Download 1 Killing with a Clean Conscience: Existential Angst and the

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Identity formation wikipedia , lookup

Social perception wikipedia , lookup

Group dynamics wikipedia , lookup

In-group favoritism wikipedia , lookup

Albert Bandura wikipedia , lookup

Lawrence Kohlberg wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
1
Killing with a Clean Conscience: Existential Angst and the Paradox of Morality
Gilad Hirschberger
Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya
Tom Pyszczynski
University of Colorado, Colorado Springs
"Self-interest, or rather self-love, or egoism, has been more plausibly substituted as the
basis of morality.”—Thomas Jefferson
Two thousand years ago, in the wake of the unsuccessful Bar Kochbah revolt against the
Romans, a debate ensued between the disciples of two Jewish sages, Rabbi Akiva and Rabbi
Ishmael. The students of Rabbi Akiva, a messianic ultra-nationalist who inspired the revolt
against the Romans, interpreted their teacher's position to say that the laws and restrictions of the
bible pertain only to fellow Jews and do not apply to interactions with non-Jews. Rabbi Ishmael,
a universalist who advocated moderation and dialogue with the Romans, delivered a starkly
different message to his disciples and emphasized the "humanness" of all people. He contended
that biblical law was intended for all people, and that “thou shalt not kill” applies to Jews and
non-Jews alike (Arad, 2010, personal communication).
This ancient debate reflects a conflict between competing conceptions of morality that
persists today. To whom should moral consideration be extended? In most intergroup conflicts,
each side views its opposition to the other as a morally righteous and religiously sanctioned
2
response to the evils of the other side. Thus, the moral issues that arise in the context of clashes
between groups often hark back to the ancient debate between Rabbis Akiva and Ishmael.
Our discussion is intended to shed light on issues that have been debated by generations
of theologians, moral philosophers, and social scientists. Do morals encourage people to
transcend egotistical concerns and consider the condition of the "other," or do they serve the
opposite function of glorifying one's self-image and group’s status under the guise of benevolent
concern? Do morals reflect context-dependent and relativistic prescriptions for human behavior
or universal rules of human conduct? How can people commit acts, under the guise of moral
righteousness, that most post outsiders would view as immoral and even evil? How do the grouplevel and individual-level functions of moral behavior relate to each other?
In this chapter we focus primarily on the role of moral concerns in intergroup conflicts
and the potential of moral principles to both defuse and intensify conflicts between groups.
Paradoxically, moral strivings often increase the viciousness of interactions between people with
different group identities, partly because of the diverse moral principles cultures use to govern
behavior. Although moral principles are typically thought of as promoting more humane and
other-oriented behavior (e.g., Aquino et al., 2009), they can also promote the defense of one’s
own group, which often includes vanquishing another group, perceived as evil. To shed light on
these issues, we use terror management theory (TMT; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon,
1997) to explain how moral principles, which promote group cohesion, are used by in the selfregulation of individuals (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981). We suggest that moral behavior
involves implicit calculations aimed at enabling an individual to maintain self-esteem by means
of enhancing a moral self-image. This often entails sacrificing one set of moral values for
another, which in many cases leads to a ‘righteous’ crusade or jihad against another group.
3
We begin with an overview of TMT and consider contemporary theories of morality
through the lens of this perspective. We then discuss how terror management concerns affect the
self-regulatory processes through which people control their own behavior to meet the dictates of
their culture. We then describe ongoing research on terror management and morality that
examines moral perceptions and judgments in the context of intractable political conflict. In light
of this research, we go back to the fundamental question of whether there are universal
conceptions of right, wrong, good, and evil, and whether moral concerns can influence the course
of violent intractable political conflict.
Terror Management Theory
TMT contends that the uniquely human awareness of the inevitability of personal death
generates the potential for overwhelming terror because it frustrates the very basic desire to live.
This potential for terror drives people to construct their reality in a way that infuses life with
meaning, value, and hope of transcending death. Terror management research has demonstrated
that investing in a cultural worldview and defending it against threats is an essential means of
defending against existential terror. Cultural worldviews are symbolic, social constructions of
reality that (a) imbue life with meaning and order, and answer basic questions about existence;
(b) establish standards of value that if met provide a sense of personal self-esteem and group
value; and (c) give hope of life continuing after physical death, either literally in the form of an
afterlife or symbolically in the form of contributions to something greater and more enduring
than the physical self. Thus, to achieve psychological equanimity in the face of mortality, people
need to feel that they are valuable contributors to a meaningful and important world as defined
by their culture.
4
Because this protection from existential terror is provided by ideas, concepts, values, and
symbols, the existence of others who share one’s worldview lends credibility to it and increases
its ability to protect one from anxiety. Problems arise, however, when one encounters people or
ideas that threaten the validity of cherished beliefs about oneself and one's group because this
undermines the social consensus supporting one’s worldview and self-esteem, thereby reducing
their ability to quell anxiety.
To cope with the threat to emotional security posed by groups with different worldviews,
people often try to convert others to their worldview (e.g., missionary work) or incorporate the
non-threatening aspects of a competing worldview into their own pre-existing worldview (e.g.,
the recent increase in the popularity of Americanized Middle-Eastern food). Another strategy is
to derogate the adherents of competing worldviews (e.g., referring to Islam as an "evil religion")
or to employ violent means to defeat or completely annihilate those who pose a significant
challenge to one's core beliefs. Although most wars and violent conflicts involve disputes over
territory, resources, or other concrete issues, they also invariably involve a clash of worldviews.
We argue that protecting oneself from existential fear motivates violent, aggressive responses
when worldviews are challenged. Perceiving a severe threat to core beliefs and values often
precipitates a lifting of moral prohibitions against killing that exist in the vast majority of
cultures.
Empirical Support for TMT
To date over 400 studies conducted in diverse cultures in over 21 countries have
provided convergent evidence for hypotheses derived from TMT (see Greenberg, Solomon, &
Arndt, 2008, for a comprehensive review). Much of this evidence comes from studies showing
5
that reminders of death (mortality salience, or MS) cause people to bolster their defenses of their
cultural worldview, in an effort to defend self-esteem, and to maintain their close relationships.
Research has also implicated terror management processes in promoting violent solutions
to political conflicts. Specifically, MS primes (a) increased support among American
conservatives for extreme military interventions in the “war on terror,” including nuclear and
chemical weapons that would kill thousands of civilians (Pyszczynski et al., 2006, Study 1); (b)
increased support among Iranian students for suicidal terrorism against Western targets
(Pyszczynski, et al., 2006, Study 2); (c) increased support among Israelis for a pre-emptive
nuclear strike on Iran (Hirschberger, Pyszczynski, & Ein-Dor, 2009); and (d) increased Israelis'
support of violent retributions for acts of violence conducted against them, even if the acts of
violence were deemed ineffective (Hirschberger, Pyszczynski, & Ein-Dor, 2010). These studies
suggest that in times of war and terrorist threat, when death-related concerns are likely to be
more prominent than usual, and when hostility toward one’s culture on the part of another group
is rampant, people are especially likely to consider violent solutions attractive as means of
resolving conflicts.
This research implies that when increased protection from existential fear is needed due
to increased salience of death and vulnerability, as is typically the case in times of war and
conflict, people will work especially hard to meet the standards prescribed by their cultural
worldviews. But cultural worldviews and the standards they prescribe are complex and multifaceted. According to theories of self-regulation (Carver & Scheier, 1981), self-regulatory efforts
are oriented toward meeting whichever of the many standards a person holds is most salient or
accessible at the time. From a terror management perspective, conflict and war highlight the need
to defeat an opposing worldview, but also to perceive one's group as having the upper moral
6
hand. Because violence against another group is often at odds with values of compassion, or with
self-perceptions of moral superiority, managing conflicting moral motivations may explain the
many paradoxes that violent conflict entails.
Research has demonstrated that even in the context of violent political conflict,
highlighting moral values that promote peaceful attitudes such as compassion (Rothschild,
Abdolhossein, & Pyszczynski, 2009), shared humanity with others (Pyszczynski et al., 2010),
and secular pacifist norms (Jonas et al., 2008, Study 2) can moderate the impact of MS on
support for violent solutions to conflict. For example, Pyszczynski et al. (2010) found that
inducing Americans, Palestinian citizens of Israel, or Israeli Jews to consider the shared global
consequences of climate change reversed the increase in support for violence that MS produced
under control conditions, causing MS to increase support for peace, and these effects emerged
even at the height of Israeli military action in Gaza in 2009. These findings suggest that in
addition to encouraging defense of one’s worldview, which sometimes involves increased
hostility toward outgroup members, reminders of death also encourage greater adherence to
fundamental cultural values, which in most cases include sanctions against violence and
encouragement of compassion for others. Although most people in modern cultures think of
violence as immoral and peace-making and compassion as moral, recent theories of moral
behavior suggest that both forms of behavior can stem from striving for morality.
Fundamental Foundations of Morality
Moral foundations theory (Haidt & Graham, 2007) identifies five foundational themes
that characterize the moral domain: harm/care, fairness/reciprocity, ingroup/loyalty,
authority/respect, and purity/sanctity (for a full description of the five themes see Graham &
Haidt, this volume). From this perspective, gut level moral intuitions are the primary cause of
7
reactions to morally significant persons and situations, and they play an important role in driving
more abstract moral reasoning. Haidt and Graham argue that all humans react emotionally, to a
greater or lesser extent, to behavior and situations that impinge on all five of these dimensions of
morality. They also argue that whereas politically liberal individuals are especially committed to
the harm/care and fairness/reciprocity dimension, conservatives are roughly equally committed
to all five, which means the place more emphasis than do liberals on ingroup/loyalty,
authority/respect, and purity/sanctity (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009).
Looking back at over 25 years of terror management research in light of Haidt and
Graham's (2007) conceptualization suggests that moral concerns have been a primary focus of
this research and are likely to be important sources of protection from existential anxiety. For
instance, in the first TMT study (Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon ,1989),
municipal court judges who were reminded of their own mortality recommended higher bonds
for an alleged prostitute compared to judges in the control condition, especially if the judges
were morally opposed to prostitution. This finding indicates that MS increases moral judgments
that involve Haidt and Graham's (2007) purity dimension. Research conducted by Goldenberg
and her colleagues (e.g., Cox et al., 2007; Goldenberg et al., 2001) indicates that MS increases
feelings of disgust towards various bodily functions and sensations – further suggesting that
death concerns amplify responses related to the moral purity dimension.
Research pertaining to the fairness dimension of morality has shown that MS increases
concerns with fairness (van den Bos & Miedema, 2000), such as greater agreement with hatecrime legislation (Lieberman, Arndt, Personious, & Cook, 2001; Study 1). But other research
indicates that MS leads to more blaming of innocent victims (Hirschberger, 2006). Thus, it
appears that MS does not necessarily lead people to support the fair treatment of others. Rather,
8
it increases the motivation to believe in a just world and act in a manner that supports this belief
– in some cases showing compassion and in other cases setting it aside (Hirschberger, 2009).
Just as MS does not indiscriminately increase the desire for fairness, it also does not
uniformly increase the motivation to care for others. Research indicates that MS increases
prosocial responses only toward worldview-relevant causes (Jonas, Schimel, Greenberg, &
Pyszczynski, 2002), and only when the prosocial cause is perceived as non-threatening
(Hirschberger, Ein-Dor, & Almakias, 2008). Thus, MS increases other-oriented responses most
clearly when they benefit one’s own group, worldview, or self-esteem. When the prosocial cause
is non-beneficial or even threatening to oneself, death concerns often induce selfish,
ethnocentric, or callous responses to the plight of others (Hirschberger, 2009).
Research also shows that death concerns influence responses to those high and low on
social hierarchies corresponding to Haidt and Graham's authority dimension. For example,
Landau et al. (2004) found that MS led to increased support for President George W. Bush in the
months after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks in the United States, and Cohen et al.
(2004) found that MS increased support for a hypothetical charismatic leader over a merely
competent one.
Perhaps the most thoroughly investigated influence of death concerns on morality is in
the realm of intergroup relations. Numerous terror management studies have shown that MS
increases in-group loyalty and out-group rejection (for a review see Greenberg et al., 2008).
Recently, this research has been extended to the study of political conflict and to the moralityladen decisions people make when deciding how to deal with an adversarial group (e.g.,
Hirschberger & Pyszczynski, in press).
9
Moral foundations theory seems particularly useful in understanding recent TMT findings
related to political conflict, because ethical and moral dilemmas involve conflicts between
different specific moral standards to which individuals are committed to varying degrees, and
between the welfare of the individual, the in-group, and the out-group (Haidt & Graham, 2007).
Because behavior is regulated on an individual level and is motivated by the death anxiety
aroused when individuals feel they are falling short of their moral standards, these conflicts are
usually resolved by doing whatever is most likely to enable one to view oneself as a moral
person. In this sense, moral behavior often appears to be selfish or ethnocentric. This tendency is
exacerbated by the importance that cultures and individuals place on morality over other
dimensions of personal value (e.g., Baumeister, 1991; Haidt & Kesbir, 2007).
We suggest that moral values are imbued with such extreme significance because, in the
vast majority of cultures, they are seen as having been directly delivered to humankind by a deity
that can provide or refuse access to literal immortality. Cultures began to link moral behavior to
one’s fate after death very early in the history of religion (Wright, 2009). This contingency
between moral behavior and one’s fate after death was a powerful inducement to look beyond
one’s selfish concerns to the welfare of others, at least others in one’s own group. If the absolute
annihilation that death might entail is the ultimate fear, then the promise of continued life after
death is the ultimate incentive for behavior that benefits the group. We suspect that the
juxtaposition of a supernatural force with concerns about cleanliness, purity, and disgust might
have been a human innovation that gained popularity, in part, because of the increased control it
gave group leaders over members.
Cleanliness – in all of its meanings – is next to godliness! And human beings want to be
as close to the gods as they can get. As Goldenberg et al. (2001) suggested, one of the primary
10
means through which people deny their mortality is by distancing themselves from other
animals, denying their creatureliness, and imbuing their lives with a spiritual dimension. All
three major Western religions subscribe to the belief that “God gave man dominion over all
things” (Genesis 1:26). By construing human life as rooted in a spiritual realm, people distance
themselves from and elevate themselves above everything that dies. A large body of research has
documented the role that spiritual longings and distancing oneself from animals play in denying
death (for a review, see Goldenberg et al., 2001).
We contend that this concordance of in-group loyalty with maintaining purity plays a
significant role in the moral processes that govern intractable intergroup conflicts. In the next
section we consider how the regulation of moral behavior can both inflame and defuse such
conflicts. We focus especially on the ongoing conflict in the Middle East, which impinges in
various ways on the entire world.
Morality in Times of Conflict
Moral issues lie at the heart of virtually all intergroup conflicts. Although selfish and
ethnocentric desires for resources, territory, and tangible goods are usually involved, what is
typically at stake is who has the right to these things, who deserves them, who has treated whom
unfairly, and how the deity wants these resources to be distributed. Given the security that people
acquire from viewing themselves as morally righteous, it is no surprise that all sides to conflicts
construe themselves as righteously fighting against undeserving, unjust, evil usurpers of their
rights, a fight in which the deity and all that is good are invested in their victory.
War and violent conflict diminish a person’s ability to achieve a balance between one’s
own interests and the needs of others, because during conflicts the stakes are high and threat
looms large. Consequently, the motivation to uphold a moral self sometimes leads people to
11
construe the conflict in a way that enables them to maintain a moral self-image while engaging in
behavior that violates even their most sacred values. Terror-management concerns play a role in
the processes of moral amplification, moral disengagement, and the maintenance of moral
identity, all of which serve to justify acts of dubious morality.
Moral Amplification
Moral amplification refers to "the motivated separation and exaggeration of good and evil
in the explanation of behavior" (Haidt & Algoe, 2004, p. 323). In times of war and elevated
violence, moral amplification is a powerful justification mechanism, because people are
motivated to view conflict as a battle of light against darkness in which they or their group are
guardians of the light. This leads them to dismiss complex explanations that include a
consideration of the grievances of the other side and to avoid considering ways that their own
group may have contributed to the perpetuation of conflict. We contend that this need to perceive
the other as the epitome of evil is not driven by an objective appraisal of the facts, but by the
existential benefits of ingroup loyalty and purity (Haidt & Graham, 2007).
Research suggests that when death is salient moral amplification increases. First, MS
increases the need for a structured, meaningful world and decreases tolerance for ambiguity
(Landau, Johns, et al., 2004). These findings suggest that under conditions of MS, people prefer
to perceive the world in clear-cut terms, with little ambiguity or complexity. Second, MS is
known to increase punishment meted out to legal and moral transgressors (Florian & Mikulincer,
1997; Rosenblatt et al., 1989), and to increase the need to believe in a just world (Hirschberger,
2006). These findings suggest that the perception of a structured and unambiguous world
facilitates the exaggeration of differences between good and evil.
12
Recently, we have collected data directly linking moral amplification to the Middle East
conflict. This research indicated that reminders of death and the Holocaust led Israeli Jews to
perceive Israeli Arabs as having malevolent intentions toward them and toward the State of Israel
(Hirschberger, Canetti, Pyszczynski, Kahn & Gubler , 2010). When death was salient, Israeli
Jews simplified the complex identity of Israeli Arabs, who are typically torn between their
conflicting identities as Palestinians and Israelis (e.g., Kimhi, Canetti, & Hirschberger, 2009),
and preferred to perceive them in simple, clear-cut terms -- as enemies of Israel. This process of
moral amplification via social categorization may support discriminatory attitudes toward
minority groups.
Moral Disengagement
Given the nearly universal moral injunctions against harming others, it may not be
enough to amplify and exaggerate moral differences between us and them. Research conducted
in the past two decades suggests that an additional mechanism, moral disengagement, often
facilitates support for violent conduct. In the words of Albert Bandura “self-sanction plays a
central role in the regulation of inhumane conduct … self-sanctions can be disengaged by
reconstruing conduct as serving moral purposes, by obscuring personal agency in detrimental
activities, by disregarding or misrepresenting the injurious consequences of one’s actions, or by
blaming and dehumanizing the victim” (1998, p. 161). All of these activities help to justify
transgressions against humane moral values.
Moral disengagement has been studied in the context of war and terrorism and was found
to be positively related to support for military attacks against Iraq and Yugoslavia (McAlister,
2001), and to be related to support for harsher punishments for the perpetrators of the 9/11
attacks (Aquino, Reed, Thau, & Freeman, 2007). In a large, representative US sample studied at
13
the time of the 9/11 attacks, moral disengagement completely mediated the influence of the
September 11 attacks on support for immediate retaliatory strikes (McAlister, Bandura, & Owen,
2006). Based on research showing that reminding Americans of the 9/11 attacks increases the
accessibility of death-related thoughts (Landau et al., 2004), McAlister et al.’s findings (2006)
can be viewed as a dramatic field demonstration of the influence of death reminders on support
for retaliatory violence.
One of the central mechanisms of moral disengagement is dehumanization -- divesting
the victim of all human qualities. Research has shown that dehumanization of a target increases
aggressiveness towards the target (Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, & Regalia, 2001),
and that when a group is dehumanized people do not feel obliged to apply moral standards to
them (e.g., Bar-Tal, 1990). However, anti-racist norms and self-monitoring are likely to prevent
people from endorsing blatant dehumanization. Instead, they are more likely to view victimized
others as less than fully human or as infrahuman. Studies of infrahumanization using a subtle
task examining the attribution of emotions to in-group and out-group members (Leyens et al.,
2000) has shown that reminding people of killings conducted by their group led them to
infrahumanize the victimized group (Castano & Giner-Sorolla, 2006).
Infrahumanization serves a central role in the TMT analysis of moral behavior not only
because thoughts of death increase the derogation of worldview violators, but also because the
need to elevate oneself and one's group above animal creatureliness and to affirm one's unique,
non-animalistic humanity serves an important existential function (Goldenberg et al., 2001). In
support of the role of these processes in infrahumanization, research has shown that MS
increased humanization of the in-group in three studies conducted with people from three
different cultures (Vess, Heflick, & Goldenberg, 2008).
14
In a related vein, a provocative study has recently indicated that the deaths of outgroup
members who threaten one’s worldview can serve a terror management function (Hayes,
Schimel, & Williams, 2008). In this study, Christian participants read either a worldviewthreatening news article about the Muslimization of Nazareth or a non-threatening article. Half of
the participants were informed at the end of the article that a large group of Muslims had died in
a recent plane crash on their way to Nazareth. Results indicated that reading about the
Muslimization of Nazareth increased the accessibility of death-related thoughts and the defense
of participants' Christian worldview. However, these increases were not observed among
participants who learned that many Muslims died in a plane crash. This finding suggests that not
only does MS facilitate the use of lethal violence against out-group members, as previous
dehumanization research had shown, but also that the death of worldview violators can be
soothing and reassuring. This disturbing finding may explain the bloodthirsty nature of much
violent conflict.
However, there is also a more optimistic side to the TMT-infrahumanization link. Motyl,
Hart, and Pyszczynski (2010) recently demonstrated that whereas MS increased support for a
pre-emptive strike on Iran among Americans high in religious fundamentalism, when violence
was infrahumanized – described as an animalistic behavior that humans share with other animals
– MS no longer increased support for violence. Thus, when faced with the association between
violence and animality, MS led participants to view violence as repugnant. This finding suggests
that the same desire to view oneself as a transcendent spiritual being that sometimes increases
violence toward an outgroup can also be used as an antidote to such violence.
If one way to reduce intergroup violence is by infrahumanizing violence, another way
might be to humanize an adversarial out-group. A series of studies conducted on Americans,
15
Israeli Arabs, and Israeli Jews demonstrated that when people are induced to consider the shared
humanity between their group and other groups, MS decreases support of violence and increases
support for peace and reconciliation between groups (Pyszczynski et al., 2010).
The Paradox of Moral Identity
Recent research documents a variety of ways in which the motivation to maintain a
positive moral identity can lead to behavior that is callous or even harmful to others. Moral
identity refers to the extent to which moral beliefs and values are either chronically present or
contextually activated, and it has been shown to influence moral conduct. Moral identity is
generally thought to promote the upholding of moral standards (Blasi, 1984). Consistent with
this view, research has indicated that construing one’s moral identity as important results in
greater concern for the needs and welfare of others (Aquino & Reed, 2002). It seems that a
strong moral identity fosters a more inclusive attitude toward others and extends group
boundaries to include people who are typically excluded from one’s in-group (see Haslam, this
volume). Because people are motivated to maintain self-consistency (e.g., Blasi, 1984), those
with a strong moral identity should be motivated to uphold moral values and behave as moral
exemplars in order to maintain a positive self-image (Aquino, Freeman, Reed, Lim, & Felps,
2009). Moreover, when the accessibility of one’s moral identity is enhanced, moral behaviors
should be more likely.
Research generally supports this idea and indicates that priming moral identity increases
intentions to behave in a prosocial manner (Aquino et al., 2009). In a conceptually similar study,
participants seized the opportunity to behave unethically when it was possible, unless they were
first asked to read a moral code, which eliminated these effects (Shu et al., 2009). Reading the
moral code presumably activated participants’ moral identities, which reduced unethical
16
behavior. Priming moral identity may also counter the effects of moral disengagement in
response to war crimes committed by one’s in-group (Aquino et al., 2007).
However, recent research suggests that it may be premature to conclude that activating a
person’s moral identity always increases prosocial behavior. Because behaving ethically
enhances moral identity, the motivation to behave ethically following a moral identity boost may
actually be diminished because the need to view oneself as a moral person has already been
satisfied. In this case a strong sense of moral identity may ironically license unethical attitudes
and behaviors (Mazar & Zhong, 2010). Research supports this contention and indicates that after
participants acted in a gender-egalitarian manner and validated their moral identity, they
subsequently displayed more gender-discriminatory behavior (Monin & Miller, 2001, and
Chapter X, this volume). Apparently, behaving morally satisfied moral identity needs and
diminished the motivation to continue behaving morally. In another study, reminding people of
their humanitarian nature reduced charitable donations (Sachdeva, Iliev, & Medin, 2009).
Moreover, in a study of environmental values and behaviors (Mazar & Zhang, in press), although
mere exposure to “green” (environmentally friendly) products increased prosocial inclinations,
purchasing green products had the ironic licensing effect of lowering prosocial behavior and
inducing more cheating and stealing. This study revealed an important distinction between
activating constructs related to moral issues, which may induce prosocial attitudes and behaviors,
and engaging in actual moral behaviors, which may have the ironic effect of licensing
subsequent conduct that ignores others’ welfare.
The licensing effect of immoral behavior seems particularly relevant to political conflict
because the motivation to justify morally questionable behavior may be elevated. The tendency
to morally amplify the righteousness of one’s group and one’s cause in times of conflict is likely
17
to increase this licensing effect. In a series of studies, we (Hirschberger & Pyszczynski, 2010)
examined whether MS had different effects on the moral attitudes of Israeli political hawks and
doves with regard to the conflict between Israel and the Palestinians, and whether moral identity
primes would moderate the effect. The research was conducted several months after the outbreak
of violence between Israelis and Palestinians in 2009.
In the first study, we assessed the moderating effect of participants' political orientation
on the impact of MS on responses to a particularly salient moral dilemma – justifying civilian
casualties. Participants were randomly assigned to an MS or pain-salience condition and read a
vignette that described Palestinian civilian casualties during the conflict in Gaza. Results
confirmed our expectations that right-wing, hawkish participants would respond to MS with
greater justification of civilian casualties, whereas those with a left-wing, dovish orientation
exhibited the opposite effect.
In the next study, we examined whether priming moral identity moderates the impact of
death reminders on moral attitudes. Because the moral dilemma in question pertained to the
conduct of an entire group, and not that of a specific individual, we believed that group moral
identity was especially relevant and we therefore primed group moral identity. For similar
reasons, we chose to prime a reminder of collective death – the Holocaust instead of one’s own
death. Participants were first assigned to read a Talmudic passage that either characterized the
Jewish people as a charitable and compassionate nation (moral condition) or a passage warning
against displaying too much compassion and advising that showing compassion to the cruel is
akin to being cruel to the compassionate (control condition). Then participants were assigned to
either a Holocaust salience or a control pain salience condition and completed the measure of
justifying civilian casualties that was used in Study 1.
18
Among participants who were not primed with moral identity, those with a right-wing
orientation responded to the Holocaust prime by rating civilian casualties as more justified than
those with a left-wing political orientation, replicating the findings of Study 1. However, much to
our initial dismay, the moral identity primes increased justification of civilian casualties in the
Holocaust salience condition among left-wing participants to the level expressed by right-wing
participants. It appears that moral identity primes had a paradoxical effect on the resolution of
moral dilemmas by reducing the discrepancy between participants’ desire to view themselves as
moral and the morally questionable attitude that Palestinian civilian casualties are acceptable.
The fact that this effect was most pronounced among left-wing participants is consistent with
other findings from this line of research indicating that such persons generally find civilian
casualties less morally acceptable. Thus, when confronted with moral violations by their group,
reading Talmudic passages that bolstered their sense of the morality of their group may have
ironically facilitated shifting away from their typical moral stance of viewing civilian casualties
as wrong.
To clarify the processes involved in this effect, we conducted another study in which we
primed moral identity and examined whether moral disengagement would mediate the effects of
MS on the justification of civilian casualties during conflict. To extend the generality of our
findings to individual moral identity, we used Aquino et al.'s (2007) individual moral identity
manipulation. To get a clearer picture of the role of death concerns, we used a standard MS
manipulation rather than the manipulation of Holocaust salience, because the latter was likely to
arouse a multitude of thoughts and feelings in our Israeli Jewish sample. In addition, we
measured two facets of Bandura's (1986) conceptualization of moral disengagement – moral
justification and advantageous comparison. The dependent variable was based on the same
19
questions regarding the justification of civilian casualties used in the previous studies. Results
indicated that MS increased moral justification, advantageous comparison, and the justification
of civilian casualties, and that this occurred primarily in the moral identity condition –
replicating the findings of Study 2. Moreover, the impact of MS on the justification of civilian
casualties was significantly mediated by moral justification. These results support our claim that
the paradoxical influence of priming moral identity when death is salient involves processes of
moral disengagement that facilitate the justification of morally objectionable conduct.
We suspected that one of the reasons that priming moral identity in our studies increased
rather than decreased support for indiscriminant violence was that all of our studies referred to
violent acts that had already been committed. Moral dilemmas regarding past events may be
viewed by participants as counterfactual and as requiring pointless moral agonizing. Because
history cannot be changed, people may be motivated to justify past group transgressions,
especially when their moral identity has been boosted and their mortality is salient. Thus we
conducted a fourth study to determine whether similar facilitation of future violence by boosting
one’s moral identity would occur. We replicated the design of Study 3 and added an additional
variable: Half of the participants were asked about the 2009 War on Gaza, which took place
before the study was conducted, and the other half were asked to imagine a future violent
outbreak between Israel and the Palestinians. We also measured feelings of shame and guilt
following the reaction to the moral dilemma. We hypothesized that when mortality was salient
and moral identity was not primed, left-wing Israelis would experience greater shame and guilt.
However, when mortality was salient and moral identity was primed, we expected a reduction in
shame and guilt among this group. If this was the case, it would further explain the licensing
20
effect of priming moral identity by showing that the self-conscious emotions that typically
restrain morally questionable behavior are alleviated when moral identity is boosted.
Results in the past-attack (Gaza War) conditions supported our predictions and replicated
our previous findings. MS increased justification of civilian casualties among participants who
were primed with moral identity. These findings indicate, as others have suggested, that
processes of moral disengagement may be invoked retrospectively to justify a prior unethical act
(Anand et al., 2005). In the future-scenario condition, there were no significant effects of MS or
moral identity primes on support of civilian casualties. This suggests that elevating one’s moral
identity is less effective in justifying future indiscriminant violence.
MS and moral identity primes also affected shame and guilt. As expected, MS increased
feelings of shame and guilt for past violence when moral identity was not primed. However,
moral identity primes reduced feelings of shame and guilt in the MS condition. This shows that
moral disengagement facilitates violence by reducing self-conscious emotions reflecting
discomfort with one's own actions or those of one’s in-group. In the future-violence condition we
obtained the opposite response, indicating that MS increased feelings of shame when moral
identity was primed. This finding is in keeping with past research on moral identity primes (e.g.,
Aquino et al., 2007) showing that when focused on possible future transgressions, priming moral
identity inhibits support for immoral conduct.
According to Bandura (1991), most people develop personal standards of moral behavior
that serve a self-regulatory role by promoting good behavior and inhibiting bad behavior.
Behaving in ways that counter these standards results in self-censure and guilt feelings. Thus,
individuals are likely to behave in ways that are consistent with their internal moral standards.
The research described here indicates that past moral violations do not necessarily promote
21
guilty, remorseful behavior. Sometimes people resolve the disparity between moral values and
immoral conduct by rationalizing and justifying past transgressions to reduce the dissonance
between values and behaviors.
This attempt to reconcile the discord between moral self-perceptions and immoral
conduct by cognitively reconstructing the meaning and severity of the transgression has been
labled moral hypocrisy by Batson and his colleagues (e.g., Batson et al., 2002). Moral hypocrisy
occurs when people delude themselves into believing that serving their self-interest does not
violate their moral principles. The greater one is self-deceived in believing that one is moral
despite behaviors that suggest otherwise, the more one seems sincere to others. Therefore, selfdeception in the moral domain is adaptive and enables one to convince oneself and others of
one's moral stature. Our findings are congruent with this definition of moral hypocrisy and
indicate that moral hypocrisy is primarily observed in relation to past conduct.
Conclusions
The ongoing intractable conflict between Israel and the Palestinians concerns territorial
disputes, differing historical perspectives, and clashing religious beliefs. We contend that
underlying these momentous issues is an existential conflict over truth, identity, belongingness,
and moral righteousness. Both Israelis and Palestinians view themselves as sitting on the edge of
an existential abyss with no margin for error. Under such circumstances, when basic existence
seems to be imperiled, in-group unity and consensus are of utmost importance, dissent is akin to
treason, and empathy toward the other side is seen as weakness and cowardice. Morals play an
important role in the perpetuation of such conflicts, and in conceptualizing conflict as an epic
battle between good and evil. Unfortunately, it seems that to fight and kill fellow human beings it
is necessary to abandon the basic morality of civilized life, and this requires profound mental
22
adjustments. As English poet Wilfred Owen wrote from the trenches of WWI: "Merry it was to
laugh there – / Where death becomes absurd and life absurder / For power was on us when we
slashed bones bare / Not to feel sickness or remorse of murder."
We have shown in this chapter that existential concerns perpetuate and amplify
justification of intergroup violence, but we contend that these same concerns may foster peaceful
relations as well. At this moment in history when "the true enemy is war itself" people may
realize that peaceful attitudes are consistent with individual self-interest and with the moral
values that justify and perpetuate these interests.
References
Anand, V., Ashforth, B. E., & Joshi, M. (2005). Business as usual: The acceptance and
perpetuation of corruption in organizations. Academy of Management Executive, 19, 9–23.
Aquino, K., Freeman, D., Reed, A., II, Lim, V. K. G., & Felps, W. (2009). Testing a
social-cognitive model of moral behavior: The interactive influence of situations and moral
identity centrality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 123-141.
Aquino, K., & Reed, A., II (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1423–1440.
Aquino, K., Reed, A., II , Thau, S., & Freeman, D. (2007). A grotesque and dark beauty:
How moral identity and mechanisms of moral disengagement influence cognitive and emotional
reactions to war. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 385-392.
23
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In W. M.
Kurtines & J. L. Gerwitz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development (Vol. 1, pp. 45103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1998). Mechanisms of moral disengagement. In W. Reich (Ed.), Origins of
terrorism (pp. 161-191). Washington, DC: Woodrow Wilson Center Press.
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Pastorelli, C., & Regalia, C. (2001).
Sociocognitive self–regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 125–135.
Bar-Tal, D. (1990). Causes and consequences of delegitimization: Models of conflict and
ethnocentrism. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 65-81.
Batson, C. D., Thompson, E. R., & Chen, H. (2002). Moral hypocrisy: Addressing some
alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 330-339.
Baumeister, R. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford Press.
Blasi, A. (1984). Moral identity: Its role in moral functioning. In W. Kurtines & J.
Gewirtz (Eds.), Morality, moral behavior, and moral development (pp. 128–139). New York:
Wiley.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). A control-systems approach to behavioral
selfregulation.In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 2, pp.107140). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
24
Castano, E., & Giner Sorolla, R. (2006). Not quite human: Infrahumanization in response
to collective responsibility for intergroup killing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
90, 804-818.
Cohen, F., Solomon, S., Maxfield, M., Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2004). Fatal
attraction: The effects of mortality salience on evaluations of charismatic, task-oriented, and
relationship-oriented leaders. Psychological Science, 15, 846-851.
Cox, C. R., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2007). Mother’s milk: An
existential perspective on negative reactions to breastfeeding. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 33, 110-122.
Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of death and the judgment of social
transgressions: A multidimensional of terror management theory. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 73, 369-380.
Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Kluck, B., & Cornwell,
R. (2001). I am not an animal: Mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human
creatureliness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 427-435.
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. (2009). Liberals and conservatives use different sets of
moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029-1046.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T, &. Solomon, S, (1997). Terror management theory of selfesteem and social behavior: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61-139). New York: Academic
Press.
25
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Arndt, J. (2008). A basic but uniquely human motivation:
Terror management. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.). Handbook of motivation science (pp.
114-134). New York: Guilford Press.
Haidt, J., & Algoe, S. (2004). Moral amplification and the emotions that attach us to
saints and demons. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of
experimental existential psychology (pp. 322–335). New York: Guilford Press.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral
intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98-116.
Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2007). In the forest of value: Why moral intuitions are different
from other kinds. In H. Plessner, C. Betsch, & T. Betsch (Eds.), Intuition in judgment and
decision making (pp. 209-230). New York: Psychology Press.
Hayes, J., Schimel, J., & Williams, T. J. (2008). Fighting death with death: The buffering
effects of learning that worldview violators have died. Psychological Science, 19, 501-507.
Hirschberger, G. (2006). Terror management and attributions of blame to innocent
victims: Reconciling compassionate and defensive responses. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 91, 832-844.
Hirschberger, G. (2009). Compassionate callousness: A terror management perspective
on prosocial behavior. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Prosocial motives, emotion, and
behavior: The better angels of our nature. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Hirschberger, G., Canetti, D., Pyszczynski, T., Gubler, J., & Kahn, D. (2010). The
commonalities and differences between group mortality and individual mortality: A terror
management perspective on past victimization effects. Unpublished manuscript. IDC Herzliya.
26
Hirschberger, G., Ein-Dor, T., & Almakias, S. (2008). The self-protective altruist: Terror
management and the ambivalent nature of prosocial behavior. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin. 34, 666-678.
Hirschberger, G., & Pyszczynski, T. (in press). An existential perspective on ethnopolitical violence. In P. R. Shaver & M. Mikulincer (Eds.), Understanding and reducing
aggression, violence, and their consequences, Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Hirschberger, G., & Pyszczynski, T. (2010). Terror management and the licensing effect
of moral identity. Unpublished manuscript. IDC Herzliya.
Hirschberger, G., Pyszczynski, T., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009).Vulnerability and vigilance:
Threat awareness and perceived adversary intent moderate the impact of mortality salience on
intergroup violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 597-607.
Hirschberger, G., Pyszczynski, T., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009). An existential quest for justice:
Mortality salience increases support for vindictive yet inefficient violent retributions.
Unpublished manuscript. IDC Herzliya.
Jonas, E., Martens, A., Niesta, D., Fritsche, I., Sullivan, D., & Greenberg, J. (2008).
Focus theory of normative conduct and terror management theory: The interactive impact of
mortality salience and norm salience on social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 95, 1239-1251.
Jonas, E., Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2002). The Scrooge Effect:
Evidence that mortality salience increases prosocial attitudes and behavior. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1342-1353.
27
Kimhi, S., Canetti-Nisim, D., & Hirschberger, G. (2009). Terrorism in the eyes of the
beholder: The impact of causal attributions on perceptions of violence. Peace and Conflict:
Journal of Peace Psychology, 15, 75–95.
Landau, M. J., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Martens, A.
(2004). A Function of form: Terror management and structuring of the social world. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 190-210.
Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Miller,
C. H., Ogilvie, D. M., & Cook, A. (2004). Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality salience
and reminders of 9/11on support for President George W. Bush. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136-1150.
Leyens, J.-P., Paladino, P., Rodriguez, R., Vaes, J., Demoulin, S., Rodriguez, A., et al.
(2000). The emotional side of prejudice: The attribution of secondary emotions to ingroups and
outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 186–197.
Lieberman, J. D., Arndt, J., Personius, J., & Cook, A. (2001). Vicarious annihilation: The
effect of mortality salience on perceptions of hate crimes. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 547566.
Mazar, N., & Zhong, C. (in press). Do green products make us better people?
Psychological science.
McAlister, A. L. (2001). Moral disengagement: Measurement and modification. Journal
of Peace Research, 38, 87–99.
McAllister, A. L., Bandura, A., Owen, S, V. (2006). Mechanisms of moral
disengagement in support of military force: The impact of Sept. 11. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 25, 141-165.
28
Monin, B., & Miller, D. T. (2001). Moral credentials and the expression of prejudice.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 33–43.
Motyl, M., Hart, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2010). When animals attack: The effects of
mortality salience, infrahumanization of violence, and authoritarianism on support for war.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 200–203.
Pyszczynski, T., Abdollahi, A., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., & Weise, D.
(2006). Mortality salience, martyrdom, and military might: The great Satan versus the axis of
evil. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 525-537.
Pyszczynski, T, Motyl, M., Vail. K. E. III., Hirschberger, G., Rothschild, Z., & Arndt, J.
(2010). A collateral advantage of drawing attention to the problem of global warming: Increased
support for peace-making and decreased support for war. Unpublished manuscript. University of
Colorado.
Reed, A., II, & Aquino, K. (2003). Moral identity and the expanding circle of moral
regard toward out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1270–1286.
Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989).
Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those
who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–
690.
Rothschild, Z. K., Abdollahi, A., & Pyszczynski, T. (2009). Does peace have a prayer?
The effect of mortality salience, compassionate values, and religious fundamentalism on hostility
toward ourgroups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 816-827.
Sachdeva, S., Iliev, R., & Medin, D. L. (2009). Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The
paradox of moral self-regulation. Psychological Science, 20, 523-528.
29
Shu, L. L., Gino, F., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). Dishonest deed, clear conscience: Self
preservation through moral disengagement and motivated forgetting. Unpublished working
paper.
Vees, J., Heflick, N. A. & Goldenberg, J. L. (2008). We are more human than you: Ingroup humanization as an existential defense. Manuscript in preparation. University of Padova,
Italy.
van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters: The
influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 79, 355-366.
Wright, R. (2009). The evolution of God. New York: Little, Brown, & Co.