* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Click here for document about independent behaviour
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
Explanations of independent behaviour, including locus of control, how people resist pressures to conform and resist pressures to obey authority Independent behaviour: Locus of Control refers to the extent to which individuals believe that they can control events that affect them. Individuals with a high internal locus of control believe that events result primarily from their own behaviour and actions. Those with a high external locus of control believe that powerful others, fate, or chance primarily determine events. Those with a high internal locus of control have better control of their behaviour and tend to exhibit more political behaviours than externals and are more likely to attempt to influence other people; they are more likely to assume that their efforts will be successful. They are more active in seeking information and knowledge concerning their situation than do externals. The propensity to engage in political behaviour is stronger for individuals who have a high internal locus of control than for those who have a high external locus of control. Study 1 Elms and Milgram (1974) investigated the background of some of the disobedient participants from Milgram’s first four obedience experiments. They found that disobedient participants had a high internal locus of control and scored higher on a scale that measured their sense of social responsibility Study 2 Atgis (1998) looked at other studies (so carried out a meta analysis) that considered locus of control and conformity. He found that there was a +0.37 correlation between having an external locus of control and conformity. While this correlation may seem small, it is statistically significant. Atgis’s work suggests that having an internal locus of control links to independent behaviour Study 3 Williams and Warchal (1981) studied 30 university students who were given a range of conformity tasks based on Asch’s study. Each student was also assessed using Rotter’s locus of control scale. They found that those who conformed did not score differently on the locus of control scale but they were less assertive, so assertiveness may have more to do with conformity than locus of control. Evaluation Applications It may be possible to encourage more independent behaviour when conformity and obedience is destructive. This is possible only if people can change from external to internal locus of control, so this could be a consideration during education of children. Reliability Research is far from consistent about the role of locus of control and independent behaviour. For example, a study by Schurz (1985) using Austrian participants who were asked to give increasingly painful bursts of ultrasound to a learner, and who were told that the highest level could cause skin damage, revealed no link between locus of control and obedience among the 80% of participants that went all the way to the maxim ultrasound level. Therefore scientists are yet to be convinced about the role of LOC and independent behaviour Evaluation Social Desirability Bias: Locus of Control is measured by questionnaire or interview, of method in and this would lead to the possibility of social desirability bias. Participants may not want terms of to be honest with the researcher so that they are seen in a better light. Correlational validity research, such as that by Atgis (1998) lacks the control and manipulation of experiments to establish cause and effect. It is feasible that other variables, such as assertiveness, or type A behaviour, may confound both LOC and independent behaviour. Resisting the pressures to conform: Factors that make conformity to a majority influence less likely: Desire to retain a sense of individuality. Sometimes we may want to be different to other people around us, to be individuals rather than members of a group. This is particularly true in Western cultures where it seems that people may feel uncomfortable if they are the same as others around them all the time. Snyder & Fromkin (1980) compared two groups of American students to see which was most likely to conform. One group were told they had attitudes that were the same as 10,000 other students, and the other group was told their attitude was very different to 10,000 other students. The group who were told their attitude was the same were more likely to resist conforming than the group who were told they had individual attitudes. This showed that students who were led to believe they already had a conforming attitude made extra effort to assert themselves as individuals. Internal Locus of Control. Most people like to feel they have control over the things that happen to them in their lives, and if pressured to conform they may feel their control has been threatened. Burger (1992) used a rating scale to classify people as either high or low in their desire to be in control. Those who were low in their desire to be in control were more happy to receive help with a difficult puzzle than those who scored highly. High control scorers reacted with irritation at the offer of help as they felt their ability to remain individual was being threatened. Support from an ally. Unanimity is vital for conformity, so that when an ally is present, people within the minority are much more likely to take a stand against the majority, the pressures of normative informational influence are removed. In Asch’s original study, the conformity rate decreased to 5% is thre participant was supported by another confederate. Resisting the pressures to obey: Factors that make obedience less likely Feeling responsible and empathetic. Some of Milgram’s participants refused to continue delivering electric shocks when they thought the victim was distressed or in pain. When participants were able to see or even touch the victim, obedience dropped to 40% and 30% respectively. Only 16% of female students in an Australian study (see Kilham and Mann, 1974) gave the full shock level of 450 volts. The low obedience rate was attributed to the learner being another female student, just like the ‘teacher’. This lead to more empathy for the victim and less obedience to the experimenter. Disobedient role models. Watching other people be disobedient may encourage resistance to blind obedience. In 1955 Rosa Parks refused to obey the orders of a bus driver when he told her to allow a white person to sit down, and in so doing became a disobedient role model for other black people to resist white control. In Milgram’s experiment, participants found it easier to refuse to obey the order to give electric shocks when they could see another participant also disobey. Two fellow ‘teachers’ (confederates of the experimenter) were in the room - one reading the list of word pairs, the other informing ‘Mr Wallace’ whether he was right or wrong and the naive participant administering the shocks. When one ‘upset’ confederate quitting at 150v and the other at 210v, only 10% gave the full shock level, with 50% refusing to carry on past 150v. Questioning motives and status of authority. When people are able to question the legitimacy of an authority figure, they find it easier to remain independent. When Milgram moved his experiment to an office building, obedience rates dropped to 48% because the status of the building conveyed the idea to participants that the authority figure was less legitimate.