectives are those adjectives that
satisfy Analytic Tests 7, 8, and 9. Like nouns, this group is sometimes subdivided into common (e.g., honest, alive) and proper (e.g., Atlantic, Indian).
Proper descriptive adjectives are typically derived from proper nouns and
many do not allow comparison, though they regularly end in derivational
suffixes typical of adjectives.
Aside from descriptive adjectives, traditional grammars recognize as
adjectives other forms which are not formally adjectives but may modify
nouns. Throughout this section, we have tried to justify their exclusion from
the adjective category. Table 7 identifies some of these subclasses, each of
which confuses a word’s part of speech with its function.
Noun as Adjective
Pronoun as Adjective
Adverb as Adjective
Possessive Adjective
Demonstrative Adjective Interrogative Adjective
Relative Adjective
Numeral Adjectives
Article as Adjective
Phrase as Adjective
Clause as Adjective
Easter bonnet
This situation
Far South
Someone’s lunchbox
Such effrontery
Whose signature is this?
The person whom I will help
Five guesses
The truth
Members in good standing
Anyone who desires an education
table 7: traditional subclasses of adjectives
The attempt to classify such a disparate group of structures as adjectives
destroys the possibility of any consistent system of parts of speech. In our
approach, none of the above categories exist. Instead, we would call the
members of Table 7 “noun as modifier,” “pronoun as modifier,” “article as
modifier,” “phrase as modifier,” etc.
Adverbs
The traditional definition of adverb is “a word used to modify a verb, an adjective, or another adverb.” This definition is clearly functional and actually
represents the typical functions of adverbs (or at least, adverb phrases) fairly
well, e.g., Run quickly, extremely adroit, remarkably cleverly.
178
The Major Parts of Speech
Exercise
To what extent do the italicized adverbs below conform to the traditional definition?
a. Atwood writes clearly.
b. Clearly, Atwood wrote the letter.
c. This sample is obviously atypical.
d. Obviously, this sample is atypical.
e. Belinda smiled hopefully.
f. Hopefully, Belinda will bring some refreshments.
The definition above omits an important function of adverbs, namely, modifying a complete sentence, as in b, d, and f in the exercise just above, and
in (9):
(9) Frankly, I don’t like calamari.
Here the adverb frankly indicates that the speaker feels he or she is being
candid in uttering the sentence.
And as we have now grown to expect from functional definitions, the
traditional definition of adverb will predict false positives, i.e., predict that
certain words or expressions are adverbs when they are not. For instance,
compare cautiously with with caution in The officer approached the motorist
cautiously/with caution. Both expressions function identically—both tell us
the manner in which the officer approached the motorist, i.e., both modify the verb approached. However, while cautiously is definitely an adverb,
with caution is just as definitely a prepositional phrase. We will formally
distinguish true adverbs from other phrases that can fulfill some of the same
functions as adverbs, but we will refer to all expressions that function like
adverbs with the cover-term adverbial.
Our approach, as usual, will be to begin with a formal characterization
of adverbs. We will then return for a brief review of the functional division
of adverbs according to what they modify—sentences, verbs/verb phrases,
adjectives, and adverbs. Finally, we will indicate some of the traditional semantic categories of adverbs.
Formal characteristics of adverbs
Many adverbs cannot be inflected, but those that can are indistinguishable in that respect from adjectives. Hence we can minimally adapt the
formal criteria we used for adjectives in 7a and 7b to apply to adverbs:
179
Delahunty and Garvey
analytic test 11a. A word may be an adverb if it can be made comparative
and/or superlative by the addition of the suffixes {-er} and {-est}. (Applies to short
words.)
analytic test 11b. A word may be an adverb if it can be made comparative and/or superlative by being modified by more and most. (Applies to longer
words.)
analytic test 11c. A word may be an adverb if it can be modified by intensifiers such as very, quite, or rather.
Test 11a, in fact, rarely applies, since the language contains relatively few
one-syllable adverbs. Hard and fast are examples. One such form, well, has
irregular comparative and superlative forms, better and best. Colloquially,
words such as quick, soft, slow are inflected for the comparative:
(10) She threw it quicker/softer/slower than anyone expected.
Such usages, though, are usually regarded as prescriptively incorrect, the forms
more quickly, etc., being preferred. In general, Test 11b serves as the norm of
prescriptively acceptable comparison of adverbs:
(11)a.She threw it more quickly/softly/slowly/cautiously than anyone
expected.
b.That is most often the case.
Exercise
Apply Tests 11a, b, and c to the following words to show that they may
all be adverbs: far, long, often, soon.
Derivational tests also apply to adverbs, though there are only a few adverbial suffixes:
analytic test 12. A word may be an adverb if it actually ends in an adverbial derivational suffix.
Some typical adverbial suffixes are listed in Table 8.
180
The Major Parts of Speech
-lyquickly, frequently, awkwardly, cautiously (when {-ly} is
added to adjectives)
-wise
lengthwise, otherwise
-ward
homeward
table 8: adverbial derivational endings
Exercise
Use the derivational endings listed in Table 8 to show that the following words may all be adverbs: fondly, gradually, northward, onward,
recently, colorwise, edgewise.
Because there are so few adverb-creating suffixes, it might seem difficult to
distinguish between adjectives and adverbs. In practice, however, it is usually
fairly simple to tell them apart because of their positions in sentences.
analytic test 13a. Adverbs do not occur in the positions typically occupied
by adjectives.
analytic test 13b. Adverbs tend to be relatively movable in a sentence.
Let us use the adjective frequent and its related adverb frequently as examples of these criteria. As we have seen, adjectives can occur between determiners and nouns or after be-become-seem verbs, as in (12a) and (12b):
(12) a. Harriet was a frequent visitor.
b. Harriet’s visits were frequent.
Adverbs in these positions are ungrammatical:
(13) a. *Harriet was a frequently visitor.
b. *Harriet’s visits were frequently.
The sentences below show the results of applying Test 13b to the adjective
frequent and the adverb frequently:
(14) a. Harriet was a frequent visitor.
181
Delahunty and Garvey
b.*Frequent, Harriet was a visitor
c.*Harriet was frequent a visitor.
d.*Harriet was a visitor frequent.
e. Frequently, Harriet was a visitor.
f. Harriet was frequently a visitor.
g. Harriet was a visitor frequently.
The portability of adverbs in sentences is not random; they tend to occur
in three positions: (a) at the beginning of a sentence; (b) at the end of a sentence; and (c) in the sequence of auxiliary verbs, especially after the first. Due
to restrictions on particular adverbs, not all will occur in all three positions:
(15) a. I will never leave you.
b.*Never I will leave you.
c. Never will I leave you.
d. *I will leave you never.
Exercise
Using Tests 13a and 13b, determine which of the following words may
be adverbs: academically, budgetwise, ever, friendly, portly, recently,
reluctantly, southward, friendly, still, ungodly, unholy, worldly. Confirm your results by applying Tests 11 and 12.
Functions of adverbs
Adverbs serve as heads of adverb phrases (AdvP). Like adjectives, however,
they accept only a few preceding modifiers (mainly more/most, very, and quite)
and a limited range of following prepositional phrases (bolded) (e.g., more
rapidly than a speeding locomotive). Again, since most adverbs are unmodified, many grammarians include among their functions those that properly
apply to AdvPs. We will have more to say about adverb phrases in our chapter
on Phrases.
As we noted, adverbs and adverb phrases modify sentences, verbs/verb
phrases, adjectives and adverbs. We briefly discuss and illustrate these functions.
Sentence modifiers have two major functions. They can indicate a speaker’s
evaluation of the truth of the sentence, as in (16a), or the speaker’s feelings
about the situation represented by the sentence, as in (16b) and (16c).
182
The Major Parts of Speech
(16) a. Clearly/apparently/obviously, Wonkers is a schizo.
b. Frankly/honestly, my dear, we don’t want a dam.
c. Luckily/fortunately, I regained control of the car.
Sentence modifiers also connect one clause or part of a clause with another,
as in (17a)-(17c).
(17) a. The paramedics arrived and eventually Oscar was stabilized.
b. Summer arrived; however, the weather remained poor.
c. He gambled away his inheritance, and consequently had to work for a living.
The class of expressions referred to as “transition devices” by composition
teachers includes such connective adverbs.
The following examples illustrate adverbs (bolded) modifying verbs/verb
phrases (18a,b), adjectives (19a,b), and adverbs (20a,b) (italicized):
(18) a.
b.
(19) a.
b.
(20) a.
b.
He waved frantically.
She coolly aimed the pistol.
It’s a wonderfully wicked play.
It was hideously deformed.
They approached extremely hesitantly.
He reacted remarkably angrily.
Exercise
Prescriptive grammarians often object to the use of hopefully as a sentence modifier, as in Hopefully, my paycheck will arrive soon. Check
the usage labels on this word in a current dictionary. Does your dictionary mention this use of hopefully? If so, is it mentioned disapprovingly?
What (if any) reasons support the disapproval of this word? What does
the word mean? Is it an adverb or some other part of speech? What kind
of adverb is it? Does it differ grammatically or semantically from other
adverbs in its class?
Semantic subcategories of adverbs
Adverbs are often classified semantically in terms of Time, Place, Manner,
Frequency, and Degree. Table 9 illustrates these categories.
183
Delahunty and Garvey
meaning
Time
Place
Direction
Manner
Frequency
Degree
examples
today, yesterday, now, then
here, there
northward
well, slowly, convincingly, quietly
often, regularly
completely, thoroughly, absolutely
table 9: semantic classes of adverbs
These categories are worth remembering, since most of them also apply to
prepositions, which will be considered in our chapter on Minor Parts of
Speech. In addition, some of these adverbs (e.g., then, there) serve as substitutes for prepositional phrases.
Our analysis eliminates the traditional categorization of words such as very
and quite as adverbs, though the examples below show that they can modify
adjective and adverbs:
(21) a. very old
b. quite frequently
c. only occasionally
Traditionally, these words are often lumped together with degree adverbs. We
have already classified these words as intensifiers.
Exercise
Apply Analytic Tests 12-13 to demonstrate that the italicized words in
(21) are not adverbs.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Bauer, Laurie. 1983. English Word-formation. London, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
_____ 1988. Introducing Linguistic Morphology. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh
University Press.
Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad, and Edward
Finegan. 1999. Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow,
UK: Longman.
Celce-Murcia, Marianne, and Diane Larsen-Freeman. 1999. The Grammar
184
The Major Parts of Speech
Book: An ESL/EFL Teacher’s Course. (2nd ed.) Rowley, MA: Newbury House
Publishers, Inc.
Cruse, Alan. 2004. Meaning in Language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Frawley, William. 1992 Linguistic Semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Gee, James Paul. 1996. Social Linguistics and Literacies: Ideology in Discourses.
(2nd ed.). London, UK: Taylor and Francis.
Gregory, Howard. 2000. Semantics. London, UK: Routledge.
Hudleston, Rodney and Geoffrey Pullum. 2002. Cambridge Grammar of the
English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
McBeth, Mark. 2001. In C.G. Panetta (ed.) Contrastive Rhetoric Revisited and
Redefined. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
Noguchi, R. 1991. Grammar and the Teaching of Writing: Limits and
Possibilities. Urbana-Champaign: NCTE.
Pinker, S. 1999. Words and Rules. New York: Basic Books.
Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech, and Jan Svartik. 1972.
A Grammar of Contemporary English. London, UK: Longman.
Quirk, Randolph, and Sidney Greenbaum. 1973. A Concise Grammar of
Contemporary English. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
Radford, Andrew. 1988. Transformational Grammar. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Weaver, Constance. 1996. Teaching Grammar in Context. Portsmouth, NH:
Boynton/Cook Heinemann.
glossary
abstract noun: noun that denotes entities apprehended by the mind, e.g.,
truth, belief. See concrete noun.
adjective phrase: a phrase with an adjective as its head.
adjunct: modifier within a verb phrase.
adverbl phrase: a phrase with an adverb as its head.
attributive adjective: function of an adjective (phrase) that precedes (or occasionally follows) its head noun. See predicate adjective.
category change: See conversion.
collective noun: a noun that denotes a group of individuals (army, jury,
the public, The United States).
common noun: a noun that refers to classes rather than to specific individuals, e.g., tissue, box, xylophone. See proper noun.
comparative: degree of an adjective or adverb, signaled by -er or more.
complement: expression that completes a construction. See object comple-
185
Delahunty and Garvey
ment, subject complement.
concrete noun:
noun that denotes an entity that can be apprehended by
any one of the five senses, e.g., sneeze, floor, paper. See abstract noun.
content word: words (nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs) that express
the major information of a sentence. See function word.
conversion: change of part of speech without change of form.
count noun: a noun that represents entities that can be individuated and
counted, and hence can be made plural, e.g., typewriter, diskette, page. See
non-count noun.
degree adverb: adverb indicating the extent to which an adjective or adverb
applies. See intensifier.
descriptive adjective: any adjective that meets the formal requirements for
adjectives.
function word (also called structure word): words representing grammatical information. See content word.
functional shift: See conversion.
gerund: in traditional grammar, a verb phrase that functions as a subject
or object.
head: main word of a phrase.
infinitive: (a) a form of a verb without any inflection—i.e., the form that
one would look up in a dictionary, e.g., eat. Abbreviated as V. (b) the same
form of a verb when preceded by to, e.g., to eat.
intensifier: words such as very and quite that modify adjectives and adverbs.
intransitive verb: a verb that does not accept a direct object.
linking verb: a verb that is followed by a subject complement.
main verb: head of a verb phrase or predicate.
modifier: optional expression that qualifies or restricts the denotation of
another expression.
nominalization: the process by which a word (or group of words) becomes
a noun (or noun phrase), often through the addition of a derivational suffix.
non-count noun (also called mass): a noun thought of as representing
things in the world as undifferentiated masses, whose parts are not identified as discrete individuals (rice, sugar, milk). See count noun.
object complement: expression that complements a direct object. See subject complement.
participle: the Ving or Ven inflectional form of a verb; in traditional grammar, a verb form that modifies a noun.
past participle: the Ven inflectional form of a verb.
positive: degree of adjective or adverb that is not compared. See comparative and superlative.
186
The Major Parts of Speech
predicate adjective:
adjective (phrase) that appears after the verbs be, become, seem, etc. See subject complement.
present participle: the Ving inflectional form of a verb.
principal parts: a list of the infinitive and inflectional forms of a verb: V,
Vs, Ving, Ved, and Ven.
productivity: the degree of freedom with which a linguistic process, e.g.,
nominalization, applies to items in its range.
progressive: aspect indicated by be + Ving.
proper noun: a noun that refers to individual entities rather than to classes
of entities, e.g., Thomas Jefferson, Denver, the Koran. See common noun.
sentence modifier: a function of adverbials to describe such things as the
speaker’s manner of presenting information in a sentence, or the speaker’s
judgment about the truth of the sentence.
stem: form of word to which affixes may be attached.
structure word: See function word.
subject complement: function of an adjective phrase or noun phrase after
verbs such as be, become, and seem.
superlative: degree of an adjective or adverb, signaled by -est or most.
transitive verb: a verb that requires a direct object.
zero derivation: See conversion.
Appendix
to major parts of speech
Prototypes
Perhaps the greatest frustration for students—and teachers—of grammar
is the discovery that seemingly clear and airtight definitions and tests fail
to work smoothly in all cases. We argued in our chapter on Major Parts of
Speech that one source of this difficulty is the faulty status of definitions, for
instance, those that determine parts of speech on the basis of their meanings
or functions. Our system augments such definitions with a set of formal
conditions pertaining to morphological (inflectional and derivational) and
syntactic (positional) characteristics of words. As some of our exercises demonstrate, not all conditions will apply in all cases. For instance, the condition that nouns can be made plural might seem to exclude many words that
are clearly nouns by other criteria, e.g., cattle and furniture. On the other
hand, these words can accept the genitive, as in the cattle’s thirst and the
furniture’s delivery. Additionally, the {-ure} morpheme on furniture is typical
to nouns: armature, ligature, caricature, and signature, though other words
besides nouns may appear to end in the {-ure} morpheme: mature (adjective
or verb), and insure (verb). In other words, we seem to find cases where our
187
Delahunty and Garvey
conditions (a) fail to apply to all members of a parts-of-speech class, and (b)
seem to apply to words outside the class that the conditions are chosen to
identify.
Two natural reactions to this situation are possible. One response would
simply be to ignore the anomalies and present the conditions as absolutes.
This approach requires that conditions be both necessary and sufficient.
(Conditions are necessary if they all have to apply; they are sufficient if no
other conditions are needed.) This approach has the serious disadvantage of
colliding head-on with reality.
A second response would give up the entire enterprise of defining parts
of speech as too haphazard to be worth doing. But if we adopt this course
of action, we will never learn anything about language and our students will
wrongly assume that it is utterly chaotic.
As you might suspect, we view both of these extreme positions as fundamentally wrong—wrong about the nature of language and wrong about the
way in which language should be studied. Let us now examine why.
Let’s begin with two exercises (or party games, if you prefer). First, ask
a group of your friends to make a list of ten birds. If you tally your lists of
birds, you will find that certain names appear early on many lists (e.g., eagle,
robin, sparrow) while others appear later (e.g., owl, crow). Also, some names
will appear on almost all lists while others (e.g., chicken, penguin, ostrich)
will appear less frequently or not at all.
The next step is to ask your subjects why they made their choices. They
will probably say that birds have feathers, lay eggs, and are able to fly. And
in the clearest cases, all these conditions apply. However, in less clear cases,
some but not all of the conditions apply or the conditions conflict. For example, robins have feathers, fly, and lay eggs, while chickens typically do not
fly, or at least do not fly very far; penguins and ostriches do not fly at all.
While our experiment tells us about how we categorize actual birds, it
also tells us about how we use the word bird. When we use an expression
like bird, we group together objects not so much on the basis of a rigid set
of characteristics, but on a set of criteria or conditions that we use flexibly.
In this way we group together objects on the basis of what the philosopher
Ludwig Wittgenstein called “family resemblances.” You might envision,
then, a target, with some objects close to the bull’s eye—those entities that
are clearly birds. Those entities that fall closest to the center of the target are
called prototypes. They possess all of the features typical of the category.
Toward the periphery of the target lie entities that are less “birdy,” according
to how many of the conditions they meet. In language, also, the boundaries
between classes of words may be fuzzy. That is, just as we may occasionally
188
The Major Parts of Speech
have trouble deciding whether to call a particular creature a bird (e.g., is
archaeopteryx a bird?), we may also have difficulty deciding whether a word
is a noun or an adjective (e.g., poor). See figure 5.1 in Aitchison (2003: 56).
Of course, not all conditions are of equal importance. Some are essential; their lack disqualifies something as being an instance of a category.
For instance, a bird must lay eggs rather than produce live young. At the
other extreme, some conditions are excluded; they must not be present. A
bird cannot have four legs. Between these extremes lie other conditions. Expected conditions are associated with normal or typical characteristics, such
as flight for birds. Creatures that lack an expected feature—e.g., penguins,
which cannot fly—may serve to make the object an atypical or defective
member of its class, without disqualifying it from membership altogether.
Some conditions are merely possible; they result from common associations of the object. For example, birds commonly eat worms. Yet one would
hardly be surprised at a bird that ate only seeds. Finally, certain conditions
are unlikely, though not strictly impossible. A 1000-pound animal that met
all the essential bird criteria might strain the imagination, but we would
probably be willing to categorize it as a bird (Cruse, 1986).
We thus seem to identify objects on the basis of their resemblance to
certain prototypes—an object which we consider the most typical member
of the category. In other words, prototypes share all of the necessary and
expected conditions, perhaps some of the possible ones, and none of the
excluded or unlikely ones.
How does the notion of prototypes relate to grammar? Well, labels such as
“noun” and “verb” have much the same status as “bird.” We can state a set of
conditions—inflectional, derivational, and syntactic—that allow us to classify
words in a relatively consistent and logical fashion. However, cases arise when
not all of the conditions apply. That is, certain nouns may be less “nouny”
than others. Nevertheless, nouns demonstrate a family resemblance to one
another because they share many characteristics. Of course, you can expect to
encounter words that cause difficulties, since the borders of the noun category
are fuzzy. For instance, consider the following words that end in -ing: interesting, meeting, sing, singing. Let us consider the condition of taking the plural
morpheme, along with the related feature of appearing in the noun slot two
____. We can thus immediately eliminate interesting (*two interestings) and
perhaps sing (?two sings), unless we are Native Americans. We can immediately
qualify meeting as a noun (two meetings). Singing raises some problems: is two
singings grammatical? Speakers will vary in how they answer this question,
indicating that the expression lies on the border of the noun category. Note,
however, that the variation has no impact on the force of the conditions. Even
189
Delahunty and Garvey
someone who accepts the phrase as grammatical will readily agree that it is not
a typical use of the word singing. Thus we might conclude that the capacity to
be made plural is an expected—though not essential—condition for nouns.
You might object that the notion of prototypes leads to linguistic anarchy. Perhaps there are no essential conditions. Moreover, if standards are
flexible, aren’t we in danger of measuring with a rubber ruler? This reaction,
however initially reasonable, has no real justification. In fact, our position
allows the maintenance of analytic standards without reducing grammar
either to legalistic rigidity or anarchic mush.
For one thing, English itself is grammatically flexible. The prevalence
of conversion from one part of speech to another with no formal change
provides one clear example. English allows almost any word to be converted
to another part of speech, at least in restricted contexts, as the following suggest:
(1) a. Ifs, ands, or buts (Subordinating and coordinating conjunctions to nouns)
b. Whys and wherefores (Interrogatives to nouns)
c. But me no buts. (Coordinator to verb and noun)
What we see here are instances of linguistic creativity, the ability to
make infinite use of finite linguistic resources, which as we’ve seen, is
characteristic of all languages. Clearly, the concepts we use to describe
language should be able to account for the linguistic characteristics we
actually observe. Categories arranged around prototypes and which allow
fuzzy boundaries allow us to do this. Such categories do not require that
we abandon our standards of linguistic usage; rather, they should encourage us to study and understand the language as it is used, not merely judge
it on the basis of simplistic a priori assumptions.
Lest this sound too abstract, let’s consider some practical consequences of
the fact that not all criteria apply to every word in a class. One consequence
is that we must accept that all major classes of words consist of subclasses of
those words, e.g., those nouns that cannot be made plural (e.g., information,
independence). Some of the nouns that cannot be made plural constitute the
subclass of non-count (mass) nouns, though there are many other subclasses
of nouns, of which we’ve seen a few. Teachers should be knowledgeable
about the most important subclasses of words and their linguistic properties, e.g., count and non-count nouns, transitive and intransitive verbs, and
so on, and be able to accurately present them to their students, especially to
those students whose first language is not English.
190
The Major Parts of Speech
A second consequence of prototypes is that they allow us to see similarities between classes of words. For instance, they encourage us to ask questions such as how is a noun like a verb? This question may seem like a riddle,
but if we consider the two subclasses of verbs—transitive and intransitive—
on the formal basis that the former but not the latter may take an object, as
in (2a,b):
(2) a. The Broncos defeated the Jets.
b. The rabbit disappeared.
In (2a), defeat is inflected for past tense and so is clearly a verb, and because
it is followed by the direct object (the Jets) it is transitive. In (2b), disappeared
is likewise a verb, but it is intransitive since it cannot be followed by a direct
object, as (3a,b) show:
(3) a. *The rabbit disappeared itself. (i.e, made itself disappear.)
b. *The magician disappeared the rabbit.
Corresponding to the verbs in (2), are nouns that resemble them:
(4) a. The Broncos’ defeat of the Jets.
b. The rabbit’s disappearance.
Note that example (4a) contains the noun defeat (it can be made plural),
which has been converted from a verb and which is modified by the genitive phrase The Broncos’, which corresponds to the subject of the verb defeat
in (2a), while the prepositional phrase of the Jets corresponds to the direct
object the Jets in (2a). Likewise, the derived noun disappearance in (4b) corresponds to the verb disappear in (2b). However, just as the verb disappear
cannot accept an object, the noun disappearance cannot take an of-phrase
complement corresponding to a direct object, as shown by the ungrammaticality of (5a,b). (Compare to (3a,b).)
(5) a. *The rabbit’s disappearance of itself.
b. *The magician’s disappearance of the rabbit.
In other words, certain nouns have restrictions that closely parallel those of
transitivity on verbs.
The facts of English grammar thus suggest that the rigid separation of
parts of speech conceals a potentially rich network of similarities among
191
Delahunty and Garvey
categories, similarities that might prove interesting for teachers of writing. A
prototype approach encourages one to explore, rather than ignore, possible
connections between categories.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s f o r a p p e n d i x
Aitchison, J. 2003. Words in the Mind. (3rd ed.) Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Cruse, A. 1986. Lexical Semantics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Labov, W. 1973. The boundaries of words and their meanings. In C.-J. N.
Bailey and R.W. Shuy, (eds). New Ways of Analyzing Variation. Washington,
DC: Georgetown University Press.
192
7 The Minor Parts of Speech
key concepts
Minor parts of speech
Pronouns
Articles
Auxiliary verbs
Negation
Prepositions
Intensifiers
Conjunctions
i n t ro d u c t i o n
In addition to the major parts of speech—nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs—there are many minor classes of words. Their number varies according to the level of detail in a particular analysis; more important is the clarity
with which classes are distinguished. Our approach in the previous chapter
used formal criteria to try to arrive at consistent assignments of words to
parts of speech. Our approach in this chapter will be similar.
The minor word classes are often referred to as closed classes. This is
because they have relatively few members, which are added to only occasionally. As a result, some of the minor form classes can be defined simply
by listing their members. We will distinguish about a dozen of these classes,
but the largest of them, the prepositions, has only about 50 members. Native English-speaking students do not need to memorize the members of
these classes; they can become as consciously familiar as they need to be with
them through presentation and practice. Some memorization may help
some non-native English speaking students, but, again, proper presentation
and lots of practice should enable them to develop control over these words.
Minor word classes have several properties in common. First, they tend
not to alter the basic content of a sentence. For this reason, they have sometimes been called function or grammatical words, in contrast to the major
parts of speech, called content words. Content words bear the main semantic burden in communication. They are the words that you would use
to send a text message: Broke. Send money. Minor words, in contrast, signal
modificational, relational, and interactional aspects of meaning; contextual
information; or redundant grammatical information. Compare I am broke;
will you please send some money with Broke. Send money. The words omitted
from the second version are all minor class words. Members of the minor word classes occur more frequently than mem193
Delahunty and Garvey
bers of major classes. All of the 50 most common words in English are
function words; they account for about 60% of words used in speech and
45% of those used in writing. (You can check this for yourself by counting the words in this paragraph, then counting the words belonging to the
major parts of speech, and subtracting that number from the whole. The
remainder is the number of words belonging to the minor parts of speech.)
Though sometimes used to dramatize the sorry state of English, this statistic
is as true of Henry James’ prose as of the most pedestrian discourse. This is
because the minor words are essential for indicating important modifications to, and relations among, the content words, regardless of whether the
content words were chosen by the brilliant or the dull. Because they are
important for integrating content words into the structural organization of
sentences, minor class words are sometimes referred to as structure words.
In presenting the minor word classes, we will proceed mainly by listing some or all of their members. We will also note semantic, functional,
and formal characteristics. Except for pronouns, the formal properties of
these items do not (in English) include inflectional or derivational marking.
Rather, they emerge from the item’s ability to combine with other words,
phrases, or sentences. For instance, after is a preposition because it can combine with a noun phrase (bolded), as in after the announcement. The combination of a preposition and its following noun phrase is called a prepositional phrase. We will examine this and other phrases in our chapter on
Phrases. Since minor class members may enter into several different types of
combination, they will sometimes (like content words) be members of more
than one class. For example, when after is followed by a clause, as in After
the announcement appeared, we received many phone calls, it is traditionally
thought of as a subordinating adverbial conjunction.
p ro n o u n s
The traditional definition of “pronoun” is “a word used in place of one or
more nouns.” Let’s test the adequacy of this definition by examining some
examples:
(1) a. Jonathan felt sorry for Jeremy, so he repaired his bike for him.
b. Because he wanted to sell it, Jonathan repaired his bike.
c. Jonathan repaired his bike.
The traditional definition is a formal one; you can easily test it by replacing each
one of the pronouns with either of the nouns Jonathan or Jeremy in (1a-c).
While the replacement definition of pronouns seems to work well enough
194
The Minor Parts of Speech
for (1a-c), ask yourself what their replaces in sentence (2):
(2) All of the members of the class elected Juan as their representative.
Clearly, if it replaces anything, it replaces a version of All of the members of
the class, which is very definitely not a noun or mere list of nouns. In fact,
all of the members of the class is a noun phrase, a group of words that has a
noun (in this case, members) as its head word. Sentences like (2) (and there is
an infinite number of them) show that the standard definition of a pronoun
must be amended at least to read “a noun or noun phrase.” But even this
reformulation is not accurate. If we replace the noun members in sentence
(2) with a pronoun, it becomes ungrammatical:
(3) *All of the them of the class elected Juan as their representative.
A pronoun replaces a noun only when that noun is the only word, and therefore the head, in its phrase. We must conclude that pronouns always replace
entire noun phrases. For the moment, let’s define a pronoun as “a word that
replaces a noun phrase.”
Any discussion of pronouns must address the issue of how we decide what
a particular pronoun refers to in a specific sentence. For example, one very
likely interpretation of (1c) is Jonathan repaired Jonathan’s bike. On this interpretation, his is assumed to refer to whoever Jonathan refers to, presumably
Jonathan. (Because a pronoun and its antecedent refer to the same entity in
the discourse world, they are said to co-refer.) So the noun phrase, Jonathan,
is used to determine the referent of his. A noun phrase that determines the referent of a pronoun is said to be that pronoun’s antecedent. This term used to
mean “going before,” and in most cases, a pronoun’s antecedent does precede
the pronoun, though sentence (1b) shows that an antecedent can sometimes
follow its pronoun.
However, sentences (1a-c) are systematically ambiguous. They can mean
either (1d-f) or (1g-i), respectively:
(1) d.Jonathan felt sorry for Jeremy, so Jonathan repaired Jeremy’s bike
for Jeremy.
e.Because Jonathan wanted to sell it, Jonathan repaired Jonathan’s
bike.
f. Jonathan repaired Jonathan’s bike.
g.Jonathan felt sorry for some person, so Jonathan repaired that
person’s bike for that person.
195
Delahunty and Garvey
h.Because some person wanted to sell it, Jonathan repaired that person’s bike
i. Jonathan repaired some person’s bike.
That is, what a pronoun refers to is not always determined by another noun
phrase in its sentence. Rather, what a pronoun refers to may be determined by
the situational context in which the language is used, as is typically the case in
spoken interaction.
We’d like you to notice now that pronouns are typically shorter and
communicate far less information than their antecedents. For example,
their in (2) provides only the information that more than one entity is
being referred to (along with the grammatical information that their is in
the genitive case), clearly far less information than its antecedent, All of the
members of the class, provides. Using pronouns instead of full noun phrases
avoids repetition and reduces the production demands on the speaker or
writer and processing demands on hearers or readers. However, because pronouns provide so little information, hearers/readers
expect that speakers/writers will use them only when it is easy to determine
what they refer to. If a pronoun’s referent is not easily or unambiguously determined, hearers/readers may quickly give up trying to interpret the piece
of discourse in which it occurs. In face-to-face communication, the hearer
can simply ask the speaker to clarify an unclear reference. But in written
communication, this is typically not possible. Because confusion in spoken
language can be fairly readily clarified, it tends to have more pronouns than
written language. Beginning writers (and sometimes even more advanced
ones) often use pronoun patterns typical of spoken language and so must
be taught to ensure that the antecedents/referents of their pronouns will be
clear to a reader who cannot ask for clarification.
In English, pronouns and their antecedents must have the same person,
number, and gender; that is, pronouns must agree with their antecedents on
these grammatical categories. All of the sentences in (1) illustrate agreement.
Jonathan and Jeremy are each third person, singular, and masculine, and thus
require the pronouns he, his, or him.
Exercise
Evaluate the traditional definition of “pronoun.”
196
The Minor Parts of Speech
The traditional definition of “pronoun” applies most readily to some occurrences of the third person pronouns he, she, it, and they. While I and you
might be taken as substitutes for noun phrases like the speaker and the addressee, respectively, this seems unnatural. Would a person who says (4a) of
himself say (4b) of himself under the same circumstances? How about (4c)
and (4d)?
(4)
a. I feel so broke up, I wanna go home. (The Beach Boys)
b. The speaker feels so broke up, he wants to go home. (Not The
Beach Boys)
c. I love you.
d. The speaker loves the hearer.
We think not. We think that definitions of pronouns that regard them as
replacements for other expressions are fundamentally flawed. We prefer a
more general definition:
A pronoun is a minimal linguistic form which refers to speaker(s), addressee(s),
and other(s), and provides such grammatical information as person, number,
gender, case, and humanness.
These minimal forms reduce the effort required in speaking and writing by
eliminating the need to repeat longer expressions when we wish to refer to
someone or something more than once. For second and subsequent references we select just enough information to allow our audience to keep track
of what we’re speaking or writing about. If we choose to use pronouns,
because the information they communicate is so minimal, we must use
them only when the information they communicate is sufficient to allow an
audience to easily figure out what they refer to, either from the co-text or
from the situational context. The grammatical agreement expected between
a pronoun and its antecedent is crucial for making the pronoun’s reference
clear.
English contains several different types of pronouns. We will list each
type below according to the grammatical categories they represent.
Personal pronouns
Table 1 identifies the categories of personal pronouns:
197
Delahunty and Garvey
person
case
First
Nominative
Objective Genitive
singular
I
me
my
mine
plural
we
us
our
ours
Second
Nominative
Objective
Genitive
you
you
your
yours
you
you
your
yours
Third Nominative
Objective
Genitive
gender
singular
Masculine Feminine Neuter
he
she
it
him
her
it
his
her
its
his
hers
its
plural
they
them
their
theirs
table 1: person, number, gender, a n d c a s e o f pe r s o n a l
pronouns
As Table 1 indicates, the personal pronouns represent the grammatical
categories of person, number, case, and gender. Number (in modern English) simply distinguishes singular (one) from plural (more than one). The
pronoun forms I, you, he/she/it represent distinctions within the person category. Person differentiates speakers and those associated with them (first
person: I, we) from addressees (second person: you), and from entities that
are neither speaker nor addressee (third person: she, he, it, they).
The many different forms of the modern English personal pronoun system hint at the morphological complexity of the language a millennium
ago. For instance, Old English had pronouns that referred specifically to
two people (called “dual” pronouns), thereby creating a three-way number
distinction. (We still have the word both to refer to two entities and either/
neither to refer to a choice between two entities.)
Standard English is unusual among languages in that it makes no distinction in the personal pronouns between second person singular and plural—
you does for both. Many non‑standard dialects of English do differentiate
singular and plural, e.g., by adding either the ordinary nominal plural ending {‑s} (youse) or by adding {all} (you-all or y’all).
198
The Minor Parts of Speech
Languages with different forms for second person singular and plural include German du (informal sg.) and ihr (informal pl.); Spanish tu (informal
sg.) and vosotros (informal pl.); French tu (sg.) and vous (pl.). In French, this
distinction does double duty. It can indicate not only the person and number distinction, but also certain aspects of the relationship between a speaker
and addressee(s), most notably their relative social statuses and the degree of
intimacy between them. Thus, while the French singular pronoun tu may be
used by an adult to a child, the child would normally use vous to the adult. Tu
can be used between people who are relatively friendly or familiar with each
other; vous would be used among people who are not on friendly or familiar terms, or in formal situations. The other languages have other pronouns
which indicate analogous social distinctions. German uses Sie as a polite or
formal second person pronoun, pronounced the same as sie, the third person
plural pronoun. Some varieties of Spanish use usted as a polite second person
singular form, and ustedes as a polite second person plural form. In earlier periods of English, thou and its forms thee, thy, and thine were used informally
while you and its forms were for formal use. The dimensions of status and
familiarity have been extensively discussed by linguists and anthropologists
under the terms power and solidarity, respectively, which we return to in
our chapter on Language Variation in Book II.
The person distinction is required also to account for certain verb forms,
which are most obvious in the present tense singular forms of the verb be:
first person am; second person are; third person is. Regular verbs in the present tense distinguish third person singular from all other persons by marking it with the ending {‑s}: He/she/it gives; I/you/we/they give. Modal verbs do
not indicate person at all.
Case of personal pronouns
English masculine and feminine pronouns come in three different forms: he,
him, his; she, her, hers. These different forms are said to represent different
cases of the pronouns. Which case of a pronoun to use depends upon the
relation of that word to other parts of the sentence: we use he and she when
the pronoun is the subject of a sentence; him and her if it is the object of a
verb or a preposition; and his and her if the pronoun modifies a noun. We
will use the traditional names to refer to these cases: he/she are in the nominative case; him/her are in the objective (a.k.a. accusative) case; and his/her
are in the genitive.
English also differentiates other pronouns according to case. Thus I, you,
we, they are all nominative; me, you, us, them are all objective; and my, mine,
your, yours, our, ours, their, theirs are all genitive.
199
Delahunty and Garvey
You will no doubt have noticed that there are two genitive forms of certain pronouns, such as my and mine. The forms corresponding to my (your,
our, their) are used when they directly modify a noun. Otherwise we use the
other genitive forms: That is my horse as opposed to That horse is mine. The
former are sometimes misleadingly referred to as possessive adjectives, as
they occur before the nouns they modify in the positions believed (wrongly)
to be typical of attributive adjectives. The latter are often distinguished as
possessive pronouns because they appear to replace possessive nouns or
noun phrases, e.g., compare That bike is hers with That bike is Kelly’s.
English nouns functioning as heads of subjects do not differ in form from
nouns functioning as heads of objects, so we do not distinguish between
nominative and objective cases in those instances. Grammarians occasionally refer to the nominative/objective form of nouns as the common case.
Earlier forms of English, the classical languages (Latin and Greek), and
modern languages such as Finnish have much more elaborate case distinctions than modern English. Table 2 provides a list of some traditional case
names and their functions.
name
Nominative
Objective
Genitive
Dative
Ablative
Vocative
grammatical function
subject
object
modifier, complement
recipient, beneficiary
place from where
addressee
table 2: traditional case names and functions
Many languages require case markings on parts of speech besides nouns
and pronouns. Modern German, for instance, makes case differentiations
on both articles and adjectives.
Gender of personal pronouns
The pronoun system of English distinguishes three genders: masculine
(forms of he), feminine (forms of she), and neuter (not neutral!) (forms of
it), distinguished primarily according to the nature of the objects they refer
to. Masculine pronouns refer to males, primarily human males; feminine
pronouns refer to females, primarily human females; and neuter pronouns
refer either to non-human animals or to entities that are non-animate and
consequently are not differentiated according to sex. Infants whose sex is
200
The Minor Parts of Speech
unknown are also occasionally referred to by neuter pronouns. A system in
which the gender of a word depends upon characteristics of its referent is
called a natural gender system.
Other languages, such as French, German, Italian, Spanish, and Gaelic,
have grammatical gender systems. The choice of gender is not dependent
upon characteristics of a word’s referent; rather, words may be assigned to
gender classes, often according to formal linguistic criteria. In Italian and
Spanish, for example, words ending in {-a} are typically feminine; in German, words ending in {-chen} are typically neuter. However, many nouns
in these languages are assigned to gender classes somewhat arbitrarily and
so when learning a noun one must also learn its gender. Also in these languages, the gender system is reflected not only in the pronouns and nouns,
but in adjectives and articles, too. In Spanish, a noun and any article or
adjective modifying it must agree in gender; if the noun is masculine, then
any associated article or adjective must be masculine (e.g., el libro blanco,
lit. the book white, “the white book”). If the noun is feminine, its modifiers must also be feminine (e.g., la casa blanca, lit. the house white, “the
white house”).
In recent years the English gender system has given rise to much discussion of the issue of sexism in language and the need to develop forms that
are sex‑neutral. Standard written English makes it difficult not to refer to
the sex of a human referent when choosing a personal pronoun, regardless
of whether the person’s sex is relevant or even known or knowable. For example, compare the sentences Every doctor works hard for her patients and
Every doctor works hard for his patients. The first suggests that all doctors
are women; the second that they are all men. Clearly neither need be true.
Traditional prescriptive grammars have required that the pronoun after
quantifiers such as every and some be masculine—and in general that the
generic pronoun be the masculine one. Many people find this norm to be
objectionable and would like to find expressions that would not give any
indication of the referent’s sex for use in situations where sex is irrelevant.
Many writers now use forms of they when a generic pronoun is required.
The following is from a Cambridge University Press publication: . . . while
someone is taking their turn in a conversation, . . . (Meyer 2002: 76).
And growing numbers of organizations require that their publications be
sex‑neutral. We return to this topic in our chapter on Language Variation
in Book II.
201
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. In the passage below (a) identify all the personal pronouns; (b) specify the antecedent of each pronoun; and (c) indicate the case, number,
and gender of each pronoun.
Hercules was the strongest man on earth and he had the supreme self-confidence magnificent physical strength gives. He considered himself on an
equality with the gods—and with some reason. They needed his help to
conquer the Giants. In the final victory of the Olympians over the brutish
sons of Earth, Hercules’ arrows played an important part. He treated the
gods accordingly. Once when the priestess at Delphi gave no response to the
question he asked, he seized the tripod she sat on and declared that he would
carry it off and have an oracle of his own. Apollo, of course, would not put
up with this, but Hercules was perfectly willing to fight him and Zeus had to
intervene. The quarrel was easily settled, however. Hercules was quite goodnatured about it. He did not want to quarrel with Apollo, he only wanted an
answer from his oracle. If Apollo would give it the matter was settled as far
as he was concerned. Apollo on his side, facing this undaunted person, felt
an admiration for his boldness and made his priestess deliver the response.
(From Edith Hamilton, Mythology)
2. We hope that you noticed in the passage just above that (a) all of the
pronouns in the passage are in the third person and (b) they all have
an easily determined antecedent in the passage. Consider now first and
second person pronouns. Is it possible for them to have a verbal antecedent, or do they always refer to some entity outside the text in which
they occur? (A word with this latter property is said to be “deictic,”
which we discuss further below.) Try to think of examples to support
your position. Does your analysis affect our definition of pronouns?
3. Briefly discuss the use of pronouns in the Hercules text in light of our
revised definition of pronoun and our discussion of pronoun use.
4. For each of the following pronouns give all the grammatical categories needed to fully characterize it: they, me, your, him, our, mine. For
example: hers—3rd person, singular, feminine, genitive case.
Demonstrative pronouns
English contains only four demonstrative pronouns; they appear in Table 3.
202
The Minor Parts of Speech
singular
plural
nearer
this
these
farther that
those
table 3: demonstrative pronouns
Demonstrative pronouns have the effect of “pointing out” entities, often for
the purpose of contrast or selection.
(5) a. Press this button, not that one.
b. I’ll take one of these and one of those.
As the examples suggest, speakers may accompany demonstratives with pointing gestures. These forms are sometimes called deictics, after a Greek word
meaning “to point.” In written prose, of course, gestures are not available, so
writers must take care to make the referents of the pronouns clear:
(6) Harry told Mabel that Maude had written the letter. This is typical.
What is typical? Harry’s telling Mabel? Harry’s telling anyone? Maude’s
writing letters?
Like the genitive personal pronouns, demonstratives may function as
heads or as modifiers, but with no change in form.
(7) a. That is a serious mistake. (Head)
b. That mistake is serious. (Modifier)
Reflexive and intensive pronouns
Reflexive and intensive pronouns have the same forms; they begin with a
personal pronoun, generally in the genitive case, and end in the morphemes
{-self} or {-selves}, for singular and plural, respectively. The forms are listed in
Table 4.
person
singular
First
myself
Second
yourself
Third
himself
herself
itself
table 4: reflexive/intensive pronouns
plural
ourselves
yourselves
themselves
203
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. Identify the forms in Table 4 that include a genitive personal pronoun.
2. Identify the case of the non-genitive personal pronouns in the forms
in Table 4.
3. Many non-standard English dialects use the forms hisself and theirselves. Why do you think they do that?
We say that these forms are both reflexive and intensive because they are
used in two quite distinct ways. We say they are reflexive when they are used
as the object of a verb or preposition (8a-c); we say they are intensive when
they are used as modifiers (9, 10).
(8) a. Adelaide hurt herself.
b. Adelaide bought herself a new Lamborghini.
c. Rudy talks to himself a lot.
The pronouns in (8a-c) are reflexive: in (8a) herself is the direct object of
hurt; in (8b) it is the indirect object of buy; in (8c) himself is the object of
the preposition to. The pronouns and their antecedents are in different noun
phrases.
An intensive pronoun may occur within the noun phrase of its antecedent, typically following and modifying its antecedent directly:
(9) Adelaide herself completed the audit.
However, an intensive pronoun may also be moved away from the phrase
it modifies:
(10) Adelaide completed the audit herself.
Sentences with reflexives cannot be related in the way that (9) and (10) are.
In other words, sentence (8a) cannot be rearranged as (11) without significantly changing its meaning:
(11) ?Adelaide herself hurt.
204
The Minor Parts of Speech
Indefinite pronouns
Indefinite pronouns constitute a loose category of words brought together
traditionally by the semantic fact that they do not refer to a specific person,
place, thing, or idea. The common indefinites are listed in Table 5.
all (1)
anybody (2)
each (1)
everyone (2)
most (1)
no one (2)
one (1)
several (1)
someone (2) another (1)
anyone (2)
either (1)
few (1)
neither (1)
none (2) other (1)
some (1)
such (1)
any (1)
both (1)
everybody (2)
many (1)
nobody (2)
nothing (2)
others (2)
somebody (2)
table 5: indefinite pronouns (1 = may be head or modifier; 2 = may be head only)
Occasionally, students will misapply the semantic definition and label as
indefinites generic nouns such as people, collective nouns such as group or
crowd, and abstract nouns such as concern or beauty. Formally, indefinite pronouns have little if anything in common. They are a “leftover” class to which
pronouns that fit in no other category are relegated. The general semantic notion that unifies a majority of indefinites is that of “quantity,” e.g., all, many,
no, etc. For this reason, members of Table 5 are sometimes assigned to a separate class called quantifiers.
Indefinites have a limited range of functions, acting only as heads or modifiers. The functions of individual words are indicated in Table 5.
Exercise
Select any five indefinites labeled as (1) in Table 5. For each, give an
example sentence in which the pronoun is used (a) as a head and (b)
as a modifier.
Wh-words
Wh-words, such as who and what, occur in a wide range of constructions. In traditional grammars, they are called interrogative or relative
pronouns. These forms are usually distinguished by the constructions in
205
Delahunty and Garvey
which they function, but there is little formal reason to separate them. We
thus list them as a single group in Table 6. We will briefly explain their
range of functions here and go into more detail in other chapters.
Some of the words in Table 6 are traditionally called pronouns. Because of
our emphasis on form, we will not use this label. Some of the members of the
group function as pronouns in certain constructions but not in others. To call
them pronouns on this limited basis confuses form with function.
who
whom
what
whose
where
why
how
table 6: wh-words
which
when
whether
The label wh-word is a mnemonic that clearly applies to all members of the
class except how. Nevertheless, this form deserves inclusion on the basis of its
grammatical behavior.
Wh-pronouns perform three distinct functions:
a. Introducing information questions
b. Introducing relative clauses
c. Introducing noun clauses
We will illustrate these functions with who, which, and where. We will also
indicate cases in which these words can occur as headwords and as modifiers.
An information question requests that the hearer respond with some information beyond a mere “yes” or “no.” These questions appear with all whwords (except whether). It is in this role that wh-words are traditionally called
interrogative pronouns.
(12) a.
b.
c.
d.
Who invented the telescope? (Head)
Which do you want? (Head)
Which donut do you want? (Modifier)
Where did she find that hat? (Head)
Exercise
For each of the wh-words in Table 6 (except whether), create three
wh-questions.
206
The Minor Parts of Speech
Wh-words also introduce relative clauses (in square brackets in (13)).
These clauses modify nouns. In our chapter on Phrases we will see how they
follow a head noun as part of a noun phrase. Most wh-words can introduce
relatives.
(13) a.
b.
c.
d.
Anyone [who wants a ticket] should call Herman.
The book [which you requested] is out of print.
The locale [where the movie is set] is fictional.
The person [who called you] left no message.
Exercise
1. For each of the wh-words in Table 6 (except what and whether), create three sentences with relative clauses.
2. Create a sentence with a relative clause beginning with what. Have
you ever come across such a relative clause before? How would you
change it to make it acceptable in formal English?
Finally, wh-words serve to introduce noun clauses (in square brackets in
(14)), which are entire clauses that function as if they were noun phrases. (For
this reason, the entire clause can often be replaced by a simpler noun phrase
or by a pronoun.)
(14) a. I don’t know [who can get you a leash that big].
b. Tell me [which tranquilizer is the strongest].
c. Kong didn’t say [where he dropped those banana peels].
Exercise
For each of the wh-words in Table 6 (except whether), create three
sentences containing noun clauses.
Among the wh-words we can detect a second gender system at work in
English: who refers to humans; what to non-humans; and which to both humans and non-humans.
207
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. Which wh-word cannot introduce relative clauses in standard English?
2. Create three sentences with whether. What grammatical function(s)
does it play in your sentences?
articles
Articles are the last minor class associated with nouns. They always function as
modifiers of the head noun in a noun phrase, and traditionally are assumed to
contain only two words: the indefinite article a(n) and the definite article the.
(15) a. a visitor
b. the United Nations
The indefinite article has two written forms, a before a spoken consonant
and an before a spoken vowel; the corresponding spoken forms are /ey/,
/{/, and /{n/, at least when spoken in isolation. The definite article may
be pronounced /D@/ before a spoken consonant and /Di/ before a spoken
vowel, though there is no indication of this in the standard spelling. A may
occur only with a singular noun, whereas the may occur with a singular or
a plural one.
Generally, a(n) and the are the only articles recognized for English. However, some linguists would claim that there is a plural of a, namely, some
(pronounced with a very reduced vowel): You have a visitor vs. You have some
visitors. And a case can be made for a zero article: The visitors must sign in
vs. Visitors must sign in and A/the moose blocked the path vs. Moose is good
to eat.
While the two traditional articles are easily recognized, their meaning
is quite complex. They are part of a system of devices that enable communicators to introduce people and things into a discourse and then keep
track of them as the discourse continues. Other devices in this system are
pronouns and demonstratives, and the distinction between common and
proper nouns.
Somewhat simplistically, the system works as follows: indefinite articles
signal the entry of a new entity into the discourse, e.g., Once upon a time
there was a big bad wolf. Once an entity has been introduced, it can be
referred to by an appropriate personal pronoun, e.g., He lived all alone.
Or it can be assigned a proper name, e.g., He was called Edgar, which can
208
The Minor Parts of Speech
then be used to refer to it, e.g., One day, Edgar was out looking for his next
meal. Later references may be marked by a definite article, e.g., When the
wolf came to the edge of the forest, he spotted some children. When we want
to distinguish one entity from another of the same kind, we can use the
demonstratives, e.g., Edgar said to himself, “This child looks far tastier than
that one.” (Using the plural forms of the demonstratives, we can, of course,
distinguish multiple entities, e.g., “But those children look tastiest of all!”)
The definite article, the demonstratives, the personal pronouns, and
proper nouns all signal definiteness. One major purpose of marking a noun
phrase as definite is to indicate the speaker’s/writer’s assumption that the
intended hearer/reader can readily identify what it refers to. The general
pattern is that we provide our audiences with only as much information
as we think they will need to identify what an NP refers to. (We thereby
minimize repetition and the amount of effort we must expend in producing
our utterances and the amount of effort we require the audience to expend
in interpreting them.) And we can rank the definite expressions according
to the amount of information they provide: NPs with demonstratives provide more information than NPs with definite articles, which provide more
information than proper names, which provide more information than pronouns.
Another reason for making a noun phrase definite is to indicate that all
the entities relevant in a situation are being referred to. Imagine a situation
in which there are books strewn on a desk. If we say, The books should be put
back on the shelves, we will be understood to mean all the books, not just some
of them. If we want just some of the books reshelved, then we have to find a
characteristic common to the ones we want reshelved and mention that in our
sentence, e.g., The linguistics books should be reshelved. In this case, we will be
understood to mean all the linguistics books.
The indefinite article, a(n), indicates that the speaker/writer assumes that
the hearer/reader can not readily identify the referent of the NP. Contrast
(16a) and (16b):
(16) a.
b.
I saw the wolf. (Speaker presents information as readily
accessible to the hearer; e.g., speaker and hearer have already
identified a wolf and are now referring to it again)
I saw a wolf. (Speaker presents information as not readily accessible to the hearer; e.g., speaker is introducing reference to a wolf
into the conversation)
It is for this reason that the indefinite article is normally used to introduce a
209
Delahunty and Garvey
new entity or topic into a discourse.
Another important meaning associated with the indefinite article is categorization or classification: an indefinite NP denotes a member of the category named by the head noun of the NP. Dictionary definitions make use of
such categorization. For example:
(17) catbird n. A North American songbird (Dumetella carolinensis)
having predominantly slate plumage (American Heritage Dictionary 4th ed. p. 292)
Here the AHD defines catbird by categorizing it as a (kind of ) North American songbird. Check several other dictionary entries to see if they also use the
indefinite article in this way.
Whether a noun is count or non-count affects which articles may modify
it. Count nouns may be modified by both articles—by a if the noun is singular (a calculator), and by the if the noun is either singular or plural (the
calculator/s). Non-count nouns may occur only with the (the information); indefinite non-count nouns occur with no overt article (information) or with
some (some information).
Definite and indefinite NPs may refer either to one or more members of a
class of entities or to the entire class. For example, The pig in The pig is a filthy
animal may be interpreted as referring to a single, specific pig or to the entire
pig species. When we refer to an entire class, we are said to be making generic
reference. When we refer to one or more specific entities, we are making specific reference. We can also make generic reference using indefinite NPs: A pig
is a filthy animal; Pigs are filthy animals.
Exercise
1. Here are some rules that are typically given for when to use the definite article. For each rule, create three short examples to illustrate it.
a. Use the to mark a noun phrase whose referent is identifiable in
the situational context, e.g., Where’s the dean’s office?
b. Use the to mark a noun phrase whose referent has already been
introduced, e.g., A rabbi and a priest went into a bar. The rabbi
said to the priest . . .
c. Use the to mark a noun phrase whose referent is unique, e.g., the
earth, the tallest mountain, the fifth man.
d. Use the with adjectives and nouns that name groups of people,
especially when we are referring to the entire group, e.g., the
210
The Minor Parts of Speech
poor, the Russians.
e. Use the with some geographical proper nouns, e.g., the Pacific,
the Philippines, but cf. Spain not *the Spain.
f. Use the in certain date formats, e.g., the 4th of July.
2. For each of the following categories of words, determine whether
they do or do not typically occur with the:
a. Names of years, e.g., 1984.
b. Names of professions, e.g., accounting; the law.
c. Names of languages, e.g., English.
d. Names of meals, e.g., brunch.
e. Names of individuals, e.g., Albert.
f. Titles and names together, e.g., President Kennedy.
3. Here are some rules that are typically given for when to use an
indefinite article. (Remember to use a as the singular indefinite and
either some or no article at all for the plural, e.g., a book, some books,
books.) For each rule, create a short example to illustrate it.
a. Use an indefinite article for the first mention of an entity or entities in a discourse, e.g., For a soccer fan, few things are as exciting as the World Cup. The surgeon introduced some radioactive
dye into the patient’s arteries. Brain injuries are frequent in
combat [NB no article modifies Brain injuries].
b. Use an indefinite article or no article to classify/categorize, e.g.,
She is a doctor. They are doctors.
c. Use an indefinite article or no article with certain numbers, e.g.,
a hundred, thousands.
4. English articles pose considerable difficulties from ESL and EFL learners, at least partly because of the complexity of the English system,
but also because languages differ in whether they have articles at all
(Latin had none), how many articles they have (Irish has only a definite
article), and what information the articles communicate (Spanish and
French articles include information about the number and gender of
the nouns they modify; German articles are marked for gender, case,
and number). The following are texts by learners of English. Examine
each noun phrase in them and discuss any that seem not to be idiomatic
English because of the writer’s choice of definite or indefinite expression.
a. With the high technology people have taken more information
211
Delahunty and Garvey
about world.
b. The average citizen in my country had access to a fast communication.
c. Many people didn’t have computer and television and didn’t use
internet.
d. Starcraft is very famous game.
e. My professor teach us throughout the computer.
f. People listen the music everywhere.
g. Because of improvement in technology people come to know how
then can compete.
h. . . . industry that have possibility of pollution . . .
i. In past years, we had to use public phone to call in the street.
j. We had to wait for long time to call.
k. Using mobile phone is become very important to our lives.
l. People can use internet to shop.
m. Government give the money to old people.
n. Technology has unbelievably improved the lifestyle of average
citizen.
5. The following excerpt is from Jane Austen’s Sense and Sensibility
(1811/1961: 1-2). We have highlighted several NPs. Read through the
passage and then for each highlighted NP, determine whether it is definite or indefinite. If it is definite, specify the grammatical device that
makes it so (e.g., definite article, pronoun, proper name, etc.). Then,
using the discussion of articles above, say why each NP is definite or
indefinite, and if definite, say why it has the form it has.
The family of Dashwood had been long settled in Sussex. Their estate was large, and their residence was at Norland Park, in the centre
of their property, where for many generations they had lived in so
respectable a manner as to engage the general good opinion of their
surrounding acquaintances. The late owner of this estate was a single
man, who lived to a very advanced age, and who for many years of
his life had a constant companion and housekeeper in his sister. But
her death, which happened ten years before his own, produced a great
alteration in his home; for to supply her loss, he invited and received
into his house the family of his nephew, Mr. Henry Dashwood, the
legal inheritor of the Norland estate, and the person to whom he
intended to bequeath it. In the society of his nephew and neice, and
their children, the old gentleman’s days were comfortably spent. His
212
The Minor Parts of Speech
attachment to them all increased. The constant attention of Mr. and
Mrs. Henry Dashwood to his wishes, which proceeded not merely
from interest, but from goodness of heart, gave him every degree of
solid comfort which his age could receive; and the cheerfulness of the
children added a relish to his existence.
By a former marriage, Mr. Henry Dashwood had one son; by
his present lady, three daughters. The son, a steady, respectable young
man, was amply provided for by the fortune of his mother, which had
been large, and half of which devolved on him on his coming of age.
By his own marriage, likewise, which happened soon afterwards, he
added to his wealth. To him, therefore, the succession to the Norland
estate was not so really important as to his sisters; for their fortune,
independent on what might arise to them from their father’s inheriting that property, could be but small.
a u x i l i a r y ve r b s
In this section we discuss auxiliary verbs, which we mentioned in passing
in our chapter on Major Parts of Speech. Auxiliary verbs always occur with
a main verb, though the main verb may be “understood,” that is, omitted
and implied, e.g., Did John leave? He did [leave]. There are only a few auxiliaries in the language, but each plays several important grammatical and
semantic roles. The English auxiliaries are: be, have, and the modal verbs,
can, could; may, might; shall, should; will, would; must; and do, which has
no meaning but patterns grammatically like a modal. Be is used with a
verb in its Ving form to indicate the progressive aspect, e.g., The students
are working on their term papers. Be is also used with a past participle verb
form, Ven, to create the passive voice, e.g., This book was written by two
loony linguists. Have is used with a Ven form to create the perfect aspect,
e.g., The semester has come to an end. Do and the modals are followed by
a verb in its uninflected form, the form used to cite it in a dictionary, e.g.,
We shall overcome. We discuss the progressive and perfect aspects in our
chapter on Basic Clause Patterns.
Auxiliary verbs are optional elements in a clause, but up to four may occur together. Regardless of how many occur, they always follow this order:
modal, have, progressive be, and passive be, as you can demonstrate for yourself by rearranging them in, She may have been being spied on by Homeland
Security agents. You should find that all other orders are ungrammatical.
The first auxiliary in a sentence is extremely important. First, it is the one
213
Delahunty and Garvey
that is marked for tense, if the clause is finite. In She should have been working on her term paper, should is the first auxiliary and its {-d} indicates that it
is in the past tense. You can convince yourself of the accuracy of this rule by
removing the past tense marker from should and placing it on any of the other
auxiliaries and main verb, e.g., *She shall had been working on her term paper.
You should find that all other orders are ungrammatical. We discuss tense in
our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns.
Second, in interrogative clauses, it is always the first auxiliary that is
moved to the left of the subject. If we turn She could have been seriously injured! into a question, it is the could (as the first auxiliary) that moves: Could
she have been seriously injured? Again, you can test the accuracy of our rule
by moving other auxiliaries to the left of the subject, e.g., *Have she could
been seriously injured?
Third, in negated sentences, the negative particle not is typically placed
after the first auxiliary, e.g., He has not been studying very hard lately. Try
placing it elsewhere in the sequence to see what happens.
Modal verbs
If a modal verb occurs in a clause, it will, as we noted, be the first of any
auxiliaries that clause contains. Besides occupying the same sentential position, modals express related concepts. These concepts include notions such as
(a) necessity, either logical or social (obligation): You must read the book; (b)
possibility, logical or social (permission): He may leave the room; (c) ability:
He can do long division in his head; or (d) intention, either definite or conditional: I will/would/shall/should write another 10 pages today. What these modal
concepts all have in common is that they indicate the basis for the speaker’s
judgment or belief about the truth of the sentence.
We can view the {‑d/t} at the end of the second member of each pair of
modals as a variant of the past tense inflection {‑ed}. This allows us to regard
each pair (excluding must) as comprising a present and a past tense form. To
see why this is so, consider the phenomenon called backshifting. Backshifting
involves the change from present tense to past tense forms when direct speech,
e.g., John is flying to Toronto tomorrow, is converted into indirect speech when
the verb of the main clause is in the past tense: John said that he was flying to
Toronto tomorrow. Sentences involving modals require a shift from the basic
form to the past tense, {-d/t}, form: John will fly out tomorrow becomes John
said that he would fly out tomorrow.
214
The Minor Parts of Speech
Exercise
The following text is from the mystery novel Farriers’ Lane, by Anne
Perry (1993: 285). It occurs after the discovery of a policeman’s (Paterson) body hanging in his bedroom. (a) Identify all the modal verbs
in the passage. (b) Discuss each one using the framework for understanding modals presented just above. Remember that each modal
may serve more than one purpose. (c) You should also note that the
modals occur in the later part of each paragraph. Why do you think
the author shifted to the use of modals as she did? (d) The novel contains many passages with lots of modal verbs in them. Why do you
think that modals might suit a mystery writer’s purposes?
He touched Paterson’s hand. The body swung very slightly. The
flesh was cold, the arm rigid. He had been dead several hours. He was
dressed in plain dark uniform trousers and tunic, which was torn, his
sergeant’s insignia ripped off. He still wore his boots. It was nearly
midday now. Presumably it was what he had worn when he came
home from the last duty of the day before. If he had slept here, risen
in the morning and dressed ready to go out, the body would still have
some warmth left, and be limp. He must have died sometime late
yesterday evening, or during the night. It would almost certainly be
the evening. Why should he be wearing his street clothes all night?
The hook was in the middle of the ceiling, about ten or eleven
feet high, where one would expect to find a chandelier. There was no
furniture near enough to it for him to have climbed on. It had taken a
strong man to lift Paterson up and then let him fall from that height.
He must have used the rope as a pulley over the hook. There was no
conceivable way Paterson could have done it himself, even supposing
he had some cause to, or believed he had.
Negation
Negating a clause is primarily done by inserting the negative particle not. If
the clause has one or more auxiliary verbs, not is typically placed after the
first auxiliary, though it can occur elsewhere: He must not have arrived yet.
If the clause does not have an auxiliary, then the “dummy” auxiliary do is
inserted into the first auxiliary position and not is placed after it: He did not
arrive on time. Cf. *He not arrived on time.
215
Delahunty and Garvey
Not may be contracted (i.e., reduced) and attached to the auxiliary immediately before it: He didn’t arrive on time; He mustn’t have arrived yet.
Mood, modality, aspect, tense, voice, and negation may be combined:
Couldn’t she have been being followed by the FBI?
p re p o s i t i o n s
Prepositions (P) combine with noun phrases to form prepositional phrases
(PPs). They are important to English because PPs play a wide range of grammatical roles. In other languages—and in earlier stages of English—prepositions play a less significant role because some of their jobs are carried out
by inflectional affixes. Prepositions also express many of the major semantic
relations that integrate parts of a sentence into a grammatical and meaningful
whole. It is thus important for teachers and students to become familiar with,
not learn by heart, the approximately 50 members of this class.
about
along
at
beside(s)
concerning
in
on
since
toward up
above
amid(st)
before
between
down
inside
onto
through
under
upon
across
among
behind
beyond
during
into
out
throughout
underneath
with
after
around
below
but (= except)
except
like
outside
till
until
within
against
astride
beneath
by
from
of
over
to
unto
without
table 7: single-word prepositions
In spite of the significance of prepositions, standard grammars often assign them rather vague definitions, such as “a word that shows the relation of
a noun or pronoun to some other word in a sentence” or, misleading ones,
such as “a word followed by a noun or a pronoun.” English prepositions are
uninflected words that take NP objects to form prepositional phrases. In functional terms, a preposition in a PP functions as the head of that prepositional
phrase. The preposition signals the grammatical and/or semantic role played
by the PP in its clause.
PPs play a broad range of roles in English phrases and sentences, including
modification of nouns, e.g., in The trunk of the car, the PP of the car consists
of the preposition of and its object the Shrew and modifies the noun trunk.
PPs complement verbs and adjectives, e.g., in give it to her, the PP to her is a
216
The Minor Parts of Speech
complement of the verb give, and in conscious of her surroundings, the PP of
her surroundings complements the adjective conscious.
The following are some examples of PPs; the preposition is italicized:
(18) a. of my toe
b. to Tangiers
c. beneath contempt
Exercise
1. Select five prepositions from Table 7 and create a prepositional
phrase around each using the phrases in (18) as models.
2. In the paragraph just beneath Table 7, (a) identify all of the prepositions, referring to Table 7 as necessary, and (b) paraphrase the meaning
of each preposition.
Though prepositions are generally followed immediately by noun phrases,
in wh-clauses a NP may be moved away from its preposition:
(19) a. What did you call about? (cf. You called about what?)
b. She asked what you called about.
c. The lamp which you called about has been sold.
In these cases, the NP objects of the prepositions (what and which) have
moved elsewhere in the sentence, and in fact precede their prepositions. In a
sense the preposition has been abandoned by its object NP; for this reason, we
call such cases preposition stranding. The stranding of prepositions is sometimes criticized in prescriptive circles, but in many cases unstranded prepositions sound either stilted or downright ungrammatical:
(20) a. ?About what did you call?
b. *She asked about what you called.
c. The lamp about which you called has been sold.
Aside from their behavior in such cases, though, prepositions are formally very
simple.
Before examining the semantics of prepositions, we should mention one
further formal complexity—the tendency of prepositions to enter into complex frozen expressions that resemble idioms; Table 8 lists some examples.
217
Delahunty and Garvey
according to
apart from
by means of
by reason of
by way of
in accord(ance) with
in case of
in opposition to
along with
as for
with regard to
by virtue of
except for
in addition to
in compliance with
in place of
with respect to
round about
with reference to
on account of
out of
in spite of
instead of
in regard to
table 8: multi-word prepositions
The structure of these multi-word prepositions falls into two patterns: (a)
preposition + noun + preposition (P + N + P; e.g., by means of, in case of) and
(b) miscellaneous word + preposition (X + P; e.g., according to, because of). It
would, of course, be possible to view such expressions—particularly the P +
N + P type—as simply combinations of two prepositional phrases; however,
most speakers perceive them as grammatical units. We will not take a hardand-fast position on this issue, but simply note, as do most grammars, the
presence of such constructions.
Exercise
Select five of the multi-word prepositions from Table 8 and create a PP
around each one, e.g., in spite of his insistence.
Prepositions cover a wide range of meanings. Traditional categories, along
with some typical examples, are given in Table 9.
Place (Locative):
Direction:
Time (Temporal):
Manner:
Accompaniment:
Instrument (Means):
Recipient:
Beneficiary:
Miscellaneous:
table 9: semantic types of
218
above, around, at, behind, beneath, between
up, down
about, after, at, during, for, since
with (exit with a flourish)
with (went with Flora)
by, with (open it with a knife)
to (gave it to Lucy)
for (did it for Lucy)
of, about, like, without
prepositions
The Minor Parts of Speech
Exercise
Create three PPs to represent each of the semantic categories in Table 9.
Many individual prepositions have several meanings. For example, what
meanings of around and beneath occur in sentences (21) and (22)?
(21) a.
b.
(22) a.
b.
They walked around the statue.
I’ll return around 5:00.
Horace stood beneath the Balancing Rock.
Horace is beneath contempt.
This variety of meanings sometimes creates confusion for students, particularly those who simplistically associate certain structures with prepositions. For instance, the indirect object construction (e.g., He gave Hilda
the bike; He bought Hilda a bike) is sometimes associated with paraphrases
using the prepositions to (He gave the bike to Hilda) and for (He bought a
bike for Hilda). However, this semantic relation emerges only when to has
a Recipient meaning and for has a Beneficiary meaning. For example, the
indirect object sentences in (23a, b) can be rephrased as the sentences with
to and for in (23c, d). In contrast, when we try to rephrase the to and for
in sentences (24a, b) as indirect object sentences, the results, (24c, d), are
ungrammatical. This is because to and for in (24) do not have Recipient
and Beneficiary interpretations, respectively.
(23) a. I offered Hickle a dozen widgets.
b. I made Hickle an artificial earlobe.
c. I offered a dozen widgets to Hickle.
d. I made an artificial earlobe for Hickle.
(24) a. I sent Hickle to the lake.
b. I made an artificial earlobe for $3,000.
c. *I sent the lake Hickle.
d. *I made $3,000 an artificial earlobe.
Exercise
What, if any, difference in meaning do you perceive between He bought
Hilda a bike and He bought a bike for Hilda? (Hint: which sentence
more strongly suggests that Hilda actually got the bike?)
219
Delahunty and Garvey
Two other potential problems for students derive from the ability of many
of these words to occur as particles (25a), and as adverbs, modifiers of verbs
(25b).
(25) a. I called my sister up.
b. I looked up.
Let’s consider the complexities of the word down using the following
sentences as our data:
(26) a. I cut down the tree.
b. I fell down the hill.
c. I cut the tree down.
d.*I fell the hill down.
e.*Down the tree I cut.
f. Down the hill I fell.
g. I cut it down
h.*I fell it down.
i. *I cut down it.
j. I fell down it.
Sentences (26a, b) appear to be parallel because in both, down appears before
the NPs the tree and the hill. However, this parallelism is broken in (26c, d). In
(26c) down is grammatical after the NP, but in (26d) it is not. This difference
in behavior suggests that down may represent two different parts of speech in
these sentences. Semantically, we sense an idiomatic unity in cut down that we
do not sense in fell down. In fact, cut down could be replaced by one word:
toppled or felled. On formal and semantic grounds, then, down seems to represent different parts of speech in (26a) and (26b). The fact that down the hill
seems to have moved as a single unit in (26f) suggests that in that and related
sentences, down is a preposition heading a prepositional phrase. For down in
(26a), we have no ready-made traditional label. In such sentences we will call
it a particle, using a term coined recently by linguists, and verb + particle
combinations like cut down, look up we will call phrasal verbs.
Next, consider the word down in the sentences below:
(27) a. I fell down the hill.
b. I fell down.
Assuming that down is a true preposition in (27a), we note in (27b) that
220
The Minor Parts of Speech
down is not followed by a noun phrase, nor has it been stranded, since no
noun phrase that could be construed as its object occurs elsewhere in the
sentence. Moreover, we do not infer from (27b) that I fell down some inclined place—any more than we understand that some object did or did not
cause me to fall. Since we cannot apply any test of moveability to the right
that helped us to identify particles, we have no justification for calling down
in (27b) a particle. Must we then invent a new part of speech? The answer
is YES—unless we can fit the word into some other existing part of speech.
Can you think of a candidate for the word down? Consider sentences (28ad):
(28) a.
b.
c.
d.
Harriet visited often.
Often, Harriet visited.
I fell down.
Down I fell.
These sentences provide evidence that down in (27b/28c) may be an adverb,
since it fits criteria for adverbs (namely, that of relative moveability), that it
modifies a verb, and that it represents direction. In practice, we would like
to have more support for our analysis than this, but sometimes we do not
have that luxury.
We should note, however, the semantic motivation for the tendency
of prepositions to blend with adverbs. If you turn back to our discussion
of adverbs, you will notice that the meanings expressed by adverbs (e.g.,
Time, Place, and Manner) partially coincide with those of prepositions.
This tendency for parts of speech to overlap in meaning is just one more
reason to prefer an analysis that separates them on the basis of form.
intensifiers
Our earlier discussion of adjectives and adverbs made reference to a class
of words specifically associated with them. This class includes words like
more, most, very, quite, rather, somewhat, and a few others. Traditional
grammars often call such words degree adverbs, as if they were ordinary adverbs—like extremely and thoroughly—that happen to indicate the extent
to which the meaning of the adjective or adverb holds. If you consider the
criteria for adverbs that we suggested earlier, you will quickly realize that
the words we have listed as intensifiers—even though they do indicate
degree—do not share the formal or functional characteristics of adverbs.
For instance, intensifiers do not have the moveability of true adverbs, nor
can they occur in the comparative or superlative constructions, nor can
221
Delahunty and Garvey
one intensifier be modified by another.
(29) a. *morer
b. *mostest
c. *more quite
d. *most rather
e. *very quite
f. *rather very
In contrast, real degree adverbs (e.g., extremely and thoroughly) do allow
these possibilities.
(30) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
more extremely
quite extremely
very thoroughly
rather thoroughly
extremely thoroughly
We will thus refer to the members of this small class of words as intensifiers
rather than as adverbs.
conjunctions
The word conjunction indicates the major role of these words, namely,
to join (junction) together (con-) two or more grammatical elements called
conjuncts. The difference between coordinating and subordinating conjunctions reflects the differing grammatical statuses of the conjuncts that
are united.
Coordinating conjunctions
The major one- and two-word coordinating conjunctions appear in Table
10.
Single Word Coordinating Conjunctions: and, but, or
Minor or Marginal Coordinating Conjunctions: for, so, nor
Multi-word Coordinating Conjunctions (Correlative Conjunctions):
both…and; not only…but (also); either…or; whether…or; neither…nor
table 10: coordinating conjunctions
222
The Minor Parts of Speech
Since multi-word coordinators require their members to correlate (relate together) with their conjuncts, they are often called correlative conjunctions.
(31) a. Jack and Jill ran up the hill. (Single word)
b. Both Jack and Jill ran up the hill. (Correlative)
The units connected by coordinators may be of any size—word, phrase,
clause, or sentence. We show some typical instances below.
Two words
(32) a. Tarzan and Jane [got married] (nouns)
b. wrote and sang [the song] (verbs)
c. can and will (modals)
d. eager and willing (adjectives)
e. wildly and frantically (adverbs)
f. he and she (personal pronouns)
g. this and that (demonstrative pronouns)
h. any and all (indefinities)
i. who and why (wh-words)
j. in and about (prepositions)
Two phrases
(33) a. many readers and some literary critics (noun phrases)
b. may disagree and often have disagreed (verb phrases)
c. extremely old and completely dilapidated (adjective phrases)
d. very boldly and amazingly often (adverbial phrases)
e. of the people and for the people (prepositional phrases)
Two clauses
(34) a. who comes early and who brings a camera (relative clauses)
b. that I am right and that you are wrong (noun clauses)
c. after the game ended and before the cleanup crew arrived (adverbial clauses)
Two sentences
(35) Lou admitted his mistake and Bud forgave him.
Generally, the two conjuncts will be of the same type (i.e., noun and
noun, verb phrase and verb phrase, relative clause and relative clause). How-
223
Delahunty and Garvey
ever, in some instances formally dissimilar structures may be conjoined, as
in (36).
(36)quietly and without leaving a trace (adverb and prepositional
phrase)
The two unlike conjuncts must be functionally and semantically similar.
Sentence (36) conjoins two modifiers that indicate Manner.
Other cases in which coordinators appear to connect unlike units arise
when ellipsis occurs, as in (37):
(37) Lou admitted his mistake, but Bud didn’t.
In such examples, however, the difference in conjuncts is illusory, for the
second conjunct can be reconstructed as a structure formally comparable to
the first, as the paraphrase (38) indicates.
(38) Lou admitted his mistake, but Bud did not admit his mistake.
The literal meaning of and is equivalent to mathematical +, or logical
&. So Jack and Jill means Jack +/& Jill. So if Jack and Jill went up the hill
is true, then Jack went up the hill is true and Jill went up the hill is true.
However, in certain contexts, and communicates more than just +/&. For
example, we would normally interpret Jack fell down and broke his crown
to mean Jack fell down and then because he fell down he broke his crown. So,
amongst other meanings, and can communicate the order in which events
took place and that an earlier event caused a later one.
The literal meaning but is pretty much equivalent to that of and,
namely that both conjuncts are true. However, but adds the complication that an expectation set up by the first conjunct is to be rejected. For
example, if you go to the dean’s office and ask her assistant whether she is
in, you might get the response, She’s in, but you can’t see her now. In this
case, She’s in is true and You can’t see her now is true. But acknowledges
that if She’s in is true, then you might reasonably expect that you would
be able to see her; however, it rejects this expectation.
Like and, or can conjoin an indefinite number of expressions. Generally
when or conjoins expressions it indicates that only one of the expressions is
true. For example, if I say George, Dick, or Albert should go to jail then I am
saying that only one of the three should go to jail. However, I can override
this exclusivity by adding something like, or all three.
224
The Minor Parts of Speech
Subordinating conjunctions
Subordinating conjunctions, as the name suggests, differ from coordinators by connecting structures of unequal grammatical status. In subordination, one of the structures is grammatically superior or dominant and the
other is grammatically inferior or subordinate. The subordinate structure
is a sub-part of the larger, dominant structure. As you’d expect, the subordinate structure is the one introduced by the subordinating conjunction.
A second difference between coordinating and subordinating conjunctions is that the latter have a restricted range; they can connect clauses only.
Thus a structure introduced by a subordinating conjunction will be a subordinate clause. (It is, of course, possible for one clause to be subordinate
(bolded) to a clause that is itself subordinate (underlined), for example, The
TV news reported that the nominee claimed that he was not a crook.)
We will investigate subordinate clauses more fully in our chapter on Multiclause Sentences. For the moment, we will simply mention three important types
of subordinate clause and identify the conjunctions that may introduce them.
The subordinating conjunctions are classified according to the type of
clause they introduce. The three types of subordinate clauses are adverbial,
nominal, and relative.
Subordinating adverbial conjunctions
Adverbial clauses, like adverbs, function as modifiers of verbs or sentences.
They are introduced by a group of words that we will call subordinating
adverbial conjunctions (SAC). Table 11 lists the main SACs.
Time:
Place:
Manner:
Reason or Cause:
Result:
Comparison:
Purpose:
Condition:
Concession:
after, as, as long as, as soon as, before, just as,
now that, since, until, till, when, whenever,
while
where, wherever
as, as if, as though
as, because, inasmuch as, since
so...that, so that, such...that
as, as...as, just as, so...as, than
in order that, lest, so, so that, that
as long as, if, on (the) condition that, provided,
provided that, unless
although, even if, even though, though, while, whereas
table 11: subordinating adverbial conjunctions
225
Delahunty and Garvey
Table 11 groups SACs semantically, in a way that makes clear their overlap with adverbs. As we have just seen, prepositions also overlap with adverbs, so it should come as no surprise that prepositions have affinities with
SACs. These affinities are more than semantic. Several items of Table 11 also
appear on the lists of prepositions (Table 7 and 8). Because of this overlap,
students may experience difficulties in telling a preposition from a SAC—
and a prepositional phrase from an adverbial clause. A simple way to keep
the two clear is to remember that a preposition only occurs in construction
with a following noun phrase and a SAC is followed by a clause. Let’s consider an example.
(39) a. I left after the party.
b. I left after the party ended.
In (39a), after is followed only by a noun phrase (the party) and so must be a
preposition. In (39b), after is followed by both a noun phrase (the party) and
a verb phrase (ended) that together constitute a clause; thus after is a SAC in
(39b). We can confirm our formal analysis further by moving the group of
words after the party.
(40) a. After the party, I left.
b.*After the party, I left ended.
Since phrases often move as a unit, the prepositional phrase in (39a) can be
relocated at the front of the sentence, as it has been in (40a). But in (39b),
after the party cannot be moved, as the ungrammaticality of (40b) shows. Thus
it must not be a complete expression. In fact, the structure governed by after
in (40b) is the clause the party ended, as (41) shows.
(41) After the party ended, I left.
Clauses like after the party ended are adverbial because they function much
as adverbs do, they have meanings similar to those of adverbs, and they are
relatively moveable.
Exercise
Create at least one sentence containing an adverbial clause for each of
the semantic categories represented in Table 11.
226
The Minor Parts of Speech
Nominal conjunctions
Nominal clauses function just like noun phrases typically function—i.e., as
subjects, objects, and complements. When they do, they are introduced by a
set of subordinating conjunctions that includes most of the wh-words listed
in Table 6 along with the word that. Thus, once you know the wh-words, you
do not need to learn a separate list of nominal subordinating conjunctions. To
illustrate, note the sentences in (42).
(42) a.
b.
c.
d.
I didn’t know [who(m) I should call.]
[What you don’t know] might hurt you.
[Why Zangooli fled] is not clear.
I suspected [that he was wanted by the police.]
To assure yourself that the clauses truly have a nominal function, replace
them with the pronouns it or that.
Exercise
Create at least six more examples of sentences containing nominal
clauses modeled on (42a-d). Test that you really have created nominal
clauses by replacing them with it or that.
Relative conjunctions
Relative clauses function as modifiers of the nouns they follow. Typically, they
are introduced by a wh-word, (in this function, traditionally called relative
pronouns), or by that. For example:
(43) a.
b.
c.
d.
Anyone [who knows the answer] will receive a prize.
The cat [that caught the mouse] was jubilant.
The reason [why she left] wasn’t clear.
I anticipate the day [when the world will be at peace.]
Exercise
Create at least six more examples of relative clauses modeled on (43ad). Identify their conjunctions.
227
Delahunty and Garvey
Some complexities of subordinating conjunctions
Subordinating conjunctions have several properties that make them more
complicated than this basic presentation suggests. One that deserves mention is the tendency of subordinating conjunctions to be omitted from sentences in which their presence is easily inferred. Examples from each type
occur in (44).
(44) a. I am so tired [_____ I could sleep on a bed of nails.]
(SAC: so . . . that)
b. Everyone said [_____ they had a good time.]
(Nominal: that)
c. The reason [_____ she left] wasn’t clear.
(Relative: why or that)
A note on that
Grammatically, that is particularly interesting, largely because it belongs to
at least four different parts of speech. First, it can be a demonstrative pronoun that functions either as a modifier (e.g., That answer is correct) or as
the head of a noun phrase (e.g., That is correct). Second, it can introduce a
relative clause (e.g., The answer that she gave was correct). Third, that can act
as a noun clause connector (e.g., I said that the answer was correct). Fourth,
it can appear as part of a subordinating adverbial conjunction indicating
either result or purpose (e.g., The answer was so persuasive that it astounded
us all. The answer was phrased so that it would confuse everyone).
So how can you determine which class that belongs to in a particular
sentence? One useful test is that of substitution. If you can substitute it for
that you have a headword demonstrative; if you can substitute the, you have
a modifying demonstrative; if you can substitute who or which for that, it
introduces a relative clause. If you cannot make any of these replacements,
you have either a noun clause connector or a SAC. Distinguishing the SAC
is very simple, since it occurs normally with the word so either next to it or
nearby.
Other minor parts of speech
While our catalogue of parts of speech includes nearly all the words of English, we should ask whether other categories might be identified. There is no
reason in principle to believe that we have discovered all the parts of speech,
any more than to believe that we have discovered all the inhabitable planets in
our galaxy. Certainly, we would expect to find other parts of speech if we dealt
with languages other than English. Japanese, for instance, has words similar to
228
The Minor Parts of Speech
our prepositions, except that they follow rather than precede their associated
noun phrases. Thus English of a book would be rendered in Japanese as hon ni
(literally, book of). Because they follow their nouns, these Japanese words are
often called postpositions.
Those familiar with traditional grammar will also recall one part of
speech that we have not mentioned, the interjection. This class includes
words such as shucks, darn, gee, wow, and a host of saltier expressions. Usually a grammar will list the tamer interjections and let the matter drop.
Interjections have some interesting properties. First, they are not grammatically connected to other parts of the sentences in which they occur, and
consequently are typically separated from the remainder of their sentence
by commas. They typically indicate the speaker’s attitude or feelings about
what he is expressing (e.g., Well, our budget deficit is not as large as that of the
Axis of Evil).
Individual expressions also have certain unusual properties. Darn, for
example, enters into a variety of constructions:
(45) a. Darn it!
b. That darn cat!
c. I don’t give a darn.
In (45a), darn, seems to act like a verb expressing a wish for damnation
(darn, of course, is a euphemism for damn), though its literal meaning is
rarely intended, since even atheists can use it. In (45b), darn seems to modify cat though it is not an adjective by formal criteria: *that darner cat, *that
darnest cat, *that very darn cat, though we can say, The darndest thing happened. (45c) suggests that darn could be a noun, though we don’t seem to be
able to give more than one darn at a time: ?I don’t give two darns.
Aside from interjections, we have already seen one important way in
which new parts of speech may emerge. They may be distinguished from
other classes of which they were thought to be normal members. For instance, we separated intensifiers from the category of adverbs and particles
from prepositions. In distinguishing a group of words as a separate part of
speech, linguists attempt to direct our attention to a set of formal, functional, or semantic similarities and differences. As a result, some categories will be particular to a specific book rather than to English grammar in
general. The practice of reclassifying words is, in fact, relatively common,
especially as we discover more about language. Studying parts of speech
mirrors the study of ecological characteristics of plants and animals in nature, so it should not be surprising that, as we learn more about a particular
229
Delahunty and Garvey
species, we discover unexpected similarities between it and other apparently
unrelated species. Thus if prepositions and adverbs are not as distinct as we
once thought, our discovery of this fact derives from our closer observation
of their verbal ecology.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Austen, Jane. 1811/1961. Sense and Sensibility. New York: Washington Square
Press.
Celce-Murcia, Marianne, and Diane Larsen-Freeman. 1999. The Grammar
Book: An ESL/EFL Teacher’s Course. 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Heinle and Heinle.
Cleary, L.M. and N. Lund. 1993. Debunking some myths about traditional
grammar. In L.M. Cleary and M.D. Linn (eds.). Linguistics for Teachers. pp.
483-490. New York: McGraw Hill.
Huddleston, Rodney and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar
of the English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
McCawley, James D. 1981. Everything that Linguists have Always Wanted to
Know about Logic*. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Meyer, Charles F. 2002. English Corpus Linguistics: An Introduction. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Perry, Anne. 1993. Farrier’s Lane. New York: Fawcett Crest.
Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech, and Jan Svartik. 1972.
A Grammar of Contemporary English. London, UK: Longman.
Quirk, Randolph, and Sidney Greenbaum. 1973. A Concise Grammar of
Contemporary English. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
Radford, Andrew. 1988. Transformational Grammar. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Yule, George. 1998. Explaining English Grammar. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
glossary
active:
a grammatical voice, expressed without be + Ven. See passive.
(also called objective): case of pronouns associated with direct
objects and objects of prepositions.
adverbial clause: subordinate clause that functions as an adverbial. See
nominal clause and relative clause.
aspect: a category of a verb phrase signaled by inflection, auxiliary verbs,
and other constructions, e.g., progressive, perfect, habitual.
attributive noun phrase: a NP that provides a description but does not
refer to any particular individual. See referring noun phrase.
auxiliary verb: a verb used with a main verb to indicate aspect, voice, and
accusative
230
The Minor Parts of Speech
modality.
case form:
one of the inflectional variants of a noun, pronoun, adjective, or
(in some languages) article.
common case: the uninflected form of English nouns found in subject and
object functions.
conjunct: a grammatical element connected by a coordinating or subordinating conjunction to another grammatical element.
conjunction: a function word that joins grammatical elements either as
coordinate to each other or as one subordinate to the other.
content word: nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs. See chapter on Major
Parts of Speech.
coordinating conjunction: a function word such as and, but, or, etc. that
connects grammatically equal elements. See subordinating conjunction.
co-reference: property of noun phrases denoting the same entity; applies
to a pronoun and its antecedent or to two noun phrases.
correlative conjunction: a multiple-word coordinating conjunction, e.g.,
both...and, either...or.
declarative (also called indicative): in traditional grammar, the mood of a
sentence used to make an assertion.
definiteness: property of some NPs (and the (pro)nouns and articles they
contain), which denote a speaker’s assumption that their referent can be
readily identified. See indefiniteness.
deictic: words like the demonstratives, whose referents depend upon the
situations they occur in, and which change from use to use.
demonstrative: the deictic words this, that, these, and those. See deictic.
function words (also called structure words): words such as prepositions, auxiliaries, and articles that are used frequently in a language to signal
recurrent semantic and grammatical information.
gender: linguistic category distinguishing words or the entities they denote as
masculine, feminine, or neuter; signaled by pronouns and suffixes. Languages
other than English may signal gender by adjective inflection, articles, verb agreement, etc.
generic: property of an article whereby it designates an entire class. See
specific.
generic pronoun: pronoun that makes no gender distinction, e.g., one.
genitive: case signaled by ’s and s’, indicating a variety of semantic relations,
including possession, authorship, involvement with, and many less specific
relations. Also called the possessive case.
imperative: in traditional grammar, the mood of a sentence used to give a command.
231
Delahunty and Garvey
indefiniteness: property of some NPs (and the (pro)nouns and articles they
contain), which denote a speaker’s assumption that their referent cannot be
specifically identified. See definiteness.
indicative: See declarative.
information question: a question, introduced by a wh-word, requesting
information rather than a yes-no response. See yes-no question.
intensifier: a function word (typically more, most, very, quite, rather, and
somewhat) used to modify an adjective or an adverb.
intensive pronoun: a pronoun ending in -self or -selves that ordinarily occurs within the noun phrase of its antecedent, following and modifying the
antecedent directly, e.g., I myself did it. See reflexive pronoun.
interactional force: the function of a sentence in a discourse to make assertions, ask questions, issue orders, etc.
interjection: a word, often not grammatically integrated with a sentence,
that expresses the emotions, etc., of the speaker, e.g., Ouch!, Wow!
interrogative: in traditional grammar, the mood of a sentence used to ask a
question.
mood: in traditional grammar, the category indicating whether a sentence
makes an assertion, asks a question, issues an order, etc.
nominal clause (also called noun clause): a subordinate clause that functions as subject, object, or complement. See adverbial clause and relative
clause.
nominative: the case associated with the subject function.
noun phrase: a phrase with a noun as its head word.
noun clause: See nominal clause.
objective case: See accusative case.
particle: a function word, which, with a verb, constitutes a phrasal verb,
e.g., call up my sister/call my sister up.
passive: a voice expressed by the form be + Ven.
perfect: an aspect of a verb phrase, expressed by have + Ven.
person: grammatical category distinguishing the speaker (first person), addressee (second person), and entity spoken about (third person).
phrasal verb: an idiomatic unit consisting of a verb and particle
possessive case: See genitive case.
postposition: a word analogous to a preposition, but appearing after its object NP; appears in Japanese and Old English, but not in modern English.
preposition stranding: ending a clause or sentence with a preposition
whose object has been moved.
preposition: a function word that serves as the head of a prepositional
phrase, e.g., in, on, with, of.
232
The Minor Parts of Speech
prepositional phrase:
phrase consisting of a prepostion and NP.
an aspect of a verb phrase signaled by be + Ving.
quantifier: words such as someone, something whose referents are often
vague. See indefinitness.
referring noun phrase: a NP that denotes a particular entity or set of entities. See attributive noun phrase.
reference: the entities, qualities, situations, or events identified by (the use
of ) linguistic expressions.
reflexive pronoun: a pronoun ending in -self or -selves that functions as the
head of a NP, e.g., I hurt myself. See intensive pronoun.
relative clause: a subordinate clause that modifies a head noun; often introduced by a wh-word or that.
relative pronoun: in traditional grammar, a wh-word or that introducing
a relative clause.
specific: property of articles whereby they designate particular members of
a class. See generic.
structure word: See function words.
subjunctive: in traditional grammar, the mood of a sentence used to indicate wishes, contrary-to-fact conditions, probability, possibility, etc.
subordinating adverbial conjunction (sac): conjunction such as when, if,
because, etc. that introduces a subordinate adverbial clause.
subordinating conjunction: a function word such as if, when, because,
that, who, etc., that connects two clauses, making one of secondary grammatical status, specifically a modifier or a complement.
truncated passive: a passive sentence without a by-phrase.
wh-question: a question beginning with a wh-word that asks for more information than just yes or no. See information question, yes-no question.
wh-word: a function word such as who, why, which that introduces questions, relative clauses, and nominal clauses.
yes-no question: a question that can be appropriately answered with yes or
no. See information question, wh-question.
progressive:
233
Delahunty and Garvey
234
8 Word Meaning
key concepts
Dictionary entries
Sense relations
Models of word meaning
Mental dictionaries
i n t ro d u c t i o n
In this chapter we discuss word meaning. While it’s uncontroversial that
words mean, it is far from clear how they mean, or indeed what meaning is.
Because dictionaries are so familiar, we begin our discussion from the point
of view of dictionary entries, which are designed primarily to describe the
meanings of words, though they do much else besides. We discuss two approaches to modeling word meaning, and then move to a discussion of the
meanings of words as they might be stored in human minds and of the ways
in which book and mental dictionaries are alike and different.
We would be surprised if anyone reading this book had never consulted
a dictionary; however, our experience over several decades of teaching about
language is that very few people read the introductions (front matter) of dictionaries they may have had for many years. Indeed, our experience strongly
suggests that most people believe in the myth of “The Dictionary,” a unique,
authoritative, and accurate source of information on words, their spellings,
meanings, and histories, of which actual dictionaries are merely longer or
shorter versions.
Everyone, especially teachers, should be aware that dictionaries are not
all cut from the same cloth. Rather, they differ in substantial ways, which
their users ignore at the cost of misinterpreting what they read. The goals of
the exercise just below are to raise your awareness of the differences among
dictionaries, to show you that it is essential to adopt as critical a stance
toward dictionaries as you would toward any other commercial product,
and to encourage you to examine dictionaries carefully as you buy them for
yourselves, have them bought for your schools, or recommend them to your
students.
More generally, teachers and students should have some appreciation of
the complexity of issues regarding linguistic meaning, a topic that has challenged western thinking for over two and a half millennia. We have included
several items in our References and Resources to this chapter that we hope
will help develop that appreciation. The chapter will give you a basic vocabulary for use in conceptualizing and discussing meaning, as well as concepts
235
Delahunty and Garvey
to augment our discussion of morphology and parts of speech.
dictionary entries
Dictionaries are probably the sources of information on words you are
most familiar with, so we begin our discussion of words by exploring the
information dictionaries provide and the ways in which they present it.
Exercise
1. Just to see how much you may have been taking for granted, and
how much of that was right and how much was mistaken, write a 6-7
page critical review comparing/contrasting two reasonably substantial
dictionaries (i.e., compact, collegiate or larger; pocket dictionaries are
too small) suitable for your purposes, e.g., for your own personal or
professional uses or to recommend to the kinds of students you may
teach. We think you will learn a lot about dictionaries by comparing/
contrasting a learner’s dictionary with one for native English speakers. Make sure to give their full names, editions, publishers, dates of
publication, and sizes. Indicate the size of the dictionaries by number
of pages or entries. Your instructor should approve your choice of dictionaries before you begin. As this is to be a critical review, you should
clearly articulate for yourself and your readers the criteria you use to
evaluate the dictionaries.
Compare/contrast their front matters (i.e., everything from the
front cover to the beginning of the alphabetical listing of words) and
their back matters (i.e., everything from the end of the alphabetical
listings to the inside back cover). Pay particular attention to:
a. the dictionaries’ range of contents (e.g., illustrations, proper
names, maps, etc.);
b. the information included in the entries (e.g., pronunciation
[what systems are used to describe it, e.g., IPA or some other
system?], syllabication [what is meant by this in the dictionaries?], etymology, part(s) and subcategories of speech [what range
of these is used in the dictionaries?], definitions, etc.);
c. the ways in which definitions are organized (e.g., earlier to later,
most general to most particular, most frequent to least, etc.);
d. the ways in which your dictionaries deal with expressions related
to the head word, including derivationally related forms, compounds, phrases, idioms, homographs, etc.;
e. the ways in which your dictionaries deal with controversial usage
236
Word Meaning
issues (e.g., the use of hopefully as a sentence adverb or the
modification of unique);
f. (for learners’ dictionaries) the defining vocabulary, if a special
one is used;
g. (for learners’ dictionaries) the grammatical information included
and how it is presented;
h. who the publishers say the dictionaries are designed for. Discuss
the ways in which the dictionaries are or are not appropriate
for your purposes (e.g., for students you might imagine yourself
teaching);
i. the databases on which the dictionaries are based (e.g., are they
based on large, computerized, collections of texts? What kinds of
texts are included?).
Illustrate your review with appropriate examples from the dictionaries. A very useful source of information and ideas on both native speaker
and learner dictionaries is Howard Jackson’s Words and their Meanings,
but there are many more books on lexicography worth reading for this
project.
2. Imagine a class of students that you might reasonably expect to
teach. What criteria would you use to select a dictionary for the classroom? To ask students to buy for themselves? For yourself?
Dictionaries are designed to provide readily accessible information about
the words of one or more languages. Many dictionaries provide far more
information than that. They may include lists of colleges and universities,
US presidents, the US constitution, and the like. While they may expand
their domains in certain respects, they may narrow them in others. Some
dictionaries are designed for college students and include words that the
editors believe are most relevant to that market segment (and we must never
forget that dictionaries are commercial products and that there is no such
thing as The Dictionary); other dictionaries are devoted to slang; still others
to technical fields such as medicine or linguistics (useful ones to complement this book would be Crystal’s A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics,
Matthews’ Concise Oxford Dictionary of Linguistics, and Johnson and Johnson’s Encyclopedic Dictionary of Applied Linguistics).
Other dictionaries are for people learning English (or some other language) as a second or foreign language, like the Cambridge International
Dictionary (CIDE), Random House English Learner’s Dictionary, and Harp237
Delahunty and Garvey
er Collins Beginners ESL Dictionary/Collins CoBuild New Student’s Dictionary, Longman Dictionary of Contemporary English (also available online),
and the American Heritage Dictionary for Learners of English. Many learner’s
dictionaries provide simplified definitions, often by using a limited “defining vocabulary” of about 2,000 of the most frequently used English words.
They also typically provide considerably more grammatical information
and examples of the uses of the words than dictionaries prepared for native
speakers. For example, in addition to the grammatical information provided
in its entries, the HarperCollins Beginner’s ESL Dictionary provides a very
useful 220 page synoptic “English grammar guide.” Bilingual dictionaries
provide definitions in one language for words in another.
To make our discussion concrete and specific we will make use of the following entry from The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language
(AHD) (Fourth Edition).
jeal.ous (jĕl @s) adj. 1. Fearful or wary of being supplanted; apprehensive of losing affection or position. 2a. Resentful or bitter in rivalry;
envious: jealous of the success of others. b. Inclined to suspect rivalry. 3.
Having to do with or arising from feelings of envy, apprehension, or
bitterness: jealous thoughts. 4. Vigilant in guarding something: We are
jealous of our good name. 5. Intolerant of disloyalty or infidelity; autocratic: a jealous God. [Middle English jelous, from Old French gelos,
jealous, zealous, from Vulgar Latin *zēlōsus, from Late Latin zēlus, zeal.
See zeal]—jeal ous.ly adv.—jeal ous.ness n.
Dictionaries differ in the categories of information they include in their
entries and in the ways in which they organize that information. Editors try
to chose the most readable presentation for each entry. But practices vary,
and teachers should be aware of the variations and choose appropriate dictionaries for themselves and their students.
Entry and entry-word
The entire paragraph quoted above is called an entry; the first (bolded)
word of the entry is its head- or entry-word. Ordinary dictionaries facilitate finding information about the headwords by arranging them alphabetically.
238
Word Meaning
Exercise
What advantages and disadvantages might come from arranging the
entries of a dictionary alphabetically?
A typical native speaker dictionary provides substantial information in
each entry. In the entry above, the conventional spelling is given by the
entry word; if there had been another well-accepted spelling, it would have
been included after the entry word. The spelling includes syllabication information, in this dictionary by means of a raised dot in the entry word.
Syllabication in the entry word tells writers where they may hyphenate the
word at the end of a line of type; it is only indirectly related to pronunciation and is becoming irrelevant as we rely on the justification programs in
our word processors to space letters for us.
Pronunciation
The pronunciation of the word is given in parentheses after the headword.
AHD uses a mix of ordinary English letters, letters with diacritics, joined
letters (ligatures), and one letter, @, from the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). The sound value of each letter in the pronunciation guide is
indicated by reference to an English word. This kind of system works if you
know how to pronounce the reference words as the lexicographers expect,
but if you don’t know how that word is pronounced, or if you pronounce it
in an unusual way (for example, according to a non-standard dialect), then
the dictionary’s pronunciation guide may be quite misleading. AHD, like
many dictionaries, repeats the list of reference words on each second page.
Syllabication (or syllabification) in the pronunciation section separates the word into its component spoken syllables and typically also indicates stress. AHD inserts a hyphen or stress mark between each syllable
in the pronunciation and marks the syllable with the main stress by a following . For example, the most usual pronunciation of Mongolia is given
as (mŏng-gō -lē-@, . . .).
Learners’ dictionaries typically use IPA symbols to indicate pronunciation. These symbols have fixed sound values, independent of anyone’s native
language, dialect, or idiosyncrasies, so they avoid some of the problems associated with native speaker dictionary pronunciation guides. However, if,
like most American students, you don’t know the sound values of the IPA
symbols, they are quite unhelpful. It is important to understand your dictionary’s way of indicating pronunciation, and perhaps to learn a relevant
set of the IPA symbols.
239
Delahunty and Garvey
Parts of speech
After the pronunciation comes the headword’s part of speech. AHD uses
the nine traditional parts of speech: adjective, adverb, article, conjunction,
interjection, noun, preposition, pronoun, and verb. It distinguishes definite
and indefinite articles and transitive, intransitive, and auxiliary verbs. It also
marks some singular and plural nouns and lists prefixes and suffixes. Some
dictionaries may use terms that are unfamiliar to you, such as the Oxford
English Dictionary’s (OED) substantive (abbreviated sb.). OED is also unusually fine-grained as it designates nouns as either of action or of agent (n.
of action/agent).
Many entry words belong to several different parts of speech, and different dictionaries have different ways of handling this. Some include them all
in a single entry, called a combined entry by AHD. Others give a separate
entry to each different part of speech that the word belongs to, essentially
treating each different part of speech associated with a spelling as a homograph (see below).
Learners’ dictionaries tend to give more grammatical information than native speaker dictionaries. They try to provide the grammatical information that
is particularly helpful for learners. English learners tend to have difficulty with
the count/non-count distinction in English nouns, so for each noun, CIDE
indicates whether it is count [C] or non-count [U]. Similarly, while most
adjectives may occur before the noun they modify as well as in the predicate
of a subject complement clause such as Frederika is very tall, some adjectives
may occur only before their nouns (e.g., former, live) and some only in the
predicate (e.g., aghast, alive, asleep, awake). Generally native speaker dictionaries, such as Webster’s New World Dictionary (WNWD), do not provide
this information, but learners’ dictionaries typically do. CIDE uses [before n]
for the former and [after n] (somewhat misleadingly) for the latter. WNWD
merely provides a very few illustrative examples of the predicative use, which,
of course, do not tell a reader whether he or she may use the adjective before
a noun.
Run-ons
Dictionaries also differ in how they deal with words and other expressions
that are related to the headword. AHD adds the adverb and noun forms at
the end of the entry for jealous because their meanings are straightforwardly
inferable from the headword’s meaning and their forms. However, if the
meanings of the derived words are not readily predictable from the meaning
of the entry word and the derivation, then the derived word may get its own
entry. For example, AHD separates hereditarian from hereditary.
240
Word Meaning
Dictionary practices are not always consistent. While AHD lists retaliation, retaliative, retaliatory, and retaliator as run-ons at the end of the entry
for retaliate, it gives retrench and retrenchment separate entries, even though
the meanings of the latter are readily derivable from those of the former.
Check your dictionary for its policies.
Etymology/word history
After the definitions of the word, AHD provides a brief sketch of the history
or etymology (not entomology) of the word. In this case, modern English
jealousy is descended from Middle English jelous, which was borrowed from
the Old French word gelos, which in turn came from Vulgar (i.e., ordinary
spoken) Latin *zēlōsus (* indicates that the form does not occur in any manuscript but has been reconstructed according to generally accepted linguistic principles of language change), which descended from Late Latin zēlus.
(Many dictionaries abbreviate the names of languages and historical stages
of languages; check your dictionary’s list of abbreviations for expressions
like ML and ME.) AHD is unusually helpful in providing for many words
a paragraph-length Word History separate from the etymological sketch
within the entry.
Typically, learners’ dictionaries do not include etymological information,
though some language teachers believe that such information can be useful.
Usage
Usage is the study of the ways in which expressions of a language are used
by the speakers of that language, especially in formal speaking and writing.
Linguists view usage descriptively, that is, they study how expressions are actually used. Others adopt a prescriptive approach to usage, that is, they seek
to impose rules of correctness based on criteria other than the practices of
the users of the language. English dictionary users expect guidance on how
expressions are (or should be) used, especially when usages are controversial.
And indeed, many dictionary editors see it as their duty to provide authoritative advice on the usage of the headwords or of particular senses. For many
words whose usage is controversial, AHD provides a very useful, critical,
paragraph-length Usage Note, based on comments by its usage panel leavened by the linguistic expertise of its Usage Consultant, Geoffrey Nunberg.
Other dictionaries use other devices to provide usage information. Typical is Webster’s New World Dictionary’s use of short Usage Labels. For
example, WNWD attaches the rubric [Now Rare] to its version of AHD’s
sense 5 of jealous. As dictionaries differ on whether they include usage advice as such, as well as on the number of usage labels and their meanings,
241
Delahunty and Garvey
their readers are best advised to read their front matters.
Some dictionaries embed usage information as though it were grammatical
information. A dictionary that ignores or treats a controversial usage issue as a
straightforward grammatical one misinforms its readers. For example, CIDE
says that unique is grammatically [not gradable], “being the only existing one
of its type . . .” According to this grammatical categorization, expressions such
as almost unique and very unique should be ungrammatical, though they are
widely used by native English speakers, including highly educated ones. This
puts the grammatical horse before the usage cart. Languages change, and one
way in which they change is by extending the range of ways in which words
may be used, for instance by broadening the scope of a non-gradable adjective
by allowing it to be modified. Unique is only a non-gradable adjective if speakers of English treat it consistently that way. But they don’t, and no dictionary
can put that genie back in its bottle. What CIDE ought to have done was alert
its users to the fact that under some circumstances, some people will object
to modified unique. What it actually does, somewhat contradictorily, is add
“more generally, unusual or special in some way.” Note that unusual and special are gradable adjectives. It is best to read the front matter to find out what
your lexicographers have been up to, though they are not always consistent.
You might compare the CIDE entry for unique with that in AHD, especially
its Usage Note for that word.
Exercise
Check your dictionary for how it deals with usage issues, and then
check unique, hopefully, infer, irregardless. Compare your dictionary’s
approach with the AHD’s Usage Notes on each of these; you might also
consult a usage dictionary such as the Harper Dictionary of Contemporary Usage (Morris and Morris 1985) to see what it says about these
words.
Dictionaries tend to lump several different linguistic categories together under Usage Labels. WNWD usage labels include a word’s frequency of use
(archaic, obsolete, rare), its level of formality (colloquial, slang), its field
(poetic), and its region (dialect, British, Canadian).
Lexical fields
Words may have different (though related) meanings in different fields; that
is, in different topics, disciplines, work and play domains, and the like. For
242
Word Meaning
example, the word morphology is used in linguistics, biology, and various
other sciences. Dictionaries have a variety of ways of dealing with field information. The following partial entry from Webster’s New Twentieth Century
Dictionary Unabridged (2nd Edition) (WNTC) illustrates how editors may
use the separation of senses to separate fields by embedding the field name
(bolded) in the definition.
mor.phol o.gy . . .
1. the branch of biology that deals with the form and structure of animals and plants, without regard to function.
2. the branch of linguistics that deals with the internal structure and
forms of words: with syntax, it forms a basic division of grammar.
3. any scientific study of forms and structure, as in physical geography,
etc.
4. (a) form and structure, as of an organism, regarded as a whole; (b)
morphological features collectively, as of a language. (WNTC: 1170; emphasis added)
Other dictionaries, such as WNWD, use italicized labels like Chem. that
precede the definition:
bi.na.ry . . . 4. Chem. composed of two elements or radicals, or of one
element and one radical (WNWD: 141)
Cross referencing
To indicate the relations that words develop, maintain, and break off with
other words, dictionaries cross-reference words in various ways. WNWD
uses “see,” “also,” same as, and several other expressions for this purpose.
au.to.mat.ic . . . SYN see spontaneous (WNWD: 95)
coffee klatch (or klatsch) same as kaffeeklatsch (WNWD: 275)
Inflections
All dictionaries provide some information about the inflections of major
parts of speech, generally only those that are irregular either in spelling or
in pronunciation (though some dictionaries, including AHD and Webster’s,
provide all verb inflections). The inflections are typically abbreviated by
omitting the unchanged part of the word, for example:
243
Delahunty and Garvey
a.lum.na . . . n., pl. -nae
a.lum.nus . . . n., pl. -ni (WNWD p. 41)
ser.aph . . . n., pl. -aphs, -aphim (WNWD p. 1299)
AHD lists verb inflections in the following order: past tense (-ed form),
past participle (-en form), present participle (-ing form), and third person,
singular, present tense (-s form). Where individual inflected forms would
occur at some distance from the main entry in the alphabetical listing, they
may be cross referenced to it, as in WNWD:
sang . . . alt. pt. of sing (WNWD p. 1261)
sing . . . sang or now rarely sung, sung, singing (WNWD p. 1329)
sung . . . pp. & rare pt. of sing (WNWD p. 1427)
Senses
Senses are the definitions associated with the entry word. In AHD, they follow the part of speech label. In WNWD, they follow the etymology. Definitions are the lexicographers’ attempts to represent the meanings associated
with the head word. These are typically given in words, though there are pictorial dictionaries for children and many dictionaries include illustrations of
various sorts. The definitions given for a word in one dictionary are likely to
be very similar to the definitions given for that word in other dictionaries.
This is because modern English dictionaries are representatives of a lexicographical tradition that is many centuries old; it is also because crafting
definitions within the conventions imposed by that tradition is constraining
and difficult, and because lexicographers look to see how their competitors
have crafted their definitions. We will look at some of the devices lexicographers use to craft definitions below.
If every form were associated with only a single meaning, and if every different meaning were associated with only a single form, then the lexicographer’s task would be considerably simplified, although dictionaries might be
rather larger than they are now. However, as our examples have shown (and
as a quick flick through a dictionary will confirm), many, if not most, entry
words are associated with multiple meanings. Given that, lexicographers
have to decide on the best strategy to represent the form-meaning connection. Should there be one entry with lots of senses? Or should there be multiple entries whose headwords are spelled identically but whose meanings
differ?
Lexicographers have developed strategies for dealing with such situations. Generally, if the meanings associated with a single spelling are his244
Word Meaning
torically descended from the same earlier form, and are clearly closely
related to each other, then they will be grouped under a single headword.
Such a headword is polysemous. Morphology is presented as a polysemous
word in AHD, WNWD, and WNTC, though not in CIDE.
Once dictionaries allow polysemous entries, the editors have to decide on
how to order the senses in an entry. Webster’s groups them so that the most
similar are presented together under the same number, separated if necessary by letters. As most words have more than a single meaning, most entries
will be organized in this way.
read1 . . . 1. a) to get the meaning of (something written, printed, embossed, etc.) by using the eyes, or for Braille, the finger tips, to interpret
its characters or signs b) clipped form of proofread (WNWD p. 1181)
par.a.site . . . 1. a person, as in ancient Greece, who flattered and amused
his host in return for free meals 2. a person who lives at the expense of
another or others without making any useful contribution or return;
hanger-on 3. Biol. a plant or animal that lives on or in an organism of
another species from which it derives sustenance or protection without
benefiting the host and usually doing harm. (WNWD p.1031)
Dictionaries differ in the principles they use to order the senses in an
entry. WNWD uses a mix of historical and logical ordering:
The senses of an entry have, wherever possible, been arranged in semantic order from the etymology to the most recent sense so that there is a
logical, progressive flow showing the development of the word and the
relationship of its senses to one another. (p. xii)
This principle is clearly evidenced by the entry for parasite above. The first
sense is the original and the others derive from that both logically and historically.
AHD orders senses “with the central and often the most commonly
sought meaning first.”
CIDE gives each separate set of closely related senses its own entry and
labels each entry with a guide word chosen to help the user home in on the
entry s/he wants:
oc.cu.py . . . fill
oc.cu.py . . . take control (CIDE p. 973)
245
Delahunty and Garvey
Dictionaries may provide separate, cross-referenced entries for separate
spellings of words, even where the meanings are identical. Usually only one
head word will be provided with a full entry. For example, WNWD has the
entries je.had and ji.had, but the entry for je.had has a same as cross reference to ji.had, where the full entry is given.
If both spellings and meanings have diverged, then alphabetization will
separate the entries, and any cross reference may be in the historical section of the entry. Flower and flour were both spelled flour earlier in English,
and both derive from Latin flor- (“flower”). Flower means the blossom and
seed-producing parts of plants, but flour has specialized and now means the
ground “flower,” or best part of a grain, mainly of wheat. The separate spellings usefully separate the meanings for us. Note, however, that flower and
flour are pronounced identically, so they are homophones.
If a single spelling has two or more quite unrelated meanings, then lexicographers will typically assign a separate entry for each (set of ) unrelated
meaning(s). WNWD distinguishes homographs by superscript numerals:
dam1. . . 1. a barrier built to hold back flowing water
dam2. . . 1. the female parent of any four-legged animal (WNWD p. 356)
Both homophones and homographs may be grouped together under the
term homonym.
Sense relations
So far we have looked at the overall organization of entries. Let’s now look
at how the meanings of words are expressed.
If the dictionary is not a bilingual one, then the definitions are expressed
in the same language as the headword, so there is a built-in circularity. For
example, the first sense of salt in WNWD is “sodium chloride,” (p. 1257)
and the definition WNWD gives for sodium chloride is “common salt” (p.
1352).
If you don’t know the words used in the definition, you can’t figure out
the meaning of the headword. English learners’ dictionaries attempt to deal
with this problem by using a defining vocabulary of words they assume to be
known to all or most high-beginner or intermediate learners of the language,
often the 2,000 most frequently used words of the language. CIDE uses several criteria besides frequency in choosing words for its defining vocabulary:
the words must have the same meaning in both US and British English, be
easy for learners to understand, be up-to-date, not be easily confused with
other English words, not be easily confused with foreign words, and be useful
246
Word Meaning
for explaining other words (CIDE p. 1702).
Exercise
Discuss the defining vocabularies of at least two other learners’ dictionaries.
Native speaker dictionaries assume that their users have a much larger
vocabulary, although the fact that modern dictionaries typically include
even the most basic words means that they must define these words in less
basic terms. For example:
hole . . . 1. a hollow or hollowed-out place; cavity; specif., a) an excavation or pit . . . (WNWD p. 668)
If possible, a single word equivalent, that is, a synonym, may be used:
to boot besides (WNWD p. 163)
apteryx . . . same as kiwi (WNWD p. 69)
agree . . . 1. to consent or accede (WNWD p. 27)
Synonymy is usually defined as words that have the same meaning, though
it is very unlikely that any two words will have exactly the same meaning. In
her lively and lucid study Words in the Mind, Jean Aitchison (2003, 3rd ed.)
observes that we tend to pursue something desirable (e.g., knowledge, a career)
but chase things such as runaway horses (p. 94). For some speakers, chasing
evokes the notion of speed, while pursuing does not necessarily do so. Synonyms thus have to be thought of as two “circles of meaning” that overlap to
a greater or lesser extent.
Partial synonymy is much more common than full synonymy. Typically,
synonyms are distinguished by subtle meaning differences that challenge
lexicographers, linguists, and second language learners, though generally
not native speakers. Usage labels may help to distinguish among partial synonyms: words may differ in style (to stick to something is neutral, to cleave to
something is poetic), or in the places where they are typically used (elevator is
US usage, lift is British).
247
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
The following sets of words are partial synonyms. Identify how they
are similar and how they differ: car-automobile; silver-argent; cruxcross; disconcert-rattle; truck-lorry; soda-pop-soft drink; cat-kitty;
make-fabricate; facile-skillful; irritate-annoy-aggravate; woodchuckgroundhog; buy-purchase. Putting the words in sentences will help you
distinguish among them. So will consulting a good dictionary.
WNWD and WNTC provide lists of synonyms distinguished by comments after the main body of the entry. After the synonyms, they provide
lists of antonyms. Antonyms are traditionally defined as words with opposite meanings, such as up and down, good and bad, and the like, though
they must share some important aspect of their meanings. For instance, large
and small share the notion of size. However, apple and eraser are not antonyms
because they share little, if any, meaning aside from “physical object.” We
distinguish several types of antonym (Cruse, 1986, 2001).
Complementary antonyms are pairs of words such that if one word applies the other cannot, for example, alive and dead. If a person is alive, he
or she cannot be dead, and vice versa. Other examples are hit-miss, pass-fail,
open-closed.
Gradable antonyms denote opposing positions on some scale; for example, hot and cold indicate opposite positions on a temperature scale. Because scales are continuous phenomena, we can indicate varying positions
on them by modifying the words, e.g., hotter, hottest, awfully hot, miserably
cold. The values between and beyond the antonyms may also be lexicalized.
In between hot and cold we have warm, tepid, cool, and beyond hot and cold
there is burning, scalding, and freezing, among others. Other gradable pairs
include tall-short, wide-narrow, big-small, strong-weak, heavy-light, high-low.
You probably noticed that the members of these pairs are not entirely
parallel; one seems to be more basic, or unmarked, than the other. We
use the basic, unmarked form to ask questions when we have no specific
expectation that the marked form describes the situation, i.e., when the
question is not loaded toward the marked form. For example, ordinarily if
we want to know how strong someone is we simply ask How strong is he or
she? If, however, we assume that this person is weaker than some norm, then
we use the marked member of the word pair: How weak is he or she? (The
marked/unmarked distinction is important in certain literary theories; see
Barthes’ S/Z.)
You probably also noticed that the scales we use depend on what we’re
248
Word Meaning
measuring; for instance, a small pumpkin is very likely to be much larger
than a big pea.
Non-gradable antonyms cannot be modified, often because they denote
absolute differences; e.g., metabolic is non-gradable: it does not accept the
comparative or the superlative or modification by degree adverb or intensifier (* more metabolic, *most metabolic, *excessively metabolic, *very metabolic).
Other non-gradables include absolute, sonic, utter.
Reversive antonyms are words that represent movement in opposite directions, such as advance-retreat, go (away)-come (back), ascend-descend, risefall, go-return, fill-empty, open-close.
Conversive antonyms represent the same situation from two points of
view. For example, if the cat is higher than the bird, then the bird is lower
than the cat. The vertical relationship between the bird and the cat can be
viewed from two points of view: lower than and higher than. Another example is wife-husband: if Tarzan is Jane’s husband, then Jane is Tarzan’s wife.
Other examples include buy-sell, give-receive, own-belong to, above-below.
While synonyms and antonyms are words at the same semantic level,
word meanings may also be hierarchically related to each other. For example, a mallard is a kind of duck which is a kind of bird. We can represent
this relation as a tree:
Bird
Duck
Mallard
The meaning “bird” is included in the meaning “duck,” which in turn is
included in the meaning “mallard.” Or from the bottom up: the meaning
“mallard” includes the meaning “duck,” which includes the meaning “bird.”
This relationship is called hyponymy. The lower terms are the hyponyms of
the higher terms, which are the superordinates or hypernyms of the lower
terms. Similarly, the meaning of rose includes the meaning of flower. Consequently, if something is a rose then it must also be a flower. Conversely,
the set of things we call roses is included in the set of things we call flowers.
A superordinate term may have many subordinate terms, called co-hyponyms or coordinates:
249
Delahunty and Garvey
Chop saw
Miter saw
Saw
Table saw
Ripsaw
Hand saw
Hacksaw
In this instance, the meaning and the form saw occur in each of the hyponyms, which, in spite of their spellings, are all compound words. We must
mention here that not all groups of words that could be regarded as constituting a set of coordinates have a lexical superordinate. As far as we know,
there is no single term that encompasses doors and windows, even though
these are openings in walls for light, air, people, and refrigerators to pass
through.
Dictionaries make extensive use of hyponymy to define words. For instance, WNTC defines orator as “a person who delivers an oration.” (p.
1257) and an oration as “an elaborate speech or discourse . . .” (p. 1257). So
an orator is a kind of person and an oration is a kind of speech. The remaining parts of the definition tell us what kind of person an orator is and what
kind of speech an oration is, as well as how orators are distinct from other
kinds of persons, and orations from other kinds of speeches.
Dictionaries also make use of part/whole and part/part relationships
to define words. There are several types of these. When these relationships
apply to unified objects, they are called partonymy, or less transparently,
meronymy. For example, the covers and pages are parts of books; the engine,
trunk, carburetor, and fan belt are parts of cars. The crankshaft is a part of
the engine of a car. WNWD defines cap.i.tal2 as “the top part of a column
or pilaster” (p. 210). Meronymic relationships apply not only to physical objects but extend to temporal relationships (day/week), events (inning/baseball
game), and even to quite abstract entities (self‑control/maturity).
Because hyponymy and partonymy differ in the semantics of the relationships—kind of vs. part of—they differ in how lower order terms relate
to superordinates of superordinates. In hyponymy, the lower order term is
a kind of its superordinate and of its superordinate’s superordinate; for instance, a standard poodle is a kind of poodle, and a poodle is a kind of dog. But
a standard poodle is also a kind of dog. On the other hand, a lower order term
in a partonymy may or may not be a part of the superordinate; for instance,
a page is a part of a book and a book may be part of a library, but it would
certainly be odd to claim that a page is part of a library.
Other part/whole relations refer not to parts and wholes of unified ob250
Word Meaning
jects but to entities associated with each other in a situation. Metonymy
is the basis for many shifts of meaning. It involves the use of an expression
denoting one person or thing to refer to someone or something associated
with it. The use of a restaurant customer’s order to refer to the customer is a
very productive source of metonymy. For instance, a waiter might say, The
fishburger wants more French fries, to identify a particular customer and their
request. The use of personal names to refer to events that the individual
named is responsible for is also productive: Bush invaded Iraq. Metonymy
is occasionally the basis for permanent shifts of meaning; look up bead in a
comprehensive dictionary with etymological information such as AHD.
Metaphor is yet another relationship among words. It is based on perceived similarities between entities, and word meanings are often extended
to denote entities similar in some ways to the ones more typically denoted
by the word. Many metaphors are based on body parts; for example, AHD
(p. 807) includes in its meanings for head the head of a boil, the head of
a tool such as a hammer, a head of cabbage, the head of a group, the head
of a phrase, and lots of others, all metaphorically derived from the central
meaning of head, namely that mass of bone and brain that sits atop your
neck. Mouth and foot also have multiple metaphoric meanings, which your
dictionary should list.
Because metaphorical senses are extensions of the basic senses of words,
they develop historically later than them. Some extensions may be haphazard; for instance, we do not think of the nose of a river or a bottle. But there
may be some general principles in language for metaphorical creation. For
instance, English seems to have a principle by which color words may be
extended to psychological states: e.g., blue (sad), red (with anger), green (with
envy), yellow (cowardly), black (mood). (See Lakoff and Johnson 1980.)
Exercise
1. Compare and contrast a regular dictionary with a thesaurus, paying
particular attention to the ways in which both are organized and the
ways in which meanings are represented. What purposes do you think
each was designed for?
2. Rhetoricians, literary critics, and others interested in figures of
speech (tropes) have distinguished many types and subtypes. Those related to metonymy are particularly interesting. You might investigate
synecdoche and antonomasia and discuss their implications for word
meaning. Lakoff and Johnson (1980) is a thought-provoking discussion
251
Delahunty and Garvey
of figures of speech, especially of metaphor.
How do we know words have meaning?
By posing this question, we do not intend to cast doubt on the proposition
that words have meanings. Rather, we want to spell out some good reasons
to believe it. In our chapter on Concepts of Language, we observed that our
linguistic competence allows us to do many things. (Recall that competence is
unconscious linguistic knowledge, which includes knowledge of the meanings
of words; examples such as the ones below tell us only that such knowledge
must exist, not what it actually is.) Our competence enables us to distinguish
between well‑ and ill‑formed strings of words and to detect grammatical structure. Crucially for our current discussion, it enables us to detect meaning
relations among expressions, including, whether an expression has a coherent meaning or not (1a), whether expressions paraphrase each other, that is,
whether they are synonymous (1b), whether words are related by hyponymy
(1c), partonymy (1d), antonymy (1e), whether a word (fan) is ambiguous
(1f), and whether a word is used metaphorically (1g), as well as all the other
meaning relations we identified above. These abilities are strong evidence that
word meanings are real and not just figments of linguists’ imaginations.
(1)
a. Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
b. groundhog—woodchuck
c. lizard—reptile
d. elbow—arm
e. big—small; above—below; open—closed
f. That fan is very annoying.
g. I’ll dig with it.
Sentence (1g) is from Seamus Heaney’s poem “Digging.” In the minimal
context given in (1g), it would probably be understood as a spade or some
other such implement, and dig would be interpreted as turning over soil in
a garden or the like. However, in the larger context of the entire poem, it refers to a pen in the poet’s hand and dig must be interpreted metaphorically;
consequently, the sentence is ambiguous between a literal and a metaphorical meaning.
Examples such as those in (1) could easily be multiplied, but these few
should make clear a simple idea: linguistic competence includes an unconscious
knowledge of the literal meanings of words. While this conclusion might seem
trivial, it conceals several less-than-obvious points. First, it suggests that
252
Word Meaning
speakers carry around in their minds something like a dictionary of their
language. However, there is good evidence that speakers’ mental dictionaries
are quite different from the book dictionaries of a language. For instance,
no book dictionary will tell you that the words idea and sleep cannot literally
be combined as subject and predicate. (Linguists often use the terms lexicon
or mental lexicon to refer to this aspect of our linguistic competence and to
emphasize its difference from written dictionaries.) In fact, the nature of
the mental lexicon is still unclear; we will explore some of its characteristics
below.
Second, you should not confuse knowing the meaning of a word with
being able to give it a satisfactory definition. Definition‑stating is a learned
ability and is only marginally necessary in most communication; it is also far
beyond the normal capacities of people. The eminent lexicographer Sidney
Landau expresses the point simply (by “general definer,” he means one versed
in common, rather than technical, vocabulary):
It is difficult to find highly skilled general definers. Such people are about
as rare as good poets . . . there are probably fewer than a hundred experienced general definers in the whole of the United States. (Landau 1984:
235)
Exercise
Without consulting a dictionary, state the meaning(s) of the words below:
a. situation
b. pong (as in “ping-pong ball”)
c. if
d. of
e. vacillate
What problems did you run into? How did you solve them?
Third, whatever the nature of the mental lexicon, it clearly must show that
words are related to one another. To put it negatively, words are not just listed
in our competence, in alphabetical or any other simple order. Rather, they
are, as we have seen, interconnected in complex ways. These interconnections determine which words can and cannot occur together in grammatical
constructions—e.g., as in (1a). Interconnections relate families of words related by polysemy, synonymy, meronymy, antonymy, and other sense relations.
253
Delahunty and Garvey
Some models and explanations of word meaning
Since published dictionaries do not offer a very useful model of our lexical
competence, linguists have struggled to present more plausible ones. Besides
having to account for the observations noted above, they must also explain the
fact that, while the human brain is finite, an individual’s vocabulary may be
very large. Estimates for an educated person’s vocabulary run anywhere from
50,000 to 250,000 words. The largest unabridged dictionaries of English contain well over half a million entries. Clearly, however, no two individual speakers of a language have exactly the same vocabulary. If this is so, how can we
hope to describe the vastness and variability of lexical competence? A general
solution is to describe not the vocabulary of a single individual or the entire
word‑hoard of English, but instead to envisage the general properties according
to which the vocabulary of any individual—or of any language—can be constructed. There are two basic models of lexical structure, the network model
and the componential model.
The network model
The network model (N‑model) posits that semantic competence is to be explained on the assumption that words have certain primitive semantic relations with each other. In other words, our semantic competence does not
consist of knowing definitions at all, but rather of knowing how words relate
to each other. You may recall from your literary theory classes that this is close
to the Saussurean/structuralist approach. The primitive relations most commonly explored in the N‑model are the ones we’ve been discussing and are
listed and exemplified again in Table 1.
relationship characteristics
Synonymy
extensive overlap
in meaning
Antonymy
oppositeness of meaning
along related dimensions
Hyponymy
meaning inclusion
Partonymy/Meronymy part‑whole relationship
Metonymy
co-elements in a situation
Metaphor
similarity
examples
large/big
chase/pursue
large/small
strong/weak
rose/flower
keyboard/laptop
writer/book
foot of person/
foot of bed
table 1. lexical relations recognized in the network‑model
Although there are many other lexical relations, these are the most fre254
Word Meaning
quently mentioned in the network literature. For further elaboration, see
Cruse (1986, 2001).
The network model characterizes not just the semantic relations among
separate words, it can also describe the relationships between the senses of
individual words. For instance, if you look up the noun order in a dictionary,
you will find its meanings broken down by numerals and letters to include
such different notions as: 1. a condition of arrangement, 2. customary procedure, 3. something requested for purchase, 4. a monastic group, etc. Each one
of these senses enters into different network relations with the senses of other
words. For instance, sense 1 of order would be an antonym of one sense of disorder; sense 3 might refer to a whole of which the word entrée (in a restaurant)
represents a part.
Exercise
1. Using the N model, indicate how each of the following word pairs are
related. Write down any difficulties you have in coming to a decision.
a. forward—backward
b. casual—formal
c. car—wheels d. car—passenger
e. journey (verb)—travel (verb)
f. week—semester
g. freshman—sophomore
h. turkey (fowl)—turkey (undesirable person)
i. brain (body part)—brain (very intelligent person)
2. Using the N model, indicate the semantic relations among the words
in each of the groups below. To simplify your work, write the group of
words in a circle and draw lines between related words; label each line
with one of the network relations. Later, redraw your diagram to show
relations clearly.
a. car, truck, locomotive, wheels, trunk, hood, horn, vehicle
b. delay, linger, loiter, procrastinate, hasten, hurry, stampede (all
as intransitive verbs)
c. selfish, egocentric, altruistic, giving
The componential model
The componential model (C‑model) is based on the premise that word
255
Delahunty and Garvey
meanings are complex and can be viewed as composed of basic, indivisible
units of meaning. These units are usually called components, though sometimes you will see them referred to as features or sememes. Components
are often regarded as pure concepts, not to be equated with the words of any
language, which is why they are typically written in capital letters. From
this point of view, a word is essentially a shorthand way of grouping a set of
concepts under a single label. Some of the concepts that have been proposed
by various linguists as components are listed in Table 2.
animate (alive)
curved horizontal intention
married
place
vertical
become female
human
know
not
self
young
cause flat ingest
male
old
size
table 2. some proposed universal semantic components
The components listed in Table 2 are just a sample of those that have
been proposed, but they are adequate to illustrate the thrust of the C‑model.
For instance, in this model the word alive is shorthand for the component
ANIMATE; dead is shorthand for NOT, ALIVE; die for BECOME, NOT,
ALIVE. Kill adds the component CAUSE, and suicide adds SELF. (The components are independent of the parts of speech of the words to which they
apply.)
You might object that such definitions are grossly oversimplified. A valid
concern. At the very least, how the components are related to each other is
a very important aspect of word meaning. Simplistically adding the components BECOME, NOT, and ALIVE together does not adequately define die.
These issues raise technicalities which need not detain us here. For ways to
deal with them you might read work on this topic, e.g., Ch. 2 of Jackendoff
(1995).
It is important to distinguish between the universality of the list of components and their language specific uses. The features mentioned in Table 2
are quite likely to be universal, that is, having the potential to be used in the
creation of word meanings in any and all languages. While there may be components that are specific to individual languages, there are linguists who claim
to have identified a universal set of semantic primitives. (Anna Wierzbicka
probably makes the strongest claim in that regard—see Wierzbicka 1992, for
256
Word Meaning
example; Goddard 1998 is an accessible introduction to that style of doing
semantics.)
While linguists may claim that the sets of primitives they propose are universal, no one claims that they are bundled together in the same way in all
languages. For instance, while both English and French both make use of the
component FEMALE, they use it in different ways. Both languages indicate
the female member of certain pairs of words morphologically: lion, lioness;
lion, lionne. However, the two languages differ in that French has separate
(though related) words for MALE and FEMALE cousins (cousin, cousine);
English does not. (For an amusing compilation of words with remarkable
meanings see de Boinod 2006.)
We must also distinguish between central and more marginal aspects of a
word’s meaning. You might argue that cannibal suggests primitiveness, warfare, initiation, or absorption of the characteristics of the person devoured.
However, these are not essential components of the meaning of cannibal; a
cannibal is still a cannibal even if he is a highly educated rugby player. The
marginal aspects of the meaning of cannibal can be regarded as its connotations. The connotations of words are often variable across speakers of a
language and typically express emotional associations. Different words that
may be used for the same things may convey different feelings about them;
for example, woman and lady may refer to the same entities, but they convey
rather different attitudes toward them.
Exercise
1. (a) Using the components in Table 2 and any others you might need,
identify the meaning components shared by the words in each of the
sets below and the components that distinguish the members of the
sets:
a. ram, ewe, lamb
b. boar, sow, piglet
c. stag, doe, fawn
d. bull, cow, calf
e. stallion, mare, foal, filly, colt
f. man, woman, child, girl, boy
(b) Using the components you identified, characterize the meanings of
ewe, fawn, man, filly.
2. Examine the words below. Which of the components from Table 2
might the words represent? For each word, identify one component not
257
Delahunty and Garvey
in Table 2.
a. bachelor
b.spinster
c. teach
d.skyscraper
e.table
f. thicken
3. Identify words whose meanings are represented by the following
combinations of components. If no such word exists in English, indicate
that fact. If you know a language besides English, identify words in that
language that correspond to the set of components.
a. YOUNG + HUMAN + FEMALE
b.YOUNG + HUMAN + MALE
c. YOUNG + NOT + HUMAN
d.YOUNG + NOT + HUMAN + EQUINE
e.YOUNG + NOT + HUMAN + FELINE
f. NOT + HUMAN + MALE + EQUINE
g. NOT + HUMAN + FEMALE + EQUINE
h.CAUSE + NOT + INGEST
4. Examine your answers to Exercises (1), (2), and (3). What technical
problems arose in applying the C model? Consider the use of NOT.
5. Examine your analyses in Exercises (1) and (2). Do you see any cultural bias in your analysis or in the C model in general? If so, what is
that bias? How would you go about correcting it within the framework
of the C model?
6. Describe the connotational differences among the members of the
following sets of words:
a. violin—fiddle
b.careful—scrupulous
c. curious—inquisitive—nosey
d.politician—statesman
e.thin—slender—skinny
So, how effectively does the C‑model account for lexical competence? Actually, reasonably well (though we would have to specify how the components
258
Word Meaning
can be combined). First, we can use it to explain why some sentences are
semantically anomalous. For example, in sentence (1), Colorless green ideas
sleep furiously, the head of the subject, ideas, has the components NOT +
ANIMATE; in contrast, the predicate sleep requires that its subject have the
component ANIMATE. This shows that in addition to using components to
define individual words, we can also use them to specify how words can combine with each other. Such specifications are called selectional restrictions;
they identify the semantic (literal) limitations on the components of words
put together in close grammatical relationships such as subject and predicate,
verb and object, head and modifier, etc. Semantic anomaly, in short, will result when selectional restrictions are violated.
Sense relations also can be described in terms of components. Words
are synonymous to the extent that they share components. In simple cases,
antonyms share all components except perhaps just one; e.g., alive and dead
share the component ANIMATE, although the latter also has the component
NOT.
Finally, lexical ambiguity is represented in the C‑model by assigning to the
same word two different sets of components. Polysemy is explained as having at least one common component and at least one different component.
So the various senses of mouth will share the component of OPENING and
will be distinguished by such components as ANIMATE, SIZE, FLAT, and
CURVED.
Exercise
1. Explain the following semantic oddities by noting the selectional
restrictions that the sentence violates. Do not hesitate to use components beyond those mentioned in Table 2.
a. ?Monica elapsed.
b. ?John accidentally resembled his sister.
c. ?I lost my dog a grief ago.
2. For each pair of words, indicate which components they share and
which components distinguish them. (Again, use components beyond
those noted in Table 2 as you need to.)
a. car—automobile
b. chase—follow
c. huge—humongous
d. building—skyscraper
259
Delahunty and Garvey
3. Write down as many meanings as you can think of for each of the
following words. (Do not use a dictionary.) Which of the meanings are
related and which are not? How can you show this difference using semantic components?
a. ring
b. order (noun)
c. order (verb)
d. of
Relationships between the N‑model and the C‑model
Which of the two approaches to word meaning is better? One might favor
the N‑model on the grounds that, when asked for the meaning of a word,
people tend to provide synonyms rather than fully specified definitions.
As we saw, the ability to state adequate definitions is beyond the capabilities of most speakers; recall Landau’s remark above. Psycholinguistic
experiments likewise favor the N‑model as the more natural. (See Aitchison 2003: Chapters 7 and 8.) On the other hand, there seems to be some
overlap between the two approaches: synonymy and antonymy, at least,
suggest that two (or more) words share aspects of meaning.
Both approaches fall short of fully describing the meanings of words.
The fact that synonyms are rarely if ever perfect poses a challenge to the Nmodel, and subtle meaning differences require positing ever more semantic
components. We encourage you to review work by Goddard, Jackendoff,
and Wierzbicka for ways to deal with such problems.
Some problems might be addressed by combining the two approaches, in
the form of a “componentially‑augmented network,” which would draw on
the strengths of both approaches. For example, such an augmented network
model might allow us to reduce repetition of components in the specifications of related words. For instance, the fact that anything with the component HUMAN is also ANIMATE is a major redundancy that might be
represented in people’s minds through a taxonomy of animate beings as
hyponyms of the superordinate category of animacy. Put diagrammatically:
260
Animate Entities
Human
Man
Woman
Nonhuman
Alligator
Armadillo
Word Meaning
So, a lower category inherits or includes the characteristics of all the categories above it on the tree. For example, woman is human and animate while
armadillo is nonhuman but animate by virtue of their relationships with
other words and by virtue of the meaning components associated with those
other words. Abbreviatory rules like these are called redundancy rules.
Exercise
1. The vast number of lexical items in any language makes it unlikely
that a small set of lexical relationships or components will suffice to
differentiate all words. For example, we know that high and deep have
a great deal of meaning in common—e.g., vertical measurement—but
nonetheless they are semantically distinct, as is shown by the anomalies in (a) and (b):
a. The river is 50 feet deep/*high.
b. The mountain is 14,000 feet *deep/high.
High and deep and their derivatives are thus not synonyms; the first indicates “measurement to the top”; the second denotes “measurement
to the bottom” from some vantage point (Room, 1981, p.62). However
unable speakers might be to articulate this difference, the consistency
of their semantic judgments in cases such as (a) and (b) indicate that
they do know the meanings of these items. Create sentence pairs like
(a) and (b) that clearly distinguish between the pairs of words below
(from Room) and on the basis of your sentences accurately characterize
the meaning differences between the word pairs.
a. astronomy—astrology
b. crime—offense
c. regret—remorse
2. Standard dictionaries (and style manuals) often attempt to distinguish the following word pairs.
a. infer—imply
b. include—comprise
Does your dictionary distinguish them? How? Does this distinction match
your use of these words and your observations about how they are used?
mental dictionaries
At this point we must ask whether the book dictionaries we are accustomed
to are accurate models of the dictionaries we have in our minds/brains,
which allow us to perform as speakers of a language. While we know a great
261
Delahunty and Garvey
deal about book dictionaries, the research on mind dictionaries is, in comparison, in its infancy.
First, do our mental dictionaries use the same strategy as book dictionaries to allow speedy access to words? Remember that this is accomplished in
book dictionaries by alphabetization. We probably have between 50,000
and 250,000 words tucked away in our minds, most of which we can access
fairly easily. We can recognize a word in about a fifth of a second (often even
before we have heard the entire word), consequently, searching such a large
data base requires that it be structured so as to allow rapid searching.
Second, do book dictionaries include all the information about individual
words and the relations they enter into that our mental dictionaries include?
We saw that hyponymy is the major relation used by book dictionaries to
define words. Psycholinguistic research shows that where a superordinate
term is well-established, it comes readily to mind in word-search errors and
in word-association tasks. However, this research also shows that co-hyponymy/coordination is the most important psychological bond among words.
In word-association tests, coordinates are very frequently elicited; in wordselection errors, the wrong word is far more likely to be a coordinate of the
intended word than otherwise (Aitchison 2003). How often do we say left
when we mean right, or up when we mean down?
Exercise
1. List the first five words that come to your mind upon hearing/reading
the word spoon. For each of these words determine its sense relation to
spoon, that is, whether it is a coordinate, superordinate, subordinate,
and so on. If one of your words bears a relationship to spoon that we
have not mentioned, try to articulate what that relationship might be.
What do these relationships tell you about your mental representation
of spoon?
2. Keep a list of lexical errors you make. Include both the intended
word and the one produced in error. After you’ve collected 20 or so,
identify the semantic relationships between the right and wrong words.
Why do you think you made those particular errors and not some possible others? What might these errors tell you about how your mental
dictionary is organized? There’s a large research literature on this topic. Look in your university library for items on slips of the tongue; look
at work on this topic by Victoria Fromkin.
262
Word Meaning
Book dictionaries are designed primarily to support reading and writing.
(What are thesauruses designed for?) Mental dictionaries evolved primarily
to support speaking and hearing. For instance, when we “have a word on the
tip of our tongue,” we have most of the word, just not all of it. Besides its
meaning, we are likely to have some but not all of its pronunciation, usually
a sort of skeleton that may include the number of syllables in it, where the
main stress falls, and perhaps its first and last syllables.
The syntactic information in our heads is far richer than the syntactic
information in even the most elaborate learner’s dictionary, and even in
the most comprehensive modern grammar. For example, native speaker
book dictionaries typically make a two-way distinction between transitive and intransitive verbs, that is, between those that do and those that
do not take a direct object. But some verbs take an indirect object as well
as a direct one (e.g., give), so the book dictionary fails to make a distinction among verbs that our mental ones make. In fact, even this three-way
distinction between intransitive, transitive, and bitransitive verbs barely
scratches the surface of what we know about the syntactic frames that
verbs fit into. Native speaker book dictionaries generally provide no more
information about the syntactic frame, or grammatical context, that specific words require. Learner’s dictionaries are often far more elaborate in
this respect. CIDE, for example, distinguishes among verbs that take an
object followed by an adjective or adjectival phrase (e.g., drive X crazy),
verbs that take an object followed by a noun or noun phrase (e.g., crown
her empress), and verbs that take an object followed by a noun or adjective
phrase (e.g., consider him incompetent/a quack), to mention but a few. (See
CIDE’s front matter discussion of its grammar labels.)
As we saw, book dictionaries make extensive use of hyponymy in their
definitions. Remember that saying that one word is a hyponym of another
is to say that the referents of the hyponym are a subset of the referents of
the superordinate word. Another, more contorted way to say this is to say
that the members of the category represented by the hyponym are a subset
of the members of the category represented by the superordinate word. At
this point we should take a closer look at how categories and words are
related and what it means to belong to a category.
We’ll make the simplest possible assumption about the relation between
words and categories: words name categories—of entities, events, qualities,
relationships, and the like. One version of the traditional school definition
of “noun” is “a noun is the name of a person, place, thing, or idea.” One
problem with this is that nouns (except proper nouns), like all other words,
name categories of persons, places, things, and ideas, not just individual
263
Delahunty and Garvey
ones: dog represents all dogs, not just Lassie or Snoopy.
Categories need discussion. Let’s imagine that we are in a context in
which we are talking about technical matters and that we are expected to be
technically correct. A simple example of such a context might be a discussion about plane figures in a geometry course. In such a context, when we
use the word square, we mean “a plane figure having four equal sides and
four right angles” (WNWD p. 1381), no more and no less. The elements
of the definition, “plane figure,” “four equal sides,” and “four right angles,”
are all necessary to define square and together they are sufficient for its
definition. When scholars try to define technical concepts they generally
try to define them in terms of necessary and sufficient conditions. If they
succeed, then, in principle it is possible to decide for any item whether it is
a representative of that category or not. Given our definition of square, we
can decide for anything whether it is a square or not. The world, however, is
not always as rigid as a geometry class.
Imagine now that we have been rescued from the math discussion, and
we go to a birthday party where there is a flat layer cake cut into squares.
If you noticed that the pieces did not meet the mathematical definition,
could you reasonably object that the pieces are not really squares? Anyone
objecting on those grounds wouldn’t deserve any cake. As far as we know,
there is no English word for the almost square pieces that a flat cake is cut
into, so until someone invents such a word and it is widely accepted, we
can use square and our audience will accommodate us. These kinds of accommodations lead to rampant polysemy in much of the vocabulary. So it
is important to remember that polysemy, accommodation, and context are
inextricably intertwined.
If words and categories were all defined in necessary and sufficient terms,
then categories could be kept clearly distinct, as squares and triangles are in
geometry. But if we can bend these definitions, or if we cannot provide necessary and sufficient definitions for categories, then the boundaries between
categories may get quite fuzzy. In fact, many natural categories are like this.
Where exactly does red end and orange begin? Where do animals end and
plants begin? We are unlikely to get unanimous agreement on the answers
to such questions. Indeed an article in the June 2008 issue of Scientific
American grapples with the problem of defining “species” (Zimmer 2008).
Nonetheless, in ordinary, non-technical company, we cut each other some
slack by not expecting words always to be used with technical rigidity. We
can also indicate when we are using words imprecisely by using hedges,
such as like or sort of; or we can indicate that we are being technically correct by including expressions such as technically—Technically, a phoneme is a
264
Word Meaning
contrastive sound unit.
Many dictionary definitions are expressed in terms of the function that
something serves. For example, wnwd defines hinge as “a joint or device
on which a door, gate, lid, etc. swings” (p. 664). Suppose that no one had
oiled the hinge in decades and it could no longer swing—is it still a hinge?
The answer is undoubtedly “yes,” just as a dog with no tail is still a dog.
These may be defective members of their categories, but they are still members. How much change must be endured before something is no longer
accepted as a member of its original category? What does a dog have to give
up before it is no longer a dog? Whatever the answer to that question is, it is
clear that we can adjust our assumptions about what it takes to be a member
of a category to accommodate defective members.
As we have seen, categories typically have many members, in fact, potentially indefinitely many. The more general categories have multiple subcategories, which in turn may have their own subcategories. However, some
category members are viewed as better members of the category than others.
For instance, chairs and sofas are regarded as better items of furniture than
refrigerators; robins and sparrows are better birds than penguins or ostriches; shirts and skirts are better pieces of clothing than shoes and socks. This
layering of category members extends even to things defined in terms of
necessary and sufficient conditions, such as prime numbers: for example, 3
is a better prime number than 23, even though both fit the technical definition. It has been argued that categories are structured around a central, most
typical member, or prototype, e.g., chairs in the case of furniture, robins in
the case of birds, and so on.
Exercise
Let’s try to determine the prototypical member of a category. As quickly as you can, write out a list of ten vegetables. Then compile the lists
of all the students in your class. Order the vegetable names according to their frequency in the lists. Then take the three most frequent
words and check how early they occur in the lists. Generally, the most
frequent ones will occur early. The earliest and most frequent word
probably represents the most prototypical member of the category of
vegetables.
Dictionaries indicate certain aspects of prototypicality in their definitions. For example, WNWD (p. 1051) describes a penguin as a “flightless
265
Delahunty and Garvey
bird.” This negative definition suggests that the typical bird is not flightless;
similarly, WNWD describes dodoes (p. 414) as having “rudimentary wings
useless for flying” and ostriches (p. 1007) as “having small, useless wings.”
Presumably prototypical wings are not useless for flying.
Book and mental dictionaries differ also in the amount of information
they provide about the collocational properties of words. For example, if
we leave fat, bacon, butter, or oil sitting around long enough it will become
rancid, e.g., rancid bacon; however, if we leave fruit, vegetables, or eggs sitting, they will become rotten, e.g., rotten apple. So rancid collocates with the
words for fatty or oily substances, and rotten collocates with words for fruits,
vegetables, and the like.
One word collocates with another if they occur together in phrases
more frequently than their meanings alone would predict. For example,
green collocates with envy, as in green with envy, far more frequently than
other color names, for example blue. Likewise, blue collocates with face,
as in blue in the face, far more frequently than other color names (except
perhaps for red, as in red in the face). So, collocations are relatively predictable co-occurrences of words in phrases. Mental dictionaries include far
richer collocational information than book dictionaries do.
The interpretation of a word may depend on what it collocates with. So
dirty means “unfair” when it collocates with fight, but “soiled” when it collocates with clothes, and is ambiguous with hands.
Exercise
1. What words collocate with sweet? How does the meaning of sweet
change as its collocates change?
2. Think of three other words besides dirty and sweet and their collocates, and describe how your words change meaning as their collocates
change.
We can look at collocation as largely a matter of field. When the polysemous word morphology collocates with words like derivational and inflectional, then we know we are in the field of linguistics and that it is to be
interpreted as denoting word-structure. Until recently linguists paid relatively little attention to collocation. But with the development of very large
computerized databases of spoken and written language (corpora) and the
programs to search them (concordancers), we can expect collocation to be266
Word Meaning
come an important area of research and to provide significant insights into
how words and larger expressions are organized in our minds. Book dictionaries are more and more dependent on such databases and will incorporate
more collocational information as time goes on.
Collocational expectation is a matter of degree. Some collocational restrictions are quite narrow, others are more liberal. Collocates may become
rigidly fixed, in which case they have calcified into idioms, expressions
whose meanings are not derivable from their words and syntax. Examples
include kick the bucket for “die,” tie the knot for “marry.” Note that these
expressions have both a literal and an idiomatic meaning. The audience has
to work out which meaning is intended in a particular context.
It should come as no surprise that because the psychological bonds between collocates may be very strong, words regularly elicit their collocates in
word association tests.
Dictionaries differ in how they treat idioms. Some may not include them
at all. WNTC and WNWD include idioms at the end of the entry for one
or more of the main words of the idiom (kick the bucket is listed under
bucket, though not under kick).
Exercise
1. What words collocate with blond, false, artificial, herd, flock, ream,
spick, husband, deal, bumper? Some of these words allow only one or
two collocates; the remainder allow for (far) more. Identify as many
collocates as each word allows, up to a maximum of five. Check a pair
of dictionaries, including a learner’s, to see if and how collocational
information is included.
2. Make a list of 10 idioms. What are their meanings? Can all of them
be taken both literally and idiomatically? Do your examples suggest any
connection between idiom and metaphor? Check a pair of dictionaries,
including a learner’s, to see if and how idioms are included. If you know
some people learning English as a second language, ask them if they
understand the idioms you have chosen.
c o n c l u d i n g re m a rk s
We saw in this chapter that dictionaries, especially larger, more comprehensive ones, provide enormous amounts of information about the words of a
language. We also saw that learner’s dictionaries tend to provide more infor267
Delahunty and Garvey
mation about the grammatical structures associated with individual words
than native-speaker dictionaries do. We investigated lexical relations such
as synonymy, antonymy, hyponymy, and the like, and the ways in which
dictionaries make use of these relations in their definitions. Our discussion
of mental dictionaries showed that they are not alphabetically organized,
and that for all the information contained in even the most comprehensive
dictionary, our mental dictionaries include even more information for each
entry. We discovered that word meanings tend to be fuzzy and prototypically organized.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Aitchison, Jean. 2003. Words in the Mind. 3rd ed. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. 2000. 4th ed. Boston, MA:
Houghton Mifflin Co.
Barthes, Roland. 1970. S/Z. Paris: Senil.
Cambridge International Dictionary of English. 1995. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Cruse, Alan. 1986. Lexical Semantics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
_____2001. The lexicon. In Mark Aronoff and Janie Rees-Miller. (eds.) The
Handbook of Linguistics. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Crystal, David. 2003. A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics. 5th ed. Oxford,
UK: Blackwell.
de Boinod, Adam J. 2006. The Meaning of Tingo and Other Extraordinary Words
from Around the World. London, UK: Penguin.
Goddard, Cliff. 1998. Semantic Analysis: A Practical Introduction. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
HarperCollins ESL Beginners Dictionary/Collins CoBuild New Student’s Dictionary.
2nd ed. 2003. New York: HarperCollins.
Jackendoff, Ray. 1995. Languages of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackson, Howard. 1988. Words and their Meanings. London, UK: Longman.
Johnson, Keith and Helen Johnson. (eds.). 1998. Encyclopedic Dictionary of
Applied Linguistics. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Lakoff, George and Mark Johnson. 1980. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Landau, Sidney. 1984. Dictionaries: The Art and Craft of Lexicography. New
York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.
Longman Dictionary of Contemporary English. 2005. 4th ed. Harlow, UK:
Longman.
Matthews, Peter. 1997. Concise Oxford Dictionary of Linguistics. Oxford, UK:
268
Word Meaning
Oxford University Press.
Morris, William and Mary Morris. (eds). 1985. Harper Dictionary of
Contemporary Usage. 2nd ed. New York: Harper Perennial
Random House Webster’s English Learner’s Dictionary. 1999. New York: Random
House.
Room, A. 1981. Dictionary of Distinguishables. Boston, MA: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Webster’s New World Dictionary of the American Language, 2nd College Edition.
1980. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Webster’s New Twentieth Century Dictionary Unabridged. 1983. 2nd ed. New
York: Simon and Schuster.
Wierzbicka, Anna. 1992. Semantics, Culture, and Cognition: Universal Human
Concepts in Culture-Specific Configurations. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Zimmer, Karl. 2008. What is a species? Scientific American Vol. 298, No. 6:
June 2008 pp. 72-79.
glossary
accommodation:
adjusting our linguistic expectations and practices to specific circumstances.
antonyms: words representing opposing values on some dimension. See complementary antonyms, conversive antonyms, gradable antonyms, nongradable antonyms, reversive antonyms.
co-hyponym: See coordinates.
combined entry: a dictionary entry that includes all the parts of speech to
which the entry word belongs.
complementary antonyms: pairs of words such that if one applies the other
cannot, e.g., alive/dead.
componential model (C‑model): model of lexical meaning that assumes that
word meanings are complex and can be viewed as composed of smaller units
of meaning called semantic components, semantic feature, and sememe.
concordancer: computer program for doing linguistic analysis on a corpus.
connotations: aspects of the meanings of words that indicate the speaker’s/
writer’s attitude toward the word’s referent(s).
conversive antonyms: words that represent a situation from different points
of view, e.g., if X is Y’s husband then Y is X’s wife.
coordinates: expressions that have a common hyponym, e.g., phonebook and
textbook are coordinates of book.
corpus/ora: computerized collections of texts designed to allow computerized linguistic analysis. See concordancer.
269
Delahunty and Garvey
cross-reference:
to another.
notation in dictionaries that directs readers from one entry
defective members:
members of a category that do not meet one or more of
the characteristics typical of members of the category, e.g., flightless birds.
entry: the block of text in a dictionary that includes the entry word or
headword and all the information associated with it.
entry word: the highlighted expression at the beginning of a dictionary entry about which the remainder of the entry provides linguistic information.
etymology: the information in a dictionary entry that describes the history
of the entry word.
gradable antonyms: words that represent opposing values on a continuous
dimension, e.g., tall/short.
headword: See entry word.
hedges: expressions that allow communicators to weaken their commitment to the truth of a claim, e.g., I believe that Darwin is correct.
homographs: two or more separate words with the same spelling but unrelated meanings. See homophones, homonyms.
homophones: two or more separate words with the same pronunciation but
unrelated meanings. See homographs, homonyms.
homonyms: two or more separate words with the same pronunciation or
spelling but with two or more unrelated meanings, e.g., date meaning type
of fruit and arrangement to meet. See homophone, homograph.
hypernym: see superordinate.
hyponym: the less inclusive word in hyponymy, e.g., scalpel is a hyponym of
surgical instrument because it is a kind of surgical instrument.
hyponymy: a sense relation between expressions such that the entities denoted by one expression are included among the entities denoted by another, e.g., teaspoon/spoon. The relationship can be paraphrased as X is a kind of
Y, thus a teaspoon is a kind of spoon. See coordinate, hypernym, hyponym,
superordinate.
co-hyponym: see coordinate.
idiom: expression whose meaning cannot be determined simply from the
meaning of its component words and their syntactic organization, e.g., the
proverbial meaning of Every cloud has a silver lining.
inflection: markers on words to indicate such grammatical information
as tense, person, and number, e.g., the {-s} suffix added to English verbs to
indicate third person, singular, present tense.
lexical field: set of expressions in a language having to do with concepts
in a single domain, e.g., the set of technical terms in linguistics.
ligature: a letter created by combining two or more characters, e.g., .
270
Word Meaning
marked: the member of a pair of related expressions that is more complex
semantically and/or formally than the other member, e.g., stallion is marked
in relation to horse because the former includes the meaning male, whereas
a horse may be either male or female. See unmarked.
mental dictionary: mental store of words and word-like expressions, including information on their phonological, morphological, syntactic, semantic, discourse, and pragmatic properties. Also called mental lexicon or
just lexicon.
meronymy: see partonymy.
metaphor: a figure of speech in which an expression that is typically used
to denote one thing is used to denote another thing similar in some way to
the first. Metaphor may be the basis for certain meaning extensions, e.g., the
foot of a mountain.
necessary conditions: the conditions that must be met for something to be
a member of a category, e.g., in geometry a triangle must be a plane figure,
must have three sides, and the ends of the three sides must meet to create
three angles. See sufficient conditions.
network model (N‑model): model or theory of word meanings that specifies the sense relations among words.
non-gradable antonyms: antonyms, typically adjectives, that typically do
not allow degree modification, e.g., clockwise in clockwise motion cannot be
modified by expressions such as very, cf. *very clockwise.
partonymy: a sense relation between expressions such that the entities denoted by one expression represent parts of the entity denoted by another,
e.g., blade/knife. The relationship can be paraphrased as X is a part of Y, thus
a blade is a part of a knife.
polysemy: situation in which one expression has two or more clearly related
meanings.
prototype: theory of categorization that posits that membership in categories is a matter of degree rather then of necessary and sufficient conditions
and that members of a category are ranked according to their degree of
similarity to the prototype or best example of the category.
redundancy rules: rules that aim to eliminate repetition of information
among words that are hyponymically related.
reversive antonyms: words that represent movement in opposite directions.
run-ons/ins: expressions related to the entry word that are included at the
end of a dictionary entry but are undefined because their interpretations are
deemed to be predictable from their forms.
selectional restrictions: semantic requirements that must be met for expressions to go together without anomaly in close grammatical relationships
271
Delahunty and Garvey
such as subject and predicate, verb and object, head and modifier, etc.
semantic component: basic, indivisible unit of linguistic meaning.
semantic feature: see semantic component.
semantic relations: see sense relations.
sememe: see semantic component.
senses: distinguishable meanings of expressions.
sense relations: relations based on the senses of expressions. See antonym,
hyponymy, metaphor, metonymy, partonymy, synonym.
sufficient conditions: the set of conditions such that if something meets
them, then that is enough to determine that it belongs to a category, e.g.,
if something is a plane geometrical figure and has three sides whose ends
meet to create three angles, then that is sufficient to classify that figure as a
triangle.
superordinate: the more inclusive expression in hyponymy, e.g., chair is
superordinate to armchair because an armchair is a kind of chair.
syllabication: indications in the spelling of an entry word (usually raised
dots) of where the word may be divided at the end of a line of type; also,
indications in the pronunciation of an entry word of where the word divides
into spoken syllables.
syllabification: indications in the pronunciation of an entry word of where
the word divides into spoken syllables.
synonymy: a sense relation in which two or more expressions have the
same meaning.
syntactic frame: a representation of the syntactic context(s) into which
an expression may be inserted, e.g., a transitive verb must occur in a verb
phrase that contains a direct object.
unmarked: the member of a pair of related expressions that is less complex
semantically or formally than the other member, e.g., horse is unmarked in
relation to stallion because the former includes no information about the
animal’s sex whereas the latter includes the meaning male. See marked.
usage: from a descriptive point of view, the ways in which expressions in a
language are actually used in discourses; from a prescriptive point of view,
the ways in which commentators claim expressions ought (or more typically,
ought not) to be used in discourses.
usage labels: expressions in dictionary entries designed to inform users
about the entry word’s usage.
usage note: short, critical essays appended to a dictionary entry when the
usage of the entry word is particularly controversial.
word history: short essay appended to a dictionary entry when the history
of the entry word is particularly noteworthy.
272
9 Phrases
key concepts
Definition of phrase
Modification and complementation
Adverb phrases
Prepositional phrases
Adjective phrases
Noun phrases
Verb phrases
i n t ro d u c t i o n
No doubt you have noticed that our discussion of parts of speech required
us to consider the phrases they occurred in. Although traditional grammars
often treat word classes apart from their roles in larger structures, it is really
not possible to do so. For one thing, we cannot study a word’s functions
without viewing it in a larger setting. For another, a single word may constitute a phrase. For instance, a noun phrase may contain just a noun—its
head. Likewise, a verb phrase may contain just a verb. Phrases, then, are
units of one or more words. They are the lowest syntactic unit.
It is important for us to know about phrases and to be able to distinguish
them from words and clauses. This knowledge is essential in at least the two
following situations.
Journeyman writers often produce fragments, that is, parts of sentences
punctuated as if they were sentences. These fragments are rarely just random
strings of words; rather, they are typically internally grammatical. They are
in fact phrases. Fragments are objected to because they are not the type of
expression that more experienced writers would use in the context. They are
often a reflection of linguistic patterns used in speech and indicate that the
writer has not yet mastered the stylistic differences between the spoken and
written modes.
Languages differ in the orders they impose on sequences of words. For
example, in English (and many other languages), adjectives typically precede the nouns they modify, whereas in Spanish (and many other languages), adjectives typically follow the nouns they modify. Language learners
must learn the orders expected in the target language. Their teachers must
know the ordering possibilities and be able to articulate them in ways their
students can learn from.
As we examine phrases, then, we study how words relate to each other
in the smallest of the larger linguistic structures. In our chapters on Basic
273
Delahunty and Garvey
Clause Patterns and Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns, we examine the
ways in which phrases form clauses. Our discussion here will treat the five
major phrase types in English:
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
Adverb Phrase (AdvP)
Prepositional Phrase (PP)
Adjective Phrase (AP)
Noun Phrase (NP)
Verb Phrase (VP)
We will discuss each of the five types in a similar way. First, we will examine their basic functional patterns; then how those functions are realized by
structural possibilities; and, where appropriate, we will explore some of the
complexities associated with each type of phrase. Whenever such complexities lead us to topics considered in another chapter, we will provide a brief
commentary and defer fuller treatment to a later time.
what is a phrase?
Traditionally “phrase” is defined as “a group of words that does not contain
a verb and its subject and is used as a single part of speech.”
This definition entails three characteristics: (1) it specifies that only a
group of words can constitute a phrase, implying that a single word cannot;
(2) it distinguishes phrases from clauses; and (3) it requires that the groups
of words believed to be a phrase constitute a single grammatical unit. We
accept (2) and (3), but must revise (1).
We reject the claim that single words cannot constitute phrases. First, a
word and a phrase may play identical grammatical roles in a clause, as (1)
and (2) demonstrate:
(1) Most of the members of the genus avis fly.
(2) Birds fly.
Most of the members of the genus avis is the subject of (1) and birds is the subject of (2), showing that single words and phrases can function identically
in clauses. There are two inferences that we can draw from this fact: (a) a
subject can consist of either a single noun or a noun phrase, or (b) subjects
are phrases, and so whatever functions as a subject must be a phrase. If we
assume (a), then whenever we define subject (and any other grammatical
function, such as predicate, direct object, indirect object, etc.), we must
always specify that it can be expressed as a word or as a phrase. Linguists
274
Phrases
would say that this formulation is more complex than it needs to be because
it fails to articulate a more general pattern. The broader generalization is that
these grammatical relations are always expressed as phrases and phrases can
consist of either a single word or a unified group of words. Below, we will
show how and when words can be phrases.
Second, single words and phrases may be replaced by identical proforms.
We can replace the subjects of both (1) and (2) with They:
(1) a. They fly.
(2) a. They fly.
Again, there are two inferences we can draw: (a) pronouns can replace either a noun or a noun phrase, or (b) pronouns replace phrases. Again, (b)
is more general, but it does require us to specify when words can function
as phrases.
A single word may be a phrase when it is the head of that phrase. The
head of a phrase is the phrase’s central element; any other words (or phrases)
in the phrase orient to it, either by modifying it or complementing it. The
head determines the phrase’s grammatical category: if the head is a noun,
the phrase is a noun phrase; if the head is a verb, the phrase is a verb phrase,
and so on. The head can also determine the internal grammar of the phrase:
if the head is a noun, then it may be modified by an article; if the head is
a transitive verb, it must be complemented by a direct object. Heads also
determine such things as the number of their phrases: if the head of an NP
is singular, then the NP is singular; if the head is plural, then the NP is
plural. Crucially, the head of a phrase may occur alone in the phrase, that is,
without modification or complementation.
Let’s look a little closer at what expressions may be replaced by pronouns.
Specifically, let’s test the claim made in many textbooks that pronouns can
replace nouns or noun phrases. Consider (3):
(3) Fooster hates the cabbage.
NP
Det
N
the
cabbage
If we replace the NP the cabbage in (3) with the pronoun it we get the perfectly grammatical (3a):
275
Delahunty and Garvey
(3) a. Fooster hates it.
NP
Pron
it
However, given the typical textbook definition of pronoun as a word that
can replace either nouns or noun phrases, we should be able to replace just
the noun cabbage in (3) with it. However, when we do so, we create the
wildly ungrammatical (3b):
(3) b. *Fooster hates the it.
So, why is (3a) fine but (3b) is not? To create (3a) we replaced the entire
phrase the cabbage, but for (3b) we replaced only a part of the phrase. It appears that when we pronominalize we must replace an entire phrase with a
pronoun, not just a random piece of it. It follows that if we can successfully
replace an expression with a pronoun, then that expression must be a complete phrase. To check this, consider what happens when we replace cabbage
in (3c) with a pronoun; we get the grammatical (3d):
(3) c. Fooster hates cabbage.
(3) d. Fooster hates it.
NP
N
cabbage
So cabbage is just a noun in (3) and therefore cannot be replaced by a pronoun; but in (3c) it is both a noun and a noun phrase (as the diagram
shows), and so can be pronominalized, proved by the fact that (3d) is grammatical.
Let’s add just one more test to the two tests for phrasehood we’ve already
used (capable of functioning as a grammatical relation and capable of being
replaced by a pronoun): if an expression can be moved from one part of a
sentence to another without any internal reorganization, then that expression is a phrase. We can use our cabbage sentences for this test too.
We can successfully move the cabbage in (3) to the left of the subject, giving us:
(3) e. The cabbage, Fooster hates.
276
Phrases
But when we try to move just the N cabbage, the result is ungrammatical,
just as when we tried to pronominalize cabbage in (3):
(3) f. *Cabbage, Fooster hates the.
Analogously, when we move cabbage in (3c) in which cabbage occurs
alone, the result is also grammatical:
(3) g. Cabbage, Fooster hates.
So, we’ve applied three tests—ability to function as a grammatical relation, pronominalization, and movement—and all three have yielded the
same results: a phrase may consist of a unified group of words, or of a single
word as long as that word is the phrase’s head.
There is an important methodological precept here: the more arguments
you can marshal in favor of your analysis and definitions, the more confidence you can place in them.
Our new, improved definition of “phrase”: a phrase is a grammatical unit,
intermediate between a word and a clause, which may consist of just one word
(its head) or its head and expressions (including other phrases) that modify or
complement it (see below). This definition retains the traditional distinctions between word and phrase and between phrase and clause. It adds the
requirement that phrases have heads and allows a phrase to consist of just its
head.
In considering word classes, we examined the most important ones first.
In this chapter, we will present the three less complex types first— adverb,
prepositional, and adjective. The reason for this seemingly backwards approach is that the two major phrase types—noun phrases and verb phrases—often include the minor types as subparts. But first we must make a
brief detour to discuss the important distinction between modification and
complementation.
modification and complementation
The head of a phrase may be modified or complemented by other words,
phrases, or sentences within the phrase. We begin with complementation as
it is perhaps the more easily understood.
When one element in an expression creates the grammatical expectation
that another expression will also occur, the expected element complements
the expecting element. For example, transitive verbs create the expectation
of an object, as in Sheila fractured [her ankle]; bitransitive verbs create the
277
Delahunty and Garvey
expectation of two objects, as in Sally gave [her] [a shot of morphine]; certain
other verbs create the expectation of two complements, though one or both
need not be an NP, as in She put [her first aid kit] [away/in the truck]. Generally, although verbs (in English) require a subject, subjects are not usually
said to complement the verb.
Verbs are the primary complement-requiring elements in language, but
other parts of speech may require complements too. Prepositions typically
require an NP complement—on may be complemented by a phrase denoting notions such as location or time, as in on [the pavement], on [your mark],
on [time]. Certain nouns may be complemented by clauses, as in the belief
[that diseases are caused by evil spirits].
Modification occurs in a construction in which an expression is accompanied by an element not grammatically required by it. For example, because nouns do not typically require adjectives, eager modifies fans in eager
fans. Verbs and adjectives do not typically require that they be accompanied
by adverbials, so violently modifies swore in swore violently, and disappointingly modifies slow in disappointingly slow.
Modification may be restrictive or non-restrictive. When one word restrictively modifies another, the modifier restricts the potential reference of
the modified. For example, in the phrase long books the adjective long restrictively modifies the noun books. If the word books were to occur alone,
then it could potentially refer to any and all types of books. The modifier
restricts the reference of the phrase to just those books that are long. Nouns
may have many modifiers, as in tall, black, neutered, male, domestic, shorthaired cat. Here we have six modifiers, each restricting the potential reference of the word cat. The result of piling up these modifiers is that the actual
referent of the phrase must satisfy all of them—it must be a cat that is tall,
black, neutered, male, domestic, and short-haired. Each modifier acts like a
criterion that the ultimate referent(s) of the phrase must satisfy.
There are two main classes of modifying words in English—adjectives
and adverbs. Adjectives modify nouns and adverbs modify pretty much everything else—verbs, adjectives, other adverbs, and sentences. They modify
these in much the same way as adjectives modify nouns—by adding criteria
that must be met. For example, in ran quickly, quickly modifies ran and
therefore requires that whoever ran didn’t run in any old way, but did it
quickly. Other examples include expressions like take regularly, needs help
immediately. Likewise, intensely in intensely bright requires that the brightness be intense (cf. specially packaged, medically appropriate). Irritatingly in
irritatingly slowly requires that whatever is going on must not only be going
on slowly, but so slowly as to be irritating to someone (cf. extremely cleverly).
278
Phrases
Unfortunately in Unfortunately, he didn’t make it back requires not only that
he didn’t make it back, but also that (the speaker feels that) it is unfortunate
that he didn’t (cf. Sadly, she’s no longer with us, Hopefully, it won’t happen
again).
Nouns may be restrictively modified by clauses, called relative, adjective,
or defining clauses, bolded in the man who knew too much. Notice that
there is no comma between the noun man and the beginning of the restrictive relative clause. Sentences may be restrictively modified by adverbial
clauses, bolded in Though he liked her a lot, he was afraid to ask her for a
date. Here a comma is preferred, especially if the adverbial clause is relatively
long.
Notice that none of the modifiers are required or implied by the words,
phrases, or sentences they modify. These words, phrases, and sentences
would be grammatically complete without the modifiers—though of course
adding or removing modifiers affects the meaning and potential referents of
the modified elements.
Non-restrictive modifiers, or appositives, add information that is not
essential for the identification of the referent of the phrase so modified.
In written English, appositives are set off by commas—The President of the
US, who is in his 7th year in office, has only one more year to serve. In cases
like this, the writer assumes that the reader will know who the President of
the US is and so does not need the appositive information to identify him.
Nonetheless, the writer adds the information that the President is in his
7th year in office as a sort of secondary predicate in addition to the primary
one, namely, that he has only one more year to serve. In spoken English,
appositives are set off from the remainder of the sentence by brief pauses
(hence the commas) and a drop in pitch. From a writer’s or speaker’s point
of view, it is essential to decide whether the audience does or does not need
the modifier to identify the referent of the phrase.
t h e a d ve r b p h r a s e
(advp)
The following are examples of adverb phrases:
(4) a. adamantly (adverb alone)
b. quite reluctantly (adverb modified by intensifier)
c. extremely clumsily (adverb modified by degree adverb)
From a functional point of view, each AdvP must contain a head, which
must be an adverb; this adverb may be modified by an intensifier, as in
(4b), or by a degree adverb, as in (4c). Examples of these are listed in Table 1.
279
Delahunty and Garvey
very
quite
rather
too
more/most
only
somewhat
extraordinarily
reasonably
particularly
extremely
terrifically
table 1: typical intensifiers and degree adverbs
The following formula encapsulates the functional properties of AdvPs:
(5) (Modifier) + Head [In formulae like this, parentheses indicate
optional elements.]
The structures associated with (4a), (4b), and (4c) may be represented by
the following trees:
(6)a.
AdvP
Adv
(6)b.
adamantly
AdvP
Int
quite
(6)c.
AdvP
Adv
Adv
extremely
clumsily
Adv
reluctantly
As we noted for single adverbs (i.e., adverb phrases composed of just a head
adverb), adverb phrases are relatively movable within a sentence, although the
changes in position may be accompanied by changes in meaning, for example:
(7)
(8)
280
a.
b.
c.
a.
b.
Frankly, my dear, I don’t give a damn.
My dear, I frankly don’t give a damn.
My dear, I don’t give a damn, frankly.
Luckily, his fall was broken by deep snow.
His fall was broken by deep snow, luckily.
Phrases
Exercise
1. For each of the following AdvPs identify its head adverb. If it has a
modifier, identify that and determine its part of speech.
a. quickly
b. very quickly
c. particularly extravagantly
2. Draw brackets around each adverb phrase in the sentences below.
Then underline the head adverb.
a. They surrendered peacefully.
b. I go to the movies quite frequently.
c. Esmeralda acted awfully strangely.
d. Very slowly, we edged down the mountain.
e. Somewhat reluctantly, she returned home a week early.
3. Draw a tree diagram like those in (6) above for each of the AdvPs you
identified in the sentences in Exercise (1) above.
Before we move on, we want to broach the topic of how phrase structure
trees are created. They are said to be “generated” by phrase structure rules
(PSRs) such as:
(9) AdvP –—> (Int) Adv
This is to be read as: An adverb phrase (AdvP) consists of (——>) an optional intensifier followed by an adverb.
As we know, however, adverbs may be modified by either an intensifier
or another adverb, for example, extremely quickly. We represent this choice
by placing the items to be selected from in curly brackets: {Int/Adv}. If all
these elements are optional, then the curly brackets are put in parentheses:
({ }). So, a more complete PSR for AdvPs would be:
(10) AdvP –—> ({Int/Adv}) Adv
We read this as: an AdvP consists of an optional intensifier or adverb and
an adverb.
281
Delahunty and Garvey
A note on “Adverbial”
The term “adverbial” refers to adverb phrases and all other expression types
that function in the ways that adverb phrases do, namely, as modifiers of
almost all parts of speech except nouns. Besides adverb phrases, prepositional phrases (bolded), e.g., She drove with great caution (cf. She drove
cautiously), noun phrases (bolded), e.g., They do that a lot, (cf. They do that
frequently), and deictic words (bolded), e.g., There’s nobody here may function as adverbials.
t h e p re p o s i t i o n a l p h r a s e
(pp)
The following are typical prepositional phrases:
(11) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
on the waterfront
of human bondage
beyond the blue horizon
from the halls of Montezuma
with malice toward none
From a functional point of view, PPs are very simple: they consist of a head
preposition and an object or complement, which is typically an NP. We can
represent this as:
(12) Head + Object
From a structural point of view, each of the PPs in (11) consists of a preposition followed by a noun phrase, and we can represent their basic structure as:
(13)
PP
P
NP
This phrase structure tree is generated by the following PSR:
(14) PP ——> P NP
We read this PSR as: a PP consists of a P followed by an NP. Noun phrases
are discussed in more detail later in this chapter. All you need to know now
is the list of single- and multi-word prepositions presented in the chapter on
Minor Parts of Speech.
282
Phrases
Prepositional phrases are relatively uniform constructions: spot a preposition and the NP that immediately follows it, and you can be fairly certain
that you have identified a PP. However, you should recall that some apparent
prepositions are actually particles and that others may be subordinating adverbial conjunctions.
Exercise
Draw a tree diagram for each of the phrases (11a-c).
In (11d,e) we find two PPs, one inside the other. You can visually represent
(11e) as:
(15)
P
with
PP
NP
N
malice
PP
P
toward
NP
Pron
none
It may seem odd to treat a preposition as the head of a phrase, because
traditional grammar may have persuaded us to regard the preposition as insignificant. In fact, prepositions express meanings that encompass the entire
range of key semantic relations in a sentence. Another sign of the importance
of prepositional phrases is their ability to appear in so many structures—within noun phrases, verb phrases, and adjective phrases.
The second part of the PP is a noun phrase that functions as its complement or object. This terminology also suggests the central role of the preposition within its phrase. Just as verbs may govern direct and indirect object NPs,
prepositions govern object NPs.
283
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. Here are several prepositional phrases. For each, identify its head P
and NP complement/object:
a. on the ropes
b. under the boardwalk
c. on a slippery slope
d. around midnight
e. beyond the horizon
2. In the sentences below, draw brackets around the prepositional
phrases. Circle each preposition and underline its NP object. Be sure to
note where PPs contain other (embedded) PPs. Can prepositions take
objects that are not NPs?
a. I put the dynamite in a safe place.
b. In Warden’s house, smoking is not allowed.
c. I thank you from the bottom of my heart.
d. Hilda peeked from behind a tree.
e. After all of his warnings about the dangers of cigarettes, Benjy
consumed a cut of meat with a huge amount of cholesterol.
f. Oscar resigned in the face of increasing evidence of his association with disreputable companies.
3. Using the movement test, show that the italicized sequences are
phrases:
a. In times of danger, everyone must rally behind the leader.
b. At the end of the day, he is always very tired.
c. It is easy to identify phrases with examples like these.
4. From the discussion above, identify the ideas that show how a preposition is the head of its phrase. Consider also how the following sentences add further support for this claim:
a. Sheila hit the ball almost into the parking lot.
b. Werner spilled oil all over his new jacket.
Try to think of other sentences similar to these. How do they call for a
revision of our formal and functional formulas for PPs?
Prepositions are often simply characterized as linking words, and this
is an accurate characterization as far as it goes. However, we’d like to have
a more complete concept of how they work. Typically prepositions have
284
Phrases
meanings and these meanings connect their objects to other parts of the
sentences in which they occur. For example, in (16), to indicates that its
object NP represents the recipient of the money:
(16) Tony donated $10,000 to the hospital.
In (17), for indicates that the cardiac laboratory is to be the beneficiary of
the money:
(17) The money was for the cardiac laboratory.
Notions such as recipient and beneficiary are called semantic roles,
about which we will have much more to say in our chapter on Basic Clause
Patterns.
t h e a d j e c t i ve p h r a s e
(ap)
Each of the following is an AP:
(18) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
f.
Impertinent (adjective alone)
Very impertinent (intensifier + adjective)
Unaware of any wrongdoing (adjective + PP)
Disappointed that someone had lied (adjective + clause)
Afraid to make a move (adjective + infinitival)
Fully aware of his surroundings (adverb + adjective + PP)
From a functional perspective, adjective phrases may be analyzed as:
(19) (Modifier) + Head + (Complement)
The modifiers may be either intensifiers or degree adverbs, just as in AdvPs;
the complements may be PPs, finite clauses, or infinitivals. Only some types
of adjectives take complements—mainly those that denote mental or emotional states, e.g., aware, afraid, sorry, disappointed, astonished, hopeful, sad.
We can represent the structures of (18a-f ) as the following trees, respectively:
285
Delahunty and Garvey
(18)a’.
AP
(18)b’.
A
Int
AP
(18)c’.
A
A
AP
PP
impertinent
very impertinent
unaware
of any
wrongdoing
(18)d’. AP
(18)e’.
AP
A
S
A
Inf disappointed that someone had lied
afraid
to make a
move
(18)f ’. AP
AdvP
A
fully
aware
PP
of his surroundings
These trees can be generated by the following PSR:
(20) AP ——> ({Int/AdvP}) A ({PP/S/Inf })
We read this as: an adjective phrase consists of an optional intensifier or adverb phrase, an adjective, and an optional PP, S, or infinitival.
Exercise
1. Each of the following is an adjective phrase. Identify its head adjective, any modifiers, and any complements. For each modifier and
complement you find, indicate its part of speech.
a. sad
b. quite attractive
c. extremely volatile
d. disappointed that he has to leave
e. eager to get on with his life
2. In the following clauses, draw brackets around each AP, underline
the head adjective, and identify any modifiers and/or complements
and provide their parts of speech. Don’t forget to use formal criteria to
286
Phrases
check that the word you underline actually is an adjective.
a. The undernourished animals recovered.
b. My boss is happy with my work.
c. Mindy was completely unaware of his ability to dance.
d. The reasons for his sudden resignation eluded even the most astute observers in the company.
e. Afraid of real combat, George bought a large squirtgun.
f. George is extremely generous to his wealthy friends.
3. For each of the following adjectives, create at least three APs:
aware, afraid, sorry, disappointed, astonished, hopeful, sad. In the
APs you create, include at least one PP complement, one finite clause
complement, and one infinitival complement.
4. Draw a tree diagram with full detail (i.e., include part of speech
labels for each word and internal phrase) for the expression very sure
of himself.
APs have three main functions. First, they may directly modify nouns. In
this function they are often called attributive adjectives, as in friendly dogs.
Second, APs may complement subject NPs. In this function they are referred to as predicative or subject complements. Predicate adjectives occur
after verbs of the be-become-seem type.
(21) a. Faust is anxious.
b. Mephistopheles became despondent.
c. Wagner seems puzzled.
Third, an AP may function as an object complement, that is, as the
complement of the object of a clause:
(22) a. We consider him foolish.
b. Your attitude makes me angry.
Adjectival object complements are particularly common in certain set
phrases, such as make X clear. Table 2 contains a sample of such set phrases.
cut X short
drain X dry
pack X tight
push X open
287
Delahunty and Garvey
keep X loose
leave X clean
make X plain
wash X clean
put X straight
set X right
shake X free
work X loose
table 2: adjectives as object complements (x = direct object)
the noun phrase
(np)
We begin our discussion of noun phrases (NP) with NPs that consist of just
a single word and discuss their functional and then their formal properties.
Then we will move on to various types of multi-word NPs.
Simple NPs: single word phrases
The left-hand column in Table 3 lists categories of single words that may
constitute an NP, and which must consequently be its head; the italicized
expressions in the right-hand column in Table 3 are examples of single-word
NPs belonging to the corresponding category.
category of head word
Noun, count
Noun, non-count
Subject and object personal pronouns
Genitive personal pronoun
Indefinite pronoun/quantifier
Wh-word/pronoun
table 3: single-word nps
example
Wombats are playful.
Cabbage is nutritious.
They saw her.
Mine are chartreuse.
None were found.
Who placed the call?
All of the word categories in the right-hand column are noun-like, so in
order to abbreviate and to simplify matters, let’s refer to them all as nominals. Every NP, like every other phrase, must have a head, and any nominal
can be the head of an NP.
From a structural point of view, we can represent the possibilities in Table 2 in the following simplified tree structure:
(23) a. NP
288
Nominal
Phrases
This tree is generated by the PSR:
(23) b. NP ——> Nominal
We can read this as saying that an NP consists of any kind of nominal.
More complex NPs
We begin this section by presenting two very general functional formulas
for NPs. We give these two because it would be confusing to combine them
into a single formula.
(24) a. (Premodifier*) + HEAD + (Postmodifier*)
(Asterisks denote elements that may appear more than once.)
b. (Complement) + HEAD + (Complement)
Formula (24a) states that a noun phrase must contain a head word
(which, of course, must be a nominal) but need not contain anything else.
If the NP has more elements than the head, it may contain one or more premodifiers (modifiers that precede the head) and/or one or more postmodifiers (modifiers that follow the head). This formula thus abbreviates several
possibilities:
(25) a.
b.
c.
d.
Head
Premodifier(s) + head
Head + postmodifier(s)
Premodifiers(s) + head + postmodifiers(s)
Formula (24b) states that a noun phrase must contain a head, which may
be preceded or followed by a complement. It also abbreviates several possibilities:
(26) a.
b.
c.
d.
Head
Complement + Head
Head + Complement
Complement + Head + Complement
We will deal with these possibilities in sequence.
289
Delahunty and Garvey
More complex NPs: single-word premodifier + head
Table 4 illustrates NPs whose heads (bolded) are modified by single-word
premodifiers (italicized). The part of speech of the premodifiers is given in the
left-hand column.
form of premodifier
Article
Adjective Phrase
Demonstrative pronoun
Genitive NP
Genitive pronoun
Noun Indefinite pronoun/quantifier
Wh-word
Numeral
Ordinal
Quantifier
Negative
example
The wombats escaped.
Strong winds.
That vase is valuable.
Sheila’s serve is powerful.
Her serve is powerful.
Metal plates shielded the instruments.
Some survivors remained.
Which lobster do you want?
Seven boxes fell.
Second thoughts assailed us.
Several vats of beer.
No accidents were reported.
table 4: single-word premodifiers
Exercise
In each sentence below, identify all the NPs; then identify the part of
speech of the head and of any premodifier(s) in each NP:
a. The evidence was unconvincing.
b. Party invitations are always welcome.
c. Many people have visited that exhibition.
d. Their intuitions make them remarkable therapists.
e. Dust mites cause serious allergies in some people.
The range of premodifiers of noun heads is large, including nearly all the
parts of speech. The items in Table 4 present the basic possibilities. The most
frequently used modifiers are the articles, which we briefly discussed in our
chapter on Minor Parts of Speech. Here we will elaborate on that discussion.
We noted that one major use of an article is to indicate whether the NP
in which it occurs is definite or not: if the NP is definite, then the speaker/
writer assumes that the hearer/reader can identify the referent of the NP; and
if the NP is indefinite, then the speaker/writer assumes that the hearer/reader
290
Phrases
cannot identify its referent.
Yet another meaning associated with NPs is that of referentiality. A referring
NP may be either definite or indefinite but it denotes a particular entity or set of
entities: the bold NP in The/A man sat down refers to some particular man.
The opposite of a referring NP is an attributive or non-referring one.
An attributive NP provides a description but does not refer to any particular
individual(s). Anyone or anything that fits the description will do. Attributive
NPs can often be paraphrased by whoever . . ., whatever . . ., or any . . ., as in
The man who /Whoever steals my purse steals nothing.
(27) a. I saw the elephants at the zoo. (referential and definite)
b. The next caller will win a vacation to Miami. (attributive and
definite = whoever is the next caller)
c. I want an elephant. Its name is Big Bob. (referential and indefinite)
d.I want an elephant. Any pink one will be fine. (attributive
and indefinite.)
Finally, NPs can have a generic or non-generic reference. Generic reference designates an entire class (i.e., category, set) of entities. A non-generic
reference designates a particular member or members of a class.
(28) a. Cats are skilled predators. (generic, indefinite)
b. A cat is a skilled predator. (generic, indefinite)
c. A cat is asleep on the table. (non-generic and indefinite)
d.The cat is asleep. (non-generic and definite)
e. The cat is a skilled predator. (ambiguous: generic or nongeneric and definite)
Exercise
Identify each italicized expression as (a) definite or indefinite, (b) referential or attributive, and (c) generic or non-generic. You will have
to imagine a situation in which each sentence is used. Note where
ambiguities arise.
a. I need a sandwich.
b. I need a part for my car.
c. Alice wants to protect the elephant.
d. The president’s detractors must be insane.
291
Delahunty and Garvey
The only somewhat difficult case in Table 4 is the noun modifier, that is,
the case where a noun modifies a head noun, as in metal plates. Remember that metal is not an adjective for formal reasons—e.g., it cannot be
compared or intensified: *metaler, *more metal, *very metal. Noun modifiers
appear frequently when one speaks of a material out of which something is
made, but the semantic range of such constructions is extensive:
(29)
a. government spying
b. state law
c. pie chart
d. desert safari
e. Sunday newspaper
f. stone wall
g. plastic cups
h. cardboard boxes
Exercise
1. Try to describe the semantic/meaning relations between the head
noun and its noun modifier in each of the constructions in (29).
2. What do writing handbooks say about the use of noun modifiers?
Examine a piece of real-life prose, identifying various types of premodifiers. Can you determine different writing styles according to their
variety?
More complex NPs: head + prepositional phrase
Most of the simple premodifiers above contain one word. The least complex
postmodifier—and by far the most common—is the prepositional phrase
(PP). Remember that PPs consist of a preposition and a noun phrase. So this
simple postmodification will have the structure: N + PP. (31a-f ) are examples,
each with the structure:
292
Phrases
(30)
NP
N
concerns
PP
P
about
NP
Art
the
N
future
(31) a. songs about rebellion
b. clocks on the wall
c. walks with my mother
d. arguments about abortion
e. reasons for my hesitation
f. sources of concern
Exercise
Provide a fully labeled tree diagram for each of the NPs in (31a-f).
A problem that arises with expressions in which a N is followed by a PP
is whether the N and PP actually combine to form a noun phrase, as in the
examples in (31), or whether they are simply a non-unified sequence of N
followed by PP, as in Put the book on the shelf. In this expression, the N book is
not combined with the PP on the shelf into an NP. It is important to have ways
of identifying which kind of expression we are dealing with.
Remember that we said that one test for phrasehood is the possibility of
being replaced by a single word. In the case of NPs these words would be
pronouns. So, if a sequence of words can be replaced by a pronoun, then it is
very likely an NP. For instance, you could replace all of the expressions in (31)
by some form of the word they. Let’s call this test the Pronoun-Substitution
(Pro-Sub) Test. To see how it works, let’s consider (32):
(32) a. Woody admired the picture on the wall.
b. Woody put the picture on the wall.
293
Delahunty and Garvey
Applying the Pro-Sub Test to the picture on the wall in (32a) we get (33a):
(33) a. Woody admired it.
This is grammatical, so in (32a) the picture on the wall is a unified NP.
When we apply the Pro-Sub Test to the same sequence of words in (32b)
we get (33b):
(33) b. *Woody put it.
This is ungrammatical, showing us that the picture on the wall in (32b) is not
a unified NP.
If we now apply the Pro-Sub Test to the picture in (32b) we get:
(34) Woody put it on the wall.
This is grammatical, showing us that the picture in (32b) is separate from on
the wall.
These patterns of grammaticality lead to the conclusion that (32a) contains
an NP made up of a head with a PP postmodifier and that (32b) contains the
simpler NP the picture followed by a separate PP on the wall.
We can represent these by the following tree structure diagrams:
(32)a’.
NP
(32)b’.
Art
N
PP
the
picture
on the wall
VP
V
put
NP
the picture
PP
on the wall
Exercise
1. English contains several different types of pronouns—demonstratives,
wh-pronouns, and the like—and pronoun substitution tests can be created using any of them. Using wh-pronouns we can create the wh-question
Test. In this version of the Pro-Sub Test, you replace the sequence under
analysis with an appropriate question word and turn the sentence into a
question. Let’s apply this test to (32a,b) just as we applied the original
Pro-Sub Test. First replace the picture on the wall in (32a) with an appropriate wh-word and turn the result into a question. If the result is grammatical then the sequence is a unified NP. If the result is not grammatical
294
Phrases
then you have evidence that the sequence is not a unified NP. Now apply
the same test to the sequence in (32b). Follow the same procedure and
logic. You should find again that in (32a) the picture on the wall is an NP,
but that in (32b) the picture on the wall is not.
2. You will recall also that movement is a good test for phrasehood. The
active-passive relationship you read about in our chapter on Minor Parts
of Speech provides the basis for a movement test that can identify NPs.
Recall from that discussion that the passive subject NP corresponds to
the active object NP and the active subject NP corresponds to the NP
that is the object of passive by. These correspondences are indicated by
subscripts in:
Active: [1 Masked raiders] breached [2 the security system].
Passive: [2 The security system] was breached by [1 masked raiders].
Given that subjects are generally NPs, if an expression can be turned
into a passive subject then it is an NP. We can apply this test to (32a,b):
(32) a. Woody admired the picture on the wall.
c. The picture on the wall was admired by Woody.
Because (32c) is grammatical the sequence the picture on the wall
must be an NP in (32a).
(32) b. Woody put the picture on the wall.
d. *The picture on the wall was put by Woody.
Because (32d) is ungrammatical, the sequence the picture on the wall
cannot be an NP in (32b).
Use the passive test to show that the italicized phrases in the following sentences are NPs:
a. Grammatical tests prove grammatical categorizations.
b. Teenagers mow lawns.
c. Obsessive-compulsives write grammar books.
d. The military developed the internet.
e. The teachers forced the unfortunate students to read grammar
books.
295
Delahunty and Garvey
There is also a paraphrase test for a noun head + PP. If you can insert the
words which is/was or that is/was between the noun head and the PP, the
construction is probably of the head + postmodifier type. We call this the
Whiz-test. (Wh comes from which; iz comes from the pronunciation of
is). Applying this test to (32a) and (32b) we end up with the paraphrases
(32e,f ), respectively.
(32) e. Woody admired the picture which was on the wall.
f. *Woody put the picture which was on the wall.
That (32e) is grammatical indicates that on the wall is a postmodifier of picture in (32a) and thus that the picture on the wall is a phrase in that sentence;
the ungrammaticality of (32f ) indicates that on the wall is not a postmodifier of picture in (32b) and thus that the picture on the wall is not a phrase
in that sentence.
Thus we can conclude that the picture on the wall in (32a) is a unified NP
containing a head noun and a following PP. In contrast, in (32b), the picture
on the wall is not a unified NP.
Our tests demonstrate aspects of the process of grammatical reasoning—the use of tests, the need for several tests, consideration of multiple
hypotheses, and the role of grammaticality judgments. A further dividend is
that the tests we have just described will apply to just about any type of NP,
not just those involving PP postmodifiers.
More complex NPs: multiple premodifiers
Our examples so far have dealt only with single word premodifiers, but premodifiers can be multiplied, as (35) shows.
(35) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
the two culprits (article + numeral)
those metal plates (demonstrative + noun)
several other candidates (quantifier + indefinite)
one such oddity (numeral + indefinite)
a second chance (article + ordinal)
Exercise
Identify the premodifiers and their types in:
a. several handsome geldings
b. long boring sessions
c. three French hens
296
Phrases
d. a rock quarry
e. many such steamy scenes
More complex NPs: phrasal premodifiers
Multiple one-word premodifiers cause little trouble for students. But phrasal
prenominal modifiers are more complicated. Table 5 presents some major
types. (We deal with verbal phrases in our chapter on Multi-Clause Sentences.)
form of premodifier
Genitive NP (GenNP)
Adjective phrase (AP)
Verbal phrase (VblP)
example
This friend’s hobby is knitting.
Very old memories return easily.
Carelessly organized meetings annoy everyone.
table 5: phrasal premodifiers
Phrasal premodifiers can be expanded, adding greater complexity to the
structure. Moreover, genitive NPs and APs readily combine with other structures to create heavily premodified NPs:
(36)a. My friend’s hobby is interesting.
(GenNP my friend’s modifies hobby; genitive pronoun
(GenNP) my modifies friend.)
b.All my friend’s hobbies are interesting.
(GenNP my friend’s modifies hobbies; my modifies friend; all
modifies my friend’s hobbies)
c. All my friends’ very old plates (three premodifiers: quantifier all;
Gen NP with genitive premodifier my friends’; AP with intensifier very old)
d.Those very old counterfeiting plates belonged to Capone.
(three premodifiers: demonstrative those; AP with intensifier
very old; verbal phrase counterfeiting)
Exercise
Describe the meaning difference between (36b) and All my friends’
hobbies are interesting. Note the positions of the apostrophes.
297
Delahunty and Garvey
Genitive NPs raise two further issues of complexity. First, they are closely
related to postmodifiers that use a prepositional phrase headed by of. Compare the following.
(37) a. my friend’s hobbies
b. the hobbies of my friend
c. my friend’s house
d. the house of my friend
e. the house of the friend that I met in Palo Alto
f. the birth of a daughter
g. a daughter’s birth
h. my daughter’s birth
i. the home of the brave
j. ?the braves’ home [Note: (37i) and (37j) have different meanings.]
k. a cup of soup/coffee/tea
l. *a soup’s/coffee’s/tea’s cup
m. a wedge of cheese
n. *a cheese’s wedge
o. a pat of butter
p. *a butter’s pat
q. a ream of paper
r. *a paper’s ream
s. a fistful of dollars
t. *a dollar’s fistful
These examples indicate that the choice of premodifier genitive vs. of-genitive depends on various factors:
a. The length of the GenNP: the longer the GenNP, the more likely we are to use an of-genitive, e.g., (37e).
b.Whether the entity represented by the genitive is animate or not;
if it is, we are more likely to use the premodifier genitive; cf. the
cat’s fur vs. ?the wheel’s rim).
c.If the GenNP is a pronoun, we strongly prefer the premodifier
genitive; cf. her car vs. *the car of her, Sophie’s Choice vs. ?A Choice
of Sophie(’s).
d.Note the meaning difference between her photographs and photographs of her. The first can have many meanings, e.g., photographs
she owns/took/ordered/designed/etc. The second means photographs in which she is pictured.
e. An NP containing a premodifier genitive is definite, but a post298
Phrases
modifier genitive allows indefinite determiners to modify the
head noun; compare the definite NP Oscar’s friend with the indefinite NP a friend of Oscar’s.
While native speakers are not likely to have trouble with such complexities, non-native students may encounter serious difficulties with this construction.
The second complexity is that genitive NPs themselves contain a NP.
When one structure contains another structure, we say that the second
structure is embedded in the first. The NP Harry’s wife’s paintings contains
a genitive NP within a genitive NP. In other words, Harry’s is embedded
within Harry’s wife’s, which in turn is embedded in Harry’s wife’s paintings.
This structure is represented in (38):
(38)
NP-1
Premodifier-A
(GenNP)
NP-2
Premodifier-B
(GenNP)
NP-3
Head-A
’s
Head-B
’s
Head
N
Harry
’s wife
’s
paintings
Embedding allows one function (or form) to contain other functions (or
forms). An understanding of embedding is critical to analysis of grammatical
structures with any significant degree of complexity. Let’s illustrate this fact
with diagram (38). NP-l (Harry’s wife’s paintings) consists of a premodifier of
the form GenNP (Harry’s wife’s) and a head noun (paintings). The GenNP
in turn consists of a full NP (NP-2), along with the genitive inflection ’s. In
other words, the form NP-2 is contained in the form NP-l. NP-2 contains a
premodifier (Harry’s) and a head noun (wife). Finally, premodifier-B contains
a single noun head (Harry), the ‘s, and no premodifiers.
299
Delahunty and Garvey
Under NP-3, we could have chosen a premodifier with a possessive pronoun and a noun modifier to give us my uncle Harry’s wife’s paintings. We could
even have selected another GenNP under NP-3, in which case we might have
gotten Harry’s cousin’s wife’s paintings. In fact, we could (in principle) go on
to infinity, producing ever longer and more genealogically bizarre structures:
Harry’s aunt’s cousin’s son’s granddaughter’s niece’s sister’s step-child’s friend’s paintings.
Embedding enables language to be infinite in the number and length
of the sentences it can create. Fortunately, speakers tend to use these possibilities sparingly, though occasionally writers such as Dylan Thomas, Henry
James, and William Faulkner toy with them. As you progress through this
book, you will see the pervasiveness of embedding.
Before we leave premodifiers of Ns we must address one final matter,
namely the order of premodifiers. There are many proposals in the grammatical and linguistic literature, many of remarkable complexity. The following, adapted from Frawley (1992: 482-3) is a partial list:
Det > quantity > value > physical property > age > color > Head
The
five
good
long
old brown tables
Other languages allow different orders, so your non-native English speaking
students may come up with utterances that violate the order rules, such as the
following from a Korean student:
the weakness of the each student
Exercise
Identify and draw brackets around each NP and underline its headword.
Using Tables 3 and 4, identify the type of each premodifier in the NP.
a. We noticed several suspicious details.
b. My best friend’s parents gave his younger sister a European tour
as a graduation present.
c. Three false alarms were mysteriously called in during exam week.
Complex NPs: The range of postmodifiers
As complicated as possessives are, we easily recognize the infrequency of
expressions such as Harry’s uncle’s cousin’s sister’s paintings. Much more common—and much more complex—are the various sorts of phrases and
300
Phrases
clauses that follow head nouns. We have already examined the prepositional
phrase, probably the simplest postmodifier. Yet even this innocent construction raises the specter of mind-boggling expansions. Like possessive NPs,
prepositional phrases contain noun phrases, which can contain prepositional phrases, which can contain other NPs which can contain a PP . . . all the
way to the linguistic loony bin. In case you have doubts, consider the NP
in (39):
(39) The book in the drawer of the desk in the office of the leader of
the rebellion against the oppression of readers of tales of adventures on far planets of the galaxy . . .
Complexity is due also to the potential for various sorts of postmodifiers,
each more structurally intricate than the premodifiers. We treat these structures more fully in other chapters. For the present, we will introduce the
major types of postmodifiers and comment briefly on them.
postmodifier type
example
Adjective phrase (AP)
[Anyone fond of kumquats] should buy this cookbook.
Appositive NP (AppNP)
[His nominee, an infamous scoundrel,] is unlikely to be elected.
Relative clause (RC)
[The contestant who guesses the title] will win a trip to Tahiti.
Appositive relative (AppRC)
[G.W. Bush, who is the 43rd President of the US,] is only 60.
Verbal phrase (VblP)
[The contestant guessing the title] will win a vacation in Tahiti.
[The person seated at the president’s right] is her bodyguard.
[The player to watch] is Tzrdsky.
table 6: complex postmodifiers
We have seen adjective phrases (APs) that function as premodifiers. Such
constructions tend to be brief—one or two words if the adjective is not coordinated. However, some adjectives can, like nouns, appear with their own
postmodifiers. (In the example in Table 6, of kumquats is a PP that complements fond; since that PP contains an NP, expansions like that in (39) are
possible.) APs with complements or postmodifiers almost always occur in the
301
Delahunty and Garvey
postmodifier position of noun phrases. Postmodifying APs also tend to allow
the Whiz-test: Anyone who is fond of kumquats.
If the head of the NP is an indefinite pronoun such as someone, something, anything, nothing, then any attributive AP will occur after it:
(40) a. I heard something strange.
b. I haven’t heard anything new.
c. I see nothing unusual.
Appositive noun phrases (AppNPs) and Appositive Relative Clauses
(AppRCs) occur as “parenthetical asides” after their head noun. They are
usually blocked off in writing by surrounding commas (dashes are also possible). In speech, they are surrounded by perceptible pause and often a fall
in voice pitch, akin to the aside spoken by a stage actor. The appositive NP
has the same referent as the rest of the NP. Thus in Table 6 his nominee and
an infamous scoundrel designate the same individual. Since appositives can
be expanded just like any other NP, they allow for infinite embedding. Sentence (41) suggests the possibilities.
(41) His nominee, an infamous scoundrel with principles learned from
years of service in one of the most corrupt political machines ever devised by the devious minds that have blemished history, is unlikely
to be elected.
Appositives provide extra information that is generally viewed as not being required for the identification of the referent of the NP. Some handbooks
say that they can be omitted without changing the meaning of the sentence
they occur in. This is quite misleading. The meaning of the sentence certainly
changes, though what the affected NP refers to may not.
Verbal phrases (VblPs), which will be dealt with further in our chapter on
Multi-clause Sentences, are like adjective phrases: short VblPs precede noun
heads; longer VblPs, which may possess their own range of objects, complements, and modifiers, follow the head noun within a noun phrase. In general,
short modifiers tend to precede head nouns and longer ones tend to follow
them.
Relative clauses were introduced in our chapter on Minor Parts of Speech
and will be more fully discussed in our chapter on Modifications of Basic
Clause Patterns. These clauses usually begin with a wh-word, that, or no introducer at all: The soldier who died . . . , The thing that gets me . . . , The book
[ ] you wrote . . .
302
Phrases
Exercise
Draw brackets around each NP and underline its headword. Using Tables
4, 5, and 6, indicate the type of each premodifier and/or postmodifier
that you find.
a. Don’t go out in the midday sun.
b. The cat near the window is Salome.
c. Alvin set the goldfish bowl near the window.
d. I saw the cat near the window. (ambiguous: analyze two different
ways)
e. The squirrel that Bonzo, my pet chimp, chased became quite
flustered.
f. Some friends of Boris gave him a box filled with his favorite candy
as a going-away present.
g. The witnesses at the scene noticed a stranger who drove away in
a red station wagon full of flowers.
Complements in NPs
Complements in NPs typically follow the head N, though some may occur
before it, giving us the formula:
(42) (Complement) + H + (Complement)
The complements before the head may be either nouns or, more rarely, adjectives:
(43) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
a fiction writer cf. someone who writes fiction.
an economics professor cf. someone who professes economics.
a technical writer cf. someone who writes technical manuals/
materials.
a financial adviser cf. someone who advises on financial matters.
an ecological expert. (Huddleston and Pullum 2002: 439)
When the complement follows the head N it must be either a PP or a clause:
(44) a. the trip to Disneyland (PP)
b. the claim that the war is justified (that-clause)
c. the question whether we’ve won (embedded/indirect
question)
d. the question ‘Are we there yet?’ (quoted question)
303
Delahunty and Garvey
e. the request to withdraw (to-infinitival clause)
Noun complement clauses are also discussed in our chapter on Multiclause Sentences.
Nominalization
In our chapter on Morphology and Word Formation, we describe how a word
belonging to one part of speech may be derived from a word belonging to a
different part of speech. Nominalizations are nouns derived from words belonging to other parts of speech. Here we focus only on nouns derived from
verbs. These derived nouns can head noun phrases, just like any other noun.
However, their relationship to verbs allows them to have subjects and objects.
For example, amusement is derived from amuse, which is a transitive verb and so
grammatical in a sentence with both a subject and a direct object, such as (45):
(45) a. The clown amused the children.
We can nominalize (45a) as the NP (45b):
(45) b. The clown’s amusement of the children.
Notice that in the nominalization, the subject of the clause in (45a) shows
up as a genitive premodifier, The clown’s, and that the direct object of the
verb in (45a), the children, shows up as the object of the preposition of.
Exercise
1.
Nominalize the following sentences.
a. Werner inspected the package.
b. Pamela enjoys bobsledding.
c. The truth gradually emerged.
2. Change the following noun phrases to sentences by reversing the nominalization.
a. Manuel’s toleration of teasing
b. Oscar’s avoidance of hard work
c. Helen’s expectation that she would be rescued
3. Nominalization occurs more frequently in written texts, and especially technical and academic texts, than in speech. Pick a paragraph
304
Phrases
in a scientific text and a similar sized section of dialog in a novel and
compare the number of nominalizations in the two.
Complex NPs: Coordination
Perhaps on the principle that too much of a good thing is impossible, languages allow us to repeat NPs indefinitely. Coordinated NPs are joined by a
coordinating conjunction, such as and or or, as in (46):
(46) My sister and/or her best friend will deliver the letter.
Such structures are relatively simple to deal with—except for one problem.
Consider the ambiguous sentence (47):
(47) Old men and women will be served first.
Who will be served first? Old men and all women? Old men and old women? The answer seems to depend on whether the premodifying adjective
old applies to men only or to the conjunction of men and women. To differentiate these possibilities, we must allow not only full NPs to coordinate but
also heads of NPs. We represent the ambiguity diagrammatically in (48).
(48)a.
AP
Old
A
(48)b.
NP
AP
N
Conj
men
and
N
women
NP
Conj
NP
N
N
NP
NP
A
Old
men
and
women
305
Delahunty and Garvey
Diagram (48a) represents the situation in which old modifies the NP
men and women; (48b) represents the situation in which old modifies only
men.
t h e ve r b p h r a s e
(vp)
We begin with the functional formula for VPs and then examine the forms
that can satisfy those functions.
(49) VP Functional Formula
(AUXILIARY*) + HEAD + (OBJECT*) + (COMPLEMENT*) + (MODIFIER*)
This formula states that a VP must contain a head word, optionally preceded by one or more auxiliaries, and optionally followed by object(s), complements, and/or modifier(s).
Simple VPs: head alone
Single-word VPs always consist of a head word that is an intransitive verb,
bolded in the examples in (50). The syntactic structure of such intransitive
verb phrases is given in (50’):
(50) a.
b.
c.
d.
Hector walks.
(50’)
All the employees agree.
The lemmings followed.
Cynthia lied/laughed/coughed/died/ . . .
VP
V
In school grammars, the terms main verb and simple predicate sometimes
are used for the head word of the VP.
VPs: auxiliaries and head
In our chapter on Major Parts of Speech, we distinguished between main
verbs and auxiliary verbs. The discussion primarily concerned head verbs.
However, heads often occur in the company of other verbs, called helping
verbs, auxiliaries, or simply Aux, some of whose functions we described in
our chapter on the Minor Parts of Speech.
The major auxiliary verbs in English are have, be, and do. (In our chapter
on Minor Parts of Speech we briefly discussed the modal auxiliaries will,
would, can, could, shall, should, may, might, and must.) The uses of have, be,
and do are illustrated in (51):
306
Phrases
(51) a. The zombies departed from Hector’s house. (head alone)
b. Hector is acting strangely. (be + head verb in Ving form)
c. Hector has never looked at me like that. (have + head verb in
past participle form)
d. Hector does not eat vegetables. (do + head verb in infinitive
form)
e. Hector has been consorting with the zombies. (have + be in
past participle form + head verb in Ving form)
As these examples show, a verb phrase will generally contain one head verb;
in English, auxiliaries always precede the main verb. The auxiliaries may be
separated from the verb, either through interruption by items like never as in
(51c) or by inversion as in Has Hector seen Oswald? Let’s examine these auxiliaries in more detail.
Have is the auxiliary associated with the perfect aspect. It can accept all
of the four potential inflections of a main verb, although its third person
singular present tense and past tense forms are slightly irregular:
(52) a.
b.
c.
d.
have sung
has sung
had sung
having sung
These examples also demonstrate that auxiliary have is followed by a verb
(whether another auxiliary or a main verb) in the past participle form, which
we have abbreviated Ven. So our formula for auxiliary have is HAVE + Ven.
The syntactic structure that corresponds to this formula is:
(53)
HAVE Ven
A word of caution: have may also serve as a head verb. If a single instance of
have is the only verb in a clause, then it is a main verb and therefore head of its
VP: I have a cold. If two instances of have occur, the first is an auxiliary, as in I
have had a cold for two weeks, and the second is the head verb.
Exercise
Using (a) as a model, identify the two elements of the perfect in each
307
Delahunty and Garvey
of
the following examples:
a. We have eaten all the pizza.
b. They have been in there for hours.
c. Bill has seen the light.
d. The fugitive has taken the bus.
e. The police have blocked the roads.
f. They haven’t found her yet.
Be is the auxiliary associated with the progressive aspect, which is compatible with almost all the possible forms of be:
(54) a. be singing
b. am singing
c. is singing
d. are singing
e. was singing
f. were singing
g. been singing
h.*being singing
Be, have, and do are the most irregular verbs in English. Their standard
English forms are listed below in Tables 7.
a. be Person 1
2
3
Present
Singular
Plural
am
are
are
are
is
are
Past participle (Ven form)
been
Present participle (Ving form) being
Gerund (Ving form)
being
b. have
Present
Third person singular
has
All other persons and numbers have
Past
All persons and numbers
308
Past
Singular
was
were
was
had
Plural
were
were
were
Phrases
Past participle (Ven form)
Present participle (Ving form)
Gerund (Ving form)
c. do
Present
Third person singular
does
All other persons and numbers do
Past
All persons and numbers
did
Past participle (Ven form)
Present participle (Ving form)
Gerund (Ving form)
done
doing
doing
table 7: forms of
b e , h a ve ,
and
had
having
having
do
Auxiliary be will always follow auxiliary have when they occur together in
the same clause. Whichever verb follows progressive be assumes its present
participle form, Ving. Our formula is BE + Ving, and the syntactic structure
corresponding to this is:
(55)
BE
Ving
Exercise
Using (a) as a model, identify the two elements of the progressive in
each of the following sentences:
a. The students were acting out.
b. Everyone was talking during class.
c. Ted and Sheila are getting divorced.
d. Sheila and Roger are planning to elope.
e. Ted is feeling blue.
f. Their friends are acting surprised.
309
Delahunty and Garvey
When perfective have and progressive be occur together in a clause, (1)
have precedes be; (2) be, as the verb immediately to the right of have, occurs
as a been; and (3) the verb immediately to the right of progressive be occurs
as Ving. The associated syntactic structure is:
(56)
HAVE
BEen
Ving
Exercise
Using (a) as a model, identify and distinguish the elements of the perfect and the progressive in each of the following sentences:
a. I have been searching for that for ages. (Bold = perfect; underlined = progressive)
b. We had all been hoping for better weather.
c. Eleanor has been dating JD for several months now.
d. JD has been seeing a physical therapist for his damaged knee.
e. The plants have been doing better since you started talking to
them.
f. My computer has been crashing a lot lately.
Like have, be may serve as a main verb. When it does, as in Wiggles is a
friendly boa, it is a linking verb. When two instances of be occur, the same
generalization holds as for have—the first is an auxiliary (as in Wiggles is being
affectionate) and the second is the head verb.
The auxiliary be is a bit more complex than auxiliary have because it also
occurs in the passive. The functional formula for the passive is Be + Ven, and
the corresponding syntactic structure is:
(57)
BE
Ven
310
Phrases
Exercise
Using (a) as a model, identify the two elements of the passive in each
of the following sentences:
a. This book was written by a nice derangement of linguists.
b. The current global warming was predicted by scientists almost
fifty years ago.
c. The Mona Lisa was painted by Leonardo da Vinci.
d. Global warming is caused by excessive hydrocarbon use.
e. Passive sentences are marked by a form of be and a verb in its
past participle form.
f. Movies are intended to communicate ideologies.
Can the passive be occur with the progressive be? Sentence (58) shows that it can:
(58) That song is being sung poorly.
(58) also shows that the passive be follows the progressive be. We can represent
the structure of the verb phrase in (58) as:
(59)
BE
Ving
Ven
Finally, we can combine all of the auxiliaries above into a single verb phrase.
Can you think of what such a verb phrase might be? Let’s look at it structurally:
(60)
HAVE
BEen
BEing
Ven
sung
311
Delahunty and Garvey
To imagine such an ungainly (but grammatical) verb phrase in a sentence,
consider (61), which denotes either the singing of one extremely long song or
repetitious performances of the same song.
(61) That song has been being sung for hours, and I’m sick of it.
Exercise
Using (a) as a model, identify and distinguish the elements of the perfect, the progressive, and the passive in each of the following sentences:
a. Actors have been being nominated for Oscars for over fifty years
now. (Bold = perfect; italics = progressive; underlined = passive)
b. Time and energy have been being wasted on that project for a
long time.
c. The children must not have been being well cared for.
d. The parents should have been being monitored all along.
e. Administration policy hasn’t been being properly scrutinized by Congress.
f. Air travelers from the Middle East have been being harassed by
security officers since 9/11.
A note on Do
Do, the last auxiliary, is something of an exception, as the sentences below
suggest:
(62) a. Jason does not/doesn’t sing.
b. Does Jason sing?
c. Jason DOES sing.
d.*Jason may do sing.
e. *Jason do may sing.
f. *Jason is doing sing.
g. *Jason does be singing. (grammatical in some dialects of English)
h.*Jason has done sing.
i. *Jason does have sing.
j. *Jason is done sing.
k.*Jason does be sung.
What can we learn from this odd pattern of sentences? First, when do occurs
312
Phrases
with a main verb, that verb is in its base (V) form, represented by the formula DO + V. Second, do cannot occur with any of the other auxiliaries, either
before or after them. Third, do may occur when the main verb is negated by
not or n’t (62a), in a question (62b), or in an emphatic sentence (62c).
Fourth, the auxiliary do differs from the Pro-Verb Phrase do. Consider
sentence (63).
(63) Did1 she do2 so too?
In this example, Did1 is an auxiliary, while do2 so acts as a Pro-Verb Phrase,
an expression that substitutes for a verb phrase.
Auxiliary do and main verb do also differ, as the fact that they can cooccur shows: [AuxDid] you [MV do] the dishes?
One handy test for identifying VPs is the Do-So Test. One simple application of this test is to substitute do so for the VP (ensuring that the tense
of do matches that of the original VP):
(64) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
The zombies did so. (= 51a)
Hector is doing so. (= 51b)
Hector has never done so. (= 51c)
Hector does not do so. (= 51d)
Hector has been doing so. (= 51e)
In another version of this test, the sentence in question is coordinated with
one similar to it; in this case the elements reverse to form so do:
(65) a. Hector juggles and so does Zenobia.
b. All the employees agree and so does their supervisor.
c. The lemmings jumped and so did the zoologists.
In short, the expressions do so and so do replace a VP, including its objects,
complements, and modifiers, but excluding negation and auxiliaries.
Exercise
In the sentences below, draw brackets around the main verb. Draw formulas or trees similar to those used in the preceding section to describe
the structure of the auxiliaries + main verb.
a. I have bought all my textbooks for next semester.
b. I have a few dollars for fun.
313
Delahunty and Garvey
c.
d.
e.
f.
Sylvia has been doing a lot of work lately.
Of course we are being ironic.
Zelda was congratulated for her good work.
The job has been completed.
VP: head + object(s)/complement
A phrase that obligatorily follows a verb head is called an object or complement. These terms are sometimes interchangeable, although tradition has
tended to attach “object” to some constructions and “complement” to others. The reasons for the variation are obscure. The label “object” dimly suggests the goal or purpose of the verb head, though neither of these semantic
labels applies to every structure so labeled. The term “complement” suggests
the notion of completing (hence the spelling) the verb in some way. Table 8
lists the four main types of objects and complements.
type
example
Direct Object
The Vikings demanded [tribute (NP)].
Indirect Object
Waldo gave [his sister (NP)] a dictionary.
PP Complement
Waldo gave a dictionary [to his sister (PP)]
Subject Complement
Freud was [a prude (NP)]/[prudish (AP)].
Object Complement
I consider Jung [a quack (NP)]/[unreliable (AP)].
Sentence Complement I realize [that tests should be easier (S)].
table 8: objects and complements of verbs
Table 8 reveals that NPs can serve any object or complement function,
that adjective phrases can also act in complement functions, and that an
entire clause or sentence can act as the complement of certain verbs.
An important grammatical notion associated with the direct object is
that of transitivity. A transitive verb takes a direct object; an intransitive
verb does not. Thus the sentences (66a-d) contain transitive verbs and those
in (67a-d) contain intransitive verbs. The direct objects in (66) are italicized.
(66) a.
b.
c.
d.
Moriarty eluded Sherlock.
Everyone avoided me.
Sarah gave him some good advice.
I consider Jung a quack/unreliable.
(67) a. We walked.
314
Phrases
b. Smoke rises.
c. Harrison confessed.
d. Everyone in the room laughed.
In English, a large number of verbs can be either transitive or intransitive,
sometimes with a considerable difference of meaning:
(68) a. The fire smoked.
b. Shelley smoked. (Ambiguous)
c. Shelley smoked the salmon.
Thus whether such verbs are transitive or intransitive can only be determined by their use in an actual clause. One simple test is that an intransitive
verb can potentially end a complete clause, as in (67).
Two structures require not one but two phrases to follow the verb. The
indirect object construction, e.g., (66c), typically calls for a direct object
also. Verbs that enter into such constructions are said to be bitransitive or
ditransitive. The object complement construction (e.g., (66d)) requires a
direct object preceding the complement. We deal in more detail with these
constructions in our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns.
Verbs with objects may imply end products and/or bounded activities:
(69) a. He built a workbench.
b. He builds workbenches.
(69a) implies that a workbench came into being as a result of the building; it also views the workbench building event as having an end point,
namely the completion of the workbench. (69b) is in the simple present
tense and represents multiple, discrete, workbench building events, as the
plural, workbenches, makes clear. In this respect, it is like a punctual verb,
such as cough, punch, or kick.
Verbs without objects often imply no natural end-point. Compare (70a)
with (70b):
(70) a. She ran.
b. She ran a marathon.
(70a) implies no particular end to the running; in fact, it is compatible with
and she’s still running. (70b), on the other hand, looks at the marathon-running
as a single event with a natural completion, namely, the end of the marathon.
315
Delahunty and Garvey
The simple present version of (70b), She runs marathons, implies multiple
individual marathon-running events. Note the plural, marathons.
Verbs allow or select complements of various syntactic forms. For instance,
when wait is transitive, its complement may be either an NP (e.g., Wait your
turn!) or a PP (e.g., We’ll wait for the next bus). Anyone learning the language
must learn the restrictions and possibilities associated with verbs.
Exercise
Using (a) as a guide, identify and distinguish the various objects and
complements in the following sentences. Be sure to identify the entire
object/complement expression in each instance.
a. The people elected Oscar (Direct Object) poet laureate (Object
Complement).
b. She must be really intelligent.
c. Her daughter became a famous scientist.
d. Fred smokes cigarillos.
e. The students gave the new teacher a welcome gift.
f. The class named Rodriguez “classmate most likely to succeed.”
g. Many people believe that James Joyce was the greatest novelist
of the 20th century.
VP: head + modifier(s)
To distinguish them from modifiers of nouns, modifiers of verbs often have
special names such as adverbial. The most frequent modifiers come in only
four formal types, as indicated in Table 9.
type of modifier
Adverb phrase
Prepositional phrase
Adverbial clause
Noun phrase
example
We left early.
We stayed in Helsinki.
We left after it started to snow.
We enjoyed it a great deal.
table 9: modifiers of verbs
Adverbial clauses begin with the subordinating adverbial conjunctions
mentioned in the chapter on Minor Parts of Speech.
Like single adverbs, the phrasal and clausal modifiers are somewhat movable in the sentence:
316
Phrases
(71) a. We eagerly waited for our turn.
b. After it began to snow, we left.
Sometimes a short (1-2 word) adverbial will appear within the verb phrase:
(72) a. We do occasionally eat out.
b. She must have often donated her legal services.
Noun phrase adverbials, such as a lot in (73a), may be confused with direct
objects. However, they will never become the subject of a corresponding
passive sentence, as the ungrammaticality of (73b) shows:
(73) a. Harry entertains a lot.
b. *A lot is entertained by Harry.
The adverbials that modify verbs can be grouped semantically according
to the semantic roles that they express. The most common appear in Table
10. These roles should remind you of the meanings associated with adverbs
and prepositions listed in our chapters on Major and Minor Parts of Speech.
semantic role
Time Place
Manner
Reason
Purpose
examples
He left early.
We left on Monday.
I’ll leave when the moon turns green.
She stopped there.
She relaxed on the sofa.
She stopped where the victim was found.
The troupe exited gracefully.
The troupe exited with grace.
He left out of spite.
He left because he was miffed.
He left to milk the cow.
table 10: some semantic roles of adverbials
Exercise
Draw brackets around each VP in the sentences below. Underline the
headword and indicate the type of object, complement, or modifier
that accompanies the headword.
317
Delahunty and Garvey
a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
f.
Angela offered the job to her former rival.
Angela offered Archie the job.
We left for Austin in the morning.
In the morning, we left San Antonio for Austin.
Your proposal seems quite reasonable.
Eat this, if you dare.
Complex VPs: combinations of functions
Though we have illustrated separately each of the functions accompanying
the verb head, the options in the formula stated at the beginning of this section allow for more than one function to appear with the verb. Consider, for
example, the sentences in (74).
(74) a.
b.
c.
d.
She has been speaking for three hours.
(auxiliaries + head + PP-modifier)
Scott offered Zelda a ride since her car was out of gas.
(head + indirect object + direct object + adverbial clause
modifier)
Hortense never becomes angry.
(adverb phrase + head + AP-subject complement)
The remains will be shipped to Cleveland on Wednesday.
(auxiliaries + head + PP-modifier + PP-modifier)
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Biber, Douglas, Susan Conrad and Geoffrey Leech. 2002. Longman Student
Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, UK: Pearson Education
Ltd.
Brown, Keith and Jim Miller. 1991. Syntax: A Linguistic Introduction to
Sentence Structure. 2nd ed. London, UK: Harper Collins Academic.
Carter, Ronald and Michael McCarthy. 2006. Cambridge Grammar of English.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Frawley, William. 1992. Linguistic Semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Greenbaum, Sidney and Randolph Quirk. 1990. A Student’s Grammar of the
English Language. London, UK: Longman.
Huddleston, Rodney and Geoffrey Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar of
the English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____ 2005. A Student’s Introduction to English Grammar. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
318
Phrases
Liles, Bruce. 1987. A Basic Grammar of Modern English. 2nd ed. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Matthews, Peter. 1997. The Concise Oxford Dictionary of Linguistics. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
glossary
adjective phrase:phrase
headed by adjective.
phrase headed by adverb.
adverbial: an expression that functions like an adverb phrase, namely as a
modifier of a verb, verb phrase, adjective, adverb, clause, or sentence.
appositive noun phrase: NP that occurs as a “parenthetical aside” after its
head noun.
auxiliary verb (also called aux or helping verb): verb that accompanies a
main verb in a clause.
bitransitive (also called ditransitive): verb phrase having a direct and an
indirect object.
complement: one expression that grammatically completes another.
degree adverb: adverb indicating the extent to which an adjective or adverb
applies.
ditransitive: See bitransitive.
do-so test: a substitution test used to identify a verb phrase.
embed: to include one structure inside another structure.
finite: a clause whose verb is marked as present or past tense or which contains a modal. See non-finite.
function: role played by one expression in another.
gerund: a non-finite verbal phrase whose first verb is in its Ving form and
which functions in the range of NPs.
helping verb: See auxiliary verb.
infinitive: a non-finite verbal phrase that functions in the range of NPs,
modifiers, or complements.
intensifier: member of a small word class which intensifies the meaning of
its head word.
intransitive: verb that cannot take a direct object.
logical subject: in traditional grammar, a word or phrase referring to either
the agent of an action or the understood subject of a sentence.
modal auxiliary: one of the auxiliaries will, would, can, could, shall, should,
may, might and must.
non-finite: a clause which is not marked for tense nor includes a modal.
See finite.
noun modifier: a noun that modifies a head noun.
adverb phrase:
319
Delahunty and Garvey
noun phrase:
phrase headed by a noun.
NP in VP required by transitive or bitransitive verb.
object of a preposition: a noun phrase required by a preposition in a prepositional phrase.
participle: a verbal phrase whose first verb is Ven or Ving and which functions as a pre- or post-modifier in an NP.
passive test: a test used to determine the object of an active clause by making it passive.
phrase: a grammatical unit containing a head word and any complements
or modifies.
prepositional phrase: phrase headed by a preposition.
pro-sub test: a test used to identify a noun phrase by substituting a pronoun for it.
pro-verb phrase: a form such as do so that replaces a verb phrase.
topicalization test: a test used to determine whether a structure is a phrase
by moving it to the beginning of its sentence.
transitive: verb that requires a direct object.
verb phrase: a phrase headed by a verb.
verbal phrase: a non-finite verb phrase that functions in ways other than as
the predicate of a finite clause. See gerund, infinitive, participle.
wh-question test: a test to identify a noun phrase by replacing it with a
wh-question word and then recasting the sentence that contains it as a question.
whiz-test: a test to identify a noun phrase with a head + postmodifier structure by inserting who or which plus a form of the verb be after the presumed
head word.
object:
320
10 Basic Clause Patterns
key concepts
Clause elements: subjects, predicates
Subjects
Semantic roles
Auxiliary verbs
Tense and aspect
Basic clause patterns
Appendix: time and tense
i n t ro d u c t i o n
Until now, we have examined the grammar of English in bits and pieces—phonemes, morphemes, words, and phrases. In this chapter, we put
these pieces together into the basic grammatical structure of language—the
clause. Clauses are basic for several reasons. First, you need only one of
them to make a sentence, though, of course, sentences may consist of an
indefinite number of clauses. Second, in actual communication, shorter utterances are usually reconstructed and understood by reference to clauses.
For instance, over here might be understood as I’m over here or Shine the light
over here. The grammatical importance of clauses probably reflects the fact
that the clause most directly represents the most fundamental structure of
meaning—the proposition (a description of a state of affairs whose truth
may be asserted, questioned, or otherwise manipulated). It doesn’t make
much sense to say that a clause represents a complete thought, as school
grammars often do, unless we know what a complete thought is. A proposition is the best model of a complete thought (whatever it is) that we have.
For the present, we will proceed on the assumption that the sense of clausehood is intuitive, based on our competence as native speakers and perhaps
on our status as human makers of meaning, although students may need
practice in identifying clauses.
Of the many reasons why we should know about clauses, we’ll briefly
discuss just four. First, clauses are an important punctuation unit. When a
clause constitutes a whole sentence, in written English it must begin with a
capital letter and end in a period or its equivalent. When multiple clauses
combine to constitute a sentence, the individual clauses may require special punctuation, such as separation by commas. Certainly, the single-clause
sentence is the best unit to begin teaching punctuation with.
Second, and relatedly, writing teachers are concerned about sentence
fragments, that is, non-sentential units improperly punctuated as sentential
321
Delahunty and Garvey
units. As we noted earlier, fragments are typically internally grammatical,
that is, they are well-formed phrases or clauses. Students must learn the
differences between clauses and their constituent units and between clauses
and sentences in order to learn to punctuate appropriately.
Third, a developed writing style requires control of a range of sentence
types, from sentences with just a single clause through sentences with an
indefinite number of clauses. A traditional technique for helping student
writers expand their repertoires is sentence combining, or more accurately,
clause combining. Teachers wishing to create clause combining exercises
for their students must know about clauses, especially if they want to tailor the exercises to their students’ actual needs.
Fourth, an important aspect of standard English grammar is subjectverb agreement. That is, the subject and verb of a clause must grammatically agree with each other in person and number. In order to be able to
teach subject-verb agreement, teachers must know about subjects, verbs,
and clauses. And, while the general agreement principle is quite simple, it is
quite intricate in its details (which we deal with in our chapter on Usage in
Book II).
In this chapter we first examine the internal organization of clauses. Next
we discuss the subject function, illustrating the use of formal characteristics
to identify it. We then discuss the semantic roles subjects and other phrases
may play in clauses. The main part of the chapter provides details on seven
major clause patterns in the language.
elements of the clause
The basic functional analysis of a clause is very simple:
(1) A clause is a grammatical unit that contains a subject and a predicate.
That’s all. Only a subject and a predicate are needed. No less than a subject
and a predicate will do. (Recall our discussion of necessary and sufficient
conditions!) According to this definition all of the expressions in (2) are
clauses. (Subjects are italicized; predicates are bolded.)
(2) a. Birds twittered.
b. All the birds of the neighborhood congregated in the venerable elms in the park.
c. that no one approves of the decision
d. which I lost
322
Basic Clause Patterns
e. whenever the phone rings
Traditional (Reed/Kellogg) sentence diagramming represents the functional
subject/predicate relationship, as in (3).
(3) Head of subject
head of predicate
Birds
twittered
Structural diagrams represent the basic elements of clauses, as in (4).
(4) S
NP
VP
S is the abbreviation for “sentence,” and is the category/part of speech label
for both clauses and sentences. The tree (4) is generated by the phrase structure rule (4a).
(4) a. S  NP VP
This says that the category S is composed of an NP followed by a VP. The
constituents of NPs and VPs are discussed in our chapter on Phrases. The
NP in (4) functions as the subject of its clause while the VP functions as the
predicate. We deal with subjects in our Subjects section and with predicates
in our Basic Clause Patterns section later in this chapter. As we go along, we
will introduce complexities to the diagramming systems as needed.
In contrast to the expressions in (2), those in (5) are not clauses.
(5)
a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
twittered
all the birds of the neighborhood
approves of the decision
over here
when in the course of human events
The fact that a group of words has a certain length or can be understood
in some context is inadequate to define a clause. (5b), for example, contains
more words than (2a,d, or e). Likewise, over here can be understood if one
imagines a context.
Clause patterns provide the basic skeletons of English sentences. Full
sentences consist of clause patterns either minimally or extensively devel323
Delahunty and Garvey
oped—through expansion of their component phrases and/or by including
more than one clause.
In the following pages, we will consider first the subject function in ways
that will apply to all basic clause patterns. We will indicate the fundamental forms of subjects and then their meanings, stated in terms of semantic
roles.
We will then turn to the various types of predicates, pointing out their
functional and formal characteristics. Since different patterns have differing
types of objects and complements, we will describe each pattern in a way that
expands slightly on the simple subject + predicate division.
subjects
The traditional definition of subject is “what the sentence is about,” (a.k.a. the
topic of the sentence). A traditional grammarian would say that sentence (6)
is about Oscar.
(6) Oscar willed Elmer his worm‑farm.
What it says about him is that he willed Elmer his worm-farm. Predicates,
from a traditional point of view, complete a sentence by saying something
about its subject. This function is sometimes referred to as the comment of
the sentence.
Subjects tend to refer to entities that are assumed to be already familiar to
the hearer; they often represent what has been variously referred to as “known,”
“old,” or “given” information. Predicates generally contain the “new” information in a sentence. The traditional definition of subject is neither a formal,
functional, nor semantic one. Rather, it defines a subject in terms of how
the sentence in which it appears relates to the ongoing play of meaning in a
discourse. Unfortunately, unless we have a way to accurately identify what a
sentence is about, this definition is unusable. And even when we can reliably
identify what a sentence is about, the definition may give incorrect results, as
in (7):
(7) And speaking of subjects, we should identify them formally.
In (7), given the appropriate context, we might argue that And speaking of is
a marker of topichood and so the topic of (7) is the phrase subjects, though
subjects is not the subject of the sentence.
The shift from grammar to discourse is particularly serious when we look
at more complicated sentences, such as (8).
324
Basic Clause Patterns
(8) Whenever you feel like raking those leaves, go ahead and do it,
because I won’t rake them.
What is the topic of this sentence? Raking the leaves? If so, it isn’t a subject.
If only subjects can qualify as topics, then either you or I or both must be its
topics. Can a sentence have two topics? The traditional definition doesn’t say.
Is every subject a topic? The traditional definition doesn’t say. If every subject is not a topic, how do you identify the topic? The traditional definition
doesn’t say. In short, one cannot identify topics of sentences out of context,
and when we examine sentences in context, the topics may well turn out not
to be subjects. Thus the traditional discourse‑based criterion for establishing a
grammatical category reflects a mistaken notion of grammatical criteria. And
it just doesn’t work.
Exercise
Using only the traditional definition of subject, try to identify the
whole subjects of the following sentences:
a. Oscar closed the door.
b. Amanda helped herself to the nectarines.
c. One usually takes a long time to recover from a back injury.
d. No one understands me.
e. Advantage was taken of the loophole by the cabinet ministers.
f. It is clear that power breeds corruption.
g. It rained yesterday.
h. There will be more rain tomorrow.
What kinds of problems did you run into? Besides the definition, what
assumptions did you have to make?
Identifying a subject
In this section we present a formal characterization of subject to replace the
definition based on discourse function. We can provide a more accurate and
more general characterization by using the position, agreement patterns, and
case markings of subjects in clauses. Consider the following sentences:
(9)
a.
b.
c.
d.
I am at home.
You are at home.
He/she/it is at home.
We/you/they are at home.
325
Delahunty and Garvey
There would be universal agreement that the subjects of the clauses in (9) are
the pronouns, I, you, etc. From these simple clauses we can derive a general
pattern, namely that an English subject typically occurs more or less immediately before the verb of its clause.
Exercise
Using only the fact that an English subject occurs more or less immediately before the verb(s) of its clause, identify the whole subject of
each of the following sentences:
a. Oscar made most of the children laugh most of the time.
b. Many deer are killed on the roads each year.
c. It is raining.
d. In spite of his stature, Tom Thumb ran for election to high office.
e. As for TV bloopers, they should be left on the cutting room floor.
f. Bill, with great skill and daring, quickly extricated himself from
the web of intrigue.
g. Rarely have I been so disgusted.
h. There are a number of rhetorical problems here.
What problems did you run into, and how did you solve them?
From our discussion of pronouns in our chapter on Minor Parts of
Speech, we hope you remember that pronouns, if they replace anything,
replace entire noun phrases. It follows that if we replace the pronouns in (9)
with more complex NPs, then those more complex NPs (bolded) must also
be the subjects of their clauses.
(10) a.
b.
c.
d.
Olive is at home.
Olive and Popeye are at home.
The one you’re looking for is at home.
All the little children are at home.
Many school grammars, writing manuals, and composition textbooks
identify the head of the subject phrase as the subject of the clause. Some
texts are more careful and distinguish the simple subject from the whole
subject. By simple subject they mean the head of the subject phrase. Logically, however, if a pronoun is the subject of a clause, then any expression,
regardless of how complex, that replaces the pronoun must also be the sub326
Basic Clause Patterns
ject of that clause. We will follow the practice of linguists: when we speak
of the subject of a clause, we mean the entire subject. If we wish to refer to
the head of the subject, then we will speak of the head of subject. We will
not use the terms simple subject nor will we ever use the term subject to
refer just to head of the subject. Similar remarks apply to predicates, objects,
complements, and modifiers. So, please, when we ask you to identify the
subject, object, etc. in an expression, identify the entire phrase, not just its
head word.
Notice now that as we changed the subject of the clauses in (9), we also
changed the form of the verb. Thus, I goes with am; we/you/they with are;
he/she/it with is. When two (or more) parts of an expression are mutually
dependent in this way they are said to agree with each other; that is, when
one is altered the other must also be altered.
Notice too that in order to create grammatical sentences our subjects
must agree with their verbs. If they don’t, the resulting strings of words are
not well‑formed standard English sentences:
(11) a. *I is at home. (grammatical in some varieties)
b. *We/you/they am at home.
c. *He/she/it are at home.
The verb be is the most morphologically complex verb in English. It has
more forms than other verbs (see the chart in our chapter on Phrases), and
so shows the agreement between subject and verb most clearly. But a reduced
version of this agreement pattern can be seen also in other verbs:
(12) a. I/you/we/they/Bill and Molly like rutabagas.
b. He/she/it/Fred likes rutabagas.
Most verbs have only two forms in the present tense, one that ends in
‑s and another that has no ending. The ‑s form occurs with third person
singular subjects. The uninflected form occurs with all other subjects. So, in
general, English subjects must agree with their verbs, as well as occur before
them.
We can use these characteristics of subjects to determine just which of
several phrases in a clause is its subject. Suppose, for example, that we have a
sentence in the past tense with several NPs in it, and we wish to decide just
which of these is the subject. Now, the ‑s marker does not occur in the past
tense; there are no verbs of the form *likeds in English, and, except for be,
the English past tense shows no indication of agreement. So to observe the
327
Delahunty and Garvey
agreement pattern we must change the past tense verb to the present tense.
Then we can systematically change the NPs in the sentence and observe
whether we must also change the verb. When we find the one (and there will
be only one) NP that forces us to change the verb in order to create a grammatical sentence, we will have found the subject of the sentence. Consider:
(13) a. I liked your poem.
Change (13a) to present tense:
(13) b. I like your poem.
Remember that only a third person singular subject requires the ‑s ending
on the verb. Note that your poem is third person. If your poem were the subject, the verb would be likes. Therefore your poem is not the subject. I is not
third person singular, but if we change it to She we have to change the verb
to likes to maintain grammaticality.
(13) c. She likes your poem.
We can conclude that She is the subject of (13c), and because She replaced I in
(13b), I must be the subject of (13b) and also of (13a).
Exercise
Using only subject-verb agreement (that is, change the NPs and observe whether the verb form must also change), identify the whole
subject in each of the following sentences.
a. He eats a bagel every morning.
b. She sees her dentist at least twice a year.
c. Gasoline costs a fortune these days.
d. He sent his mother flowers for Mother’s Day.
e. Chickens cross roads.
f. There are several cookies in the box.
g. It is raining.
What problems did you encounter? How did you solve them?
Another way to determine the subject of a sentence is to replace all its NPs
with pronouns. The NP that can only be replaced by a pronoun in the nomi328
Basic Clause Patterns
native case form will be the subject. Thus:
(14) a. The man handed the child to the girl.
When we replace each NP with an appropriate pronoun we get:
(14) b. He handed him to her.
The only nominative pronoun is he, which replaced the man, so the man must
be the subject of (14a).
Exercise
Using only the case of pronouns, identify the whole subject of each of
the following sentences:
a. Oscar lies beautifully.
b. Oscar wrote searingly witty plays.
c. Mary sent her sister to the movies.
d. The books, Mary put on the shelves.
e. Our bikes were stolen by the Over-the-Hill Gang.
f. It is raining.
What problems did you encounter? How did you solve them?
It is important to have a variety of ways of identifying subjects, because it
is not always easy to identify them in a specific sentence. For example, more
than one NP may occur before the verb in a sentence:
(15) Bill, Fred likes.
This is called a topicalized sentence. The first NP, Bill, is not the subject,
as we can see by substituting pronouns and observing their case markings:
(16) a. Him, he likes.
b.*He, he likes.
c. *He, him likes.
d. *Him, him likes.
(16a) is the only grammatical reformulation of (15), and as he is the only
nominative pronoun, it must be the subject of (16a); and as it is Fred that is
329
Delahunty and Garvey
replaced by the nominative pronoun, Fred must be the subject of (15).
Fred is also the subject by the agreement test. If we replace Fred by a first
person form, we are forced to change the verb to like. This does not happen if
we change Bill to a first person form.
(17) a. Bill, I like.
b.*Bill, I likes.
(18) a. Me, Fred likes.
b. *Me, Fred like.
As we noted elsewhere, the more tests we have to support an analysis, the
more confident we can be in that analysis, and it is especially satisfying when
all the tests give the same result.
Exercise
For each of the following sentences, identify its (entire) subject, using
whichever of our criteria are most appropriate and convincing:
a An afternoon nap is a must.
b.My bookstore just ran out of comics.
c. In the beginning, there was chaos.
d.Things aren’t any better now.
e.Bill seems to have gone ahead.
f. Margaret has been awarded a fellowship.
g. There is a house in New Orleans.
h.It was a blast.
i. It was Jack that built the house.
What criteria did you use in each case? What problems did you encounter? How did you solve them?
s e m a n t i c ro l e s
Now that we have developed ways to identify the subject of a clause, let’s
examine another traditional definition of subject: the subject represents the
doer of the action. This characterization sometimes helps:
(19) a. The eagle swallowed a trout.
b. Jesse dismissed her campaign manager.
c. Abercrombie embezzled $1,000,000.
330
Basic Clause Patterns
However, as a general characterization of the subject, it will not do. We saw
in our chapter on Major Parts of Speech that not all verbs denote actions.
For example, be, belong, become, seem, ache, know, and own denote states.
How then can we use the definition to identify the subjects in (20)?
(20) a.
b.
c.
d.
That sculpture belongs to the Art Institute.
Egworm seems moody today.
My sinuses ache.
Who owns the earth?
The situation is even more complex than this, because even verbs that
do denote actions may have subjects that do not denote the doer of that action (assuming that a doer is a person or at least an animate entity that does
something):
(21) a.
b.
c.
d.
The keys opened the door.
Fred received a letter from the IRS.
The storm knocked out the power lines.
The heavy oaken door opened silently.
In this section, we define a set of terms developed by linguists to describe
the semantic roles of subjects, as well as of objects and other phrases in
clauses. Consider the sentences:
(22) a. John broke the windshield.
b. John approached Mary.
John is the subject of both sentences in (22), and in traditional grammar
would have been defined as the “doer” of the actions of breaking the windshield or approaching Mary. Glossing the subject in this way is an attempt
to provide a general statement of the semantic relation between the subject
and the verb in an indefinite number of sentences. Modern linguists have
attempted to give a more precise characterization of this relationship. They
would say that John is the Agent of these two sentences. Agent is defined as
the animate instigator of the action denoted by a verb.
The term Agent contrasts with other terms in a set of semantic roles that
may be assigned to subjects and other grammatical relations. Compare the
sentences of (22) with the sentences of (23):
(23) a. The hail broke the windshield.
331
Delahunty and Garvey
b. The wind knocked down the power lines.
In (23a), The hail cannot be the Agent of the action denoted by the
verb. This is because hail is inanimate, and so cannot be agentive by our
definition. We will refer to the inanimate cause of an event as the Force.
While a Force cannot be animate, it must have its own potency.
To insist on distinguishing Agent from Force is not to play a mere
terminological game lacking empirical consequences. To appreciate the
difference between Agent and Force in sentences (22) and (23), consider
what happens when we add adverbs of willfulness to the sentences.
(24) a. John deliberately broke the windshield.
b. John deliberately approached Mary.
(25) a.*The hail deliberately broke the windshield.
b.*The wind deliberately knocked down the power lines.
(24a,b) are perfectly innocuous sentences requiring no special interpretation. (25a,b), on the other hand, can only be interpreted if we personify hail
and wind.
Consider now:
(26) John is in the kitchen.
In (26), John, although animate, is in no sense the “doer” or instigator of an
action, and therefore is not an Agent. We will refer to the semantic relationship that John bears in (26) as the Theme of the sentence. Theme is the NP
referring to the entity whose movement, existence, location, or state is predicated.
For example, the italicized phrases below are Themes:
(27) a.
b.
c.
d.
The balloon floated into the sky.
The king is in his counting house.
Elves no longer exist.
Frederika is very tall.
The movement or location may be metaphorical:
(28) a. Harold went from bad to worse.
b. Susan is in a foul mood.
c. Leslie weighs 145 lbs.
332
Basic Clause Patterns
Consider now the roles played by the italicized noun phrases in:
(29)John is currently in Turkey walking along the Dardanelles on his
way from Pakistan to Malta.
Here John is the Theme, as it refers to the entity whose movement is in
question; Turkey is his Location; the Dardanelles is his Path; Pakistan is his
Source; and Malta is his Goal.
The Path role is played by the NP referring to the route along which the
referent of the Theme moves. For example:
(30) We left by the rear entrance.
The Location role is played by the phrase that designates the place or state
at or in which the referent of the Theme is at a particular time. For example:
(31) John is in bed/in Boston/in a foul humor/in his evening wear.
The Source role is played by the phrase indicating the location from which
the referent of the Theme moves.
(32) We took the candy from the baby.
The Goal role is played by the phrase that indicates the place or state to
which the referent of the Theme moves.
(33) We sent it to the Pentagon.
(34)
Source
Goal
John went from New York to New Orleans.
his bed his bath
silly
serious
The Time role is played by a phrase indicating when a situation occurred:
(35) a. Let’s meet at midnight.
b. At dawn the generals led their armies out to battle.
Other semantic roles include:
333
Delahunty and Garvey
Experiencer: the animate entity inwardly or psychologically affected by the
event or state.
(36) a. Henry knows all the answers.
b. We all feel the pain of loneliness occasionally.
Stimulus: the cause of an experiencer’s psychological state.
(37) a. Jack likes Turkey Giblets.
b. Alan is afraid of spiders.
Patient: the animate entity physically affected by the state or event.
(38) a. The speeding car struck Bill a glancing blow.
b. The surgeons operated on her for several hours.
Instrument: the object with which an act is accomplished. Instruments are
usually inanimate and lack their own potency:
(39) a. John opened the door with the crowbar.
b. The crowbar opened the door.
Recipient: the animate being who is the (intended) receiver of the referent of
the Theme.
(40) a. Some students give teachers gifts.
b. Teachers sometimes get gifts from their students.
Benefactive: the animate being affected (positively or negatively) by the occurrence denoted by the verb.
(41) a. I cut the grass for my grandmother.
b. I baked Sandy a birthday cake.
Effected/Factitive: the entity that comes into existence by virtue of the event
denoted by the clause.
(42) a. Frankenstein created a monster.
b. Those two wrote this book.
334
Basic Clause Patterns
Attribute: a status, property, or characteristic ascribed to some entity.
(43) a. Bullwinkle is the game warden.
b. The people elected Barack Obama President of the US.
The game warden is a status ascribed to Bullwinkle by virtue of the state of
being denoted by is in (43a), and President of the US is attributed to Barack
Obama by virtue of elected in (43b).
Empty/Expletive: a phrase that does not refer to anything.
(44) a.
b.
c.
d.
It is snowing.
It is six p.m.
I would appreciate it if you turned down the music.
There are a number of issues to be considered.
Typically, NPs with Empty semantic roles are either it or there. Because
they are semantically vacuous, these NPs cannot sensibly be questioned:
(45) a. *What is snowing?
b. *What is six p.m.?
c. *What would you appreciate if I turned down the music?
d. *Where/what are a number of issues to be considered?
Let’s look now at the kinds of semantic roles that subjects can play. Subjects can play most, if not all, of the roles we have mentioned.
(46) a. The horse bucked the rider. (AGENT)
b. The storm knocked out the phone lines. (FORCE)
c. The rider felt the pain. (EXPERIENCER)
d. Spiders freak me out. (Stimulus)
e. He underwent a heart transplant. (PATIENT)
f. Fred is the strongest candidate. (THEME)
g. This key opens the strongbox. (INSTRUMENT)
h. Fred got a birthday kiss from his mom. (RECIPIENT)
i. Oscar had his own cake made for him. (BENEFACTIVE)
j. Man evolved from apes. (FACTITIVE)
k. Texas is where the best hotsauce comes from. (SOURCE)
l. Colorado is where we’re going. (GOAL)
m. Spain is where the rain falls. (LOCATION)
335
Delahunty and Garvey
n. Today is the last day to register. (TIME)
o. It is raining. (EMPTY)
Exercise
Using (a) as a model, identify the (whole) subject and then its semantic
role in each of the following sentences:
a. [Macmillan] gave his wife a ring. AGENT
b. The ring was delivered by a liveried messenger.
c. It had been crafted by a skilled goldsmith.
d. Lightning causes forest fires.
e. Carelessness causes injuries.
f. Plastic is derived from petroleum.
g. There are only a few good tickets left.
h. This project cost me a great deal of time.
What problems did you encounter? How did you solve them?
The preceding discussion should make it clear how misleading it is to define
the subject in terms of only a single role, such as “doer of an action.” The
exercise should give you an indication of how to adequately assign the roles
in specific instances.
As we progress through the various sentence patterns, we will take the
subject for granted, except for instances in which its form or semantic role
helps us to understand the pattern.
a u x i l i a r y ve r b s
Before we deal with the various basic clause patterns, we must discuss a
characteristic that all patterns have in common—their capacity to include
auxiliary verbs such as be, have, do and the modal verbs can, could, may,
might, shall, should, will, would, must. As we noted in other chapters, these
occur before the main verb of the clause:
(47) a. Bill may/must/might leave.
b. Bill is leaving.
c. Bill has left.
d.*Bill left has.
The modal verbs and do are followed by a verb in its infinitival form; the
progressive be is followed by a verb ending in ‑ing; and the perfective have is
336
Basic Clause Patterns
followed by a verb in its past participle form.
The passive be must be mentioned here, too. Unlike the progressive be, it
is followed by a past participle:
(48) Bill was followed by the FBI.
A clause may contain several auxiliary verbs:
(49) Bill may have been being followed by the FBI.
But they will always occur in the order:
(50) (Modal) (Perfective have) (Progressive be) (Passive be)
Each auxiliary is enclosed in parentheses because each is optional. and
each clause may contain from zero to four auxiliary verbs.
Although the order of auxiliary verbs is invariant, the position of the first
auxiliary verb with respect to the subject of its clause depends upon the type
of clause involved. In indicative clauses, it occurs between the subject and
the verb phrase. In interrogatives, the first auxiliary is placed to the left of
the subject. In negative clauses, the negator, not, is placed immediately after
the first auxiliary verb. Compare the indicative (a), interrogative (b), and
negative (c) clauses below:
(51) a.
b.
c.
(52) a.
b.
c.
(53) a.
b.
c.
(54) a.
b.
c.
(55) a.
b.
c.
Bill must leave.
Must Bill leave?
Bill must not leave.
Bill is leaving.
Is Bill leaving?
Bill is not leaving.
Bill has left.
Has Bill left?
Bill has not left.
The postcard was mailed yesterday.
Was the postcard mailed yesterday?
The postcard was not mailed yesterday.
Bill should have been being followed by the FBI.
Should Bill have been being followed by the FBI?
Bill should not have been being followed by the FBI.
337
Delahunty and Garvey
Because the placement of the first auxiliary verb is affected by whether its
clause is interrogative or indicative, we place it in a special phrase, which we
call AUX, for auxiliary. Because every clause may include auxiliary verbs, we
include AUX in all the formal patterns that we present below. We will deal
in more depth with the placement of multiple auxiliaries in our chapter on
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns.
tense and aspect
What we are calling “basic clauses” here are sentences consisting of a single
clause, which we referred to as “simple sentences” in our Skeleton of English
Grammar chapter.
All English main clauses and many types of subordinate clause must be
marked for tense and may be marked for aspect. We briefly introduce these
two grammatical categories here and discuss tense more thoroughly in the
Appendix to this chapter.
Tense
Both tense and aspect involve reference to time. Time is a continuum on
which events succeed each other from the past through the present to the
future. The past is prologue and the future is the yet-to-be-written postscript.
English allows many ways to refer to time. In John Kennedy was assassinated at
12:30 p.m. on November 22, 1963, for example, the PPs, at 12:30 p.m. and on
November 22, 1963, identify the time of day and the date of Kennedy’s assassination. The time is given in terms of the two twelve hour periods into which
we divide days; the date is given in terms of the day of the month and the year
in the western calendar.
Tense is the set of grammatical categories that languages use to relate the
time of the situation denoted by the clause to the time at which the clause
is said or written. Grammatical categories, as we have seen, are represented by
elements of the grammar of a language rather than by its words. English grammatically distinguishes three tenses: past, present, and future. The past and
present tenses are indicated by inflectional morphemes: the regular past tense
is indicated by {-ed}, though there are many irregular forms, such as was/were,
had, did, won, bought. The regular present tense has no marking, unless the
subject of its clause is third person singular, in which case the verb takes the
{-s} inflection, though, again, there are a few exceptions, such as is, has. The
future is typically indicated by a modal verb, especially by will. For example:
(56) a. She liked her linguistics classes. [Past tense]
b. She likes her linguistics classes. [Present tense]
338
Basic Clause Patterns
c. She will like her linguistics classes. [Future tense]
If there is no auxiliary verb in a clause, the tense is marked on the main
verb, as in (56a,b). If the clause does contain an auxiliary, the tense will be
marked on the first auxiliary. Will in (56c) is in the present tense; its past
tense is would. Compare the present and past tense forms of the auxiliaries
in:
(57) a. Oscar is/was playing bridge.
b. Oscar has/had dealt the cards.
Exercise
To convince yourself that only the first auxiliary verb in a sequence may
be marked for tense, observe what happens when you move the tense
marking from the first to the other verbs in The pie might have been
eaten by the cat, e.g., *The pie may had been eaten by the cat. You
should find that placing the tense marker on any verb other than the
first one results in ungrammaticality.
Very simplistically, the past tense indicates that the situation represented by
the clause occurred prior to the time at which the clause is uttered; the present
tense indicates that the situation represented by the clause overlaps the time
at which the clause is uttered; and the future tense indicates that the situation
is spoken about prior to when it occurs (if it ever does). For instance, in (58),
note the correlations between the time adverbs, yesterday, today, and tomorrow,
and the verb forms in the clauses they belong to:
(58) Yesterday, Oscar inherited a million dollars; today he owns a
Testarossa; tomorrow he will be broke again.
While these tense forms are quite simple, their actual uses are quite
complex, and we deal with them more fully in the Appendix to this chapter.
Exercise
The following text is from Edith Wharton’s The House of Mirth
(1905/1989: 10-11). (a) Identify all the tense forms in the text. (b)
339
Delahunty and Garvey
Discuss the systematic difference between the use of past and present
tense forms in the passage. Why do you think the author used the two
tenses in this way? What would happen to the text if you were to rewrite it so that all the present tense forms were changed to past tense
forms, and vice versa? Why would that be so?
She began to saunter about the room, examining the bookshelves between puffs of her cigarette smoke. Some of the volumes
had the ripe tints of good tooling and old morocco, and her eyes lingered on them caressingly, not with the appreciation of an expert, but
with the pleasure in agreeable tones and textures that was one of her
inmost susceptibilities. Suddenly her expression changed from desultory enjoyment to active conjecture, and she turned to Selden with a
question.
“You collect, don’t you—you know about first editions and
things?”
“As much as a man may who has no money to spend. Now and
then I pick up something in the rubbish heap; and I go and look at
the big sales.”
She had again addressed herself to the shelves, but her eyes now
swept them inattentively, and he saw that she was preoccupied with a
new idea.
“And Americana—do you collect Americana?”
Selden stared and laughed.
“No, that’s rather out of my line. I’m not really a collector, you
see; I simply like to have good editions of the books I am fond of.”
She made a slight grimace. “And Americana are horribly dull, I
suppose?”
Aspect
Aspect is the set of grammatical devices that languages use to categorize
situations according to such characteristics as occupying an expanse of time,
taking only an instant of time, being repeated, and being complete. Although tense is probably much more familiar to you than aspect, aspect
occurs more frequently than tense in the world’s languages.
English uses the auxiliary verbs be and have and the idiom used to to create three grammatical forms that are regarded as aspects: the progressive, the
perfect, and the habitual, respectively. We distinguish between the (a), (b),
340
Basic Clause Patterns
and (c) forms in (59) and between (60a) and (60b):
(59) When we arrived,
a. he made sandwiches.
b. he was making sandwiches.
c. he had made sandwiches.
(60) a. He cycled to work.
b. He used to cycle to work.
The clause in (59a) has a simple past tense form, but is not marked for
aspect. The bolded elements in (59b) (a form of the verb be followed by a
verb ending in {-ing}) mark the progressive aspect. Sentences like (59c) are
said to be in the perfect aspect, characterized by a form of have followed by a
past participle (Ven) (bolded). Sentence (60b) illustrates the habitual aspect,
indicated by the expression used to followed by an uninflected verb (bolded).
The auxiliaries be and have of the progressive and perfect aspects can be either
past or present tense. These aspects can occur together in sentences:
(61) a. He has been making sandwiches for over an hour.
b. He used to be writing a book.
Sentence (61a) combines perfect and progressive; (61b) combines habitual
and progressive.
Tenses and modals can also combine with the progressive and perfect aspects. Had in (62) indicates both past tense and perfect aspect; been making
indicates the progressive. (63) combines the present tense modal, may, with
the perfect, have, and the progressive, been making.
(62) He had been making sandwiches for hours by the time we arrived.
(63) He may have been making bombs, for all we know.
While tense links the situation represented by a sentence to the time at
which the sentence is uttered, aspect represents features of the temporal structure of the situation the sentence describes. The progressive aspect characterizes an event as (a) enduring for a period of time, (b) temporary, and (c) not
necessarily complete.
The perfect indicates that a situation that obtained in the past is still relevant at some later time. If the sentence is a present perfect (e.g., I have lived
here for seven years), the relevant later time is the time of utterance. If the sen341
Delahunty and Garvey
tence is a past perfect (e.g., I had met him several times by 2004), the relevant
later time is prior to the time of utterance and may be specified by an adverbial; 2004 has this function in the current example. If the sentence is a future
perfect (e.g., I will have lived here for 30 years by 2010), the relevant time is
later than the present and may be specified by an adverbial such as by 2010.
The habitual aspect indicates that a situation continued for such an extended period of time in the past that it can be taken as a characteristic of
the entire period. We explore the complexities involved in interpreting these
forms in more detail below.
Progressive aspect
We begin by illustrating three features of the interpretation of the progressive aspect. The form indicates that the event is viewed as involving a period,
rather than merely a point of time:
(64) The jet changed direction.
(65) The jet is changing direction.
The first of these two sentences is neutral with respect to whether the change of
direction is sudden or gradual. The second sentence characterizes the change
of direction as taking time. How much time is not at issue.
The situation represented in the progressive aspect is viewed as temporary
rather than permanent, as illustrated in (66) and (67).
(66) I live with my parents.
(67) I am living with my parents.
The progressive sentence is readily compatible with a continuation such as,
while my own house is being rebuilt. The simple present is not quite so natural
with that continuation.
The progressive suggests that the situation it represents is not necessarily
complete, as (68) and (69) show.
(68) The man died.
(69) The man was dying.
The latter sentence is compatible with a continuation that indicates that the
dying process was never completed: but we managed to save his life. The former
is not.
The progressive frequently represents a situation during which another
342
Basic Clause Patterns
situation occurs:
(70) The burglar was leaving by the rear window when/as the police
arrived.
It is also interpreted as indicating repeated action when its main verb represents events as taking only a point of time. Compare the following:
(71) John is hitting his carpet.
(72) John is vacuuming his carpet.
Hit is understood as taking only a point of time; vacuum as requiring a period.
So a natural interpretation of the first is that John repeatedly hits his carpet;
the second most naturally represents continuous activity, not repeated vacuumings.
The final issue we raise in regard to the progressive has to do with the
classes of verbs that can occur in this aspect. We noted in our chapter on Major Parts of Speech that all verbs except those that refer to states can appear as
progressives. Thus verbs representing events, whether momentary (hit, wink),
transitional (arrive, leave), action (drink, type), or process (grow, widen) occur
with the progressive. State verbs, which include verbs of perception (hear, see),
cognition (know, understand), or having and being (be, contain, own) are not
compatible with the progressive.
There are however, certain state verbs that appear to be exceptions to this
generalization. One can say, for example, John is being silly. When we view
sentences such as this from the perspective we have developed we can see
that it really is not particularly exceptional. Compare it to John is silly. The
latter sentence suggests that silliness is a more or less permanent or typical
characteristic of John, while the former suggests merely that while John is
currently silly, he is not necessarily typically or permanently so. If we were
to paraphrase the former sentence, we would probably use a verb such as act:
John is acting silly. So it appears that be in this kind of sentence is interpreted
as an activity rather than a state verb. We saw this kind of recategorization
of words earlier in our discussion of mass and count nouns.
Exercise
1. For each of the following sentences, indicate the meaning of the
progressive by selecting the appropriate choice in parentheses. (One of
these sentences raises questions about the generalizations above. Can
343
Delahunty and Garvey
you find it?)
a. The plane is taxiing. (The action does/does not take a period of
time.)
b. I watch television. (Is/is not compatible with continuation “while
my clothes are drying.”)
c. I was watching television. (Is/is not compatible with continuation
“while my clothes were drying.”)
d. I am earning $4.75 an hour. (Suggests permanence/impermanence.)
e. (1) As the beast advanced, (2) the hikers were planning their
escape route. (Event (1) occurred during event (2)/Event (1) did
not occur during event (2).)
f. Sarah was jumping well at the meet. (Implies repeated jumping/
continuous activity.)
g. Oscar is being a ninny. (Implies that Oscar is a temporary/permanent ninny)
2. Other verbs besides be may be followed by Ving forms, e.g., The
Energizer Bunny keeps on going, and going, and going. The rescuers
continued working through the night. (a) What other verbs can you
think of that take the progressive Ving form? (b) What meaning do
these verbs have in common?
3. The following text is adapted from “A New List for a New Year,”
an article by David Noonan, Newsweek (January 21, 2008 p. 18). The
article is about making a list of illnesses besetting oneself and one’s
friends. (a) Identify all the instances of the progressive in the text,
making sure not to be misled by non-progressive Ving forms and progressives with an omitted but understood wh-word and form of be. (b)
Identify the tense of each of the progressives you listed. (c) List the
different forms of the progressives you identified. (d) Discuss the reasons why the author might have chosen the progressives in this text.
Are they consistent with the discussion of the progressive just above?
I got the idea for my list a couple of days after Christmas, during a memorial service for an old friend who died of Huntington’s
disease. . . . As I sat listening to stories about my friend, I thought
about my brother, John, who was diagnosed with oral cancer in 2005
and who is now doing fine. I also thought about my father, who beat
colon cancer 22 years ago; a younger member of my extended family
344
Basic Clause Patterns
who is living with bipolar disorder, and a friend facing the daily challenge of hepatitis C. You see where this is going. . . .
With my list in hand, I’m working toward a new way of thinking about health and disease. Illness is the rule, not the exception.
That may sound depressing, but if I accept the idea—and how can I
not when I read all those names?—then I have to do more than wait
around for the next dreaded phone call. . . .
Another thing I can do is be more useful to the people on my
list. Like most folks, I usually make it a point not to think about
a friend’s or relative’s health problems when I’m hanging out with
him. But talking can be a good thing. It may not be easy—for either
party—but there’s no doubt that sharing the burden of an illness can
be beneficial.
Perfect aspect
The perfect aspect represents an earlier situation as being relevant in some way
at a later time, so it must be distinguished from the simple past tense. Situations that continue right up to the time of reference can be viewed in this way,
as can very recent situations:
(73) I have been a taxpayer since 1980.
(74) By 2005, I had been a taxpayer for 23 long years.
In (73) the time of reference is the time at which the sentence is uttered, so the
present perfect is used; in (74) the time of reference is 2005, which is before
“now,” so the past perfect is used. Both sentences imply that the condition of
being a taxpayer continued up to the reference time.
The situation need not be a state or condition. When the verb represents
an event, the sentence may represent repetition of that event. This usually
requires an appropriate adverbial phrase:
(75) We have visited Norway every July for 15 years.
The relevance of the situation represented by the sentence need not be as
clear as the continuation of the situation itself. We have visited Norway suggests that we made at least one visit during the period leading up to the time
of utterance. The situation may also be interpreted as an event that resulted in
a state that continued to the time of reference. The bus has stopped implies that
the bus is now stopped, just as Mother has arrived implies that Mother is now here.
345
Delahunty and Garvey
The relevant time span can also be interpreted as shortly before the reference time, or recently:
(76) I had (just) finished another paper by then.
(77) Have you seen my spectacles (recently)?
Exercise
1.
of
Using the discussion just above, identify what is implied by the use
the perfect in each of the following sentences.
a. Mary has played tennis for seventeen years.
b. Mary has played tennis.
c. Mary has just played tennis.
d. Martin has given a Groundhog Day party for 17 years.
2. Why is the following interchange odd?
Zeke: Have you cooked dinner?
Clem: Yes, about ten years ago.
(What happens if you add ever or yet to Zeke’s question?)
3. The following is an extract from a Scientific American article by Rob
Dunn, (December 2007: 46) on the surprising speed at which evolution
can occur. (a) Identify all the instances of the perfect aspect in the passage. You should find that almost all the sentences in the passage are
in the perfect. (b) What is the tense of each perfect clause? (c) Why do
you think the author chose the perfect aspect as basic for this piece?
To help with this last question you might try re-writing the passage
without using the perfect, for example, in the simple past tense, and
comparing the two versions.
We see rapid evolution most often where some force (often us)
has given it a jump start by suddenly and dramatically altering an
organism’s environment. Rats have developed smaller bodies when
introduced to islands. Trophy fish have also adopted smaller body
sizes in response to fishers’ preference for big fish (which, if killed, do
not breed). Mayflies in streams where trout were released now forage
at night to avoid the fast-swimming predators. Many hundreds of
herbivorous species have switched to novel, sometimes toxic, food
sources introduced by humans and have come to specialize in consuming those new resources. Various native species have evolved in
346
Basic Clause Patterns
response to newly arrived competitors. Cedar trees have begun making toxins to protect themselves from being eaten by deer now roaming in their formerly benign habitats. Mussels in New England have
evolved the ability to detect invasive green crabs and produce thicker
shells where the crabs are present.
Habitual aspect
The habitual aspect represents states or habits that are characteristic of an
entire period. The English habitual marker used to is generally understood to
indicate a situation that obtained in the past: We used to have five cats; We used
to drive to work every morning. However, these sentences do not strictly entail
that these situations no longer hold. We could add to either of these sentences
the continuation . . . and in fact we still do.
Note the spelling: it is used to not use to. Used is in its past tense form, as
you would expect for a verb referring to events in the past.
Exercise
Why do we have the tendency to spell used to as use to?
b a s i c c l a u s e p a t t e rn s
The basic clause patterns differ from each other by the type of main verb in
their verb phrases, that is, their predicates. The verb types are differentiated
from each other by the functions and phrases they require to be present or
to be absent in the VP. We resume our use of formal tree diagrams and the
Reed/Kellogg system of functional diagramming.
Basic clause pattern 1: intransitive
The simplest clause pattern corresponds to the functional formula in (78a)
and exemplified in (78b):
(78) a. Basic clause pattern 1
Subject Verb Head
(78) b. Edgar
will speak
Formally, this pattern contains an NP with a VP whose head verb is
intransitive. (We abbreviate this verb as Vi.) The clause can contain no objects or complements. The formal pattern is (79a), whose corresponding tree
diagram is (79b):
347
Delahunty and Garvey
(79) a. NP AUX [VPVi]
(79) b. S
NP
N
Edgar
AUX
will
VP
Vi
speak
Examples of this pattern are:
(80) a. Edgar spoke.
b. Edgar spoke eloquently.
c. Edgar spoke to the crowd.
d. Edgar spoke eloquently to the crowd.
e. Edgar will speak to the crowd this evening.
f. Edgar spoke eloquently to the crowd after the protest march.
g. Edgar spoke eloquently to the crowd after the protest march
had concluded.
h. Edgar will have been speaking for an hour in just a few moments.
Exercise
Identify the whole subject, any auxiliaries, and the whole VP in each of
the sentences in (80).
The greater length and complexity of some of these sentences arise not from
changes in the basic clause pattern of the simpler examples, but because of
choices in auxiliaries and modifiers. Modifiers in the VP and auxiliaries have
no effect on the basic pattern. This fact holds true even when, as in (80g), a
modifier is a clause: the modifying clause naturally has its own pattern (here
also pattern 1), but as a modifier, it is irrelevant to the pattern of the clause
that contains it.
Basic clause pattern 2: simple transitive
If clauses represent propositions, and propositions represent situations, then
many situations involve more than a single essential participant. Some require two, others three, and a few four. The next five clause patterns consist
of ways to add participants represented by various types of objects and/or
complements. Note that all of the clause patterns require a subject, either
348
Basic Clause Patterns
overt or “understood.”
Pattern 2 has the functional structure (81a), exemplified by (81b):
(81) a. Basic clause pattern 2
(81) b. Adam likes ribs
Subject Verb Head Direct Object
The formal pattern, (82a), contains a VP consisting of a transitive verb, (Vt),
and an NP. Its corresponding tree diagram is (82b):
(82) a. NP AUX [VPVt NP]
(82) b.
NP
N
S
AUX
VP
Vt
Adam
likes
NP
N
ribs
By definition any verb that takes an object is transitive. The objects are italicized in the following examples of pattern 2:
(83)a. Adam likes ribs.
b. Eve enjoys apples.
c. The snake held a particularly luscious Granny Smith.
d. Occasionally, Adam would accept small appealing gifts from Eve.
e. Adam likes those who offer something for nothing.
Regardless of the complexity of the direct object NPs in (83), these sentences still represent pattern 2.
A convenient test for this pattern is to replace the NPs with appropriate
pronouns. The result will be of the following pattern:
(84) He/she/it/they ‑ Verb ‑ him/her/it/them
Nominative
Accusative
The nominative pronouns replace the subject NP; the accusative pronouns
replace the object. By this test, sentences (83a) and (83e) both reduce to He
349
Delahunty and Garvey
likes them. As in the case of the intransitive pattern, modifiers and auxiliaries
do not affect the basic pattern.
Another test of objecthood is based on the fact that many clauses of pattern
2 may be passivized. Generally, direct objects may be passivized; modifiers can
never be. The (a) clauses below are the active counterparts of the passive (b)
clauses.
(85) a.
b.
(86) a.
b.
The entire family can enjoy nature movies.
Nature movies can be enjoyed by the entire family.
Multinational corporations exploit poor countries.
Poor countries are exploited by multinational corporations.
The object of the active clause corresponds to the subject of the passive.
You should convince yourself that nature movies and poor countries really are
the subjects of (85b) and (86b), respectively, by applying the tests for subjecthood we developed earlier.
As a final remark on objects, note that most of the semantic roles available
to subjects are available also to objects, so objects may be interpreted in a
wider range of ways than just the traditional “entity affected by the action of
the verb.”
Exercise
(a) Identify the (whole) object in each of the following clauses, then (b)
identify the object’s semantic role:
a. Bill moved the table.
b. Bill made the table.
c. The divorce upset him.
d. The doctor stitched the wound.
e. We use a word processor for our work.
f. The fund drive benefited the local radio station.
g. We left the room.
h. We approached the border.
i. We skied the mountain trails.
j. We would appreciate it greatly if you would leave.
What problems did you encounter? How did you solve them?
Basic clause pattern 3: subject complement
The subject complement construction resembles the direct object pattern in
350
Basic Clause Patterns
having three basic elements. The main difference between the two lies in the
nature of the head verb and the semantic relations it creates. The functional
formula is (87a), exemplified by (87b):
(87) a. Basic clause pattern 3
Subject Verb Head Subject Complement
(87) b. Mary
became
famous
The pattern is slightly complicated by the fact that two different forms
can act as subject complement: a noun phrase or an adjective phrase. The
italicized phrases in (88) and (89) illustrate NP and AP complements, respectively.
(88) a.
b.
c.
(89) a.
b.
c.
Mary became a doctor.
You are a nuisance who ought to be barred from the pool.
He proved a success at ice‑carving.
Mary became famous.
I am quite aware of her foibles.
He proved unwilling to cooperate with my attorney.
The formal formula for pattern 3 is NP AUX [VPV {NP/AP}], which
combines the two formulae, (90a, b):
(90)a. NP AUX [VPV NP]
(90)b. NP AUX [VPV AP]
We represent the VP portions of these formulae as (91a, b):
(91) a. VP
V
(91)
NP
b.
VP
V
AP
NP complements are sometimes called predicate nominals or predicative
nominatives. AP complements are sometimes referred to as predicate adjectives.
A pattern that is helpful in distinguishing subject complements from
objects and modifiers is number agreement with the subject NP. If we make
the subject NP plural, we must also make a subject complement NP plural.
Compare (88a) with:
351
Delahunty and Garvey
(92) a. Mary and Terry became doctors.
b. *Mary and Terry became a doctor.
c. *Mary became doctors.
Because English APs are not marked for number, this test does not work for
AP subject complements.
Meaning can also help identify a subject complement. First, the subject
and the subject complement must denote the same entity. This may be indicated by assigning the subject and the complement the same subscript:
(93) Maryi is a doctori.
Thus Mary and a doctor apply to the same individual. As we noted above,
the semantic role of the complement is Attribute. The subject complement
denotes either a permanent or a temporary status, characteristic, or property
of the subject. Understanding the function in this way will allow you to distinguish pattern 3 from pattern 2, where the NP following the verb group
does not ascribe a characteristic to the subject:
(94) Mary visited a doctor.
Of course, the entire VP assigns a characteristic (of visiting a doctor) to
Mary, but our test applies only to the structure after the verb. In (94) we are
referring to two distinct individuals; in (93) we refer only to one.
Semantics also enters into the identification of subject complements because of the nature of the head verb. We can describe the basic meaning
of the verb in subject complement clauses as BE/BECOME. These are the
primitive notions of state and change of state. (Do not confuse this difference with stative and activity meanings of verbs.) These are the linking
verbs we met in our chapter on Minor Parts of Speech. We list some of them
in Table 1.
be (am, is, are, was, were, be, being, been)
appear
become
feel
go
grow
look
prove
seem
smell
taste
turn
table 1. some linking verbs
352
get
make
sound
Basic Clause Patterns
However, nearly all of these verbs may have meanings other than BE or
BECOME. When they have those other meanings, they may not take a subject complement. In the following examples, the (a) clauses contain subject
complements, and the (b) clauses represent some other pattern.
(95) a.
b.
(96) a.
b.
(97) a.
b.
(98) a.
b.
Hoolihan appeared weak.
Hoolihan appeared. (Pattern 1)
Boris felt sorry.
Boris felt pain. (Pattern 2)
Newton proved unreliable.
Newton proved the theorem. (Pattern 2)
The milk turned sour.
Osgood turned away. (Pattern 1)
We can use passivization to distinguish between VPs containing subject
complements and those containing direct objects. We can often passivize a
direct object (99) but never a subject complement (100).
(99) a. Einstein proved the theory.
b. The theory was proved by Einstein.
(100)a. Einstein proved the better physicist.
b.*The better physicist was proved by Einstein.
Exercise
(a) Identify the (whole) subject complement in each of the following
clauses. (b) Using one appropriate criterion, prove that the phrase you
identified as the subject complement really is the subject complement.
(c) Draw both Reed/Kellogg and structural diagrams for each clause.
a. Elena grew tired.
b. The onions smelled sulphurous.
c. The anchovies tasted fishy and salty.
d. The sax sounded off-key.
Clause pattern 4: object complement
The object complement pattern may be viewed as a combination of pattern 2, the basic transitive pattern, and pattern 3, the subject complement
pattern. Just as pattern 3 includes a subject complement, pattern 4 adds
a complement to its direct object. The functional formula for pattern 4 is
353
Delahunty and Garvey
(101a), exemplified by (101b):
(101)a. Basic clause pattern 4
Subject Verb Head Direct Object Object Complement
(101)b. I
consider
Elvira
weird
The two formal patterns corresponding to (101a) are (102a,b) which we
can abbreviate as (102c):
(102)a. NP AUX [VPV NP NP]
b. NP AUX [VPV NP AP]
c. NP AUX [VPV NP {NP/AP}]
The trees corresponding to the two VPs involved are:
(103)a. VP
(103)b.
VP
V
NP
NP
V
NP
AP
Object complements are similar to subject complements in four respects.
First, an object complement may be formally expressed as either an NP or
as an AP, italicized in:
(104)a.
b.
c.
(105)a.
b.
c.
I consider Elvira a weirdo.
We proclaimed her our champion.
She painted the room a ghastly color.
I consider Elvira weird.
We found her guilty.
She painted the room mauve.
The second feature common to subject and object complements is that
in both, an NP complement must agree with its antecedent (the subject or
the object):
(106) I consider Elvira and Elvis weirdos/*a weirdo.
Here again, because English APs cannot be marked for plural, there can be
no agreement between an AP object complement and the direct object of
its clause.
354
Basic Clause Patterns
To see the third parallel with subject complements, we must observe
the semantic relation between the direct object and its complement. If you
consider carefully the sentences in (104) and (105), you will notice that
the semantic relations between the object and its complement are BE and
BECOME. For instance, in (104c) the room becomes a ghastly color. Here
again the semantic role associated with the complement is Attribute.
The final similarity between subject and object complements is that the
complement phrase and the subject or object to which it is semantically
linked refer to the same entity or entities. We indicate this by identically
subscripting the object and the complement:
(107)a. NP AUX [VPV NPi NPi]
b. NP AUX [VPV NPi APi]
Object complements have one further defining trait, the meaning of the
head verb. Examine Table 2 to see if you can identify any semantic common
denominators.
appoint
declare
imagine
call
designate
make
choose
elect
name consider
find
paint
prove
table 2. typical object complement verbs
The two semantic classes that unite most of these words are CONSIDER
TO BE (consider, imagine, think) and CAUSE TO BECOME (most of the
others), illustrated by the examples in (108) and (109), respectively.
(108)a.
b.
(109)a.
b.
We find his conclusion ridiculous.
They called each other liars.
The president named him Secretary of the Bubblegum Department.
The children painted all the walls kelly green.
Object complement verbs, just like subject complement verbs, have a variety of meanings, not all of them compatible with object complements. The
(a) version of each clause below contains an object complement; the (b)
versions contain the same verb in a different pattern.
(110)a. We declared Woople the winner.
b. We declared a holiday. (Pattern 2)
(111)a. Scott and Zelda painted the town red.
355
Delahunty and Garvey
b. Scott and Zelda painted the door. (Pattern 2)
(112)a. Weskin’s false predictions proved him a hoax.
b. Weskin’s false predictions proved his undoing. (Pattern 3)
There are many idiomatic constructions that involve an AP object complement. Some of these appear in Table 3, where X denotes a variable direct
object. (This should be familiar to you from our discussion of object complements as VP in our chapter on Phrases.) As will be clear, nearly all these expressions indicate the notion of CAUSE TO BECOME.
cut the story short cut X short
drain X dry
make X plain
leave X clean
make X clear
make X possible
pack X tight
push X open
put X straight
shake X free
slam X shut
table 3. some idiomatic object complement expressions
Exercise
(a) Identify the (whole) object and object complement phrases in each
of the following clauses. (b) Using one appropriate criterion, prove that
the phrase you identified as the object complement really is an object
complement. (c) Draw both Reed/Kellogg and structural diagrams for
each clause.
a. Oscar slammed the door shut.
b. The president appointed Wilson Secretary of the Navy.
c. Sharon painted her room lime green.
d. The preacher declared them husband and wife.
Basic clause pattern 5: double object (indirect object and direct
object)
This basic clause pattern again involves a head verb followed by two functions, as in:
(113)a. Subject Verb Head Indirect Object Direct Object
b. William gave Susie roses
c. William gave roses
Susie
356
Basic Clause Patterns
As the formula indicates, the last two functions occur in this order: indirect object (IO)—direct object (DO). (Recall that in pattern 4 the DO appeared immediately after the verb head.) Formally, both objects are typically
NPs. The formal version of pattern 5 is:
(114)a. NP AUX [VPV NP NP]
(114)b.
V
VP
NP
NP
In each of the following examples the IO is italicized:
(115)a.
b.
c.
d.
Willard gave Susie roses.
The eighteenth century brought England great prosperity.
She paid her creditors a part of the debt.
Oscar made his friend a beautiful desk.
In patterns 4 and 5, a verb may be followed by two NPs. These two structures can be readily distinguished. In pattern 4, the two NPs refer to a single
entity; in pattern 5, each NP refers to a separate entity.
As usual, semantics plays a role in the pattern, both in the nature of
the verb and in the semantic role of the indirect object. The verb has the
prototypical meaning of GIVING or of BENEFITING. In (115a,b,c),
the notion of giving is clear; (115d) illustrates the meaning of benefiting.
On these semantic grounds, we can identify certain verbs that take indirect objects. We list a typical sample in Table 4.
allow buy
hand
pardon
sell
teach
ask
deny
leave
pay
send
tell
assign
forbid
lend
refund
show
throw
bequeath
forgive
make
refuse sing
write
bring
grant
owe
remit
spare
table 4. some double object verbs
The list in Table 4 hides some complexities. First, most of the verbs
commonly occur in patterns that do not have explicit indirect objects:
(116)a. Allison asked a profound question. (Pattern 2)
b. Walpole refused. (Pattern 1)
357
Delahunty and Garvey
c. Finkle made a successful legislator. (Pattern 3)
d. We made Portnoy our representative. (Pattern 4)
Second, many verbs not on the list can be understood as having a Beneficiary, especially if they refer to some type of production:
(117) Wanda baked Phyllis a birthday cake.
Third, the action denoted by the verb may involve something that is not
literally “transferred” to a recipient, nor is it always “beneficial”:
(118) Roscoe gave Morgentherp a sound thrashing.
Since the meaning of verbs is so flexible, it is more illuminating to consider the semantic roles assigned to the noun phrases in this pattern. The
subject is likely to be a causer such as an Agent or Stimulus:
(119)a. Anderson bought us souvenirs. (Agent)
b. The accident taught us a bitter lesson. (Stimulus)
Indirect objects tend to be Recipients or Benefactives; direct objects tend
to be Themes. But this is just a tendency, not an absolute restriction. In
(120), for example, the subject is an Agent, the direct object a Factitive, and
the indirect object is a Source.
(120) They asked me an unanswerable question.
There is a relatively reliable test for distinguishing pattern 4 from pattern
5 constructions. We will call this the Dative Test. (Dative is the Latinate
term for the case of indirect objects.) Pattern 5 clauses can generally (though
not always) be paraphrased by a clause pattern in which the order of the two
object NPs is reversed, and a preposition is inserted before the second (corresponding to the IO). To usually indicates Recipient; for usually indicates
Beneficiary. The sentences in (121) paraphrase a selection of those above:
(121)a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
358
Willard gave roses to Susie.
The eighteenth century brought great prosperity to England.
She paid a part of her debt to her creditors.
Warthog built some kitchen shelves for his aunt.
Wanda baked a birthday cake for Phyllis.
Basic Clause Patterns
f. Anderson bought souvenirs for us.
One drawback of the Dative Test is that it does not distinguish between
the Recipient and Goal meanings of the preposition to. Only the former is
relevant in this context, so (122a) reflects pattern 2 with a verb modifier, not
pattern 5:
(122)a. Anderson sent the children to the lake.
b.*Anderson sent the lake the children.
Also, the test will not work with certain fixed indirect object constructions:
(123)a. It cost me a fortune.
b. *It cost a fortune to/for me.
c. He gave me a ring. (ambiguous)
d. He gave a ring to me. (unambiguous)
The object complement construction has no such paraphrase:
(124) *We elected president to/for him.
Moreover, we can apply passive to the IO in pattern 5 clauses, just as we
can to the DO in pattern 4:
(125) I was given the roses by Warthog.
Some varieties of English even allow the direct object of pattern 5 clauses
to be passivized:
(126) The roses were given Susie by Warthog.
The object complement NP in the VP of pattern 4 clauses cannot be passivized:
(127) *President was elected him by the voters.
A final restriction on this pattern is that the direct object NP cannot be a
pronoun:
359
Delahunty and Garvey
(128)a. Wanda baked Phyllis the cake.
b.*Wanda baked Phyllis it.
Exercise
(a) Identify the (whole) direct and indirect object phrases in each of
the following clauses. (b) Using the most appropriate criterion, prove
that the phrase you identified as the indirect object really is the indirect object. (c) Draw both Reed/Kellogg and structural diagrams for
each clause.
a. She gave the dog a bone.
b. We made each of the children a balloon animal.
c. Oscar refused his uncle entry.
d. Wanda sang the babies a lullaby.
Basic clause pattern 6: Recipient/Benefactive
Like patterns 3, 4, and 5, this basic clause pattern includes a head verb followed by two functions:
(129)a. Subject Verb Head Direct Object Recipient/Benefactive
b. Willard
gave
roses
to
Amanda
The formal version of pattern 6 is:
(130)a. NP AUX [VPV NP PP]
(130)b.
V
VP
NP
P
PP
NP
Examples of this pattern include:
(131)a.
b.
c.
d.
360
Willard gave roses to Amanda.
The eighteenth century brought great prosperity to England.
Oscar baked a birthday cake for Amanda.
Oscar pulled the weeds for his friends.
Basic Clause Patterns
While the meanings and semantic roles represented by this pattern are not
identical to those represented by pattern 5, they are similar enough so that
many traditional and school grammars refer to the NP expressing the Recipient or Benefactive roles as an indirect object. From a formal point of
view, this NP is the object of the preposition that governs it, so in keeping
with our formalist preferences, we will restrict the term indirect object to
the NP that occurs directly after the verb and before the direct object in
sentences of pattern 5.
As we noted in our discussion of pattern 5, the two patterns cannot be
viewed simply as variants of each other. That is, we cannot always rephrase a
sentence in one pattern as a sentence in the other pattern. Such substitutions
may either change the meaning or result in ungrammaticality. In particular,
if the direct object of a pattern 6 sentence is a pronoun, as in (132a), then
rephrasing the sentence as a pattern 5 sentence results in an ungrammatical
sentence like (132b):
(132)a. We made it for Oscar.
b.*We made Oscar it.
Sentences such as (133a) are ambiguous. They can describe a telephone
call or a gift of a ring. However, their pattern 6 counterparts, (133b), can
only describe a gift-giving:
(133)a. Oscar gave Amanda a ring.
b. Oscar gave a ring to Amanda.
Exercise
(a) Identify the (whole) Recipient/Benefactive phrase in each of the
following clauses. (b) Using one appropriate criterion, prove that the
phrase you identified as a Recipient/Benefactive phrase really is a Recipient/Benefactive phrase. (c) Draw both Reed/Kellogg and structural
diagrams for each clause.
a. She gave a bone to the dog.
b. We made balloon animals for the children.
c. Oscar wrote the plays for his adoring public.
d. Wanda sang a lullaby for the babies.
361
Delahunty and Garvey
Basic clause pattern 7: intransitive Location and Goal clauses
Sentences like (134a and b) respectively represent an entity as being in a
Location and an entity moving to a Goal (which, of course, is a Location to
or toward which something moves or is moved):
(134)a. The king was in his counting house.
b. The king went to the council chamber.
We can represent these functionally as:
(135) Subject Verb Head Location/Goal
The Location and Goal roles can be represented either as a PP (as in (134a,b)
or as an AdvP, as in:
(136)a. The King is away (on business).
b. The King has gone away.
So they can be represented formally as:
(137)a. NP AUX [VPV {PP/AdvP}]
We can represent these VPs by the trees:
(137)b. VP
V
(137)c. PP
VP
V
AdvP
Basic clause pattern 8: transitive Location and Goal clauses
Sentences like (138) represent someone or something moving an entity to or
toward a Goal (italicized):
(138) Oscar put his bicycle in the laundry room/away.
We can represent these functionally as:
(139) Subject Verb Head Direct Object Location/Goal
And formally as:
362
Basic Clause Patterns
(140)a. NP AUX [VPV NP {PP/AdvP}]
We can represent these VPs by the trees:
(140)b. V
VP
NP
(140)c. PP
V
VP
NP
AdvP
You probably noticed the parallel between patterns 3/4 and patterns 7/8. Patterns 3 and 7 are the intransitive version of patterns 4 and 8. Patterns 3 and 7
represent, respectively, an attribute and a Location/Goal of the subject, while
4 and 8 represent, respectively, an attribute and a Location/Goal of the direct
object.
Note that pattern 8 clauses cannot be rephrased as pattern 5 clauses:
(141) *Oscar put the laundry room his bicycle.
Before we leave these clause patterns, we should briefly discuss just why we
regard them as basic. First, they are all simple sentences; that is, none of them
includes another clause within it. Second, they can all be elaborated by the addition of various types of optional modifiers, such as adverbial phrases. Third,
and most important, is the fact that each pattern is a direct reflection of the
class of verb that heads the verb phrase. In particular, each pattern is a reflection of the semantic roles assigned by the verb to the phrases (if any) in the
predicate. Each pattern is also a reflection of the formal requirements imposed
by its head verb. Some verbs require two NPs, some an NP and a PP, and some
nothing at all. Thus verbs impose both semantic and formal requirements on
the clauses they head.
Exercise
(a) Identify the (whole) Location or Goal phrase in each of the following
clauses. (b) Using one appropriate criterion, prove that the phrase you
identified as a Location/Goal phrase really is a Location/Goal phrase.
(c) Draw both Reed/Kellogg and structural diagrams for each clause.
a. The king is in his counting-house.
b. Eleanor put the eggs in the refrigerator.
c. I left my laptop on the bus.
d. My son eventually put his clothes away.
363
Delahunty and Garvey
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Berk, Lynn M. 1999. English Syntax: From Word to Discourse. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Biber, Douglas, Susan Conrad, and Geoffrey Leech. 2002. Longman Student
Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, UK: Longman.
Brown, Keith and Jim Miller. 1991. Syntax: A Linguistic Introduction to
Sentence Structure. 2nd ed. London, UK: HarperCollins.
Carter, Ronald and Michael McCarthy. 2006. Cambridge Grammar of English.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Celce-Murcia, Marianne, and Diane Larsen-Freeman. 1999. The Grammar
Book: An ESL/EFL Teacher’s Course. 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Heinle and
Heinle, Inc.
Greenbaum, Sidney and Randolph Quirk. 1990. A Student’s Grammar of the
English Language. London, UK: Longman.
Huddleston, Rodney and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar
of the English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____ 2005. A Student’s Introduction to English Grammar. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Jackson, Howard. 1990. Grammar and Meaning: A Semantic Approach to
English Grammar. London, UK: Longman.
Liles, Bruce. 1987. A Basic Grammar of Modern English. 2nd ed. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Noguchi, Rei. 1991. Grammar and the Teaching of Writing: Limits and
Possibilities. Urbana-Champaign: NCTE.
Quirk, Randolph, and Sidney Greenbaum. 1973. A Concise Grammar of
Contemporary English. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
Thornbury, Scott. 2006. Grammar. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Wharton, Edith. 1905/1989. The House of Mirth. New York: Scribner’s.
Yule, George (1998). Explaining English Grammar. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
glossary
agent:
the semantic role that denotes the animate instigator of an action.
agree: a grammatical relationship in which the form of one element (e.g., a
subject) varies with the form of another element (e.g., a verb).
attribute: the semantic role that indicates the status, property, or characteristic ascribed to some entity.
benefactive: the semantic role that indicates an animate being who benefits
(positively or negatively) from an event.
comment: the part of the sentence that says something about the topic. See
364
Basic Clause Patterns
topic.
(also called factitive): the semantic role that indicates the entity
that comes into existence by virtue of an event.
empty/expletive: the semantic role assigned to a phrase that neither refers to
an entity nor denotes an attribute.
experiencer: the semantic role that indicates the animate entity inwardly or
psychologically affected by an event or state.
factitive: See effected.
force: the semantic role that indicates the inanimate cause of an event. Has
its own potency.
goal: the semantic role that indicates the place or state to which something
moves.
instrument: the semantic role that indicates the object (usually inanimate)
with which an act is accomplished. Lacks power of its own.
linking verb: a verb, such as be or become, that serves as the main verb in
basic clause pattern 3; grammatically links subject and subject complement.
location: the semantic role that indicates the place or state at or in which
something is at a particular time.
patient: the semantic role that indicates the animate entity physically affected by the state or event.
predicate adjective: the syntactic function of an adjective phrase that follows a linking verb.
predicate nominal (also called predicate nominative): the syntactic function of a noun phrase that follows a linking verb.
recipient: the semantic role that indicates the animate being who is the
(intended) receiver of the Theme.
semantic role: a semantic relation between a noun phrase and a verb or
other role assignor in a clause.
source: the semantic role that indicates the location from which something
moves.
stimulus: the semantic role that indicates the trigger of an experiencer’s
psychological state.
theme: the semantic role that indicates the entity whose movement, existence, location, or state is predicated by a verb.
time: the semantic role that denotes the time at which an event occurred.
topic: the part of a sentence about which the remainder of the sentence says
something. See comment.
effected
appendix: time and tense
We briefly discussed tense and aspect earlier in this chapter. Here we elabo365
Delahunty and Garvey
rate on our discussion of English tenses. While we believe that every (English) teacher should have a good understanding of the tense system, we feel
that a more detailed knowledge is most essential for teachers responsible for
the education of students for whom English is a second (or later) language.
However, as we ground our discussion of English tenses in their characteristic uses in texts, we believe that all teachers will find information of value
in this appendix.
As we noted, every main clause and many types of subordinate clause in
English are marked for tense. Tense is one way of indicating when the situation represented by the clause occurs. In order to sensibly talk about tense,
we must first talk a little about time and some other ways in which time is
indicated in English.
We conceptualize time as a line stretching indefinitely far into the past and
indefinitely far into the future. The past is behind us and the future before us.
(Note the spatial metaphor here.) The past is time already gone; the future is
time yet to come. However, times are past, present, or future in relation to
some chosen point of time. That point can be either fixed or variable. The
fixed time may be the time of an important cultural event, for example, the
birth of Christ in the western tradition or the birth of Mohammed in the
Muslim tradition. The variable time is typically when an utterance is spoken,
the time of utterance, “now.” However, in narratives and other text types,
“now” is determined within the text.
Identifying the time at which situations occur relative to the fixed point
of time depends on systems for measuring time. We use cyclic occurrences
such as days, months, and years for measurement from the fixed time point.
So, as we write this, the year is the 2008th year following the birth of Christ.
The choice of the beginning year is relatively arbitrary, but some selection
must be made and going back to the Big Bang is a bit inconvenient.
Exercise
1. Do all languages/cultures have seven day weeks? Two day weekends?
Four season years? Twelve month years? Solar years?
2. Do other languages/cultures divide their days into segments similar to
ours?
3. English has a number of ways of expressing dates, some used primarily in the written language and some more likely to occur in the
spoken language. For example, 8/22/07 is a written form. (a) Create
366
Basic Clause Patterns
at least three more dates in this pattern and then fully and accurately
describe the pattern. (b) Create at least three date expressions based
on one other pattern that we use, then fully and accurately describe
that pattern as well. (c) Is this second pattern more likely to occur in
written English than in spoken, or vice versa? Imagining that you are
teaching these patterns to someone who has never encountered them
might help you to articulate them precisely.
4. English has several ways of expressing time, some used primarily in written language and some more likely to occur in the spoken
language. For example, as this is being written it is 1:20 p.m. (a) Create at least three more time expressions of this sort, then fully and
accurately describe this pattern. (b) Create at least three time expressions based on one other pattern that we use, then fully and accurately describe that pattern as well. (c) Is this second pattern more
likely to occur in written English than in spoken, or vice versa? Again,
imagining that you are teaching these patterns to someone who has
never encountered them might help you to articulate them precisely.
We shift our focus now to a discussion of a system of time reference that
employs an implicit and constantly moving reference point. Imagine that
you call the garage where you left your car for repairs to ask if the car is
ready. The receptionist who answers the phone says, The mechanic is working
on it right now. When does now refer to? We can paraphrase the receptionist’s answer as The mechanic is working on it as I speak. So now refers to the
time at which the utterance it occurs in is said.
Exercise
How about then? Does then simply refer to times other than those we
would refer to as now, or is it somewhat more complicated? Make up
some sentences with then and work out the rules for using it.
Many expressions besides now and then depend for their full interpretation on identifying the time at which they are said. They include today,
yesterday, and tomorrow. Today refers to the 24 hour period beginning at
midnight and extending to the next midnight, within which the expression
is said. So if on 6/25/2008 I say I’ll do it today then I am saying that I will
367
Delahunty and Garvey
do it sometime during 6/25/2008. Yesterday refers to the day before the day
in which the expression is said, and tomorrow refers to the day following the
day during which the expression is said. Expressions that depend for their
full interpretation in this way on when (where, and by whom) an expression
is uttered are called deictic expressions and the time (place, and speaker)
are called the deictic centers.
Tenses
It is important to distinguish time from tense. Time is a non-linguistic phenomenon experienced by human beings; tense is a grammatical category,
one of several ways that languages may use to refer to time. English and
many (though by no means all) other languages have grammaticized ways
of indicating temporal characteristics of states of affairs, that is, ways that
have become part of the grammar (rather than the vocabulary) of the language.
A tense is a grammaticized way to indicate when a state of affairs occurs
relative to the time of utterance, that is, the time at which that situation is
spoken about. Generally, past tense refers to a time earlier than the time of
utterance; future tense refers to a time later then the time of utterance; and
present tense refers to a time that is identical to, or more typically, overlaps
the time of utterance.
Past tense
We begin with the English past tense because we think that its regular forms
and meanings are simpler than those associated with the other tenses.
Past tense forms
The regular past tense is formed by adding the suffix {-ed} to the word stem,
for example: delay, delayed; print, printed. This suffix has three main allomorphs: [t], [d], and [Id] or [@d]. {-ed} is pronounced [I/@d] when the
word it is attached to ends in a [t] or [d], e.g., lifted; [t] when it is attached
to a word that ends in a voiceless sound other than [t], e.g. kissed; and [d]
otherwise, e.g., rowed.
The past tense marker {-ed} is spelled in various way. If the word ends in
<e> then only <d> is added, locate: located; if the word ends in a consonant
followed immediately by <y>, change the <y> to <i> and then add <ed>:
empty, emptied; if the word ends in a double consonant then add <ed>:
dress, dressed; but if the word ends in a stressed syllable with a single vowel
followed by a single consonant, then double that consonant and add <ed>:
drop, dropped; prefer, preferred.
368
Basic Clause Patterns
Past tense meanings
The basic meaning of the past tense is very simple. It indicates only that
something was the case in the past, that is, before “now.” If I say John was
in his room, then I am claiming that the description John is in his room held
in the past. It says nothing about whether John is still in his room; he may
or may not be, as can be seen from the fact that the sentence above can be
followed by either and he’s still there or but he’s not there now.
The situation represented as occurring in the past may involve a single
point of time (1a), repetition of events (1b, c), a period of time (1d), or a
state (1e), respectively:
(1)
a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
The clock struck one.
The clock chimed twelve times.
We visited each other every weekend.
The noise droned on and on.
For many years Chris owned a Ferrari.
The past tense is compatible with completed situations and, indeed,
many past tense sentences do denote situations that ended in the past. The
suggestion that the situation was completed in the past is due to a number
of factors, but not to the past tense form itself. For example, if the event
was instantaneous, as in The balloon burst, or if the event has a natural
culmination, such as I wrote a poem, He fell asleep, or He read the book, the
situations will be interpreted as completed. Adverbials can be added to indicate specific lengths of time, for three hours, or a particular period of time,
yesterday, or at a particular time, at midnight.
To indicate that an activity occurred regularly or repeatedly in the past,
we can use adverbials that denote repetition, e.g., I walked to school every
day. Generally, though not necessarily, this will be interpreted to mean that
I no longer walk to school. If we wish to indicate the period during which
these walks took place we can add an adverbial denoting a period of time,
e.g., as a child. If the situation continues through the time of utterance, we
can add an adverbial to that effect, e.g., as I do to this day.
In texts it is not unusual for a time to be established early, and if that time
is in the past, then succeeding clauses are likely to be in the past tense. For
example, in the following text the first sentence establishes the 19th century
as the time reference; as a result, the verb (bolded) in each succeeding clause
is in the past tense:
369
Delahunty and Garvey
The 19th century has been called the “linguistic century.” During that
time scholars carried out a great deal of research into the dialects of several European languages. They explored the historical relations among
the languages of Europe and parts of Asia, and developed concepts that
are still in use. In short, 19th century philologists laid the foundations of
modern linguistics.
A number of researchers have argued that the past tense indicates “distal
events,” by which they mean situations or events removed in one way or
another (not just in time) from the current situation, for example, hypothetical or conditional situations:
(2) a. If I were a rich man, I’d buy an Aston Martin.
b. If wishes were horses, beggars would ride.
In conditionals the past tense may be chosen instead of the present to give
the impression that the condition is unlikely to be fulfilled. Compare:
(3) a. If he has time, John will volunteer at the homeless shelter.
b. If he had time, John would volunteer at the homeless shelter.
Sentence (3a) represents a condition that is more likely to be true than the
condition represented in sentence (3b).
Distancing oneself from what one is saying can be used also to indicate
politeness or deference. Compare:
(4) a. I wonder if I can borrow your car.
I want to ask you whether your class is full.
b. I wondered if I could borrow your car.
I wanted to ask you whether your class was full.
The bolded verbs of the (4a) sentences are in the present tense while the
bolded verbs of (4b) are in the past. We hope you will agree that the (4b)
sentences are more deferential than the (4a) sentences.
We can also put wishes and desires under the heading of “distal events.”
These are distal because they are removed from actuality. We regularly find
the past tense after verbs such as wish: I wish I had more money. In this sentence, even though the wish is to have more money now and in the future,
the verb had is in the past tense.
370
Basic Clause Patterns
Exercise
The following text is from the novel, Gilead, by Marilynne Robinson
(2006: 75-6). (a) Identify all the past tense verbs, both regular and irregular in the passage. (b) Discuss each one using the framework for
understanding past tense presented in the discussion just above. (c)
You should also note that the excerpt includes present tense verbs in
addition to the past tense ones. Why do you think the author shifts from
past to present tense when she does? (d) The past tense is the basic,
most frequently used tense in this novel. Why do you think that a writer
might choose the past tense as the basic one for a novel?
My father was born in Kansas, as I was, because the old man had
come there from Maine just to help the Free Soilers establish the right
to vote, because the constitution was going to be voted on that would
decide whether Kansas entered the Union slave or free. Quite a few
people went out there at that time for that reason. And, of course,
so did people from Missouri who wanted Kansas for the South. So
things were badly out of hand for a while. All best forgotten, my father used to say. He didn’t like mention of those times, and that did
cause some hard feelings between him and his father. I’ve read up on
those events considerably, and I’ve decided my father was right. And
that’s just as well, because people have forgotten. Remarkable things
went on, certainly, but there has been so much trouble in the world
since then it’s hard to find time to think about Kansas.
The simple present/non-past tense
Forms
Pronunciation
The English simple present tense has two forms. One is created by adding
the suffix {-s} to the word stem if the subject it agrees with is third person,
singular; for example, She reads very quickly. With other subjects the form
used is the uninflected form; for example I/you/we/they read very quickly.
The {-s} suffix has three allomorphs: [s], [z], and [Iz] or [@z]. [I/@z] occurs
when the verb ends in a sibilant consonant, e.g., passes; [s] occurs when the
verb ends in a voiceless non-sibilant, e.g., pats; and [z] occurs when the verb
ends in a voiced non-sibilant consonant, e.g., pads and paws. Remember, we
371
Delahunty and Garvey
are describing the pronunciation here, not the spelling.
Spelling
The present tense marker is spelled in a variety of ways, too. If the word ends
in <s, z, sh, ch, x, o>, then it is spelled <es>: passes, waltzes, crushes, catches,
xeroxes, echoes, vetoes; if the word ends in a consonant followed immediately
by <y> then change <y> to <i> and add <es>: empty, empties; otherwise it is
spelled <s>: gets, steps.
Meanings
The simple present tense indicates that a state of affairs holds at the time
of utterance. The situation may extend indefinitely far into the past and/or
into the future. All the present tense indicates is that the description applies
at the time of utterance. However, this meaning interacts in interesting ways
with the semantics of the main verb, and the present tense is used conventionally for certain types of contexts and purposes, even when present time
is not intended.
Present tense of state verbs
The present tense interacts semantically with verb class, particularly with
state and activity verbs. We will begin by illustrating the present tense with
state verbs as these are simpler. For example, Jordan is ill merely asserts that
Jordan is ill as the utterance is spoken. He may have been ill for an indefinite
time in the past, he may be ill for an indefinite time in the future, or he may
have just become ill and may recover immediately. We can demonstrate that
the present tense is indifferent to the length of time that a situation lasts by
showing that it is compatible with contradictory adverbials of time. Compare Jordan is ill; he’s been ill for years now and it seems that he’ll be ill for years
to come with Jordan is ill; he got a sudden fever which will disappear as rapidly
as it came.
There are several kinds of state verbs, of which be is the most frequent.
Other state verbs in the present tense are interpreted pretty much as be is,
that is, as denoting a time that extends through the time of utterance and
indefinitely far backwards and forwards. For example:
Relationship verbs:
Leonardo owns a Ferrari 500 F2.
Mia resembles her mother.
Emotion and feeling verbs:
372
Basic Clause Patterns
I love my mother.
I feel pretty, oh so pretty.
Sense perception verbs:
I taste the saffron.
I hear it now.
Cognitive state verbs:
I know how to cook paella.
I doubt his story.
Because the simple present of state verbs denotes a state of affairs which
includes the present time but may extend indefinitely into the past or the
future, we can indicate the extent of the time involved by adding various
temporal adverbials, such as now, these days. For example, She loves you now
but it won’t be for long, We all live in a yellow submarine these days, We are here
now but we’re leaving shortly.
Present tense of activity verbs
With activity verbs the present is interpreted somewhat differently. I exercise
denotes a series of events rather than a single exercise event. The series may
extend indefinitely into the past and the future. Even though I may not be
exercising as I utter the sentence, the period of time occupied by the series
includes the present time. So, activity verbs in the simple present can denote repetitions of events that overlap the present time. This usage is often
referred to as the iterative.
Temporal adverbials may be added to indicate how regularly or frequently the events take place (everyday, regularly, frequently), when they occur (at
noon, whenever I feel like it), when they begin (from 6 a.m.), when they end
(till late at night), and how long they last (for an hour, while watching The
Young and the Restless). For example, I exercise everyday for an hour from 6 to
7 a.m.
Many textbooks claim that the present tense of activity verbs denotes habitual or regular actions. However, their examples typically include adverbials indicating regularity or habituality. The present tense itself says nothing
about frequency or regularity; the adverbials do this. One can say without
contradiction, I exercise, though neither frequently nor regularly.
Timeless truths
The simple present is used also for “universal or timeless truths,” permanent
373
Delahunty and Garvey
states of affairs, such as, A square has four equal sides, Dogs bark, Tigers are
dangerous, Verbs denote activities and states. This usage is used by lexicographers in defining words in dictionaries and by textbook writers to explain
technical terms, as a quick review of this book will show.
Exercise
Dictionary definitions are often expressed with the simple present
tense. Find five words in your dictionary that are defined using the
simple present tense.
Closely related to timeless truths are states of affairs that are treated as
if they were permanent, such as New York lies along the Hudson, The Eiffel
Tower stands in central Paris. We know New York has lain along the banks of
the Hudson for as long as it has existed and that the Eiffel Tower has stood
in Paris since it was built, and if we expect no change in these locations, we
can use the simple present.
Play-by-play
The simple present can be used for “play-by-play” commentary (a.k.a. “in
situ narrative”) on sports or ceremonies. In this use, activity verbs denote
events that are almost simultaneous with the utterance that describes them:
McCool steps into the ring. He looks his opponent in the eye. Ripkin swings
and . . . misses. Ronaldo shoots and it’s a goal.
Related to the play-by-play usage is a speaker’s commentary on what they
themselves are doing. For example, when a speaker is demonstrating something, say, following a recipe, they may use the simple present: I now add the
crushed garlic and the chopped onions. Or in relatively formal business letters,
one may write: I enclose my check for $50.00.
Historical present
To communicate immediacy, the present tense is often used to refer to past
events in narratives and stories, a use that is often referred to as the historical
present, for example, So the cop comes over to us and says . . .
Scheduled events in the future
The simple present may also be used to denote future scheduled or planned
events: Tom leaves for Dallas tomorrow. Adverbials indicating when the event
is to take place may be included. In the example above, no repetition of
374
Basic Clause Patterns
events is denoted, but other examples may be interpretable as denoting either a single future scheduled event or a series of events: Flight 1750 leaves
at 6 p.m. unless a unique time is understood from the context or specified in
the sentence: Flight 1750 leaves at 6 p.m. tomorrow.
This usage is not possible with events that are not schedulable. For example, It freezes tonight is strange because, even if the freeze were accurately
and confidently forecast, it is still not scheduled. However, even though
scheduling is typically done by humans, nature may be regular enough so
that certain events may be regarded as scheduled. We can say, El Niño returns next year during the year prior to when we know that phenomenon
regularly returns.
However, Comrie (1985: 47 fn 15) points out, sentences like “The train
departs at five o’clock tomorrow morning” is not synonymous with sentences
like “The train is scheduled to depart at five o’clock tomorrow morning.” The
first “does say explicitly that the train will depart at the said hour, and moreover that this is the result of scheduling; the latter, however, says only that
according to the schedule five o’clock is the train’s departure time, but does
not say that the train will in fact leave according to that schedule.” So there
is no contradiction in saying The train is scheduled to leave at five o’clock tomorrow morning, but in fact it won’t leave till six, but it is contradictory to
say The train departs at five o’clock tomorrow morning, but in fact it won’t leave
till six.
Performatives
Performatives (utterances that name the verbal act as they are performing it,
such as, I admit that I am guilty, which both admits and names the admitting) always occur in the simple present tense because their effect coincides
with the time of the utterance. Performative utterances often contain such
adverbs as now and hereby: I now pronounce you husband and wife, I hereby
declare this park open. These adverbs indicate the simultaneity between the
time of the utterance and the time of its effect. Other performative verbs
include promise, reject: I promise that I will never do that again, I reject your
suggestion that I resign.
Exercise
1. Collect five naturally occurring sentences referring to scheduled future events and test the claim we have made by checking the tense(s)
used.
375
Delahunty and Garvey
2. Find five more performative verbs and put them in appropriate sentences. Include now and hereby to be sure you really have created performative sentences. What happens if you change the simple present to
another tense and/or aspect?
Reviews
Reviews of books, records, videos, movies, software, and the like are primarily (though certainly not exclusively) written in the simple present. For
example, This review, therefore, describes not only the contents. . . (College
English 40, 1: 72). The book is suitable for students. . . (Language 73, 4: 862).
Reporting verbs such as say, suggest, and claim generally occur in the
simple present in reviews to represent what the author of the reviewed item
has written: In a recent book on literacy, for example, Baynham (1995) suggests that literacy always serves social purposes. . . (TESOL Quarterly 30, 1:
163).
Exercise
Pick a review from any of the journals in the fields of TESOL, linguistics, or
English studies and identify the reporting verbs and the tenses they occur in.
Hear and tell
In ordinary conversation when we want to report what someone recently
said to us, we can use the simple present of verbs such as hear and tell: I hear
you passed your exams, Maya tells me that you have been ill.
Exercise
The following text is from the novel, Mr Phillips, by John Lanchester
(2000: 28). (a) Identify all the simple present tense verbs, both regular
and irregular in the passage. (b) Discuss each one using the framework
for understanding present tense presented just above. (c) You should
also note that a few past tense verbs also occur. Why do you think the
author shifts to the past tense as he does? (d) While the past tense is
the basic, most frequently used tense in novels, the basic tense of this
novel is the present tense. Why do you think that the writer might have
chosen the present tense as basic for this novel? To help you answer this
question, you might change all the present tense verbs into the past
376
Basic Clause Patterns
tense and then compare the two versions of the passage.
At about seven o’clock, Mr Phillips hears the dustbin lorry turn into
the far end of the street. The dustmen call to each other, shout, bang
bins, swear, make noises that are associated with the effort of heaving
bags up onto the back of the cart, all the sounds which are always
different but always the same. The lorry is part of its being Monday,
a process which started last thing on Sunday night with remembering to put out the rubbish—which is more complicated than it once
was, since the council now recycles rubbish, and there are differentcoloured plastic bags and different weekly schedules for paper and
plastics and bottles. Cardboard, however, you still have to either put
in with the normal rubbish or take it up to the council dump by the
dog track, which Mr and Mrs Phillips have formally decided, after
doing it what felt like a million times, they can no longer be bothered
to do.
The future
The grammar of the future is more complex than that of the past or present
tenses. There are several grammatical forms, each with different interpretations. In English, these forms are not inflectional. The most important one
(the will future) is created with a modal verb. The fact that the main English
future is modal has led some linguists to claim that English has no future
tense. This is certainly true if by “tense” we mean inflected verb forms denoting future situations. However, English has several grammatical means of
indicating futurity.
As you might expect but probably didn’t think to put it quite so oddly,
“the future is what now is prior to” (McCawley 1981: 341), and the grammatical future forms locate “situations in time subsequent to the present
moment” (Comrie 1985: 43).
However, the future is conceptually different from the present and the
past. Arguably, there is only one actual past and one actual present, but
there are many futures. There is the future that we have planned, the future we predict, the future we wish for, the future that we have to bring
about, and lots of others. And, of course, there is the future that will actually occur, although, as McCawley remarks, “[t]he notion of ‘actual future’
may give one a queasy feeling, in view of the fact that one has very little
conception of which of the infinitely many possible futures is the actual
one; . . . Nonetheless, speakers of natural languages frequently indulge
377
Delahunty and Garvey
in the rashness of making statements that purport to describe the actual
future” (McCawley 1981: 342-4). So, there are lots of possible futures,
which is why it makes a great deal of sense for a language to use its modal
system as its main grammatical way of denoting futurity. In this section we
describe the various grammatical forms used to denote future in English
and the particular kinds of futures each denotes.
Will
Forms
The form often called the “simple future” is created by using the modal verb
will followed by an uninflected verb form: Jake will bring it. Sometimes,
especially in formal contexts and in British English, shall may be used instead of will when the subject of the sentence is first person (I or we): I shall
return, We shall overcome.
Meanings
The will future simply indicates that a situation will obtain in the future
relative to the time of utterance. When the situation begins and how long
it lasts are irrelevant to the will future. It is compatible with situations that
began in the past, held through the present, and will continue indefinitely
into the future: Deborah still loves Raymond and will love him forever. In
this example, still indicates that the state of affairs, Deborah loves Raymond,
held in the past and has held up to “now.” Forever obviously indicates that
the situation will continue indefinitely.
The will future is also consistent with situations that begin and end
within a very short time in the future: The clock will chime once at exactly
midnight tonight. When a state of affairs begins and ends can be indicated
by various temporal adverbials.
How long a state of affairs lasts may also be indicated by the nature of the
main verb, specifically by whether or not it is punctual, as well as by various
adverbials: The balloon will pop presently, Timothy will remain in prison for
the rest of his life. Pop denotes an event that takes no more than a moment of
time; remain denotes a state that occupies an indefinite period of time.
The will future is compatible also with events that occur at specific times
in the future: I’ll give you your money at noon tomorrow. It is also compatible
with vague times: The Earth will certainly be hit by a large meteor, but we have
no way of knowing just when that will happen.
Like the present and the past, the future interacts with the type of main
verb involved. With state verbs, the will future indicates an unchanging
378
Basic Clause Patterns
state of affairs that will hold in the future; context or adverbials may indicate
when it began or when it will end: He will be here tomorrow from 3 to 4 p.m.
With activity verbs, the future can denote either single or repeated occurrences of events. Just which is determined by other parts of the sentence,
such as an object or a temporal adverbial: He will eat a sandwich, He will sing
a song, He will eat sandwiches (every day for the next week, just as he did this
week).
Whether an event recurs regularly and habitually is independent of the
future tense marker. Habituality is indicated by objects or adverbials: I will
be late for class every day next week, I will go to an occasional movie next
year.
Consistent with its denotation of future situations, will may be used for
intentions, predictions, conditions, requests, offers, and promises:
Intentions:
I’ll stay for the entire week! We won’t forget to write
to you.
Predictions:
You will enjoy good fortune throughout your life.
Conditionals: If the rain continues, the river will overflow its banks.
When the weather warms up, people will throng to
the beaches.
Requests: Will you bring the beer this time?
Offers: I’ll bring the beer, if you want.
Promises: I’ll bring the beer, I promise.
I promise that I’ll be on time next time.
The forms of will can occur in elliptical responses: Will you, Heather, have
this man, Henry, to be your lawful wedded husband? I will. Don’t be late! I
won’t.
Be going to V
Form
This future form is created with the expression be going to followed by the
uninflected form of a verb: Jake is going to leave soon.
If the be is the first auxiliary verb in a tensed clause, it must be inflected
to indicate the person and number of its subject as well as the tense of the
clause. It may be inflected for present or past tense. With the past tense we
create a “future of a past”: John was going to leave, but he missed his plane.
Stylistically, be going to is less formal than will and is more frequent
in conversational than in written English. It is often pronounced [g@n@],
379
Delahunty and Garvey
which is typically spelled “gonna.”
Meanings
Be going to communicates a more certain future than will, either because the
speaker strongly intends to bring that future about or it has already been
planned: I am going to finish this if it kills me. It’s all arranged; I am going to
fly to Los Angeles next week.
The future may also be quite certain because it results from causes operating in the present: The temperature is dropping; it’s going to snow. He’s 12
points ahead; he’s going to win the game.
When used to express the future result of a current cause, be going to
communicates an inference that the result will occur almost immediately.
This suggestion of immediacy appears to be due to pragmatic factors. If the
result is not going to be immediate, then the speaker may override the inference with a temporal adverbial such as next month, next year, in 2009. For
example, Current weather trends indicate that we are going to have very cold
weather next week/month/year.
In spite of their differences, will and be going to are often interchangeable
without much apparent effect, for example: They will be in the hospital for ten
days/They are going to be in the hospital for ten days. I think it’ll rain/I think it’s
going to rain.
In other cases, substituting be going to for will causes significant changes
of meaning. For example, expressing an intention with will allows it to be
interpreted as an offer: I’ll bring dessert, whereas expressing that intention
with be going to suggests a fixed plan: I’m going to bring dessert. It may also
suggest the speaker’s expectation that someone might try to thwart their
intention: compare, I’ll answer it with I’m going to answer it when the phone
rings. Similarly, substituting be going to for will in requests and promises
causes them to seem either preemptory and intrusive or questions about
statements of fact: Are you going to bring the beer this time? I am going to help
you on Saturday (whether you like it or not).
In some contexts, substituting will for be going to results in ungrammaticality. Compare If it’s going to rain, you should bring an umbrella with * If it
will rain, you should bring an umbrella.
In continuous text be going to and will may sometimes alternate in any order:
It’ll be cooler tomorrow, because it’s going to be cloudy in the afternoon. It’s going to be
cooler tomorrow, because it’ll be cloudy in the afternoon.
Present progressive
Just like the simple present tense, the present progressive with an appropri380
Basic Clause Patterns
ate temporal adverbial may also be used to express a planned future event:
I am having a birthday party this year. Grandpa is coming to visit next week.
John is leaving on the six o’clock bus.
Modals
We can use modal verbs + infinitives to express how likely a future situation
is: I will/may/might leave. Or whether it is required: I must/should leave. Or
permitted: I may leave. Or (physically) possible: I can lift that for you.
Exercise
The following paragraph is (a slightly amended) part of the small print
from an advertisement for a Southwest Airlines Visa card that appeared in Newsweek (January 21, 2008: between pp. 52-3). (a) Identify all of the expressions in the paragraph that refer to future time. (b)
When is that future time? (c) Why is will used as the primary expression referring to the future in this paragraph? (d) Besides being part
of an ad, what other genre(s)/domain(s) does this paragraph belong
to? (e) Why do you think the will future is appropriate for that/those
genre(s)/domain(s)? (f) There are several clauses/sentences that do
not refer to future time. Identify those clauses/sentences and identify their tense(s). Discuss why the writer(s) of this text used those
tenses where they did.
You will earn 2 Reward Dollars for each $1 of net purchases made
directly from Southwest Airlines, including flight purchases and
Southwest Airlines Vacations package purchases. You will earn
2 Reward Dollars for each $1 of net purchases at participating
Southwest Airlines Rapid Rewards Preferred Hotel and Rental
Car Partners. You will earn 1 Reward Dollar for each $1 of all
other net purchases. Each year you will receive 2,400 Reward
Dollars (2 Rapid Rewards credits) after your Anniversary. “Anniversary” means the year beginning with the date of your account opening through the first statement after the anniversary
of the date of your account opening, and each twelve months
thereafter. Once you earn $1,200 Reward Dollars you will receive 1 Southwest Airlines Rapid Rewards credit.
381
Delahunty and Garvey
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s f o r a p p e n d i x
Comrie, Bernard. 1985. Tense. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lanchester, John. 2000. Mr Phillips. New York: Penguin.
McCawley, James. 1981. Everything that Linguists have always Wanted to Know
about Logic*. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Robinson, Marilynne. 2006. Gilead. New York: Picador.
382
11 Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
key concepts
Transformational grammar: deep structure, surface structure,
transformations
Movement
Assignment of semantic roles
Deletion
Insertion
Discourse functions of modified clauses
i n t ro d u c t i o n
In our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns, we dealt with simple sentences,
that is, sentences that contain only one clause, consisting of an NP subject,
AUX, and VP predicate. In this chapter we examine sentences in which
elements of simple sentences are moved from their canonical positions, are
understood even though no words are present, and in which certain kinds
of elements are inserted into clauses to fulfill certain functions. In order
to explain the phenomena of movement, deletion, and insertion, we will
introduce elements from a theory of syntax called transformational grammar. Specifically we will distinguish between a clause’s deep structure (DS),
the nature of which we will make clear later, and its surface structure (SS),
i.e., the form it actually occurs in. We will also introduce transformational
rules, i.e., the rules that move, delete, and insert linguistic elements in the
journey from deep to surface structure. As we go along we will also discuss the discourse functions of the sentence types we introduce. (To follow the development of one strand of transformational grammar see Carnie
2007; Chomsky 1964; Culicover 1976, 1982; Radford 1981, 1988, 1997a,
1997b.)
m ove m e n t
We begin by examining sentence variations that involve the movement of
elements from one position to another in a clause. We begin with one of the
simplest of these variations—particle movement.
Particle movement
Particles are words such as up or over that may occur as prepositions when they
take their own NP objects or as adverbs when they modify Vs or VPs. When
these words combine with a verb to create an idiomatic unit, they are called
particles. For example, combining look and up creates the phrasal verb look
383
Delahunty and Garvey
up in the following sentences:
(1) a. Bill looked up the number.
b. Bill looked the number up.
These two sentences differ only in the position of the particle up. In the
first it occurs between the verb and the object; in the second, to the right
of the object. Both sentences contain the same words, and the phrases, particularly the NPs, perform the same grammatical functions in both: Bill is
the subject; the number is the direct object. Moreover, the two sentences are
synonymous. By this we mean specifically that the semantic roles (see our
chapter on Basic Clause Patterns) assigned to the NPs are the same in both
sentences. Bill is the Agent and the number is the Theme. As native speakers we can assert these facts with certainty, and expect universal agreement
among English speakers. Because knowledge of the relationships between
sentence pairs like these is part of every native English speaker’s competence,
it must be expressed in a grammar. At issue is how to do this.
We can assign the following analyses to sentences (1a,b)
(2)a.
S
NP
VP
N
V
PRT Bill
DET
looked
(2)b. up
N
number
VP
V
NP
DET
384
the
N
S
NP
NP
Bill
looked
the
PRT
N
number
up
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
These structures can be generated by the phrase structure rules:
(3) a. S ——> NP AUX VP (i.e., S consists of NP, AUX, and VP)
b. VP——> V PRT NP (cf. tree (2a))
c. VP——> V NP PRT (cf. tree (2b))
Now, if the two sentences (1a, b) can be created (“generated”) by the
rules in (3), why can’t we declare victory, leave well enough alone and go
on to the next topic? The answer is that while the two rules (3b, c) might
be just coincidentally similar, speakers of English know that the sentences
they represent are related, that they are variant forms of each other. If we
assume that a goal of our grammar is to represent native speakers’ linguistic
knowledge—their linguistic competence—then we have to represent the
relationship between pairs of sentence like (1a,b). We will adopt a strategy
similar to the one we adopted in phonology and morphology, namely that
variant forms (e.g., allophones and allomorphs) are assumed to be different representations or manifestations of the same abstract form. So, (1a,b)
are different surface manifestations of an abstract structure that they have
in common, much as the allomorphs /s/, /z/, and /@z/ are manifestations
of the morpheme {-s}.
This common abstract form is the deep structure. The actual strings of
words and their structural organizations are the surface structures. The deep
structure and surface structure of a sentence are connected by transformational rules, or just transformations, which move, delete, or insert items.
We will make a few assumptions about deep structures. First, they are
created by phrase structure rules (PSRs), like those in (3). Second, elements
that are semantically closely related must be syntactically closely related in
DS. So, for example, in particle sentences such as (1a,b), because the particle and verb are closely associated semantically, they must be closely associated in DS. Consequently, we should prefer look up the number rather than
look the number up as the DS arrangement of words. Third, as we will discuss
in greater depth later, if a verb assigns a semantic role to an NP, then that NP
must be in the same clause as the verb.
We have now argued that (2a) represents the DS of both (1a, b). The surface structure of (1a) is identical in all relevant respects to its deep structure.
The surface structure of (1b) is slightly different from its deep structure,
specifically regarding the position of the particle. We account for this slight
difference by hypothesizing a transformation (called Particle Movement
(PM)) which moves a particle from its deep structure position beside its
verb, places it to the right of the direct object, and attaches it to the VP. This
385
Delahunty and Garvey
yields the surface structure (2b).
The transformational approach has the advantage of expressing, by the
common deep structure, native speakers’ knowledge that pairs of sentences like (1a,b) are grammatically related. The grammar represents the two
sentences as transformational variants of each other, analogous to the ways
in which allophones and allomorphs are related to each other as variants of
underlying abstract phonemes or morphemes. Assigning sentences a common deep structure also expresses the fact that the sentences are broadly
synonymous with each other, specifically in that a given noun phrase will
have the same semantic role in all.
Exercise
1. Create five pairs of sentences analogous to (1a,b), using the particles
up, out, over, and on.
2. Draw the DS and SS trees for each pair.
3. What condition must be added to the particle movement transformation to correctly account for the following data?
a. The witness picked Fred out.
b. The witness picked out Fred.
c. The witness picked him out.
d. *The witness picked out him.
(i.e., what condition must be added to ensure that the grammar does
not predict that (d) is grammatical?)
A deep structure expresses what surface structures have in common and provides a basis for movement, deletion, and insertion. Transformations may
create multiple surface structures from a single deep structure.
A simplified, overall model of a transformational grammar is:
(4)
PSRs
Semantic role assignment ——> DS <—— lexical (word) insertion
386
SS
SS
SS
...
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
A note on the discourse functions of particle movement
Particle movement has no effect on the meaning of sentences to which it has
applied. The semantic roles of the NPs in the sentences affected by PM are
exactly the same as those in sentences unaffected. Are both sentence types
freely interchangeable in all contexts then?
The answer to this should be clear from the exercise above. PM is generally optional but it must apply if the object is a pronoun. It seems plausible to assume that at least one factor in determining where to place the
particle is the informational status of the object NP. If the NP represents
old, known, or given information (and pronouns are typically in this category), then the particle moves to the right. If the NP represents new
information, the particle is placed between V and NP. This is an example
of the interaction between the syntactic and pragmatic components of the
grammar.
Another factor, and probably the more important one, is the length of
the object NP. The longer the NP the more likely the particle is to appear
before, rather than after it, as the following sentences show:
(5)
a. He looked it up.
b. He looked the number up.
c. He looked his boss’s number up.
d. ?He looked the number that he had written on the back on
a match book up.
e. *He looked the number of the house where the cat that
killed the rat that ate the malt lived up.
f. He looked up the number of the house where the cat that
killed the rat that ate the malt lived.
These examples illustrate the effect that length (or weight) can have on
where a phrase can occur in a sentence.
Teaching phrasal verbs
We include this brief section primarily for teachers of ESL students because
phrasal verbs can present them with significant difficulty, primarily because
they are unusual in the languages of the world, but also because of their
idiosyncratic syntax and idiomatic characteristics.
Phrasal verbs consist, as we have seen, of a verb and particle. Their semantics is often idiomatic; that is, the overall meaning of the phrasal
verb cannot be determined from the typical meanings of the verb and particle, for example, the “provide care for” meaning of look after.
387
Delahunty and Garvey
There are several syntactic classes of phrasal verbs for students and teachers to contend with:
(6) a. Intransitive: give in, move on (Because these have no direct
object, PM is impossible.)
b. Transitive with optional PM: bring back, look up, cut out.
c. Transitive with obligatory PM: shut someone up, *shut up
someone.
d. Transitive with prohibited PM: do without something, *do
something without.
Good learners’ dictionaries (e.g., Cambridge International Dictionary of
English) provide useful expansions on this necessarily brief discussion.
assignment of semantic roles
To p i c a l i z a t i o n Phrases are assigned semantic roles by virtue of their grammatical relations
within the sentence, specifically by virtue of their relation to a verb, preposition, or noun. Many, but by no means all, current theories make the assumption that an NP can be assigned only one semantic role in any clause.
In the interests of simplicity we will adopt that assumption in this section.
We will assume also that, with very few exceptions such as expletive it
and there, every NP in a sentence must be assigned one but no more than
one semantic role. Consider now the sentences:
(7) a. Bill carried Mary.
b. Mary, Bill carried.
In both of these sentences Bill is the Agent and Mary is the Theme. Note
however that Mary occurs in two different positions in these sentences. In
(7a) it occurs in the direct object position; in (7b) it is in what is known as
the topic position. Speakers of English know that both of these sentences
denote that Bill carried Mary. Let’s assume that we have a rule whereby carry
assigns Theme to its direct object in sentences like (7a). Must we now assume that we need another rule to account for the assignment of Theme to
Mary in the topic position? Let’s examine that possibility. Such a rule might
say: assign Theme to the NP directly to the left of the subject of the clause in
which the assigning verb occurs. However, when we examine some more data
we can see that this becomes impossible.
388
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
(8) a. Mary, Fred thinks that Bill carried.
b. Mary, Susan believes that Fred thinks that Bill carried.
c. Mary, I know that Susan believes that . . . that Bill carried.
Exercise
Insert 1, 2, and 3 further expressions of the form NP Vs that in the position occupied by the ellipses in (8c).
In each of the sentences of (8) Mary is understood as the Theme of carried. But we can insert as many that-clauses between Mary and carried as we
wish. There is no principled bound to the number of such clauses. It follows
that for an infinite number of sentences we would need an infinite number
of rules to assign Theme to Mary. Clearly such a set of rules could not exist
in anyone’s mind. We must therefore find an alternative way of assigning a
semantic role to all these different (but related) positions.
Rather than devise a complex set of rules that would directly assign
Theme to Mary in all these positions, linguists have simplified the assignment process by assuming something like the following: a verb such as carry
assigns Theme to its deep structure direct object. This phrase may afterwards
be moved by transformations, though it will always be associated with its
original, deep structure, position and therefore with its semantic role.
In support of this idea, note that we cannot put another NP into the
object position and still interpret the resulting S as Mary is carried by Bill.
(9) Mary, Bill carried Susie.
We can only interpret Mary in (9) as an addressee and not as a direct object.
Susie has taken over that role. Because Mary can no longer be associated
with the DO position it cannot be interpreted as Theme. So the deep structure position with which a moved phrase is associated and from which it
derives its semantic role must be empty.
Let’s now think about the position that the topicalized phrase occupies
in surface structure. There are various possibilities. One is that topicalization
inserts the topicalized phrase directly under S. However, we will introduce
a new node, COMP (for COMPLEMENTIZER), which requires a special
phrase structure rule to generate it:
(10) S'——> COMP S (S' is pronounced as S-bar)
389
Delahunty and Garvey
(11)
S'
COMP
S
We will assume that topicalization inserts the moved phrase into COMP.
The surface structure of (7b) would therefore be represented as:
(12)
S'
COMP
S
NP
NP
VP
N
N
V
Mary
Bill
carried
COMP is a position outside of S that occurs in every S'. It is never assigned a semantic role directly. A phrase in COMP acquires its semantic role
by virtue of its association with some deep structure position. This association is represented by the transformation that moves the phrase. As every
NP must be assigned a semantic role, it follows that there can be no NP in
COMP in deep structure. We will call positions that cannot be assigned a
semantic role directly in DS non‑argument positions. This is often abbreviated as A' (read A‑bar) in the literature. Argument (A-) positions are
those positions within S to which semantic roles may be assigned in deep
structure. They include, as you no doubt guessed, subjects, objects, and objects of prepositions. We will discover later that while these positions may be
assigned a semantic role, they occasionally are not. So topicalization moves a
phrase from an A position to which a semantic role has been assigned to an
A' position.
Besides moving an NP, as in the examples above, topicalization can move
any phrase to COMP:
(13) a. Mary, I like. (NP topic)
b. Into the valley of death, I will not go. (PP topic)
c. Intelligent, he is not. (AP topic)
d. I said that Fred would go home, and go home he will. (VP topic)
390
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
e. Quickly she left. (AdvP topic)
Exercise
1. Draw the deep structure and surface structure trees for each sentence
in (13).
2. What PSRs created the DSs?
3. Describe the changes that the topicalization transformation makes
to the DS as it changes it into the SS.
Discourse functions of topicalization
Every native speaker of English knows that topicalized and non-topicalized
sentences express essentially the same message: the semantic roles of the
various NPs in the sentences are unchanged by the movement. There is
nonetheless a difference between the two forms. The non-topicalized is a
more general‑purpose construction than the topicalized, which we would
use only in special contexts.
The most typical reaction speakers of English have upon hearing sentences such as:
(14) The forks, you put on the left.
is that the entity or entities referred to by the topic NP, the forks, have already been introduced (directly or indirectly) into the discourse. So (14)
might occur as the answer to:
(15) Which side do I put the forks on?
A second and closely related use of topicalized sentences is to refer to
an entity that is a member of a set of related entities that has already been
introduced into the discourse. A typical other sentence to go along with (14)
might be:
(16) and the knives you put on the right.
Knives and forks are members of the set of silverware items, and if you
came upon a conversation in which (14) was the first sentence you heard,
391
Delahunty and Garvey
you could reasonably assume that the speakers had already mentioned
silverware, and one speaker was now listing the individual members of the
silverware set and telling a child (for example) where each one goes in a
place setting. So, the NP in topic position can refer to a member of a set
of items that has already been introduced into the discourse.
Just to show you that we’re not simply making this up out of hot air,
consider the following authentic example.
(17) He’d have been lucky to get thirty percent . . ., sixty one, Snow
gave him (Le Carré 1962/1980: 74)
Here the topic is a student’s grade. Thirty and sixty one percent are members of the set of possible grades. Sixty one can be topicalized because it is
a member of the already mentioned set and contrasts with thirty.
To sum up our discussion of topicalization: topicalization moves any
phrase from its position within S to the COMP position. The moved phrase
retains the semantic role it was assigned at deep structure. The old position
may not be filled with another phrase. No morphological changes occur
either to the moved phrase or to other elements of the sentence. Topicalization is used to refer to entities that are members of sets or lists that have
already been introduced into the discourse (Ward and Birner 2001).
Subject-auxiliary inversion
As we noted in our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns, the order of subject
and first auxiliary verb is reversed in yes/no questions (18a-f ), and sentences
that begin with negative adverbs such as rarely (19):
(18) a. Can I have another go?
b. Have you taken the trash out?
c. Are you leaving now?
d. Will you be OK?
e. Were you hurt in the accident?
f. Could I have forgotten my keys again?
(19) Rarely have I seen such a magnificent sunset!
As you recall, the immediate constituents of S are NP AUX VP. Using
these as our base, we can relate a yes/no question to its declarative counterpart by assuming that both have the same deep structure (NP AUX VP), and
that there is a transformation (called Subject/Auxiliary Inversion (SAI))
that moves AUX to the left of the subject in the question (AUX NP VP).
392
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
We will assume that a moved AUX is adjoined to S rather than just
inserted directly under it. Adjunction to S involves first creating a second S
node above the original one and then inserting AUX (or more generally the
adjoined phrase) under the newly created node. SAI applies to (20a) to give
(20b):
(20)a.
S
NP AUX
(20)b.
AUX
S
VP
S
NP
VP
The technical reasons for adjunction need not detain us here.
Exercise
Draw the deep and surface structure trees for the sentences in (18)
and (19). Describe what the SAI transformation does to the DS tree to
create the SS tree.
In our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns, we showed that auxiliary verbs
are optional and that when they occur they do so in the order:
(21) (Modal) (Perfective have) (Progressive be) (Passive be)
SAI affects only the first verb in the sequence, regardless of which it is. In
fact, we will assume that only the first auxiliary verb is actually in the AUX
position when SAI applies. Later in this chapter we discuss the positions of
the other verbs. SAI creates sentences such as:
(22) a. Must you leave?
b. Has she left?
c. Is she leaving?
Exercise
Draw tree diagrams of the deep and surface structures of sentences
(22a,b,c) above.
393
Delahunty and Garvey
If the basic sentence has no auxiliary verb, then we put a form of do into
the first auxiliary slot (Do Insertion or Do Support), transfer the tense
from the main verb, and apply SAI to it:
(23) a. He works out really hard.
b. He does work out really hard.
c. Does he work out really hard?
d. *Works he out really hard? (cf. German, earlier forms of English,
Spanish)
Exercise
What is the DS of (23a, b, c)? Hint: all three have the same DS.
Wh-movement
Relative clauses frequently begin with a wh-phrase. Relative wh-phrases may
be NPs (which), PPs (to whom), and APs (how tall). Wh-phrases get moved
to COMP from a deep structure position within a clause, as in the relative
clause (italicized) below:
(24) The person to whom you spoke is no longer here.
The deep structure of this relative clause is:
(25) a.
S
NP
Pron
you
VP
V
spoke
PP
P
NP
to
Pron
whom
The wh-phrase is moved into a COMP by a transformation called whmovement:
394
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
(25) b.
S'
COMP
S
PP P
NP
to
Pron
NP
VP
Pron
V
whom
you
spoke
Exercise
1. Describe what wh-movement does.
2. Draw the DS and SS trees for the following two bracketed relative
clauses: The book [which you wrote], The man [whom you spoke to].
Wh-questions
Wh-questions, such as Who will you invite to the dance?, which ask for a
phrase as a reply, involve two transformations: subject auxiliary inversion
and wh-movement. Let’s assume that SAI occurs before wh-movement. The
deep structure of this question is:
(26) a.
S
NP
AUX
Pron
You
Modal V
will invite
VP
NP
who
PP
P
to
NP
DET
the
N
dance
Applying SAI to this creates:
395
Delahunty and Garvey
(26)b.
S
AUX Modal
S
NP
Will
Pron you
VP
V
invite
NP
PP
who P
NP
to
DET
N
the
dance
And applying wh-movement to this yields:
(26)c.
S'
COMP NP
Who
AUX
Modal
will
S
S
NP
VP
Pron V
you
invite
396
PP
P
to
NP
DET
the
N
dance
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
Wh‑movement may also occur in indirect questions without SAI:
(27) We wondered [what Nancy would say].
Clearly, one or more transformations can apply to derive a sentence’s surface
structure from its deep structure.
deletion
Besides moving them around in sentences, transformations may also delete phrases. “Understood” elements in clauses or sentences are typically
regarded as having been deleted by a transformation. We briefly examine
two deletion rules: Imperative Subject Deletion and VP Deletion.
Imperatives
(28) a. Get out now!
b. Behave yourself! (cf. *Behave herself!)
c. A: Do it! B: No. You do it!
There are reasons to believe, however, that imperatives have deep structure subjects. First, English sentences in general have subjects, and it would
be odd if one class of sentences lacked them. Second, and much more convincingly, imperatives interact with reflexive pronouns in ways that would
be hard to explain if they had no subject. Consider:
(29) a. Bill shaved himself.
b. You shaved yourself.
c. I shaved myself.
d. They shaved themselves.
e. You shaved yourselves.
f. We shaved ourselves.
g. *You shaved myself/himself/herself/themselves/ourselves.
A reflexive pronoun in the direct object position must agree in person,
number, and gender with the subject of its clause. That is, the form of the
reflexive pronoun is dependent upon the grammatical characteristics of the
subject. Clearly, for this to be the case there must be a subject for it to depend on. Bearing this in mind, consider:
(30) a. Shave yourself/yourselves!
397
Delahunty and Garvey
b. *Shave myself/himself/herself/themselves/ourselves.
We can explain this pattern of data by assuming that the deep structure of
(30a,b) is:
(30) c. You shave yourself.
Because the only reflexive that can occur here is the second person reflexive
(either singular or plural), the (understood) subject of a reflexive imperative
must be you. But the subject you can be, and generally is, deleted—by a
transformation called Imperative Subject Deletion.
The AUX of imperative sentences also seems to be special. Non‑imperative AUX phrases may contain a modal; an imperative AUX may not, even
though an imperative sentence may contain other auxiliary verbs, as the
following show:
(31) a. Close the door!
b. *Must close the door!
c. *Will eat your vegetables!
d. Have the dishes washed when I return!
e. Be studying when I return!
f. Be gone by daybreak!
If we assume that the AUX phrase can be occupied by either a modal or a
marker that indicates that the sentence is an imperative, then Imperative
Subject Deletion will correctly capture the facts. Consequently the deep
structure of Close the door! is:
(32) NP
S
AUX
You
[imper]
VP
V
NP
close
the door
Exercise
Which applies first, Imperative Subject Deletion or the rule that checks
that a reflexive pronoun has an appropriate antecedent? Why?
398
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
A note on the pragmatics of imperatives
Imperatives are traditionally thought of as the form used for giving orders.
Recent research suggests that there is more to be said about them than this,
and that they raise interesting issues of language use. Fundamentally, a speaker
using an imperative assumes that the addressee will do what the sentence expresses. However, studies of politeness suggest that speakers must concern
themselves with their addressee’s face, that is, their public self‑image and desire to be unimpeded (Brown and Levinson 1987). Speakers must be careful
not to deny aspects of an addressee’s self‑image or unreasonably impose upon
him or her. Imperatives can only be politely used when they do not violate
aspects of an addressee’s face.
Addressees can be assumed to be willing to cooperate with speakers without loss of face under several types of circumstances. First, if the speaker has
the authority to order the addressee to carry out the act expressed by the
imperative. Second, if the addressee can be assumed to want to carry out the
act. For example, recipe directions are expressed in the imperative, at least
partly because their readers may want to carry out the directions. Similarly,
people who have asked for directions can be assumed to want to carry them
out:
(33)Take College Blvd. south for three blocks, then turn right on
Horsetooth, . . .
Imperatives are polite also in (good) wishes:
(34) a. Get well soon!
b. Have a nice day!
Or in offers:
(35) a. Let me take care of that.
b. Have another piece of cake.
c. Come on in.
Or in warnings:
(36) Watch out!
We hope you remembered that the grammatical categories indicative,
interrogative, and imperative belong to the category of grammatical mood.
399
Delahunty and Garvey
Exercise
1. From the point of view of the discussion of face, explain why the use
of the imperative in the “wish” Go to hell! is impolite.
2. The following (slightly adapted) recipe is for a beet salad, taken
from Saveur (March 2006: 40). Like most recipes it consists of a list of
ingredients followed by a list of instructions on what to do with them.
And like most recipes, the instructions are expressed in the imperative
mood. (a) Identify all the imperative clauses. (b) Write the instruction
section below using a mood other than the imperative. (c) Why do you
think that the typical mood of the instruction section of recipes is the
imperative? (d) What other changes must you make? Why?
FOR THE BEETS:
3 medium beets (1 lb.), trimmed
Salt
¼ cup extra-virgin olive oil
2 tbsp. balsamic vinegar
Salt and freshly ground black pepper
For the beets: Put beets into a medium pot, cover with salted water,
and bring to a boil over high heat. Reduce heat to medium-low and
simmer until tender, 1½-2 hours. Remove beets from pot, let cool
slightly, then peel and cut into 1” chunks. Toss beets, oil, and salt
and pepper to taste in a bowl.
VP deletion
Yet another deletion rule deletes the VP of a sentence when it is identical to
the VP of a preceding sentence, as in:
(37) a. Fred can go to the movies, and Mary can too.
We understand (37a) as:
(37) b. Fred can go to the movies, and Mary can [go to the movies]
too.
If we take (37b) as representing the deep structure of (37a) and posit a
transformation that deletes the VP of a second clause (bracketed in 37b)
when it is identical to the VP of an earlier clause, then we will have account400
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
ed for our understanding of these two sentences. In general, deletion can
take place when it is possible to figure out easily from the context, linguistic
or non‑linguistic, just what has been deleted. Deletion under identity as in
(37a) is a typical case in which the prior linguistic context allows the recovery of the deleted elements.
VP deletion is more complex than we have represented it. When we examine it more closely we discover some interesting things about the structure of
the VP, as the following sentences show:
(38) a. John must have been learning Spanish, and Fred must have
been learning Spanish too.
b. . . . and Fred must have been too.
c. . . . and Fred must have too.
d. . . . and Fred must too.
e. . . .?and Fred, too.
In sentences (38b‑d), respectively, we have deleted [V NP], [been V NP],
[have been V NP]. In general, transformations operate on phrases rather than
arbitrary strings of words, and if we assume that this must be the case, then
each of the sequences [V NP], [been V NP], and [have been V NP] must be
a phrase. It follows that VP is composed of several levels of phrase structure,
and that VP deletion may apply to any of these levels:
(39) V3
have
V2
been
V1
V
NP
learning Spanish
Some discourse functions of deletion
The fundamental function of deletion is to make processing discourse easier
for both its producers and its audiences. The producer has to say or write
less; the audience has less to hear and process. Our minds derive inferences
(jump to conclusions) more rapidly than they process linguistic input, so
deletion takes advantage of that difference in processing speed by reducing
linguistic input and allowing the mind to fill in the blanks. And sometimes,
401
Delahunty and Garvey
too, to say less is to say more . . .
insertion
There are clause types which, under certain circumstances, require the insertion of apparently superfluous expressions. We briefly discuss clauses whose
subject is non-referential there.
Existential there sentences
English contains pairs of sentences such as the following, which, like active
and passive pairs, are essentially synonymous, but with some subtle differences:
(40) a. A unicorn is in the garden.
b. There is a unicorn in the garden.
The second differs from the first in that its subject contains “existential there”
(which does not refer to a specific location), and the phrase that corresponds
to the subject of the first sentence (a unicorn) occurs after the verb be. How
are we to represent the similarities and differences between such sentences?
As you have probably guessed at this point, a modern linguist is likely
to say that they are transformationally related. That is, they both have deep
structures similar to (40a), but the derivation of (40b) involves a transformation that moves the subject phrase into the VP and another that inserts
there into the vacated subject slot.
What gives us the right to say that the NP that follows the verb is the
subject in existential there sentences, when subjects generally occur before
their verbs in English clauses? Normally English verbs agree with their
surface structure subjects. In existential there sentences, however, the verb
agrees with the NP that follows it in surface structure:
(41) a. There are unicorns in the garden.
b.*There is unicorns in the garden.
(41b) is unacceptable in written or formal English, although its contracted
form, (42), is acceptable in informal, colloquial varieties:
(42) There’s unicorns in the garden.
Some discourse functions of existential there sentences
No doubt you noticed that the deep structure subject of existential there
402
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
sentences is typically an indefinite NP. Indefinite NPs typically represent
information that is assumed to be new to the audience, and typically is
placed later in sentences. Old information is generally expressed early in a
sentence, often in the subject. Topics are generally old information, and so
are frequently expressed in the subject phrase. Obviously, discourse must
have ways of introducing new topics, and existential there sentences, by
placing the new topic after the verb, are a natural way of accomplishing
this. They are also used to assert the existence of entities and to summarize
information (Huckin and Pesante 1988).
Exercise
1.
Identify the DS and SS of:
a. There is a house in New Orleans.
b. There is a god.
c. There may have been water on Mars.
2. Apply there insertion to:
a. A solution is available.
b. A skeleton was in the closet.
c. Pilgrims may have been here in the past.
p a s s i ve
We introduced the passive voice in various other chapters, so here we begin
with a functional description:
(43) Subject Be Ven (Agentive phrase)
Passive sentences include some form of the verb be followed by a verb in its
past participle form. Passives may include an agentive (by) phrase. Formally
they consist of:
(44) NP AUX [VPV+en (by NP)]
For example:
(45) a. The children were fed by the baby-sitter.
b. The pretzels were eaten by the mice.
c. The children were fed.
403
Delahunty and Garvey
d. The pretzels were eaten.
As a general rule, passive sentences have active counterparts, although a
missing agent phrase may have to be expressed as an indefinite pronoun:
(46) a.
b.
c.
d.
The baby-sitter fed the children.
The mice ate the pretzels.
Someone fed the children.
Someone/something ate the pretzels.
The active subject corresponds to the NP in the passive by‑phrase, and the
passive subject corresponds to either a direct or indirect object in the active:
(47) a. The package was sent to Amanda.
b. Amanda was sent the package.
Or occasionally to the object of a preposition:
(48) My bed has been slept in.
In the kind of analysis we are using here, the deep structures of synonymous active and passive sentences are taken to be identical, and the passive
transformation (a) creates the by-phrase, (b) moves the subject NP into it,
(c) moves the direct object NP into the now-vacant subject position, (d) inserts the verb be, and (e) changes the morphology of the verb to the passive
participle form. The deep structure of (45b) and (46b) is:
(49)a.
S
NP
DET
AUX
N
VP
V
The
404
mice
ate
NP
DET
N
the
pretzels
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
(46b) results when the passive transformation does not apply to (49a); (45b)
results when it does. The surface structure of (45b) is:
(49)b.
S
NP
AUX
VP
DET
V
N
PP
P
The
pretzels
were
NP
DET
eaten
by
the
N
mice
A note on the use of passive sentences
Style manuals and many school grammars and composition textbooks advise students (and writers generally) to avoid the passive. However, research
has demonstrated that student writers use the passive less often than expert
writers (Garvey and Lindstrom 1989). It would appear therefore that what
students need to learn is how to use the passive appropriately, a piece of
advice that applies to all sentence types.
There are two major traditional objections to passives. First, they are
alleged to be deceitful: they can and often do omit reference to the agent
responsible for an event. Second, because they begin with an NP whose semantic role is not an Agent and include a form of be, they are alleged to be
“weak.”
In response to the first objection we say that omitting pieces of sentences
is not something that only passives can do. Many if not all sentence types
can. So in this regard writers need to decide just what information must be
presented and what can be omitted from a text. This is a matter of audience,
not just of grammar.
In response to the second objection, most languages of the world have
constructions that correspond to the English passive. It would be most unlikely for these constructions to have developed and been retained if they
were not of considerable value. What they do is allow the information rep405
Delahunty and Garvey
resented in the active to be restructured. Because old, familiar, or given information tends to be placed before new information so that it can be easily
connected with its prior context, passive allows old information that would
be placed later in the corresponding active to be appropriately placed earlier.
Additionally, truncation (deletion) of the agentive phrase from a passive
allows speakers and writers to efficiently avoid repeating information that
may be readily derivable from the context. Omission of the agentive phrase
also allows scholars a polite way to criticize each other (Meyers 1989).
In fact, all languages provide means to allow information to be restructured. In English alone we have at least the following:
(50) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
f.
g.
h.
i.
j.
k.
l.
The mice ate the pretzels. (Active)
The pretzels were eaten by the mice. (Passive)
The pretzels were eaten. (Truncated passive)
The pretzels, the mice ate. (Topicalization)
What ate the pretzels were the mice. (Wh-cleft)
What the mice ate were the pretzels. (Wh-cleft)
The mice are what ate the pretzels. (Reversed wh-cleft)
It was the mice that ate the pretzels. (It-cleft)
It was the pretzels that the mice ate. (It-cleft)
It was that the mice ate the pretzels. (Inferentials)
Not that the mice ate the pretzels. (Not that sentence)
The thing is that the mice ate the pretzels. (Thing sentence)
The order in which information is deployed in a sentence depends upon
a number of factors, including whether it is already familiar to the audience,
whether it is topical, and whether the speaker/writer wishes to give it special
prominence. Each of the constructions illustrated just above has its own idiosyncratic textual effects, and so must be used in appropriate contexts. We will
discuss a number of these in our chapter on Multi-Clause Sentences.
Exercise
1. Draw both the DS and SS trees of The baby-sitter fed the children
and The children were fed by the baby-sitter.
2. Find five passive sentences in an authentic text. Replace them with
their active counterparts, if possible. Then try to articulate why the
author used the passive instead of the active in each case.
406
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Brown, Penelope and Stephen Levinson. 1987. Politeness: Some Universals in
Language Usage. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Carnie, Andrew. 2007. Syntax: A Generative Introduction. Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Chomsky, Noam. 1964. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Culicover, Peter. 1976. Syntax. New York: Academic Press.
_____ 1982. Syntax. 2nd ed. New York: Academic Press.
Garvey, James J. and David H. Lindstrom. 1989. Pros’ prose meets the
Writers’ Workbench. Computers and Composition 6(2): 81-109.
Huckin, Thomas and L. Pesante. 1988. Existential there. Written
Communication 5(3): 368-391.
LeCarré, John. 1962/1980. A Murder of Quality. New York: Bantam.
Meyers, Greg. 1989. The pragmatics of politeness in scientific articles. Applied
Linguistics 10(1): 1-35.
Radford, Andrew. 1981. Transformational Syntax: A Student’s Guide to Chomsky’s
Extended Standard Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____ 1988. Transformational Grammar: A First Course. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
_____1997a. Syntax: A Minimalist Introduction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
_____ 1997b. Syntactic Theory and the Structure of English: A Minimalist
Approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Ward, Gregory and Betty J. Birner. 2001. Discourse and information
structure. In Deborah Schiffrin, Deborah Tannen and Heidi E. Hamilton
(eds.) Handbook of Discourse Analysis. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
glossary
argument:
any expression syntactically required by another expression, e.g.,
the direct object is an argument of a transitive verb.
argument position: a deep structure position to which a semantic role may
be assigned by a verb, preposition, or other semantic role assignor. See nonargument position.
complementizer: an expression that introduces a subordinate clause, e.g.,
that; the non-argument, structural position (comp) that such an expression
occupies, viz immediately under S' (S-bar).
deep structure: abstract level of syntactic/structural representation posited
by transformational grammar and created by phrase structure rules, into
which lexical insertion rules place lexical items, and which transformational
407
Delahunty and Garvey
rules alter by moving, deleting, or inserting items.
deletion: the removal of parts of a linguistic expression by transformation,
especially when the deleted elements can be readily inferred from the context.
do insertion/support: transformational rule that places a form of the auxiliary do into clauses that include no other auxiliary, to indicate emphasis or
negation or to allow subject auxiliary inversion.
existential there: use of the form there in simple be sentences with indefinite subjects that denote the existence of the subject’s referent, e.g., There is
a house in New Oreleans.
face: our expectation that others will respect our public self-image (positive
face) and desire not to be imposed upon (negative face); concept in politeness theory.
imperative subject deletion: transformational rule that deletes the deep
structure subject of imperative clauses (typically argued to be you).
movement: class of transformational rules designed to explain why expressions that are not in their deep structure position, are interpreted as if they
were. See subject auxiliary inversion, topicalization, wh-movement.
non-argument position: a structural position to which a semantic role may
not be assigned directly by a verb, preposition, or other semantic role assignor, e.g., the COMP position.
particle: minor uninflected part of speech, including words such as up,
over, which can also be used as adverbs and prepositions, and which combine with verbs to create phrasal verbs.
passive voice: one of many marked English sentence constructions that allow the redistribution of information in clauses; contrasts with the active
voice in that it includes a form of be associated with a past participle verb,
its subject typically corresponds to an active object, and the object of its by
phrase (if it has one) corresponds to the active subject.
phrasal verb: idiomatic verbal unit composed of verb and particle, e.g., look
up, hook up.
phrase structure rules (psrs): rules posited by transformational grammar
that create deep structure trees.
politeness: theory designed to account for the use of expressions that orient
to an interlocutor’s face.
semantic roles: set of meaning relations between expressions in sentences,
especially between verbs and noun phrases, e.g., Agent, Theme, etc.
subject auxiliary inversion (sai): movement transformation that takes the
first auxiliary verb in a clause and moves it to the left of the clause’s subject;
applies in interrogatives.
408
Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns
surface structure:
level of syntactic/structural representation posited by
transformational grammar and derived from deep structure by applying
transformational rules.
topicalization: one of many marked English sentence constructions that
allow the redistribution of information in clauses; places to the left of the
subject a phrase that would normally occur elsewhere in a sentence.
transformation/transformational rule: device posited by transformational grammar that transforms deep structures into surface structures by
moving, deleting, or inserting expressions.
truncation: the deletion of the agentive/by-phrase of a passive sentence.
wh-movement: transformation that moves a wh-phrase into COMP.
wh-question: interrogative sentence form in which wh-movement has applied.
409
12 Multi-Clause Sentences
key concepts
Multi-clause sentences: complex, compound, compound-complex
Recursion
Complex sentences
Finite and non-finite clauses
Coordination: compound, compound-complex sentences
Miscellaneous information-restructuring sentence types
i n t ro d u c t i o n
The discussion in this chapter depends on a distinction between sentences
and clauses. Clauses, as we noted, are grammatical units comprising one
subject and one predicate, and consequently, one main verb. Sentences are
grammatical units comprising one or more clauses. An understanding of
the ways in which clauses combine to form sentences is valuable to teachers helping students develop more sophisticated writing skills. Multi-clause
sentences allow the integration of several propositions into a single grammatical unit. One hallmark of good style is an appropriate sequencing of
simple and complex sentences. The ability to compose appropriately complex sentences can be fostered by clause-combining exercises.
multi-clause sentences
The clauses that constitute multi-clause sentences can be combined either
by coordination, subordination, or both, called complex, compound, and
compound-complex sentences, respectively. The most inclusive clause in
each sentence is its main clause (in light italics in the examples just below),
which must be marked as either present or past tense, that is, it must be
finite. A subordinate clause is grammatically dependent on some element
in another clause; it may function as a subject, complement, or modifier in
the higher clause.
A complex sentence contains one or more subordinate clauses (bolded),
e.g., Hofstetter believes that he is being targeted by Homeland Security
agents. In compound or coordinated sentences, two or more clauses are
brought together as grammatical equals, connected by a coordinating conjunction (bolded) to one another e.g., Many people can identify parts of speech
but they cannot justify their identification. Because no clause in a compound
sentence is subordinate to another (by definition), all the clauses in a compound sentence are main clauses.
Compound-complex sentences are a combination of complex and com411
Delahunty and Garvey
pound sentences. They may consist of coordinated clauses (bolded) that are
subordinate to another clause, e.g., Edgeworth believed that novels should
have redeeming social value and that her writing might help improve
social conditions; or one or more of the coordinate clauses may include
one or more subordinate clauses (bolded), e.g., Compound-complex sentences
consist of at least two coordinate clauses and at least one of those must contain a
clause which is subordinate to it.
To orient ourselves, let’s recall that in our chapter on Major Parts of
Speech we distinguished between intransitive verbs (i.e., those that are incompatible with a direct object, such as cought, laugh, lie), transitive verbs
(i.e., those that require a direct object, such as bite, consume, transmit), bitransitive verbs (i.e., those that require a direct and an indirect object, such
as give, offer, send,), linking verbs (i.e., those that “link” a subject with a subject complement, such as be, become, seem), and object complement verbs
(i.e., those that require both a direct object and a complement associated
with that object, such as consider, elect, name). We revisited this subcategorization of verbs in our chapter on Phrases, where we dealt with the distinct
VPs associated with each type. In our chapter on Basic Clause Patterns we
added NP subjects to those VPs to create clauses built around each of these
verb types. We illustrated objects with NPs, and complements with NPs
and APs. In this chapter we substitute clauses for the NPs that functioned in
the chapter on Basic Clause Patterns as subjects, direct objects, indirect objects, and subject and object complements. We will begin with subordinate
finite clauses (clauses that are marked for either present or past tense) and
move on to non-finite clauses (those that are not marked for present or past
tense). Then we will continue the discussion we began in Phrases and Minor Parts of Speech on modifying clauses, including relative clauses (which
are modifiers in NPs), and adverbial clauses (which modify Vs, VPs, and
clauses). We wrap up this chapter with a brief discussion of a few sentence
types that are designed to allow for alternative ways of presenting information in sentences.
We begin with a brief overview of how one clause is included within
another, technically known as recursion.
re c u r s i o n
The possibility of creating multi-clause sentences depends on a characteristic
of language called recursion. Recursion is the possibility of allowing a grammatical category to recur inside another instance of the same category, for
example, an NP within an NP, or an S within an S, and so on.
412
Multi-Clause Sentences
(1) a.
b.
c.
d.
She said something. (One clause)
She said [that I don’t know anything]. (Two clauses)
She said [that I don’t know [what I want]]. (Three clauses)
She said [that I don’t know [what I want [Bill to do]]. (Four
clauses)
e. She said [that I don’t know [what I want [Bill to do]] . . .
(Indefinite number of clauses)
We can represent this schematically as:
(2)
S1
. . . S2. . .
. . . S3 . . .
. . . S4. . .
Recursion is one of the most important characteristics of natural language
because it is the basis of language’s open-endedness, its creativity.
Because one clause can be included (embedded) in another, a given
sentence may have any number of clauses. The main clause is the one that
is not embedded in any other clause. In all the sentences of (1), She said X
is the main clause. All other clauses are subordinate. However, it should be
clear from (2) that not all subordinate clauses are directly subordinate to
the main clause—they may be subordinate to other subordinate clauses. In
(2), S1 is the main clause and all the others are subordinate to it. However,
only S2 is directly subordinate to S1; S3 is directly subordinate to S2, and
S4 is directly subordinate to S3.
complex sentences
In this section we will illustrate the range of functions of subordinate clauses
using only finite clauses, that is, clauses that are marked as either past or
present tense or that contain a modal.
Clauses that function in the nominal range
The subordinate clause in a complex sentence may function as its subject,
direct object, indirect object, object of a preposition, or as a complement.
413
Delahunty and Garvey
Clauses that function as subjects
Subordinate clauses can appear as subjects of main clauses:
(3) a. That students enjoy grammar proves my point.
b. That he fled will convince the jury of his guilt.
c. That this arrangement may not work out is very upsetting.
We can apply our usual types of tests to show that these embedded clauses
are subjects. We can replace them with ordinary NPs:
(4) a. This fact proves my point.
b. His flight will convince the jury of his guilt.
c. That possibility is very upsetting.
The pronouns that appear in this position must be in the nominative case:
(5) a. They prove my point.
b.*Them prove my point.
Notice that when the subject of a sentence is an embedded sentence, the
verb of that sentence is singular; that is, sentential subjects such as those above
are regarded as singular.
Exercise
1. Create five more sentences in which a clause functions as subject.
2. Apply the NP and pronoun tests described just above to demonstrate
that the subordinate clauses really are subjects.
Clauses that function as direct objects
The italicized clauses in (6) are the direct objects of the higher verb:
(6) a. John claims he has earned his first million already.
b. We believe he exaggerates a great deal.
We can demonstrate that the embedded structures in (6) (typically called
complement clauses) are the direct objects of the verbs like claim and believe by using a number of tests. The first test is that NPs substitute for them:
414
Multi-Clause Sentences
(7) a. John claims silly things.
b. We believe his exaggerations.
We can also substitute accusative pronouns for them:
(8) a. He claimed them.
b. We believed them.
The embedded clauses bear the same grammatical relationship to the
verbs of their sentences as the NPs that replace them, and pronouns that
replace them must be in the accusative case. These are clearly direct object
NPs, so the clauses they replace must also be direct objects.
We now introduce a slight complication to the pattern above. Sentences
(6a,b) can be paraphrased as (9a,b) respectively:
(9) a. John claims that he has earned his first million already.
b. We believe that he exaggerates a great deal.
These sentences include that at the beginning of the embedded clause.
Words that introduce clauses in this way have various names. Traditionally, as we saw in our chapter on Minor Parts of Speech, that (and similar
words) has been called a subordinating conjunction. Because it introduces
complement clauses, many linguists refer to it as a complementizer. Because the complementizer occurs in the COMP position, as we described
in our chapter on Modifications of Basic Clause Patterns, it must be part of
the subordinate clause, as shown by the fact that whenever we move a clause
(italicized), its complementizer (bolded) must move too. Compare (6a) and
(6b) with (10a) and (10b), respectively;
(10) a. It is that he has earned a million that John claims.
b. It is that he exaggerates that we believe.
If we leave the complementizer in its old position (italicized), the result is
ungrammatical.
(11) a. *It is he has earned a million that John claims that.
b. *It is he exaggerates that we believe that.
When we move elements, we move entire phrases, not just parts of them.
Sentences (10) and (11) show that the complementizer is an integral part of
415
Delahunty and Garvey
an embedded sentence.
As we hope you remember from our chapter on Minor Parts of Speech,
the complementizer that must be distinguished from the demonstrative pronoun that. The two words just happen to be spelled identically, but within
the system of English grammar they function rather differently. The demonstrative that contrasts with this, these and those, with which it forms a
subsystem within the grammar. The complementizer that does not contrast
with the demonstratives. There are no sentences of English in which an embedded clause is introduced by this or those:
(12) *We believe this/these/those he is a great grammarian.
The complementizer that is optional when the embedded clause is a direct
object, though not when the embedded clause is the subject:
(13) a. That he is a great grammarian is not widely known.
b.*He is a great grammarian is not widely known.
Exercise
From newspapers collect 10 sentences containing finite subordinate
clauses introduced by the complementizer that and 10 more without
that. Is that truly optional or does its presence or absence convey some
meaning? You might consult Biber et al (2002: pp. 321ff.)
Further support for our claim that these embedded clauses are direct objects comes from the fact that they can be passivized, as is typical of object
NPs:
(14) a. That he has earned his first million already is claimed by John.
b. That he exaggerates is believed by many.
Exercise
1. Make up 5 new complex sentences with finite subordinate clauses as
their direct objects.
2. Make a list of the tests for direct object clauses presented above.
Using these tests, show that, in each of the sentences you constructed
416
Multi-Clause Sentences
in Exercise (1), each embedded clause is in fact the direct object.
Indirect question clauses, such as those italicized below, are another type of
direct object clause. They are sentences in which the verb of the main clause
names a questioning speech act, such as ask, wonder, and the like, and the
subordinate clause is a wh- or if-clause with no subject-auxiliary inversion:
(15) a. I wonder who the culprit is. [wh-clause]
b. I asked him whether he was ready to leave. [whether clause]
These can be paraphrased as direct questions such as, “Who is the culprit?” I
wonder and “Are you ready to leave?” she asked. Notice that subject-auxiliary
inversion occurs in direct questions, but not in indirect questions.
Indirect questions must be distinguished from similar sentences with whclauses in direct object position such as:
(16) I know what the thief took.
These cannot be paraphrased as direct questions, but can be paraphrased by
expanding the wh-phrase into a full NP:
(17) I know which thing(s) the thief took.
Exercise
Create another five sentences with finite indirect questions clauses in
them. Show that your subordinate clauses really are indirect question
clauses by rephrasing them as direct questions. Also, create or collect
five direct questions and turn them into indirect questions. (Carter and
McCarthy 2006 pp. 804-24 provide an excellent overview of the ways in
which speech is represented in English discourses.)
Clauses that function as indirect objects
In (18) the italicized clause is the indirect object of gave:
(18) We gave whoever was there a French pastry.
We can demonstrate that this indirect question is the IO of this sentence
417
Delahunty and Garvey
by applying the usual tests—Pro-Sub and passive:
(19) a. We gave him a French pastry.
b. Whoever was there was given a French pastry.
IO clauses are much more restricted than subject or direct object clauses.
They seem to be restricted to clauses that refer to animate entities, which is
not altogether surprising when we consider the typical semantic roles of the
IO phrase, namely, Recipient or Beneficiary.
Exercise
1. Make up five new complex sentences with finite subordinate clauses
as their IOs.
2. For each of the sentences you constructed in Exercise (1) show that
the embedded clause is in fact an IO.
Clauses that function as objects of prepositions
Prepositions also may take sentential objects, most readily when they begin
with who(ever) and similar words (20a-c). The following italicized clauses
are the objects of the prepositions that precede them:
(20) a. We gave the pastry to whoever would eat it.
b. We left the crumbs for whichever birds came by.
c. We slept in what we had worn all day.
We know that the clause is the object of the preposition that precedes
it because if we substitute a pronoun for the clause it must be in its object
form:
(21) a. We gave the pastry to her.
b. We left the crumbs for them.
We can also isolate the entire prepositional phrase:
(22) a. It was to whoever would eat them that we gave the pastries.
b. It was to her that we gave the pastries.
418
Multi-Clause Sentences
Exercise
1. Make up five new sentences with finite subordinate clauses as objects of prepositions.
2. For each of the sentences you constructed in Exercise (1) show that
your embedded clause is in fact the object of its preposition. Use the
tests described in the discussion.
Clauses that function as complements
Subordinate clauses also function as subject or object complements and as
complements within NPs.
Subject complements:
Linking verbs often allow their subject complements to be expressed as clauses:
(23) a. The proposal is that we should teach language, not grammar.
b. The problem is that it is not my phone.
c. The claim is that analyses must be supported by arguments.
Exercise
Create five more sentences containing finite subject complement clauses. Think of ways to demonstrate that the italicized clauses in (23a,b,c)
really are subject complements.
Object complements:
Some verbs that take object complements allow those complements to be
expressed as clauses:
(24) a. She dyes her hair whatever color her car is.
b. They elected her whatever she wanted to be.
Exercise
Create five more sentences containing finite object complement clauses. Think of ways to demonstrate that the italicized clauses in (24a,b)
really are object complements.
419
Delahunty and Garvey
Complements in NPs:
Certain classes of nouns take complements, which may be expressed as clauses:
(25) a. The idea that the Earth is only a few thousand years old has been
utterly disproved.
b. The claim that genetics determines character is intriguing.
Note the overlap between nouns that take complement clauses and nouns
that can occur as the head of the subject of a sentence with a subject complement clause, e.g., idea. In fact, a NP with a complement clause can typically
be rephrased as a subject complement sentence with a clausal complement;
compare the subject of (25a) with The idea is that the Earth is only a few thousand years old.
Exercise
Create five more sentences containing NPs that contain complement
clauses, like those in (25a,b). For each, provide evidence that your
noun complement clauses really are noun complement clauses.
Clauses that function as modifiers
We turn now to clauses that function as modifiers of various elements in
sentences. We begin with relative clauses (RCs), which occur in NPs and
modify their heads, for example, We all know the person whom/that you
spoke to. Later we will deal with clauses that modify Vs, VPs, and other
clauses, namely adverbial clauses.
Clauses that modify nouns (relative clauses)
Relative clauses (RCs) (also, but misleadingly, called adjective clauses), follow the head nouns they modify and may begin either with that, a wh-word
such as who or which, a phrase with a wh-word in it, or no special word at
all. Relative clauses must be divided into two types, restrictive and non‑restrictive (or appositive) relatives. In written English, appositive relatives are
separated from their head noun by a comma and end with another comma.
Restrictive relatives are not set off by commas. The presence or absence of
commas reflects a semantic difference between these two types, although
there are formal differences between them too, which we deal with below.
We begin by illustrating some of the variety of restrictive relatives.
420
Multi-Clause Sentences
(26) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
The man that we bought the boat from skipped town.
The man who(m) we bought the boat from skipped town.
The man from whom we bought the boat skipped town.
The man whose boat we bought skipped town.
The man 0 we bought the boat from skipped town.
We want you to notice a number of features of these clauses. First, the
finite RCs can be introduced by that (26a), a wh-word (26b), a phrase containing a wh-word (26c,d), or no introducer (0) at all (26e).
Second, each of the relative clauses has a “gap,” a position, which if the
clause were rephrased as a complete sentence, would have to be filled. In the
sentences in (27), we indicate the gap as e. In (27a) the part of the NP that
is modified by the RC, the man, is interpreted as the subject of the RC, so
the gap is in the RC subject position. In (27b) the part of the NP modified
by the RC, the boat, is interpreted as the direct object of the RC, so that the
gap is in the RC DO position; in (27c, d) the part of the NP modified by
the RC, the man, is interpreted as the object of a preposition in the RC, so
the gap occurs after the preposition in each case. This claim is supported
by the fact that the preposition is not followed immediately by its object,
indicating that the gap is the OP position.
(27) a. The man that/who e sold us the boat skipped town. [Subject]
b. The boat that/which/0 the man sold us e broke down. [Direct
object]
c. The man that/who/0 we sold the boat to e is very upset.
[Object of preposition]
d. The man that/who/0 we bought the boat from e skipped
town. [Object of preposition]
Third, the wh-word is interpreted as coreferential with (i.e., referring
to the same entity as) the head noun and any other modifiers of the NP
that contains the RC. If we were to rephrase the relative clauses in (27) as
independent sentences, we would replace the relative pronouns (where they
occur) with the head noun and any modifiers, giving us:
(28) a.
b.
c.
d.
The man sold us the boat.
The man sold us the boat.
We sold the boat to the man.
We bought the boat from the man.
421
Delahunty and Garvey
Fourth, if the RC contains a wh-phrase, then that phrase is coreferential
with the gap in the RC. We indicate coreferentiality by identical subscripts:
(29) a. [The man]i whomi we bought the boat from ei skipped town.
b. [The man]i [from whomi]j we bought the boat ej skipped town.
If there is no introducer, or if the introducer is that, then the head N and
any modifiers is directly coreferential with the gap:
(30) [The man]i (that) we bought the boat from ei skipped town.
Fifth, if the wh-word is the genitive whose, then the rest of the NP modified by whose must move to the COMP position along with whose.
(31) a. The man whose boat we bought skipped town.
b.*The man whose we bought boat skipped town.
Sixth, if the wh-word is governed by a preposition, then the preposition
may or may not move to the front of the sentence with it, as in (32a,b).
(32) a. The man from whom we bought the boat skipped town.
b. The man whom we bought the boat from skipped town.
If the introducer of the relative is that, or if there is no introducer, then no
movement of the preposition can take place, as the ungrammaticality of
(33a,b) shows:
(33) a. *The man from that we bought the boat skipped town.
b. *The man from we bought the boat skipped town.
Some grammarians call both the wh-words and that relative pronouns.
This appears to us to ignore differences between them such as the ones we
just noted. We will therefore distinguish between wh-words, which are true
pronouns, and that, which we have called a complementizer. That, as a complementizer, is morphologically invariant and appears only at the beginnings of subordinate clauses.
The restrictive relatives clauses that we have been examining are typically
interpreted as providing information necessary for identifying the referent
of the entire NP. Another kind of relative clause, the non-restrictive, supplies extra information that is not considered necessary to identify the refer422
Multi-Clause Sentences
ent of the NP:
(34) a. The claim, which is fully supported by the evidence, . . .
b. The claim which is fully supported by the evidence . . .
The non‑restrictive relative, (34a), refers to some claim and then adds the
supplementary information that the claim is fully supported by the evidence. The reader/hearer is assumed to know which claim is being referred
to without this extra information. The restrictive relative, (34b), refers to a
claim that is assumed to be identifiable only by using the information in the
relative clause to distinguish the intended claim from other claims.
One syntactic effect of this difference between restrictive and nonrestrictive relatives is that the head of a non-restrictive, but generally not of
a restrictive, may be a proper noun:
(35) a. Bill, who is well known to all of us, will sing his favorite tune
“Home on the Range.”
b. *Bill who is well known to all of us will sing his favorite tune
“Home on the Range.”
One explanation for this is that the referents of proper nouns are assumed
to be identifiable by hearers/readers without extra information. Restrictive
relatives, whose information is assumed to be essential for the identification
of the referent, are therefore redundant with proper nouns. Non-restrictive relatives modifying proper nouns, whose information is assumed to be
supplementary, are not redundant. We do, however, find sentences such as
the following, which might be used in a situation in which there are several
individuals called Bill. In that case the usual assumption associated with
proper names may be suspended and the specific Bill being referred to can
be identified by a restrictive clause:
(36) The Bill who has the rose between his teeth . . .
We turn now to some formal differences between restrictive and nonrestrictive relatives that we mentioned above. Restrictive relative clauses may
be introduced by either a wh-word, that, or zero. Non‑restrictive clauses
may be introduced only by wh-words.
(37) a. Mr. Pferdfeld, whom we have just met, . . .
b.*Mr. Pferdfeld, that we have just met, . . .
423
Delahunty and Garvey
c. *Mr. Pferdfeld, we have just met, . . .
Moreover, restrictive relative clauses may be moved away from the nouns
they modify, but non-restrictives may not:
(38) a. A man who was from Iceland came in.
b. A man came in who was from Iceland.
(39) a. Bill, who was from Iceland, came in.
b.*Bill came in, who was from Iceland.
We should also mention here, that other modifiers in NPs can be restrictive
or non‑restrictive, such as PPs.
(40) a. Dell computers with touch-screen capability . . .
b. Dell computers, with touch-screen capability, . . .
So far we have described only full finite relative clauses. However, we also
find reduced relative clauses:
(41) The man standing near the entrance is my father.
(41) can be interpreted as an elliptical version of (42):
(42) The man who is standing near the entrance is my father.
Reduction of this sort is common when the implied material is a wh-word
and an inflected form of be, which as we saw before, is referred to as whizdeletion.
Reduced relatives may also function as non‑restrictive modifiers:
(43) a. Astrid, standing near the entrance, was almost trampled in the rush.
b. Astrid, who was standing near the entrance, . . .
Exercise
1. Using (a) as a model, for each sentence below: (1) identify the relative clause; (2) determine whether a wh-word, that, or zero introduces
the clause; (3) identify the expression modified by the clause; (4) locate the gap in the clause; (5) “normalize” the clause by expressing it
as an independent sentence as in (28); and (6) identify the grammatical
424
Multi-Clause Sentences
function of the gap in the clause.
a. [The one] (3) [(1) RC that (2) I choose e (4)] will be rewarded. I
choose the one (5). Direct object (6).
b. The guy who brought the whoopie cushion will be disciplined.
c. The person you give it to will just throw it away.
d. Zelda bought the pink flamingo that Scott liked so much.
e. Wanda asked the man she considered the main suspect where he
had been on the night of the robbery.
f. They laugh best who laugh last.
2. Using (a) as a model, combine the clauses in each of the following
pairs of clauses so that one member of each pair becomes a full relative
clause modifying a noun phrase in the other:
a. The avocados taste delicious. The avocados came from California. The avocados that came from California taste delicious. The
avocados that taste delicious came from California.
b. I am writing a term paper. The term paper is taking me forever.
c. The computer is acting up. My wife bought me the computer.
d. The book is an excellent source of information. I got this idea
from the book.
e. The appendix provides answers to the questions. The appendix is
at the end of the book.
3. If any of the full relative clauses you created in (2) can be reduced
(e.g., by whiz-deletion), then do so, e.g., The cheese which is from Wisconsin . . ., The cheese from Wisconsin . . .
4. Each of the following sentences contains a relative clauses. Using (a)
as an example, separate each sentence into two clauses, analogous to
the pairs of clauses in Exercise (2):
a. The clause that includes all the subordinate clauses in a sentence
is the main clause. The clause is a main clause; The clause includes all the subordinate clauses in a sentence.
b. We discuss a few sentence types that allow the rearrangement of
phrases.
c. An absolute construction is a construction that is grammatically
set apart from the rest of the sentence.
d. The constructions that we have been examining are restrictive relatives.
e. The sentences whose properties we discussed are quite compli425
Delahunty and Garvey
cated.
(Note: Clause-combining, as in exercise (2), and clause-decombining
as in exercise (4), are valuable tools for teaching relative clauses. The
technique can also be used with other multi-clause sentence types.)
5. The difference between written restrictive and non-restrictive relatives is indicated through punctuation. How is it indicated in speech?
6. In the following sentences identify each relative clause and indicate
whether it can be restrictive or non-restrictive. (Punctuation has been
omitted intentionally.) If a sentence can be either, discuss the difference of meaning. Punctuate each sentence according to your interpretations.
a. Everyone who viewed the exhibit was satisfied.
b. The visitors who viewed the exhibit were satisfied.
c. Wendy who comes from Wyoming knows a lot about ranching.
d. I hit the brakes which caused the car to fishtail.
Clauses that modify verbs (adverbial clauses)
Adverbial clauses are typically introduced by what have been traditionally
called subordinating adverbial conjunctions (SACs) and generally fulfill
the same functions as AdvPs, indicating such concepts as time, place, condition, cause, and purpose. They appear in the positions typical of AdvPs
(initial, medial and final). Again we begin with finite adverbial clauses and
return to non-finite ones later.
Time clauses
(44) a.
b.
c.
d.
e.
After you left the party, things really began to swing.
As soon as the mailman came, Terry ran to the door.
Before Bush was elected, there was more money for schools.
Since the shuttle crashed, NASA has been demoralised.
When she died, she left her pets $1,000,000.
Place clauses
(45) a. Wherever you find cotton, you will also find the boll weevil.
b. Double quotes should be used only where they are appropriate.
426
Multi-Clause Sentences
Cause clauses
(46) a. Because he hoped to elude his pursuers, Fred continued his trek.
b. Because/since/as funding is scarce, research is hampered.
Purpose clauses
(47)a. We packed food for 6 meals so (that) we could stay out overnight.
b. In order that we could do well, we formed study groups.
Result clauses
(48) a. She was so stunned that she could not speak.
b. The shooting star moved so quickly that I almost missed it.
Conditional clauses
Conditionals are particularly important for ESL students, and are included in
all ESL textbooks. The subordinate clause (typically introduced by if or when)
describes a condition that must be met if the situation in the main clause is to
occur. Discussions of conditionals usually divide them into two major types,
the hypothetical and the real.
The subordinate clause of a hypothetical conditional represents a situation
that either cannot or is unlikely to be met (49a), while real conditions can be
met (49b):
(49) a. If I were eight feet tall, (then) my dunk shot would be better.
b. If I practice more, my swing will improve.
Reduced adverbial clauses
Like relative clauses, some adverbial clauses may also be reduced by deleting
a form of be and a pronominal subject that is coreferential with an NP in the
higher clause, but leaving its SAC. (50a) illustrates a full finite adverbial and
(50b) its reduction.
(50) a. While she was living in Africa, Sheila learned Swahili.
b. While living in Africa, Sheila learned Swahili.
Exercise
1. Make a list of 10 common subordinating conjunctions, for example,
427
Delahunty and Garvey
until, once, whenever. Create or find at least one sentence that contains a subordinate adverbial clause introduced by each of these conjunctions.
2. Provide example sentences to demonstrate that adverbial clauses
can, like AdvPs, appear in initial, medial, and final positions. Is one or
more of these positions more common or natural? Does the status of
the clause as finite or non-finite affect its potential to occupy various
positions?
3. For each sentence you created or found for Exercise (1), identify the
subordinate adverbial clause from the remainder and express it as a
separate clause.
4. Using ten different SACs, create ten pairs of clauses that can be combined so that one member of the pair becomes a subordinate adverbial
clause (e.g., When Pavlov rang his bell; Pavlov’s dogs salivated can be
combined as When Pavlov rang his bell, his dogs salivated or Pavlov’s
dogs salivated when he rang his bell.
5. In an authentic text (i.e., a magazine, book, journal, etc.), find
ten adverbial clauses. For each clause say whether it is a time, place,
cause, result, or conditional clause. For any conditional clauses you
find, say whether they are hypothetical or real.
finite vs. non-finite clauses
Consider now the formal differences between the italicized clauses in the
following sentences:
(51) a. We think the ghost appears at midnight.
b. We want the ghost to appear at midnight.
We can see that both are clauses since each has a subject NP (the ghost) and
a VP (appear/s at midnight). However, the italicized clause in (51a) is a finite
clause: it is in the present tense, and its subject agrees with its verb. We could
even include a modal in it:
(52) We think the ghost will appear at midnight.
428
Multi-Clause Sentences
In contrast, the non-finite clauses like those in (51b) do not allow for agreement or modals:
(53) a. *We want the ghost to appears at midnight.
b. *We want the ghost will appear at midnight.
c. *Oscar’s must reading of the play . . .
d. *The book must lying on the table . . .
e. *The bones must gnawed by the dogs . . .
By the term finite, then, we mean that a clause is marked with a present
or past tense inflection or includes a modal auxiliary. An easy way to spot a
finite clause is to look at its first verb; if it is in the present or past tense or if
it is a modal, then the clause is finite. Otherwise it is non-finite. Additionally, if a subordinate clause is, or can be, introduced by that, then it is finite.
Exercise
For each of the highlighted clauses below, determine whether it is finite or non-finite.
a. Glen claims that he is the world’s greatest limerick poet.
b. Malcolm intends to return to school in the fall.
c. For us to win, we must first qualify for the tournament.
d. To err is human; to forgive is divine.
e. That the Earth is warming is truly cause for alarm.
Non-finite clauses
Non-finite clauses are always subordinate. They fall into three categories—
gerunds, infinitives, and participles.
Gerunds
Gerunds are almost complete clauses whose first verb is a Ving form and
which always function as NPs, therefore as subjects, objects, or objects of
prepositions:
(54) a. Giving grammar lectures is always a challenge. [Subject]
b. Bill’s leaving town confirmed his guilt. [Subject]
c. We encourage discussing language. [Direct object]
d. We give discussing language our highest priority. [Indirect object]
429
Delahunty and Garvey
e. We give our highest priority to discussing language. [Prepositional object]
Gerunds can generally be paraphrased as finite clauses. For example, Bill’s
leaving town can be paraphrased as (55a,b, or c), among others:
(55) a. Bill left town.
b. Bill will leave town.
c. Bill is leaving town.
The subject of the finite paraphrase (Bill) may show up as a genitive phrase
(Bill’s) in the gerund, although in many varieties of English (especially informal ones) it may be in the objective case:
(56) I don’t like his/him being out late at night.
The direct object of the finite paraphrase of a gerund may show up as the
object of the preposition of in the gerund:
(57) a. Oscar read the poem.
b. Oscar’s reading of the poem.
Exercise
(a) Identify the entire gerund phrase in each of the following sentences,
and (b) identify the grammatical role the gerund plays in the sentence
in which it occurs.
a. Parting is such sweet sorrow.
b. We anticipated their losing in the final.
c. The teacher had no objection to my leaving class early.
d. Higgins gave riding his best try.
e. My most pleasurable activity is hiking in the mountains.
f. They taught him skiing.
Infinitives
We will divide infinitives into bare infinitives (58a) and to-infinitives
(58b, c):
(58) a. I saw/heard/felt them leave through the window.
430
Multi-Clause Sentences
b. I want them to leave through the window.
c. For them to leave through the window would be insulting to
our host.
The infinitives in (58a-c) are predicates and as such assign a semantic
role to some NP. In these three examples, the infinitive predicates assign the
role Agent to them. However, note that them is in the accusative rather than
the nominative case and so seems also to be an object of the higher verbs in
(58a, b) and of for in (58c).
In a bare infinitive, the verb appears in its uninflected form. This form of
infinitival clause occurs after modals, with verbs of perception such as see,
hear, and feel, and with other verbs such as let, make, and do.
(59) a. She may/might/should/will/would/can/could/must leave.
b. Leave immediately is what he should do.
c. I saw John take it.
d. I heard Mehta conduct Beethoven’s Ninth.
e. I felt it move under my hand.
f. We let him come in.
g. We made her leave.
h. Do come in.
In a to-infinitive, the verb appears in its uninflected form after to (which
is simply a marker of the infinitive, not a preposition):
(60) a. To leave now would cause a lot of trouble.
b. We want to leave immediately.
c. The goal is to leave as soon as possible.
d. We chose her to be the next president.
e. The horse to back is Ashkenazy Anchovy.
f. To get there before dawn, we must leave at 2 a.m.
The italicized parts are to‑infinitival clauses.
To-infinitives function as subjects (60a), objects (60b), complements
(60c-d), relative clauses (60e), and adverbials (60f ).
Exercise
(a) Identify the entire infinitival phrase(s) in each of the following sentences; (b) for each infinitival you identify, say whether it is a bare
431
Delahunty and Garvey
infinitive or a to-infinitive; and (c) identify the grammatical role the
infinitival plays in the sentence in which it occurs.
a. To err is human; to forgive is divine.
b. Musselwhite intends to consider his options.
c. Musslewhite’s intention to consider his options has his boss worried.
d. They felt the earth shake.
e. To be or not to be is the question.
What complexities did you discover? How did you deal with them?
Participles
We distinguish two types of participles—Ving (a.k.a. present) participles
and Ven (a.k.a. past) participles. Both act as noun modifiers. The first verb
of a present participle is a Ving verb:
(61) a. The book lying on the table is free to whoever wants it.
b. Anyone hoping to get on the boat must have a ticket.
The first verb of a past participle is in the Ven form:
(62) The bones gnawed by the dogs are scattered throughout the yard.
Many participles can be paraphrased as full tensed clauses:
(63) a. The book which is lying on the table . . .
b. Anyone who is hoping to get on the boat . . .
c. The bones which were gnawed by the dogs . . .
Participles may often be viewed as reductions of these full tensed clauses by
whiz-deletion. However, when the verb of the participle is a state verb such
as resemble, the present participle and a finite paraphrase are not so easily
related:
(64) a. A mountain resembling an elephant . . .
b.*A mountain which is resembling an elephant . . .
c. A mountain which resembles an elephant . . .
The participles we’ve discussed have all occurred after the noun they modify,
just as relative clauses do. However, participles may occur to the left of the subject
of the sentence in which they function (we’ll call them preposed participles):
432
Multi-Clause Sentences
(65) a. Pressed by reporters, the president acknowledged that the war
was a fiasco.
In cases like (65a), the participle non-restrictively modifies the subject of
the main clause (the president), as the paraphrases (65b and c) show:
(65) b. The president, pressed by reporters, acknowledged that the war
was a fiasco.
c. The president, who was pressed by reporters, acknowledged that
the war was a fiasco.
Alternatively, the participle phrase in (65a) can be paraphrased as a full or
reduced adverbial clause:
(65) d. When (he was) pressed by reporters, the president acknowledged
that the war was a fiasco.
Sometimes, speakers (and more problematically, writers) will produce
preposed participles that cannot be associated with the subject of the sentence to which they are attached:
(66) Pressed by reporters, the war was acknowledged to be a fiasco.
When a preposed participle cannot easily be associated with an NP in the
main clause, it may difficult to interpret. Such difficult-to-interpret participles are referred to in style manuals and composition textbooks as dangling
participles, which writers are advised to avoid.
In certain cases, often called absolute constructions (italicized), a participle may contain a subject (bolded):
(67) a. All things being equal, we decided to take the train.
b. His soul riven by guilt, Aaron plunged from the precipice.
The term absolute suggests a construction set apart from the rest of the
sentence. An absolute modifies the entire sentence that follows it, much as
an adverbial clause does.
Exercise
(a) Identify the entire participial phrase in each of the following sen433
Delahunty and Garvey
tences; (b) for each participle you identify, say whether it is a Ving
(present) or a Ven (past) participle; and (c) identify the expression
modified by the participle.
a. A miser parted from his money must surely be desperate.
b. The guy giving directions is as lost as everyone else.
c. His lead cut in half, Tiger redoubled his efforts.
d Rejected by the publisher, Ashley consigned his manuscript to the
flames.
e. Having been issued a second yellow card, Renoldo had to sit out
the next game.
f. All being fair in love and war, chess players routinely try to distract their opponents.
g. Remaining students must register at the department office.
Auxiliary verbs in non-finite constructions
Except for modals, non-finite VPs may have a range of auxiliary verbs. A few
of the possibilities are indicated below. (Note that the first auxiliary never
carries a tense inflection.)
(68) a. It is exhilarating to have jumped with a bungee cord. (Infinitive without subject)
b. It is wonderful for Wanda to be jumping from a bungee cord.
(Infinitive with for-to + subject)
c. Egbert regrets having jumped from a bungee cord. (Gerund
with out subject)
d. Waldo’s being prevented from bungee jumping relieved Wanda.
(Passive gerund with subject)
e. Having consulted the reputable sources, I then consulted my
horoscope. (Present participle)
f. Having been advised, I decided to face the bungee jump.
(Present, pefect, passive participle)
g. My guru having been consulted, I decided to face the bungee
jump. (Absolute passive participle with subject)
One final matter pertaining to the forms of participial or reduced relative clauses: as these names suggest, the verb of the reduced relative clause is
in participial form, either ‑en (past participial) or ‑ing (present participial)
form, and elements of the clause are omitted. Consider:
434
Multi-Clause Sentences
(69) a. People sentenced to life in prison . . .
b. Anyone walking on the grass . . .
And compare them with:
(70) a. People who are/were sentenced to life in prison . . .
b. Anyone who is/was walking on the grass . . .
Clearly, the relative clauses in (69a/70a) are passives, and in (69a) the relative pronoun and the form of passive be are omitted. An analogous omission
of who is/was occurs in (69b).
However, the terms past and present are misleading, as they suggest that
participial clauses imply past or present time. The clauses are not in fact
restricted to these interpretations:
(71) People sentenced to life in prison from now on will have to pay
for their keep if the proposed new law goes into effect.
(72) Anyone walking on the grass at that time was subject to a stiff
fine.
In (71) the past participial relative actually refers to a future time, from now
on. In (72) the present participial relative refers to a past time, at that time.
In short, the actual time reference of the reduced relatives is governed by
elements other than their verbs.
c o o rd i n a t i o n
Compound sentences
Compound sentences are the result of combining clauses by coordinating
them with one another. They are typically, though not necessarily, conjoined
by the coordinating conjunctions and, or, or but.
(73) a.
b.
c.
d.
The king is in his counting house and the queen is in her
parlor.
The police must charge you or they must release you.
You must remain here but your partner may go.
The TV is on; the beers are chilled; the teams are on the field;
we’re ready for action.
435
Delahunty and Garvey
Compound-complex sentences
The sentences of (73) illustrate coordination of main clauses. However, subordinate clauses, both finite and non-finite, may also be coordinated:
(74) a. We left because we were tired and because the lecture was boring.
(Conjoined finite adverbial clauses)
b. Alex wanted to sing and to play the piano.
(Conjoined to-infinitive complement clauses)
c. Anyone who attends classes and who pays attention should pass
the course. (Conjoined relative clauses)
d. Climbing Denali and winning the biathlon were Meg’s greatest
accomplishments. (Conjoined gerunds functioning as subject)
Sentences like those in (74), which include coordinated subordinate
clauses, are compound-complex sentences, as are sentences with coordinated main clauses, one or both of which contain at least one subordinate
clause:
(75) The king, who doesn’t like to be disturbed, is in his counting
house, and the queen, who is much more sociable, is in her parlor,
where she is surrounded by her sycophants.
In (75) the coordinated main clauses are The king is in his counting house and
The queen is in her parlor.
m i s c e l l a n e o u s i n f o rm a t i o n - re s t r u c t u r i n g
sentence types
In this section we describe sentence types with subordinate clauses that do
not fit neatly in the categories above: extraposition, NP‑movement (or
raising), tough‑movement, cleft, and pseudo‑cleft constructions.
Extraposition
Clauses that are interpreted as subjects may occur not only in the main clause
subject position but also at the right hand end of the main clause predicate.
Compare the following pair:
(76) a. That Oscar writes poetry upsets his parents.
b. It upsets his parents that Oscar writes poetry.
These two have essentially the same meanings. In both, the italicized clause
436
Multi-Clause Sentences
is interpreted as what upsets Oscar’s parents, though in (76a) the clause
appears as the subject of the main clause, whereas in (76b) that position is
occupied by expletive (a.k.a. empty, dummy) it and the clause occurs at
the end of the predicate. The rule that connects these two constructions is
called extraposition, a name which suggest that the subject clause has been
moved (-posed) outside (extra-) its normal position.
We have already exemplified another construction in which a clause is
extraposed, but repeat our example here for convenience:
(77) a. A man who was from Iceland came in.
b. A man came in who was from Iceland.
Again, both sentences have identical meanings even though in (77b) the
relative clause modifying man appears at the end of the predicate. The rule
that links these two sentence types is called extraposition from NP.
One reason why English (and many other languages) allows extraposition
and extraposition from NP is that clauses tend to be relatively long and so if
they are not moved, they may disrupt the subject‑predicate structure of the
sentences in which they are subordinate. Sentences are generally somewhat
easier to process (understand) when the clause has been extraposed. Another
reason might be that while NPs require case marking, subordinate clauses do
not, and so may be moved to positions that do not receive case.
Exercise
1. Apply extraposition to the following sentences:
a. That dictionaries have poetic qualities has often been proposed.
b. That we should carefully study Diamond’s theories on the collapse of civilizations is abundantly clear.
c. That grammatical subjects are not always topics has been repeatedly shown.
2. Reverse the extraposition in the following sentences:
a. It must be recalled that the earliest inhabitants of the Americas
arrived from Siberia.
b. It is extremely likely that the researchers’ claims are true.
c. It is utterly inconceivable that the press secretary would tell
such an egregious lie.
437
Delahunty and Garvey
NP movement (raising) clauses
Consider now the following two sentences with almost identical meanings:
(78) a. It seems that Oscar has upset his parents.
b. Oscar seems to have upset his parents.
In both Oscar is interpreted as the subject of has/have upset his parents. However, in (78a) it occurs as the subject of the subordinate clause, whereas in
(78b) it occurs as the subject of the higher verb seem. Note that the clause in
(78a) is finite, whereas the clause in (78b) is non‑finite. In fact, Oscar cannot
occur as the subject of an infinitival clause after seem, appear, or turn out:
(79) *It seems Oscar to have upset his parents.
In general, these verbs require that the phrase understood as the subject
of their infinitival complements be moved (“raised”) to become the subject
of the higher verb.
Tough movement
A similar (though by no means identical) movement may occur in sentences
with adjectives such as easy or hard and NPs such as a pain or a treat (and
many other expressions with similar meanings). These are often cutely called
“Tough movement” sentences. Tough movement relates:
(80) a. It is tough to live with Hilda.
b. Hilda is tough to live with e.
Note that (80b) ends with a preposition whose object, Hilda, is missing (indicated by e), or more accurately, displaced: it appears as the subject of the
main clause. Note too that the main clause subject of (80a) is the expletive
it. This it does not refer to anything and occupies a position that receives
no semantic role (which is why it is an expletive or dummy). Hilda, on the
other hand, receives its thematic role from the subordinate verb and preposition. It is as if Hilda had been moved from the position marked e in the
subordinate clause into the higher subject around the adjective tough, hence
the name Tough movement. As another example, compare It is distressing to
have to deal with wasps and Wasps are distressing to have to deal with.
A very reasonable question to ask at this point would be: why does English maintain pairs of sentences such as (78a, b) and (80a, b) whose members have identical meanings? While we do not have a definitive answer, we
438
Multi-Clause Sentences
believe that the reason has to do with the discourse functions of subjects.
Typically, though by no means always, subjects function as the topics of
their sentences. Topics refer to the entities that the sentences are about. So
(78b) is about Oscar in a way that (78a) is not; similarly, (80b) is about
Hilda, whereas (80a) is not. We would use the (b) sentences of these pairs
in discourse contexts slightly different from the contexts in which we would
use the (a) versions.
We turn now to a pair of sentence types that have characteristics akin to
finite relative clauses, the it-cleft and pseudo‑cleft constructions.
It-clefting
The following is an it-cleft sentence, and we will refer to the phrase in square
brackets as its focus and to the italicized clause simply as its clause:
(81) It was [Henry Ford] who invented the assembly line.
Clefts consist of an expletive it higher subject, a form of be, a focus phrase
(which may be any phrase type except VP), and a clause that looks like (but
actually isn’t) a finite relative clause.
The clause is like a relative in that it may be introduced by a wh-word, that,
or (in some cases) nothing at all:
(82) It was Henry Ford (who/that/zero) invented the assembly line.
It also contains a “gap,” which is interpreted as if it were “filled” by the focus
phrase, so that the clefts above mean in essence:
(83) Henry Ford invented the assembly line.
The fact that a cleft can be reduced in this way has led some grammarians to
suggest that the focus was actually moved out of the clause into its position in
the higher clause.
Cleft foci are often interpreted as contrasting with some other phrase. For
example, you might use a cleft such as the ones above if you thought that the
audience believed that Roger Smith invented the assembly line:
(84) It was Henry Ford, not Roger Smith, who invented the assembly
line.
If the focus is a PP, then the sense of contrast may fade somewhat:
439
Delahunty and Garvey
(85) It was in 1789 that the French Revolution broke out.
The construction here suggests something like specifically in 1789, but could
also be used if a hearer believed that the French Revolution broke out in 1689
or 1799.
The clause of a cleft sentence is usually interpreted as known information,
known either to the hearer or by people generally. It is not, however, assumed
to be currently in the hearer’s consciousness (Prince 1978).
Exercise
For each of the following it-cleft sentences, create a brief text into
which it fits naturally.
a. It is Obama who leads the delegate count.
b. It is the few, the powerful, and the famous who shape our collective destiny.
c. It is this level of production excellence that rescues Spielberg’s
movie from being merely a thriller.
d. It is urban life that is associated with excitement, freedom, and
diverse daily life.
Pseudo‑clefting (wh-clefting)
Like it-clefts, pseudo‑clefts “cleave” a sentence around a form of be:
(86) What irritates me is [the amount of sports on TV].
Again, we will refer to the italicized expression as the pseudo‑cleft clause and
to the bracketed phrase as its focus, which are linked by a form of be. The
clause may begin only with the wh-word what:
(87) a. *Who plays golf is Fred.
b. *Which ate the mouse was the cat.
c. *When I arrived was lunchtime.
And again, the basic meaning may be represented by a simpler sentence in
which the focus phrase replaces what:
(88) The amount of sports on TV irritates me.
440
Multi-Clause Sentences
But just as clefts do not mean exactly what their non‑cleft counterparts
mean, neither do pseudo‑clefts and their non-cleft counterparts. The clause
of a pseudo‑cleft represents information that the speaker assumes to be in the
consciousness of the hearer at the time the sentence is uttered (Prince 1978).
Exercise
1. For each of the following pseudo-clefts, create a brief text into
which it fits naturally.
a. What sets the US apart from all other countries is its venerable constitution.
b. What makes blogging special is that it allows individuals to rapidly express and disseminate their thoughts.
c. What must be remembered is that the effects of one’s actions are
never fully calculable.
d. What intrigues me is that morals are also subject to fashion.
e. What we must learn first is not that terrorists are uniquely evil
but that all targeting of civilians is immoral.
2. In the following text, slightly adapted from Martha Grimes’ novel
The Stargazy (1998: 3), find at least one of each of the following: (a)
a complex sentence; (b) a compound-complex sentence; (c) a relative
clause; (d) an it-cleft sentence; (e) a pseudo-cleft sentence; (f) an
extraposed sentence; (g) an NP movement sentence; (h) a to-infinitive
clause; (i) an adverbial clause; (j) a Ving (present) participle; (k) a Ven
(past) participle; and (l) a gerund. Be sure to identify the entire expression in each case.
That was how she felt now. She would have preferred the isolation not be a freezing one, but personal discomfort bothered her
only insofar as it kept her from performing. She had trained herself
to withstand any discomfort that could come along, discomforts of
either body or mind. The mind was more difficult, being limitless.
She raised her eyes for a moment to look up at the stars. In the course
of her studies, she had read that what fueled the stars was the merging
of atoms. Fusion science. What fascinated her was the notion that
the amount of energy in was the amount of energy out. There was
an equation: Q=1. And this, she had to imagine, was perfect balance,
like that of the Alexander Column. It was perfect balance that she
was after; it was all that she was after. She wanted to get to that point
441
Delahunty and Garvey
where nothing resonated, where the past could not pretend to shape
itself into the present, where planes had clear sharp edges to which
nothing clung. People didn’t come into it; they weren’t part of the
equation. What relationships she’d had had been brief and in her control, though her partners didn’t seem aware of this. It was astonishing
that people could be so easily hoodwinked, so easily led.
c o n c l u d i n g re m a rk s
In this chapter, we explored the major grammatical structures that enable
English speakers to create sentences of infinite length and complexity. Despite the intricacy and variety they make possible, finite and non-finite subordinate clauses are individually fairly simple. Each has a rather limited set
of formal properties, and we have had to add very few functions to describe
their workings. But the process of embedding—of building structures within structures—multiplies the potential for variety exponentially. We have
only scratched the surface of that potential. If you want to see some dramatic demonstrations, pick up a sonnet by Shakespeare, a poem by Dylan
Thomas, or a piece of prose by Henry James, William Faulkner, or Virginia
Woolf. Or pick up an essay that you yourself have written recently. You will
probably amaze yourself with the complexity of your own language.
re f e re n c e s a n d re s o u rc e s
Berk, Lynn M. 1999. English Syntax: From Word to Discourse. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Biber, Douglas, Susan Conrad, and Geoffrey Leech. 2002. Longman Student
Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, UK: Longman.
Carter, Ronald and Michael McCarthy. 2006. Cambridge Grammar of English.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Grimes, Martha. 1998. The Stargazy. New York: Onyx.
Huddleston, Rodney and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar
of the English Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
_____ 2005. Student’s Introduction to English Grammar. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Prince, Ellen. 1978. A comparison of wh-clefts and it-clefts. Language 54. 4:
883-906.
glossary
absolute construction:
subordinate expression with no explicit grammatical link to the clause it depends on.
442
Multi-Clause Sentences
adjective clause:
common misnomer for relative clause.
clause that functions in the range of adverbials, viz. modifies verbs, verb phrases, and sentences.
appositive: expression that adds non-restrictive information. See non-restrictive relative clause.
clause: grammatical unit comprising a subject and a predicate.
complex sentence: sentence containing one or more subordinate clauses.
compound sentence: sentence consisting of two or more coordinate clauses.
Also called a coordinate sentence.
compound-complex sentence: sentence that includes both coordinate and
subordinate clauses.
dangling participle: participle occurring to the left of a main clause whose
implied subject cannot be interpreted as coreferential with the main clause
subject.
embedding: incorporation of one clause within another, as subject, object,
complement or modifier, or more generally, the inclusion of one expression
within another. See recursion.
expletive it: occurrences of it that are non-referential, e.g., in the subjects of
sentences to which extraposition has applied.
extraposition: movement of a clausal subject to the end of its sentence and
insertion of expletive it in the subject position.
extraposition from np: movement of a relative clause out of its NP to the
end of its sentence.
finite clause: clause marked for present or past tense. See non-finite clause.
gerund: noun phrase derived from a verb phrase or from a clause, whose first
verb is Ving.
indirect question: clause subordinate to a verb such as ask, wonder, in a
sentence that can be rephrased as a direct question.
infinitive: non-finite clause (1) whose first verb is in its base (uninflected)
form (bare infinitive), or (2) whose first verb is marked by the particle to (to­infinitive).
it-cleft sentence: complex sentence beginning with expletive it, followed
by a form of be, then by a focused phrase, which is followed by a finite clause
containing a gap of the same grammatical type as the focus phrase, in a position from which the focus phrase was ostensibly moved. Used to structurally
highlight the focus phrase, often for purposes of contrast. See pseudo-cleft
sentence.
main clause: clause to which any other clauses in a sentence are subordinate.
non-finite clause: clause that is not marked for present or past tense (see
finite clause); including gerunds, infinitives, and participles.
adverbial clause:
443
Delahunty and Garvey
non-restrictive relative clause: relative clause
whose information is not
necessary to the identification of the referent of the NP of which it is a part.
See restrictive relative clause.
np movement: (apparent) movement of an NP from the subject of a subordinate clause into the subject position of verbs such as appear and seem. Also
called raising.
participle: type of non-finite clause. See past participle, present participle.
past participle: type of non-finite clause whose first verb is in its Ven (past
participle) form and which functions as a modifier, typically in an NP.
present participle: type of non-finite clause whose first verb is in its Ving
(present participle) form and which functions as a modifier, typically in an
NP.
pseudo-cleft sentence: complex sentence whose subject consists of a whclause and whose main verb is a form of be, whose complement is a focused
phrase that is coreferential with the wh-phrase in the subject clause. Used to
structurally highlight the focus phrase, often for purposes of contrast. See itcleft sentence.
raising: See np movement.
recursion: property of natural language that allows expressions to include
expressions of the same type, e.g., clauses within clauses, thus creating the
potential for infinitely long and infinitely many expressions. See embedding.
reduced relative clause: relative clause whose complementizer or whphrase, subject, and copula have been deleted.
relative clause: clausal modifier in an NP. See non-restrictive relative
clause, restrictive relative clause.
restrictive relative clause: relative clause whose information is necessary
for the identification of the referent of the NP of which it is a part. See nonrestrictive relative clause.
sentence: grammatical unit consisting of one or more clauses.
subordinate clause: clause that is grammatically dependent on an element
of another clause.
tough movement: (apparent) movement of an NP from a to-infinitival complement clause to the subject of a predicate such as tough, easy, hard, a pain,
or a treat.
wh-cleft sentence: See pseudo-cleft sentence.
whiz-deletion: deletion of wh-phrase or complementizer, subject, and copula from a modifying clause.
444
Subject Index
Index
A
absolute construction 425, 442.
See also noun
abstract noun 185, 186
accent 9, 20, 105
acceptable 8, 25, 46, 51, 180,
207, 402
accommodation theory. See linguistic accommodation
accusative case 232, 415. See
also case
acquisition of language 51
active vocabulary 9, 19. See
also passive vocabulary
active voice 408. See also passive
voice, voice
adjective 25, 86, 134, 139, 152,
166, 167, 168, 169, 171,
172, 173, 174, 175, 176,
177, 178, 181, 184, 185,
186, 187, 189, 201, 217,
222, 223, 229, 231, 232,
240, 242, 263, 277, 278,
279, 283, 285, 286, 287,
292, 301, 302, 305, 314,
319, 351, 365, 420, 438,
443. See also adjective clause 443
adjective inflection 231
adjective phrase 86, 171, 185,
187, 263, 285, 286, 319,
351, 365. See also phrase
adjunct 185
adverb 25, 168, 178, 179, 180,
181, 182, 183, 185, 186,
187, 221, 222, 224, 232,
237, 240, 249, 277, 279,
280, 281, 282, 285, 286,
318, 319. See also major
part of speech
adverbial 179, 180, 181, 185,
194, 223, 225, 226, 227,
228, 230, 232, 233, 279,
282, 283, 316, 317, 318,
319, 342, 345, 363, 369,
379, 380, 381, 412, 420,
426, 427, 428, 433, 436,
441, 443
adverbial clause 226, 227, 230,
232, 233, 279, 318,
428, 433, 441, 443. See
also clause
adverb phrase 279, 281, 286,
318, 319. See also phrase
affix 130, 141, 166, 168. See
also prefix, suffix
affricate 96, 117. See also consonant, manner of articulation
agent 61, 240, 319, 364, 404,
405. See also semantic role
agreement 5, 24, 55, 105, 196,
197, 231, 264, 322, 325,
326, 327, 328, 330, 351,
354, 384, 429
Aitchison 18, 19, 31, 49, 189,
192, 247, 260, 262, 268
allomorph 125, 128, 131, 139,
141, 142
allophone 112, 113, 117, 125
alveolar 94, 95, 96, 117, 125,
129. See also consonant,
place of articulation
alveo-palatal 94, 117. See
also consonant, place of
articulation
ambiguity 39, 51, 53, 77, 259,
305
445
Delahunty and Garvey
analytic linguistics 48
antecedent 195, 196, 197, 202,
204, 231, 232, 354, 398
antonym 248, 255. See also complementary, conversive,
gradable, non-gradable,
reversive, antonym, sense
relation
antonymy 252, 253, 260, 268,
272. See also sense relation
A-position. See argument position
A’-position. See non-argument
position
applied linguistics 26, 48, 51
appositive 279, 302, 319, 420,
443
approximant 96. See also central,
approximant glide, lateral
approximant semi-vowel
arbitrariness 43, 51
arbitrary 32, 51, 54, 366, 401
article 10, 78, 84, 153, 156,
175, 178, 201, 208, 209,
210, 211, 212, 231, 240,
264, 275, 290, 296, 344,
346. See also definite,
indefinite article
articulatory phonetics 89. See
also phonetics
aspect 8, 80, 159, 160, 163,
164, 165, 166, 187, 213,
216, 231, 232, 233, 248,
253, 256, 307, 308, 321,
322, 338, 340, 341, 342,
343, 345, 346, 347, 365,
376
aspiration 110, 113, 117
assessability 15, 19. See
also learnability, teach-
446
ability
assimilation 114, 117
attribute 160, 363, 364, 365.
See semantic role
attributive 185, 200, 231, 233,
287, 291, 302
attributive adjective 185
attributive noun 231, 233
attributive noun phrase 231, 233
auxiliary verb 65, 66, 67, 141,
164, 165, 166, 231, 319,
337, 338, 339, 379,
392, 393, 394, 408. See
also helping verb, verb
B
back vowel 99, 101, 104, 105,
117. See also vowel
base 47, 86, 141, 262, 313,
392, 443
basic clause pattern 348, 356,
360, 365. See also indirect
object, intransitive location, object complement, recipient/benefactive, simple
transitive
benefactive 364. See also semantic role
beneficiary 62, 200, 285
benefit 17, 54
bilabial 96, 117. See also consonant, place of articulation
bilingual 48, 52, 246
bitransitive verb 320. See also ditransitive verb,verb
bound morpheme 123, 132, 141,
142. See also morpheme
branch 51, 55, 243
broad phonetics. See also narrow
phonetics, phonetics
Subject Index
by-phrase 233, 404, 409. See
also passive voice, voice
C
case 35, 52, 60, 68, 80, 83, 85,
109, 110, 114, 128, 135,
136, 149, 153, 158, 159,
171, 172, 180, 195, 196,
197, 198, 199, 200, 202,
203, 204, 208, 209, 211,
218, 224, 230, 231, 232,
241, 265, 267, 292, 293,
300, 301, 313, 325, 329,
330, 338, 350, 358, 369,
397, 401, 406, 414, 415,
421, 423, 430, 431, 437,
441. See also accusative,
common dative, genitive,
nominative, possessive,
objective case
case form 231, 329
category change 152, 185. See
also conversion
central approximant. See also approximant
central vowel 101. See also vowel
channel 6, 19, 26
clause 36, 54, 55, 58, 60, 62,
64, 65, 67, 69, 70, 71,
72, 79, 80, 86, 87, 128,
135, 141, 159, 166, 183,
194, 207, 213, 214, 215,
216, 223, 224, 225, 226,
227, 228, 230, 232, 233,
240, 274, 277, 279, 285,
287, 301, 302, 303, 304,
307, 309, 310, 314, 315,
316, 318, 319, 320, 321,
322, 323, 324, 326, 327,
330, 334, 336, 337, 338,
339, 341, 346, 347, 348,
349, 350, 351, 353, 354,
355, 356, 358, 360, 361,
362, 363, 365, 366, 369,
379, 383, 385, 388, 394,
397, 400, 402, 407, 408,
411, 412, 413, 414, 415,
416, 417, 418, 419, 420,
421, 422, 423, 424, 425,
426, 427, 428, 429, 431,
433, 434, 436, 437, 438,
439, 440, 441, 442, 443,
444. See also adverbial,
complement, conditional,
embedded, finite, indirect
object, infinitive, main,
nonfinite;; See also noun,
noun complement, object
complement,result clause,
that-clause, basic clause
pattern
cleft 406, 436, 439, 440, 441,
443, 444. See also it-cleft,
pseudo-cleft, wh-cleft
coda 106, 117, 119. See syllable
co-hyponym 269, 270. See
also coordinate, sense relation
collective noun 185. See
also noun
combined entry 240, 269
comment 301, 324, 364, 365.
See also topic
common case 200, 231. See
also case
common noun 149, 152, 185,
187. See also noun
comp 389, 390, 392, 394, 395,
396, 408, 409, 415, 422.
447
Delahunty and Garvey
See also complementizer
comparative 76, 127, 140, 142,
173, 174, 177, 180,
185, 186, 222, 249. See
also superlative
competence 36, 37, 38, 39,
40, 43, 51, 52, 53, 54,
55, 131, 252, 253, 254,
258, 321, 384, 385. See
also linguistic competence,
performance
complement 70, 71, 74, 155,
171, 185, 186, 187, 191,
200, 216, 217, 232, 233,
237, 240, 277, 278, 282,
283, 284, 286, 287, 289,
303, 304, 314, 315, 316,
317, 318, 319, 350, 351,
352, 353, 354, 355, 356,
359, 365, 411, 412, 413,
414, 415, 419, 420, 436,
443, 444. See also object,
subject, complement
complementary 112, 117, 125,
269
complementary antonym. See
also antonym, sense relation
complementary distribution 112,
117, 125
complement clause 240, 420,
444. See also clause
complementizer 407, 415, 416,
422, 444. See also comp
complex postmodifiers 301
complex sentence 411, 413, 441,
443, 444. See also sentence
complex words 121, 138
componential model l. See
448
also network model
component of language 53
composition text 52
compound-complex sentence 441,
443. See also sentence
compounding 136
compound sentence 411, 443.
See also sentence
compound word 132, 141, 142.
See also appositional,
coordinative, endocentric,
exocentric compound
conative meaning 30. See
also meaning, persuasive
meaning
concordancer 20, 269. See
also corpus
concrete noun 185, 186. See
also noun
conditional clause 428. See
also clause
conjunction 194, 222, 225, 228,
231, 233, 240, 305, 411,
415. See also coordinating, correlative, nominal,
relative, subordinating,
subordinating adverbal
conjunction
consonant 96, 102, 105, 106,
109, 110, 112, 113, 114,
117, 118, 119, 129, 130,
208, 368, 371, 372. See
also ner of articulation,
nasality, place of articulation, voiced sound, voiceless sound
constituent 75, 106, 121, 125,
133, 134, 141, 142, 322
content word 186, 231. See
also major part of speech
Subject Index
context 5, 13, 20, 29, 33, 37,
47, 90, 111, 138, 153,
154, 170, 196, 197, 210,
252, 263, 264, 267, 272,
273, 323, 324, 325, 359,
375, 379, 401, 406, 408.
See also co-text, discourse,
linguistic, situational
context
contrast 8, 41, 108, 109, 118,
126, 193, 203, 219, 222,
236, 251, 259, 296, 323,
416, 429, 439, 443, 444.
See also distinctive
convention 32, 52, 54
conventional 7, 13, 32, 52, 132,
239
conventionality 32, 52
conversion 130, 131, 141, 151,
152, 185, 186, 187, 190.
See also functional shift,
zero derivation
conversive antonym. See also antonym
coordinate 4, 70, 71, 72, 231,
262, 270, 305, 412, 443.
See also co-hyponym
coordinate sentence 443. See
also sentence
coordinating conjunction 231,
305, 411. See also conjunction
coordinative compound 141. See
also appositional compound, compound word
corpus 20, 52, 79, 269. See
also concordancer
corpus linguistics 52
correlative conjunction 231. See
also conjunction
co-text 197. See also context
linguistic context
count noun 156, 186. See
also mass noun, noun
creativity of language 42, 52, 53
critical thinking 9, 13, 14, 20,
149
cross-reference 243, 270
cultural transmission 35, 52
D
dangling participle 443
dative case. See case
declarative 66, 67, 231, 232,
392. See also indicative
mood
deep structure 134, 383, 385,
386, 389, 390, 391, 392,
394, 395, 397, 398, 400,
402, 404, 407, 408, 409.
See also surface structure,
transformation
defective member 189
definite article 208, 209, 210,
211, 212. See also article
definiteness 209, 231, 232
degree adverb 186, 249, 279,
319. See also intensifier
deictic 202, 231, 282, 368
deletion 114, 119, 383, 386,
397, 400, 401, 406, 408,
409, 424, 425, 432, 444
demonstrative 202, 203, 223,
228, 231, 296, 297, 416
demonstrative pronoun 228, 416.
See also pronoun
derivation 129, 130, 136, 142,
151, 152, 156, 159, 168,
187, 240, 402
derivational morpheme 76, 129,
449
Delahunty and Garvey
141, 166. See also morpheme
descriptive adjective 186. See
also adjective
descriptive grammar 20. See
also grammar
descriptive rule. See rule
determiner 175, 176
diachronic linguistics 48, 52. See
also synchronic linguistics
diacritic 110, 112, 117
dialect 26, 52, 111, 151, 167,
239, 242
dialectology 48, 52
dictionary entry 269, 270, 271,
272. See also entry word,
head word
diminutive 33, 35, 52
diphthong 105, 117, 118. See
also monophthong, vowel
directive 27
direct object 54, 62, 87, 171,
186, 187, 191, 204, 263,
272, 274, 275, 288, 304,
314, 315, 318, 349, 350,
353, 354, 355, 356, 357,
358, 359, 361, 363, 384,
385, 388, 389, 397, 404,
407, 412, 413, 415, 416,
417, 418, 421, 430. See
also object
discourse 5, 6, 7, 11, 14, 20,
24, 37, 38, 39, 57, 58,
87, 88, 148, 155, 194,
195, 196, 208, 210, 211,
232, 250, 271, 324, 325,
383, 387, 391, 392, 401,
402, 403, 439
discourse context. See context,
co-text
450
discourse function 325
distinctive 110, 117, 119, 122.
See also contrast
ditransitive verb. See bitransitive
verb
do-insertion. See do-support
do-support. See do-insertion
duality 52
dummy it. See expletive it
E
effected 365. See also factitive,
semantic role
elements of clause. See predicate,
subject
embed 242, 319
embedded clause 415, 416, 417,
418, 419. See also clause
empty 43, 249, 365, 368, 372,
389, 437. See also expletive semantic role
endocentric compound 142. See
also compound
entail 347
entry word 239, 240, 244,
269, 270, 271, 272. See
also dictionary entry, head
word
error 26, 121, 158, 262
etymology 236, 241, 244, 245,
270. See also word history
existential there 402, 403, 408
exocentric compound 142. See
also compound word
experiencer 334, 365. See
also semantic role
experiential meaning. See ideational, referential meaning,
meaning
explain 15, 27, 31, 37, 41, 42,
Subject Index
43, 45, 55, 102, 206,
254, 259, 374, 383, 397,
398, 400, 408
expletive 365, 388, 437, 438,
439, 443. See also empty,
semantic role
expletive it 388, 438, 439, 443.
See also dummy it
expressive meaning 30, 52. See
also meaning
extraposition 436, 437, 443
extraposition from NP 437
F
face 6, 14, 26, 57, 129, 196,
266, 284, 399, 408, 434
factitive 365. See also effected,
semantic role
feature 105, 113, 114, 118, 129,
175, 189, 272, 354
feature changing rule 118
feminine gender. See gender
field 6, 29, 242, 243, 266, 270,
435
finite clause 287, 320, 428, 429,
443, 444. See also clause
first person 4, 198, 199, 232,
330, 378. See also person
focus 3, 7, 18, 26, 149, 304,
367, 439, 440, 443, 444
foot 106, 107, 118, 251, 254,
271. See also syllable
force 28, 91, 154, 189, 232,
332, 346, 365. See
also semantic role
form 4, 5, 14, 18, 23, 27, 30,
31, 36, 40, 41, 45, 48,
53, 55, 63, 66, 79, 82,
83, 86, 87, 88, 123, 125,
126, 127, 128, 129, 130,
137, 138, 142, 152, 153,
157, 165, 166, 167, 168,
170, 171, 177, 180, 186,
187, 193, 197, 199, 200,
203, 206, 212, 213, 214,
216, 221, 231, 232, 241,
243, 244, 245, 248, 250,
260, 274, 290, 293, 297,
299, 307, 308, 309, 311,
313, 319, 320, 327, 328,
329, 330, 336, 337, 341,
342, 344, 347, 364, 366,
369, 371, 378, 379, 383,
385, 389, 394, 397, 399,
402, 403, 404, 405, 408,
409, 411, 418, 424, 427,
429, 431, 432, 434, 435,
439, 440, 443, 444
formality 26, 27, 55, 242
frame 153, 154, 177, 263, 272
free morpheme 122, 123, 131,
142. See also morpheme
fricative 94, 95, 96, 118, 125.
See also consonant, manner
of articulation
front vowel 100, 101, 105, 118.
See also vowel
function 36, 37, 42, 43, 57, 70,
72, 82, 84, 86, 87, 88,
122, 142, 148, 155, 157,
171, 172, 176, 177, 178,
179, 185, 186, 187, 193,
194, 200, 203, 206, 207,
208, 225, 226, 227, 231,
232, 233, 243, 265, 274,
275, 277, 282, 287, 299,
301, 314, 318, 319, 322,
324, 325, 342, 352, 365,
401, 411, 413, 414, 416,
417, 418, 419, 420, 424,
451
Delahunty and Garvey
425, 429, 431, 432, 439
functional diagram. See ReedKellogg diagram
functional shift 130, 152, 186.
See also conversion, zero
derivation
function word 186, 187, 231,
232, 233. See also minor
part of speech, structure,
word
G
gender 26, 55, 80, 196, 197,
198, 201, 202, 207, 211,
231, 397. See also feminine, grammatical, masculine, natura,l neuter, gender
generic pronoun 201, 231. See
also pronoun
generic reference 210, 291. See
also specific reference
genitive case 158, 159, 196,
202, 203, 232. See
also case, possessive case
genitive pronoun 297. See
also pronoun
genre 6, 20, 95, 160, 164, 381
gerund 166, 186, 319, 320,
430, 434, 441, 443. See
also verbal phrase
given information 387, 406. See
also information
glide 96, 104, 115, 119. See
also approximant, labiovelar, palatal glide
glottal 96, 118. See also consonant, place of articulation
goal 43, 44, 57, 86, 314,
365, 374, 385, 431. See
also semantic role
452
gradable adjective 25, 177, 242
gradable antonym. See antonym
grammar 4, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14,
15, 17, 20, 21, 26, 38,
40, 42, 44, 45, 46, 52,
53, 57, 82, 85, 86, 115,
122, 128, 160, 166, 175,
186, 187, 189, 190, 191,
229, 230, 231, 232, 233,
238, 243, 263, 275, 283,
295, 319, 321, 322, 324,
331, 338, 368, 377, 383,
384, 385, 386, 387, 405,
407, 408, 409, 414, 416,
419, 429. See also descriptive, internalized, prescriptive grammar
grammatical analysis 35, 84, 86
grammatical category 86, 152,
232, 275, 325, 368, 412
grammatical function 87, 122,
142, 200, 208, 274, 424.
See also grammatical relation
grammatical gender 201. See
also gender
grammaticality 53, 294, 296,
328. See also ungrammaticality
grammatical reasoning 296
grammatical relation 276, 277.
See also grammatical function
grammatical role 70, 430, 432
grammatical structure 85, 252,
321
H
habitual aspect 341, 342, 347.
See also aspect
Subject Index
head word 73, 84, 195, 232,
236, 244, 246, 288,
289, 306, 320, 327. See
also dictionary entry, entry
word
helping verb 319. See also auxiliary verb
high vowel 118. See also vowel
homograph 240, 270
homophone 270
hypernym 270. See also sense
relation, superordinate
hyponym 263, 269, 270. See
also sense relation
hyponymy 249, 250, 252, 262,
263, 268, 270, 272. See
also sense relation
hypothesis 16, 20
I
ideational meaning. See experiential, referential meaning,
meaning
ideology 7, 20
idiom 267, 270, 340
imperative 66, 67, 80, 232,
398, 399, 400, 408. See
also imperative subject
deletion, mood
imperative subject deletion 408
indefinite article 156, 208, 209,
210, 211. See also article
indefinite pronoun 302, 404. See
also pronoun
indicative 66, 80, 138, 231,
232, 337, 338, 399. See
also declarative mood
indirect object 54, 62, 155, 204,
219, 263, 274, 283, 315,
318, 356, 357, 358, 359,
360, 361, 404, 412, 413,
417, 429. See also object
indirect object clause. See basic
clause pattern, clause
indirect question 303, 417, 443
infinitive 170, 186, 187, 307,
319, 431, 432, 434, 436,
441, 443. See also clause,
nonfinite clause, verbal
phrase
infinity of language 43, 53. See
also recursion
inflection 127, 150, 152, 159,
165, 166, 186, 214, 231,
270, 299, 338, 429, 434
inflectional morpheme 76, 142.
See also morpheme
information iv, 7, 12, 15, 16,
28, 29, 37, 49, 53, 54,
82, 83, 86, 87, 115, 121,
127, 136, 144, 147, 149,
156, 157, 163, 164, 171,
186, 187, 190, 193, 196,
197, 206, 209, 210, 211,
231, 232, 233, 235, 236,
237, 238, 239, 240, 241,
242, 243, 251, 262, 263,
266, 267, 268, 270, 271,
272, 279, 302, 324, 366,
387, 403, 405, 406, 407,
408, 409, 411, 412, 422,
423, 425, 436, 440, 441,
443, 444. See also given,
new, old information
information question 206, 232,
233. See also mood, whquestion, yes/no question
instrument 270, 365. See also semantic role
intensifier 86, 186, 222, 232,
453
Delahunty and Garvey
249, 279, 281, 285, 286,
297, 319. See also degree,
adverb
intensive pronoun 204, 232, 233.
See also pronoun, reflexive
pronoun
intention 4, 6, 53, 54, 214, 256,
380, 432
interactional force 232
interchangeability 35, 53, 54
interdental 118. See also consonant, place of articulation
interjection 229, 232, 240
internalized grammar 52, 53. See
also grammar
interpersonal meaning. See meaning, phatic meaning, social
meaning
interrogative pronoun. See pronoun
interrogative sentence 409. See
also mood, wh-question,
yes/no question
intransitive clause. See basic
clause pattern
intransitive verb 186, 306, 314,
315. See also transitive
verb, verb
intuition 53, 168. See also linguistic intuition
inversion 307, 392, 395, 408,
417. See also subject-auxiliary inversion
it-cleft 439, 440, 441, 443, 444.
See also cleft, pseudo-cleft,
wh-cleft
L
labiodental 118. See also consonant, place of articulation
454
labiovelar glide. See approximant
glide
language 3, 4, 5, 7, 8, 9, 10,
11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16,
17, 18, 20, 21, 23, 24,
25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30,
31, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36,
37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42,
43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48,
49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54,
55, 57, 58, 70, 71, 74,
76, 79, 81, 82, 83, 84,
86, 87, 88, 89, 105, 107,
108, 110, 111, 112, 113,
115, 117, 119, 122, 125,
126, 128, 129, 133, 136,
137, 138, 140, 147, 150,
152, 155, 156, 164, 173,
180, 188, 190, 196, 198,
201, 213, 230, 231, 235,
237, 238, 239, 241, 243,
246, 247, 251, 253, 254,
256, 257, 258, 261, 266,
267, 270, 272, 273, 278,
300, 316, 321, 322, 338,
366, 367, 368, 378, 399,
412, 413, 419, 429, 430,
442, 444
language acquisition 16, 41, 48,
49. See also acquisition of
language
lateral approximant. See approximant
lax vowel 104, 105. See
also vowel
learnability 15, 20. See also assessibility, teachability
learning ii, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 16,
18, 26, 35, 40, 41, 42,
50, 54, 116, 128, 129,
Subject Index
156, 201, 237, 267, 316,
401
learning environment 35
letter 6, 30, 62, 68, 81, 82, 87,
90, 111, 113, 114, 123,
136, 137, 149, 155, 179,
203, 239, 270, 305, 321,
331
lexeme 77, 87, 88. See
also word form
lexical field 270
lexical insertion 407
lexical verb. See major part of
speech, verb
lexicon 253, 268, 271. See
also mental dictionary
ligature 187, 270
linguistic accommodation. See accommodation theory
linguistic competence 36, 40, 52,
53, 55, 252, 253, 385.
See also linguistic performance
linguistic context 401. See
also context, co-text
linguistic insecurity 8, 9, 20
linguistic intuition 53
linguistic meaning 53, 235, 272.
See also meaning
linguistic performance. See linguistic competence
linking verb 171, 175, 186, 310,
365. See also verb
location 27, 74, 278, 332, 333,
365, 402. See also semantic role
location clause. See basic clause
pattern
logical subject 319. See also subject
low vowel 118. See also vowel
M
main clause 70, 71, 72, 214,
366, 411, 413, 417, 425,
427, 433, 436, 437, 438,
443. See also clause
main verb 60, 61, 62, 64, 65,
66, 69, 87, 141, 162,
164, 165, 186, 213, 214,
231, 306, 307, 310, 313,
336, 339, 343, 347, 365,
372, 378, 394, 411, 444.
See also verb
major part of speech. See adjective, adverb, content word,
noun, verb
manner of articulation 91, 93,
96, 118. See also affricate,
approximant, consonant,
fricative, nasal, stop
marked 70, 79, 209, 211, 214,
248, 271, 272, 311, 319,
338, 339, 341, 352, 354,
366, 408, 409, 411, 412,
413, 429, 438, 443. See
also unmarked
masculine gender. See gender
mass noun. See count noun
meaning 5, 8, 12, 17, 28, 29,
30, 32, 33, 34, 37, 43,
46, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55,
57, 75, 76, 77, 78, 82,
83, 86, 87, 108, 110,
117, 118, 122, 124, 125,
126, 131, 133, 135, 136,
137, 138, 139, 142, 148,
152, 155, 158, 159, 160,
161, 163, 164, 170, 172,
184, 193, 203, 204, 208,
455
Delahunty and Garvey
210, 213, 217, 219, 220,
221, 222, 224, 229, 235,
240, 244, 245, 246, 247,
248, 249, 250, 251, 252,
253, 254, 256, 257, 260,
261, 263, 266, 267, 269,
270, 271, 272, 279, 280,
291, 292, 297, 298, 302,
315, 321, 324, 343, 344,
352, 355, 357, 358, 361,
369, 372, 380, 387,
408, 416, 426, 440. See
also conative, experiential,
expressive, ideational,
interpersonal, linguistic,
metalinguistic meaning;
See also phatic, persuasive,
poetic, referential, social,
textual meaning
meaningless 33, 46, 52, 53
medium 6, 7, 19, 21, 30, 400
mental dictionary 262, 271. See
also lexicon
meronymy 250, 253, 271. See
also sense relation
metalinguistic meaning 30, 53.
See also meaning
metaphor 252, 267, 271, 272,
366. See also sense relation
metathesis. See sense relation
metonymy 251, 272. See
also sense relation
mid vowel 118. See also vowel
minor part of speech. See conjunction, function word, intensifier, particle, preposition,
pronoun, structure word,
wh-word
modal verb 214, 338, 377, 378.
See also auxiliary verb
model 36, 39, 40, 45, 52, 53,
456
55, 254, 255, 256, 258,
259, 260, 269, 271, 307,
309, 310, 311, 312, 321,
336, 386, 424, 425
modifier 63, 64, 70, 71, 74, 75,
79, 84, 85, 87, 132, 133,
135, 148, 155, 173, 178,
183, 185, 186, 187, 200,
205, 228, 233, 259, 272,
278, 279, 281, 286, 292,
300, 306, 316, 317, 318,
319, 320, 348, 359, 411,
443, 444
monophthong 117, 118. See
also diphthong, vowel
mood 79, 80, 231, 232, 233,
251, 332, 399, 400. See
also declarative, imperative, indicative, interrogative, subjunctive
morph 125, 142. See also morpheme
morpheme 35, 53, 76, 77, 87,
122, 123, 124, 125, 127,
128, 129, 131, 132,
138, 139, 141, 142,
166, 187, 189, 385. See
also bound,derivational,
free, inflectional morpheme
morphology 37, 38, 52, 53, 121,
126, 129, 138, 148, 236,
243, 266, 385, 404
motivated 32, 33, 54
movement 6, 101, 163, 164,
249, 271, 277, 284, 295,
332, 333, 365, 383, 386,
387, 391, 394, 395, 396,
397, 408, 409, 422, 436,
438, 441, 443, 444
multilingual 52
Subject Index
multi-word preposition. See preposition
N
narrow phonetics. See broad phonetics, phonetics
nasal 90, 91, 92, 93, 96, 112,
114, 115, 118, 119. See
also consonant, manner of
articulation
nasality 91. See also consonant,
manner of articulation
natural gender 201. See also gender
necessary condition. See sufficient
condition
network 191, 254, 255, 260,
271. See also semantic
network
network model 254, 255, 260,
271. See also componential
model
neuter gender. See gender
new information 7, 387, 406.
See also given, old information, information
node 389, 393
nominal 151, 171, 198, 225,
227, 230, 232, 233, 288,
289, 365, 413
nominal clause 230, 232. See
also noun clause
nominal conjunction. See conjunction
nominalization 186, 187, 304
nominative case 199, 328, 414,
431. See also case
non-argument 407, 408
non-argument (A’) position.
See argument (A) position
non-contrastive 117, 119. See
also contrast, distinctive,
non-distinctive
non-count 149, 155, 156, 157,
162, 186, 190, 210, 240,
288. See also mass noun,
noun
non-distinctive 117, 119. See
also contrast, non-contrastive, distinctive
non-finite clause 443, 444. See
also clause
non-gradable antonym. See antonym, sense relation
non-restrictive relative clause 443, 444. See also relative
clause, restrictive relative
clause
noun 24, 63, 76, 78, 79, 83,
86, 94, 124, 126, 127,
129, 130, 133, 134, 138,
148, 149, 150, 151, 152,
153, 154, 155, 156, 157,
158, 159, 166, 169, 171,
172, 173, 175, 176, 177,
178, 185, 186, 187, 189,
190, 191, 194, 195, 196,
197, 199, 200, 201, 204,
206, 207, 208, 209, 210,
211, 216, 217, 218, 221,
223, 224, 226, 227, 228,
229, 231, 232, 233, 240,
255, 260, 263, 273, 274,
275, 276, 277, 278, 279,
282, 283, 288, 289, 290,
292, 293, 296, 299, 300,
301, 302, 304, 319, 320,
326, 333, 351, 358, 365,
386, 388, 408, 420, 421,
423, 425, 432, 443. See
457
Delahunty and Garvey
also abstract, collective,
common, concrete, count,
mass, non-count, proper
noun, major part of speech
noun complement clause.
See complement clause
noun inflection 150
noun modifier 173, 292, 300,
319
noun phrase 86, 153, 154, 155,
171, 186, 187, 194, 195,
196, 204, 207, 208, 209,
210, 211, 221, 226, 228,
231, 232, 233, 263, 273,
274, 275, 276, 282, 283,
288, 289, 292, 293, 302,
319, 320, 351, 365, 386,
425, 443. See also phrase
NP-movement clause. See raising
construction
nucleus of syllable. See syllable
416, 417, 418, 419, 421,
425, 429, 430, 431, 438,
443. See also complement,
direct, indirect object, object of preposition
object complement 155, 186,
287, 315, 353, 354, 355,
356, 359, 412, 419. See
also complement
object complement clause. See basic clause pattern
objective case 232, 430. See
also case
object of preposition. See object,
preposition stranding
old information 403, 406. See
also given, new information, information
onomatopoeia 54
onset of syllable. See syllable
ordinary language philosophy 21
O
P
object 7, 8, 36, 53, 54, 61, 62,
63, 64, 66, 70, 71, 79,
82, 87, 155, 163, 171,
183, 186, 187, 189, 190,
191, 199, 200, 204, 216,
217, 219, 221, 231, 232,
233, 242, 248, 256, 259,
263, 264, 272, 274, 275,
277, 282, 283, 284, 285,
287, 288, 295, 304, 306,
314, 315, 316, 317, 318,
319, 320, 327, 334, 349,
350, 353, 354, 355, 356,
357, 358, 359, 360, 361,
363, 365, 379, 384, 385,
387, 388, 389, 397, 404,
407, 408, 412, 413, 415,
palatal 94, 95, 96, 104, 115,
117, 129. See also consonant, place of articulation
palatal glide 96, 104, 115. See
also approximant glide
paradox 54
participle 76, 79, 127, 128, 139,
140, 165, 166, 167, 170,
175, 186, 187, 213, 244,
307, 308, 309, 311, 320,
337, 341, 403, 404, 408,
432, 433, 434, 441, 443,
444. See also past, present
participle
particle 134, 214, 215, 220,
221, 232, 383, 384, 385,
386, 387, 408, 443. See
458
Subject Index
also phrasal verb
particle movement 383, 386, 387
part of speech 16, 53, 77, 78,
83, 130, 133, 142, 147,
148, 150, 152, 153, 166,
171, 178, 183, 190, 221,
229, 240, 244, 274, 281,
286, 287, 290, 304, 323,
408. See also major, minor
part of speech
partonymy 250, 252, 271, 272.
See also sense relation
passive test 295, 320
passive transformation 404, 405
passive vocabulary 9, 21. See
also active vocabulary
passive voice 213, 403, 408. See
also active voice, voice
past participle 76, 79, 127, 128,
139, 140, 166, 167, 170,
175, 186, 213, 244, 307,
311, 337, 341, 403, 408,
432, 444. See also participle
path 208. See also semantic role
patient 211, 365. See also semantic role
perfect aspect 165, 166, 213,
307, 341, 345, 346. See
also aspect
performance 10, 23, 38, 43, 54.
See also linguistic performance
person 4, 5, 20, 41, 52, 76,
77, 79, 80, 124, 127,
135, 140, 142, 147, 148,
152, 155, 165, 178, 195,
196, 197, 198, 199, 201,
202, 203, 205, 207, 212,
232, 244, 245, 248, 250,
251, 254, 255, 257, 263,
270, 301, 307, 308, 309,
322, 327, 328, 330, 331,
338, 371, 378, 379, 394,
397, 398, 420, 425. See
also first, second, third
person
personal pronoun 198, 201,
203, 204, 208, 288. See
also pronoun
persuasive meaning. See conative
meaning, meaning
phatic meaning. See interpersonal,
social meaning, meaning
phoneme 82, 87, 108, 110, 111,
112, 113, 117, 119, 123,
125, 138, 264
phonetics 37, 54, 89, 107, 108,
111, 119. See also articulatory phonetics
phonological environment 128
phonology 37, 38, 52, 54, 89,
107, 108, 110, 111, 115,
119, 125, 385
phrasal verb 232, 383, 408. See
also particle
phrase 33, 54, 55, 59, 60, 62,
66, 72, 73, 74, 85, 86,
87, 88, 115, 119, 132,
133, 135, 136, 141, 153,
154, 155, 159, 164, 171,
172, 178, 179, 185, 186,
187, 190, 191, 194, 195,
196, 204, 207, 208, 209,
210, 211, 216, 217, 220,
221, 223, 224, 226, 228,
231, 232, 233, 251, 263,
272, 273, 274, 275, 276,
277, 278, 279, 281, 282,
283, 284, 285, 286, 287,
459
Delahunty and Garvey
288, 289, 292, 293, 296,
297, 298, 301, 302, 306,
307, 311, 312, 313, 314,
316, 317, 318, 319, 320,
323, 324, 326, 327, 333,
335, 337, 338, 345, 351,
353, 355, 356, 360, 361,
363, 365, 385, 386, 387,
389, 390, 392, 393, 394,
395, 398, 401, 402, 403,
404, 406, 407, 408, 409,
417, 418, 420, 421, 422,
425, 430, 431, 433, 438,
439, 440, 443, 444. See
also adjective, adverb,
noun, prepositional verb,
verbal phrase
phrase structure 281, 282, 323,
385, 389, 401, 407, 408
phrase structure rule 323, 389.
See also rule
phrase structure tree 282
place of articulation 91, 94, 119.
See also alveolar alveolar,
alveo-palatal, bilabial, glottal, interdental, labiodental,
palatal; See also velar
poetic meaning 30, 54. See
also meaning
politeness 370, 399, 407, 408
polysemy 253, 264, 271
positive degree 173. See
also comparative superlative
possessive adjective. See genitive
case, possessive pronoun,
pronoun
possessive case 158, 232. See
also case, genitive case
possessive pronoun 300. See
460
also genitive case, possessive adjective, pronoun
postmodifier in noun phrase.
See premodifier in noun
phrase
postposition 232
poverty of the stimulus 41, 54
power ii, 7, 20, 64, 65, 168,
199, 325, 331, 332, 365.
See also solidarity
pragmatics 21, 37, 38, 52, 54,
55, 87, 399, 407
predicate 53, 54, 55, 58, 59, 60,
61, 62, 69, 72, 87, 171,
172, 185, 186, 187, 240,
253, 259, 272, 274, 279,
306, 320, 322, 323, 324,
351, 363, 365, 383, 411,
436, 437, 443, 444. See
also elements of clause
predicate adjective 171, 185,
187, 365. See also object,
subject complement
predicate nominal 171, 365. See
also object, subject complement
prefix 123, 124, 139, 142. See
also affix, suffix
premodifier in noun phrase.
See postmodifier in noun
phrase
preposition 54, 74, 140, 155,
159, 170, 194, 199, 204,
216, 217, 218, 220, 221,
226, 232, 233, 240, 282,
283, 284, 292, 304, 320,
358, 359, 361, 388, 404,
407, 408, 413, 418, 419,
421, 422, 430, 431, 438.
See also minor part of
Subject Index
speech, multiword preposition, object of preposition
prepositional phrase 179, 191,
194, 216, 217, 220, 224,
226, 233, 282, 292,
298, 301, 320, 418. See
also phrase
preposition stranding 54, 217,
233. See also object of
preposition, preposition
prescriptive grammar 12, 13, 20,
21, 44, 52. See also grammar, rule
present participle 127, 165, 175,
187, 244, 309, 432, 434,
444. See also participle
prestige 81
principal parts of verbs 166, 167.
See also verb
principle of language 150
productivity 152, 187
progressive aspect 159, 160, 163,
165, 213, 308, 341, 342.
See also aspect
pronoun 63, 79, 83, 158, 172,
178, 194, 195, 196, 197,
198, 199, 200, 201, 202,
203, 204, 205, 207, 208,
212, 216, 228, 231, 232,
233, 240, 275, 276, 288,
290, 293, 294, 297, 298,
300, 302, 320, 326, 328,
329, 330, 359, 361, 387,
397, 398, 404, 414, 416,
418, 435. See also demonstrative, expletive,
generic, genitive, indefinite,
intensive, interrogative
pronoun; See also personal,
possessive, reflexive, rela-
tive pronoun
proper noun 149, 185, 187, 423.
See also noun
proposition 252, 321
pro-sub test 320
prototype 192, 265, 271
pro-verb phrase 320
pseudo-cleft 441, 443, 444. See
also cleft, it-cleft, wh-cleft
Q
quantifier 233, 288, 290, 296,
297
question 17, 23, 38, 42, 47, 59,
60, 62, 65, 84, 161, 189,
191, 202, 206, 214, 232,
233, 248, 252, 265, 294,
303, 313, 320, 333, 340,
346, 357, 358, 376, 392,
395, 409, 417, 432, 438,
443. See also interrogative,
mood, wh-question, yes/no
question
R
raising construction. See NPmovement, clause
rapid fading 31, 54
realization 142
realize 58, 63, 83, 222, 314
recipient 6, 62, 87, 200, 285,
358, 365. See also semantic role
recipient/benefactive clause.
See basic clause pattern
reciprocity 35, 53, 54
recursion 42, 54, 412, 443, 444.
See also infinity of language
reduced adverbial 433
461
Delahunty and Garvey
reduced relative 424, 434, 444
Reed-Kellogg diagram. See functional diagram
reference 17, 74, 89, 128, 136,
167, 175, 196, 197, 209,
210, 218, 221, 231, 233,
239, 243, 246, 270, 278,
291, 321, 338, 345, 346,
367, 369, 405, 435. See
also deictic, generic, specific reference
referential meaning 30, 54. See
also experiential, ideational
meaning, meaning
referring noun phrase 231, 233
reflexive pronoun 232, 233, 397,
398. See also intensive pronoun, pronoun
register 26, 55, 336, 434
relative clause 207, 224, 228,
230, 232, 233, 279, 394,
422, 423, 424, 425, 426,
434, 437, 439, 441, 443,
444. See also adjective
clause, non-restrictive,
restrictive relative clause
relative conjunction. See conjunction
relative pronoun 233, 435. See
also pronoun
relevance 11, 345
restrictive relative clauses 424.
See also non-restrictive,
relative clause
result clause. See clause
reversive antonym. See antonym
rhyme 29, 89, 106, 119, 132.
See also syllable
root of a word 77, 124, 126
rounded 100, 101, 104, 119. See
462
also vowel
rule 36, 37, 38, 40, 41, 46, 53,
55, 105, 109, 112, 113,
114, 115, 116, 117, 118,
119, 160, 210, 211, 214,
323, 345, 388, 389, 398,
400, 404, 408, 409, 437.
See also descriptive phrase,
structure, prescriptive
grammar, transformational
rule
run-in. See run-on
run-on. See run-in
S
schwa 103, 119, 130. See
also vowel
second person 198, 199, 202,
232, 398. See also person
segment deletion rule 114, 119
semantic component 272. See
also semantic feature
semantic feature 272. See also semantic component
semantic network. See network
model
semantic relations 135, 216, 232,
254, 255, 272, 283, 351,
355. See also sense relation
semantic role 216, 317, 336,
350, 352, 355, 357, 364,
365, 385, 386, 388, 389,
390, 392, 405, 407, 408,
431, 438. See also agent,
attribute, benefactive, effected, empty, experiencer,
expletive, factitive, force;
See also goal, instrument,
location, path, patient,
recipient, source, stimulus,
Subject Index
theme, thematic role; See
also time
semantics 17, 21, 37, 38, 52,
54, 55, 87, 164, 217,
250, 257, 357, 372, 387
semantics of prepositions 217
semantics of verbs 164
sememe 272. See also semantic component, semantic
feature
semiotics 34, 55
semivowel 119. See also approximant, glide
sense ii, 12, 20, 21, 39, 43,
46, 53, 70, 77, 109, 148,
159, 168, 172, 217, 220,
241, 245, 246, 253, 255,
262, 270, 271, 272, 321,
332, 378, 439
sense relations 253, 271, 272.
See also antonymy, cohyponymy, homonyn,
hypernym, hyponymy,
meronymy, metaphor; See
also metonymy, partonymy,
synonymy
sentence modifier 183, 187
simple sentence 168. See
also clause, sentence
simple trasitive clause. See basic
clause pattern
situational context 196, 197, 210.
See also context
social meaning. See interpersonal,
phatic meaning, meaning
sociolinguistics 48, 55
solidarity 199. See also power
sonorant 95, 119
sound symbolism 33, 55
source 64, 65, 84, 85, 86, 137,
187, 235, 237, 251, 365,
425. See also semantic role
specific reference 210. See
also generic reference,
reference
speech act 417
spoken language 30, 31, 196,
366, 367
standard english 11, 21
standardization 14, 23, 24
standardized tests 14
stem of word. See root of word
stimulus 41, 42, 54, 365. See
also semantic role
stop 91, 92, 94, 95, 96, 113,
118, 119, 161, 164. See
also consonant, manner of
articulation
structure word 186, 187, 233.
See also function word,
minor part of speech
style 9, 10, 21, 23, 25, 26, 44,
53, 55, 147, 247, 257,
261, 322, 411, 433
subject 3, 10, 13, 36, 46, 53,
54, 55, 58, 59, 60, 61,
63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 69,
70, 71, 72, 79, 80, 86,
87, 128, 155, 156, 163,
171, 172, 186, 187, 191,
199, 200, 214, 231, 232,
240, 253, 259, 272, 274,
276, 278, 287, 295, 304,
317, 318, 319, 322, 323,
324, 325, 326, 327, 328,
329, 330, 331, 336, 337,
338, 348, 349, 350, 351,
352, 353, 354, 355, 358,
363, 364, 365, 371, 378,
463
Delahunty and Garvey
379, 383, 384, 388, 392,
395, 397, 398, 402, 403,
404, 408, 409, 411, 412,
413, 414, 416, 417, 418,
419, 420, 421, 427, 428,
429, 430, 432, 433, 434,
435, 436, 437, 438, 439,
441, 443, 444. See also elements of clause, logical
subject, predicate
subject-auxiliary inversion 417
subject complement 171, 186,
187, 240, 318, 350, 351,
352, 353, 355, 365, 412,
419, 420. See also complement
subject-verb agreement 322, 328.
See also agreement
subjunctive 233. See also mood
subordinate 70, 71, 72, 135,
173, 225, 230, 231, 232,
233, 249, 262, 338, 366,
407, 411, 412, 413, 414,
415, 416, 417, 418, 419,
422, 425, 427, 428, 429,
436, 437, 438, 442, 443,
444. See also hyponymy,
sense relation
subordinate clause 70, 71, 72,
225, 230, 232, 233, 338,
366, 407, 411, 413, 415,
417, 427, 429, 436, 438,
444. See also adverbial,
noun, relative clause
subordinating adverbial conjunction 194, 228, 233. See
also conjunction
subordinating conjunction 225,
231, 233, 415. See
also conjunction, coordinat-
464
ing conjunction
sufficient condition. See necessary
condition
suffix 33, 124, 129, 138, 139,
140, 142, 148, 151, 152,
158, 174, 180, 186, 270,
368, 371. See also affix,
prefix
superlative 76, 127, 140, 142,
173, 174, 177, 180,
186, 187, 222, 249. See
also comparative
superordinate 249, 250, 260,
262, 263, 270, 272. See
also hyponymy, sense relations, subordinate
suppletion 127, 142
surface structure 383, 385, 386,
389, 390, 391, 393,
397, 402, 405, 409. See
also deep structure, transformation
syllabication 107, 236, 239, 272
syllabification 239, 272
syllable 102, 105, 106, 107,
117, 118, 119, 123, 136,
174, 180, 239, 368. See
also coda, foot, nucleus,
onset, rhyme
synchronic linguistics 48, 55. See
also diachronic linguistics
synonymy 247, 253, 260, 268,
272. See also sense relations
syntactic frame 263, 272
syntax 37, 38, 45, 48, 52, 55,
243, 267, 383, 387
Subject Index
T
teachability 15, 21. See also assessibility, learnability
tense 24, 45, 76, 79, 80, 102,
105, 118, 119, 124, 126,
127, 128, 130, 140, 142,
163, 165, 166, 167, 175,
191, 199, 214, 216, 244,
270, 307, 313, 315, 319,
321, 327, 328, 338, 339,
340, 341, 344, 345, 346,
347, 365, 366, 368, 369,
370, 371, 372, 373, 374,
375, 376, 377, 379, 380,
381, 394, 411, 412, 413,
428, 429, 434, 443. See
also aspect
tensed clause 379. See also finite
clause, that-clause
tense of verbs 76
tense vowel 105, 130. See
also vowel
text 3, 6, 16, 17, 19, 21, 25,
26, 27, 29, 31, 52, 55,
57, 84, 88, 126, 148,
157, 162, 170, 193, 197,
202, 215, 270, 305, 339,
340, 344, 366, 369, 371,
376, 380, 381, 405, 406,
428, 440, 441
textual meaning 30, 55. See
also meaning
that-clause 303. See also clause,
finite, tensed clause
thematic role 438. See also semantic role
theme 365. See also semantic role
theoretical linguistics 55
theory 20, 40, 42, 51, 55, 254,
271, 353, 383, 408
third person 76, 80, 196, 197,
198, 199, 202, 232, 244,
270, 307, 327, 328, 338,
371. See also person
time 5, 6, 7, 13, 14, 17, 20, 23,
24, 26, 27, 34, 40, 43,
44, 55, 60, 64, 74, 79,
99, 107, 153, 162, 166,
177, 208, 212, 215, 216,
228, 229, 267, 274, 278,
312, 321, 325, 326, 333,
336, 338, 339, 340, 341,
342, 343, 344, 345, 346,
365, 366, 367, 368, 369,
370, 371, 372, 373, 375,
377, 378, 379, 380, 381,
426, 428, 435, 441. See
also semantic role, tense
topic 4, 5, 16, 34, 49, 86, 130,
201, 210, 235, 256, 262,
281, 324, 325, 364, 365,
385, 388, 390, 391, 392,
403. See also comment
topicalization 320, 388, 389,
390, 391, 392, 408, 409
topicalization test 320
tranformational rule. See transformation
transformation 385, 386, 390,
391, 392, 393, 394, 397,
398, 400, 402, 404, 405,
408, 409. See also deep,
surface structure
transitive clause. See basic clause
pattern
transitive verb 187, 272, 275,
304, 314, 349, 407. See
also intransitive verb, verb
tree diagram 85, 281, 283, 287,
465
Delahunty and Garvey
293, 347, 349
truncated passive 233. See also byphrase, passive voice
U
ungrammaticality 191, 226, 296,
317, 339, 361, 380. See
also grammaticality
universal 105, 125, 256, 257,
326, 373, 384
unmarked 248, 271, 272. See
also marked
unrounded vowel 119. See
also vowel
usage 16, 17, 23, 24, 25, 44, 45,
48, 53, 55, 56, 121, 128,
177, 183, 190, 236, 241,
242, 247, 272, 373, 374,
375
usage note 272
V
velar 95, 96, 104, 119. See
also consonant, place of
articulation
verb 25, 35, 36, 60, 61, 62, 64,
65, 66, 67, 69, 74, 79, 83,
84, 86, 87, 94, 114, 124,
127, 128, 129, 130, 134,
139, 140, 141, 152, 156,
159, 160, 161, 162, 163,
164, 165, 166, 167, 168,
169, 170, 171, 175, 178,
179, 182, 183, 185, 186,
187, 189, 190, 191, 199,
204, 213, 214, 217, 220,
221, 223, 224, 226, 229,
231, 232, 233, 240, 243,
244, 255, 259, 260, 272,
273, 274, 275, 277, 278,
466
283, 304, 306, 307, 309,
310, 311, 312, 313, 314,
315, 317, 318, 319, 320,
322, 326, 327, 328, 329,
330, 331, 332, 334, 336,
337, 338, 339, 341, 343,
345, 347, 349, 350, 351,
352, 355, 356, 357, 358,
359, 360, 361, 363, 364,
365, 369, 370, 371, 372,
377, 378, 379, 383, 384,
385, 387, 388, 389, 392,
393, 394, 402, 403, 404,
407, 408, 411, 412, 414,
417, 428, 429, 431, 432,
434, 438, 443, 444. See
also auxiliary, bitransitive, intransitive, linking, main verb,
major part of speech, transitive
verb
verbal phrase 319, 320. See
also gerund, infinitive, participle
verb phrase 86, 164, 171, 185,
186, 224, 226, 231, 232,
233, 272, 273, 275, 306,
307, 311, 312, 313, 317,
319, 320, 337, 363, 443.
See also phrase
voice 31, 38, 65, 79, 80, 213,
216, 230, 231, 232, 302,
403, 408. See active, passive
voice
voiced sound. See consonant, voiceless sound
voiceless sound 113, 368. See
also consonant, voiced sound
vowel 33, 55, 81, 92, 96, 98,
99, 100, 101, 103, 104,
105, 106, 109, 112, 113,
Subject Index
114, 117, 118, 119, 127,
129, 130, 208, 368. See
also back, central, diphthong,
front, high, lax, low, mid,
monophthong, rounded,
tense, unrounded vo
VP 171, 274, 294, 306, 307, 313,
314, 316, 317, 320, 323,
347, 348, 349, 351, 352,
354, 356, 357, 359, 360,
362, 363, 383, 384, 385,
390, 392, 393, 394, 395,
396, 397, 398, 400, 401,
402, 404, 405, 428, 439.
See also verb phrase
W
wh-cleft 406, 444. See also cleft,
it-cleft, pseudo-cleft
whiz-deletion 424, 425, 432, 444
whiz-test 320
wh-movement 394, 395, 396, 408,
409
wh-question 233, 294, 320, 409.
See also information, question, mood, yes/no
wh-question test 320
wh-word 206, 208, 227, 232, 233,
294, 302, 344, 420, 421,
422, 423, 424, 439, 440
word 4, 12, 15, 16, 20, 29, 30,
32, 33, 34, 36, 39, 45, 52,
53, 54, 56, 60, 67, 73, 74,
75, 76, 77, 78, 81, 83, 84,
86, 87, 88, 92, 93, 96, 97,
104, 106, 107, 109, 110,
111, 112, 114, 115, 118,
119, 121, 122, 123, 124,
126, 128, 129, 130, 131,
132, 133, 135, 136, 137,
138, 139, 141, 142, 147,
148, 150, 151, 152, 153,
154, 155, 158, 161, 165,
168, 169, 170, 171, 172,
173, 174, 175, 176, 177,
178, 180, 183, 186, 187,
188, 189, 190, 193, 194,
195, 198, 199, 201, 202,
203, 206, 208, 216, 218,
220, 221, 222, 223, 227,
228, 231, 232, 233, 235,
236, 238, 239, 240, 241,
242, 243, 244, 245, 246,
247, 248, 249, 251, 252,
253, 254, 255, 256, 257,
258, 259, 260, 261, 262,
263, 264, 265, 266, 267,
268, 269, 270, 271, 272,
273, 274, 275, 276, 277,
278, 282, 287, 288, 289,
290, 292, 293, 294, 296,
297, 302, 304, 306, 307,
317, 319, 320, 327, 344,
350, 368, 371, 372, 386,
420, 421, 422, 423, 424,
439, 440
word form 87, 88
word history 241, 272. See also etymology
Y
yes/no question 392. See also information, question, interrogative
sentence, wh-question
Z
zero derivation 130, 151, 152, 159,
168, 187. See also conversion, functional shift
467
The English Language
Written in a clear style, it guides its readers on topics including basic assumptions about
language and discourse, pronunciation, word-formation strategies, parts of speech, clause
elements and patterns, how clauses may be combined into sentences, and how clauses
and sentences are modified to suit speakers’ and writers’ discourse purposes.
The English Language avoids presenting the language as set of arbitrary facts by grounding its conclusions in the analytic methods that have characterized the best grammatical
and linguistic practices for hundreds of years. Although its perspectives derive from
modern-traditional and generative grammar, its goal is to provide its readers with a broad
spectrum of basic knowledge about English. Its stance is rigorously descriptive, but the
object of its description is the standard variety of the language, thus making it an invaluable resource compatible with a wide range of purposes, including educated engagement
with the language issues that periodically convulse the media and educational institutions.
Each chapter contains a glossary of terms, a list of readings, and numerous exercises
(many using authentic texts).
Gerald P. Delahunty is Associate Professor of Linguistics and English and Assistant
Chair of the Colorado State University Department of English, where he teaches courses
on all aspects of linguistics and occasional courses on Irish literature. He has published
on syntactic theory, English syntax, sociolinguistics, and Irish archaeology.
James J. Garvey taught linguistics and literature courses in the English Department at
Colorado State University. He died tragically in 2006.
Perspectives on Writing
Series Editor, Mike Palmquist
The English Language
Grounded in linguistic research and argumentation, The English Language: From Sound
to Sense offers readers who have little or no analytic understanding of English a thorough
treatment of the various components of the language. Its goal is to help readers become
independent language analysts capable of critically evaluating claims about the language
and the people who use it.
Delahunty
Garvey
The
English
Language
From Sound
to Sense
The WAC Clearinghouse
http://wac.colostate.edu/
816 Robinson Street
West Lafayette, IN 47906
w w w.parlorpress.com
S A N: 2 5 4 - 8 8 7 9
ISBN 978-1-60235-180-6
WAC
Clearinghouse
Parlor
Press
Gerald P. Delahunty
James J. Garvey