Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Chapter 2: Understanding the Proximate and Ultimate Causes of Bird Song 2.1 Work on the development of singing behavior in male white-crowned sparrows has demonstrated that the birds must learn to sing a particular dialect of the full song of their species. This finding eliminates the genetic differences hypothesis for the song differences between white-crowns in Marin and in Berkeley. Would we be right, therefore, to conclude that the genetic information present in the cells of white-crowned sparrows is irrelevant for the development of the bird’s singing behavior? In this regard, what importance do you attach to the finding that whitecrown males apparently can learn their species’ song far more easily than the song of other sparrows? What about the finding that white-crown males that hear whitecrown song only during a 40-day period early in life can nevertheless generate a complete song, although they do not start singing until 3 to 4 months after exposure to a tutor tape? To say that the behavioral differences between two individuals are not genetic does not mean that the behavior itself can develop without the genetic information possessed by the two individuals. Both Marin and Berkeley birds have genes, which could be identical in both individuals, and that are vital to the development of their brains. The genes in question influence the structure and capacities of their brains, an essential contribution if their brains are to grow and acquire the ability to learn things. The fact is that all male white-crowns have learning biases that steer them toward the acquisition of a local dialect of their own species. Thus, the gene–environment interactions that take place during development produce a very special kind of brain with limited, but adaptive, properties. 2.2 A natural experiment sometimes occurs in Australian woodlands when galahs (a species of parrot) lay eggs in nest holes in trees that are then stolen from them by pink cockatoos (another parrot). Thus, the cockatoos become foster parents for baby galahs. The young foster-reared galahs produce begging calls and alarm calls that are identical to those produced by galahs cared for by their genetic parents. However, the adopted galahs eventually give contact calls very much like those of their adoptive cockatoo parents, as you can see from Figure 2.6 of the textbook. (The birds produce these signals to help maintain contact with others when traveling in flocks [1037].) Someone claims that these observations show that galah begging and alarm calls are genetically determined, whereas contact calls are environmentally determined. Explain why this claim is wrong (you may find it helpful to read ahead in Chapter 3). Then defend the superficially similar statement that the differences between the alarm calls given by adopted galahs and by their cockatoo foster parents are the result of genetic differences between them. What other behavioral differences are the result of differences between the social environment of the adopted galahs and that of certain other individuals? All behavioral traits require both genetic and environmental inputs in order to develop. Therefore, none of the calls of a galah can be purely genetic or purely environmental. Instead, the brain and the vocal apparatus of the parrot could only develop under the influences of both genetic information and the material and experiential environment. The differences, however, between the alarm calls of two individuals could be the product of differences either in the birds’ DNA or their environments. The difference between adopted galahs and the cockatoos in their alarm calls appears to be genetic given that the environment of foster reared galahs is essentially the same as that for cockatoo nestlings. On the other hand, the difference in the contact calls given by galahs that have been adopted by cockatoos and those that are reared by their genetic parents can be attributed largely to differences in the acoustical and social environments of the two categories of galahs. Because galahs that have been adopted by cockatoos do not have the same experiences as galahs reared by their parents, they undergo a slightly different set of gene environment interactions, which lead to the development of a different vocalization. 2.3 Naturally, female starlings possess the same ZENK gene males do. In the female brain, the ventral caudomedial neostriatum, or NCMv, responds to signals sent to it from auditory neurons that fire when the bird is exposed to sounds, such as those made by singing male starlings. When captive female starlings are given a choice between perching next to a nest box where they can hear a long starling song versus perching next to another nest box where a shorter starling song is played, they spend more time at the long song site (Figure 2.10 of the textbook). What proximate hypothesis could account for the song preferences of female starlings? What prediction can you make about the activity of the ZENK gene in the NCMv of female starlings exposed to long versus short songs? How might you check your prediction? What would be the scientific point of collecting the data necessary to evaluate your prediction? The brain cells that process acoustical stimulation have the genetic information needed to produce one pattern of activity in response to long songs and another in response to short songs and to provide stronger rewarding sensations when the birds hear long songs. This difference in neural activity should be related to differences in activity in the ZENK gene. This prediction could be checked by sacrificing samples of female starlings shortly after they had listened to tapes of long or short songs; the NCMv could then be assayed for the ZENK gene product. The point of collecting these data would be to test the hypothesis that cells in the NCMv possessed a gene or genes that contributed to the physiological mechanism underlying song preferences by female starlings. 2.4 In the greater spear-nosed bat, unrelated females roost together in groups. The members of any given group tend to produce similar calls, but different groups have different calls. If you transfer some young bats into a new group and, at the same time, move some of their siblings into still another group, the shifted bats eventually change their calls to resemble those in their new groups [128]. What proximate hypothesis and what ultimate hypothesis on the acquisition of acoustical communication are supported by these results? The proximate hypothesis would be that the differences between the calling patterns of different groups was due to differences in the social influences and experiences operating in the different groups. The ultimate hypothesis is that social integration of group members is facilitated by the ability of individuals to mimic the signals given by others in their group, perhaps especially the calls produced by dominant group members. 2.5 At the proximate level, two hypothesis have been advanced to account for the ability of some yearling white-crowned sparrows to match the dialect of neighboring males. The late acquisition hypothesis states that yearlings have a developmental window similar to that of very young birds, which enables them to listen to and learn directly from their immediate neighbors, overriding (if need be) any dialect learning that took place earlier in life. In contrast, the selective attrition hypothesis argues that fledglings can memorize a number of dialect versions of their species song early in life; then, after settling next to some older males, these birds gradually discard certain of their learned variants until they are left with the one that best matches the dialect of their neighbors. In light of these two alternative hypotheses, what significance do you attach to Figure 2.20 of the textbook? Outline the entire scientific process here, from the question that motivated the work to the scientific conclusion. The causal question: what causes young male white-crowns to sing like their neighbors? The two hypotheses generate different predictions with the late-acquisition hypothesis predicting that young males should be able to learn a dialect novel to them long after the early period of song acquisition documented by Peter Marler. The selective attrition hypothesis, in contrast, states that the birds have actually learned several songs early in life but that later, when confronted by a rival with a particular song type, the young birds will come to drop the nonmatching songs and sing the matching type more often. Figure 2.20 provides direct evidence of this latter pattern, which in turns gives support to the selective attrition hypothesis, which is the scientific conclusion of this work. 2.6 William Searcy and a team of researchers played taped songs to captive female song sparrows that had been given hormone implants shortly after being taken to the laboratory [1078]. The recorded songs came from male song sparrows that lived in the population from which the females had been taken as well as from males living various distances (18, 34, 68, 135, and 540 kilometers) from the female subjects. Songs from males living 34 or more kilometers from the populations from which the females came were not nearly as effective in eliciting the precopulatory display as songs from local males; in contrast, songs from males living only 18 kilometers away were about as sexually stimulating as local songs. These data have relevance for more than one ultimate hypothesis on song learning by male sparrows. What are the hypotheses, and what importance do these findings have for them? One hypothesis is that females prefer songs of males in their natal neighborhood because these males will supply locally adapted genes to their offspring if chosen as mates. Alternatively, females may prefer the songs of males that most closely match those that the females heard early in life because adult males capable of singing good matches to the songs that the males heard early in their lives must have had “good” brains as youngsters, which made it possible for them to mimic the songs they were hearing accurately. Males of this sort may either be genetically superior or be in superior condition with the capability of providing superior parental care. Both hypotheses generate the prediction that females will be more stimulated by males living in their natal area or close to it (since these nearby males will probably sing the same or a similar dialect). The findings support both hypotheses. 2.7 What features of language learning in humans are similar to song learning in birds? Do these similarities suggest certain hypotheses on the proximate bases of human language learning, especially the genetic and developmental components? Do comparisons with birds also suggest some interesting hypotheses on the adaptive value of learned language for members of our species? After you have attempted to answer these questions, go to the ISI Web of Science, if it is available at your college or university library, and try to find references on the shared proximate and ultimate causes of vocal learning by humans and birds. You may find it useful to know that those who have written knowledgeably on this subject include Peter Marler and Fernando Nottebohm. Among the shared features are the apparent window of language learning that makes it much easier for an infant or young person to learn a language than an older child or adult. Another is the stunning ease with which these complex acoustical signals can be learned by very young infants. Another is the acquisition of local dialects by both birds and humans, a function of the capacity of members of both species to match the communication sounds they hear precisely. And infants, like young white-crowns, go through a pre-language babbling phase in which they practice the sounds they can make and match them to their memories of the sounds they have heard others make, a process that requires the child to be hearing itself (not deaf). To do these things, humans, just like white-crowned sparrows, must possess genetic information that helps make the development of a learning-capable brain possible. The human brain is one, like the whitecrown’s, that is very strongly influenced not just by the acoustical environment, but also by the social environment, with human infants highly attuned to the sounds produced by their primary caretaker, usually their mother. One ultimate hypothesis that might apply to both birds and humans is that the skill with which a person uses his group’s speech could be important in mate choice, with females evaluating male language ability unconsciously as an indicator of the developmental history of potential mates. Language acquisition takes place during a very demanding period of growth, and persons handicapped by inadequate diets may exhibit life-long deficits in speech and body condition, which should make them less likely to elevate the fitness of a partner.