* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Evolution of colour vision
Koinophilia wikipedia , lookup
Artificial gene synthesis wikipedia , lookup
Gene expression programming wikipedia , lookup
Genome (book) wikipedia , lookup
Designer baby wikipedia , lookup
Point mutation wikipedia , lookup
Gene therapy of the human retina wikipedia , lookup
Microevolution wikipedia , lookup
Evolution of colour vision After J Neitz, J Arroll, M Neitz Optics & Photonics News, pp. 26-33, Jan 2001. Neural mechanisms of seeing colour Light sensitive receptors neural components for processing extracting relative responses from neighbouring receptors wavelength sensitive encoding output to labelled lines Black and white perception Small cluster of receptors illuminated by a small spot of light information gathered from illuminated receptors from their immediate neighbours Brain nerve fibres receive output from cluster of receptors from the “white” labelled lines cluster of receptors from the “black” labelled lines one of the two outputs is inverted compared to the other Hue perception Encoding in two components, each of them responsible for a pair of sensations, sensations in each pair are opposed to one another, blue-yellow hue system red-green hue system each draws from a common set of photoreceptors: L, M, S; outputs via different neural components: different labelled lines. Cone photoreceptors log cone action sensitivity 1 0 -1 -2 L-cone -3 M-cone -4 S-cone -5 -6 -7 -8 350 450 550 wavelength, nm 650 750 Hue systems blue-yellow(B-Y): output from the S cones, comparing it to L + M cone responses red-green(R-G): output from the L cones, comparing it to M cone responses only blue-yellow system draws from S cones, S cones differ from M and L in physiology and retinal distribution B-Y more vulnerable: toxic exposure, eye diseases, trauma Different evolutionary history Blue-Yellow colour vision system Trichromatic colour vision in mammals: only in man and some subset of primates Some mammals are monochromats Most mammals are dichromats, e.g. dog, system is homologous to the “blue-yellow” system Cone photopigment sensitivity of dogs Dogs have two types of conepigments most similar to human S and L pigments. The bar at the bottom approximates how a dog can distinguish among colours Tomatoes: which one is ripe, seen by a dog Tomatoes: which one is ripe, seen by a trichromat Photopigments and their genes Composition of the photopigments chromophore: 11-cis-retinal protein component, covalently bound: opsin In terrestrial animals the chromophore is the same, the opsin varies the opsin tunes the absorption maximum the opsins belong to a comon family Photopigments and their genes Molecular genetic methods can deduce the amino acid sequencees of photopigment opsins The two classes of dichromatic pigments have strikingly different amino acid sequences (50 %): Indication for early differentiation of the S and L photopigments in evolutionary terms Photopigments and their genes evolution of colour vision S and L pigments amino acid sequences different Seven amino acid changes produce the 30 nm difference between the M and L pigments Extrapolation and speculation: 6 % difference in amino acid sequence required for the 100 nm shift between S and L cones Speculation on evolution Comparison: differences in rod pigments of species as clock, constant rate genetic drift S and L/M cone differentiation about 1000 million years ago (MYA) Oldest fossils: 6000MYA Speculation on evolution Dichromacy almost as old as vision Distinction among colours, humans see 200 grey levels Dichromacy: 50 discernible chromatic steps, provides 10.000 steps Wavelength sensing is as fundamental to vision as is light detection Red - Green colour vision system L and M photopigments individually polymorchic, on average difference: 15 amino acids Genetic clock estimate: L and M difference 50 MYA (Old and New World primates split about 60 MYA) Three neuronal line pairs: (Black-White, Y-B, R-G) 100 steps in R-G direction: 106 distinguishable colours Beyond trichromacy Non-mammal diurnal vertebrates (birds, fish, etc.) have four photopigments: also UV Mammals were nocturnal when appeared at the time of the dominance of dinosaurs Nocturnal ancestors of modern primates were reduced to dichromacy Primates invented trichromacy separately Neural circuits for redgreen colour vision Diurnal primates: acute spatial vision: small receptive fields (midgets), contacting single cones Opponent signals from surrounding neighbours: new receptor (L or M) compares also colour, no new wiring needed Mammalian visual cortex molded by experience Directions of colour vision research L and M photopigment genes might misalign during meiosis and recombine: mixed sequences might occur Variants common in L gene, females have two X chromosomes, the two might have different L pigments X-chromosome inactivation can produce two L cones in females: four spectrally different receptors. Directions of colour vision research The two L cones are very similar: few steps of colour discrimination Females found who showed increased colour discrimination ability L/M cone ration can change from 1:1 to 4:1, with no measurable colour vision difference: plasticity of nervous system? Chromatically altered visual environment has long term influence on colour vision Further speculation If neural circuits for colour vision are sufficiently plastic gene therapy could replace missing photopigments could add a fourth cone type