Download What paradox? A response to Naigles (2002)

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Japanese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Navajo grammar wikipedia , lookup

Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Inflection wikipedia , lookup

Sanskrit grammar wikipedia , lookup

Germanic strong verb wikipedia , lookup

Germanic weak verb wikipedia , lookup

Causative wikipedia , lookup

Chinese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Junction Grammar wikipedia , lookup

Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup

English clause syntax wikipedia , lookup

Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup

Construction grammar wikipedia , lookup

Pleonasm wikipedia , lookup

Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Italian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Russian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup

Hungarian verbs wikipedia , lookup

Sotho verbs wikipedia , lookup

Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup

Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Georgian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Icelandic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Kagoshima verb conjugations wikipedia , lookup

Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup

Pipil grammar wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
317
Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit
Discussion
What paradox? A response to Naigles (2002)
Michael Tomasello a,*, Nameera Akhtar b
a
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Inselstrasse 22, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, CA 95064, USA
1. Introduction
Naigles (2002) claims that there currently exists a paradox in the study of child language
acquisition, which she then proceeds to resolve. But there is no paradox.
1.1. Infant statistical learning
The supposed paradox is that even prelinguistic infants find abstract patterns in speechlike stimuli with relative ease in statistical learning experiments, whereas children struggle
well into the preschool years to find abstract syntactic patterns in experiments involving
meaningful language. But the obvious point is that the findings on statistical learning are
about infants’ skills of auditory perception, not language processing. Thus, infants find
patterns in other kinds of auditory stimuli, for example, musical tones (Saffran, Johnson,
Aslin, & Newport, 1999), and they even show the same ability with sequentially presented
visual stimuli such as colored lights turning on and off (Kirkham, Slemmer, & Johnson, in
press). And then there is the fact that tamarin monkeys find patterns in speech-like stimuli
in much the same way as human infants (Hauser, Weiss, & Marcus, 2002). These facts (the
latter two not cited by Naigles) are of crucial importance in establishing just what is going
on in the statistical learning experiments, namely, that all kinds of primates, including
humans, are incredibly skillful at finding patterns in all kinds of perceptual stimuli.
These pattern-finding skills comprise a necessary component in children’s acquisition
of language – children could not learn the meaningful syntactic constructions of a natural
language without them – and so their existence in prelinguistic infants is among the most
important discoveries in recent years in the study of child language acquisition. But
nobody, not even Naigles, thinks that infants in the statistical learning experiments are
processing language, and so it is a bit odd to compare infants’ ease of perceptual pattern
finding in these studies with their difficulties in finding patterns among meaningful syntactic constructions a year or more later. There is no paradox.
* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M. Tomasello), [email protected] (N. Akhtar).
0010-0277/02/$ - see front matter q 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0 010-0277(03)00 048-9
318
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
1.2. Learning syntactic constructions
One part of Naigles’ attempted resolution is a critique of studies – many of them ours –
which find that children do not make abstractions across syntactic constructions until
relatively late in the acquisition process (often after 3 years). She thinks they make
abstractions earlier – which helps resolve the paradox – but she does not present this
research clearly and accurately. Succinctly put, there are five different methods that all
converge on the conclusion that children’s early language revolves not around abstract
categories and rules, but rather around specific lexical items and expressions (with some
early constructions containing some abstract slots). Naigles discusses four of the five
methods (the fifth is not yet published), and her most general and important criticisms
are two. The first is that children are not learning the meaning of the experimental verbs in
the way that experimenters want them to and think they are. The second is that the
experiments sometimes produce unwanted priming effects. While these criticisms are
valid in some cases (especially some of our earliest studies), they do not hold uniformly
across all the different kinds of studies – and they do not hold at all for two of the methods.
1.2.1. Spontaneous speech
As Naigles reports, children’s early language looks item-specific in basically all of the
world’s languages that have been studied (see Tomasello, 2000, for a brief review). For
example, children learn certain kinds of syntactic constructions with some verbs, other
kinds with other verbs – sometimes with little or no overlap between them. And the learning
process looks piecemeal in the sense that the constructions used with some verbs are much
more complex and advanced than those used with other verbs. One methodological point that
Naigles does not note is that since Tomasello (1992) was a diary study in which a relatively
complete record of one child’s entire language output was recorded (i.e. all new constructions
were recorded), from this study we can make claims with some confidence about what the
child did not do. This is important in establishing the item-specificity of early language since
these data characterize the structure of each construction not only positively but negatively as
well. Nevertheless, data from spontaneous speech are never definitive in identifying underlying cognitive or linguistic representations; experiments are needed.
1.2.2. Nonce verb production studies
The largest body of studies are nonce verb production studies in which children are
taught a novel verb in one linguistic construction and encouraged to produce it in another.
Depending on exactly how one counts, there are over a dozen such studies (with several
times that many experimental conditions) done by a handful of different experimenters –
although our labs have done most of those with the youngest children. The basic finding is
very clear. No matter the kind of construction in which they learn the novel verb, and no
matter how they are encouraged to produce utterances themselves with the novel verb,
young children – below 3–4 years of age – are very conservative; they are loathe to use the
novel verbs in constructions in which they have not heard them used. Even when the most
generous scoring criteria are used (a child uses a novel verb creatively one time no matter
how many times she uses it in the construction in which she heard it used), less than onequarter of 2.5 year olds are productive with the novel verbs and it is not until after 3 years
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
319
of age that the majority of children can produce one syntactically productive utterance
with the novel verb (see Tomasello, 2000, for a review).
Naigles lodges two criticisms of these experiments. The first is that the children are not
learning the meanings of the verbs the way the experimenters want them to, and since
some verbs cannot be used in some constructions children are doing what they should do
with these verbs given the meanings they have attached to them. But the first point is that
older children do not have this problem at all; they are quite productive with the novel
verbs. Why aren’t they misled about the verb’s meaning in the same way as the younger
children? Furthermore, in several studies that Naigles does not cite (e.g. Brooks & Tomasello, 1999; Tomasello & Brooks, 1998) younger children participate in control conditions
in which they hear the novel verbs used in the same target constructions that are being
pulled for in the experimental conditions (typically, the English transitive construction).
And the younger children have no problems in this case. Also, in some of our studies (e.g.
Akhtar & Tomasello, 1997; Dodson & Tomasello, 1998; Tomasello, Akhtar, Dodson, &
Rekau, 1997 – the latter two not cited by Naigles) we tried to make sure that during
training children get a transitive meaning from the syntax of the model without hearing
the verb in the transitive construction itself; for example, we say to them “Look at what
Ernie’s doing to Bert. It’s called dacking.” But this still does not help them to use the novel
verb creatively in the transitive construction.
The other criticism is that because children are hearing and learning the novel verb
multiple times in one construction, the verb’s usage in this construction becomes
entrenched (primed) and this inhibits them from using it in a novel construction. This is
possible, but again it must be noted that if this effect is indeed at work, it does not hold for
older children who are productive with the novel verbs. And there are no data to suggest
that younger children are more subject to priming effects than older children.
1.2.3. Weird word order studies
The priming explanation does not work at all for the weird word order studies (AbbotSmith, Lieven, & Tomasello, 2002; Akhtar, 1999). In these studies children hear novel
verbs used in weird word orders (e.g. “Ernie the car dacking”) and then later are encouraged to use them with new characters. Older children tend to correct these novel word
orders, saying things like “Bert’s dacking the tree” whereas younger children are much
more likely to continue using the novel word order. On the surface priming could be a
problem here also since children hear the weird word order multiple times (though again it
doesn’t seem to bother the older children), however there is an important control condition
that Naigles neglects to mention. Children are also given known English verbs in weird
word orders exactly the same number of times (e.g. “Ernie the car pushing”), and even the
youngest subjects correct these to SVO order almost all the time. This effectively rules out
priming as the sole explanation of the findings in the main experimental conditions.
1.2.4. Nonce verb comprehension studies
With the nonce verb comprehension studies, the issue is again verb meaning (and possibly
priming) in the sense that in most comprehension experiments children have to first learn the
verbs and then act them out in response to requests to do so. Perhaps during learning they do
not learn the right meaning. But because we were worried about this ourselves, in Akhtar and
320
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
Tomasello (1997) we used a new method. We did not really teach the verb at all in any
construction. What we did was teach the activity that one does with an apparatus and two
characters: on repeated occasions one character acted in a particular way on another on this
apparatus, with the characters being manipulated on many occasions by the children themselves. The children thus knew the game. Then, on their first hearing of the novel verb, we
pushed the apparatus in front of them in preparation for a new round of the game, handed them
two characters and told them “Make Ernie meek Bert”. The only question here was which
character was made to be the agent and which the patient; the children knew the game. But
again the pattern of results was the same: the older children did this well, whereas the younger
children were basically at chance. We believe this study is a particularly strong demonstration
since (i) it does not involve children having to produce just-learned words or overcome other
performance demands, (ii) there are no priming effects, and (iii) there is no question of
problems with intended verb meaning.
1.2.5. A priming study
Finally, we have one study that is still in press that looks at priming directly (Savage,
Lieven, Theakston, & Tomasello, in press). Following Bock and others who have studied
priming in adults (e.g. Bock & Griffin, 2000), we looked for evidence of structural priming
in young children. The basic idea is that children look through a book containing pairs of
pictures. For each pair they first hear (and in some conditions produce themselves by
repeating) one described with, for example, a passive sentence (e.g. “The car got smashed
by the tree”); then they are asked to describe the second picture of the pair. The second
picture contains different characters and different actions, and so the question is whether
children will also use a passive sentence for this picture even though there will be very low
lexical overlap with what they just heard for the first member of the pair (if they are going
to construct a passive sentence it will have to be something like “The balloon got popped
by the pin”). The answer is, once again, older children are subject to such structural
priming effects, but younger children – below 4 years of age – are not. However, and
importantly, in other experimental conditions the younger children were subject to priming effects when the sentence they heard/used for the first member of the pair contained
pronouns and other closed class morphemes that they could repeat in their own sentence
for the second picture (e.g. in both cases “He got VERBed by it”). This is actually
relatively direct evidence that children’s underling representations contain specific lexical
content, in this case pronouns as arguments and some closed class morphemes constitutive
of particular syntactic constructions.
Although there is only this one study using this methodology with young children, we
believe it is especially telling because in this study children do not have to learn anything
new; they simply use English verbs they already know in constructions they already know
with those verbs. And priming is of course a widely accepted method for establishing the
nature of underlying cognitive representations in Cognitive Science in general.
1.3. Preferential looking
Although we have argued that these various studies do not conflict with studies of
children’s pattern-finding abilities in perceptual tasks (such as the statistical learning
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
321
experiments with infants), they may – and we emphasize may – conflict to some degree –
and we emphasize to some degree – with findings from studies using a preferential looking
methodology. In this method children hear a sentence such as “Ernie is gorping Bert” and
are simultaneously exposed to two videos with different ongoing actions, only one of
which matches the sentence. No method is perfect – not this one nor any of those outlined
above – but we will not get into criticisms of the method itself or the studies done with it
(we have done this already to some degree in Tomasello & Abbot-Smith, 2002). Here we
simply point out that the most straightforward use of this methodology to test the questions
being asked by the above-listed methodologies has never been done. That is, no one has
ever simply presented children with the sentence “Ernie is gorping Bert” and two videos in
one of which Ernie is doing something to Bert and in the other of which Bert is doing
something to Ernie. This would be a direct test of English-speaking children’s understanding of the transitive construction, including its characteristic ordering of agent and
patient. 1
If such a study and/or related studies are done and the finding is that children show skills
at a somewhat younger age than in the other methodologies listed above, that will be
important, albeit not so surprising, information. It will not be so surprising because, for all
the reasons Naigles enumerates, comprehension precedes production in many areas of
cognitive development. This is especially true when looking measures are used to assess
comprehension, for reasons that are not 100% clear (e.g. see Hood, Carey, & Prasada,
2000).
1.4. The situation as we see it
Presumably everyone would agree that the primary questions in the field are not about
age but about process. In our view, all evidence points to the fact that young children’s
syntactic constructions become abstract in a piecemeal fashion, mostly independently of
one another, based on what they have heard in the input for each of the constructions (note
that this is even true of the transitive and intransitive constructions studied in preferential
looking studies; see Tomasello, in press; Tomasello & Abbot-Smith, 2002). In the beginning, there are no abstract linguistic categories such as agent and patient, much less subject
and object, that cut across all of their language. This is a genuine empirical discovery
about children’s early language, and, following in the footsteps of Braine (1976) and
others, we are proud to have contributed to it (see also Lieven, Pine, & Baldwin, 1997;
Pine, Lieven, & Rowland, 1998). Importantly, we are also now beginning to discover –
using training methodologies – the precise amount and nature of linguistic experience
children need to make syntactic abstractions in particular cases (for example, see AbbotSmith, Lieven, & Tomasello, 2003; Childers & Tomasello, 2001).
It would also seem to be the case that even within a construction children may show
evidence for abstract linguistic representations at different ages in different experimental
322
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
methodologies. For example, even we were surprised by how old children were before
they showed effects from structural priming. One interpretation of this pattern of results is
that the different methodologies are sensitive to different levels and types of construction
strength (based mainly on token frequency of the construction in the child’s linguistic
experience) and construction abstractness (based on type frequency, mainly with respect
to verbs). And it is even possible for young children to have partially abstract knowledge
of constructions. For example, children’s earliest representations of the English transitive
construction may be based on an association between the number of arguments and the
semantic feature “cause” (Fisher, 1996), as well as on knowledge about how specific
lexical items (e.g. case-marked pronouns) combine with abstract linguistic categories
(Childers & Tomasello, 2001). And so even within constructions development may
proceed in a somewhat piecemeal fashion as well.
Naigles’ central claim is captured in her title: “Form is easy but meaning is hard”. But
perceptual pattern recognition, as occurs in statistical learning experiments, does not
involve linguistic form – only sounds (sometimes involving “speech-like stimuli”). The
syntactic abstractions involved in language processing – as in the language learning
experiments involving nonce verbs and the like – are concerned with complex linguistic
symbols known as syntactic constructions, which comprise both a form and a function
(Goldberg, 1995). What is hard for nascent language learners, apparently, is finding
patterns in the way complex and meaningful linguistic symbols are constructed and
used in the language they are learning.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Julian Pine, Fanklin Chang, Elena Lieven, Kirsten Abbot-Smith,
Adele Goldberg, and Gina Conti-Ramsden for helpful comments.
References
Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2002). What preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical word orders. Cognitive Development, 16, 679–692.
Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M (2003). Some factors affecting acquisition of the English transitive
construction. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring basic word order: evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal
of Child Language, 26, 339–356.
Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children’s productivity with word order and verb morphology.
Developmental Psychology, 33, 952–965.
Bock, J. K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: transient activation or implicit
learning?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177–192.
Braine, M (1976). Children’s first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41(1, Serial No. 164).
Brooks, P. J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Developmental Psychology, 35, 29–44.
Childers, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of the English
transitive construction. Developmental Psychology, 37, 739–748.
Dodson, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Acquiring the transitive construction in English: the role of animacy and
pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 25, 555–574.
M. Tomasello, N. Akhtar / Cognition 88 (2003) 317–323
323
Fisher, C. (1996). Structural limits on verb mapping: the role of analogy in children’s interpretations of sentences.
Cognitive Psychology, 31, 41–81.
Fisher, C (2000). Who’s blicking whom[quest ]: word order in early verb learning. Poster presented at the 11th
International Conference on Infant Studies, Brighton, UK.
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: a construction grammar approach to argument structure, Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Hauser, M. D., Weiss, D., & Marcus, G. (2002). Rule learning by cotton-top tamarins. Cognition, 86, B15–B22.
Hood, B. M., Carey, S., & Prasada, S. (2000). Do 2-year-olds predict physical events that young infants seem to
know?. Child Development, 71, 1540–1554.
Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P (in press). Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a
domain general learning mechanism. Cognition.
Lieven, E., Pine, J., & Baldwin, G. (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal
of Child Language, 24, 187–220.
Naigles, L. R. (2002). Form is easy, meaning is hard: resolving a paradox in early child language. Cognition, 86,
157–199.
Pine, J., Lieven, E., & Rowland, G. (1998). Comparing different models of the development of the English verb
category. Linguistics, 36, 4–40.
Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by
human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27–52.
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M (in press). Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic
representations: lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental
Science.
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: a case study of early grammatical development, Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence?. Cognition, 74, 209–253.
Tomasello, M (in press). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2002). A tale of two theories: response to Fisher. Cognition, 83, 207–214.
Tomasello, M., Akhtar, N., Dodson, K., & Rekau, L. (1997). Differential productivity in young children’s use of
nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language, 24, 373–387.
Tomasello, M., & Brooks, P. J. (1998). Learning to produce passive utterances through discourse. First
Language, 53, 223–237.