Download Why are sexually selected weapons almost absent in females?

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Inbreeding avoidance wikipedia , lookup

Sexual dimorphism wikipedia , lookup

Sexual selection wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females UNCORRECTED PROOF
Why are sexually selected weapons almost absent in females?
Anders BERGLUND
Department of Ecology and Genetics/Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyv. 18d, 752 36 Uppsala, Sweden
Abstract In sex role reversed species, predominantly females evolve sexually selected traits, such as ornaments and/or weapons.
Female ornaments are common and their function well documented in many species, whether sex role reversed or not. However,
sexually selected female weapons seem totally absent except for small wing spurs in three jacana species, present in both males and
females. This poor female weaponry is in sharp contrast to the situation in species with conventional sex roles: males commonly have
evolved sexually selected weapons as well as ornaments. At the same time, females in many taxa have naturally selected weapons,
used in competition over resources or in predator defence. Why are sexually selected weapons then so rare, almost absent, in females?
Here I briefly review weaponry in females and the function of these weapons, conclude that the near absence of sexually selected
weapons begs an explanation, and suggest that costs of sexually selected weapons may exceed costs of ornaments. Females are more
constrained when evolving sexually selected traits compared to males, at least compared to those males that do not provide direct
benefits, as trait costs reduce a female’s fecundity. I suggest that this constraining trade-off between trait and fecundity restricts
females to evolve ornaments but rarely weapons. The same may apply to paternally investing males. Whether sexually selected
weapons actually are more costly than sexually selected ornaments remains to be investigated [Current Zoology 58 (4): – , 2013].
Keywords Sexual selection, Female weapons, Female ornaments, Sex role reversal
1 Introduction
Traits used as weapons in fights between individuals are common in animals, and are found in both males and
females of many species (Emlen, 2009). Weapons (for definitions, see Box 1) may serve functions in for instance
predator defence, defence against suitors, in resource competition, or in mating competition. For females, some
Received Mar. 10, 2013; accepted May 2, 2013.

Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]
© 2013 Current Zoology 1 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females additional explanations to weapon evolution have been suggested: they may exist due to a genetic correlation with
the same trait in males (interestingly, the reverse has not been suggested, i.e., males possessing weapons due to a
genetic correlation with the corresponding female trait). Moreover, another suggestion for the evolution of female
weaponry, the male mimicry hypothesis, is about avoiding intimidation of sons by weapons in adult males – if the
mother is also armed, sons may be less scared by adult males and stay with the mother for longer (Estes, 1991). So
which of these functions do female weapons serve? I will briefly go through some well researched taxa, and
discuss how female weapons are used. I will not dwell on differences between adaptive and non-adaptive (i.e.,
genetic correlations between the sexes) explanations, and I will not try to differentiate between the explanations
relying on natural selection (e.g., anti-predator, resource defence, male mimicry), but I will contrast natural
selection explanations with sexual selection ones.
2 The Use of Female Weapons
In bovids (cattle, sheep, goats and antelopes) males typically have horns, but in many species females also
possess such weapons, albeit usually smaller and differently shaped (Caro et al., 2003; Bro-Jørgensen, 2008;
Stankowitch and Caro, 2009). In male bovids, horn evolution is thought to be driven by male-male competition for
females, whereas female horns may serve as defence against predators or in female-female competition over
territories (Roberts, 1996; Stankowitch and Caro, 2009). Indeed, horn size in females has been used to quantify the
proportion of male weaponry that can be due to natural selection, the remainder is then assumed to be sexually
selected (Bro-Jørgensen, 2007). Large, conspicuous, open-living bovids typically have females with horns used in
predator defence, and such horns are thought to have evolved repeatedly in this family (Stankowitch and Caro,
2009). In smaller, inconspicuous territorial bovids, such as many duikers, females defend their territories against
other females aided by their horns. Horns of this type are thought to have evolved once, and then only among the
duikers (Stankowitch and Caro, 2009).
In cervids (deer), females have antlers in only one species, the reindeer, but these are again thought to be
naturally selected. Females use them to establish dominance, in particular over males. In fact, females keep their
antlers for longer than males, thus securing feeding sites for themselves and their calves (Espmark, 1964).
Other ruminants also seem to lack sexually selected weapons (Box 1) in females: ossicones (horn-like skincovered protuberances) in giraffes are used in male-male “necking” fights, whereas females have proportionately
smaller ossicones, skulls and necks and are not known to use them in combat or dominance (Simmons and
Scheepers, 1996). In the related okapi, however, females are territorial (Hart, 1992) and may use their ossicones in
territory defence against other females. In pronghorns, finally, female have pronghorns but presently seem not to
use them at all, although these horns have been suggested to be anti-predator devices against now extinct predators
(Byers, 1999). Thus, in no ruminant species females are known to use their horns in intrasexual combat over males,
presumably because sex role reversal (Box 1) seems absent in this taxon.
The lack of sexually selected weapons in females seems in fact to be the rule in all mammals. For instance, in
boars, elephants and rhinos females use tusks and horns as tools or anti-predator devices, i.e. these traits are
naturally selected or due to a genetic correlation with male traits (Estes, 1991). In male narwhales tusks may be
2 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females used in aggressive encounters (Silverman and Dunbar, 1980). Some females may also develop tusks, but their
function, if any, is unknown.
Birds pose a much more interesting taxon for the search for female sexually selected weapons. Several sex role
reversed species exist, especially among waders such as phalaropes and jacanas. Unfortunately birds are seldom
heavily armoured, but spurs exist in many species. In three species of jacana, the wattled, the northern and the
pheasant-tailed jacana, males as well as females have wing spurs (Fig. 1, Rand, 1954). They are similar in size
between sexes (Rand, 1954), but as females overall are larger than males, spurs are proportionately smaller in
females. Nevertheless, females may use their spurs in displays at mating time, “as if they were attempting to strike
each other with their sharp spurs” (Rand, 1954). These spurs are, in fact, the only (likely) sexually selected female
weapon I have found, even if the evidence is somewhat anecdotal.
In several species of swans, geese, screamers, plovers and lapwings both sexes may have wing spurs (Rand,
1954), but their function in females is not well known. As these birds typically have more conventional sex roles
(Box 1) female spurs are unlikely to be used in female-female fights over males, and their use seems primarily to
be defence against enemies or intruders (Rand, 1954), or use in competition over resources for chicks (B. Lyon,
pers. comm.). Female rallids (coots, moorhens) have huge claws (as do males), used in fights over territory borders
and resources for chicks (B. Lyon, pers. comm.). The spurs in gallinaceous birds (fowl), located on the tarsus, are
found in males only (Rand, 1954).
In some reptiles, for instance horned lizards (Phrynosoma) and thorny devils (Moloch horridus), both males
and females have spiny horns (Emlen, 2008), but these function primarily in predator defence (Young et al., 2004).
In fish sexually selected weapons are quite rare, and I know of no case where females possess such in spite of sex
role reversal being not uncommon (e.g., many pipefish species, Berglund and Rosenqvist, 2003). In some
crustaceans females are armed, like in the snapping shrimp Alpheus armatus, but those weapons are used in
territory defence (Knowlton and Keller, 1982). In several beetles females possess weapons, sometimes even larger
than those in males. This is for instance the case in the dung beetle Onthophagus sagittarius, but these females use
their horns in contests over dung (a resource for reproduction), not in fights over access to males (Watson and
Simmons, 2010).
3 Why Are Sexually Selected Weapons So Rare in Females?
Female sexually selected weaponry thus seems almost non-existent. Rather, when females possess
weapons, they are naturally selected, used in competition over resources other than mates, in predator
defence, or are non-functional (Caro et al., 2003; Bro-Jørgensen, 2007; Emlen, 2008). In other words, female
weapons may often belong to the realm of social selection (West-Eberhard, 1983; Lyon and Montgomerie,
2012; Tobias et al., 2012), but not to that of sexual selection as defined in Box 1. Still, females in many
species have sexually selected ornaments, attractive to males (e.g., Clutton-Brock, 2009). Even if the
endeavour to explain the lack of something may seem strange, I still think the paucity of female sexually
selected weaponry, in the face of frequent female ornamentation, begs an explanation. The question is then,
why do females have ornaments, but typically not weapons, to aid them in acquiring males? One possibility
3 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females is of course if weaponry and sex role reversal for some reason are found in different taxa – if so, females
would remain unarmed in the context of sexual selection. This is perhaps a possibility in mammals where sex
role reversal is uncommon (but see e.g. Kuester and Paul (1996) for a possible exception), but cannot apply
to birds, fish or insects, where we find both sexually selected weapons and sex role reversal within lineages.
Another possibility is that females, for some reason, engage in mate attraction and in dominance interactions,
but not in direct fights with other females over males. If so, sexually selected weapons would of course be
superfluous in females. However, females of many species are indeed aggressive towards one another and do
engage in direct fights over resources as well as mates (e.g., Andersson, 1994; Eens and Pinxten, 2000;
Clutton-Brock, 2012).
To take one step back, we first need to understand if costs and benefits of sexual selection differ between
males and females. If we compare non-investing males (Box 1), who do not provide females or offspring
with direct benefits such as paternal care or nuptial gifts, with females, sexual selection definitely operates
differently in the two sexes. In addition to all possible costs associated with developing and possessing
sexually selected traits that apply to both sexes, females always face an additional cost: that to their fecundity.
Resources allocated to a sexually selected trait will trade off with resources allocated to egg production in all
females, and also to any further offspring care if provided. This will constrain the evolution of sexually
selected traits in females: they need to trade ornaments for offspring (Fitzpatrick el al., 1995; Clutton-Brock,
2009; Tobias et al., 2012). Exactly the same trade-off will be present in males, if they provide females or
offspring with direct benefits (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995). In other words, anisogamy suffices to constrain trait
evolution more in females than in males, and any further parental investment (sensu Trivers, 1972) provided
for the offspring by the females may amplify the constraint. Conversely, a reversal in the difference of the
direct benefits provided by the parents, so that male parental investment exceeds that of females, may reverse
the constraint on trait evolution to operate more strongly on males. Still, even under the last scenario, females
will always produce eggs, and so never can rid themselves from the trade-off between fecundity and sexually
selected traits. Moreover, benefits from sexually selected traits may also differ between the sexes. Payoffs
from such traits may be lower in females than in males if for instance anisogamy thwarts the value of
additional matings more in females than in males. Again, this would act to produce less extreme traits in
females.
So, can this constraint on trait evolution in females explain why they evolve ornaments rather than
weapons? Possibly, but only if sexually selected weapons are more costly than ornaments. We expect
females to evolve “cheap” traits so as not to compromise their fecundity, and if weapons are more expensive
than ornaments that would explain the paucity of the former. So are weapons expensive? This is not known
and for now remains a speculation. I think it would be odd if energetic or other production costs of weapons
were prohibitive, as many females possess naturally selected weapons. Can the cost of provoking and
entering real fights with other females, risking injury, be prohibitive? If this is the case costs and benefits
from sexually selected traits could look like in Fig. 2. Here, a steep rise in costs with trait size for sexually
selected weapons compared to ornaments, together with females being more cost-sensitive than non-
4 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females investing males, prohibits females from evolving weapons but permits the evolution of ornaments.
Maximizing the difference between benefits and costs in Fig. 2 would render sexually selected weapons in
females unfeasible, in contrast to male sexually selected weapons and male and female ornaments. A similar
argument could be raised when comparing investing with non-investing males (Box 1): we would expect
sexually selected weapons in the former to be absent or small, and ornaments to prevail, i.e., the cost curves
in Fig. 2 may look similar for females and investing males. For simplicity the benefit curve in Fig. 2 is drawn
similarly for females and non-investing males. However, benefits may likely be lower in females if they
typically gain less from multiple matings compared to males. Again, this would further reduce the likelihood
that females develop sexually selected weapons.
4 Conclusion and Future Directions
Sexually selected weapons in females are near-absent, but female ornaments are not uncommon. As the
evolution of sexually selected traits in females is compromised by effects to their fecundity, females are typically
very cost-sensitive. If sexually selected weapons somehow are more costly that ornaments, this may explain the
paucity of weapons in females relative to males. We now need to understand several things to explain the absence
of female sexually selected weapons. First, we need more clearly to understand the function of female weapons. It
is not uncommon to find explanations to weapons in terms of their value to females in defending resources for
themselves and their offspring, but when males have the same weapons and defend the same resources they are
said do so to attract females. Thus, female weapons would automatically become “naturally selected” and male
weapons “sexually selected”! To avoid such just-so stories we need much better insights into how and why
weapons are used. Second, we need to compare costs of sexually selected weapons and ornaments. This may not
be an easy task – often one and the same trait functions as both a weapon and an ornament (Berglund et al., 1996),
so disentangling the two may be a formidable task. Moreover, costs must be measured in terms of costs to
individual fitness, and such measures are notoriously difficult to obtain. Nevertheless, if we succeed, we can
finally understand why something hardly exists, namely female sexually selected weapons.
Acknowledgements I thank Bruce Lyon for fruitful discussions on this topic and for good comments on the manuscript.
References
Andersson M, 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Berglund A, Bisazza A, Pilastro A, 1996. Armaments and ornaments: An evolutionary explanation of traits of dual function. Biological
Journal of the Linnean Society 58: 385–399.
Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, 2003. Sex role reversal in pipefish. Advances in the Study of Behavior 32: 131–167.
Bro-Jørgensen J, 2007. The intensity of sexual selection predicts weapon size in male bovids. Evolution 61: 1316–1326.
Bro-Jørgensen J, 2008. Dense habitats selecting for small body size: A comparative study on bovids. Oikos 117: 729–737.
5 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females Byers JA, 1999. American pronghorn: Social adaptations and the ghosts of predators past. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Caro TM, Graham CM, Stoner CJ, Flores MM, 2003. Correlates of horn and antler shape in bovids and cervids. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology 55: 32–41.
Clutton-Brock T, 2009. Sexual selection in females. Animal Behaviour 77: 3–11.
Eens M, Pinxten R, 2000. Sex-role reversal in vertebrates: behavioural and endocrinological accounts. Behavioural Processes 51: 135–
147.
Emlen DJ, 2008. The Evolution of animal weapons. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 39: 387–413.
Espmark Y, 1964. Studies in dominance-subordination relationship in a group of semi-domestic reindeer (Rangifer tarandus L.).
Animal Behaviour 12: 420–426.
Estes RD, 1991. The significance of horns and other male secondary sexual characters in female bovids. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science 29: 403–451.
Estes RD, 1991. The behaviour guide to African mammals. Berkely and Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Fitzpatrick S, Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, 1995. Ornaments or offspring: Costs to reproductive success restrict sexual selection
processes. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 55: 251–260.
Hart JA, 1992. Forage selection, forage availability and use of space by okapi Okapia johnstoni, a rainforest giraffe in Zaire.
Ongule´s/Ungulates 91: 217–221.
Knowlton N, Keller BD, 1982. Symmetric fights as a measure of escalation potential in a symbiotic, territorial snapping shrimp.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 10: 289–292.
Kuester J, Paul A, 1996. Female-female competition and male mate choice in Barbary macaques Macaca sylvanus. Behaviour 133:
763–790.
Lyon BE, Montgomerie R, 2012. Sexual selection is a form of social selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Series
B 367: 2266–2273.
Rand AL, 1954. On the spurs on birds’ wings. The Wilson Bulletin 66: 127–134.
Roberts SC, 1996. The evolution of hornedness in female ruminants. Behaviour 133: 399–442.
Silverman HB, Dunbar MJ, 1980. Aggressive tusk use by the narwhal (Monodon monoceros L.). Nature 284: 57–58.
Simmons RE, Scheepers L, 1996. Winning by the neck: Sexual selection in the evolution of the giraffe. American Naturalist 148: 771–
786.
Stankowich T, Caro T, 2009. Evolution of weaponry in female bovids. Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B 276: 4329–4334.
Tobias JA, Montgomerie R, Lyon BE, 2012. The evolution of female ornaments and weaponry:Social selection, sexual selection and
ecological competition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society series B 367: 2274–2293.
Trivers RL, 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B. ed. Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871–1971.
Chicago: Aldine, 136–179.
6 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females Watson NL, Simmons LW, 2010. Reproductive competition promotes the evolution of female weaponry. Proceedings of the Royal
Society series B 277: 2035–2040.
West-Eberhard MJ, 1983. Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. Quarterly Review of Biology 58: 155–183.
Young KV, Brodie ED, Brodie ED, 2004. How the horned lizard got its horns. Science 304: 65–65.
7 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females Box 1. Vocabulary

Natural selection: a process by which biological traits become either more or less common in a population as a
function of the differential survival and reproduction of their bearers. Natural selection includes social and sexual
selection, but when I contrast natural with sexual selection in this paper I mean natural selection exempting sexual
selection.

Social selection: a form of natural selection where selection stems from intraspecific social competition, both sexual
and non-sexual. Social selection thus includes sexual selection. My use of the term here should not be confused with
ideas unfortunately using the same vocabulary but refuting the process of sexual selection.

Sexual selection: “differences in reproductive success, caused by competition over mates, and related to the
expression of the trait” (Andersson, 1994, p. 7 lines 21–23). The word “mates” can be exchanged for “fertilisations”
to include post-copulatory processes. Sexual selection is part of social and natural selection.

Weapon: a trait used in direct fights between individuals over a resource

Sexually selected weapon: a trait used in direct fights between individuals of the same sex over access to individuals
of the opposite sex for mating purposes

Ornament: a sexually selected trait attractive to the opposite sex

Sex role reversal: when females are the predominant competitors for mates

Conventional sex role: when males are the predominant competitors for mates

Non-investing male: a male that provides females with genes but no direct benefits for her or her offspring, such as
paternal care, security or nuptial gifts

Investing male: a male that provides females with genes and also direct benefits for her or her offspring, such as
paternal care, security or nuptial gifts

8 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females Fig. 1. A female Northern jacana Jacana spinosa, with wing spur
(from http://scienceblogs.com/tetrapodzoology/2010/07/14/spurs-blades-jacanas-lapwings/)
9 BERGLUND A: Sexually selected weapons in females Fig. 2 Possible benefits and costs with sexually selected weapons and ornaments to females and non-investing males
10