Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Volume 2, Issue 1, 2008 Psychologist Bias in Implicit Responding to Religiously Divergent Nonpatient Targets and Explicit Responding to Religiously Divergent Patients Jennifer Ruff, PhD, Clinical Psychology, Fielding Graduate University, [email protected] Abstract This study examines how psychologists responded to a mainstream group believed to be most religiously diverse from them, Evangelical Christians (ECs). Clinicians were presented with two vignettes which described patients with comparable symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, who differed on either religiosity or career and volunteer activity conditions. They rated each on measures of empathy and prognosis. Clinicians completed a scale that measures attitudes about Christian beliefs that range from orthodox to liberal positions. Clinicians’ automatic responding to EC targets was also compared to automatic responding to Secular or No Religion targets on a timed implicit measure, which reduces the opportunity to censor bias. Liberality of religious attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs was associated with less cognitive and affective empathy and a poorer prognosis for the EC patient. On the implicit measure, religiously liberal clinicians’ attitudes toward Christian beliefs was associated with negative responding to EC targets compared to Secular or No Religion targets. Last, given the opportunity to do so, clinicians’ motivation to control prejudice reactions did not moderate the effects of automatic negative responding to EC’s on self-reported expressions of empathy or prognosis in relation to the EC vignette patient. The results of this study have implications for Evangelical Christian patients who may experience biased clinical judgment as a result of their religious background. Also, results should be of interest to clinicians who seek to provide sensitive and competent treatment to patients who belong to religious groups that diverge from their own, and for whom it is important to honor ethics codes which guide clinicians to respect group differences in psychotherapy. Lastly, it is suggested that clinical multicultural training programs should include training for clinical work with patients whose religious beliefs and values are different from those of the clinician. A rationale for the study with an extensive literature review is presented in Part A, followed by Part B which includes the current research.and a summary literature review. KEY WORDS: bias, stereotype, religion, values, clinician variables, patient variables, prejudice, empathy, prognosis, pathology clinical judgment, implicit processes, explicit processes, Evangelical Christian, Implicit Association Test, conservatism, liberalism, multicultural, cultural sensitivity, diversity CLINICIAN RELIGIOSITY AND RESPONSE TO DIVERGENT PATIENT RELIGIOSITY: AN INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECTS OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT STEREOTYPING ON EMPATHY AND PROGNOSIS IN INITIAL RESPONDING TO PATIENTS WHO ARE RELIGIOUSLY DIVERSE FROM PSYCHOLOGISTS A dissertation submitted by JENNIFER L. RUFF to FIELDING GRADUATE UNIVERSITY in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY IN PSYCHOLOGY With an Emphasis in Clinical Psychology ~/i, f!iltlU) --. Charles H. Elflo ,Ph.D Chair Kjell Erik Rudestam, Ph.D., Associate Dean Debra Bendell Estroff, Ph.D., Faculty Reader James Guinee, Ph.D., Extemal Examiner Copyright by Jennifer L. Ruff 2008 TABLE OF CONTENTS PARTS A & B PART A: Comprehensive Review of the Literature Page Introduction ……………………………………………………………………... 1 Diversity and worldview match………………………………………………….. 2 Religiosity: Psychologists and the general U.S. population..……….……... 7 Multicultural training and application of ethical responsibilities………….. 11 The potential for compromised clinical efficacy with religious persons…………15 Stereotyping and prejudice………………………………………………… 15 Discordant values and efficacy of practice………………………………… 20 Other barriers to effective treatment……………………………………….. 25 Sociopolitical trends, religiosity, and affect……………………………….. 27 Empathy……………………………………………………………………. 30 Religion and Mental Health………………………………………………... 34 The current research………………………………………………………………. 39 Literature on bias with religious patients……………………………………39 Social desirability…………………………………………………………... 48 Implicit versus explicit cognitive processes in impression formation……… 50 Summary…………………………………………………………………………… 60 References – Part A …………………………………………………………………62 PART B …………………………………………………………………………….83 Introduction………………………………………………………………………… 84 Religiosity as a diversity variable in clinical psychology………………………….. 85 The “religiosity gap”……………………………………………………….. 86 Religion and multicultural competence……………………………………………. 87 Neglect of religious beliefs and values as a diversity variable……………...87 The potential for stereotyping and prejudice………………………………. 89 Outcomes and value convergence…………………………………………..93 Religion and mental health………………………………………………… 95 Sociopolitical influence……………………………………………………. 98 The impact of religious neglect or bias on treatment……………………………… 99 Selection of treatment goals……………………………………………….. 100 Empathy……………………………………………………………………. 101 Literature on clinical judgment of religious patients………………………………..101 Social desirability……………………………………………………………111 Automatic versus controlled cognitive processes………………………….. 113 Statement of the problem……………………………………………………………121 Variables…………………………………………………………………… 122 Hypotheses…………………………………………………………………. 125 Methods…………………………………………………………………………….. 126 Participants…………………………………………………………………. 127 Measures…………………………………………………………………… 131 Batson’s empathy adjectives………………………………………. 131 Interpersonal Reactivity Index’s Perspective Taking Scale……….. 132 Clinical Judgment Scale…………………………………………… 133 Religious Attitude Scale…………………………………………… 134 Implicit Association Test…………………………………………... 136 Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Scale………………….138 Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale-Short Form……………139 Religious Conservatism Scale………………………………………140 Procedure……………………………………………………………………....140 Results……………………………………………………………………………… 142 Descriptive statistics……………………………………………………………142 Preliminary analyses……………………………………………………………148 Hypotheses analyses……………………………………………………………153 Summary of results……………………………………………………………..163 Discussion………………………………………………………………………….. 164 References – Part B..………………………………………………………………..176 Appendix A: Materials provided to participants…………………………………... 195 Appendix B: Distribution of variable scores………………………………………..214 Appendix C: Examination of Regression Assumptions…………………………… 222 TABLES Table 1: Sample Demographic and Background Characteristics ……..………........143 Table 2: Descriptive Statistics for Composite Measures……………………………148 Table 3: Normality Statistics for Variables Used in Hypothesis Tests……………...150 Table 4: Correlations among Variables Used in Hypothesis Tests………………….152 Table 5: Regression of Differences in Affective Empathy on Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 1.........................…….154 Table 6: Regression of Differences in Cognitive Empathy on Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 1……………………..155 Table 7: Regression of Differences in Prognosis on Liberal Attitudes in relation To Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 2………………………………………....156 Table 8: Regression of Differences in INA associated with Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 3…..……………..….157 Table 9: Regression of Affective Empathy Differences with NMR-EC with Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions as a Moderator on Liberality of Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 4…………………….159 Table 10: Regression of Cognitive Empathy Differences with NMR-EC with Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions as a Moderator on Liberality of Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 4……………………161 Table 11: Regression of Prognosis Differences with NMR-EC with Motivation to Control Prejudiced Responding as a Moderator on Liberality of Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 5……………………..………..163 Part A Introduction This paper discusses the potential for psychologist bias against religious patients, particularly religiously conservative Christian patients, as a diversity issue in need of further investigation. The dissimilarity between the religiosity of the general American population and that of psychologists is explored through an examination of studies on the religiosity gap. Trends toward psychologists’ secular and comparatively liberal worldviews and values, and the manifestation of those trends, are examined. The status of inadequate clinician multicultural training with religious groups and the consequences of the current deficit are also discussed. The possibility of the culturally insensitive or biased treatment of the group will also be explored. Factors that may contribute to differential treatment may include differences in worldviews and values between clinicians and religiously oriented patients, inadequate training in working with them as a diversity group, affective responding or stereotypic or prejudiced assumptions about religious patients, and other factors found in the theoretical and empirical literature on stereotyping and prejudice between social groups in general. The effects of bias or insensitivity on the initial impressions of patients, clinical empathy, prognosis, pathology, disrespect of the patients’ religious beliefs or values, patient concerns about entering “secular” psychotherapy, and value convergence as a clinician-perceived variable in successful treatment outcomes, are explored. An analysis of bias studies with religious patients follows, with further examination of some problematic methodology that may contribute to 2 mixed findings. Lastly, variables that may resolve some previous methodological shortcomings in the literature and suggest directions for future research are explored. Diversity and Worldview Match Weltanschauung is defined as “the overall perspective from which one sees and interprets the world,” and “a collection of beliefs about life and the universe held by an individual or a group” (American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, 2000). Culture is defined as the customary beliefs, social forms, and material traits of a racial, religious, or social group (2000). Each person’s Weltanschauung, or worldview, is often extensively informed by the culture in which he or she is immersed. The individual’s worldview assists him or her in making sense of the world, his or her place within it, and the nature of interpersonal exchanges with others. Cultural groups may further inform the individual about worldview perspectives such as which values are important and which are not, and may guide one’s initiation of, and choice of responses to, his or her experiences (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998). Cultural influence often has such an impact on one’s worldview that it must be considered by clinicians attempting to have a comprehensive understanding of their patients. The potential for clinician bias against patients of various cultural domains is evident in the APA Code of Ethics (APA, 2002), 3 Standards 2.01 and 3.01. Standard 3.01 bans discrimination against multicultural groups, and 2.01 further explains multicultural competence (MCC) as an understanding of factors associated with age, gender, gender identity, race, ethnicity, culture, national origin, religion, sexual orientation, disability, language, or socioeconomic status is essential for effective implementation of their services or research, psychologists have or obtain the training, experience, consultation, or supervision necessary to ensure the competence of their services, or they make appropriate referrals (APA, 2002, pp. 1063-1064) Not only does the APA mandate that psychologists become culturally competent, but diversity has become so celebrated that it is considered a core value, and even a fourth force in psychology by some (Cheatham, Ivey, Ivey, & Simek-Morgan, 1980). While ethnicity and race have generated a plethora of research and become the focus of a significant amount of multicultural education and training, by comparison religiosity has been an often overlooked expression of diversity in the literature and in diversity training programs (Yarhouse & Fisher, 2002). Nevertheless, religiosity must be considered from a sensitive and informed multicultural perspective. That there is a schism between the scientific worldview and one which encompasses religious elements has been evident in psychology from the early influence of Sigmund Freud. Freud described worldview as “an intellectual construction which solves all the problems of our existence uniformly on the basis of one overriding hypothesis” (Freud, 1933/1962, p. 158). He endorsed a 4 scientific worldview, which he compared at length to a spiritual worldview, and claimed that it precluded knowledge of the universe “other than the intellectual working over of carefully scrutinized observations” (Freud, 1933/1962, p.159). Indeed, psychology historians assert that naturalism, or the “doctrine that scientific procedures and laws are applicable to all phenomena” which also “assumes that all events in the world have a history that is understandable in terms of identifiable forces” (Viney & King, 1998, p. 182), has defined the current philosophy of scientific thinking since Freud’s time. Defined as such, Albert Ellis, a notable theorist credited with the development of Rational Emotive Behavioral Therapy, goes on to state that “In regard to scientific thinking, it practically goes without saying that this kind of cerebration is quite antithetical to religiosity” (Ellis, 1980, p.9) Since Freud’s time, others assert antireligious clinical perspectives and theory. More recent antireligious views include comments made by Wendell Watters, a respected professor of psychiatry and physician at McMaster University in Ontario, Canada. In reference to Christian doctrine and teachings he stated that they are “incompatible with the development and maintenance of sound health, and not only ‘mental’ health,” and that “Simply put, Christian indoctrination is a form of mental and emotional abuse” (Watters, 1992, p.10). In reference to the majority of membership in the American Psychological Association (APA), Emeritus professor of psychology at Yale University and author of over 40 books, Seymour Sarason in his Centennial Address to the APA stated that there are more than a few psychologists who regard ingredients of a 5 religious worldview as a “reflection of irrationality, of superstition, of an immaturity, of a neurosis,” and that “indeed if we learn someone is devoutly religious, or even tends in that direction, we look upon that person with puzzlement, often concluding that psychologist obviously had or has personal problems” (Sarason, 1993). In the Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R), 12 references to religion in the Glossary of Technical Terms were used to demonstrate psychopathology (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). While it is noted that the latest revision of the DSM, the DSM-IV TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), now includes more culturally sensitive language, that antireligious perspectives may have influenced the clinical judgment of psychologists and psychiatrists alike, should not be easily dismissed. Indeed, there is encouraging evidence that some psychologists’ worldviews have evolved in conjunction with the demands for multiculturally appropriate perspectives as can be seen in the morphing views of Albert Ellis. In one earlier treatise on religiousness and psychotherapy Ellis states that, “If one of the requisites for emotional health is acceptance of uncertainty, then religion is obviously the unhealthiest state imaginable” (Ellis, 1980, p. 8), which also implies by virtue of the religious person’s extreme pathology that he or she is likely the hardest to treat. Indeed, he also stated that “the best the devout religionists can do, if they want to change any of the rules that stem from their doctrines, is to change the religion itself” (Ellis, 1980, p. 31). However, Ellis later recants some of his earlier assertions and reports that his Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy is 6 compatible with some religious views and can be effectively used with patients who have devout beliefs about God without changing their religion (Ellis, 2000). Nevertheless, it is difficult to imagine that such evolution in thinking about religiosity and religious persons, as encouraging as it may be, necessarily represents a sudden and ubiquitous absence of antireligious views in psychology. Certainly, this type of antireligious thinking was common enough in the not so distant past that it was acceptable for publication in peer-reviewed journals, which one might assume have some commitment to publish culturally appropriate materials. Distance between scientific thought and religion or spirituality has likely been deliberate on the part of psychologists who sought credibility for the profession as an empirical and nonspeculative science. Strong scientific ideology may be a cause of clinician failure to assess religion and religious functioning as a part of patient experience to be regarded with seriousness and sensitivity. This separation may be problematic for several reasons according to contemporary psychologists interested in the interrelated themes between scientific and religious thought. Three important themes have emerged in the literature (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998). The first is that the lines between religion and science have begun to blur as the traditional view of science as strictly rational and empirical has been challenged. Second, therapists use their values to guide their choice of treatment goals and interventions, whether implicitly or explicitly. Last is the theme that “religion and psychotherapy, at a deep level of analysis, are functionally and even structurally equivalent” (1998, p. 2). Nevertheless, the 7 nature of the “secular field” of psychology (Bergin & Jensen, 1990) as dissimilar from that of the general population has been the focus of a fair amount of attention in the literature. Religiosity: Psychologists and the General U.S. Population Definitions of the terms “spiritual” and “religious” vary in the literature, although most authors are in agreement on core themes. For the purposes of the current discussion, and in agreement with those themes, religious persons will refer to those with affiliations with an organized religion. Religious beliefs are considered “propositional statements (in agreement with some organized religion) that a person holds to be true concerning religion or religious spirituality” (Worthington, 1996, p.2). Values are superordinate statements about what a person considers to be important in life. Worthington (1996) defines spiritual as believing in or valuing some higher power other than what is seen to exist in the material world. One may be spiritual and religious, religious but not spiritual, spiritual but not religious, or neither religious nor spiritual. It is also understood that the degree of religiosity and spirituality in endorsement of beliefs, adherence to associated values, and participation in religious or spiritual behaviors, varies between persons. The U.S. population may be considered highly religious, with an estimated 94% of the population endorsing belief in “God or some universal spirit” (Gallup, 1996), 92% being affiliated with a religion, 84% reporting religion as either very or 8 fairly important in their lives, and only 3% denying any beliefs in God. The majority of the population endorses Judeo-Christian affiliations at 76%, with 72% of those endorsing Protestant or Catholic categories (Gallup, 2006), and other estimates as high as 84% when respondents were offered more choices of religion including Orthodox Greek and Russian Christian affiliations (NewPort, 2004). The religious affiliation endorsed by the largest group of persons is the Protestant denomination at 49%. Within the Protestant subgroup, 44% endorsed the category described as either “born again” or “Evangelical.” Further, when Evangelical activities and beliefs were measured and used to define Evangelicalism, including believing the Bible is the actual word of God, being born again or having a born again experience, and encouraging others to believe in or accept Jesus Christ, approximately 53% of Protestants, or 1 in 5 Americans (22%), can be considered Evangelical Christians (Gallup, 2005). Other research estimates that 90% of Americans pray, 71% belong to a church or synagogue, and 42% attend weekly religious services (Hill et al., 2000). Psychologists have consistently been associated with lower rates of religious affiliation compared to the U.S. public, and even to other clinicians and academicians. One study (Ragan, Malony, & Beit-Hallahmi, 1980) found that psychologists were much less orthodox in both religion and spirituality than had been previously seen in the general academic population, with 34% of psychologists denying the existence of God, compared to 23% of other academicians, and 3% of the general U.S. population (Gallup, 2006). In another study (Bergin & Jensen, 1990), religious preferences were rated in an 9 interdisciplinary group of psychotherapists that included marriage and family therapists, clinical social workers, psychiatrists, and clinical psychologists. Differences were seen in atheist, agnostic, or no preference categories with a cumulative sum of 30% of clinical psychologists, 24% of psychiatrists, 13% of marriage and family therapists, 9% of clinical social workers, and 9% of the U.S. public. Clinical psychologists expressed slightly more Judeo-Christian preferences than psychiatrists at 67% and 65% respectively, but less than clinical social workers at 82%, and marriage and family therapists at 76%. However, more recently, Bilgrave and Deluty (1998) found that only 42% of counseling and clinical psychologists endorsed Judeo-Christian affiliations. While Ragan et al.’s (1980) findings that 34% of clinical psychologists denied the existence of God led authors to conclude that the atheistic stereotype of psychologists is supported, they also admit that one cannot predict that any one psychologist will be an atheist. Others point to substantial endorsement of the religious and spiritual beliefs of psychologists despite the lower frequency with which they occur relative to other groups. For instance, in Ragan’s study, 43% of psychologists endorsed belief in some transcendent deity. In another study (Bergin & Jensen, 1990), 33% of clinical psychologists positively endorsed the item that “my whole approach to life is based on my religion,” and 65% endorsed the statement, “I try hard to live my life according to my religious beliefs.” Others challenge the atheistic stereotype (Shafranske & Gorsuch, 1984; Shafranske & Malony, 1990; Smith & Orlinsky, 2004) and in particular when the criteria for spirituality are broadened to include personally meaningful experience 10 and focus on discipline, purpose, belonging, wholeness, and connectedness, therapists may have substantial spiritual interests. It has also been hypothesized that some spiritual and religious interests may be relatively unexpressed due to the secular nature of psychologists’ education and practice (Bergin & Jensen, 1990). Further, psychologists’ professional organizations concerned with religion and psychology are often comprised of a significant portion of religious psychologists interested in the interrelation of the two domains. APA’s Division 36, the division of the psychology of religion, “recognizes the significance of religion both in the lives of people and the discipline of psychology” (APA, 2007), and works toward “the re-establishment of the scientific psychology of religion”. The Christian Association for Psychological Studies (CAPS) is an organization largely made up of Evangelical Christian psychologists interested in the integration of psychology and Evangelical Christianity. Specifically, integration concerns how psychological theory and research is relevant to Christianity and the generation of study and dialogue about concepts often associated with their Evangelical faith such as forgiveness, and gratitude (see Ellens & Sanders, 2006; Lewis-Hall, Gorsuch, Malony, Narramore, & Stewart Van Leeuwam, 2006; Yangarber-Hicks et al., 2006). Nevertheless, that there are differences between the psychological community and the general population on religion and spirituality is clear. As the current Weltanschauung of the Western world includes both religious and scientific-humanistic beliefs and values (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998), we may 11 assume that like the larger population, patients seeking psychological services will also have worldviews which possess these elements. Considering the potential differences in worldview between psychologists and patients, and in particular associated values which guide what one considers important and what one does not, it is worth examining how well prepared psychologists are to work with persons who have religiously influenced worldviews which may be diverse from their own. Multicultural Training and Application of Ethical Responsibilities Even though approximately 9 out of 10 persons in the US (Gallup, 2006), and up to 65% of psychologists (Bergin & Jensen, 1990), report that religious beliefs are important in their lives, only 29% of clinical psychologists (1990), and 50% of rehabilitation psychologists (Shafranske, 2000), believe that attention to those cultural beliefs is important in their work with patients. Despite demands for the ethical and sensitive treatment of diverse groups, and the focus on training to effectively deliver multiculturally competent services, there is a noticeable gap in the ethical application of those principles with religious patients (Meyer, 1988; Yarhouse & VanOrman, 1999; Zeiger & Lewis, 1998). It may be that as a result of divergent religious beliefs and values, clinicians who do not consider religion important in their own lives may simply not consider it important in the lives of their patients and subsequently to psychotherapy. It may also be that clinicians deliberately avoid religiously 12 thematic material for a number of reasons including fear of approaching what may be considered a taboo topic. Fears may arise from awareness of a heavy humanistic influence in the field and a history of disparagement of religion from the early days of Freud to more recent theorists such as Albert Ellis. Alternatively, clinicians may believe that religious issues are generally unrelated to the clinical presentation or treatment of their patients. Another possibility is that they may have personal bias against such belief systems or those who hold the beliefs valuable. Additionally, therapists may feel inadequate in dealing with religious material in a clinical setting. Any of these factors may result in either deliberate avoidance of the topic, or the management of religiously thematic material in ways which may not respect the patient’s beliefs or value systems. Currently, levels of training in religious diversity in clinical practice are not commensurate with the general religiosity of the U.S. public (Brawer, Handal, Fabricatore, Roberts, & Wajda-Johnston, 2002; Yarhouse & Fisher, 2002). In response to whether religion was covered in predoctoral internship training programs, training directors reported that the topic was addressed in individual supervision “if appropriate,” “it comes up periodically,” “highly variable,” and “only a couple of supervisors address this issue.” Brawer et al. (2002) also found that religion as a topic covered in APA-accredited graduate programs was largely unsystematic or it was not covered at all. Another study found that only 5% of clinical psychologists had professional religious training (Shafranske, 1990). An informal study of cross-cultural and multicultural psychotherapy and counseling textbooks revealed a significant lack of coverage of religious aspects of diversity 13 (Richards & Bergin, 2000). Although multicultural writers emphasize the importance of clients’ worldviews and values, religious worldviews and values are usually not explored in multicultural texts at all, or they are given very little attention. Some studies found that as a group, counselors (Holcomb-McCoy & Myers, 1999) and clinicians (Constantine & Ladany, 2000; Worthington, Mobley, Franks, & Andreas Tan, 2000) rate themselves as multiculturally competent. However, self- and other-ratings of competency in cultural case conceptualization do not necessarily correlate significantly, and self-report measures of cultural competency have even exhibited no correlation with cultural case conceptualization ability when social desirability was controlled (Constantine & Ladany, 2000; R. Worthington et al., 2000). Research indicates that levels of multicultural training and rates of both affective and cognitive empathy are positively related to the ability to conceptualize patients’ mental health issues from a diversity perspective when rated by others (Constantine, 2001) . Further, multicultural training should increase awareness of the impact of personal values related to religious and sociopolitical beliefs on the selection of treatment goals and the course of the treatment process (Fuertes & Brobst, 2002; Holcomb-McCoy & Myers, 1999; Sue, 1998). Another study (Hansen et al., 2006) found that among the 91% of clinician respondents who rated themselves as somewhat to very culturally competent, there was a significant difference between what they believed to be important for 14 culturally competent practice and what they actually did in practice. Among commonly recommended competency practices, several were not performed by a significant percentage of the group. This included 42% who rarely or never implemented a professional development plan to increase their cultural competence, 39% who rarely or never utilized culturally specific consultation, and 27% who rarely or never referred cultural group members to a more qualified practitioner. The aforementioned studies examined clinician beliefs and practices related to ethnic and racial groups, which have received significantly more attention in the literature, psychology textbooks, and training programs than have the beliefs and practices of religious groups (Richards & Bergin, 2000). Logic would imply that even fewer steps were taken toward cultural competence in relation to religion by practitioners who have largely reported a lack of concern for religion in their clinical work. Cultural competence as perceived by the patient can have an effect on the patient’s overall satisfaction with treatment. In one study, researchers found a strong correlation between cultural group members’ ratings of clinician cultural competence and general competency and empathy (Fuertes & Brobst, 2002). However, significant differences of the variance for minority groups’ satisfaction beyond general competency and empathy were explained by multicultural competence. In summary, there is a deficit in training and education in working with religious groups. Further, there is often a difference between self- and other- 15 ratings of multicultural competency. These differences were evidenced in ratings of competency in working with racially and ethnically diverse patients. As education and training in working with racial and ethnic patients is more common than those concerning clinical work with religious patients, it is reasonable to assume that cultural competence with religious patients may be even more compromised. Before examining if, and how, the delivery of effective services to the group may be affected, it may be helpful to first explore the general stereotyping and prejudice literature. The Potential for Compromised Clinical Efficacy with Religious Persons Stereotyping and Prejudice Stereotyping can be defined as the use of expectations or beliefs associated with a group or group member based on his or her group membership. Prejudice can be defined as a valenced evaluation of that group or group member (Sherman, Conrey, Stroessner, & Azam, 2005). For the purposes of this paper, bias represents an instance of prejudice, and may manifest in either a positive direction, such as bias toward a secular or liberal worldview, or a negative direction as in a bias against a religious or conservative worldview. When there is neglect in considering a group or group member’s cultural 16 differences or the impact of one’s personal beliefs and expectations of that group or group member in a clinical setting, bias may occur. Generally, the stereotyping and prejudice literature has focused on the process between groups that are dissimilar on some variable. Byrne’s attraction paradigm posits that those who are similar in some way will be attracted to each other and those that are dissimilar will be repulsed by each other (Byrne, 1971). Byrne further hypothesized that the similarity and dissimilarity of attitudes and values is more significant in determining attraction or repulsion than are demographic variables. Bryne showed study participants the attitude scale of a stranger with either like or discordant attitudes on various topics including religion and politics, and found that “the most negative response in the similar attitude group was more positive than the most positive response in the dissimilar attitude group,” and that the attitude variables were so significant that there was no overlap between the two conditions. Subsequent research on the attraction paradigm supports Byrne’s claims for both the attraction and repulsion phenomenon (Chen & Kenrick, 2002; Newcomb, 1961); however other research yielded data that supported the repulsion hypothesis, but not the attraction hypothesis (Rosenbaum, 1986). Relevant to our current concern with potential biased responding to religious patients by religiously dissimilar clinicians, it is also noted that research that examines stereotypes and prejudice for the decades following Bryne’s work, generally focused on negative responding to out-groups. The phenomenon is so well established that minimal group paradigms, or those groups that differ only in 17 irrelevant labeling such as assignment to group “A” or group “B,” also yielded prejudiced responding (Crocker & Schwartz, 1985; Gaertner & Insko, 2000). The stereotyping and prejudice literature is voluminous. It encompasses several processes and many variables. The three categories of processes involved in the development and maintenance of stereotyping and prejudice are motivational, sociocultural, and cognitive (Hilton & Hippel, 1996). Many studies overlap categories, such as those that examine the motivational aspects of reducing cognitive load (Biernat & Korbrynowicz, 2003), or enhancing selfevaluation through social comparison (Brickman & Bulman, 1977; Festinger, 1954; Taylor & Lobel, 1989). Sociocultural models include social comparison perspectives that examine how the self is defined in relation to others (see Duckitt, Birum, Wagner, & Plessis, 2002; Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Brickman & Bulman, 1977; Crocker, McGraw, Thompson, & Ingerman, 1987; Festinger, 1954; Taylor & Lobel, 1989). Examples of sociocultural explanations of prejudice include the effect of social position and social dominance orientation on prejudice (Guimond, Dambrun, Michinov, & Duarte, 2003), and the tendency to compare oneself favorably against a less fortunate target when under threat (Taylor & Lobel, 1989). Other sociocultural processes are found in the in- and out-group similarity and dissimilarity effects literature (Byrne, 1971; Rosenbaum, 1986). Further, the strength of social identity theory, or the theory that group membership creates self-identification with an in-group that favors the in-group at the expense of the 18 out-group, has been explored in the minimal group paradigm (Crocker & Schwartz, 1985; Gaertner & Insko, 2000; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Cognitive processes examine the relationship between variables such as information-processing strategies and prejudice (Bodenhausen & Lichtenstein, 1987; Hamilton, Sherman, & Ruvolo, 1990; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986). It has long been hypothesized that stereotyping can be viewed as a cognitive construct that is utilized in order to generate and manage responses to an otherwise overwhelming amount of information (Allport, 1954; Bodenhausen & Lichtenstein, 1987; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986; Korten, 1973). The use of stereotypes and stereotype-based expectancies as a tool to reduce cognitive load has been examined in research on cognitive resource preservation (Biernat & Korbrynowicz, 2003; Crawford & Skowronski, 1998; Macrae & Milne, 1994; Yzerbyt & Coull, 1999), as well as research on the relationship between stereotyping and cognitive simplicity (Koenig & King, 1964). Motivational processes are found in much of the stereotype and prejudice literature, often overlapping with sociocultural and cognitive explanations. Those processes include the motivation to increase self-esteem under threat (Crocker & Luhtanen, 1990), to use stereotyping to reduce cognitive load (Biernat & Korbrynowicz, 2003; Crawford & Skowronski, 1998; Macrae & Milne, 1994; Yzerbyt & Coull, 1999), and to use stereotype information by those who have a preference for cognition (Crawford & Skowronski, 1998). The motivation to respond without prejudice to targets with which one might have knowledge of stereotyped associations or expectancies has also been of interest (Devine, 19 Plant, Amodio, Harmon-Jones, & Vance, 2002; Plant & Devine, 1998). Specifically, in efforts to explain differentials in how stereotype information may be behaviorally expressed, Devine et al. (2002) and Plant and Devine (1998) posit that motivation to either suppress stereotype tendencies or ignore stereotype associations moderates the expression of prejudice. The effects of both affect and cognition about targets has been examined in the literature as well. Stereotype-related affect has been operationalized in the literature in several ways including “liking” for targets or target groups (Jussim, Manis, Nelson, & Soffin, 1995), and agreement with mood-affect adjectives about targets (Jackson & Sullivan, 2001). Stereotype cognition refers to beliefs or expectations about stereotype targets. Research indicates that both affect and cognition play a role in the use of stereotyped and prejudiced responding. In summary, it is clear that stereotyping and prejudice between groups is common. A large focus of the literature is on understanding various mechanisms that contribute to the development and maintenance of stereotypes and prejudiced responding. The circumstances under which stereotyping or prejudice may occur are many. To hypothesize that a clinician’s emphasis on empathy and acceptance will necessarily preclude him or her from such a ubiquitous phenomenon is probably not realistic, although it is hoped that clinicians’ general training, clinical intention, and capacity for introspection at the least will moderate some of those tendencies. Nonetheless, the possibility of prejudice against, and stereotyping of, dissimilar groups exists, and clinicians who value empathy and patient acceptance may also demonstrate biased patterns of responding. This 20 potential is further evident in APA policies that prohibit discrimination against groups (see APA, 2002 Section 3.01). As Byrne hypothesized, those different in values and attitudes may demonstrate even more bias against the out-group than those who are demographically dissimilar. Such may be the case with clinicians who have religiously diverse attitudes and values than their patients. Discordant Values and Efficacy of Practice Without religious training, insensitive, biased, or uninformed approaches to religious patients may significantly impact treatment. For an understanding of how religiously competent services can be affected by compromised approaches, one must first look beyond religious affiliation and group membership to differences in beliefs and values and their behavioral manifestation. Value systems that arise from religious beliefs are distinct from the belief system itself. Recall that religious beliefs can be defined as “propositional statements (in agreement with some organized religion) that a person holds to be true concerning religion or religious spirituality”, and religious values can be defined as “superordinate organizing statements of what a person considers to be important,” that arise from his or her religion (Worthington, 1996, p.2). Religious values sometimes stand in contrast to humanistic values. For instance, whether one’s particular religious beliefs are consistent with Jewish, Muslim, or Christian doctrine, a value that arises from each is the importance placed on some absolute and universal ethics, with less focus on the relative 21 values and situational ethics that are typically encompassed in secular humanistic worldviews. Another religious value is that God is supreme, and humility and acceptance of divine authority are desirable virtues, rather than beliefs that either humans are supreme, or no one and nothing is supreme (Bergin, 1980). In the theoretical literature, other differences between clinical-humanistic and theistic values have been proposed and discussed as valuable distinctions by proponents of each value system (see Bergin, 1980; Walls, 1980) with a third value system, that of a clinical-humanistic-atheistic one, added (Ellis, 1980a) . These themes may appear overly simplistic; however, some contrasting themes between theistic and clinical-humanistic values are evident. Some of the thematic distinctions that can be made between religious values and humanistic ones are personal identity that is eternal and derived from the divine and one’s relationship with the divine, compared with personal identity that is ephemeral and mortal; love, affection, and self-transcendence as primary, compared with personal needs and self-actualization as primary; commitment to marriage, fidelity, and loyalty with an emphasis on family life, compared with choice of no marriage, conventional marriage, or open marriage with emphasis on self-gratification which considers family life as optional; and personal responsibility for harmful actions and changes in those actions with acceptance of guilt, suffering, and contrition as key agents of change, compared with personal responsibility for one’s own harmful actions with the minimization of guilt, and a focus on relief of suffering. 22 In summary, the proposed clinical-humanistic values can be viewed as more liberal than their theistic and conservative counterparts. The terms “conservative” and “liberal” are as operationalized by William Stone, that “a conservative person is one who is devoted to the status quo and who accepts authority and the norms of society. A liberal is change-oriented and places great emphasis on individual freedom, being opposed to the external imposition of authority” (1994, p. 701). Stone also emphasizes that there are likely no pure liberals as there are also not likely any pure conservatives, but “that they are all mixtures, to some degree, of opposing tendencies” (p. 701). Research indicates that therapists also emphasize values that are more liberal than those of their clients. They generally endorse lifestyles that are freer, particularly in the sexual area (Khan & Cross, 1983). Differences in, and attitudes toward, sexual values vary in relation to religious involvement and political affiliation as well as gender. In one study (Ford & Hendrick, 2003), politically conservative and Protestant and Catholic therapists endorsed items of “sex as an expression of love and commitment,” and beliefs about the desirability of sex as expressed exclusively within marriage and committed relationships, significantly more than did politically liberal therapists. Nonreligious and Jewish therapists endorsed greater comfort with homosexuality as natural and same-sex sexual practices. Politically liberal therapists endorsed items such as homosexuality as natural, and that marriage provides too many restrictions on sexual freedom, more frequently than conservative therapists. Overall, therapists reported being comfortable working with a wide variety of sexual values in 23 therapy. However, although clinicians report comfortability working with a variety of values, they may not be culturally competent in doing so with diverse groups. The values of the clinician may be reflected in the treatment goals they endorse, which may be discordant with the patients’ value system, or they may ignore the spiritual or religious functioning issues of the patient. Particularly if the therapist is unaware of the impact of his or her personal values on clinical work, or biases against the values of others, compromised treatment may result. The effects of reinforcement and nonreinforcement of patient values is evident in the outcomes and value convergence literature. Value convergence is often an indicator of counselor-perceived improved patient outcomes (Beutler & Bergan, 1991; Worthington, 1988), however the effects for patient- and otherrated perceptions of improvement are less strong (Kelly, 1990). One implication of these findings is that clinicians may perceive patients to be healthier when their values more closely match their own. This effect may indicate that valueladen therapy may seek to alter patient values (Beutler, Crago, & Arizmendi, 1986; Beutler & Bergan, 1991; Kelly, 1990; Richards & Bergin, 2000; Worthington, 1988). As evidenced in studies on Carl Rogers’s patterns of reinforcement and nonreinforcement of patient verbalizations related to values and differences in response style (Murray, 1956; Truax, 1966), even within a therapy orientation that focuses on acceptance and positive regard, it is indicated that the values of the therapist influence the course of therapy, and its perceived outcomes. Indeed, following a discussion on the effects of therapist values on therapy with religious patients (see Bergin, 1980) one author writes, “the fact 24 cited by Bergin that, in general, the values of psychotherapists differ from the public’s is not alarming; it is encouraging”, and further that “we should both expect and demand that the values of psychotherapists be more carefully reasoned and, on the whole, more adequate than the values of the general public” (Walls, 1980, p. 641). Further, Albert Ellis also encouraged therapists to capitalize on their power to influence value change in their patients (Ellis, 1980). However, both positions appear to be in direct conflict with APA’s Principle E (2002) which discusses clinicians’ responsibility to respect group differences. Also, when a clinician neglects to consider the nature of the patient’s religious beliefs and values, therapeutic efficacy may be affected, and harm may even result. In one case of religious neglect in therapy, a male therapist working with a Latter-Day Saint (LDS) couple recommended that the couple abstain from sexual relations for the coming week to relieve the wife’s feelings of being sexually manipulated. The male therapist then advised the husband to masturbate if he found it too difficult to refrain from being sexually active, not realizing that masturbation was considered a sin in the LDS church. The therapist further encouraged the wife to seek employment outside of the home when she relayed feelings of stress around her homemaking role, again not realizing that they both regarded her homemaking role as sacred and divinely appointed. The couple was offended by the therapist’s lack of sensitivity to their religious beliefs and terminated therapy (Richards & Bergin, 2000). 25 Other Barriers to Effective Treatment The theoretical and empirical literature also indicates that religious patients may not receive services comparable to those provided to secular patients based on several other factors. Those factors include patient comfortability in receiving services from a secular profession whose values may be markedly different from their own (Richards & Bergin, 2000). It appears there is public awareness of the differences in religious beliefs and values between clinicians and potential patients and community leaders. Even the most religiously informed clinical treatment cannot benefit religious patients if they do not attend treatment. Many patients express concern that psychologists will not understand their worldview or may see it as inferior and seek to change it, and as a result many are unlikely to seek psychological services (King, 1978). Alternately, one analogue study that utilized a young cohort found that some conservative Christians’ apprehension about therapy does not necessarily indicate that they will avoid therapy or that their beliefs about therapy as effective are necessarily compromised (Guinee & Tracey, 1997). Reduced credibility and trust with religious patients, communities, and leaders may occur and contribute to patient decisions not to avail themselves of much needed services (Richards & Bergin, 2000; Worthington & Sandage, 2001). While one could argue that it is each person’s responsibility to seek psychological help if he or she needs it, it is also appropriate that health practitioners prepare themselves to provide services that are culturally empathic 26 and competent as they strive to serve a diverse public that may have awareness of therapist’s worldviews and values that may conflict with their own. The decision not to enter therapy can have tragic results. This is illustrated in the case of a depressed religious man who refused to enter psychotherapy claiming that “those immoral anti-God psychotherapists can’t be trusted” (Richards & Bergin, 2000, p.11). His mistrust of psychotherapy unfortunately may have been shared by his pastor who did not refer him to therapy. Six weeks following a job loss, the man committed suicide. Other clinical concerns may arise in relation to religiously oriented issues in therapy. Worthington and Sandage (2001) cites five such examples. First, highly religious patients may request religious therapy and may question their therapist regarding their religious views. The therapist who is unfamiliar and untrained in working with highly religious patients may view the questioning as aggressive, defensive, or anxious. Also, patients may insist that religious influence not be part of therapy. Next, if the therapist’s approach to religion is implicit, disagreement on some fundamental beliefs may impair even the most tolerant therapists’ ability to help. Level of acculturation plays a role in the patient’s religious beliefs and identity, with generational and geographical influences sometimes contributing to religious or spiritual confusion. Patients should also be seen as part of a relational system, including as part of a couple, family, church, or community with variations in religious commitment, development, values, and functioning. Lastly, personal sociopolitical and religious values and preferences may contribute to affective or cognitive bias against 27 conservative religious persons whose beliefs and values are markedly different from those of the clinician, particularly as the impact of the clinician’s personal beliefs on therapy goes unexamined. Sociopolitical Trends, Religiosity, and Affect Multicultural training programs sensitive to religious diversity, and proponents of the representation of sociopolitical diversity in psychology, both emphasize the importance of awareness of the impact of sociopolitical influences on both research and clinical practice (Fuertes & Brobst, 2002; Redding, 2002; Wester & Vogel, 2002). Awareness of sociopolitical influences is key in providing culturally competent services to religiously diverse patients. The relationship between sociopolitical views and religion is often assumed, and perhaps largely exaggerated in the popular press. However, some evidence for a relationship between the two has been observed. To examine the importance of this relationship and its potential impact on patients, it is necessary to briefly discuss the current sociopolitical trends. The political literature explores the assumption that liberalism may be thought of as the opposite of conservatism, and that each is to some extent represented in politics by the Republican or Democratic parties (Kerlinger, 1984). While this unipolar or “polarized” view of the cultural differences of American sociopolitical groups has been viewed as overly simplistic and reductive, that a 28 political divide in America exists is generally agreed upon in the social psychology (Seyle & Newman, 2006) and political (Wallis, 2005) literature. Further, a trend for religiosity to fluctuate with sociopolitical selfidentification has empirical support. Older studies emphasized the correlation between increased religiosity and political conservatism (Allport & Ross, 1967; Batson, 1976; Gorsuch & Aleshire, 1974). More recently and more specifically, research indicated that Evangelical or born-again Christians and Mormons were most likely to identify with the Republican Party, while Buddhists, Jews, Muslims, and those with no religion, had a greater preference for the Democratic Party (The Graduate Center, 2001) . Another study (Winseman, 2005) indicated that those who reported no religion were more affiliated with liberal ideologies, belong to younger age groups, and were represented with a slight skew toward higher education (where 12% of “nones” have some college education versus 9% that have a high school education.) Further, liberal political ideology was associated with a more secular worldview than a conservative one. Those who reported having no political affiliation were also least likely to claim any religious affiliation. The relationship is explored in data provided by the Gallup Organization (M.A. Strausberg, personal communication, April 17, 2006) in which 73% of selfdeclared politically conservative persons reported that religion is very important in their lives, compared to 45% of political liberals. Further, Evangelical Christians, presumably a fairly conservative group, skew strongly Republican (Newport & Carroll, 2005). 29 The divide may contribute to affectively charged feelings about whether and how secular or religious worldviews influence American politics (Wallis, 2005). Sixty-two percent of Republican college students believe that the impact of religion on daily American life is declining, and by a margin of 7 to 1 believe this to be a “bad thing,” whereas 54% of college Democrats believe religion to be increasing in influence, and by a 2 to 1 margin believe this to be a “bad thing” (Shaheen et al., 2006). Differences between political parties in whether politicians should talk openly about their religion, and whether religion should influence policy, also reflect that divide. Psychologists’ ideology has also been explored. Seventy percent of psychologists identified themselves as Democrat and only 21% as Republican in one study (McClintock, Spaulding, & Turner, 1965). In a series of four studies between 1969 and 1989 (American Enterprise Institute, 1991), 68% of psychology faculty members self-identified as liberal and only 15% self-identified as conservative. Other literature recognizes the lack of sociopolitical diversity in the field, and in particular the absence of conservative influences, and advocates for more diverse representations and less bias in research, policy advocacy, professional education, and practice (e.g., Brand, 2002; Johnson, Nielson, & Ridley, 2000; Redding, 2001; Richards & Davison, 1992; Wester & Vogel, 2002). Redding (2001) asserts that due to an obvious trend toward sociopolitical homogeneity within the profession of psychology, and an unspoken assumption that psychologists must share the same liberal worldview, even psychologists with 30 more sociopolitical or religiously conservative views may be excluded or marginalized, which in turn can have several negative consequences. These include the impediment of services to conservative patients, biased research on social policy issues, damage to psychology’s credibility with policymakers and the public as a descriptive rather than a prescriptive science, and discrimination against scholars and students (2001), particularly those who hold more conservative worldviews and who are developing a growing sense of themselves as therapists (Wester & Vogel, 2002). In summary, as recent trends evidence some level of polarity between political parties, whether exaggerated in the popular press or not, and the relationship between religious conservatism or liberalism and political party endorsement has been seen in the empirical literature, affect surrounding either position may occur. It is possible that any negative affect or biased beliefs about groups often seen as “polar” opposites in worldviews may generalize to the clinical setting. Is it possible that empathy might be affected within the relationship if such “polar” worldviews are present? Empathy The concept of empathy has generated a plethora of research following Carl Rogers’ writings of its importance in psychotherapy. Empathy has been thought to be a primary factor in discriminating effectiveness of therapy. Indeed, Rogers (1957) made the case that empathy and related constructs are all that is 31 needed to produce positive change in a patient. Operationally, empathy has been described several ways. These include cognitive dimensions such as vicarious introspection (Kohut, 1977 p. 459), and affective dimensions or “vicariously experienced emotion” (Strayer, 1990, p.225). Either of these may be achieved “through the therapists sensitive ability and willingness to understand the client’s thoughts, feelings, and struggles from the client’s point of view,” and “to adopt his frame of reference” (Rogers, 1980, p. 85). It has also been described as seeing the world through the eyes of another (Ivey, Ivey, & Simek-Morgan, 1993). Various dimensional components of empathy have also been examined. They include empathic resonance, expressed empathy and received empathy (Barrett-Lennard, 1981), cognitive perspectives which seek to understand the thoughts and feelings of others, affective empathy in which one seeks to experience a sense of feeling and sharing in another’s emotions (Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972), and approaches which combine cognitive and affective aspects of empathy (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998; Davis, 1983; Strayer, 1990). Much of empathy-related theory and research is focused on empathy as a disposition or personality trait (see Davis, 1994; Duan & Hill, 1996; Eisenberg et al., 1994; Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983; Houston, 1990). One perspective often used in the psychological literature is Davis’s (1994) multidimensional approach to empathy. Davis’s Interpersonal Reactivity Index measures four dimensions of empathy. The constructs are Personal Distress, or the tendency to experience discomfort in response to the distress of others; Fantasy, which is the ability to transpose oneself into imaginary situations; and particularly salient to the psychotherapist is 32 Perspective Taking or the tendency to adopt the psychological view of others; and Empathic Concern or the tendency to experience warmth, concern, and compassion for others. Acceptance as a foundation to empathy has also been discussed (Ivey et al., 1993; Rogers, 1957). Rogers’ theory of unconditional positive regard as necessary for an effective therapeutic relationship, underscores the importance of acceptance of the patient. He further describes specific actions and skills in demonstrating an empathic attitude and in communicating empathy and understanding of the client. These skills generally include reflective statements that deliberately preclude the influence or communication of one’s own thoughts or ideas. It is reasonable to assume that if the influence of one’s thoughts or ideas goes unexamined, that this may be difficult or even impossible to do. Culture also impacts the understanding of, and empathy for, others (Ivey et al., 1993). Clinicians’ ability to empathize with their religiously diverse patients may significantly affect whether or not they are able to provide culturally competent services. In multicultural psychotherapy, Ivey et al. (1993) posit that “the concept of respectfully entering the other person’s world has profound implications” which echoes APA mandates that “multicultural empathy requires that we respect worldviews different from our own” (1993, p.25). Further, Ridley and Lingle (1996) defined cultural empathy as the therapist’s tendency to understand the experience of culturally diverse patients based on the therapists’ interpretations of “cultural data.” Ivey et al. (1993) stipulated that positive regard as a precursor to empathy requires that we find positives in that data, and 33 positives within the worldviews and attitudes of culturally diverse patients. Finding positives in a worldview with which one may strongly disagree, or that one has strong negative affect about, may be difficult for the most empathic psychotherapist. Subtle messages of approval or disapproval of worldviews with which one disagrees, has negative affect about, or which is judged inferior or unhealthy, may be communicated to the patient, perhaps without the awareness of the therapist. Patterns of responding may manifest in duration and frequency of eye contact, affirming or disconfirming facial expressions, verbalizations, or gestures. Even Carl Rogers evidenced repeated patterns of reinforcement or nonreinforcement of value-laden patient communications (Truax, 1966) and similarity of patient style of expression (Murray, 1956). His patterns of responding were noted to lead to altered patient behavior, despite presumed attempts to respond in an accepting and empathic manner without influence of his own thoughts, ideas, or feelings. When working with religiously diverse patients, particularly those whose religious worldviews and values may elicit affective charge or negative cognitive appraisals, it may be more difficult to have empathy for the dissimilar patient, than the religiously similar patient. One study of dimensional empathy indicated that therapists high in affective and cognitive empathy demonstrated increased cultural conceptualization skills, and those high in affective empathy were more aware of cultural factors in conceptualization that those with low empathy (Constantine, 2001). In another study (Burkard & Knox, 2004), psychologists who 34 were willing to acknowledge the impact of race in patients’ lives, demonstrated more empathy than those rated as “color blind” or racist. In conclusion, empathy is viewed as a cornerstone of psychotherapy that seeks to understand and respect the patient’s experience. However, even clinicians who most value an empathic stance toward their patients may be unaware of responding that alters the patient’s behavior. Implicit or explicit patterns of responding may be value-laden enough to change the course of therapy and treatment outcomes. Explicit recommendations to encourage value change are in contradiction to APA’s guidance (see APA, 2002, Principle E) about respecting group differences. For obvious reasons, responding to patients’ cultural values in a way that explicitly encourages change, clearly impacts one’s ability to communicate empathy and positive regard for the worldview that is perceived to be in need of change. If indeed it is true that empathy is key in forming a therapeutic relationship and effecting treatment outcomes, impaired empathy with diverse groups can impact the course and efficacy of treatment. Religion and Mental Health In addition to the possibility of transferring negative affect associated with the clinician’s personal religious and/or sociopolitical beliefs and experiences to the therapeutic setting, therapists may have cognitive appraisals of religious patients as more mentally ill than their nonreligious counterparts. If clinicians believe there is an association between religiosity and poor mental health, initial 35 impressions of the patient may be affected. Initial impressions of patients which include personal liking for the patient, therapist assessment of patient’s potential for change, and ease of patient expression, may have an effect on treatment outcomes in several ways. These include therapist satisfaction with patient progress, therapist perception of patient satisfaction, and type of termination (Brown, 1970). Also, the patient’s religiosity or associated values may be targeted for change if it is evaluated as a contributor to poor mental health. Lastly, the patient who is viewed to have poorer mental health may also be seen to have a poorer prognosis. Expectations that are based on stereotyped information have been associated with effects on information processing and judgments, information seeking and hypothesis testing, and interpersonal behavior via self-fulfilling prophecies (Hamilton et al., 1990). It is reasonable to assume that therapists may also be affected by expectations of patient prognosis or outcomes based on stereotyped appraisals of their religiosity and subsequently adjust their clinical approach accordingly. As we have seen, historically there has been some disparagement of religion within the field of psychology for various reasons. Sigmund Freud offered that God is “nothing but an exalted father” (Freud, 1913/2000, p. 256), and that all faith was at least neurotically determined (Freud, 1913/2000, pp. 174281). Freud either ignored healthy and nonpathological faith or simply did not believe that it existed. His aggrandizement of the scientific worldview and sweeping disparagement of those who endorse religious and spiritual beliefs is echoed 67 years later by Albert Ellis. Prior to recanting some of his views 36 recently (Ellis, 2000), Ellis was clear in articulating his beliefs that religious persons are quite emotionally disturbed, and even suffer from the most severe of disturbances (Ellis, 1980, p. 8). Ideological influences are evident in psychological scales purported to assess mental health or development. These do not consider an understanding of the religious persons’ worldview and may characterize religious persons as Ellis describes, irrational and dogmatic, and even morally less well developed (see Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992; Richards & Davison, 1992) . C.S. Lewis cautions against holding up a view of Christianity that a small child might take and presenting it in its concretized and overly simplistic form as an expression of the whole breadth and depth of Christian religious thought (Lewis, 2001). For instance, the assessment of narrow-minded authoritarian fundamentalism can be attributed to antireligious, and overly reductive ideological statements in the Religious Fundamentalism Scale (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992). This scale forces religious persons to choose between severely concrete “unsympathetic normative assumptions” (Watson et al., 2003) or denying their religious beliefs. An example of such splitting is the statement “whenever science and sacred scripture conflict, science must be wrong,” rather than a more culturally appropriate alternative such as “God’s hand is in all creation and in all truth; so conflicts between faith and science should not frighten us, but rather inspire us to seek God’s truth” (2003). Unfortunately, claims made by figures such as Freud (1913/2000), Ellis (1980), and (Watters, 1992), that religious persons are irrational, neurotic, and 37 generally emotionally unhealthy, and that religious psychologists may be assumed to have or have had personal problems (Sarason, 1993) cannot be easily dismissed. They have been made over time by influential psychologists, and they may represent the views of other psychologists or may further influence the therapist’s conceptualization of religious persons. On the heels of those claims, whether there is a relationship between religion and mental health is a question worth investigating. Contrary to broad assertions about the relationship between poor mental health and religiosity, two meta-analytic studies (Bergin, 1983; Gartner, Larson, & Allen, 1991) yielded no evidence and inconsistent evidence respectively. A careful review of the literature (Gartner et al., 1991) revealed that differences in variables and measures may contribute to former mixed findings. For example, trends toward negative religious coping (religious discontent, punishing God reappraisals) are associated with poorer mental health, while positive religious coping (seeking spiritual support, religious forgiveness, and benevolent religious reappraisal) is expressed more frequently and associated with better mental health (Pargament, Smith, Koenig, & Perez, 1998). The differences in findings in the two styles of religious coping emphasize that when drawing conclusions about religiosity and mental health, it is as important to operationalize and distinguish between types of religiosity, as it is to distinguish between good and bad therapy. Also, extrinsic religiosity (religiosity used as a means to another end such as increased social status) moderated an overall mild association between 38 religiousness and fewer depressive symptoms, while positive religious coping and an intrinsic orientation (one in which religion is an end in itself) were associated with lower levels of depression (Smith, McCullough, & Poll, 2003) as well as lower levels of manifest anxiety (Bergin, Masters, & Richards, 1987). Moreover, increased associations between general psychological well-being and religion were demonstrated in a Christian sample (Francis & Peter, 2002), and positive religious coping has been consistently associated with improved mental health in patients experiencing chronic pain (see Rippentrop, 2005 for review) and rehabilitation (Kilpatrick & McCullough, 1999). Lastly, a review of religiosity and mental health was summarized by the conclusion that “devout religiousness and frequent involvement in both private and public religious activities are associated with better mental health” (Koenig, 1997 p. 101). In summary, in this section we have briefly explored the frequent use of stereotyping and prejudice of out-groups under various conditions and for various reasons. Further, we have explored whether or not the effect of psychologists’ emphasis on empathy and acceptance precludes them from responding to their religious patients in ways that are culturally compromised or with less positive regard than their nonreligious counterparts. We have discussed the potential for affect to be associated with clinicians’ responses to religious, and in particular religiously conservative, patients. Several factors were explored that can contribute to the uninformed, neglectful, or biased responding to religiously diverse patients. These include the lack of multicultural training with an emphasis on the need for the personal evaluation of the impact of one’s sociopolitical 39 ideology, personal beliefs and prejudices, and training specific to assessing, understanding, and working with persons with religious worldviews. Liberal trends in academic, clinical, and research psychology have been discussed, and have been recognized and discussed by those interested in sociopolitical pluralism which includes a more adequate representation of sociopolitical diversity in psychology. Cognitive evaluations of religious persons as neurotic, irrational, illogical, emotionally unhealthy, and dogmatic have been demonstrated by primary theorists and implicitly validated through the publication of these antireligious views. Given these considerations, what does the literature to date on clinical bias with religious patients yield? The Current Research Literature on Bias with Religious Patients Studies on bias against religious patients have been inconsistent to date. Methodological concerns indicate that conclusions and generalizability of results should be considered carefully. Negative findings were reported in several studies with problematic methodology. Reed (1992) investigated clinician assessment of pathology and prognosis of religious or nonreligious couples who were referred for adoption evaluations. No bias was found in this study. Another study with negative results often cited in the literature is Wadsworth and Checketts’ study (1980) of potential clinical bias 40 in clinician/patient dyads with dissimilar religious values. Houts and Graham (1986) also found no clinical bias in prognosis, pathology, and internal versus external attributions of patient difficulties with religious patients. However, other studies uncovered bias (Gartner, 1990; O'Connor & Vandenberg, 2005.). A closer look at each study is warranted to understand findings more clearly and in determining directions for future research that seeks to resolve inconsistent results and methodological shortcomings. Two studies (Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985) failed to find significant bias in the assessment of pathology with religious persons. Both of these studies were conducted using populations in geographical regions which may be presumed to be conservative (the South and the Midwest). It is not known what impact ongoing exposure to a culture dissimilar to one’s own may have on stereotype or prejudice formation and maintenance of the group. In other words, it is possible that the dissimilar group’s status as an outgroup has less impact on stereotyping when there is significantly more familiarization with the group, than with groups that are in the minority and may thereby be more easily perceived as deviating from the norm. Work on the attitudinal effects of exposure to a target indicate that mere exposure facilitates liking, relative to attitudes toward targets to which one has no exposure (Zajonc, 1968). This exposure effect, or the phenomenon that “familiarity breeds liking,” is demonstrated in interpersonal attractiveness research in which a target is perceived as more intelligent and attractive when exposure occurs more frequently. It is possible that this effect may moderate 41 biased responding in areas in which religious groups are the norm, and thereby more familiar to clinicians. Research indicates that clinician first impressions of patients which include “liking” for them, has a significant effect on outcomes including evaluation of patient progress, eventual number of sessions, assessment of patient progress, clinician satisfaction with patient progress, and type of termination (Brown, 1970). If ongoing exposure to a cultural group or group member may increase one’s perception of a group member’s intelligence or attractiveness, and first impressions of patients including “liking” have significant effects on treatment outcomes, there is potential that in a geographical location where a religiously conservative person is a distinct minority and may be quite dissimilar in beliefs and values to a clinician, initial impressions of the patient based on any affect or cognitive evaluations about his or her religiosity unmoderated by familiarity, may significantly impact treatment outcomes. Further, Houts and Graham’s study (1986) reported no bias in prognosis, pathology, and internal versus external attributions of patient difficulties, as evaluated by either religious or nonreligious clinicians. In this study, groups of patients were assigned categories of either no mention of religion, moderately religious, or very religious. No bias was found against those designated to the very religious category relative to the other two groups, but bias was found against the moderately religious category. Closer examination of the moderately religious group reveals that persons in that group expressed doubt about their religious beliefs. It is difficult to reconcile doubt about religious beliefs with a “moderate” religious belief system. This group was rated as having more 42 psychopathology and a poorer prognosis than the other two groups. Interpretation of results in this study should be made with caution as the category designations are not representative of the descriptive narrative in the vignette. The group assignment of clinicians as either religious or nonreligious may also be problematic. Recalling Byrne’s theory that attitudes and values have more of an impact on attraction and repulsion than do demographics, dichotomous labeling of one’s religiosity appears reductive. Interpretations made based on data yielded between groups assigned in such a manner should be undertaken with caution. Similarly, Lewis and Lewis (1985) measured pathology, prognosis, and patient attractiveness rated by religious and nonreligious clinicians, as determined by clinician self-report about whether he or she was religiously affiliated. No significance was found on pathology and liking of the patient; however, the patient’s religiosity had a significant effect on clinician perception of her difficulties. Both religious and nonreligious clinicians predicted fewer sessions would be needed for progress with the religious patient than with the nonreligious patient. Of particular interest is that nonreligious clinicians rated nonreligious patients as needing almost twice as many sessions as the religious patient. Authors speculate that this finding may reflect clinicians’ belief that treatment progress may be enhanced by the religious patient’s religious background. In another study often cited for finding no bias in the clinical evaluation of religious patients (Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980), only Latter-Day Saints and “other” subjects were evaluated, and they were evaluated by psychologists in 43 Utah. Clearly, generalizing these results to clinicians from other geographic regions or to patients of other religious backgrounds is problematic. Also in this study, Wadsworth used no control vignettes and each of the four vignettes used described religiously affiliated persons, whether they were currently active or inactive participants in religious beliefs or behaviors. Another study that did not find bias (Reed, 1992) used a between-subjects design to measure psychologists’ reactions on a pathology measure to one of four patient vignettes. The patient vignettes characterized either a deeply religious or strongly atheist position, or a newly religious or newly atheistic position. This study is interesting in its approach to expand on other research (i.e., Houts & Graham, 1986) that indicated that stability of religious position may be a factor in clinicians’ determination of pathology. However, clinician religiosity was not considered in the analysis and using a homogenous group to evaluate vignettes in a between-subjects design may have mitigated negative bias such that it was undetectable. There are limitations in each of the studies discussed. One limitation is the relatively undefined dichotomization of religious versus nonreligious clinicians and/or patients represented in vignettes. The use of more descriptive categorizations of religiousness that discloses more information about religious beliefs and values, or the extent to which religious beliefs or behaviors are made manifest in one’s life, might yield more helpful information. Dimensional and descriptive narratives would be more appropriate than categorical assignment if research seeks to detect clinical bias with individuals who differ from them in 44 religious beliefs and behaviors rather than labels. Negative findings could be the result of poor clinician group assignment. In other words, religious affiliation does not a religious person make, and many levels and dimensions of religiosity may be represented in those categorical assignments. Another limitation is the inability to generalize results from research conducted in areas that may be presumed to have a fairly substantial religious population. It may be that bias would have been found in similar studies conducted in more religiously liberal areas or on a national sample. We have seen that research on the exposure effect indicates that exposure to religiously diverse groups may contribute to an increase in clinicians’ perception of patients’ intelligence and attractiveness, and therefore potentially affect clinicians’ judgment of pathology, prognosis, or empathy. Perhaps their out-group status is less pronounced as they become more familiar. Generalizability of findings to areas in which one has limited exposure to religiously diverse groups, or to a national population of clinicians, may be unwarranted. Also, each of the studies above utilized a between-subjects design with no attempts to detect favorable bias in the direction of the religious patient compared to the nonreligious patient. Therefore, negative bias may have been mitigated so that it was undetectable, compromising results. Lastly, social desirability was not controlled in any of the studies above. With the emphasis on clinical empathy and patient acceptance, and fairly ubiquitous social disapproval of prejudice, it is reasonable to assume that social desirability could have affected clinician responding in each of the studies. 45 In a within-subjects study that did reveal biased results, vignettes of patients belonging to extreme ideological groups were rated (Gartner, Hohmann, Harmatz, & Larson, 1990). Groups that were represented were right wing religious, left wing religious, right wing political, or left wing political groups. Fictitious patients were represented equally in each of either the four ideological categories or a nonideological group. This within-subjects design measured reactions of psychologists to both nonideological categories and one of the four ideological categories. Subjects rated the patient on measures of empathy, pathology, and perceived maturity of the patient. Using a national sample of clinicians, significant bias was found on every variable. As this study explored clinician reaction to liberal and conservative ideological poles, the vignettes were more informative about the patients than if they had been identified by religious affiliation alone. It is also important to note that this study utilized a withinsubjects design in a national sample of psychologists, which may be considered more religiously liberal than the general public. Another study found bias against religious patients who were rated as more mentally ill and in need of more sessions to make progress compared to their nonreligious counterparts by a group of homogenous therapists (Hillowe, 1986). In this study, therapists’ traditional and nondoctrinal religiosity was measured. Interestingly, as therapists’ nondoctrinal religious attitudes increased, the prognosis of religious patients increased relative to nonreligous patients’ prognostic ratings. Hillowe speculated that his study may have found results where others did not because of dichotomous categorization of therapists in 46 previous work, whereas the religiosity of the vignette patients and clinicians in his study were descriptive in terms of expressed beliefs and behaviors, and measured on the dimensions of traditional or nontraditional religiosity. He also believes that the interaction found between nondoctrinally religious clinicians and more positive prognostic ratings of religious patients may be due to the clinicians’ beliefs that religious patients may share a base of faith and hope that will assist them in the therapy process toward more optimistic outcomes. Bias was found in another study (O'Connor & Vandenberg, 2005) that used a between-subjects design and investigated clinicians’ evaluation of religious beliefs as more or less pathological in terms of psychosis, depending on religious beliefs that are most mainstream (Catholic), less mainstream (Mormon), and least mainstream (Nation of Islam.) The doctrine of each religion was represented by corresponding beliefs articulated by patients in vignettes. Beliefs included that one patient “came to believe quite passionately in the Mormon religion, whose tenets state that he will be transformed into a god after he dies,” that the Catholic patient believed “the Holy Spirit has given him a special strength to defend the faith,” and that the patient who was a member of the Nation of Islam “believes in the revelation that a spaceship, the Mother Wheel, has been hovering over the United States since 1929.” Four sets of vignettes depicted the various beliefs described in either religiously specific language, or in language that does not identify a specific religion, or with changes as a result of these beliefs representing either a no-harm situation (these beliefs deepened his relationship with his girlfriend), or a harm situation (the change affected a 47 relationship that had previously been a positive one to the point that the patient considered killing his girlfriend following a betrayal). Three other distracting vignettes were also used. Each participant received and rated 6 vignettes total, consisting of 3 distracter vignettes, the religious, the nonreligious, and either no harm, or harm vignettes. Less mainstream religions were considered more pathological, with Catholic beliefs being rated less pathological than Mormon beliefs, and Mormon beliefs rated less pathological than Nation of Islam beliefs. When Catholic and Mormon beliefs were associated with their religions in the vignettes, they were rated as less pathological than when they were not. However, there was no difference in the pathology rating for Nation of Islam patients in either case, with Nation of Islam beliefs rated highly and equally pathological, and significantly more pathological than Mormon or Catholic beliefs, whether identified as related to religion or not. Authors speculated that general familiarity with Catholic and Mormon beliefs may have contributed to the finding that they were less pathological, and that high pathology ratings of Nation of Islam beliefs may be related to the subject’s general unfamiliarity with them or something about their content. It should be noted that the religious beliefs in this study were rated as symptoms of the most severe mental illnesses, potentially having serious consequences for the patient. Lastly, in another study, bias was found against conservative Evangelical Christian school applicants by a national sample of professors of clinical psychology in APA -accredited doctoral programs (Gartner, 1986). This between- 48 subjects study revealed that professors were less likely to admit applicants who were either identified as born-again, or who hoped to integrate their faith and their practice of psychology, compared to similar applicants who did not mention religion. Although the Evangelical applicant was rated higher than the integrationist applicant, the differences were not significant. Bias against psychology graduate student applicants by psychology professors cannot be generalized to bias against patients by clinicians; however, the findings of bias by psychologists against a conservative Christian group in a between-subjects study are notable as bias presumably occurring between religiously dissimilar groups. Authors also note that artificially limiting the number of religious psychologists into doctoral programs, continues to perpetuate the underrepresentation of conservative religious persons in the field. As we have seen, this has implications for those who may be unlikely to seek psychological services from secular psychologists (King, 1978). Social Desirability Another factor that may complicate the results of the religious bias research that is worth investigating in more depth is social desirability. Social norms discourage prejudice against cultural groups and group members. It is logical that since all of the studies discussed used self-report measures in evaluating groups of various religious orientations, there is a possibility that clinicians may have responded to those measures in a socially desirable manner. 49 In fact, the MODE model of biased responding suggests that the more sensitive a domain is, such as social group evaluation over evaluation of food preferences for example, the more likely responses will be influenced by social desirability effects (Fazio & Olson, 2003). The MODE model emphasizes motivation and opportunity as determinants in responding with bias. Motivation may be either internally driven, such as that which may occur when one has a set of internal standards that do not approve of stereotyping or prejudiced responding. Also, motivation may be externally driven, so that one may refrain from responding in a biased manner to avoid the disapproval of others. According to the MODE model of biased responding, if one has the motivation and the opportunity to respond without prejudice, one will likely attempt to do so. Multicultural diversity and the desirability of cultural awareness and competence are emphasized in psychological research and current academic curricula (Constantine & Ladany, 2000). Psychologists are often aware of the need for awareness, knowledge, and skills in working with culturally diverse patients. They may have knowledge of stereotype information about social groups but work to control prejudiced responding for either internal or external reasons. However, when social desirability was controlled using the Marlow Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS), one study on cultural competencies yielded little correlation between competencies measured by explicit self-report versus objective other-rated measures (Worthington et al., 2000), and no correlation was found in another study (Constantine & Ladany, 2000). 50 In addition to the focus on cultural competence in clinical work, the emphasis on empathy and patient acceptance may also contribute to socially desirable responding. The motivation to respond without empathic bias on selfreport measures may not be a reflection of responding to similar patients in a natural setting. Also, self-reported empathy may not correlate with a patient’s felt sense of empathy or empathy as perceived by others. The literature indicates that there are often significant differences in self-reported empathic responding and empathy as experienced or perceived by others (Davis & Kraus, 1997; Graham & Ickes, 1997; Ickes, Marangoni, & Garcia, 1997). As a result of these findings, one must consider the results of the religious bias literature as potentially being mitigated by social desirability. Implicit versus Explicit Cognitive Processes in Impression Formation Given the nature of the clinician’s responsibility to evaluate patients thoughtfully when forming clinical judgments, cognitive processes should naturally be engaged in efforts to form a clinical impression. The use of stereotypes in impression formation has been explained as a method of simplifying and reducing information for the purposes of efficiently managing what otherwise may be an overwhelming amount of information, which may subsequently overload cognitive processes (Allport, 1954; Erlich, 1973; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986). Some theorists believe the use of stereotypes to assist in social categorization is inevitable (Erlich, 1973; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986). Simply put, in 51 order to simplify information-processing tasks, reduce and organize the information to be evaluated in a manner that makes it manageable, and to make sense of a complex world of social information, we categorize persons into groups. When one encounters a group member or group label such as African American or Evangelical Christian, if other information is lacking, stereotypes may be utilized to assist in effectively categorizing the individual. Some theorists posit that stereotypes are still common in today’s society, despite that they are openly discouraged (Devine et al., 2002). Particularly when information may be ambiguous in the early stages of treatment, stereotype applications may be more frequently utilized and may have more of an impact on first impressions, and subsequently on treatment outcomes. Impression formation utilizes both explicit and implicit cognitive processes. In particular, stereotype formation, maintenance, and behavior resulting from stereotype attitudes and beliefs, is often the result of a complex combination of motivational, cognitive, and sociocultural processes. The research on bias with religious patients has made use of self-report measures to investigate bias in empathy, pathology, prognosis, and patient maturity among other variables. The explicit self-report measures used offer research respondents the opportunity to reflect and react to questions, potentially allowing censorship of those responses for a variety of reasons. These include externally motivated social desirability effects, or those based on a set of internal standards that rejects stereotyped or biased responding. It may be assumed to some degree that if a clinician utilizes a personal set of internal standards that discourage biased responding to research 52 queries, those same standards will tend to discourage biased responding in a natural setting. However, if social desirability is one’s primary motivation to respond in an unbiased manner, that motivation is not likely to motivate unbiased responding in natural settings where one’s responses will not be judged by others. Other measures have been helpful in capturing attitudes and beliefs about social groups or group members, without allowing the subject the opportunity to censor responses. Implicit measures purport to capture attitudes outside of one’s awareness, or in a manner that does not require, or indeed may prohibit, introspection that may have censoring or reacting elements. Whether or not the subject has an awareness of having the relevant attitude, the detection of automatic attitudes compared to those endorsed in an explicit manner, has shown promise in alleviating methodological difficulties in capturing biased responding without social desirability confounds. Explicit self-report measures and implicit measures of attitude activation often exhibit low correlations in the stereotyping and prejudice literature (Devine et al., 2002; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Rudman, Greenwald, Mellott, & Schwartz, 1999), although not always (see Fazio & Olson, 2003 for review). Implicit processes have been explored in several domains including religion (Rudman et al., 1999), aggression (Berkowitz & LePage, 1967), sexism (McKenzie-Mohr & Zanna, 1990), and race (Dovidio, Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998; Sinclair & Kunda, 1999). 53 Automatic activation of attitudes has been seen in priming experiments in which attitudes have been detected following priming with some attitude object or word (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Berkowitz & LePage, 1967; Dovidio et al., 1997). For instance, developmental psychologist Leonard Berkowitz found that the presentation of an aggression-provoking cue such as a rifle, elicited aggressive responses (Berkowitz & LePage, 1967). In another study, females were rated by respondents as more dependent than males for the same behaviors following dependence but not neutral primes, and males were rated more aggressive than females following aggression primes but not neutral primes (Banaji et al., 1993). Activation of automatic attitudes has also been seen in word fragment completion tests (Dovidio et al., 1997; Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Hense, Penner, & Nelson, 1995). In one study utilizing word fragment completion tasks, Gilbert and Hixon (1991) demonstrated the utilization of stereotypes as a cognitive resource tool. In this study, in experiment 1, an Asian research confederate elicited stereotypic completions of word fragments when subjects were cognitively occupied, but not when they were not busy. In experiment 2, when stereotype activation occurred, busy subjects were more likely than not busy subjects to apply the activated stereotypes. Other research examines the tendency to explain stereotype incongruent information more often than stereotype consistent information (Sekaquaptewa, Espinoza, Thompson, Vargas, & von Hippel, 2003), ,and tendencies to attribute responsibility to a stereotype target’s internal process 54 rather than to his or her external situation (Sekaquaptewa et al., 2003; Sherman et al., 2005). Automatic attitudes have also been demonstrated to have predictive validity on behavior. In one experiment (Dovidio et al., 1997), Caucasians high in implicit prejudice had greater indications of anxiety when interacting with an African American partner than with another Caucasian partner. In other research, (Sherman, Mackie, & Driscoll, 1990), subjects’ evaluations and preferences for targets were predicted by passively activated categories of prime-relevant versus prime-irrelevant dimensions. Exposure to pornography in another study predicted ability to recall physical characteristics and sexual motivation toward a female experimenter (McKenzie-Mohr & Zanna, 1990). In other research, higher levels of prejudice predicted more attention to processing stereotype inconsistent, compared to stereotype consistent information (J. W. Sherman et al., 2005). A well known measure of implicit stereotyping that has received attention in the literature is the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998). The IAT measures the strength of associations between target groups or members of target groups and stereotype congruent or incongruent words or concepts. The strength of the association is measured in response time. The theory behind the IAT is that it is easier to select words or concepts that are highly associated with a target, rather than selecting words or concepts that are not associated with a target. Therefore reaction times will be faster when categories are matched with associated words or concepts. 55 For example, associations are made between a target group such as African Americans and clearly valenced words (e.g., poison, flower) and stereotype congruent or incongruent characterizations (e.g., wealth, welfare). Categorizations are designated by one key stroke for one group assignment and another key stroke for another group assignment. The latency in responding to instructions to categorize valenced words or concepts with either target groups or other groups using computer key strokes represents the strength of the implicit associations held by the subject. For instance, the categorization of Black stereotype congruent names such as “Latoya” and White stereotype congruent names such as “Cathy” is practiced. Then the categorization of words with a clear pleasant or unpleasant valence such as “flower” or “poison” is practiced. Following these practice categorizations, combination of the valenced words and race related concepts (or names in this case) are assigned to target categories. Individually presented Black names and pleasant words are assigned to the “Black/pleasant” category, and White names and unpleasant words are put into the “White/unpleasant” category. Then the category combinations are switched so that Black names and unpleasant words are assigned to the “Black/unpleasant” group, and White names and pleasant words are assigned to the “White/pleasant” group. Latency of responding to each categorization for each grouping combination is measured in milleseconds. In a race study utilizing these methods, (Greenwald et al., 1998), when Black names were paired with unpleasant words, response time was significantly faster than when they were 56 paired with pleasant words, indicating a negative implicit association with Black names. The IAT as a measure of implicit associations has been criticized by some (Brendl, Markman, & Messner, 2001; Karpinski & Hilton, 2001). There is some speculation that shifts in response patterns may indicate that learned response patterns are facilitated in difficult trial blocks. Also, critics claim that the tendency to categorize familiar words faster than nonwords suggests that other factors may contribute to previous findings interpreted as implicit prejudice. Conclusions drawn by authors from experiments on nonsocial group responding as predictive of behavior caused them to question the validity of the IAT as a measure of implicit responding to social groups (Karpinski & Hilton, 2001). In Karpinski and Hilton’s study (2001), candy bar and apple associations using the IAT did not correlate with participant behavior when given the choice of selecting either a candy bar or an apple to eat. Authors assert that previous results about social group associations that were interpreted as prejudiced or stereotyped responding may have been premature. They posit that the associations demonstrated may have been a reflection of environmental exposure rather than prejudices against a target group. However, Karpinkis’ own results may be inconsistent with other IAT research due to the nature of his groups. Apples and candy bars may have varying valences depending on whether health or taste is more salient to the participant, and which dimension is more salient at the time the participant is confronted with a choice of the food rather than the category designation. Research results with groups such as words versus nonwords and apples versus 57 candy bars are not consistent with research that examines responses to social groups. The results and causal inferences of one cannot be extrapolated to the other. The IAT consistently demonstrated in- and out-group biases of social groups (Greenwald et al., 1998; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Rudman et al., 1999), and social group bias has even been found in minimal paradigm research (Ashburn-Nardo, Voils, & Monteith, 2001), when group assignment is random and participants have no previous environmental association with the target group. However, the possibility that the automatic activation of attitudes about social groups may be environmentally learned is one that cannot be lightly dismissed. General research on social groups suggests that stereotypic associations of social groups are indeed often well learned, as is evident in the ability to effectively use stereotype “congruent” or “incongruent” concepts. However, even though there is evidence for predictive validity of implicit measures, not all associations result in prejudiced responding. Further explanation may lie within motivational processes as moderators of automatic associations. The MODE model of prejudice (Fazio, 1990) specifically posits that both explicit and implicit processes of responding contribute to attitudes, judgments, and behaviors. Those processes include the automatic activation of attitudes and the motivation and opportunity to respond to those attitudes with deliberation. Similarly, Devine (1989) asserts that both automatic activation of attitudes and conscious decision making about whether to judge a target and act on those 58 attitudes, contribute to response patterns to social groups. These two-stage models of prejudice are a less deterministic perspective of the meaning and impact of implicit attitudes, although they acknowledge the socialization effects of stereotype information. Devine posits that what may be activated and captured by measures such as the IAT are knowledge structures or schema of common stereotype data, rather than internalized attitudes about a group. She explains that in addition to one’s knowledge of stereotype information, one has the ability to choose to act on that knowledge or not. Devine’s two-stage model suggests that attitudes captured by the IAT may not have predictive validity because they do not represent internalized prejudices, but only stereotype knowledge and that one has the motivation to respond without prejudice to that knowledge. Plant and Devine (1998, p. 1) address “the presence of the rather pervasive external social pressure to respond without prejudice [that] has created enduring dilemmas for both social perceivers and social scientists as they try to discern the motivation(s) underlying (generally socially acceptable) nonprejudiced responses” by exploring the importance of motivations to respond without prejudice. Similarly, Dunton and Fazio’s Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Scale (MCPRS; 1997) is concerned with one’s motivation to control prejudiced responding. As Plant and Devine point out, social perceivers may experience a dilemma in choosing between stereotype knowledge and motivations to respond without prejudice. Social perceivers will likely be differentially motivated to respond to prejudice or stereotype data. The MCPRS measures the amount of 59 motivation to control prejudiced reactions using a two-factor solution. Factor 1 consists of concern with acting prejudiced due to an internal set of standards in which prejudice is found distasteful or unacceptable, and concern about how one may be perceived by others for acting in a prejudiced manner. Factor 2 measures tendencies to restrain oneself from expressing prejudice due to the possibility of confrontation with or about targets of prejudice. Motivation to control prejudiced reactions can be differentiated from social desirability measures such as the MCDS (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). The MCDS measures attempts to respond to self-report measures in a socially acceptable manner, whereas motivation to control prejudice measures assess origins of motivations to respond to situations without prejudice (Plant & Devine, 1998) or the amount of that motivation (Dunton & Fazio, 1997). The socialization effects of religious stereotype congruent associations may or may not be ubiquitous in the United States, although common knowledge of their nature dictates that they are indeed common. The activation of such attitudes may be the result of environmentally learned associations and/or internalized prejudices. An interesting note when considering whether IAT results reflect stereotype schema or internalized prejudices is that one would expect that group members would also demonstrate those associations with their own groups, if the associations only represented environmentally learned data, since socialization probably exposes them to stereotypes about their own group. However, implicit measures consistently demonstrate bias, or knowledge of stereotype data, based on in- and out- group membership. If one has automatic 60 associations about a group, it is assumed that one may seek to control the effect of those associations. Future research that purports to examine the predictive validity of automatic associations on behavior may also benefit from examining the role of motivation to control prejudice in moderating those attitudes. As applied to research on clinician bias with religious patients, motivational processes may moderate any automatic negative associations with religious persons, so that associations do not dictate the evaluators’ clinical judgment. Summary There is a schism in the scientific worldview and the religious one. Psychologists differ statistically from the general U.S. public on measures of religiosity, religious affiliation, and the importance of religion in their lives. There are clear sociopolitical trends toward liberal worldviews in the psychological community. Psychological publications have published disparaging comments about religious persons and their mental health. Similar commentary about racial or ethnic minorities is not likely to be given the same consideration, giving validation to the hypothesis that the antireligious views held by some researchers and prominent theorists, may be shared by others in the psychological community. Assertions have been made and published that psychologist value judgments are, and should be, more carefully considered and more adequate than those of the general public. 61 The religiosity gap and the general focus on multiculturalism and cultural competency has been the impetus for some research on potential bias with religious persons. Unfortunately, that research is scant relative to work in other cultural domains, and has often had methodological difficulties that did not adequately address transparency or social desirability confounds. Research that yielded positive results of bias often utilized a national population and a withinsubjects design, which is likely to be more impervious to those concerns. That positive results of bias were found in any studies that utilized self-report measures in a society in which bias is discouraged, should be viewed as significant. Also, when social desirability is controlled, results are more likely to be an accurate reflection of the bias that exists in a sample than when it is not controlled. Social desirability has been effectively controlled in research using psychologist samples, a group likely to be savvy to the purposes of such measures. Nevertheless, religious bias studies may benefit from the use of implicit measures of stereotype activation. The stereotyping literature emphasizes implicit measures in which response patterns are not likely to be affected by censoring or reactive elements. Given that some attitudes automatically activated may be environmentally learned but not acted upon, predictions about clinical judgments should not be inferred from such results alone. It is hoped that in a psychologist population, motivation to control prejudiced reactions may moderate the effect of any automatically activated attitudes about religious persons that do exist. 62 References – Part A Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Allport, G. W., & Ross, M. J. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5(4), 432-443. Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113-133. The American Enterprise Institute. (1991). Politics of the Professoriate. The American enterprise. 2(4), 86-87. Author American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. (2000). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders third revision. Washington, DC: Author. APA. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57(12), 1060-1073. APA. (2007). Psychology of Religion (APA Div. 36 website). Retrieved January 5, 2007, from http://www.apa.org/divisions/div36/ Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Monteith, M. J. (2001). Implicit associations as the seeds of intergroup bias: How easily do they take root? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 789-799. 63 Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (1995). Implicit gender stereotyping in judgments of fame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 181-198. Banaji, M. R., Hardin, C., & Rothman, A. J. (1993). Implicit stereotyping in person judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 272-281. Barrett-Lennard, G. (1981). The empathy cycle: Refinement of a nuclear concept. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 28(2), 91-100 Batson, D. C. (1976). Religion as prosocial: Agent or double agent? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 15(1), 29-45. Bergin, A. (1980). Psychotherapy and religious values. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48(1), 95-105. Bergin, A., & Jensen, J. P. (1990). Religiosity of psychotherapists: A national survey. Psychotherapy, 27(1), 3-7. Bergin, A. E. (1983). Religiosity and mental health: A critical reevaluation and meta-analysis. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 14(2), 170-184. Bergin, A. E., Masters, K. S., & Richards, S. P. (1987). Religiousness and mental health reconsidered: A study of an intrinsically religious sample. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 34(2), 197-204. Berkowitz, L., & LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7(2), 202-207. Beutler, L.E., Crago, M., & Arizmendi, T. G. (1986). Therapist variables in psychotherapy process and outcome. In S. L. Garfield & A. Bergin (Eds.), 64 Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 257-310). New York: Wiley & Sons. Beutler, L. E., & Bergan, J. (1991). Value change in counseling and psychotherapy: A search for scientific credibility. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38(1), 16-24. Biernat, M., & Korbrynowicz, D. (2003). Stereotypes and shifting standards: Some paradoxical effects of cognitive load. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(10), 2060-2079. Bilgrave, D. P., & Deluty, R. H. (1998). Religious beliefs and therapeutic orientations of clinical and counseling psychologists. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37(2), 329-349. Bodenhausen, G. V., & Lichtenstein, M. (1987). Social stereotypes and information-processing strategies: The impact of task complexity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(5), 871-880. Brand, J. L. (2002). Can pluralism be intolerant? American Psychologist, 57(4), 299-300. Brawer, P. A., Handal, P. J., Fabricatore, A. N., Roberts, R., & Wajda-Johnston, V. A. (2002). Training and education in religion/spirituality within APAaccredited clinical psychology programs. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 33(2), 203-206. Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the Implicit 65 Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 760-773. Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this "we"? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 83-93. Brickman, P., & Bulman, R. J. (1977). Pleasure and pain in social comparison. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 149-186). Washington DC: Hemisphere. Brown, R. D. (1970). Experienced and inexperienced counselors' first impressions of client and case outcomes: Are first impressions lasting? Journal of Counseling Psychology, 17(6), 550-558. Burkard, A. W., & Knox, S. (2004). Effect of therapist color-blindness on empathy and attributions in cross-cultural counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(4), 387-397. Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press. Cheatham, H. E., Ivey, A. E., Ivey, M. B., & Simek-Morgan, L. (1980). Multicultural counseling and therapy: Changing the foundations of the field. In A. E. Ivey, M. B. Ivey, & L. Simek-Morgan (Eds.), Counseling and psychotherapy: A multicultural perspective (3rd ed., p. 94). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Chen, F. F., & Kenrick, D. T. (2002). Repulsion or attraction? Group membership and assumed attitude similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 111-125. 66 Constantine, M. G. (2001). Multicultural training, theoretical orientation, empathy, and multicultural case conceptualization ability in counselors. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 23(4), 357-372. Constantine, M. G., & Ladany, N. (2000). Self-report multicultural counseling competence scales: Their relation to social desirability attitudes and multicultural case conceptualization ability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(2), 155. Crawford, M., T., & Skowronski, J. J. (1998). When motivated thought leads to heightened bias: High need for cognition can enhance the impact of stereotypes on memory. Society for Personality and Social Psychology Inc, 24(10), 1075-1088. Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. (1990). Collective self-esteem and ingroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 60-67. Crocker, J., McGraw, K., M., Thompson, L. L., & Ingerman, C. (1987). Downward comparison, prejudice, and evaluation of others: Effects of self esteem and threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(5), 907-916. Crocker, J., & Schwartz, J. M. (1985). Prejudice and ingroup favoritism in a minimal intergroup situation: Effects on self esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 379-386. Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354. 67 Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-126. Davis, M. H. (1994). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark. Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1997). Personality and empathic accuracy. In Empathic accuracy (pp. 145-167). New York: Guilford Press. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5-18. Devine, P., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(5), 835-848. Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510-540. Duan, H., & Hill, C., (1996). The current state of empathy research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 43(3), 261-274 Duckitt, J., Birum, I., Wagner, C., & Plessis, I. d. (2002). The psychological bases of ideology and prejudice: Testing a dual process model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 75-93. 68 Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudice reactions. Society for Personality and Social Psychology Inc, 23(3), 316-326. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., O'Boyle, C., Suh, K. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 776-797 Eisenberg, N., & Lennen, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 100-131 Ellens, J. H., & Sanders, R. K. (2006). Fifty years of CAPS: The director's perspectives. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 25(4), 311-321. Ellis, A. (1980a). The case against religion: A psychotherapist's view and the case against religiosity. Austin, TX: American Atheist Press. Ellis, A. (1980b). Psychotherapy and atheistic values: A response to A.E.Bergin's "Psychotherapy and Religious Values." Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48(5), 635-639. Ellis, A. (2000). Can rational-emotive behavior therapy (REBT) be effectively used with people who have devout beliefs in God and religion? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 31(1), 29-33. Erlich, H. J. (1973). The social psychology of prejudice. New York: Wiley. Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitude guides behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), 69 Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 75-109). New York: Academic Press. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297327. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-140. Ford, M. P., & Hendrick, S. (2003). Therapists' sexual values for self and clients: Implications for practice and training. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34(1), 80-87. Francis, L. J., & Peter, K. (2002). The relationship between psychological wellbeing and Christian faith and practice in an Australian population sample. Journal for the Scientific study of religion, 41(1), 179-184. Freud, S. (1962). The question of Weltanschauung. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (pp. 158-162). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1933). Freud, S. (2000). Totem and taboo. In Totem and taboo: Resemblances between the psychic lives of savages and neurotics. (A. A. Brill, Trans.). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. (Original work published 1913). Fuertes, J. N., & Brobst, K. (2002). Client's ratings of counselor multicultural competency. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8(3), 214223. 70 Gaertner, L., & Insko, C. A. (2000). Intergroup discrimination in the minimal group paradigm: Categorization, reciprocation, or fear? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(1), 77-94. The Gallup poll: Public opinion in 1996. (1996). Wilmington, DE: Scholarly Resources. The Gallup Poll. (2005). Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://poll.gallup.com/content/default.aspx?ci=16519 The Gallup Poll. (2006). Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://brain.gallup.com/documents/question.aspx?question=72715&Advan ced=0&SearchConType=1&SearchTypeAll=Universal%20spirit Gartner, J. (1986). Antireligious prejudice in admissions to doctoral programs in clinical psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 17(5), 473-475. Gartner, J. (1990). The effect of client and counselor values on clinical judgment. Counseling & Values, 35(1), 58-63. Gartner, J., Hohmann, A., Harmatz, M., & Larson, D. (1990). The effect of patient and clinician ideology on clinical judgment: A study of ideological countertransference. Psychotherapy, 27, 98-106. Gartner, J., Larson, D. B., & Allen, G. D. (1991). Religious commitment and mental health: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 19(1), 6-25. 71 Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 509-517. Gorsuch, R. L., & Aleshire, D. (1974). Christian faith and ethnic prejudice: A review and interpretation of research. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 13(3), 281-307. The Graduate Center. (2001). American Religious Identification Survey. Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://ww.gc.cuny.edu/faculty/research_briefs/aris/key_findings.htm Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women's intuition isn't greater than men's. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy. New York: Guilford Press. Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, selfesteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4-27. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464-1480. Guimond, S., Dambrun, M., Michinov, N., & Duarte, S. (2003). Does social dominance generate prejudice: Integrating individual and contextual determinants of intergroup cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 697-721. Guinee, J., & Tracey, T. (1997). Effects of religiosity and problem type on counselor description rating. Journal of Counseling and Development, 76(1), 65-73. 72 Hamilton, D. L., Sherman, J. W., & Ruvolo, C. M. (1990). Stereotyped-based expectancies: Effects on information processing and social behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 46(2), 35-60. Hamilton, D. L., & Trolier, T. K. (1986). Stereotypes and stereotyping: An overview of the cognitive approach. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 127-163). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Hansen, N. D., Randazzo, K. V., Schwartz, A., Marshall, M., Kalis, D., Frazier, R., et al. (2006). Do we practice what we preach: An exploratory survey of multicultural psychotherapy competencies. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 37(1), 66-74. Hense, R. L., Penner, L. A., & Nelson, D. L. (1995). Implicit memory for age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 13(4), 339-415. Hill, P. C., Pargament, K. I., Hood, R. W., McCullough, M. E., Swyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., et al. (2000). Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Points of commonality, points of departure. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 30, 51-77. Hillowe, B. V. (1986). Effects of religiosity of therapist and patient on clinical judgment (Paper). 94th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Hilton, J. L., & Hippel, W. v. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 237-271. 73 Holcomb-McCoy, C. C., & Myers, J. E. (1999). Multicultural competence and counselor training: A national survey. Journal of Counseling and Development, 77(3), 294-303. Houston, D. A. (1990). Empathy and the self: Cognitive and emotional influences on the evaluation of negative affect in others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5), (859-868) Houts, A. C., & Graham, K. (1986). Can religion make you crazy? Impact of client and therapist religious values on clinical judgments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(2), 267-271. Ickes, W., Marangoni, C., & Garcia, S. (1997). Studying empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant context. New York: Guilford Press. Ivey, A. E., Ivey, M. B., & Simek-Morgan, L. (Eds.). (1993). Counseling and psychotherapy: A multicultural perspective. Needham Heights, MA: Simon & Shuster. Jackson, L. A., & Sullivan, L. A. (2001). Cognition and affect in evaluations of stereotyped group members. The Journal of Social Psychology, 129(5), 659-672. Johnson, B. W., Nielson, S. L., & Ridley, C. R. (2000). Religiously sensitive rational emotive behavior therapy: Elegant solutions and ethical risks. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 31(1), 14-20. Jussim, L., Manis, M., Nelson, T. E., & Soffin, S. (1995). Prejudice, stereotypes, and labeling effects: Sources of bias in person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 228-246. 74 Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 774-788. Kelly, T. A. (1990). The role of values in psychotherapy: Review and methodological critique. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 171-186. Kerlinger, F. N. (1984). Liberalism and conservatism: The nature and structure of social attitudes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Elrbaum Associates. Khan, J. A., & Cross, D. G. (1983). Mental health professional and client values: Similar or different? Australian Journal of Sex, Marriage and Family, 4, 7178. Kilpatrick, S. D., & McCullough, M. E. (1999). Religion and spirituality in rehabilitation psychology. Rehabilitation Psychology, 44(4), 388-402. King, R. (1978). Evangelical Christians and professional counseling: A conflict of values? Journal of Psychology and Theology, 6, 276-281. Koenig, F. W., & King, M. B. Jr. (1964). Cognitive simplicity and outgroup stereotypes. Social Forces, 42(3), 324-327. Koenig, F. W. (1997). Is religion good for your health? The effects of religion on physical and mental health. New York: Haworth Press. Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press Korten, F. F. (1973). The stereotype as a cognitive construct. The Journal of Social Psychology, 90, 29-39. Lewis, C. S. (Ed.). (2001). Mere Christianity . San Francisco: Harper. 75 Lewis-Hall, E., Gorsuch, R., Malony, H. N., Narramore, S. B., & Stewart Van Leeuwam, M. (2006). Dialogue, embodiment, and the unity of faith and learning: A conversation on integration in a postmodern age. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 25(4), 331-338. Lewis, K. N., & Lewis, D. A. (1985). Impact of religious affiliation on therapists' judgments of patients. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 53(6), 926-932. Macrae, N. C., & Milne, A. B. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 37-47. McClintock, C. G., Spaulding, C. B., & Turner, H. A. (1965). Political orientations of academically affiliated psychologists. American Psychologist, 20, 211221. McKenzie-Mohr, D., & Zanna, M. P. (1990). Treating women as sexual objects: Look to the (gender schematic) male who has viewed pornography. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 296-308. Mehrabian, A., Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40(4), 525-543 Meyer, M. S. (1988). Ethical principles of psychologists and religious diversity. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 19(5), 486-488. Murray, E. J. (1956). A content-analysis method for studying psychotherapy. Psychological Monographs, 70(13). 76 Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. NewPort, F. (2004). A look at Americans and religion today. Retrieved March 28, 2006, from http://speakingoffaith.publicradio.org/programs/godsofbusiness/galluppoll. shtml Newport, F., & Carroll, J. (2005). Another look at Evangelicals in America today. Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://poll.gallup.com/content/default.aspx?ci=20242&pg=1 Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101-115. O'Connor, S., & Vandenberg, B. (2005). Psychosis or faith? Clinicians' assessment of religious beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(4), 610-616. Pargament, K. I., Smith, B. W., Koenig, H. G., & Perez, L. (1998). Patterns of positive and negative religious coping with major life stressors. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37(4), 710-724. Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 811-832. 77 Ragan, C., Malony, H. N., & Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1980). Psychologists and religion: Professional factors and personal belief. Review of Religious Research, 21, 208-127. Redding, R. E. (2001). Sociopolitical diversity in psychology: The case for pluralism. American Psychologist, 56(3), 205-215. Redding, R. E. (2002). Grappling with a diverse conception of diversity. American Psychologist, 57(4), 300-301. Reed, P. R. (1992). Psychologists' views of a patient's religiousness. Psychological Reports, 70, 1031-1036. Richards, S. P., & Bergin, A. (2000). Handbook of psychotherapy and religious diversity. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Richards, S. P., & Davison, M. L. (1992). Religious bias in moral development research: A psychometric investigation. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 31(4), 467-485. Ridley, C. R., & Lingle, D. W. (Eds.). (1996). Cultural empathy in multicultural counseling: A multidimensional process model (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rippentrop, A. E. (2005). A review of the role of religion and spirituality in chronic pain populations. Rehabilitation Psychology, 50(3), 278-284. Rogers, C. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95-103. Rogers, C. (1980). A way of being. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 78 Rosenbaum, M. E. (1986). The repulsion hypothesis: On the nondevelopment of relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 11561166. Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 17(4), 437465. Sarason, S. (1993) American psychology, and the needs for transcendence and community. American Journal of Community Psychology, 21(2). Sekaquaptewa, D., Espinoza, P., Thompson, M., Vargas, P., & von Hippel, W. (2003). Stereotypic explanatory bias: Implicit stereotyping as a predictor of discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 75-82. Seyle, D. C., & Newman, M. L. (2006). A house divided?: The psychology of Red and Blue America. American Psychologist, 61(6), 571-580. Shafranske, E. (1990). Clinical psychologists' religious and spiritual orientations and their practice of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 27, 72-78. Shafranske, E. (2000). Religious involvement and professional practices of psychiatrists and other mental health professionals. Psychiatric Annals, 30(8), 525-532. Shafranske, E., & Gorsuch, R. (1984). Factors associated with the perception of spirituality in psychotherapy. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 16(2), 231-241. 79 Shafranske, E., & Malony, N. H. (1990). California psychologists' religiosity and psychotherapy. Journal of Religion and Health, 29(3), 219-232. Shaheen, J., McLaughlin, C., King, D., Volpe, J. D., Perez, E., Phillips, J., et al. (2006). Redefining political attitudes and activism: A poll by Harvard's Institute of Politics. Retrieved August 12, 2006, from http://www.iop.harvard.edu/pdfs/survey/spring_poll_2006_execsumm.pdf Sherman, J. W., Conrey, F. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Azam, O. A. (2005). Prejudice and stereotype maintenance processes: Attention, attribution, and individuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 607-622. Sherman, S. J., Mackie, D. M., & Driscoll, D. M. (1990). Priming and the differential use of dimensions in evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(3), 405-418. Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (1999). Reactions to a Black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 885-904. Smith, T. B., McCullough, M. E., & Poll, J. (2003). Religiousness and depression: Evidence for a main effect and the moderating influence of stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 614-636. Smith, D. P., & Orlinsky, D. E. (2004). Religious and spiritual experience among psychotherapists. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 41(2), 144-151. 80 Stone, W. F. (1994). Conservatism/liberalism. In encyclopedia of human behavior (p. 701). San Diego, CA: Academic Press Strayer, J. (1990). Affective and cognitive perspectives on empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 218229). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sue, S. (1998). In search of cultural competence in psychotherapy and counseling. American Psychologist, 53(4), 440-448. Tajfel, H., Turner, J.C., In S (1986). The social identity theory of inter-group behavior. In S. Worchel & L.W. Austin (Eds.) Psychology of inter-group relations. Chicago: Nelson-Hall Taylor, S. E, & Lobel, M. (1989). Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts. Psychological Review, 96(4), 569-575. Truax, C. B. (1966). Reinforcement and nonreinforcement in Rogerian psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71, 1-9. Viney, W., & King, D. B. (1998). A history of psychology: Ideas and content (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Wadsworth, R. D., & Checketts, K. T. (1980). Influence of religious affiliation on psychodiagnosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 234240. Wallis, J. (2005). God's politics: Why the right gets it wrong and the left doesn't get it. San Francisco: Harper Collins. 81 Walls, G. B. (1980). Values and psychotherapy: A comment on "Psychotherapy and religious values." Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48(5), 640-641. Watson, P. J., Sawyers, P., Morris, R. J., Carpenter, M. L., Jimenez, R. S., Jonas, K. A., et al. (2003). Reanalysis within a Christian ideological surround: Relationships of intrinsic religious orientation with fundamentalism and right wing authoritarianism. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 31(4), 315-328. Watters, W. W. (1992). Deadly doctrine: Health, illness, and Christian God talk. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books. Wester, S. R., & Vogel, D. L. (2002). The emperor's new clothes: Sociopolitical diversity in psychology. American Psychologist, 57(4), 295. Winseman, A. (2005). Religion in America: Who has none? Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://poll.gallup.com/content/default.aspx?ci=20329 Worthington, E. L. (1988). Understanding the values of religious clients: A model and its application to counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35(2), 166-174. Worthington, E. L. (1996). Empirical research on religion and psychotherapeutic processes and outcomes: A 10-year review and research prospectus. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 448-487. Worthington, E. L., & Sandage, S. (2001). Religion and spirituality. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 38(4), 473-478. 82 Worthington, R., Mobley, M., Franks, R. P., & Andreas Tan, J. (2000). Multicultural counseling competencies: Verbal content, counselor attributions, and social desirability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(4), 460-468. Yangarber-Hicks, N., Behenskyh, C., Canning, S., Flanagan, K., Gibson, N., Hicks, M., et al. (2006). Invitation to the table conversation: A few diverse perspectives on integration. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 25(4), 338-353. Yarhouse, M. A., & Fisher, W. (2002). Levels of training to address religion in clinical practice. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 39(2), 171-176. Yarhouse, M. A., & VanOrman, B. T. (1999). When psychologists work with religious clients: Applications of general principles of ethical conduct. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 30(6), 557-562. Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Coull, A. (1999). Fencing off the deviant: The role of cognitive resources in the maintenance of stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 449-462. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 2. Zeiger, M., & Lewis, J. E. (1998). The spiritually responsible therapist: Religious material in the psychotherapeutic setting. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 35(3), 415-424. 83 CLINICIAN RELIGIOSITY AND RESPONSE TO DIVERGENT PATIENT RELIGIOSITY: AN INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECTS OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT STEREOTYPING ON EMPATHY AND PROGNOSIS IN INITIAL RESPONDING TO PATIENTS WHO ARE RELIGIOUSLY DIVERSE FROM PSYCHOLOGISTS PART B 84 Introduction Multiculturalism has been called the fourth force in psychology by some (Cheatham, Ivey, Ivey, & Simek-Morgan, 1980) and has been treated with commensurate significance in the APA’s Code of Ethics (APA, 2002). Further, the Code of Ethics bans discrimination against multicultural groups in Standard 3.01 and addresses competence in Standard 2.01 which stipulates that an understanding of factors associated with age, gender, gender identity, race, ethnicity, culture, national origin, religion, sexual orientation, disability, language, or socioeconomic status is essential for effective implementation of their services or research, psychologists have or obtain the training, experience, consultation, or supervision necessary to ensure the competence of their services, or they make appropriate referrals (APA, 2002, pp. 1063-1064) An investigation into clinician approaches to religious patients is warranted for several reasons. These include that the addition of religion as a group addressed by APA multicultural mandates is fairly recent, that there is a history of controversial relationship between science and religion, and that there is evidence of divergent religious values between patients and clinicians. Further, the current but scant research in this area yields confusing and sometimes seemingly contradictory results. 85 Religiosity as a Diversity Variable in Clinical Psychology Psychology has its roots in philosophy and may therefore be considered a close relative to theology when considered from an epistemological perspective. However, psychology has had to elbow its way into respectable standing among the hard sciences which may have caused a deliberate and excessive distance from this realm of human experience. For example, early psychologists struggled with issues concerning the selection of methodologies and methodological purity as psychology continued to develop as a discipline within the sciences, even such that “the very scientific status of psychology hinges, from some points of view, on methodological purity” (Viney & King, 1998, p. 24), a paradigm which has been inconsistent with that which frames much of religious thought. The schism between the two is often evident in the scientific literature. For instance, the National Academy of Sciences (1984, p. 6) claims that science and religion are “separate and mutually exclusive realms of human thought”, while other positions (Jones, 1994) emphasize that there are similarities, specifically with ways of knowing and attempts to structure understanding of a complex existence. At the least, it is safe to assume that within the practice of clinical psychology, patients bring subjective worldviews, beliefs, and values into the therapeutic environment, many of which are informed by their religious orientations. 86 The “Religiosity Gap” Inasmuch as there are invigorating arguments on either side about the essence or compatibility of the scientific and religious realms, the fact remains that a large portion of the population holds, and is influenced by, religious views. In fact, in the United States an estimated 94% of the population believes in “God or some universal spirit” (Gallup, 1996). The general population has also consistently endorsed religion as either “very important” or “fairly important” in their lives, with 85% endorsement of these items in 1996 (Gallup) and 84% in 2006 (Gallup). Seventy-six percent of the population endorsed Judeo-Christian religious affiliations in the categories Protestant, Catholic, Jewish, Orthodox (1% of total including both Jews and Christians) and Mormon, with 49% of those endorsing Protestant and 23% endorsing Catholic affiliations. In addition to the forced Judeo-Christian categories, 11% percent endorsed “other” (which may or may not consist of other Judeo-Christian affiliations), 11% selected the category “none”, and 1% were “undesignated” (Gallup, 2006). Other research (Hill et al., 2000) indicates that 90% of Americans pray, 71% belong to a church or synagogue, and 42% attend religious services weekly. Psychologists have typically had lower rates of traditional religious affiliation than the general population (Shafranske, 2000), lower rates than other mental health professionals including social workers, psychiatrists, and marriage and family therapists (Bergin & Jensen, 1990), and lower rates than other professionals in the natural sciences in general (Long, 1971). In one study 87 (Ragan, Malony, & Beit-Hallahmi, 1980) of 522 psychologists (a 2% random sample of the APA with a 67% response rate of usable questionnaires), 43% of members endorsed belief in some deity, and 34% denied the existence of God. Bilgrave and Deluty (1998) examined the beliefs of a sample of 237 psychologists (a 51% return rate of usable questionnaires) that included 56% clinical- and 44% counseling- psychologists drawn from selected divisions of the APA. They found that 66% of participants in this sample endorsed beliefs which included “God or a Universal Spirit” compared with 94% of the general population (Gallup, 1996), and 43% endorsed Judeo-Christian affiliations compared to the aforementioned estimate of 76% of the general population (Gallup, 2006). Other affiliations endorsed in the study were 15% “other,” 12% agnostic, 8% Eastern, and 6% atheist (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998). The divergence between psychologists and the public in endorsement of religious affiliation and religious beliefs has been referred to in the literature as the “religiosity gap” (Richards & Bergin, 2000). Religion and Multicultural Competence Neglect of Religious Beliefs and Values as a Diversity Variable Religious values are distinct from the beliefs from which they arise. Consistent with definitions found elsewhere in the literature, Worthington (1996) defines religious beliefs as “propositional statements (in agreement with some 88 organized religion) that a person holds to be true concerning religion or religious spirituality”, and religious values as “superordinate organizing statements of what a person considers important” that arise from his or her preferred religious beliefs. Religion is considered a cultural group (Richards & Bergin, 2000; Shafranske, 1996; Merriam-Webster, 2003) whose members’ beliefs and values can influence their worldview sufficiently to prompt the APA to mandate competencies in working with them, as with ethnic, racial, and other multicultural groups (APA, 2002). Also consistent with directives in working with other multicultural groups, there is a need for a culturally sensitive approach to their treatment. However, current levels of training in religious diversity in clinical practice are not commensurate with what one would expect based on the religiosity of the U.S. public (Brawer, Handal, Fabricatore, Roberts, & Wajda-Johnston, 2002; Yarhouse & Fisher, 2002). One study reported that only 5% of clinical psychologists had religious professional training (Shafranske, 1990). In another study (Brawer et al., 2002), when training directors in predoctoral internship training programs responded to whether the topic of religion was covered in their training program, they reported that the topic was addressed sporadically. Also in Brawer’s study, it was found that if the topic of religion was covered in APAaccredited training programs it was largely unsystematic, or it was not covered at all. Given the findings that the majority of Americans report that religious and spiritual beliefs are important in their lives and the APA policy about 89 competencies in clinical work with religious persons, it is interesting to note that not only is there such little participation in religious diversity training programs, but also in a study that investigated values considered relevant to therapy, only 29% of clinicians consider those beliefs important in their work with patients (Bergin, 1991). The relative disinterest compared to the interest of the general population could be the result of several factors. These include deliberate avoidance of religion resulting from fear of exploring the topic in clinical work, personal bias against it, the judgment that religious beliefs are simply unrelated to the clinical needs of patients, or general neglect of the topic as a result of its relative unimportance to the psychologist’s own life. However, given the importance of religion to the general public, clinicians may indeed undervalue the impact that a religiously influenced worldview may have on a comprehensive and respectful understanding of the patient’s experience and clinical presentation. The Potential for Stereotyping and Prejudice Stereotyping, defined as the use of beliefs or expectations associated with a group or group member based on group membership, and prejudice, defined as a valenced evaluation of that group or group member (Sherman, Conrey, Stroessner, & Azam, 2005) against religious belief systems or the values consistent with a religious worldview can contribute to bias in clinical work. Bias for the purposes of this paper is operationalized as an instance of prejudice. Neglect in considering religious worldviews for any reason may result in the 90 exhibition of positive bias in the direction of secular worldviews and/or negative bias against religious worldviews in treatment. Particularly without religious multicultural training, clinicians may be unaware of how their beliefs and value systems can affect the therapeutic process, selection of treatment goals, and subtle reinforcements of shifts in the patients’ own value system. Further, if clinicians do consider religious themes important in their work with patients, it is important to know how those beliefs and values are interpreted by clinicians who may have very different worldviews. Before examining the impact of religious stereotyping or prejudice in more depth, it is helpful to have some basic knowledge of the variety of processes and variables that contribute to either. The literature on stereotyping and prejudice development and maintenance is voluminous and encompasses several processes and examines many variables (Hilton & Hippel, 1996). Primarily there are three categories of processes: sociocultural, motivational, and cognitive. Sociocultural models of stereotyping and prejudice include social comparison models (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Brickman & Bulman, 1977; Crocker, McGraw, Thompson, & Ingerman, 1987; Festinger, 1954; Taylor & Lobel, 1989), social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979), social position effects on prejudice (Guimond, Dambrun, Michinov, & Duarte, 2003), and in- and out-group similarity and dissimilarity effects (Byrne, 1971; Rosenbaum, 1986). Motivational processes examine the use of stereotype information by those who have a preference for cognition (Crawford & Skowronski, 1998), a need for increased self-esteem under threat (Crocker & Luhtanen, 1990), and use of stereotyping to 91 reduce cognitive load (Biernat & Korbrynowicz, 2003; Crawford & Skowronski, 1998; Macrae & Milne, 1994; Yzerbyt & Coull, 1999). Cognitive processes focus on information-processing strategies (Bodenhausen & Lichtenstein, 1987; Hamilton, Sherman, & Ruvolo, 1990; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986), stereotyping as a cognitive construct (Korten, 1973), and the relationship between stereotyping and cognitive simplicity (Koenig & King, 1964). The effects of both affect and cognition have been examined together in the literature. Affect has been operationalized several ways including agreement with mood-affect adjectives following evaluations of stereotype targets (Jackson & Sullivan, 2001), and “liking” for target groups (Jussim, Manis, Nelson, & Soffin, 1995). Cognition is often defined by beliefs about stereotyped targets (Jackson & Sullivan, 2001; Jussim et al., 1995). Also, the effect of induced happiness on stereotypic judgments has been explored (Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Susser, 1994). The categories in many of these studies overlap as the literature continues to more narrowly define the mechanisms by which bias occurs. Still other literature looks to motivational processes that moderate prejudice, with some emphasis on the value that many place on responding without prejudice for the purposes of maintaining interpersonal harmony and/or due to an intrapersonal value system that disapproves of prejudiced behavior (Devine, Plant, Amodio, Harmon-Jones, & Vance, 2002; Dunton & Fazio, 1997). Yet other work examines the phenomenon of rebound effects of stereotype suppression, or the tendency 92 for stronger stereotyping behaviors to follow attempts at suppressing stereotypes (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, & Jetten, 1994). The possibility of prejudice and stereotyping of culturally diverse groups in clinical work is evident in that there are policies prohibiting discrimination against those groups (see APA, 2002 Section 3.01). The social psychology literature consistently shows negative stereotyping of members of dissimilar groups (Ashburn-Nardo, Voils, & Monteith, 2001; Devine et al., 2002; Jussim et al., 1995; Sears & Rowe, 2003; Sherman et al., 2005). Byrne (1971) offers the hypothesis that the average person is less attracted to, and maybe even dislikes, people whose values, attitudes, beliefs, and opinions are different from his or her own. Research on Byrne’s repulsion paradigm (Rosenbaum, 1986) examined participants’ responses to persons with similar and dissimilar attitudes on a number of dimensions, and contrasting with controls, the study yielded significant differences in repulsion ratings for those with dissimilar attitudes. In another study (Chen & Kenrick, 2002), the repulsion hypothesis was demonstrated in three experiments after participants learned of dissimilar controversial attitude positions of others. Further, the effects of in- and out-group bias are demonstrated in minimal group paradigms, which refer to bias effects when groups differ only in label assignment (Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Gaertner & Insko, 2000). In summary, it is evident that the circumstances under which stereotyping or prejudice may occur are many. Much of the theoretical and research literature focuses on similarity and dissimilarity of groups as a precursor to stereotyping 93 and prejudice under those varied conditions, even when that difference is only implied or completely ambiguous as is the case in the minimal group paradigm. Even with clinical training that emphasizes the impact of empathy and patient acceptance as a significant part of successful clinical treatment, practitioners who value these approaches may also demonstrate biased patterns of responding. Clinicians’ biased responding to dissimilar patients can take many forms in a clinical setting. Areas in which bias effects might manifest range from trends in value convergence to decreased empathy and the selection of treatment goals which conflict with the patients’ preferred values and worldview. An examination of the forms that clinical bias may take will begin with a brief discussion of the implications of the value convergence literature. Outcomes and Value Convergence Initial similarity of clinician and patient demographic variables including SES, ethnicity, gender, and age has been shown to have an effect on relationship enhancement and even treatment outcomes (Beutler, Crago, & Arizmendi, 1986; Kim, Gladys, & Ahn, 2005). Consistent with Byrne’s theory, Beutler and Bergan (1991) posit that the role of values may have an even greater impact on treatment bias than the roles of age, ethnicity, and gender, which often do not accurately represent group members’ attitudes and values. Further, value convergence is often an indicator of counselor-perceived improved patient outcomes (Beutler & Bergan, 1991; Worthington, 1988). However, these results 94 are inconsistent (Beutler, Machado, & Neufeldt, 1994; Beutler et al., 2004), and the effect is less strong for other- and client-rated perceptions of improvement than for counselor-perceived improvements (Kelly, 1990). Nonetheless, the phenomenon that clinicians consider patients healthier when their values more closely match their own may be evidence that judgments are being made about preferable value systems. Also, the process by which value shifts occur may indicate subtle reinforcement of movement in the direction of or away from particular values. Even clinicians who intend to be nondirective may be unaware of subtle messages of approval or disapproval communicated to the patient, such as frequency and duration of eye contact and affirming or disconfirming facial expressions, gestures, and verbalizations. Repeated occurrences of discriminatory reinforcement and nonreinforcement were seen in two independent studies of Carl Rogers demonstrating Rogerian therapy, in which patterns of responding based on value preferences (Truax, 1966), and similarity of patient style of expression and other response classes (Murray, 1956), altered patient behavior. Since Rogerian therapy is well known for its emphasis on unconditional positive regard and patient-led processes, it should be recognized that even the most nondirective, accepting therapeutic stances may have embedded within, value preferences that influence treatment. Some (Ellis, 1980) have even encouraged clinicians to emphasize value stances to capitalize on value convergence research, despite guidance of the APA Code of Ethics to respect group differences (APA, 2002, Principle E). The effect of value 95 convergence is a concern for clinicians and researchers who recognize the power of the therapist as an agent of change whose influence may be better described as one of “persuasion and conversion rather than one of healing” (Beutler et al., 2004). Religion and Mental Health More overtly, clinicians may direct conscious challenges to the patient’s religious belief system or the values that arise from it, if clinicians believe it to be less desirable than their own or pathological in some way. Sigmund Freud repeatedly professed opinions that God is “nothing but an exalted father” (Freud 1913/2000, p. 256), and that all faith was at least neurotically determined (Freud, 1913/2000, pp. 174-281), ignoring healthy and nonpathological faith. More recently, Albert Ellis stated that religion is illogical and questioned what changes religious persons could make without giving up their religious beliefs, which he claimed were characterized by inflexibility and devout “shoulds, oughts, and musts,” and even saw religion as evidence of the most severe emotional disturbance (Ellis, 1980, p. 31), although he later recants some of his earlier positions (see Ellis, 2000). Other and recent antireligious views include comments made by Wendell Watters, a respected professor of psychiatry and physician at McMaster University in Ontario, Canada. In reference to Christian doctrine and teachings he stated that they are “incompatible with the development and maintenance of 96 sound health, and not only ‘mental’ health,” and that “Simply put, Christian indoctrination is a form of mental and emotional abuse” (Watters, 1992, p.10). In reference to the majority of membership in the American Psychological Association (APA), Emeritus professor of psychology at Yale University and author of over 40 books, Seymour Sarason, in his Centennial Address to the APA stated that there are more than a few psychologists who regard ingredients of a religious worldview as a “reflection of irrationality, of superstition, of an immaturity, of a neurosis,” and that “indeed if we learn someone is devoutly religious, or even tends in that direction, we look upon that person with puzzlement, often concluding that psychologist obviously had or has personal problems” (Sarason, 1993, p. 187). In the Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R), 12 references to religion in the Glossary of Technical Terms were used to demonstrate psychopathology (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). While it is noted that the latest revision of the DSM, the DSM-IV TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), now includes more culturally sensitive language, that antireligious perspectives may have influenced the clinical judgment of psychologists and psychiatrists alike, should not be easily dismissed. Indeed, there is encouraging evidence that some psychologists’ worldviews have evolved in conjunction with the demands for multiculturally appropriate perspectives as can be seen in the morphing views of Albert Ellis. In one earlier treatise on religiousness and psychotherapy Ellis states that, “If one of the requisites for emotional health is acceptance of uncertainty, then religion is 97 obviously the unhealthiest state imaginable” (Ellis, 1980a, p. 8), implying by virtue of the religious person’s extreme pathology that he or she is likely the hardest to treat. Indeed, he also stated that “the best he can do, if he wants to change any of the rules that stem from his doctrine, is to change the religion itself” (Ellis, 1980a, p. 9). However, Ellis later recants some of his earlier assertions and reports that his Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy is compatible with some religious views and can be effectively used with patients who have devout beliefs about God without changing their religion (Ellis, 2000). Nevertheless, it is difficult to imagine that such evolution in thinking about religiosity and religious persons, as encouraging as it may be, necessarily represents a sudden and ubiquitous absence of antireligious views in psychology. Certainly, this type of anti-religious thinking was common enough in the not so distant past that it was acceptable for publication in peer-reviewed journals, which one might assume have some commitment to publish culturally appropriate materials. Contrary to these opinions, two meta-analytic studies (Bergin, 1983; Gartner, Larson, & Allen, 1991) yielded no evidence and inconsistent evidence respectively, for a link between religiosity and poorer mental health. A more recent meta-analytic study on religiousness and depression (Smith, McCullough, & Poll, 2003) found a negative correlation between symptoms and religiousness. In fact, an overall trend in good mental health was found on scales that measured anxiety, personality traits, self-control, irrational beliefs, and depression in those with an intrinsic religious orientation (religion as an end itself 98 as opposed to it being used as a means to another end; Bergin, Masters, & Richards, 1987; Smith et al., 2003). Moreover, increased associations between general psychological well-being and religion were demonstrated in a Christian sample (Francis & Peter, 2002), and improved mental health was associated with those who use positive religious coping on domains including chronic pain (see Rippentrop, 2005 for review) and rehabilitation (Kilpatrick & McCullough, 1999). Indeed, some acknowledge that the perception of religious persons as irrational, inflexible, and pathological has in fact instilled a fear of psychotherapy in potential patients who are religiously oriented (Richards & Bergin, 2000). Members of traditional religious organizations may perceive psychotherapists as incapable of, or unwilling to, work with them in a manner that is respectful and sensitive to their religiousness. Religious persons have also articulated fears that secular therapists may seek to change their religious beliefs or may misunderstand them and may even not enter therapy as a result (Richards & Bergin, 2000; Worthington, 1996). If clinicians evaluate a patient’s religiousness to be an indication of pathology, they may also rate their prognosis more negatively as a reflection of his or her perceived poorer mental health. Sociopolitical Influence Lastly, multicultural training programs stress the importance of psychologists’ awareness of their sociopolitical views, and its influence on research and in practice, particularly in the delivery of culturally competent 99 services and patient satisfaction (Fuertes & Brobst, 2002; Redding, 2002; Wester & Vogel, 2002). Political parties and policy preferences have become increasingly polarized in recent years, with even the labeling of “liberal” and “conservative” groups as “red” and “blue” contributing to further divide the groups, and contributing to the risk of increased conflict between them (Seyle & Newman, 2006). These groups are very often associated with either secular or religious worldviews, whether accurately or not, and negative stereotypes and feelings about either group and their group members and presumed values have become more charged as well in recent years ( Wallis, 2005). With the current lack of participation in clinician religious multicultural training, lack of awareness of the impact of one’s sociopolitical background is a possibility. Stereotypes and affective charge associated with religious persons or groups prominent in politics may be generalized to the clinical setting when one is confronted with religiously oriented patients. Impact of Religious Neglect or Bias on Treatment Due to the religiosity gap, a history of conflict between science and religion that includes the pathologizing of religion, sociopolitical differences that may affect clinical judgment of religiously diverse patients, and the consideration of the stereotype and prejudice literature in general, biases are likely to occur. In fact, the nature of religious bias that may occur in a clinical setting has caused some to express the concern that “ethical violations may occur when therapists 100 who are religiously uninformed, insensitive, or prejudiced ‘trample on the values’ of religious clients and in so doing alienate, offend, and even harm them” (Richards & Bergin, 2000, p. 13). Religious bias can be exhibited in many ways. Selection of Treatment Goals Bias in the direction of one value system over another may result in the selection of treatment goals which may conflict with the patient’s preferred values. For example, goals may focus on themes that devalue self-control in terms of absolute values and universal ethics consistent with the patient’s religious belief system, and instead encourage self-expression in terms of relative values, or they may encourage permissiveness in sex or sex without long-term responsibilities which may also conflict with the patient’s religious values (Bergin, 1980). In either case, the psychologist may be unaware of the patient’s religious beliefs and values and commitment to live accordingly, or he or she may have prejudice against the patient’s values, seeing the patient as less desirable or even unhealthy and in need of change to values more similar to those of the clinician. However, prohibition against such bias is articulated in the APA Code of Ethics which specifically calls upon psychologists to “be aware of and respect cultural, individual and role differences” and to “try to eliminate the effect on their work of those biases” (APA, 2002, Principle E). Prejudiced or religiously uninformed treatment can also result in bias in the direction of secular interventions that ignore religious tools that may assist 101 the patient in healing. Religious patients may benefit significantly from interventions that draw on religious themes that promote effective coping or assist in the process of change and healing. These include prayer, divine forgiveness, and socialization within their religious community (Kilpatrick & McCullough, 1999; Rippentrop, 2005). Empathy Culture impacts the empathic understanding of others (Ivey, Ivey, & Simek-Morgan, 1993). The degree to which clinicians are able to empathize with their religiously diverse patients may contribute significantly to whether they are able to provide culturally competent services. As a result of affectively charged feelings about religiously diverse patients and their worldviews, or stereotypic or prejudiced expectations and evaluations about them, empathy within clinician/patient dyads may be impacted. When religious diversity training is lacking and stereotypes or prejudices go unexamined, it is conceivable that dissimilarity of religious beliefs and values may contribute to a decrease in the clinician’s ability to assume the perspective of, or have empathic concern for, his or her dissimilar patients. Literature on Clinical Judgment of Religious Patients 102 To date, bias studies on the clinical effects of religiously divergent clinician/patient dyads have been inconsistent (e.g., Bergin, 1991; Beutler & Bergan, 1991; Gartner, 1990; Hillowe, 1986; Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985; O'Connor & Vandenberg, 2005; Reed, 1992; Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980; Yarhouse & VanOrman, 1999). Methodological concerns and inappropriate generalization of results often contribute to mixed findings. Methodological issues include participant self-report of religiosity or patient religiosity based solely on religious affiliation with no consideration of how religiosity impacts one’s life (Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985; Reed, 1992; Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980). Another methodological issue is little or no attempt to control for social desirability. As bias is generally discouraged in most social contexts and certainly when forming clinical judgments, it is notable that any positive results of bias were found at all (e.g., Gartner, 1986; Gartner, Hohmann, Harmatz, & Larson, 1990; Hillowe, 1986) in studies that did not attempt to control for it, as one might assume that participants may be motivated to conceal their bias due to social desirability effects. It is not surprising however, that if a study’s purpose is transparent, and that purpose is to detect socially undesirable bias, the results are likely to be negative for bias (e.g., Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985; Reed, 1992; Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980). Generalizability of the results of several studies is limited as they did not use a national sample. Moreover, they typically examined the clinical judgment of psychologists in geographical regions in which one could reasonably assume 103 religiosity is fairly common such as Tennessee (Houts & Graham, 1986), Iowa (Lewis & Lewis, 1985), and Utah (Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980). Indeed, one study often cited in the literature as yielding no results of bias in diagnosis included only two categories of clinicians: “Latter Day Saints” and “other” (Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980) . One study (Houts & Graham, 1986) which failed to find significant bias in diagnosis was conducted in a rural area assumed to have a more religiously conservative population (Tennessee) than might otherwise be found nationally or in urban areas, which precludes generalizability to clinicians in more religiously liberal states where clinician encounters with religiously conservative patients may be more infrequent. It is possible that when religiously conservative persons are a distinct minority of the population, their out-group status may have an impact on clinicians’ stereotypic expectation of the patient, affective “liking” of the patient, and ability to empathize with the patient, all of which might impact the course of treatment. In fact, research indicates (Brown, 1970) that even clinician first impressions of patients which include personal “liking” for them, has a significant effect on case outcomes measured by eventual number of sessions, later assessment of patient progress, type of case termination, clinician satisfaction with patient progress, and clinician perception of patient satisfaction with therapy. Houts and Graham (1986) measured prognosis, pathology, and internal versus external attributions of patient difficulties as evaluated by clinicians who self-reported as religious or nonreligious. Clinicians rated vignettes that 104 supposedly represented nonreligious, moderately religious, and very religious patients. Clinicians in this study rated the moderately religious patient, as defined by doubts about his commitment to, and strength of, religious beliefs, as having more psychopathology and a more pessimistic prognosis than those with no mention of religion or those rated as very religious. Indeed, authors found that clinicians were probably influenced by the amount of doubt the patient exhibited about his religious beliefs, rather than degree of religiosity. Specifically Houts and Graham state that “consistent with the cultural legacy of viewing religious beliefs as a crutch, the individual who expresses less than convincing endorsement of religious beliefs may be more prone to being viewed as disingenuous and disturbed” (1986, p. 270). As doubt was introduced into the moderate condition and not in the others, it makes interpretation of results difficult and extrapolation to general attitudes about patients of varying degrees of religiosity impossible. Another study (Lewis & Lewis, 1985) found mixed results. Using a 10minute videotape of a depressed patient, Lewis and Lewis measured counselorperceived patient attractiveness using the Therapist Personal Reaction Questionnaire, pathology ratings using DSM-IV diagnoses, and a Likert scale prognostic measure, rated by both religious and nonreligious clinicians determined by clinician self-report of whether he or she was religiously affiliated. No significant bias was found on pathology and liking of the patient between either the religious or the nonreligious depressed patient; however, the patient’s religiosity was seen as having a large impact on her difficulties. Authors found it difficult to interpret these results as symptoms of depression between vignettes 105 were virtually identical but speculate that whenever religion is a central concern, it naturally impacts the way they manage their problems, or that religiosity is perceived to play a causative role in the patient’s problems. Interestingly, both religious and nonreligious clinicians predicted fewer sessions needed for the religious patient than the nonreligious patient. Particularly, nonreligious clinicians rated nonreligious patients as needing almost twice as many sessions as the religious patient. Authors speculate that finding may reflect clinicians’ belief that treatment progress may be enhanced by the religious patients’ religious orientation. There are several limitations of the above studies. The relatively undefined dichotomizing of religious versus nonreligious clinicians by self-report of religious affiliation, rather than using dimensional and more descriptive categorization of religiousness is problematic (e.g., Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985; Reed, 1992; Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980). Recalling that it is hypothesized that demographic variables are poor characterizations of one’s specific beliefs and attitudes, negative findings in some of these cases may be a result of poor clinician group assignment. If indeed studies seek to detect clinician bias with individuals whose beliefs and values differ from their own, more dimensional descriptors of one’s religiosity will be a better predictor of attitudes than will group affiliation alone. In other words, reported religious affiliation, does not a religious person make. Therefore, interpretation of results of bias in relation to others who are also merely associated with a religion may not reflect bias that could be evoked when one is presented with more detailed 106 information about the effects of another’s religiousness on behaviors and attitudes. Further, several of the studies cited above utilized a between-subjects design with no attempts to detect favorable bias in the direction of the religious patient over the nonreligious patient in individual cases, which gone unmeasured may have mitigated negative bias such that negative bias was undetectable, thereby compromising results (Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985; Reed, 1992). Wadsworth and Checketts (1980) used no control vignettes and each of the four vignettes presented described religiously affiliated persons, whether currently active or inactive participants in religious behaviors or beliefs. Additionally problematic is generalizability to less conservative states. It is not yet understood what effect continued exposure to values different from one’s own may have on bias, nor was this effect discussed in any of the findings of the aforementioned studies. However, current research on the attitudinal effects of exposure to targets indicates that mere exposure facilitates liking, relative to attitudes toward targets to which one has no exposure (Zajonc, 1968). So, if one is a nonreligious clinician in the “Bible belt” immersed in a culture in which the norm consists of religious persons, is one as likely to have bias against those of orthodox religious beliefs as one might if one were part of a community in which religious persons constitute an out-group? Wadsworth and Checketts’ study examined clinical judgment of Latter-Day Saints and “Other” participants, where “Other” included all other religions, agnostics, and atheists. The method of group 107 assignment in this study often cited for providing evidence against clinician religious bias, essentially renders any generalization of results impossible. Another study (Gartner, Hohmann, Harmatz, & Larson, 1990) yielded results revealing bias. In this study participants rated vignettes of patients belonging to one of four extreme ideological groups, right wing conservative religious (Fundamentalist Christian), left wing liberal religious (Atheists International), right wing political (John Birch Society), and left wing political (American Socialist Party) groups. Fictitious patients Mr. S and Mr. W were represented equally in each of either the four ideological categories or a nonideological group. Participants received one set of two vignettes each with either Mr. S belonging to one of the four ideological groups and Mr. W who had no ideology, or Mr. W being represented in one of the four ideological categories and Mr. S who had no ideology. Each subject responded to measures of empathy, pathology, and perceived maturity of the patients. Using a withinsubjects design on a national sample, significant bias was found on each variable. This study examined the interaction of patient/clinician religious (and political) ideological poles from very conservative and very liberal patients, which is clearly more informative than mere denominational affiliation alone. It also is different from other studies in that it measured clinical judgment using a national sample and a within-subjects design. Indeed, a review of the literature reveals that there is a paucity of research into the effects of divergent clinician/patient religiosity in a national population of psychologists which, as we have discussed, may be considered quite religiously liberal compared to the general population. 108 In another analogue study, Hillowe (1986) found bias against religious patients by a homogeneous group of therapists, who evaluated religious patients as more mentally ill and in need of significantly more sessions to make progress than their nonreligious counterparts. One significant interaction was found. As therapists’ nondoctrinal religious attitudes increased, the prognosis of religious patients was significantly better than for nonreligious patients. He speculates that as a result of their own experiences these clinicians may believe that religious patients have a basis of faith and hope that can contribute to improved therapy outcomes. Hillowe also suggested that his study may have found results where others did not because previous work typically categorized patients and clinicians on religious affiliation alone, whereas the religiosity of the patients in his vignettes was expressed in their beliefs and actions, and clinicians were assessed for nondoctrinal and traditional religious beliefs as well as for religious affiliation. Bias was found against conservative Christian graduate school applicants in a between-subjects study of full-time professors of clinical psychology in APAaccredited PhD programs in the U.S. (Gartner, 1986). Professors evaluated “nonreligious,” “Evangelical,” and “integrationist” (those who seek to integrate psychological theory or research into clinical work with religious persons or those who study constructs often associated with religion such as forgiveness or gratitude; see Lewis-Hall, Gorsuch, Malony, Narramore, & Stewart Van Leeuwam, 2006; Yangarber-Hicks et al., 2006). The professors evidenced significant differences in bias between “nonreligious” applicants and “evangelical” and “integrationist” applicants on all four items rated; positive feelings about the 109 applicant’s ability to be a good clinical psychologist, doubts about that ability, the necessity of interviewing that applicant compared to other equally serious candidates, and the probability of admitting the candidate. As predicted, the Evangelical was rated more highly than the integrationist, but those differences were not significant, and both were rated significantly different than the “nonreligious” applicant. While bias against potential graduate school applicants cannot be generalized to clinical bias with patients, the processes or mechanisms that contributed to these results are unknown. Therefore, neither should the conclusion be drawn that the findings of bias against Evangelicals in this study, must be unrelated to the potential bias against Evangelical patients. Lastly, bias was found in another study (O'Connor & Vandenberg, 2005) that investigated clinicians’ evaluations of religious beliefs drawn from religions considered most mainstream (Catholic), less mainstream (Mormon), and least mainstream (Nation of Islam) as comparatively more or less pathological, specifically in terms of psychosis. Beliefs corresponding to the teachings of each were articulated by patients in vignettes. Examples included the belief that “the Holy Spirit has given him a special strength to defend the faith” in relation to the fictitious Catholic patient, that another “came to believe quite passionately in the Mormon religion, whose tenets state that he will be transformed into a god after he dies,” and that as a member of the Nation of Islam, William “believes in the revelation that a spaceship, the Mother Wheel, has been hovering over the United States since 1929” (O’Connor & Vandenberg, 2005, p. 612). 110 Four sets of these vignettes depicted beliefs such as those above which were described in religiously specific language, in language that does not identify a specific religion, and with changes as a result of these beliefs representing either a no-harm situation (these beliefs deepened his relationship with his girlfriend), or a harm situation (the change affected a relationship that had previously been a positive one to the point that the patient considered killing his girlfriend following a betrayal). Three other distracting vignettes were also used. Each participant received and rated six vignettes total, consisting of beliefs with religious language, beliefs with no religious language, and either a no-harm set or a harm set, along with three distracter vignettes. The hypothesis that beliefs associated with less mainstream religions would be considered more pathological was supported, with Catholic beliefs being rated less pathological than Mormon beliefs, which in turn were rated less pathological than Nation of Islam beliefs. When Catholic and Mormon beliefs were associated with their respective religions they were rated as less pathological than when they were not, but there was no difference in the pathology rating for Nation of Islam patients in either case. In both conditions, Nation of Islam beliefs were rated highly and equally pathological, and significantly more pathological than other religious beliefs whether identified as religiously based or not. Authors posit that general familiarity of Catholic and Mormon beliefs, whether identified or not, may have contributed to this finding, and that it is possible that high pathology ratings of Nation of Islam beliefs are related to general unfamiliarity with them, or 111 something about their content. This is consistent with earlier arguments that familiarity may influence the evaluation of groups or group members as outgroups who are likely to be evaluated more negatively. Authors express concern that beliefs of religious traditions were rated as symptoms of severe mental illnesses potentially having dire consequences for the patient. Social Desirability A complicating factor in much of the research on religious bias utilizing self-reports is social desirability. The MODE (motivation and opportunity as determinants) model of biased responding suggests that the more sensitive a domain of evaluation, such as social group evaluation, the more likely motivational factors will be evoked and represented in explicit self-report measures (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Social norms discourage prejudice against cultural groups. Particularly within counseling and clinical psychology, multicultural diversity issues and the desirability of multicultural awareness and competence are in the forefront of academic curricula and research (Constantine & Ladany, 2000). Well-meaning psychologists are interested in cultural sensitivity, awareness, knowledge, and skills. Unfortunately, research on multicultural competencies that has sought to investigate correlations between clinicians’ self-reported competencies and objective multicultural competency rating by others has found “little relation between self- and other- rated multicultural competency” when social desirability 112 was controlled using the Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS) (Worthington, Mobley, Franks, & Andreas Tan, 2000). In another study (Constantine & Ladany, 2000), after controlling for social desirability with the MCSDS, none of the self-report scores on multicultural competence correlated with multicultural conceptualization ability as rated by others. Moreover, the emphasis on empathy in psychotherapy in addition to focus on diversity, likely contributes to the motivation to respond to explicit measures without empathic or other bias, whether or not they hold biased beliefs or attitudes or their capacity for empathy is affected by diversity variables in a natural setting. Not surprisingly then, there is often a significant discrepancy between self-reported empathy and empathic accuracy in the literature as measured by other-perceived or otherexperienced empathy (Davis & Kraus, 1997; Graham & Ickes, 1997; Ickes, Marangoni, & Garcia, 1997). In this context one must consider the results of studies above on clinician/patient bias that yielded negative results as potentially being mitigated by social desirability. Examinations of bias against religious or other social groups should pay particular attention to social desirability effects on outcomes, as well as seek to uncover the particular processes that contribute to any biased responding that is detected. One avenue of inquiry may well serve both purposes: that of implicit or automatic cognitive processes. 113 Automatic Versus Controlled Cognitive Processes Clinicians have the responsibility to thoughtfully consider the patient when forming clinical judgments, so it is assumed that cognitive processes become engaged in those efforts. The use of stereotypes in making evaluations in general may be a natural cognitive strategy to simplify and reduce informational load. Cognitive theorists explain that “if we, as social perceivers, were to perceive each individual as an individual, we would be confronted with an enormous amount of information that would quickly overload our cognitive processing and storage capacities” (Hamilton & Trolier, 1986, p. 123), and indeed that stereotyping is not only common but inevitable in ordinary categorization (Erlich, 1973; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986). In order to simplify information-processing strategies, reduce the amount of information to be considered, and comprehend a complex world, we categorize persons into groups. When information is ambiguous, particularly such as when one encounters a label such as Fundamentalist Christian or African American, stereotypes of groups may be utilized to assist in that categorization. So, stereotypes and their negative application, an instance of prejudice, are still considered by some to be common in today’s society, despite now being openly discouraged (Devine et al., 2002). As discussed earlier, research indicates that first impressions early in the therapeutic relationship are indeed formed about the patient and those impressions affect case outcomes 114 (Brown, 1970). What cognitive processes are engaged when making those impressions? Do those processes rely on stereotypic categorizations? The research suggests that there are both implict and explicit processes that are engaged in impression formation, particularly concerning stereotype maintenance and behaviors resulting from stereotypic attitudes and beliefs. The research on clinician bias with religious patients has made use of explicit selfreport measures to determine bias. As has been discussed, social desirability may affect those explicit self-report measures. In order to study stereotype attitudes and beliefs without social desirability effects, nonreactive implicit measures that tap into automatic stereotyping have proven useful in the stereotyping literature (e.g., Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, & Pratto, 1992; Bargh & Pietromonaco, 1982; Devine, 1989; Dovidio, Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997; Fazio & Olson, 2003; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Sherman, 2005). Implicit measures initially purported to capture automatic attitudes that exist outside of one’s awareness (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995); however, there is no evidence that because one’s attitudes may manifest outside of one’s awareness, that one must be unaware that he or she has the relevant attitudes (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Nevertheless, they do differ from explicit self-report measures in that responses do not require, and often prohibit, introspection that may have reactive or censoring elements. Whether or not there is awareness of the attitude by the subject, that automatic attitudes can be detected without 115 introspection suffices to alleviate methodological difficulties in designing nontransparent studies to capture bias without social desirability confounds. Activation of attitudes has been seen in priming experiments in which stereotyped attitudes were activated following the presence of an attitude object or word (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Banaji et al., 1993; Berkowitz & LePage, 1967; Dovidio et al., 1997), word fragment completion tests (Dovidio et al., 1997; Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Hense, Penner, & Nelson, 1995), and studies that examine bias in the tendency to explain stereotype inconsistent information more often than stereotype congruent information (Sekaquaptewa, Espinoza, Thompson, Vargas, & von Hippel, 2003). Implicit and explicit self-report measures have often had low correlations in the domains of prejudice and stereotyping (e.g., Devine et al., 2002; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Rudman, Greenwald, Mellott, & Schwartz, 1999), but occasionally demonstrate significant correlations (see Fazio & Olson, 2003 for review). Automatic stereotype activation has been found in many domains including ageism (Perdue & Gurtman, 1990), sexism (McKenzie-Mohr & Zanna, 1990), gender stereotyping in judgments of fame (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995), aggression (Berkowitz & LePage, 1967), race (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998; Dovidio et al., 1997; Sinclair & Kunda, 1999), and religion (Rudman et al., 1999). Additionally, numerous studies on implicit processes have demonstrated predictive validity, including priming procedures (Dovidio, Evans, & Tyler, 1986; Dovidio et al., 1997; Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995; McKenzie-Mohr & Zanna, 1990; Sherman, Mackie, & Driscoll, 1990), tendencies toward an 116 explanatory bias for stereotype inconsistent behaviors (Sekaquaptewa, Espinoza, Thompson, Vargas, & von Hippel, 2003; Sherman et al., 2005; Sinclair & Kunda, 1999), the tendency to attribute responsibility to a target's internal process rather than an external situation, (Sherman et al., 2005), and greater implicit memory for stereotype consistent information versus stereotype inconsistent information (Hense et al., 1995). One of the more well known measures of implicit stereotyping is the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). The IAT measures the strength of the association between a targeted group, such as African Americans, and stereotype congruent or incongruent characterizations (e.g., welfare, prosperity) or words with evaluative valence (e.g., poison, flower), measured by response latencies of computer key strokes in correctly categorizing targeted words. The theory behind the IAT is that it is easier to decide on a key press in response to words or concepts that are highly associated with a target group rather than those that are not associated. For example, in studying race associations, a subject would first practice the categorization of “Black” and “White” names such as “Latoya” or “Cathy” and then clearly valenced words which can be described as pleasant (flower) or unpleasant (poison) into “good” and “bad” categories. Following these practice categorizations, combinations of valenced words (poison, flower) and racerelated concepts (in this case names) are assigned to the target categories of either “Black” or “White” and the valence of “good” or “bad.” Participants designate categorizations by hitting one response key for one group assignment 117 so that the individually presented words “LaToya” and “pleasant” are put into the “Black/good” group assignment and “Cathy” and “poison” are put into the “bad/White” group assignment with a different key stroke. Then the category combinations are switched, so that White names and pleasant words are categorized with the “good/White” category and Black names and unpleasant words with “bad/Black” category. Latency of responding in each block is measured in milliseconds. In a race study using these methods (Greenwald et al., 1998), when Black was paired with unpleasant words, response latencies were overwhelmingly faster than when Black was paired with pleasant words, indicating that it was easier for the respondent to associate Black with unpleasant words. The IAT as a measure of automatically activated prejudice has come under some scrutiny. Some researchers believe that shifts in response patterns and the tendency to categorize nonwords as negative suggest there may be other causes that contribute to previous findings interpreted as implicit prejudice. They speculate that difficult trial blocks may facilitate learned response patterns, and that familiarity with target words versus nonwords may also impact response patterns (Brendl, Markman, & Messner, 2001). Other experiments on nonsocial group responding on the IAT as predictive of behavior yielded inconsistent results. For example, one study (Karpinski & Hilton, 2001) found no association between candy bar and apple associations on the IAT and participants’ actual choice of one over the other. However, Karpinski’s results may be inconsistent 118 with other IAT research due to the nature of his selected categories. Apples and candy bars may be associated with varying valences based on whether health or taste is more salient to the participant, and which salient feature is the overriding one when the participant is given the opportunity to choose one. Karpinski and Hilton (2001) offer the explanation that research on social group associations using the IAT may merely be the result of environmental exposure to associations rather than prejudices that one may have about a target group. However, the data yielded in the measurement of nonwords and apples versus candy bars are inconsistent with the data found in measures of attitudes about social groups, and so it is difficult to extrapolate possible reasons for lack of results in that study to others involving social groups. In contrast to work done with either nonwords and stimuli with presumably less associated stereotyped attitudes or affect, the IAT consistently demonstrates in- and out-group biases of social groups (e.g., Greenwald et al., 1998; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Rudman et al., 1999) and even bias in minimal paradigm work (Ashburn-Nardo et al., 2001) in which participants are randomly assigned to one of two groups, having no previous environmental associations with the groups. Nevertheless, when social groups are being judged, the argument that automatic activation of associations may be environmentally learned, and not an accurate measure of internalized prejudice upon which a person will act, cannot be quickly dismissed. Another explanation may lie within motivational processes as moderators of automatic associations. The MODE model of prejudice (Fazio, 1990) explicitly points to mixed processes of automatic and deliberative 119 processes of responding which contribute to judgments and behavior. Those processes include spontaneous activation of attitudes and motivation and opportunity to respond in a controlled manner. Devine (1989) also asserts a two-stage model of prejudice which includes both automatic activation of stereotypes by some target feature or label, and conscious choice-making of whether to act on, or overcome, the activated stereotype. Her model allows for a less deterministic perspective of the effects of implicit attitudes, and acknowledges that due to socialization effects of stereotype information, what may be activated are knowledge structures, or schema, of common stereotypic data, rather than personally held attitudes about groups. While the strength of the IAT as a measure of implicit attitudes is purported to be in the automatic latency of the response, Devine’s model suggests that those attitudes represented may be overcome by a motivation to respond without prejudice despite knowledge of stereotypic schema. Plant and Devine (1998) address “the presence of the rather pervasive external social pressure to respond without prejudice [that] has created enduring dilemmas for both social perceivers and social scientists as they try to discern the motivation(s) underlying (generally socially acceptable) nonprejudiced responses” (1998, p.1) by examining the importance assigned to both internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Dunton and Fazio’s Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Scale (MCPRS; 1997) measures the amount of motivation to control prejudiced reactions using a two-factor solution. Factor 1 consists of a concern with acting 120 prejudiced due to an internal set of standards in which one finds prejudice personally distasteful and concern with how one may be perceived by others for acting prejudiced. Factor 2 measures tendencies to restrain oneself from expressing thoughts and feelings that may cause confrontation with or about targets of prejudice. Distinct from social desirability measures such as the MCDS (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) which measures attempts to respond to self-report measures for the sake of appearing in a socially acceptable manner, motivation to control prejudiced measures assesss either origins of motivations to respond to life situations without prejudice (Plant & Devine, 1998) or the amount of that motivation (Dunton & Fazio, 1997). Socialization effects of religiously stereotypical associations may or may not be ubiquitous in the United States. A consistent pattern of activation of automatic attitudes about religious persons may or may not merely be the result of the activation of socially common schematic structures. Interestingly, if such a pattern existed, and it was to be the only or a very strong contributor to the activation of automatic attitudes in general, it may be assumed that religious clinicians would also demonstrate these associations about their own group on the IAT. However, as the research cited above indicates, implicit measures consistently demonstrate bias based on in- and out- group membership. Nevertheless, it is possible that automatic schematic associations may be activated at least partially as a result of environmental exposure. It is also reasonable to assume that whether or not one has automatic associations about a group, some persons may seek to exert control over their 121 behavior beyond what automaticity might otherwise dictate. Research that seeks to thoroughly examine the predictive validity of automatic associations on behavior may also benefit by examining the role that one’s motivation to control prejudice may play in moderating those automatic activations. Specifically, the theory of motivation to control prejudiced reactions as applied to religiously biased evaluations of clinical patients implies that motivational processes may moderate any stereotype congruent automatic associations with religious persons, so that the associations would not affect the evaluator’s behavior, therefore reducing clinical bias. Statement of the Problem The purpose of this study was to examine the potential bias in clinicians against religiously dissimilar patients. Again, bias here refers to an instance of prejudice that affects behavior or judgment. In particular, responses to Evangelical Christian (EC) patients were explored versus responses to patients with no mention of religion (NMR). Data show that approximately 82% of Americans can be classified in the Christian categories of Protestant, Catholic, or “other Christian” (Gallup, 2003 cited in NewPort, 2004). A Christian conservative group was selected because of its large representation in the American population today with Gallup estimates as 44% of the total adult population in 2006 endorsing self descriptions of “born-again” or Evangelical Christian,” and reports that 1 in 5 Americans can be considered Evangelical based on endorsed 122 Evangelical beliefs and behaviors (2006). The difference in the estimated proportion of Evangelical Christians between these methods of group assignment is notable, and supports earlier discussion about the differences between selfreported religious affiliation versus internalized religiousness and religious behaviors. However, that both estimates are fairly large is evident. It is further believed that Evangelicals’ recent sociopolitical presence may elicit affect in those who have strong opposite sociopolitical leanings and who may not be aware of the impact of their sociopolitical background on their clinical judgment. Lastly, it is believed that this large population of conservative Christians may be the largest group that represents a religious position most diverse in beliefs from psychologists, a group which endorses less Christian doctrinal adherence in the literature (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998). Variables Dependent variables were empathy and prognosis. Due to some history of the assumption or exaggeration of pathology of religious persons in psychology, and stereotypic expectations such that they are typically illogical and inflexible (Ellis, 1980), it was hypothesized that clinicians who exhibit bias against them would perceive them to be more mentally ill, with cognitive traits that may hamper the process of psychotherapy. Also, consistent with Byrne’s repulsion hypothesis (Byrne, 1971), religious beliefs and values which are more dissimilar to those of psychologists may result in negative affect in relation to the group. It was 123 believed that negative affect in response to those beliefs and values would result in clinicians’ reduced empathy with Evangelical patients, who did not demonstrate motivation to control prejudice. Therefore, clinicians’ religiously liberal attitude in relation to Christian beliefs (RLACB) was an independent variable. Implicit Negative Association with Evangelical Christians was both an independent and dependent variable as indicated in the hypotheses below. The effects of two covariates were measured for the purposes of controlling effects as appropriate, social desirability (SD) and other-religion conservatism (ORC). SD has been largely uncontrolled in the bias literature and has been shown to have an impact on results when it was controlled (e.g., Constantine & Ladany, 2000; Worthington, Mobley, Franks, & Andreas Tan, 2000). As social norms continue to discourage prejudice and studies have often revealed discrepancies between self-reported empathy and empathic accuracy as measured by others (Davis & Kraus, 1997; Graham & Ickes, 1997; Ickes, Marangoni, & Garcia, 1997), it may be that previous studies did not find results of bias due to social desirability effects. Conservatism of other religious groups was also measured as it is not known what influence religious conservatism in general would have on results. It was hypothesized that conservatives of other religious groups may not view the Christian conservative group as an out-group due to the commonality of their conservatism, and therefore they may not have implicit negative associations about the group. To assess conservatism of other religious groups, several items 124 were given including items that asked respondents to select an estimation of their personal devoutness in their religious beliefs, practices, and attempts to live in a scripturally prescribed manner. It may be difficult to “not have” any preconceived ideas about social groups, and exposure to some stereotypes of religious groups may be common. This may be true particularly about conservative Christian groups as their beliefs often manifest in public behaviors or positions such as influence or presence in political groups, legislation, and policy-making. It is believed and hoped that clinicians’ training in the clinical importance of an empathic relationship and general acceptance of their patients as well as their personal reasons for controlling prejudice, would have some influence on their motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Nevertheless, it was hypothesized that those who did not exhibit motivation to control prejudiced reactions and who demonstrated stereotypic associations with Evangelical persons, would assign poorer prognoses to, and demonstrate less empathy with, Evangelical Christian patients. Put another way, it was hypothesized that those who did not act on knowledge or beliefs about stereotypes would refrain from doing so because they were aware that prejudiced responses are socially undesirable, and/or they may have internal values against responding to others with prejudice. Therefore, motivation to control prejudiced reactions was predicted to moderate the effect of automatically activated stereotyping responses on empathic and prognostic bias. 125 Hypotheses Hypothesis #1. Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs (RLACB) will be positively related to the difference in empathy expressed toward Evangelical Christians (ECs) versus No Mention of Religion patients (NMRs), where difference is determined by EC empathy scores subtracted from NMR empathy scores. Hypothesis #2. RLACB will be positively related to the difference in prognosis expressed toward ECs versus NMRs, where difference is determined by EC prognosis scores subtracted from NMR prognosis scores. Hypothesis # 3. RLACB will be positively associated with Implicit Negative Association (INA) towards ECs. Hypothesis #4. Motivation to Control Prejudice Reacting (MCPR) will affect the relation between INA and the difference between empathy expressed toward ECs versus NMRs, such that as MCPR increases, INA will be less related to the empathy difference. Hypothesis #5. MCPR will affect the relation between INA and the difference between prognosis expressed for ECs versus NMRs, such that as MCPR increases, INA will be less related to the prognosis difference. 126 Methods The research design utilized an analogue within-subjects correlational methodology that included several assessments. Transparency, which may have affected results of other studies, was addressed in three ways. First, this study sought to identify automatic attitudes that were activated spontaneously, that is, without the participant’s ability to use reactive and censoring processes to mitigate them. Second, this study made efforts to normalize the use of projections needed to evaluate ambiguous information simulating a more natural therapeutic environment when some initial hypothesis generation is expected. The manifestation of any negative content of those projections in bias against the selected dissimilar religious group is the variable of interest. Third, as social desirability measured by the Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale has been successfully used in controlling for social desirability with clinicians in other studies (e.g., Constantine & Ladany, 2000; Worthington et. al., 2000), a shortened version of the MCSDS, the MAC-Form C (Reynolds, 1982) was used to measure social desirability as a covariate in this study to control for the potential of social desirability confounds on results. Despite the current steps taken to ensure measurement of clinicians’ uncensored responses, it is uncertain that all processes, conscious or unconscious, were completely controlled for or predicted by these measures. 127 Participants Participants were drawn from a national sample of approximately 3,070 psychologists from the American Psychological Association’s Divisions 42, 29, 12, and 36, the divisions of Independent Practice, Psychotherapy, Clinical Psychology, and the Psychology of Religion respectively, as well as the California Psychological Association, the Sacramento Psychological Association, and the Los Angeles County Psychological Association, through an email recruitment process utilizing each of their listservs. The total estimated psychologists subscribing to these listservs are 3070. Each group was believed to have a large population of psychologists in clinical practice. Division 36, the American Psychological Association’s Division of the Psychology of Religion, was also selected because it is believed that there may be a shortage of conservative religious clinicians within those samples as indicated in the religiosity gap section of this paper. Due to the nature of Division 36’s members’ interest in the psychology of religion, it was hoped that member participation would increase that number for comparative purposes. Also, another 415 clinical psychologist members of the Christian Association for Psychological Studies (CAPS) received email announcements of the study. In addition, 2,511 email addresses from public directories of state psychological associations were initially gathered. After checks to eliminate overlap email addresses, the total number of psychologists who received a listserv or a personal email request for participation was approximately 4,896. The 415 members of CAPS who received 128 email announcements were unavailable for review prior to the initial analysis, so overlap could not be checked and they were not counted in the 4,896 total. However, after data collection began, several issues prompted the decision to increase the number of email addresses used for direct emailing. These included technical difficulties with access to the second website used for data collection, the Inquisit website (Draine, S., 2006, Inquisit www.inquisit.com), the elimination of Macintosh users from participation due to the operating system’s incompatibility with the site, which was an unexpected condition of the site, and significantly lower response rates from listserv invitations than to direct email invitations. Therefore, another 5,435 email addresses were collected from public directories of local chapter psychological associations, psychological associations listed by orientation and disorder or disorder cluster, and various universities with public directories. The total number of direct email addresses to which invitations were distributed was 8,361, after excluding 149 participants who “opted out” of the invitation to participate. In the recruitment email, it was requested that all respondents be clinical psychologists, and information about participants’ degrees was gathered. It is not known what other differences exist between each group; nevertheless, in an effort to balance the religiosity of clinicians to some degree and to ensure adequate sample power, that there may be other sample differences is accepted and each sample group was used. In the power analysis, I calculated the sample size based on two sets of variables to accommodate the hypotheses with the most variables, and therefore 129 the highest N. This model contained predictor set A which consisted of the covariates social desirability and conservatism as defined by devoutness in beliefs, rituals, and traditions, and attempts to live according to prescribed scripture or teachings, and set B which included clinician religiosity. As it was initially unknown what effect the covariates would have and much of the past research has failed to detect bias, a reasonable and conservative estimate of effect size was small with an f-sq of .02 as defined by Cohen (Cohen, 1988). At .05 level of significance with an f-sq of .02 for each set for a combined effect of .04, N = 383 for a power of .80. Response rates from psychologists can be quite varied. In calculations that included the possibility that each of 2 covariates would be controlled, a 7% response rate of the initial 5,581 participants (which did not include the 415 from CAPS who could not be checked for overlap, and the additional 5,435 emails added following technical caveats) was needed for a power of .80 to detect small effect sizes. In an attempt to increase participation, one $100 and one $50 cash prize or donation to Hurricane Katrina victims was offered in a drawing from all entries made from participating psychologists. The option of receiving a winning amount in cash was also made. Perhaps more importantly, the utilization of the internet in the collection of data for this study was viewed as being more convenient than the use of long forms that may appear daunting and that require return mailing. Response rates to similar research have been as low as 17% (Gartner et. al, 1990), but also as high as 62% (Lewis & Lewis, 1985) and 67% (Ragan et al., 1980) in similar research with paper mailings and no incentives. 130 Due to the convenience of internet accessibility, the addition of 5,435 potential participants added after the power analysis, and the monetary incentives, it was believed that an adequate number of responses would be received. Of the 8,361 direct email addresses that were used, 890 were returned as “undeliverable” indicating an incorrect or outdated email address was published at the time the addresses were collected, and 282 persons responded that they were unlicensed, retired, or had Macintosh operating systems. It is unknown how many of the remaining 7,189 were licensed psychologists that had Windows Operating Systems and were therefore eligible for participation, so it is difficult to estimate an accurate response rate from eligible participants. If the assumption was made that they were all licensed psychologists, the response rate was 5.3%. It is also notable that 632 participants began the study in the first data collection website used, Survey Monkey (Finlay, R., 2007, Surveymonkey.com), 546 completed it in that website, and 394 completed the entire study including the IAT on the Inquisit website (Draine, S., 2006, Inquisit www.inquisit.com), which presented the IAT that required that participants have the Windows Operating System and which experienced technical difficulties when the first invitations to participate were distributed. Twelve completed IAT data-sets could not be matched to Survey Monkey data-sets by ID numbers, as they were likely accessed from different computers at different times due to technical problems, so they were eliminated from the final data-set. The final number of usable data-sets was 382. 131 Measures Batson’s Empathy Adjectives In studying the dimensional components of empathy in altruistic versus egoistic helping, Batson (1987) examined two distinct emotional reactions to someone in distress, personal distress and empathy. While the two reactions often occur together and both may have motivational consequences in helping another, feelings of empathy are other-oriented feelings and personal distress is self-oriented. Batson characterized empathy as a vicarious emotional response to another in need, and that emotional responding is evoked when the perceiver is able to adopt the perspective of the person in need. Batson operationalized empathic emotions through the measurement of the empathy adjectives: sympathetic, softhearted, moved, warm, compassionate, and tender. Participants rated how strongly they were feeling each of six adjectives on a 7- point Likert scale, with 1 representing “not at all” and 7 “extremely.” In an examination across several studies with varying degrees and dimensions of need situations, Batson reports highly consistent and robust findings of the independent factor structure of the variables of personal distress and empathy and support from the work of others which also report similar factor structures (see Batson, 1987 for review). Factor loadings of .60 from “moved, compassionate, warm, and softhearted” were found in all six studies examined, and “sympathetic and tender” in four of five studies that used these adjectives. 132 Correlations between empathy adjectives ranged from .44 to .79 across studies. Using the other-oriented dimension of empathy, Batson’s empathy adjectives were used to measure the affective empathy responding to patients in vignettes. Interpersonal Reactivity Index’s Perspective-Taking Scale Also, one subscale from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1994) is particularly salient to the nature of clinical work. The Perspective-Taking subscale measures the tendency to adopt the psychological view of others. As Batson’s adjectives of empathic vicarious responding are believed to arise from the ability of one to take the perspective of the other, and the nature of psychological work requires the ability to adopt the psychological view of the other if one is to be able to work within the patient’s frame of reference, the Perspective-Taking scale of the IRI will be used. Internal and test-retest reliability of the IRI are adequate at .70 -.78, and .61- .81 over 2 months, respectively. Good validity of the IRI is evidenced in the predicted relationships between the subscales, convergent validity with other empathy measures, and indexes of social competence, self-esteem, emotionality, and sensitivity (Davis, 1983). However, the PT subscale items characterize one’s patterns of responding to others as a dispositional trait. These items were slightly modified to evaluate the responding of the participant to the patient in each vignette. While the scale was intended to measure trait perspective taking, the wording of most of the items lent themselves well to slight modifications. Two items relating to PT 133 behaviors in conflicts are not adaptable to the psychotherapy situation because of the nature of the interactions in clinical settings, as opposed to social settings, so they were not used. Those items are “If I’m sure I’m right about something, I don’t waste much time listening to other people’s arguments,” and “when I’m upset at someone, I usually try to ‘put myself in his shoes’ for a while.” However, others such as “I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the ‘other guy’s’ point of view” are relevant to empathic perspective taking within the therapy relationship and can be easily modified to “I find it difficult to see things from the patient’s point of view.” It is not known what effects the modifications would have on the IRI’s demonstrated reliability or validity. However, the items maintain face validity and it was believed that the scores would continue to reflect differences withinsubjects in perspective-taking empathy between vignettes. The participants were asked to respond to five items on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 (does not describe my current position/feelings with respect to this patient very well) to 4 (describes my current position/feelings with respect to this patient very well). Clinical Judgment Scale Seven clinical judgment scale items borrowed from Graham (1980) were used to evaluate clinician prognosis. Items included likelihood of clinician selection for their therapy caseload, and patient likelihood of making substantial progress measured on a 5-point Likert scale with 0 being least likely and 5 most 134 likely. Additional items measured severity of impairment, motivation for change, capacity for insight, expectations about continuation of treatment, and likelihood of making substantial progress, also measured on a 5-point Likert scale with poorest predictions rated 0 and best predictions rated 5 for each item. There were no published psychometric properties found for this assessment. One could argue that the scale has good face validity; however, actual psychometric data are lacking. As this study utilized a within-subject design, differences between scores on similar vignettes were believed to be representative of difference in the clinical judgment of prognosis of hypothetical patients in those vignettes. Religious Attitude Scale The Religious Attitude Scale (RAS; Armstrong, Larsen, & Mourer, 1962), was used to measure participant attitudes toward specific Christian beliefs. The scale identifies 16 core Christian religious concepts which are followed by three definitions of each concept. Each definition represents an orthodox, conservative, or a liberal position in relation to those beliefs. Scores on each of the dimensions are converted into one continuous score ranging from most orthodox to most liberal. RAS authors discuss that they conceptualize these positions in a continuous, rather than categorical manner, although they do not use empirical support for this position. This conceptualization is consistent with the goal in this study of assessing potential bias in a group most divergent (in this case most liberal) from the Evangelical Christian patient. 135 Participants were instructed to select the descriptive phrase for the term that best describes his or her attitude or behavior in relation to the term. A sample item includes the Virgin Mary as defined by: a.) Mother of Jesus (C), b) supposedly the mother of a prophet (L), or c) blessed mother of God (O). Authors of the scale understood each position to characterize Catholic, Protestant, and Unitarian positions respectively and standardization was done on participants who self-identified with those denominational links. Although some persons in the sample from each of the identified groups reported not agreeing with some definitions within their category, authors believe this to be an expected occurrence due to some diversity in religious concepts among groups. However, in terms of scale validity, authors argue that the distinction between group scores indicates discrete differences between the groups’ definitions of Christian concepts. Test-retest reliability for the scale in a group of 71 nonpsychiatric participants was .98. The test was normed on 121 participants with mean scores for the orthodox position 139.50, the conservative position 105.95, and the liberal position 13.48. Test-retest reliability for concepts within religious groups was .73 for the group of 27 Catholics, .67 among 25 Protestants, and .48 among the 19 Unitarians. Inter-test reliability was .66 among Catholics, .61 among Protestants, and .72 among Unitarians. Because high scores on the RAS indicate higher levels of conservatism or orthodoxy and positive correlations were predicted between more religiously liberal attitudes and bias, all items were reverse scored. 136 Implicit Association Test The Implicit Association Test (IAT) was used to assess the strength of implicit associations that indicate existing stereotypic evaluations of the group of ECs. Specifically, it assessed the strength of participants’ automatic associations of “Evangelical Christian” (EC) or “Secular or No Religion” (SNR) with words clearly valenced in terms of pleasantness or unpleasantness, and stimulus concepts which are presumed to be associated with either group. SNR was chosen as study vignettes specifically do not mention religion of patients other than the EC, and the purpose of the study was not to assess attitudes relative to other religions. The administration of the IAT in this study, utilized guidelines in the literature that were established following the investigation of the effects of variability in the usage and administration of the IAT in over 120 studies (Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005). Positively valenced terms included Marvelous, Peace, Pleasure, Beautiful, Joyful, Laughter, Lovely, and Wonderful and Happy, and negatively valenced terms included Tragic, Failure, Agony, Painful, Terrible, Awful, Hurt, and Nasty. Associations between concepts that may commonly be associated with EC versus SNR were used in the practice trial blocks for the purposes of learning the concept dimension. EC concepts included Evangelist, Religious, Conservative, Traditional, Clergy, Minister, Christ, and SNR concepts included Activist, Liberal, Atheist, Progressive, Scientist, Agnostic, and Humanist. 137 Procedures and instructions followed Nosek et al.’s (2005) IAT recommended procedures. Participants proceeded through five blocks of responding to provide data. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible while attempting to minimize errors. In the first step, participants practiced sorting items from the different concepts into superordinate categories, (e.g., clergy for the EC category and scientist for the SNR category) by using key presses, either “E” or “I” with left or right middle finger respectively on computer keyboards. In step 2, the participants learned how to sort the valenced words according to “Good” or “Bad” categories (e.g., joy for Good and horrible for Bad) using the same keys. In step 3, sorting tasks were combined so that participants were alternately categorizing either a stimulus concept or valenced word to either the EC or Good group or the SNR and Bad group. During this trial for example, one key was the correct response for EC and Good words, and one key was the correct response for SNR and Bad words. The order of presentation of learning blocks was varied by participant order such that one participant was presented with a learning block that was configured with EC /Good categories combined first, and the next participant practiced the SNR/Good categories first. A practice block of 20 trials was completed followed by a brief pause, and then a second block of 40 trials, referred to as the critical block, was presented. In step 4, the key assignment was reversed and only stimulus concepts were sorted into the two target categories (e.g., Minister for EC, Scientist for SNR). In step 5, the participants sorted valenced word items and stimulus concepts again, but to a reverse combination of target groups (EC and Bad group, or SNR and 138 Good group). Participants again sorted items into appropriate categories for 20 practice trials and then 40 critical trials. The latencies in the IAT are measured by calculating the average latency in responding between the two sorting conditions (steps 3 and 5). Reviews of the IAT indicate there is good evidence for convergent and discriminant validity (Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005; Greenwald & Nosek, 2001) as well as large effect sizes (Greenwald & Nosek, 2001). Frequent weak correlations with self-report measures (Greenwald et al., 1998) indicate that the IAT assesses constructs that are often, but not always (Fazio & Olson, 2003), distinctive from those assessed in explicit self-report measures. Greenwald and Nosek (2001) reports test-retest reliability of the IAT averages > .6 and an internal consistency average of α > .80 (see Greenwald & Nosek, 2001 for review). Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions Scale Dunton and Fazio’s (Dunton & Fazio, 1997) Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions (MCPR) subscale of Concern With Acting Prejudiced, the subscale which has demonstrated moderating effects of racial prejudice following a priming technique (Dunton & Fazio, 1997) was modified for use with religious patients. The subscale is a nine-item scale that asks participants to indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with statements about concern with acting prejudiced. Participant responses are indicated on a 7- point Likert scale, ranging 139 from -3 (strongly disagree) to +3 (strongly agree). Fazio et al. (1995) found that the measure predicted scores on the Modern Racism Scale and Dunton and Fazio (1997) reported further evidence for validity by predicting self-reported negative attitudes when motivation to control prejudiced reactions was low. High scores on Dunton and Fazio’s MCPR Scale (1997) indicate higher levels of MCPR. While the scale measures both external and internal reasons to respond without prejudice, the current interest is in how much motivation one has as a moderator to INA, not the origin of that motivation. Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale-Short Form The Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS; Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) has proven useful in controlling for social desirability in research using self-report measures with clinicians (Constantine & Ladany, 2000; Fuertes & Brobst, 2002). The MCSDS (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) is a 33-item true-false measure which has been used extensively in the literature and is a primary social desirability measure used at this time. The Marlowe Crowne Social Desirabilityshort form, or the MC-Form C (Reynolds, 1982) was used to measure tendencies to respond in a socially desirable manner. The MC-Form C developed by Reynolds (1982) is a 13-item form with a reliability level of .76 which compares favorably with the reliability of the standard form. The short form validity is demonstrated in item factor loading, concurrent validation with the Edwards Social Desirability Scale and total score correlations with the standard MCSDS. 140 Social Desirability was measured using the MC-Form C for the purposes of detecting its effect on explicit responding in this study. Religious Conservatism Scale Lastly, religious conservatism (RC) was measured using three Likert scale items that assess degree of devoutness about religious beliefs, following religious traditions or practices, and attempts to live one’s life according to religious scriptures or teachings. It was hypothesized that conservative persons of religions other than Christianity may not exhibit bias toward conservative Christians due to the commonality of their conservative approach to their religion, such that they may not be considered an out-group. This scale was author generated and as such, no psychometric properties for this measure are available. Items required that participants respond to inquiries about their degree of devoutness in each of three domains: religious beliefs, following religious traditions or practices, and attempts to live life according to religious scriptures or teachings. As the items asked participants to respond directly to items about their adherence to traditional beliefs and behaviors, the measure was presumed to have face validity. Procedure 141 Materials that were distributed to psychologists including the vignettes and measures for those who chose to participate, are contained in Appendix A. Psychologists were first emailed an invitation to participate in the study, which was described as a study investigating clinical judgments. Participants were offered chances to win a $50 or a $100 donation to the American Red Cross’s Hurricane Recovery Program, a charitable organization of their choice, or cash prizes in those amounts, with winners selected in a drawing of all participants at the study’s end. A timed trial of the study revealed the time needed to participate in the study is approximately 25 minutes, which was disclosed to participants in the informed consent. Those who chose to participate in the study were instructed to click on a website link to Survey Monkey, the first of two data collection websites that was provided in the invitation email. The link took each participant to the front page of the study which consisted of an electronic informed consent page. Following participant agreement to consent terms, including the requirement that they use a Windows Operating System to complete the last portion of the study, he or she then entered the study by clicking on another link. The first task was vignette evaluation. Participants were given instructions that “It is understood that some of the information presented in the vignettes is ambiguous, and information required for accurate clinical judgment is lacking. Due to the fact that this study requires the use of brief vignettes, it is acceptable and expected that you project a hypothesis about each patient based on all pieces of information given, ambiguous or otherwise.” Participants were then 142 asked to read two brief vignettes of patients presenting with the same number of anxiety symptoms of Generalized Anxiety Disorder. In one of these vignettes, the patient was described as an Evangelical Christian with some discussion of the salience of his participation in religious activities. In the second vignette, the patient’s volunteer activities were described, with some follow-up discussion of the salience of those activities that were comparable to those described for the Evangelical Christian vignette patient. Order effects for the presentation of each vignette were controlled by alternating the order of presentation midway through the study. Conditions were counterbalanced for the relevant conditions only, with symptoms remaining constant across each condition. Participants then responded to empathy and prognostic measures for each vignette. Then the religious beliefs and attitudes, social desirability, motivation to control prejudiced reactions, and devoutness measures were administered. After completing these tasks, respondents were asked background questions including sex, age, education, geographic region, professional degree, years in clinical practice, and religious affiliation. Finally, participants were then directed to the IAT. Results Descriptive Statistics 143 Prior to examining my data, I prepared it for analysis by creating a code book, and exploring and cleaning the data. The study did not accept missing responses, so missing values were not present. Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for the sample background and demographic characteristics. The majority of the respondents (60.5%) were female. The respondents had a broad range of ages, with the 51- to 55- year old group (21.7%) and the 56- to 60- year old group (19.1%) being the largest. Over three-quarters of the respondents (78.0%) had obtained a doctorate in clinical psychology, with an additional 12.6% having a doctorate in counseling psychology. The West (32.2%) and the Northeast (29.8%) were the most common of the eight geographic regions, and most of the respondents (81.7%) came from urban locations. The respondents had a broad range of years of clinical experience, with 21-30 years (25.7%) and 16-20 years (15.7%) as the most common levels of experience. Only 3.1% of the respondents had fewer than three years of experience, and only 2.6% had 40 or more years of experience. The most common religious affiliations were Protestant (non-Evangelical; 25.4%), followed by Jewish (18.6%), and Catholic (14.1%). A substantial percentage of respondents (20.9%) indicated that they had no religious affiliation (none/Atheist/Agnostic), and 10.7% indicated that they had a religious affiliation not listed among the seven specific choices. Table 1 Sample Demographic and Background Characteristics (N=382) Frequency Percentage 144 Gender Male 151 39.5 Female 231 60.5 20-29 10 2.6 30-35 24 6.3 36-40 33 8.6 41-45 42 11.0 46-50 50 13.1 51-55 83 21.7 56-60 73 19.1 61-65 43 11.3 66+ 24 6.3 Doctorate Clinical Psychology 298 78.0 Doctorate Counseling Psychology 48 12.6 Doctorate Education or Related 10 2.6 Other 26 6.8 Northeast 114 29.8 Southeast 22 5.8 North 0 0.0 Age Education Geographic Region 145 Frequency Percentage Midwest 45 11.8 South 22 5.8 Northwest 9 2.4 Southwest 47 12.3 West 123 32.2 Rural 70 18.3 Urban 312 81.7 1-2 12 3.1 3-5 38 9.9 6-10 57 14.9 11-15 57 14.9 16-20 60 15.7 21-30 98 25.7 31-40 50 13.1 40+ 10 2.6 Buddhist 17 4.5 Catholic 54 14.1 Evangelical 20 5.2 Primary Location Years in Clinical Practice Religious Affiliation 146 Frequency Percentage Protestant (non-Evangelical) 97 25.4 Hindu 0 0.0 Jewish 71 18.6 Muslim 2 .5 None/Atheist/Agnostic 80 20.9 Other 41 10.7 Table 2 provides descriptive statistics for the measures used. Of primary interest in this table are the reliability coefficients. The affective empathy scores (from the Batson scale) had high reliability, with values of .90 for both Evangelical Christian (EC) and No Mention of Religion (NMR) vignette patients. For the cognitive empathy scores (from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index), the reliabilities were lower but still adequate at .73 for both EC and NMR. Initially, the reliability of the prognosis scales was only .52 (for EC) and .50 (for NMR). Further examination revealed that Item 7 in the scale lowered reliability. There was a strong negative correlation (-.50) between Item 7 (which is reverse scored), and Item 8. When Item 7 is removed, the reliability coefficients increased to .64 (for EC) and .58 (for NMR). Examining Item 7 indicates that respondents may not have interpreted this question in the intended manner as will be explored in the discussion section of this study. While the coefficients of .64 and .58 are still lower than would be desired, they were deemed adequate to 147 include the prognosis scales in the hypothesis tests. For the motivation to control prejudice reactions scale, the reliability was .73 and for social desirability scores (from the MAC-Form C) the reliability was .76. The religiously liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs (.91) and the religious conservatism scale (.93) both had high reliability coefficients. 148 Table 2 Descriptive Statistics for Composite Measures (N=382) Min. Max. M SD Α Affective Empathy EC 6 42 24.62 6.91 .90 Affective Empathy NMR 6 42 25.01 6.84 .90 Cognitive Empathy EC 7 20 15.78 3.34 .73 Cognitive Empathy NMR 8 20 16.20 3.08 .73 Prognosis Vignette EC1 14 32 25.15 3.45 .64 Prognosis Vignette NMR1 15 33 25.86 3.02 .58 Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions 34 89 58.60 10.40 .73 Social Desirability 0 13 4.49 2.95 .76 RLACB 0 31 18.25 7.99 .91 Religious Conservatism 3 12 7.21 3.09 .93 Compatible Test 161 3193 1242.21 402.76 Incompatible Test 572 2218 1170.28 315.48 -1240 2213 Implicate Negative Association 1 71.93 430.76 Without Item 7 as discussed in the text. Preliminary Analyses Several preliminary analyses were run prior to examining the hypotheses. Distributions were checked to make sure that the data conformed to normality 149 assumptions of the test. Figures A1 through A8 in Appendix B display the frequency distributions with superimposed normal distributions for social desirability, religious conservatism, religiously liberal attitudes toward Christian beliefs, NMR-EC affective empathy, NMR-EC cognitive empathy, NMR-EC prognosis, implicit negative associations, and motivation to control prejudice scores. No outliers were found. Table 3 contains the normality statistics of each measure. Religious conservatism and religiously liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs (RLACB) scores were somewhat negatively kurtotic, and NMR-EC affective empathy scores were somewhat positively kurtotic. However, no transformations were performed because the sample size is large (N=382), providing robustness against violations of normality assumptions. Also, skewness is generally considered more problematic than kurtosis (Tabachnick & Fidel, 1996). Additionally, while the skewness value for several variables was statistically significant (i.e., when divided by the standard error of skewness exceeds 2), this is primarily due to the large sample size in the current study (and therefore the small standard errors). The largest value of skewness is .48, and values in this range do not pose a problem for the statistical methods employed in this study, particularly given the large sample size. 150 Table 3 Normality Statistics for Variables Used in Hypothesis Tests (N=382) Skewness SESkewness Kurtosis SEKurtosis Social Desirability .48 .12 -.49 .25 Religious Conservatism -.03 .12 -1.28 .25 RLACB -.48 .12 -1.01 .25 NMR-EC Affective Empathy Difference .44 .12 4.61 .25 NMR-EC Cognitive Empathy Difference .11 .12 -.21 .25 NMR-EC Prognosis Difference .07 .12 2.06 .25 Implicit Negative Association .43 .12 2.37 .25 Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions .18 .12 -.39 .25 Regression assumptions of homoscedasticity and the normality of residuals within each regression were examined and are contained in Appendix C which shows residual histograms (designed to test the assumption of the normality of residuals) and residual equality scatterplots (designed to test the assumption of homoscedasticity). In each figure, the regression residuals were normally distributed and the assumption of homoscedasticity was approximated. It was determined that the untransformed scores were adequate. Table 4 contains the correlations among the eight key variables in this study. Some of these correlations will be discussed in relation to specific 151 hypothesis tests; however, several will also be discussed here. First, the first column of Table 4 shows that social desirability scores were not significantly correlated with any of the other variables in the study. Therefore, social desirability will be excluded as a covariate from all subsequent analyses. Second, the correlation between religiously liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs and religious conservatism was very high (r=-.66, p<.001). As the intent was to control for conservatism in religions other than Christian religions, the high correlation between religious conservatism and high Religious Attitude Scale (RAS) scores indicates that the religious conservatism scale captures the same conservatism measured in the RAS, Christian conservatism. Therefore, whatever variance conservatism might explain, RAS already explains. In addition to being redundant, controlling for conservatism will significantly reduce the ability of RAS scores to explain variance. Therefore, neither social desirability nor religious conservatism will be included as covariates in the hypothesis tests. Finally, the remainder of the correlation matrix shows that the main independent, dependent, and moderator variables have correlations ranging from -.06 to .51, indicating that multicollinearity is not a problem in this study. 152 Table 4 Correlations among Variables Used in Hypothesis Tests (N=382) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 1. Social Desirability 1.00 2. Religious Conservatism .05 1.00 3. RLACB .03 .66*** 4. NMR-EC Affective Empathy Difference .04 .19*** .18*** 5. NMR-EC Cognitive Empathy Difference .02 -.16** .18*** .33*** 6. NMR-EC Prognosis Difference .03 -.13** .18*** .37*** .20*** 1.00 7. Implicit Negative Association -.02 .51*** .35*** 8. Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions -.05 .12* 7. 8. 1.00 -.06 1.00 1.00 .06 .09 .12* 1.00 .07 .01 .06 -.04 1.00 *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001 A final set of preliminary analyses was performed to determine if there were differences on the key variables for this study based on the order of presentation of the vignettes. Specifically, 240 of the participants viewed the 153 NMR vignette first and 142 viewed the EC vignette first, and if the order of presentation resulted in differences in scores, controlling for order of presentation would be necessary. Six independent samples t-tests were performed to compare the two groups. Results showed no statistically significant difference for RLACB (t(380)=-.72, p=.474), NMR-EC affective empathy differences (t(380)=1.45, p=.149), NMR-EC cognitive empathy differences (t(380)=-1.42, p=.156), NMR-EC prognosis differences (t(380)=1.36, p=.176), Implicit Negative Association (INA) scores (t(380)=-.15, p=.882), or Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions (MCPR) scores (t(380)=-1.08, p=.282). Therefore, there was no need to control for order of presentation in the hypothesis tests. Hypotheses Analyses In the first hypothesis, the independent variable was Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs (RLACB) as measured by the RAS, and the dependent variable was Empathy. Empathy was measured by both the Perspective-Taking (PT) subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and Batson’s empathy adjectives for each vignette, where difference refers to EC empathy scores subtracted from NMR empathy scores. Therefore analyses were performed separately for the affective empathy difference and the cognitive empathy difference. The results of the regression analysis with the NMR-EC affective empathy score as the dependent variable are shown in Table 5. Overall, RLACB scores explained 4% of the variance in the NMR-EC affective empathy 154 difference score, which was statistically significant, F(1,380)=14.38, p<.001. The positive regression coefficient for RLACB (β=.19, p<.001) indicates that more RLACB corresponded to larger NMR-EC affective empathy differences. This indicates that individuals who had more religiously liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs tended to have more affective empathy toward the patient in the NMR vignette than toward the patient in the EC vignette. Table 5 Regression of Differences in Affective Empathy on Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 1, (N=382) Constant B SEB -1.22 .46 .09 .02 RLACB β .19 t p -2.63 .009 3.79 <.001 Note. R2=.04, F(1,380)=14.38, p<.001. Table 6 shows the results of the regression analysis with the NMR-EC cognitive empathy scores as the dependent variable. RLACB scores explained 3% of the variance in the NMR-EC cognitive empathy difference, which was statistically significant, F(1,380)=12.55, p<.001. Again, RLACB had a positive regression coefficient (β=.18, p<.001), indicating that those with more liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs tended to have more cognitive empathy toward NMRs than toward ECs. 155 Table 6 Regression of Differences in Cognitive Empathy on Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 1, (N=382) Constant B SEB -11.16 .85 .15 .04 RLACB β .18 t p -13.20 <.001 3.54 <.001 Note. R2=.03, F(1,380)=12.55, p<.001. In the second hypothesis, RLACB was the independent variable and prognosis was the dependent variable as measured on the clinical judgment scale for each vignette. The difference in prognosis was determined by EC prognosis scores subtracted from NRM prognosis scores. Table 7 shows the results of the regression analysis with NRM-EC prognosis difference scores as the dependent variable. RLACB scores explained 3% of the variance in NRM-EC difference scores, and this was statistically significant, F(1,380)=12.27, p<.001. Three percent of the variance explained is quite small, and it is likely that it was found due to the power of the study and its large sample size. As with the empathy scores, the regression coefficient for prognosis differences was positive (β=.18, p<.001), indicating that the participants perceived a more positive prognosis for the patient described in the NMR vignette than the patient described in the EC vignette. 156 Table 7 Regression of Differences in Prognosis on Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 2, (N=382) B SEB Constant -.50 .38 RLACB .07 .02 β .18 t P -1.32 .187 3.50 <.001 Note. R2=.03, F(1,380)=12.27, p<.001. The third hypothesis was that RLACB was positively associated with INA. Table 8 shows the results of the regression analysis with RLACB scores as predictors of INA scores. RLACB scores explained 26% of the variance in INA scores, and this was statistically significant, F(1,380)=131.66, p<.001. RLACB had a positive regression coefficient (β=.51, p<.001), indicating that those with more liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs tended to have higher INA scores, that is, they tended to have stronger implicit negative associations toward ECs compared to those in the Secular or No Religion (SNR) category. 157 Table 8 Regression of Differences in INA associated with Religiously Liberal Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 3 (N=382) Constant RLACB B SEB -427.29 47.48 27.36 2.38 β .51 t p -9.00 <.001 11.47 <.001 Note. R2=.26, F(1,380)=131.66, p<.001. The fourth hypothesis predicted that MCPR will moderate the effect of INA on empathy such that as MCPR increases, INA will be less related to empathy differences. As is recommended by Baron and Kenny (1986), two regressions were run. Also, following procedures for examining moderation in multiple regression analyses (e.g., Aiken & West, 1991, p. 9), the predictor variables (INA and MCPR) were centered prior to computing the interaction (product) term, so that the variables were less correlated, therefore reducing multicollinearity. In the first block of the regression analysis, the effects of INA and MCPR were entered as predictors, and in the second block the centered product between INA and MCPR was entered as a predictor. The results of the regression analysis with NMR-EC affective empathy differences as the dependent variable are shown in Table 9. In Block 1 of the analysis, only 1% of the variance in the NMR-EC affective empathy difference score was explained, and this was not statistically significant, F(2,379)=1.69, 158 p=.186. This indicates that INA is not related to affective empathy expressed between groups. In Block 2, the change in R2 with the addition of the interaction term was .00, and this was not statistically significant, F(1,378)=.98, p=.322. The regression indicates that the inclusion of the product term in Block 2 of the model did not result in an increase in prediction, and therefore it is concluded that MCPR does not moderate the relationship between INA and NMR-EC affective empathy difference scores. 159 Table 9 Regression of Affective Empathy Differences with NMR-EC with Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions as a moderator on Liberality of Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 4, (N=382) B SEB Constant .39 .19 INA .00 .00 MCPR .03 .02 Constant .40 .19 INA .00 .00 MCPR .03 INA x MCPR .00 Β T p 2.09 .038 .06 1.16 .248 .08 1.47 .142 2.12 .035 .07 1.30 .195 .02 .08 1.52 .129 .00 .05 .99 .322 Block 1 Block 2 Note. Block 1 R2=.01, F(2,379)=1.69, p=.186; Block 2 Change R2=.00, F(1,378)=.98, p=.322. The results of the regression analysis with NMR-EC cognitive empathy differences as the dependent variable are shown in Table 10. In Block 1, only 1% of the variance in NMR-EC cognitive empathy differences was explained, and this was not statistically significant, F(2,379)=1.64, p=.196, indicating that cognitive empathy was not related to INA. The addition of the product term in Block 2 increased the variance explained by 1%, but again this was not statistically significant, F(1,378)=3.39, p=.067. Therefore, we can conclude that 160 MCPR does not moderate the relationship between INA and the NMR-EC cognitive empathy difference scores. It should also be noted that INA did become statistically significant in Block 2 (β=.11, p=.039), indicating that higher INA scores were associated with greater NMR-EC cognitive empathy differences (i.e., those with implicit negative associations regarding ECs relative to NMRs showed slightly more cognitive empathy toward NMRs relative to ECs). The regression coefficient (.11) was similar to the correlation between these measures (.09) in Table 4, although the bivariate correlation did not reach the level of statistical significance. 161 Table 10 Regression of Cognitive Empathy Differences with NMR-EC with Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions as a moderator on Liberality of Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 4, (N=382) B SEB -8.42 .34 INA .00 .00 MCPR .01 .03 -8.39 .34 INA .00 .00 MCPR .01 INA x MCPR .00 β t p -24.52 <.001 .09 1.79 .074 .02 .32 .750 -24.51 <.001 .11 2.07 .039 .03 .02 .42 .677 .00 .10 1.84 .067 Block 1 Constant Block 2 Constant Note. Block 1 R2=.01, F(2,379)=1.64, p=.196; Block 2 Change R2=.01, F(1,378)=3.39, p=.067. The fifth hypothesis was that MCPR will moderate the effect of INA on prognosis expressed for ECs versus NMRs, such that as MCPR increases, INA will be less related to the prognosis difference. Table 11 shows the results of the regression analysis performed to test this hypothesis. In Block 1, 2% of the variance in NMR-EC prognosis differences was explained, and this was statistically significant, F(2,379)=3.57, p=.029. The regression coefficients in 162 Table 11 indicate that INA was statistically significant as a predictor of NMR-EC prognosis differences (β=.12, p=.017), with the positive regression coefficient indicating that those with higher INA scores (i.e., those with a positive perception of NMRs relative to ECs) also tended to have greater NMR-EC prognosis differences (i.e., more positive perceptions of the prognosis for NMRs versus ECs). When the interaction term was entered in Block 2, the change in variance explained was 0%, which was not statistically significant, F(1,378)=.14, p=.711. This indicates that MCPR did not moderate the relationship between INA and the NMR-EC difference score. 163 Table 11 Regression of Prognosis Differences with NMR-EC with Motivation to Control Prejudice Responding as a moderator on Liberality of Attitudes in relation to Christian Beliefs: Hypothesis 5, (N=382) B SEB Constant .71 .15 INA .00 .00 MCPR .02 .01 Constant .72 .15 INA .00 .00 MCPR .02 INA x MCPR .00 β t p 4.67 <.001 .12 2.40 .017 .06 1.27 .204 4.68 <.001 .13 2.42 .016 .01 .07 1.29 .198 .00 .02 .37 .711 Block 1 Block 2 Note. Block 1 R2=.02, F(2,379)=3.57, p=.029; Block 2 Change R2=.00, F(1,378)=.14, p=.711. Summary of Results Hypothesis 1 was supported by the data. For both affective and cognitive empathy, there was significantly less empathy expressed in relation to the Evangelical patient relative to the patient whose religion was not mentioned. Hypothesis 2 was also supported. There was a significantly poorer prognosis 164 expressed in relation to the Evanglical patient relative to the patient whose religion was not mentioned. Using the IAT, a timed measure designed to detect attitudes of participants without giving them the opportunity to censor biased responding, Hypothesis 3 was also supported. Those clinicians with more liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs demonstrated stronger negative associations toward the Evangelical Christian target group than the Secular or No Religion target on the IAT. Hypotheses 4 and 5 were not supported in this study. Participants’ motivation to control prejudice responding had no moderating effect on implicit responding to the Christian target group in relation to explicit responding to vignette patients on empathy and prognosis measures. Discussion This study examined whether clinicians with more religiously liberal attitudes in relation to Christian beliefs responded differently to an Evangelical Christian vignette patient than to a vignette patient whose religion was not mentioned, and whether they responded with more negative associations to Evangelical Christian targets than to Secular or No Religion targets on an implicit measure. The difference between the religiosity of psychologists and the general public has been referred to as the religiosity gap (Richards & Bergin, 2000). Seventy-six percent of the population endorsed Judeo-Christian affiliations of Protestant, Catholic, Jewish, Orthodox, and Mormon recently (Gallup, 2006), and in this study, 63.3% of psychologists endorsed Judeo-Christian categories. The 165 difference is notable, but markedly different from previous research in which 43% endorsed these affiliations (Bilgrave & Deluty, 1998). It is likely that this study yielded a higher percentage of psychologists with Judeo-Christian affiliations as a result of efforts to balance the religious backgrounds of participants by recruiting participants from organizations like the Christian Association for Psychological Studies, and the American Psychological Association’s Division of the Psychology of Religion. Nevertheless, distinct differences in the Evangelical category are noted in that only 5.2% of psychologists endorsed this affiliation in this study compared to estimates of 22% of the general public (Gallup, 2005). The results of this study revealed that religiously liberal clinicians’ empathy for, and prognosis for, patients who are described as Evangelical Christian is significantly different than for patients whose religion is not mentioned. This finding of religious bias extends the clinical religious bias literature to date. Particularly, it is significant in that this study controlled for problematic methodology and design in other research that did not find bias (e.g., Houts & Graham, 1986; Lewis & Lewis, 1985; Reed, 1992; Wadsworth & Checketts, 1980). These problems include the use of dichotomous or categorical religious group assignments of participants, social desirability effects in particular related to the exclusive use of self-report measures of bias, between-subjects design, small sample sizes, and samples largely derived from rural areas in which religiously conservative persons might not be considered dissimilar, or as members of an outgroup. 166 First, as discussed in the review of religious bias literature, previous studies that did not find bias were often conducted in rural areas in which religious persons may not be considered an out-group. As we have seen, stereotyping has been consistently demonstrated in the literature with members of out-groups. A national population of psychologists from a broad geographic area was solicited for participation in this study to control for that potential problem and to increase generalizability of findings. This sample consisted of psychologists from all geographic regions; however, a large portion of participants indicated they were from urban areas. Stereotyping of this group may be more common in urban areas, in which one might assume religiously conservative persons might be considered part of a more distinct out-group. Also, social desirability and transparency, which may have been problematic in other studies, were addressed in this study in several ways. First, social desirability was assessed for potential use as a covariate. However, it did not correlate with other variables in this study and so it was not used as a covariate. It is interesting that bias was detected without having to control for social desirability in this study, despite that it is reasonable to expect that its effects may impact results in any bias or stereotyping studies. However, it may not have been necessary to control for social desirability here for several reasons. First, efforts to conceal variables of interest in bias research, unless successfully executed, may inadvertently contribute to an increase in socially desirable responding. In other words, the implication of covert attempts to uncover information about participants could cue the participant that there may 167 be a variable of interest that some might consider worth concealing. However, projections about ambiguous information, often presented in preliminary therapeutic work with patients, are likely to be common as clinicians form their initial impressions about new patients with limited information. In this study, attempts were made to normalize that process prior to the presentation of the vignettes so that evaluations could more closely approximate a natural therapeutic setting. So, it may not have been as desirable to conceal projections of expectations about vignette patients in this study. Also, liberality of clinician religiousness in relation to Christian beliefs was the independent variable in this study, rather than self-report of religious affiliation, or self-designation to dichotomous groups such as religious versus nonreligious, which may not be an adequate method of group assignment since group affiliation does not describe religiosity or religious beliefs or values in many cases. Results indicate that religiously liberal clinicians in this sample are biased against religiously conservative Christian groups or group members, and are less likely to conceal their bias due to social desirability than those who merely identify membership or affiliation with a particular religious group. Further, the IAT was used to reduce reactive and censoring elements employed in the expression of bias. A correlation between clinician religious liberality and automatic negative associations was found, indicating that religiously liberal clinicians have some bias against the Evangelical Christian group. In particular, negative associations about Evangelicals predicted a poorer prognosis for the Evangelical patient compared to the patient whose religion was 168 not mentioned. In light of this finding, and that differences were found in the selfreport expressed empathy in relation to the vignettes, it is interesting that negative associations with the Evangelical target did not predict differences in affective empathic responding to the hypothetical Evangelical patient. The reason for this finding is unclear. While the stereotyping literature has examined cognitive evaluations of targets as well as affective responding to targets, a preponderance of the literature did not yield data about the relationship between the two variables that would assist in clarifying this finding, nor did it offer any explanations in relation to the interaction of affective and cognitive empathy. It may be that something about the ability to express affective empathy toward Evangelical Christian patients is different than the general evaluative valence that clinicians associate with Evangelical Christians in general. It may also be that for those clinicians whose automatic negative associations were related to poorer prognosis for, and less cognitive affect with, Evangelical patients, there were some strong negative stereotypic beliefs which are unrelated to the ability to have affective empathy for the Evangelical patient. However, it should be noted that it is likely that flawed logic was used in the manner in which this study attempted to control for clinician religious conservatism. Because conservatism was so highly correlated with endorsement of Christian beliefs, removing the effects of conservatism would also likely have eliminated the ability of scores on the religious attitude scale to explain any difference in either the implicit or the explicit measures. So conservatism was not controlled in this study. 169 Overall, these results have significant implications for patients who are religiously dissimilar to clinicians. If clinicians estimate a poorer prognosis for patients based on religious group as they did in this study, it may be because they presume that pathology exists that is more severe than would be estimated for comparable patients from other religious groups, or from those with no religious affiliation. The literature does not support a correlation between religiosity and poor mental health. In fact, religiosity is associated with lower levels of depression (Smith, McCullough, & Poll, 2003), anxiety (Bergin, Masters, & Richards, 1987), positive religious coping with chronic pain (see Rippentrop, 2005 for review), and rehabilitation efforts (Kilpatrick & McCullough, 1999). A comprehensive review indicated that those with devout internally motivated religiousness and participation in religious activities such as prayer, scripture reading, and attendance at church or synagogue are associated with increased mental health including lower rates of anxiety, depression, suicidality, less death anxiety, substance abuse, and higher life satisfaction, self-esteem, greater well being, happiness, adjustment, social support, internal locus of control and marital adjustment and satisfaction, and quicker recovery from depression (see Koenig, 1997, pp. 101-102). Cognitive appraisals of religious patients as more mentally ill than their nonreligious counterparts can affect initial impressions of the patient, liking of the patient, the ease with which a patient may feel free to express him- or herself, and the clinician’s assessment of the patient’s potential for change. There are several ways this can impact the patient including therapist satisfaction with 170 patient progress, perception of patient satisfaction, and type of termination (Brown, 1970). Additionally, expectations that are based on stereotyped information have been associated with effects on information processing and judgments, information seeking and hypothesis testing, and interpersonal behavior via self-fulfilling prophecies (Hamilton et al., 1990). Clearly, clinicians may not be impervious to these effects and may adjust their clinical approaches accordingly. Last, if the patient’s religiosity or associated values are determined to be a contributor to poorer mental health or an impediment to therapeutic progress in some way, they may be targeted for change. Targeting patient values for change because they are seen as less favorable than the clinician’s own values although they have not been found to be pathological, may be problematic for therapists who strive to be culturally sensitive and competent and who value acceptance and positive regard for the patient as integral to successful treatment, and for patients whose religiosity and associated values may be disrespected or otherwise insensitively treated. Additionally, this can present a violation of ethical mandates that require clinicians to respect cultural differences of diverse groups. It is likely, and at least hoped that such unfounded bias is unintended and operates outside of the clinician’s awareness. Empathy has long been considered a cornerstone of an effective therapeutic relationship in which the clinician seeks to understand the patient’s experience (Ivey et al., 1993; Rogers, 1957). Bias in empathy has been related to the clinician’s ability to conceptualize a patient’s mental health issues from a 171 multicultural perspective when rated by others (Constantine, 2001). Group member ratings of general and cultural competence have also been associated with empathy, and competence as perceived by the patient can have an effect on the patient’s overall satisfaction with treatment (Fuertes & Brobst, 2002). In relation to existing clinician religious bias, it is notable that some religious persons have expressed fears that therapists will judge them or seek to change their religion or associated values (Richards & Bergin, 2000, p. 13), which is likely to be related to the religious person’s perception of potential psychologist preference or bias for his or her values over the patient’s. The results of this study have implications for clinician multicultural training programs. Given that bias was found in this study, it is a concern that clinicians who experience automatic negative bias against such a large portion of the general population, do not successfully moderate their negative responding to the group when given the opportunity to use deliberation in doing so. Devine (1989) posited the theory that one’s negative automatic associations need not be deterministic in relation to one’s behavior, if one is sufficiently motivated not to act upon the association. Further, the MODE model of prejudice (Fazio & Olson, 2003) posits that the more sensitive a domain of evaluation, such as social group evaluation, the more likely motivational factors in concealing that bias will be represented in self-report measures. As clinicians can generally be assumed to use thoughtful consideration in making clinical judgments, these results suggest that there are beliefs about religious conservative persons that affect clinical judgment such that clinicians 172 give a poorer prognosis to them, and that they do not believe these beliefs to be in need of censorship. As it has not been demonstrated that religious persons are more pathological than nonreligious persons, and in fact that their religiosity is often associated with better mental health, the nature of clinician’s beliefs about this group such that they are believed to have a poorer prognosis than their nonreligious counterparts, is of interest and may be considered a variable for future research. Diversity training is often synonymous with racial and ethnic multicultural approaches to treatment and education. As such, religiosity is an often overlooked expression of diversity in training programs and the diversity literature (Yarhouse & Fisher, 2002). If religiosity is to be considered from an informed and sensitive multicultural perspective, it is indicated that training programs should undertake to educate clinicians about culturally appropriate and competent treatment of religiously dissimilar patients. Multicultural education and literature should also endeavor to increase awareness about the impact of variables such as sociopolitical influence that are often presumed to be correlated with religiosity, and that influence on bias with religiously dissimilar patients (Fuertes & Brobst, 2002). This is particularly salient as affective charge has been increasingly associated with religiosity or religious values in the political domain (Wallis, 2005). The findings of this study extend the literature on stereotyping. Byrne’s attraction paradigm (1971) posits that those who are similar are more attracted to each other and those who are dissimilar will be repulsed by each other. 173 Importantly, he hypothesized that similarity and dissimilarity based on values and attitudes are more important in determining attraction or repulsion than are demographic variables. As has been discussed, religious affiliation does not a religious person make. Methods of group designation by self-report of religious affiliation or membership tell little of the effects of that association on one’s beliefs or attitudes. The findings of this study, which assessed attitudes toward core Christian beliefs rather than endorsement of affiliation or membership with a religious group, supports Byrne’s theory. This study also adds to the growing body of literature on negative automatic associations with social out-groups. Self-report measures of stereotyping or bias may compromise research results as prejudice is generally viewed as socially unacceptable, thereby increasing motivation to conceal bias, stereotyped beliefs, or prejudiced responding. Negative automatic associations were associated with the group most religiously diverse from the religiously conservative target. Recalling that some posit that automatic associations are demonstrated on the basis of common social knowledge of stereotypes rather than personally held beliefs (Karpinski & Hilton, 2001), this study indicates that the participant’s beliefs are a primary factor in determining negative characterization of the group. There are several limitations to this study. The large representation of urban clinicians in this sample may have contributed to the findings of bias if religiously conservative persons are considered an out-group in those areas. The sample also largely consisted of those belonging to psychological organizations, 174 which were also presumably internet savvy since the completion of the survey required internet use and even that the participants download a plug-in for the Implicit Association Test (IAT). It is unknown what differences there may be between this group and others who differ on these characteristics. Item 7 from the prognosis measure was deleted due to its lack of correlation to other variables. It is not understood why the item did not correlate with other variables. A review of both items 7 and 8 revealed that participants may have interpreted the items in much the same way and did not differentiate between the intention of each. Items 7 and 8 ask participants to rate “the number of therapy sessions required for this patient to make substantial progress,” and “number of sessions you expect that this patient will attend therapy” respectively. Also, there were considerable technical difficulties in the administration of the IAT portion of this study. Many participants who began the study, could not, or did not, finish it. Further, as one participant pointed out, there may be differences between psychologists who used MacIntosh operating systems, and those who used Windows Operating Systems which was required for the IAT portion of the study. It is not known what differences there may be between the two groups. Last, while it is difficult to estimate a response rate to this study due to technical problems, the Windows Operating System requirement, and a population which included unlicensed psychologists who were excluded from the study, the response rate overall was low. The results of this study indicate that more research would be helpful in determining which clinician or patient variables are related to religious bias, and 175 how they are related, and continued investigations into the specific processes that predict religious bias, the origins, nature, and application of that bias, and whether biases are specific to certain groups are also of interest. Future research in the area of clinician religious bias would be of service to religiously dissimilar patients if it were to seek data that could better inform and assist multicultural training programs in their efforts to provide culturally competent and informed clinical services to these patients. It is hoped that this and similar research succeeds in facilitating interest in cultural training and in the literature to more adequately address issues related to competent, respectful, and sensitive treatment of religious persons and their religiously informed values. 176 References – Part B Aiken, L.S., & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders third revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishers American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders fourth edition text revision. Washington, DC: Author. APA. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57(12), 1060-1073. Armstrong, R. G., Larsen, G. L., & Mourer, S.A. (1962). Religious attitudes and emotional adjustment. Journal of Psychological Studies, 13, 35-47 Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Monteith, M. J. (2001). Implicit associations as the seeds of intergroup bias: How easily do they take root? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 789-799. Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (1995). Implicit gender stereotyping in judgments of fame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 181-198. Banaji, M. R., Hardin, C., & Rothman, A. J. (1993). Implicit stereotyping in person judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 272-281. 177 Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 893-912. Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(3), 432-449. Baron, R.M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 11731183. Batson, D. C. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 65-122. Bergin, A. (1980). Psychotherapy and religious values. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48(1), 95-105. Bergin, A., & Jensen, J. P. (1990). Religiosity of psychotherapists: A national survey. Psychotherapy, 27(1), 3-7. Bergin, A. E. (1983). Religiosity and mental health: A critical reevaluation and meta-analysis. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 14(2), 170-184. Bergin, A. E. (1991). Values and religious issues in psychotherapy and mental health. American Psychologist, 46(4), 394-403. 178 Bergin, A. E., Masters, K. S., & Richards, S. P. (1987). Religiousness and mental health reconsidered: A study of an intrinsically religious sample. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 34(2), 197-204. Berkowitz, L., & LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7(2), 202-207. Beutler, L. E., Crago, M., & Arizmendi, T. G. (1986). Therapist variables in psychotherapy process and outcome. In S. L. Garfield & A. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 257-310). New York: Wiley & Sons. Beutler, L.E., Machado, P. P. P., & Neufeldt, S. A. (Eds.). (1994). Therapist Variables (4th ed.). New York: Wiley & Sons. Beutler, L. E., & Bergan, J. (1991). Value change in counseling and psychotherapy: A search for scientific credibility. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38(1), 16-24. Beutler, L. E., Malik, M. L., Alimohamed, S., Harwood, T. M., Talebi, H., Noble, S., et al. (2004). Therapist variables. In A. E. Bergin & S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed.). New York: Wiley & Sons. Biernat, M., & Korbrynowicz, D. (2003). Stereotypes and shifting standards: Some paradoxical effects of cognitive load. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(10), 2060-2079. 179 Bilgrave, D. P., & Deluty, R. H. (1998). Religious beliefs and therapeutic orientations of clinical and counseling psychologists. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37(2), 329-349. Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Susser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 621-632. Bodenhausen, G. V., & Lichtenstein, M. (1987). Social stereotypes and information-processing strategies: The impact of task complexity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(5), 871-880. Brawer, P. A., Handal, P. J., Fabricatore, A. N., Roberts, R., & Wajda-Johnston, V. A. (2002). Training and education in religion/spirituality within APAaccredited clinical psychology programs. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 33(2), 203-206. Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 760-773. Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this "we"? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 83-93. Brickman, P., & Bulman, R. J. (1977). Pleasure and pain in social comparison. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 149-186). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. 180 Brown, R. D. (1970). Experienced and inexperienced counselors' first impressions of client and case outcomes: Are first impressions lasting? Journal of Counseling Psychology, 17(6), 550-558. Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press. Cheatham, H. E., Ivey, A. E., Ivey, M. B., & Simek-Morgan, L. (1980). Multicultural counseling and therapy: Changing the foundations of the field. In A. E. Ivey, M. B. Ivey, & L. Simek-Morgan (Eds.), Counseling and psychotherapy: A multicultural perspective (3rd ed., p. 94). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Chen, F. F., & Kenrick, D. T. (2002). Repulsion or attraction? Group membership and assumed attitude similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 111-125. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Constantine, M. G., & Ladany, N. (2000). Self-report multicultural counseling competence scales: Their relation to social desirability attitudes and multicultural case conceptualization ability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(2), 155. Constantine, M. G. (2001). Multicultural training, theoretical orientation, empathy, and multicultural case conceptualization ability in counselors. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 23(4), 357-372. Crawford, M., T., & Skowronski, J. J. (1998). When motivated thought leads to heightened bias: High need for cognition can enhance the impact of 181 stereotypes on memory. Society for Personality and Social Psychology Inc, 24(10), 1075-1088. Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. (1990). Collective self-esteem and ingroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 60-67. Crocker, J., McGraw, K. M., Thompson, L. L., & Ingerman, C. (1987). Downward comparison, prejudice, and evaluation of others: Effects of self esteem and threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(5), 907-916. Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-126. Davis, M. H. (1994). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark. Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1997). Personality and empathic accuracy. In W. Ickes (Eds), Empathic accuracy (pp. 145-167). New York: Guilford Press. Devine, P., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(5), 835-848. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5-18. 182 Dovidio, J. F., Evans, N., & Tyler, R. B. (1986). Racial stereotypes: The content of their cognitive representations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 22-37. Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510-540. Draine, S. (2006). Inquisit 2.0.61004.5., www.millisecond.com.[Computer Software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software LLC. Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudice reactions. Society for Personality and Social Psychology Inc, 23(3), 316-326. Ellis, A. (1980a). The case against religion: A psychotherapist's view and the case against religiosity. Austin, TX: American Atheist Press. Ellis, A. (1980b). Psychotherapy and atheistic values: A response to A.E.Bergin's "Psychotherapy and Religious Values.” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48(5), 635-639. Ellis, A. (2000). Can rational-emotive behavior therapy (REBT) be effectively used with people who have devout beliefs in God and religion? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 31(1), 29-33. Erlich, H. J. (1973). The social psychology of prejudice. New York: Wiley. Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitude guides behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), 183 Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 75-109). New York: Academic Press. Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 10131027. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297327. Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229-238. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-140. Finlay, R. (2007). SurveyMonkey.com [online data collection website]. Portland, OR. Francis, L. J., & Peter, K. (2002). The relationship between psychological wellbeing and Christian faith and practice in an Australian population sample. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41(1), 179-184. Freud, S. (2000). Totem and taboo. In Totem and taboo: Resemblances between the psychic lives of savages and neurotics. (A. A. Brill, Trans.). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. (Original work published 1913). 184 Freud, S. (1964). A Weltanschauung? In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (pp. 158-182). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1933). Fuertes, J. N., & Brobst, K. (2002). Client's ratings of counselor multicultural competency. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8(3), 214223. Gaertner, L., & Insko, C. A. (2000). Intergroup discrimination in the minimal group paradigm: Categorization, reciprocation, or fear? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(77-94), 77-94. The Gallup poll: Public opinion in 1996. (1996). Wilmington, DE: Scholarly Resources. The Gallup Poll. (2005). Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://poll.gallup.com/content/default.aspx?ci=16519 The Gallup Poll. (2006). Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://brain.gallup.com/documents/question.aspx?question=72715&Advan ced=0&SearchConType=1&SearchTypeAll=Universal%20spirit Gartner, J. (1986). Antireligious prejudice in admissions to doctoral programs in clinical psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 17(5), 473-475. Gartner, J. (1990). The effect of client and counselor values on clinical judgment. Counseling & Values, 35(1), 58-63. 185 Gartner, J., Hohmann, A., Harmatz, M., & Larson, D. (1990). The effect of patient and clinician ideology on clinical judgment: A study of ideological countertransference. Psychotherapy, 27, 98-106. Gartner, J., Larson, D. B., & Allen, G. D. (1991). Religious commitment and mental health: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 19(1), 6-25. Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 509-517. Graham, S. A. (1980). Psychotherapists' attitudes toward offender clients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48(6), 796-797. Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women's intuition isn't greater than men's. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy. New York: Guilford Press. Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, selfesteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4-27. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464-1480. Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Association Test at age 3. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie, 48(2), 85-93. Guimond, S., Dambrun, M., Michinov, N., & Duarte, S. (2003). Does social dominance generate prejudice: Integrating individual and contextual 186 determinants of intergroup cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 697-721. Hamilton, D. L., Sherman, J. W., & Ruvolo, C. M. (1990). Stereotyped-based expectancies: Effects on information processing and social behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 46(2), 35-60. Hamilton, D. L., & Trolier, T. K. (1986). Stereotypes and stereotyping: An Overview of the cognitive approach. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 127-163). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Hense, R. L., Penner, L. A., & Nelson, D. L. (1995). Implicit memory for age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 13(4), 339-415. Hill, P. C., Pargament, K. I., Hood, R. W., McCullough, M. E., Swyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., et al. (2000). Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Points of commonality, points of departure. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 30, 51-77. Hillowe, B. V. (1986). Effects of religiosity of therapist and patient on clinical judgment (Paper). 94th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington D.C. Hilton, J. L., & Hippel, W. v. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 237-271. Houts, A. C., & Graham, K. (1986). Can religion make you crazy? Impact of client and therapist religious values on clinical judgments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(2), 267-271. 187 Ickes, W., Marangoni, C., & Garcia, S. (1997). Studying empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant context. New York: Guilford Press. Ivey, A. E., Ivey, M. B., & Simek-Morgan, L. (Eds.). (1993). Counseling and psychotherapy: A multicultural perspective. Needham Heights, MA: Simon & Shuster. Jackson, L. A., & Sullivan, L. A. (2001). Cognition and affect in evaluations of stereotyped group members. The Journal of Social Psychology, 129(5), 659-672. Jones, S.L. (1994). A constructive relationship for religion with the science and profession of psychology perhaps the boldest model yet. American Psychologist, 49(3), 184-199 Jussim, L., Manis, M., Nelson, T. E., & Soffin, S. (1995). Prejudice, stereotypes, and labeling effects: Sources of bias in person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 228-246. Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 774-788. Kelly, T. A. (1990). The role of values in psychotherapy: Review and methodological critique. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 171-186. Kilpatrick, S. D., & McCullough, M. E. (1999). Religion and spirituality in rehabilitation psychology. Rehabilitation Psychology, 44(4), 388-402. Kim, B. S. K., Gladys, F. N., & Ahn, A. J. (2005). Effect of client expectation for counseling success, client-counselor worldview match, and client adherence to Asian and European American cultural values on counseling 188 process with Asian Americans. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(1), 67-76. Koenig, F.W., & King, M. B., Jr. (1964). Cognitive simplicity and out-group stereotypes. Social Forces, 42(3), 324-327. Koenig, F. W. (1997). Is religion good for your health? The effects of religion on physical and mental health. New York: Haworth Press. Korten, F. F. (1973). The stereotype as a cognitive construct. The Journal of Social Psychology, 90, 29-39. Lewis-Hall, E., Gorsuch, R., Malony, H. N., Narramore, S. B., & Stewart Van Leeuwam, M. (2006). Dialogue, embodiment, and the unity of faith and learning: A conversation on integration in a postmodern age. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 25(4), 331-338. Lewis, K. N., & Lewis, D. A. (1985). Impact of religious affiliation on therapists' judgments of patients. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 53(6), 926-932. Long, E. L. (1971). Religious beliefs of American scientists. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Macrae, N. C., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 808-817. Macrae, N. C., & Milne, A. B. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 37-47. 189 McKenzie-Mohr, D., & Zanna, M. P. (1990). Treating women as sexual objects: Look to the (gender schematic) male who has viewed pornography. Merriam-Webster's On Line Dictionary (2003). Retrieved October 12, 2006, from http://www.m-w.com/dictionary/culture Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 296-308. Murray, E. J. (1956). A content-analysis method for studying psychotherapy. Psychological Monographs, 70(13). National Academy of Sciences. (1984). Science and creationism: A view from the National Academy of Sciences. Washington, DC: National Academy Press NewPort, F. (2004). A look at Americans and religion today. Retrieved March 28, 2006, from http://speakingoffaith.publicradio.org/programs/godsofbusiness/galluppoll. shtm Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101-115. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 166-180. O'Connor, S., & Vandenberg, B. (2005). Psychosis or faith? Clinicians' assessment of religious beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(4), 610-616. 190 Perdue, C. W., & Gurtman, M. B. (1990). Evidence for the automaticity of ageism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 199-216. Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 811-832. Ragan, C., Malony, H. N., & Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1980). Psychologists and religion: Professional factors and personal belief. Review of Religious Research, 21, 208-127. Redding, R. E. (2002). Grappling with a diverse conception of diversity. American Psychologist, 57(4), 300-301. Reed, P. R. (1992). Psychologists' views of a patient's religiousness. Psychological Reports, 70, 1031-1036. Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 119-125. Richards, S. P., & Bergin, A. (2000). Handbook of psychotherapy and religious diversity. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Rippentrop, A. E. (2005). A review of the role of religion and spirituality in chronic pain populations. Rehabilitation Psychology, 50(3), 278-284. Rogers, C. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95-103. 191 Rosenbaum, M. E. (1986). The repulsion hypothesis: On the nondevelopment of relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 11561166. Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 17(4), 437465. Sears, G. J., & Rowe, P. M. (2003). A personality-based similar-to-me effect in the employment interview: Conscientiousness, affect versus competencemediated interpretations, and the role of job relevance. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 35(1), 13-24. Sekaquaptewa, D., Espinoza, P., Thompson, M., Vargas, P., & von Hippel, W. (2003). Stereotypic explanatory bias: Implicit stereotyping as a predictor of discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 75-82. Seyle, D. C., & Newman, M. L. (2006). A house divided?: The psychology of Red and Blue America. American Psychologist, 61(6), 571-580. Shafranske, E. D. (1996). Religion and the clinical practice of psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Shafranske, E. P. (1990). Clinical psychologists' religious and spiritual orientations and their practice of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 27, 7278. 192 Shafranske, E. P. (2000). Religious involvement and professional practices of psychiatrists and other mental health professionals. Psychiatric Annals, 30(8), 525-532. Sarason, S. (1993). American psychology, and the needs for transcendence and community. American Journal of Community Psychology, 21(2). Sherman, J. (2005). Automatic and controlled components of implicit stereotyping and prejudice. APA Online Psychological Science Agenda 19, 3. Retrieved March 18, 2005, from http://www.apa.org/science/psa/sbshermanprt.html Sherman, J. W., Conrey, F. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Azam, O. A. (2005). Prejudice and stereotype maintenance processes: Attention, attribution, and individuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 607-622. Sherman, S. J., Mackie, D. M., & Driscoll, D. M. (1990). Priming and the differential use of dimensions in evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(3), 405-418. Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (1999). Reactions to a Black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 885-904. Smith, T. B., McCullough, M. E., & Poll, J. (2003). Religiousness and depression: Evidence for a main effect and the moderating influence of stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 614-636. 193 Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidel, L.S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper Collins. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-47). Monterey, CA: Brooks Cole. Taylor, S.E. & Lobel, M. (1989). Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts. Psychological Review, 96(4), 569-575. Truax, C. B. (1966). Reinforcement and nonreinforcement in Rogerian psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71, 1-9. Viney, W., & King, D. B. (1998). A history of psychology: Ideas and content (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Wadsworth, R. D., & Checketts, K. T. (1980). Influence of religious affiliation on psychodiagnosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 234240. Wallis, J. (2005). God's politics: Why the right gets it wrong and the left doesn't get it. San Francisco: Harper Collins. Watters, W. W. (1992). Deadly doctrine: Health, illness, and Christian God talk. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books. Wester, S. R., & Vogel, D. L. (2002). The emperor's new clothes: Sociopolitical diversity in psychology. American Psychologist, 57(4), 295. 194 Worthington, E. L. (1988). Understanding the values of religious clients: A model and its application to counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35(2), 166-174. Worthington, E. L. (1996). Empirical research on religion and psychotherapeutic processes and outcomes: A 10-year review and research prospectus. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 448-487. Worthington, R., Mobley, M., Franks, R. P., & Andreas Tan, J. (2000). Multicultural counseling competencies: Verbal content, counselor attributions, and social desirability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(4), 460-468. Yangarber-Hicks, N., Behenskyh, C., Canning, S., Flanagan, K., Gibson, N., Hicks, M., Kimball, C., Pak, J., Plante, T. ,Porter, S. (2006). Invitation to the table conversation: A few diverse perspectives on integration. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 25(4), 338-353 Yarhouse, M. A., & Fisher, W. (2002). Levels of training to address religion in clinical practice. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 39(2), 171-176. Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Coull, A. (1999). Fencing off the deviant: The role of cognitive resources in the maintenance of stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 449-462. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1-27 195 Appendix A: Materials Provided to Participants Invitation to Participate in Research Study Dear Licensed Psychologist, I am writing to request your participation in an online research study. This study examines factors and processes associated with clinical judgment which includes a unique measure that I hope you enjoy. This research is intended to complete my requirements for the doctoral dissertation at the Anonymous University in fulfillment of the PhD in Clinical Psychology under the supervision of Anonymous, Ph.D. The requirement for participants is that they must be licensed psychologists. Please note that unfortunately this study is not MAC compatible at this time. I understand that your time is valuable, and as such, your participation is greatly appreciated. At the study’s end, there will be an opportunity to enter two drawings, one each for $100 and $50 prizes of either a contribution in your name to the American Red Cross’s Hurricane Recovery Program that serves victims affected by Hurricanes Katrina, Rita and Wilma, or a cash prize in either amount. The anticipated length of time to take this study is approximately 25 minutes. Again, as data collected in this study is part of my doctoral dissertation, your time and effort is most sincerely appreciated. I hope you find the study interesting! Here is the link to the informed consent and the survey: Sincerely, Anonymous 196 Informed Consent Form Please note that this study is not MAC compatible Factors Associated with Psychologists’ Clinical Judgment You have been asked to participate in a dissertation research study conducted by Anonymous, a doctoral student in the School of Clinical Psychology at Anonymous. Participation in this research is voluntary. As such, the results of this research will be published in Anonymous’s doctoral dissertation and possibly in journals, books, or presentations. It involves the examination of various factors that relate to psychologists’ clinical judgment and is expected to contribute to psychology by providing a more thorough understanding of how those factors affect clinical practice, and essentially increase patient retention and maximize outcomes. This study involves several survey instruments. You will be directed to a page that will request that you download a program that is necessary to accept and store your responses, which will then be transmitted to the website host for data analysis at the conclusion of your participation. Please note the specific security measures that will be taken to ensure that your information is protected and that the downloaded program is used solely for data collection purposes, found later in this consent form. You will first be asked to read and respond to questions about two clinical vignettes. Several other brief instruments will follow, in which you will be asked to respond to items that measure various attitudes that may play a role in explicit processes utilized in making clinical judgments. Last, you will be asked to complete a timed measure that assesses implicit processes also believed to be a contributing factor in making clinical judgments. With the understanding that your time is valuable, a drawing will be held at the study’s end for the chance to win one of two prizes; either a $100 or $50 donation in your name to either the American Red Cross’s Hurricane Recovery Program that serves victims affected by Hurricanes Katrina, Rita and Wilma and others, the charitable organization of your choice, or the option of cash in those amounts. Participation in this study is expected to take approximately 25 minutes. Your responses in this study are kept anonymous and information gathered is used solely for the purposes of this research. Data is transmitted to the host server using SSL, which is standard data encryption technology for secure data transmission on the web. You will not be asked to provide any identifying data, unless you choose to enter the drawing at the study’s end. Personally identifying 197 data entered into the drawings will be separated from the study response data, prior to any review or analysis and discarded immediately following the conclusion of the drawing. All data will be accessible to only the researcher and her committee, and a research assistant who has signed a Confidential Assistance Agreement. There are no known or suspected risks associated with participation in this study. However, should you find anything disturbing about the study, please feel free to exit it at any time. Only data from completed studies will be saved, and if you choose to exit or refuse participation in the study you may do so without penalty. If you would like a summary of the results of this study, or if you have any questions about this research, please feel free to email Anonymous at Anonymous or call (818) 634-9022. An electronic summary of results can be emailed to you at the conclusion of the study at your request. For questions, you may also contact the dissertation committee’s chair, Anonymous, PhD., at Anonymous University. The Institutional Review Board at Anonymous retains access to signed consent forms. As this is an online study, clicking the SUBMIT button below serves as your electronic signature on your agreement to the informed consent terms. You may print and keep a copy of this agreement for your records. Please click the SUBMIT button to signify your informed consent and to be taken to the study. 198 Introduction to Vignettes It is understood that some of the information presented in vignette studies is ambiguous, and information required for carefully considered clinical judgment is lacking. Due to the fact that this study requires the use of brief vignettes, it is acceptable and expected that you project a hypothesis about each patient based on any and all pieces of information given, ambiguous or otherwise. Please read the two following vignettes. Following each, you will be asked to make some clinical judgments based on the information given. 199 Condition I: Vignette #1 Mr. Dean, a 35 year old married Caucasian male, presents to treatment with multiple symptoms. He reports that he suffers from dizziness, sweaty palms, and tension in his chest. He often feels edgy and irritable, and he has been having difficulty focusing at work, where he is a sales manager at a local telecommunications company. He notices that he often has worrisome thoughts that are intrusive and distracting and he has left work on several occasions when they have become intolerable. Mr. Dean formerly spent some time socializing with coworkers on occasional weekends, entertaining at his home or watching sports events with them. He also states that he is an Evangelical Christian who was an active member of the Evangelical Free Church until the last year and a half. He was also politically active in advocating for causes related to his faith and derived satisfaction from participating in activities that he believed represented his faith and God’s will. He complains that he misses social activities with coworkers and religious and political activities. While he is still capable of enjoying these activities, he has often had to leave events when his symptoms became “intolerable”. He has since reduced his outings significantly. He has fears of failing in his job despite having a history of reasonable career success, fears that his wife will leave him, although there is no evidence that she is unhappy, and fears that he is falling short of the expectations of his faith, though he cannot point to evidence that supports these fears. Recently he has become discouraged about this continued pattern and feels that his worry is out of control. He states that when he returns home at the end of the day, he is irritable and tired but feels he must appear “normal” to his wife and children. He expresses concerns that he needs to be seen as “perfect”. Having consulted with physicians on several occasions regarding dizziness, sweaty palms, and muscle tension, Mr. Dean understands that there are no medical causes for his symptoms. He has no therapy experience in his past, and is attending at the urging of a coworker, who has some knowledge of Mr. Dean’s experiences. 200 Condition I: Vignette II Mr. Bowery is a 32 year old divorced Caucasian man who comes to therapy with several complaints. Mr. Bowery reports that worries frequently, and that he his worry has increased in the last year. He states that at times he also has increased heart rate, feels lightheaded, and that his hands shake. He wakens in the early morning hours, feeling agitated and unable to sleep. He is tired during the day and finds it difficult to make decisions at work, where he is a junior architect in a successful company. Historically, his work performance has been marked by achievement and recognition, however he worries that he will be unable to maintain that success and move ahead in his career. He has recently also become afraid that others will notice his symptoms or that they will begin to effect his work. As Mr. Bowery’s career advancement is a primary focus of his life, he is considerably distressed by his fears of failure. Mr. Bowery has enjoyed an active social life, having a circle of good friends with whom he has enjoyed skiing, snowboarding and other activities. He has been married once and was divorced when 27 years old. He reports that he has had two meaningful relationships with women in the past 5 years and that someday he would like to be married again. He reports that he worries that he will not “find the right one” and may not marry again. Mr. Bowery states that he formerly found it rewarding to volunteer his time as a mentor to boys in a local boys’ foster home. He had also developed friendships there with other mentors and felt as though his volunteer activities imparted some special purpose in his life. Mr. Bowery reports being able to enjoy these activities still, except that lately his symptoms have left him feeling fatigued and he has been “unable to keep up” with social and volunteer activities alike. Mr. Bowery also reports that although he used to be excited about his work and his career path, often working overtime on important projects, he is spending less and less time at work. Mr. Bowery has been cleared of any medical diagnosis that may be contributing to his symptoms. He has been feeling down lately as his symptoms continue and is seeking assistance in therapy on the advice of a mentor at the boy’s home. 201 Condition II: Vignette I Mr. Dean, a 35 year old married Caucasian male, presents to treatment with multiple symptoms. He reports that he suffers from dizziness, sweaty palms, and tension in his chest. He often feels edgy and irritable, and he has been having difficulty focusing at work, where he is a sales manager at a local telecommunications company. He notices that he often has worrisome thoughts that are intrusive and distracting and he has left work on several occasions when they have become intolerable. Mr. Dean formerly spent some time socializing with coworkers on occasional weekends, entertaining at his home or watching sports events with them. Mr. Dean states that he formerly found it rewarding to volunteer his time as a mentor to boys in a local boys’ foster home. He had also developed friendships there with other mentors and felt as though his volunteer activities imparted some special purpose in his life. Mr. Dean reports that he would like to participate in these activities still, except that lately his worry and other symptoms have left him feeling fatigued and he has been “unable to keep up” with social and volunteer activities alike. While he is still capable of enjoying these activities, he has often had to leave events when his symptoms became “intolerable”. He has since reduced his outings significantly. He has fears of failing in his job despite having a history of reasonable career success, fears that his wife will leave him although there is no evidence that she is unhappy, and fears that he is falling short of the expectations of his faith, though he cannot point to evidence that supports those fears. Recently he has become discouraged about this continued pattern and feels that his worry is out of control. He states that when he returns home at the end of the day, he is irritable and tired but feels he must appear “normal” to his wife and children. He expresses concerns that he needs to be seen as “perfect”. Having consulted with physicians on several occasions regarding dizziness, sweaty palms, and muscle tension, Mr. Dean understands that there are no medical causes for his symptoms. He has no therapy experience in his past, and is attending on the advice of a mentor at the boy’s home, who has some knowledge of Mr. Dean’s experiences. 202 Condition II: Vignette II Mr. Bowery is a 32 year old divorced Caucasian man who comes to therapy with several complaints. Mr. Bowery reports that he worries frequently, and that he his worry has increased in the last year. He states that at times he also has increased heart rate, feels lightheaded, and that his hands shake. He wakens in the early morning hours, feeling agitated and unable to sleep. He is tired during the day and finds it difficult to make decisions at work, where he is a junior architect in a successful company. Historically, his work performance has been marked by achievement and recognition, however he worries that he will be unable to maintain that success and move ahead in his career. He has recently also become afraid that others will notice his symptoms or that they will begin to effect his work. As Mr. Bowery’s career advancement is a primary focus of his life, he is considerably distressed by his unfounded fears of failure. Mr. Bowery has enjoyed an active social life, having a circle of good friends with whom he has enjoyed skiing, snowboarding and other activities. He has been married once and was divorced when 27 years old. He reports that he has had two meaningful relationships with women in the past 5 years and that someday he would like to be married again. He reports that he worries that he will not “find the right one” and may not marry again. He also states that he is an Evangelical Christian who was an active member of the Evangelical Free Church until the last year and a half. He was also politically active in advocating for causes related to his faith and derived satisfaction from participating in activities that he believed represented his faith and God’s will. He complains that he misses social activities with friends and religious and political activities. Mr. Bowery also reports that although he used to be excited about his work and his career path, often working overtime on important projects, he is spending less and less time at work. He stated that he has left work or volunteer or social activities due to his symptoms. Mr. Bowery has been cleared of any medical diagnosis that may be contributing to his symptoms. He has been feeling down lately as his symptoms continue and is seeking assistance in therapy at the urging of a coworker. 203 Measures Empathy Affective Empathy: Batson’s empathy adjectives Please indicate on a 7-point scale from 1 (not at all) – 7 (extremely) how strongly you feel each emotion when thinking about this patient. 1.) 2.) 3.) 4.) 5.) 6.) sympathetic moved compassionate tender warm softhearted 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 7 Cognitive Empathy: The Perspective Taking Scale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index Please indicate the degree to which the items below describe your response to this patient on a five-point scale running from 0 (does not describe me well) to 4 (describes me very well). 1. I have a hard time seeing things from this patient’s point of view. 0 1 2 3 4 2. I am able to look at this patient’s side of things when making judgments. 0 1 2 34 3. I am able to understand this patient better by imaging how things look from his perspective. 0 1 2 3 4 4. Before making a judgment about this patient, I imagine how it would feel to be in his place. 01234 204 Prognosis Please rate the patient on each of the following items: 1.) Appropriateness for therapy. 0 (least appropriate) – 4 (most appropriate) 34 012 2.) Selection for your caseload. 0 (least likely to select) – 4 (most likely to select) 01234 3.) Severity of impairment. impaired)* 0 1 2 3 4 0 (most severely impaired) – 4 (least severely 4.) Motivation for change. 34 0 (least motivated) – 4 (most motivated) 012 5.) Capacity for insight. insight) 01234 0 (least capacity for insight) – 4 (most capacity for 6.) Likelihood of making substantial progress. 0 (least likelihood) – 4 (most likelihood for making substantial progress) 01234 7.) Number of therapy sessions required for this patient to make substantial progress. 0.) 1-5 1.) 6-10 2.) 11-15 3.) 16-20 4.) 21+ * 8.) Number of sessions you expect this patient will attend therapy 0.) 1-5 1.) 6-10 2.) 11-15 3.) 16-20 4.) 21+ *Reverse coded 205 Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Please indicate the degree to which you agree or disagree with the following statements on a scale ranging from -3 (strongly disagree) to +3 (strongly agree) 1. In today’s society it is important that one not be perceived as prejudiced in any manner. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 2. I always express my thoughts and feelings, regardless of how controversial they might be. R* -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 3. I get angry with myself when I have a thought or feeling that might be considered prejudiced. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 4. If I were participating in a class discussion and a person of another religion expressed an opinion with which I disagreed, I would be hesitant to express my viewpoint. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 5. Going through life worrying about whether you might offend someone is just more trouble than it’s worth. R -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 6. It’s important to me that other people think I’m not prejudiced. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 7. I feel it’s important to behave according to society’s standards. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 8. I’m careful not to offend my friends, but I don’t worry about offending people I don’t know or don’t like. R -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 9. I think it’s important to speak one’s mind rather than worry about offending someone. R -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 10. It’s never acceptable to express one’s prejudices. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 11. I feel guilty when I have a negative thought or feeling about a person of another religion person. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 206 12. When speaking to a person of another religion, it’s important to me that he/she not think I’m prejudiced. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 13. It bothers me a great deal when I think I’ve offended someone, so I’m always careful to consider other people’s feelings. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 14. If I have a prejudiced thought or feeling, I keep it to myself. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 15. I would never tell jokes that might offend others. -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 16. I’m not afraid to tell others what I think, even when I know they disagree with me. R -3, -2, -1, 0, +1, +2, +3 *Reverse coded 207 Social Desirability: Marlowe Crowne- Form C Please read and respond to each statement as either true (T) or false (F) about your own behavior, feelings, or attitude 1. It is sometimes hard for me to go on with my work if I am not encouraged. T F 2. I sometimes feel resentful when I don’t get my way. T F 3. On a few occasions, I have given up doing something because I thought too little of my ability. T F 4. There have been times when I felt like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right. T F 5. No matter who I’m talking too, I’m always a good listener.* T F 6. There have been occasions when I took advantage of someone. T F 7. I’m always willing to admit it when I make a mistake.* T F 8. I sometimes try to to get even rather than forgive and forget. T F 9. I am always courteous, even to people who are disagreeable.* T F 10. I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my own.* T F 11. There have been times when I was quite jealous of the good fortunes of others. T F 12. I am sometimes irritated by people who ask favors of me. T F 13. I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings.* T F *Reverse coded 208 Religious Attitude Scale Select and check for each item the one descriptive phrase that would best describe your attitude. (Scores are indicated in parentheses such that 0 = liberal, 1= conservative, and 2 = orthodox positions) 1. a.) b.) c.) God spiritual, guiding force (1) All-powerful creator of the universe (2) Man-made explanation of the unknown (0) 2. a.) b.) c.) Jesus wise prophet and successful crusader (0) God manifest in man (1) Son of God (2) 3. a.) b.) c.) Holy Ghost third person of the Blessed Trinity (2) God revealed in spiritual form (1) Supposedly a divine being (0) 4. a.) b.) c.) Virgin Mary mother of Jesus (1) Supposedly the mother of a prophet (0) Blessed mother of God (2) 5. a.) b.) c.) Saints agents effecting communication between God and man (2) good people living or having lived Christian lives (1) humans falsely elevated to holiness (0) 6. a.) b.) c.) Angels heavenly beings created in God’s likeness (2) revelation of God’s ways (1) manmade symbols of goodness (0) 7. a.) b.) c.) Devils manmade symbols of evil (0) our temptations to do evil (1) fallen angels (2) 8. a.) b.) c.) Heaven peaceful state of mind (0) the place of eternal happiness for only those who are saved (2) future life in the kingdom of God (1) 9. Hell a.) threat of future punishment for man’s sins (1) 209 b.) our earthly suffering (0) c.) place of eternal punishment for the damned (2) 10. Soul a.) Personality (0) b.) spiritual part of man, linking him to God (1) c.) Immortal, immaterial part of man (2) 11. Sin a.) falling short of our best and our misdeeds towards others (0) b.) transgression against God’s laws (2) c.) breaking an established moral and religious code (1) 12. Salvation a.) saving one’s soul, which is the ultimate end of man’s creation (2) b.) submitting to God’s will (1) c.) having fulfilled one’s purpose in life (0) 13. Miracles a.) illustrations explaining God’s ways (1) b.) unusual occurrences which do have a logical explanation (0) c.) unusual acts produced through the power of God (2) 14. Bible a.) book of history and moral behavior (0) b.) book of reverent religious writings (1) c.) revealed word of God (2) 15. Prayer a.) attempts at magical wish fulfillment (0) b.) religious meditation (1) c.) communication with God (2) 16. Rituals and sacraments a.) means of achieving grace (2) b.) manmade actions for the pleasure of mythical beings (0) c.) symbolic actions during worship (1) 210 Conservatism Scale Please assess your degree of devoutness about your own religious beliefs 0 1 not at all/ slightly devout does not apply 2 3 somewhat very devout devout Please assess your degree of devoutness in following your religious traditions or practices 0 1 not at all/ slightly devout does not apply 2 3 somewhat very devout devout Please assess your attempts to live your life according to your religious scriptures or teachings 0 don’t attempt at all/ does not apply 1 attempt slightly 2 attempt somewhat 3 attempt very much 211 Background Questionnaire Please respond to the following background questions: 1. Sex. A. M B. F 2. Age. A. B. C. D. E. F. G. H. I. 20-29 30-35 36-40 41-45 46-50 51-55 56-60 61-65 66+ 3. Education. A. Doctorate Clinical Psychology B. Doctorate Counseling Psychology C. Doctorate Education or related D. Other 3. Geographic Region A. Northeast B. Southeast C. North D. Midwest E. South F. Northwest G. Southwest H. West 4. Primary Location as urban or rural A. Rural 5. Years in clinical practice A. 1-2 B. 3-5 C. 6-10 D. 11-15 E. 16-20 F. 21-30 G. 31-40 H. 40+ 6. Religious Affiliation A. Buddhist B. Christian-Catholic B. Urban 212 C. Christian- Evangelical D. Christian- Protestant (all other denominations excluding Evangelical) E. Hindu F. Jewish G. Muslim H. None/Atheist/Agnostic I. Other This measure will be the last prior to the IAT. Following this there will be a notice: “You will now be directed to the final portion of the study, which should take approximately 5 minutes to complete. You will be asked to download a small plug-in (Active-X) that will allow for responses to be time. This plug-in cannot and does not gather any data from your computer other than what is required for data collection. I hope that you will continue on through this last piece as partial data is not usable in this study. Thank you for your continued participation!” 213 Implicit Association Test The implicit association test is not a measure, but a method of measuring automatic attitudes. It requires a process of categorization of concepts or words on line or on a dedicated computer. Therefore it cannot be replicated here. Demonstrations of the IAT can be seen at the following site: https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/demo/measureyourattitudes.html That having been said, some text is available. After completion of the measure, participants will be given feedback. An example of this would be: Below is a summary of your average response time for two different configurations: When good words were matched with the Evangelical Christian category, your response time was xxxx milliseconds. When good words were matched with the Secular/No Religion category, your response time was xxxx milliseconds. Did you respond much more quickly to one of the configurations than the other? If so, that configuration may be more consistent with your attitudes about these categories. Please press ENTER to end the study. On the next page a text box reads: Thank you for participating in this study. If you would like to be entered in the drawing for the $50 and the $100 prizes, please type your email address in the box below. Winners will be notified by email. 214 Appendix B: Distribution of Variable Scores Histograms 60 50 Frequency 40 30 20 10 0 0.00 2.50 5.00 7.50 10.00 Social Desirability Figure B1. Distribution of social desirability scores. 12.50 215 100 Frequency 80 60 40 20 0 0.00 2.00 4.00 6.00 8.00 Religious Conservatism Figure B2. Distribution of religious conservatism scores. 10.00 12.00 216 40 Frequency 30 20 10 0 0.00 10.00 20.00 30.00 Liberality of Christian Beliefs Figure B3. Distribution of religious liberality in relation to Christian beliefs scores. 217 100 Frequency 80 60 40 20 0 -20.00 -10.00 0.00 10.00 NMR - EC Affective Empathy Difference Figure B4. Distribution of NMR – EC affective empathy difference scores. 20.00 218 50 Frequency 40 30 20 10 0 -30.00 -20.00 -10.00 0.00 10.00 NMR - EC Cognitive Empathy Difference Figure B5. Distribution of NMR – EC cognitive empathy difference scores. 20.00 219 80 Frequency 60 40 20 0 -15.00 -10.00 -5.00 0.00 5.00 NMR - EC Prognosis Difference Figure B6. Distribution of NMR – EC prognosis scores. 10.00 15.00 220 100 Frequency 80 60 40 20 0 -2000.00 -1000.00 0.00 1000.00 Implicit Negative Associations Figure B7. Distribution of implicit negative association scores. 2000.00 3000.00 221 50 Frequency 40 30 20 10 0 30.00 40.00 50.00 60.00 70.00 80.00 Motivation to Control Prejudice Reactions Figure B8. Distribution of motivation to control prejudice reactions scores. 90.00 222 Appendix C: Examination of Regression Assumptions 100 Frequency 80 60 40 20 0 -5.0 -2.5 0.0 2.5 Regression Standardized Residual 5.0 Regression Standardized Residual 5.0 2.5 0.0 -2.5 -5.0 -3 -2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 2 Figure C1. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with NMR-EC affective empathy difference scores as the dependent variable for hypothesis 1. 223 40 Frequency 30 20 10 0 -2 0 2 Regression Standardized Residual 4 Regression Standardized Residual 4 2 0 -2 -3 -2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 2 Figure C2. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with NMR-EC cognitive empathy difference scores as the dependent variable for hypothesis 1. 224 80 Frequency 60 40 20 0 -4 -2 0 2 Regression Standardized Residual 4 Regression Standardized Residual 4 2 0 -2 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 2 Figure C3. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with NMR-EC prognosis difference scores as the dependent variable for hypothesis 2. 225 80 Frequency 60 40 20 0 -4 -2 0 2 Regression Standardized Residual 4 6 -2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 2 Regression Standardized Residual 6 4 2 0 -2 -4 -3 Figure C4. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with INA scores as the dependent variable for hypothesis 2. 226 120 100 Frequency 80 60 40 20 0 -5.0 -2.5 0.0 2.5 Regression Standardized Residual 5.0 Regression Standardized Residual 5.0 2.5 0.0 -2.5 -5.0 -4 -2 0 2 4 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 6 Figure C5. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with NMR-EC affective empathy differences as the dependent variable for hypothesis 4. 227 50 Frequency 40 30 20 10 0 -2 0 2 Regression Standardized Residual 4 Regression Standardized Residual 4 2 0 -2 -5.0 -2.5 0.0 2.5 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 5.0 Figure C6. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with NMR-EC cognitive empathy differences as the dependent variable for hypothesis 4. 228 80 Frequency 60 40 20 0 -4 -2 0 2 Regression Standardized Residual 4 Regression Standardized Residual 4 2 0 -2 -4 -6 -4 -2 0 2 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 4 Figure C7. Residual normality histogram and residual equality scatterplot for regression analysis with NMR-EC prognosis differences as the dependent variable for hypothesis 5