Download deviancetonormal - Electronic Journal of Human Sexuality

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Homosexualities: A Study of Diversity Among Men and Women wikipedia , lookup

Sexual fluidity wikipedia , lookup

Body odour and sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup

Sex-positive feminism wikipedia , lookup

Sexological testing wikipedia , lookup

Ego-dystonic sexual orientation wikipedia , lookup

Heterosexuality wikipedia , lookup

Penile plethysmograph wikipedia , lookup

Hookup culture wikipedia , lookup

Sexual selection wikipedia , lookup

Ages of consent in South America wikipedia , lookup

Sexual reproduction wikipedia , lookup

Age disparity in sexual relationships wikipedia , lookup

Sexual dysfunction wikipedia , lookup

Sexual racism wikipedia , lookup

Age of consent wikipedia , lookup

Sexual stimulation wikipedia , lookup

Sexual objectification wikipedia , lookup

Sexual slavery wikipedia , lookup

Sex and sexuality in speculative fiction wikipedia , lookup

Human mating strategies wikipedia , lookup

Human male sexuality wikipedia , lookup

Exploitation of women in mass media wikipedia , lookup

Sex in advertising wikipedia , lookup

Erotic plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Human sexual response cycle wikipedia , lookup

Sexual ethics wikipedia , lookup

Rochdale child sex abuse ring wikipedia , lookup

History of human sexuality wikipedia , lookup

Human female sexuality wikipedia , lookup

Lesbian sexual practices wikipedia , lookup

Slut-shaming wikipedia , lookup

Sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup

Female promiscuity wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Sexual Aggression 1
Running Head: FROM DEVIANCE TO NORMALCY
From Deviance to Normalcy: Women as Sexual Aggressors
Peter B. Anderson, Ph.D.
Dyan T. Melson, M.Ed.
University of New Orleans
Peter B. Anderson, Ph.D.
Professor and Graduate Coordinator, Dept. of Human Performance & Health Promotion
University of New Orleans-Lakefront Campus, HPC Room #109
New Orleans, LA 70148
(504) 280-7061, FAX 280-6018
[email protected]
Sexual Aggression
2
From Deviance to Normalcy: Women as Sexual Initiators and Sexual Aggressors
Abstract
The traditional gender roles of young women in the United States are changing. According to
research reports of the past few decades, women have taken a more commanding role in sexual
relationships with men. These new roles have, at times, included behavior identified as sexual
aggression. Women’s sexual aggression is the focus of this research review. The shift from a
deviance label to a normative label is supported by prevalence data combined with what we know
about the connection between women’s beliefs in male sexual stereotypes and their sexually
aggressive behaviors. Discrepancies between men’s and women’s reports of women’s heterosexually
aggressive behaviors provide additional confirmation of the prevalence of women’s heterosexual
aggression and the influence of beliefs in stereotypes. The advent of our realization of women’s sexual
aggression raises many questions that we can not currently answer, but can lead to new research
questions and theories about human sexual aggression.
Key Words: Sexual Aggression, Deviance, Normal, Women
Sexual Aggression
From Deviance to Normalcy:
Women as Sexual Initiators and Sexual Aggressors
Changing social norms and the redefinition of behaviors from a deviant classification to a
normative one does not necessarily represent an increase in that behavior, nor an approval, just an
increase in our awareness of the behavior. Deviance is defined as: “…deviating from a norm or the
accepted standard of society…”, “…a sexual pervert” (The American Heritage Dictionary, p361).
Despite the fact that one of the definitions of normal still found in the Oxford English Dictionary is
heterosexual (http://www.dictionary.oed.com, 8/13/02), same gender sexual contact is an excellent
example of a similar recent shift. Whether we, as individuals or scientists, have become more
tolerant and accepting of same gender sexual contact or just the opposite, the behavior has become
recognized in U.S. social culture as normal (i.e., “… constituting a usual or typical pattern...” The
American Heritage Dictionary, p. 894).
Sexual aggression has been traditionally linked with male sexual behavior. Characterizations
of perpetrators and rationale for their actions have soundly established this behavior in male sexuality
(Christopher, Burch, & Kisler, 2001; Koss, Gidycz, & Wisnieski, 1987; Lottes, 1991; Poppen and
Segal, 1988; Waldner-Haugrud & Magruder, 1995). Documentation pertaining to women aggressors
has been limited (Anderson & Aymami, 1993; Anderson & Sorensen, 1999; Higgs, Canavan, &
Meyer, 1992). The gender based discrepancies in accumulated evidence can be explained to some
extent by the societal embrace of male and female gender role stereotypes. Traditional gender scripts
have portrayed men as being aggressive initiators and women as passive recipients of sexual activity
(Malamuth, 1981). Recent research has emerged describing changes within these prescribed gender
roles (Byers, 1996). Some sexologists now agree that women have taken a more commanding role in
3
Sexual Aggression
4
sexual relationships and have exhibited behaviors classified as sexual aggression (Anderson &
Struckman-Johnson, 1998; O’Sullivan & Byers, 1996).
Historical Perspective
The classic review of case studies by Sarrel and Masters (1982) was the first modern study to
document women as sexual aggressors in heterosexual encounters (see Table 1 for summaries of all
the articles reviewed). This report was based on clinical reports of women sexually victimizing men.
The authors analyzed files from two sex therapy institutes. Eleven case studies were selected and
classified as forced assault, baby-sitter abuse, incest, or dominant women abuse. The cases
documented that the majority of men (10 of 11 in the sample) experienced negative emotions due to
their sexual abuse. Additionally, this negative response resulted in various forms of sexual
dysfunction for the subjects. The authors reported that male sexual abuse had never been properly
studied by social scientists. Subsequently, investigations of women's sexual aggression expanded, to
include documentation of prevalence rates, the relationship between acceptance of sexual stereotypes
and women’s sexual aggression, and conflicting reports of women’s aggression offered by men and
women (Anderson, 1986, 1989, 1998; Larimer, Lydum, Anderson, & Turner, 1999; StruckmanJohnson & Struckman-Johnson, 1992, 1994).
Prevalence
Struckman-Johnson and Struckman-Johnson (1994) examined the prevalence of men pressured
and forced into unwanted sexual activity. The authors reported that about one third of their sample of
college men had experienced pressured or forced sex since the age of 16. In evaluating the most used
tactics for the pressured or forced episodes, 88% of the pressured or forced subjects reported
Sexual Aggression
5
experiencing verbal persuasion, intoxication, emotional manipulation, and bribery, and 12% reported
experiencing the use of physical restraint, physical intimidation, harm, or threat of harm.
Larimer, Lydum, Anderson, and Turner (1999), reported that rates for their measures of
receiving (men = 21%, women = 28%) or instigating (men = 10%, women = 5%) sexual coercion were
not statistically different for college men and women, but that women were less likely to use physical
force as a tactic to obtain sexual contact (men = 5%, women = 1%). They also stated that none of the
women in their sample reported giving alcohol or drugs to a male as an attempt to gain sexual contact.
This conclusion is inconsistent with research by Struckman-Johnson & Struckman-Johnson (1994) and
others (Anderson, 1998, 1996; Clements-Schreiber, Rempel, & Desmarais, 1998).
Anderson (1998, 1996), presented self-reported prevalence rates for women’s sexual coercion
of between 25% and 40% and for physically forced sexual contact between 1.6% and 7.1%. Of
perhaps greater significance was the women’s self-reports of engaging in a classic date-rape scenario –
taking advantage of someone who was under the influence of alcohol or drugs. When asked about
initiating sexual contact with a man when his judgment was impaired by drugs or alcohol, between
32% and 51% of the women said that they had. Further, between 5% and 15% of women reported
giving a man alcohol or drugs in an attempt to have sexual contact with him.
Acceptance of Sexual Stereotypes
Struckman-Johnson and Struckman-Johnson (1992) reported that approximately 18% of
women and 22% of men believed that it is impossible to rape a man - regardless of perpetrator sex.
The authors also reported that some subjects believed that men did not experience trauma due to a
sexual assault by a woman (35% of men and 22% of women). The authors concluded that male rape
myths were more likely to be accepted if the perpetrator was a woman. Thus, the use of pressure
tactics or force by a woman could be seen as a method of foreplay and not aggression.
Sexual Aggression
6
Margolin (1990) supported the idea that women have more permission than men to violate a
partner’s sexual consent (kissing was the only behavior measured in this study). Men also received
less endorsement to withhold sexual consent. The author noted that men were more tolerant of a
woman’s violation of a man’s consent than that of a man’s violation of a women’s consent. Also, men
were more tolerant of consent violations than women’s tolerance for violations by either gender.
Craig Shea (1998) concluded, based on the results of two separate studies, that coercers could
be distinguished from other college women in that they were more likely; 1) to view traditional
women’s roles as being constricting, 2) to believe that forcing sex at times is acceptable, 3) to
acknowledge that it is acceptable for women to have and act on sexual thoughts and desires, and 4) to
admit their lust and desire for power in sexual encounters.
Clements-Schreiber, Rempel, and Desmarais (1998) attributed women’s likely use of both
overt (e.g., undress him, touch him, kiss him) and covert (e.g., get him drunk, make him jealous)
pressure tactics to obtain sex from a reluctant man to beliefs in the stereotype of a male dependent sex
drive for married women and to the belief that women’s sex drive is at least as strong as a man’s for
single women. The authors concluded that the acceptance of sexual stereotypes differentially effected
married women and single women in their likely use of pressure tactics.
Discrepancies in Male and Female Reports
Struckman-Johnson (1988), reported that sixteen percent of male participants admitted being
forced into unwanted heterosexual activity by a partner, but only 2% of female respondents reported
forcing males into sexual activity in their lifetime. In a follow-up survey, the majority of males who
had experienced forced sex (52% of 124), reported being forced into unwanted sex based upon
psychological strategies (e.g., lying, guilty for not wanting to engage in sex, and blackmail).
Furthermore, 25% of 124 males reported experiencing a combination of physical force and
Sexual Aggression
7
psychological tactics. The authors suggested that males and females both express behaviors that are
sexually exploitative and range from verbal pressure to use of physical restraint and force.
Anderson and Aymani (1993) stated that males reported being recipients of female aggression
more than females admitted to being aggressors. The largest difference between male and female
reports (41.1%) was recorded on a question measuring female adults’ initiation of sex with male
minors. The next largest difference was observed on the question asking each gender their account of a
woman initiating sex with a man when his judgment was impaired by alcohol or drugs (30.2%).
Discrepancies were concluded to be the result of sexual socialization of women and sexual stereotypes
of men. Women were believed to have answered the survey partially focusing on socially desirable
responses and partially on the myth that men will never turn down a sexual opportunity.
In a follow-up study, Anderson and Sorensen (1999) concluded that men reported significantly
more events of adult women initiating sexual contact with them while they were minors (OR=10.9), by
getting them drunk or high (OR=3.7), and by threatening to end their relationship (OR=6.3) than
women reported. All questions, except the one assessing mutually consenting sexual activity, showed
some difference in the expected direction (i.e., men reported that women were more likely to initiate or
be aggressive than women self-reported). The authors concluded that women may interpret their
sexual aggression as more normal than do men.
In addition to gender difference, discrepancies have also been highlighted in reports of sexual
aggression between groups of women themselves. Women from different regions have been
documented to report different rates in the use of sexual coercion, abuse, and physical force. Anderson
(1998) reported that women from the South reported less overall aggression (34.1% vs. 46.2%), sexual
abuse (7.3% vs. 21.1%), and physically forced sex (1.6% vs. 7.1%) but not less sexual coercion (all
differences were significant at p < .001). Regional differences were interpreted as variations in global
Sexual Aggression
8
dating messages and gender scripts for each area. Despite the similarity of the samples, other factors
(i.e. ethnicity, religion, peer group) in addition to region were discussed as being contributing factors.
Present Research and Future Implications
The research exploring women’s sexual aggression has supported the existence of and some
estimates for prevalence of this behavior. However, the studies reviewed contain several limiting
factors in the study of this component in human sexuality. Specifically, the majority of study
subjects have identical demographic profiles, in that middle to upper middle class college students in
their early 20’s serve as the main source for participants, which restricts the application of
conclusions to persons outside the study population. Dating dynamics is another limiting factor for
the investigations reviewed. All the studies reviewed developed conclusions from individuals who
were not likely dating persons within the samples. Additionally, most study participants were single.
Relationship dynamics and marital status were suggested to be responsible for some of the between
gender discrepancies in reports of experiences involving sexual aggression by women (Anderson &
Sorensen, 1999). Future studies need to incorporate and recruit couples while also exploring
relationship dynamics to determine its influence on promoting and/or predicting episodes of
aggressive behavior.
Unanswered questions raised by this line of inquiry include:

are research scientists and others willing to define sexually aggressive women as date rapists; if
not,

will definitions of men who behave exactly as women report behaving change;

how do context, gender, and power intersect in the examination and interpretation of sexual
behavior; and,
Sexual Aggression

9
should gender be a or the critical variable that determines the definition of and our reaction to
sexually aggressive behavior?
In answer to the first two questions, it may be that we live in a gendered world and this line of
scientific research is unlikely to change our gendered definition of sexual aggression. For the third and
forth questions, few studies have considered context and or power in relation to women’s sexual
aggression and much more research is needed in this area. In one study that did, Craig Shea (1998)
concluded that women who admitted their desire for power in relationships and believed that forcing
sex was, at times, acceptable, were more likely to use coercive strategies than other women. New
research should, as a beginning, make comparisons between groups of women from different positions
of power within society, different regions of the county or world, and different cultures or ethnicities.
There is also a philosophical aspect to these questions. We live in a socially constructed world.
No analysis of gendered relationships, in this case, heterosexual sexual contact, would be complete
without considering a myriad of perspectives about those relationships. Much legitimate critique of
heterosexual relationships has focused on power differentials between women and men. One form of
power is social normative legitimacy, something women have lacked, in general, for centuries. If we
answer the previous questions by codifying behaviors in a gender neutral way, that does not create
equal experiences, nor does it ensure equality. If legitimate differences exist between groups, in this
case women and men, perhaps those differences, rather than gender designation, should determine our
definitions and reactions.
There is also the concern that the study of women’s sexual aggression runs counter to work
necessary to reduce and eliminate violence against women. Clements-Schreiber, Rempel, and
Desmarais (1998) counter this argument in two important ways. First, they argued that accepting
sexual stereotypes is a concern for women because it may legitimate male lack of self control and
Sexual Aggression
10
excuse their behavior and it implies that women are only passive recipients of male sexual attention
without their own sexual agency. Second, if women accept their own nonviolent sexual coercion as
essentially normal, men will accept their own nonviolent sexual coercion as normal too, and more
women will be victimized. Clearly, much more research and discussion is needed to resolve this
complex argument.
Conclusion
Studies have revealed that young girls have become socially assertive in calling young boys on
the telephone and even asking for dates at a very early age (Anderson, Arceneaux, Carter, Miller, &
King, 1995). Also, women are now expected to take an active role in sex (O’Sullivan & Byers, 1996),
and are expressing themselves sexually in aggressive behavior patterns (Anderson & StruckmanJohnson, 1998). Rates of sexually aggressive behaviors among women vary from one segment of the
United States to another, but the evidence presented here shows that as many as 7% of women selfreport the use of physical force to obtain sex, 40% self-report sexual coercion, and over 50% selfreport initiating sexual contact with a man while his judgment was impaired by drugs or alcohol
(Anderson, 1998). Given these numbers, it is appropriate to conclude that women’s sexual aggression
now represents a usual or typical pattern (i.e., has become normal), within the limits of the data
reviewed in this paper.
Sexual Aggression
11
Table 1
Summaries and Additional Data from Study Results on Women’s Heterosexual Aggression
Authors
Sarrel & Masters,
N
Population
11
Men seeking sex
Reported negative emotional response to
therapy
sexual abuse
College men,
33% experienced pressured or forced sex
X age = 22
since age 16
1982
Struckman-Johnson &
204
Struckman-Johnson
Outcomes
1994
Larimer, Lydum,
M=165
College Students in
20.7% of men and 27.5% (ns) of women
Anderson, and Turner
W=131
Greek orgs.
experienced unwanted sexual contact;
X age = 19
.6% of men and 4.6% of women
1999
experienced physically forced sexual
contact (p < .05)
Anderson 1998
Anderson, 1996
461
212
College women
32% to 51% reported initiating sexual
X age = 22, 78%
contact with a man who was “under the
single
influence” of alcohol or drugs
College women
Women who held adversarial beliefs
X age = 22, 78%
about relationships and/or experienced
single
past sexual abuse were more likely to be
sexually aggressive toward men
Sexual Aggression
12
Table 1(continued)
Summaries and Additional Data from Study Results on Women’s Heterosexual Aggression
Authors
N
Population
Outcomes
_______________________________________________________________________________
Struckman-Johnson & M=157 College Students
Approx. 20% believed that it is
Struckman-Johnson,
W=158
X age = 20
impossible to rape a man
M=171
College Students
It is more acceptable for a woman to
1992
Margolin 1990
W=194
Craig Shea 1998
deny sexual consent than for a man
#1=171
College Women
Coercers were more likely to have
#2=261
X age = 19
experienced physical punishment as a
child, to have said “no” but meant “yes”
to sexual intercourse, and to have
experienced sexual coercion
Clements-Schrieber,
et.al., 1998
234
Canadian women
31% to 97% reported a likelihood of
X age = 39
using overt pressure (kissing, touching,
undressing) and 13% to 28% of using
covert (get him drunk, make him jealous)
to get sex from a reluctant man
Sexual Aggression
13
Table 1(continued)
Summaries and Additional Data from Study Results on Women’s Heterosexual Aggression
Authors
N
Population
Outcomes
_______________________________________________________________________________
Struckman-Johnson, 1988
M=268 College
Of those coerced, more men than women
W=355
Students
(52% vs. 16%) experienced
X age = 20
psychological force, more women than
men (55% vs. 10%) physical force, and
equal percents (10%) no consent due to
intoxication
Anderson & Aymami, 1993
M=128
College
15 of 26 items showed significantly
W=212
Students
higher support by men than women
X age = 24
Anderson & Sorensen, 1999
M=82
College
12 of 17 items showed significantly
W=163
Students
higher support by men than women
X age = 22
Sexual Aggression
14
References
American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, W. Morris, Ed. (1970). New York:
Houghton Mifflin Company.
Anderson, P. (1998). Variations in college women's self-reported heterosexual aggression. Sexual
Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 10(4), 283-292.
Anderson, P. (1996). Correlates with college women's self reports of initiating heterosexual
contact, Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 8(2), 121-131.
Anderson, P., Arceneaux, E., Carter, D., Miller, A., and King, B. (1995). Changes in the
telephone calling patterns of adolescent girls, Adolescence, 30(120), 779-784.
Anderson, P., & Aymani, R. (1993). Reports of female initiation of sexual contact: Male and
female differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22 (4), 335-343.
Anderson, P., & Sorensen, W. (1999). Male and female differences in reports of women’s
heterosexual initiation and aggression. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28 (3), 243-253.
Anderson, P. & Struckman-Johnson, C. (1998). Sexually aggressive women: Current perspectives
and controversies, New York, NY: Guilford Publications, Inc.
Byers, E. (1996). How well does the traditional sexual script explain sexual coercion? Review of a
program of research. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 8(1/2), 7-25.
Christopher, F., Burch, S., & Kisler, T. (2001, October). Men's use of sexual aggression in dating
relationships: A meta-analysis. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Society for the
Scientific Study of Sexuality, San Diego, CA.
Clements-Schreiber, M., Rempel, J., & Desmarais, S. (1998). Women's sexual pressure tactics and
adherence to related attitudes: A step toward prediction. The Journal of Sex Research, 35 (2),
197-205.
Sexual Aggression
15
Craig Shea, M. (1998). When the tables are turned: Verbal sexual coercion among college women. In
Anderson, P.B. and Struckman-Johnson, C.J. (1998). Sexually aggressive women: current
perspectives and controversies, New York, NY: Guilford Publications, Inc.
Gwartney-Gibbs, P., Stockard, J., & Bohmer, S. (1987). Learning courtship aggression: The influence
of parents, peers, and personal experiences. Family Relations, 36, 276-282.
Higgs, D., Canavan, M., & Meyer, W. (1992). Moving from defense to offense: The development of
an adolescent female sex offender. The Journal of Sex Research, 29(1), 131-139.
Koss, M., Gidycz, C., & Wisnieski, N. (1987). The Scope of rape: Incidence and prevalence of sexual
aggression and victimization in a sample of higher education students. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 55 (2), 162-170.
Larimer, M., Lydum, A., Anderson, B., & Turner, A. (1999). Male and female recipients
of unwanted sexual contact in a college student sample: Prevalence rates, alcohol use, and
depression symptoms. Sex Roles, 40, 295-308.
Lottes, I. (1991) The relationship between nontraditional gender roles and sexual coercion. Journal of
Psychology & Human Sexuality, 4 (4), 89-109.
Malamuth, N. (1981). Rape proclivity among males. Journal of Social Issues, 37, 138-157.
Muehlenhard, C., & Cook, S. (1988). Men's self-reports of unwanted sexual activity. The Journal of
Sex Research, 24, 58-72.
O’Sullivan, L., & Byers, E.S. (1996). Gender differences in responses to discrepancies in
desired level of sexual intimacy. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 8, 49-67.
Oxford English Dictionary, normal = heterosexual, Retrieved August 13, 2002, from
http://www.dictionary.oed.com,.
Poppen, P., & Segal, N. (1988). The Influence of sex and sex roles orientation on sexual
Sexual Aggression
16
coercion. Sex Roles, 19 (11/12), 689-701.
Sarrel, P., & Masters, W. (1982). Sexual molestation of men by women. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
11 (2), 117-131.
Stets, J., & Pirog-Good, M. (1989). Sexual aggression and control in dating relationships. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 19 (16), 1393-1412.
Struckman-Johnson, C. (1988). Forced sex on dates: It happens to men, too. The Journal of Sex
Research, 24, 234-241.
Struckman-Johnson, C. & Struckman-Johnson, D. (1992). Acceptance of male rape myths among
college men and women. Sex Roles, 27 (3/4), 85-100.
Struckman-Johnson, C. & Struckman-Johnson, D. (1994). Men pressured and forced into sexual
experience. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23 (1), 93-114.
Waldner-Haugrud, L. & Magruder, B. (1995). Male and female sexual victimization in dating
relationships: Gender differences in coercion techniques and outcomes. Violence and Victims,
10 (3), 203-215.