Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
RIGHT AND WRONG: THE MORAL MAZE By Lyn Corderoy Summer 2012 Grange Primary School Wickford Essex ‘We are both right and wrong. We have to choose.’ Year 5 Copyright: Farmington Trust 1 Contents Page 3. Introduction and background to the research 5. Research questions 6. What are the social and educational psychologist’s theories with regards to moral education? 12. What do the children at school understand by the terms right and wrong and how have they acquired those opinions? 30. As an educational establishment, how much can the primary school hope to influence those opinions? 32. Next steps! 36. References & Bibliography 2 1. Introduction and background to the research Right and Wrong are words that evoke strong feelings in most people; what is fascinating however, is the range, diversity and, most importantly for this study, the evolution of those feelings. This piece of research has been undertaken in response to the actions and events that emanated from a particular year group in a primary school. The study looks at the school, it’s moral and ethical education processes; the county syllabus used to guide that learning and the academic theories of both child development and moral education in order to try and give the research a baseline of reference. The actual practical research and data collection took place in the school using three art days and encompassing 260 children from years one to six. The resulting soft and hard data was analysed in order for the researcher to derive some meaning and answers to the questions posed. For the researching head teacher the end results are to be used to help enhance and further improve and develop the moral and ethical vision and learning that is taking place in the school. As a Christian head teacher in a state school the researcher has always relied on her own moral and ethical background as a foundation for her own life and as a baseline for the vision and ethos of the school in her care. However last year the school was sorely tested by a ‘troublesome’ year group; a cohort of children who, when staff looked back through their years at school, had always been challenging. A cohort who had poor attendance figures, couldn’t be trusted to behave on trips, were difficult to contain during break times and who, no matter how exciting the learning environment, were still extremely hard to motivate and inspire. The overarching feeling from the pupils was that they seemed unable to understand what the school community regarded, quite naturally, as the ‘norms’ of good behaviour! The big question that all the staff asked as the year was evaluated was… Is this ‘just a blip’ or a whole new trend? Were, perhaps, family, cultural and community influences so strong, and not seeming, with that group particularly, to have an understanding of the concepts of right and wrong and what that meant with regards to behaviour. The question that came out in the discussion was could school really impact levels of morality in the children in its care? The staff 3 worried that maybe they weren’t supporting the pupils and helping them to develop the skills and tools or moral base or code to enable them to find a way through the ‘moral maze’ of life. The school in question promotes an atmosphere of respect and tolerance and feels it has a clear moral foundation underpinning its vision and ethos. It believes that it successfully teaches the locally agreed County RE syllabus in its outstanding thematic curriculum. The school feels that there should therefore be plenty of influences in both the explicit and the implicit curriculum, to help the pupils to understand the terms ‘right and wrong’ and what that means for them in terms of behaviour. However, this particular group of pupils did not seem to have been so influenced! So the previously held view that if all the children in the school are being educated in the school’s moral and ethical learning programme, and the school’s vision and ethos and aims are underpinning and surrounding everything that is done, and all pupils have the same learning chances, then what might make the difference and affect a particular cohort or groups of children? Obviously the cohort that triggered the research are now scattered in the local secondary schools but it was still felt that the research questions could help strengthen future moral educationally development in the school. The researcher aimed to find out what might, according to social theory and educational psychology, be affecting the children’s moral development and then through an art day help them express what they perceived ‘right and wrong’ to be and, most importantly, how they had acquired those opinions. The researcher has been in primary and special schools for 30 years and firmly believes the power and importance of art within education so was convinced that the art day could prove to be immensely useful as a conduit of information collection. Over the years the researcher could quote numerous occasions when children have opened up during art sessions and when discussing their art pieces. 4 ‘I never made a painting as a work of art, it’s all research’. Pablo Picasso ‘Right can be broken by wrong.’ Year 6 What this researcher ultimately hopes to do is clarify the importance and need of ‘education’ as an influence; particularly looking at how strong or powerful that influence may be, should be or could be. To this end three research questions were devised: a) What are the social and educational psychologist’s theories with regards to moral education? b) What do the children at school understand by the terms right and wrong and how have they acquired those opinions? c) As an educational establishment, how much can the primary school hope to influence those opinions? 5 a) What are the social and educational psychologists theories with regards to moral education? Many meanings and connotations of the words ‘right and wrong’ are found; in fact everyone has a personal opinion on what they mean and how they should be used. Most people have a view that is derived from a wide range of sources including upbringing, cultural influences, education and experiences. According to the dictionary right is ‘in accordance with what is good, proper, or just: a moral, ethical, or legal principle considered as an underlying cause of truth, justice, morality, or ethics’. On the other hand, wrong is defined as ‘not in accordance with what is morally right or good’. From a Christian standpoint the researcher has always had biblical reference points and a ’Christian up-bringing’ to support her moral and ethical code. It takes very little research to find references to ‘right and wrong’ underpinning the major religions. For Christians, comments from the New Testament support their moral views as in James 4:1, ‘So whoever knows the right thing to do and fails to do it, for him it is sin’; or Romans 12:21, ‘Do not be overcome by evil, but overcome with good.’ For the Jews as seen in the Old Testament, there are a multitude of rules and guidelines in relation to right and wrong and the consequences of such actions, as in Isaiah 1:17, ‘Learn to do good; seek justice, correct oppression; bring justice to the fatherless, plead the widow’s cause’. For the Muslim believer the Koran makes reference as in 4.36, ‘Serve Allah, and join not any partners with Him; and do good - to parents, kinsfolk, orphans, those in need, neighbours who are near, neighbours who are strangers, the companion by your side, the wayfarer (ye meet), and what your right hands possess: For Allah loveth not the arrogant, the vainglorious.’ All are evidence of a moral and ethical baseline for a religious understanding of ‘right and wrong’. However, history shows us how interpretations of those 6 ideals vary enormously and the resulting behaviour likewise varies and often seems to contradict those ideals! So humans may have a variety of written moral guidelines but how they interpret them and what other influences may affect their moral choices? The concept of morality and the discussion seems to come down to the basic question of; what kind of person we are and how should we behave? The question has been asked through time, as seen from the religious writings and works from philosophers such as Aristotle (translated 1962) who expounded on his views about using good sense and doing what a ‘virtuous’ person would do. ‘Virtue is a character state concerned with choice, lying in the mean relative to us, being determined by reason and the way the person of practical wisdom would determine it’ (Aristotle, translated 1962,1107a1) That it was not, and is still not, easy to follow written guidance, laws and advice is evidenced throughout history, however, rules are still agreed by most as the way forward. Rules are often imposed externally whether through religions, political laws, socialization through community influence or the power of a dominant class system. One major problem though seems to be that all these sets of rules have different sources! So the question is - are there a set of universal moral rules? Constant discourse through history proves the importance of the question and four major ‘systems or processes’ of morality are an interesting backcloth to the discussion. The ‘Natural Law’ expounded by Thomas Aquinas (13th century), is based on the philosophy of Aristotle, and is that all of nature was devised for a natural purpose and to achieve that purpose is the ‘good’ and right thing to do; therefore anything that goes against that natural purpose would be wrong. On the other hand for ‘Utilitarianism’ the right and wrong of any action can only be judged by the outcomes. With ‘Deontology’ there is the constant battle between what a person feels is their ‘duty’ and what they are ‘inclined’ to do. Then there is ‘Social Justice’ where individual freedom is aligned with a desire to have justice for the poorest in society. All of these views however rely heavily on 7 the involvement of rational, deep thinking, willing altruistic humankind and how does that relate to moral education? Moral and ethical study, in some form or other, has been part of school life since the beginning of time, however, for this study the period of the 20th & 21st Century is of most interest. The idea that morality could be ‘caught’ rather than needed to be ‘taught’ and was just part of RE studies was prevalent in the middle 1900’s. There were also ideas like the ‘Values Clarification’ (Rath et al: 1966) from America that advocated it was wrong to impose values onto children and it was far better to allow them think through choices and have ownership of values to which they were far more likely to adhere. In 1958,1963 and 1970 Lawrence Kohlberg, moving on from the Piaget views on the development of morality, did research using the story of ‘Heinz’ that involved a series of moral dilemmas around a wife with cancer, a drug company not giving treatment and her husband stealing the cure. Kohlberg used a variety of test subjects in order to verify his findings and from the research responses he came up with three main levels of moral development that were then sub-divided into six stages. Kohlberg’s levels of morality are compared with Piaget’s views and can be seen in the table below, devised by Gross & Rolls (2004:147). Kohlbergs levels of moral development Pre conventional (stages 1 & 2) Most nine year olds and belowfew over nine Piaget type of morality Understanding rules 5-9/10 year olds: Heteronomous moral orientation (subject to another’s laws or rules) Rules represent and external law unilateral respect Conventional (stages 3&4) Most adolescents and adults Post conventional (stages 5&6) 10-15% of adults, not before mid30s 10 year old and Mutual respect above: Autonomous moral orientation (subject to one’s own rules or laws) Moral judgement and punishment Objective /external responsibility Belief in: expiatory punishment moral realism collective punishment immanent justice Internal responsibility Belief in: Principle of reciprocity moral relativism 8 No longer believes in: Collective punishment immanent justice. Although exhibiting many different views both Piaget and Kohlberg agree on the fact that child development stages effect, and are related to, stages of moral development. The use of these six moral stages in relation to the data collected is of great interest to the researcher and the following summary from a piece by Crain (1985) discussing and explaining the stages will be referred to later in reference to the data from the ‘Right and Wrong’ art days. Pre conventional morality Stage one: Obedience and punishment orientation is when the child is not a thinking member of society rather that morality is an external force and something they must do or they will be punished. Stage two: Individualism and exchange involves a belief in more than one viewpoint and punishment is something to be avoided. Also there is the view of fairness, it is wrong for someone to be ill and the drug company not to give the cure so it’s right to take it and make it fair. Conventional morality Stage three: Good interpersonal relationships is far more complex and related to expectation of family and the community and involve the ideas of good behaviour, good motives and feelings such as concern for others, empathy and love. Stage four: Maintaining the social order and concern for society with an emphasis on duty, respecting laws and authority, for this stage stealing is wrong not just because you will be punished as in stage one but because the laws are there for society as a whole. Post-conventional morality Stage five: Social contract and individual rights is when people start questioning what makes a good society, they evaluate and consider rights and values, thinking of society and democracy and how it works and what it ought to value. 9 Stage six: Universal principles, this is based around impartiality and respect for all. It stems from the idea of justice for all and could include acts such as civil disobedience, for example Martin Luther King advocated justice regardless of race, he also agreed that there must be laws and democratic processes so would accept the penalties for his actions. Alongside the discussion on the stages of moral development, this researcher has also looked into the work of Bandura from the 70’s through to the 90’s. He developed a principle called ‘social learning theory’ (SLT) ‘Of the many cues that influence behaviour, at any point in time, none is more common than the actions of others… SLT is often called modeling or observational learning. The SLT emphasizes the importance of observing behaviours, attitudes and emotional reactions to others.’ (Bandura:1986. 32) He firmly believed that children learn through imitating or observing the behaviour of others and then internalizing what they have witnessed. For Levine and Munsch (2011) this process of imitation is achieved in four parts; that of actually watching and seeing an action from a person, then memorizing what has been seen, next working how they can imitate the action themselves before finally having the desire to actually want to do the action themselves. This process would seem to supports much of what the researcher has discovered when looking at the data findings and especially concerning who by and how the children have been influenced. For Bandura (1977) aggressive individuals are not born with those traits but learn them just as they would any other social behaviour. This learning may happen through reinforcement, any association with a person’s self-esteem and / or observation of that behaviour. For Gross and Rolls (2004) how we develop moral understanding is a many faceted process that involves conscience as a guide to wrong doing, an understanding of morals so that judgments can be made, learning behaviour appropriate to a culture and finally the development of emotions such as concern for others. Bandura clarifies his view on the complexity of the processes and the skills and tools an individual needs in order to be successful. 10 ‘In the social cognitive learning theory ...it is one and the same person who does the strategic thinking about how to manage the environment and later evaluates the adequacy of his or her knowledge, thinking skills, capabilities, and action strategies... (Bandura1997: 5) He uses the term ‘self-efficacy’ when explaining his views that a person needs power and abilities to produce an outcome or intended result and that needs to be achieved by people working together. ‘Unless people believe they can produce desired effects for their actions, they have little incentive to act.’ (Bandura 1997:3) and this is not necessarily an individual act or process, ‘People do not live their lives in isolation; they work together to produce the results they desire’ (Bandura 1997:7) So he believes individuals devise a personal moral code that they then use as a route map and ‘rule book’ to regulate themselves. We know how strong this personal code can be when we look through history at figures that have suffered and even died for their self-respect. He doesn’t, however, advocate that humans are only puppets to ‘external forces’ but that they do have the opportunity to control their lives. So is school providing the SLT skills and tools necessary to support the pupils no matter what ‘life’ throws at them? It would seem if we take the approach of Bandura it is vital that the school develop the necessary cognitive tools and attributes so that children can acquire the knowledge and problem solving skills they will need in order to participate effectively in society. Once they are in society their knowledge and thinking skills will be continually tested, evaluated and used to look at them selves and decide if they are successful members of their community. So then ‘A fundamental goal of education is to equip students with selfregulatory capabilities that enable them to educate themselves. Self-directedness not only contributes to success in formal instruction but also promotes life long learning’. (Bandura 1997:174) For the researcher the final word in this section on the theory of moral development comes from Levine and Munsch (2011). After all the discussion on how we can provide moral educate the main focus and aim must be… 11 ‘We want children to develop an internal sense of morality - that they will choose what is right because of their own thought and feelings not because of the external consequences’. (Levine & Munsch 2011:407) b) What do the children at school understand by the terms right and wrong and how have they acquired those opinions? When deciding what form the research process was going to take it was suggested that using the arts would be both interesting and exciting. As an advocate of art as the inspiration to learning this filled the researcher with delight! Obviously the procedures for accurate data collection needed to be adhered to, and ethical and practical considerations had to be considered and carefully planned. Being the head of a primary school that was used to innovative and ‘different’ learning sessions, getting the staff and governors on board was not a problem; in fact there has been a buzz of anticipation throughout the project and keen interest in the results. The researcher knew that the resulting data would be a mixture of qualitative and quantative and obviously be limited as a piece of research by the nature of it only taking place in one school. In consequence a case study approach seemed to be the most appropriate. Because using the ‘Arts’ as a base for research is not prevalent it seemed wise to look at some work done in that area. McNiff (2007) is highly in favour of the arts as research tools and explained the interesting, though not always easily analysed, nature of the results. ‘Artistic inquiry, whether it is within the context of research or an individual person’s creative expression, typically starts with the realization that you cannot define the final outcome when you are planning to do the work. As contrasted to scientific methods, you generally know little about the end of an artistic experiment when you are at the beginning. In the creative process, the most meaningful insights often come by surprise, unexpectedly, and even against the will of the creator.’ (McNiff 2007:40) He was not wrong! The whole process of data collection has been fascinating and highly informative on lots of levels, not least a chance to communicate properly with the children and allow them time to completely express their 12 opinions in a confidential and non threatening manner. The three days themselves were hugely entertaining, extremely messy but interestingly devoid of any anti-social or ‘wrong’ behaviour, even though there were groups of 90 children doing ‘messy art’ in the hall on each occasion. To provide continuity and accuracy of data each day was lead by the researcher and wherever possible were completed in a very similar form. The only real changes and adaptations were when explanations were needed for the very youngest group and a drama session was done with the eldest children. The researcher explained the idea of the day to the children, asking them to help her with this piece of research (most pupils when questioned thought that research was; ‘finding our information from books and computer’). It was explained that this was ‘finding out’ information from people and today it was their personal opinions that were wanted, it would be a secret so could they be honest and think of as much information as they could. The session started with a talk partner time where they were asked to share up to 5 things that they felt were ‘right’ – this was done to break the ice and start the thinking process and on each occasion as little input by way of suggestions were given by the researcher or the staff present. Each child was then given paper and pen and asked put a tick or write the word ‘right’. They then had 5 to 10 minutes to mind-map (drawing or writing) out all their ideas (this time limit was adjusted by the researcher once it became obvious that most of the children had run out of ideas). The activity was done with their back to their talk partner and in ‘secret’, in order to keep each child’s data as ‘clean’ as possible. The children then flipped their paper and drew a picture of 13 themselves in the centre, the next task was to draw around them all the ‘influences’ that gave them their opinions/ideas/views on what was ‘right’, again they were given approximately 5-10 minutes to complete the task. After a ‘brain break’ to stretch and relax the children then completed the same process but this time with reference to the term ‘wrong’. The whole process included all the children as they had the option to draw and/or ask for words if they wanted. The SEN children were supported if they wished for help or looked unsure, but all ideas noted were strictly their own. In the opinion of this researcher, Knowles (2008) encapsulates the ‘theme’ of the day that all could be, and in fact were, valid participants in the process. ‘Art in research puts a premium on evocation, even when it has sections or aspects of it that are descriptive in character...art is present in research when its presence enable one to participate vicariously in a situation.’ (Knowles 2008:6) The second stage of the day was less prescribed and more open to the interpretational skills and imagination of the children. Their challenge was to produce either one or two art pieces on the theme of ‘right and wrong’. Pupils could use any available art form and materials and the adults would support if they need, but helping with the art rather than giving them the actual ideas – only a very few had to be questioned to guide them toward their art piece or pieces. Whist they worked adults wandered and discussed and made notes on the artwork. 14 Susanne Langer (1957) argues that works of art represent the artist’s ability to create a structure of forms that are in parallel to the forms of human experience. So any art form can provide a means through which feelings can be explained. In her opinion language is the most useful scientific device humans have created, but that arts provide access to qualities of life and experiences that language alone cannot hope to compete with. So the purpose of the art days, it was hoped by the researcher were to be a doorway to the children’s true moral understanding. ‘Life is in two parts you have to try and choose the right side.’ Year 5 If school was to gain a positive impact from the results of the research then we needed to think like McNiff, ‘Change and insight in the personal realm are increasingly being recognized as a key source of corresponding social change. Therefore, the way in which we treat the humble images of our art-based research may have a definite impact on how we engage the world.’ (McNiff : 37) The following table compares the children’s quotes and therefore ideas of what constitutes ‘right and wrong’. Although some translating from the original quotes was needed for the younger children, most is as written or drawn on their mind maps. The researcher has sub-divided ‘right & wrong’ into three sections in order to help analysis. The first section is related to the first part of the research day that was children’s opinions on right, and who or what has influenced them and helped them to have that opinion. 15 ‘Bad has good around it.’ Year 2 RIGHT a) Rule means it is right b) Feels right to me c) Right because it fits the social norm Children’s ideas of right taken from quotes from the art days. Y1&2 Rule means it is right Feels right to me letting friends play with you being good hugging playing with others playing with my dogs going to a friends house when they Y3&4 Y5&6 Pink pen tickled pink tick well done star team points learning smiley face stamps stop look listen don’t fight be good don’t disobey laws adding up don’t swear feeding living things heath and safety going to school To have peace to have doctors school food and water 1+1=2 to earn not to steal have law and discipline the law to keep bad people away prisons or jail for people who deserve it send violators of the law to jail for their full sentence Law is right otherwise the world would be mad time and clocks or we would never sleep no weapons for killing follow the laws tell the truth when some one says excellent to you think you know website that I’m happy everyday doing the right thing solving things friendship to be loved and love love you and everyone around you what you believe in is right for you stop wars going for gold 16 want you to giving presents making someone happy sun drying out a flood being happy reading a book to someone stories doing the right thing the sun shining My friends think they are wrong but I think they are perfect in every way friendship being helpful letting me go to sleep playing with someone if they say they want to play with you smiling caring for each other sport doing someone’s hair fair amount of work same amount of sweets as my sister self defence gym trail best in school learning having fun kindness happy go to praise pod going out with the family helping mum and dad looking after yourself you get the right privileges for your age thinking rainbows sleep in the middle of the day Everyone to have the opportunity to do what they wish scoring a hat trick having a go at everything helping others choice to do what you want be one have your own opinion being yourself the Collaborative Learning Centre sports to have fun helping each other having lots of friends not just one being proud if you get something right having fun as well as learning to talk to each other helping each other don’t leave people out try your hardest never give up think before you speak be happy learn from your mistakes praise others eating chocolate Oxfam have a belief and faith work to be scared helping people from other countries doing things for charities sharing looking after others cheering someone up variety of subjects at school being patient West Ham in the play off final trees for fresh air playing football smile at each other being fit and not just sitting on the sofa having a fun time laughing concentration playtime RSPB to protect people and animals have adventures 17 having celebrations and parties give to receive art confidence have clothes thumbs up nature able sleep be your own person Right because it fits the social norm be nice to people smile reading sharing eating nicely learning helping writing playing nicely with your sister being good at school saying nice things doing as you are told sharing my toys getting a tick when you do something right manners kindness listening to mummy and your teacher helping an old man across the road being healthy saying hallo back not getting into trouble saying you can be friends look at people when they are talking to you tidy your mess putting things where they belong going to work at the right time using your brain being sensible using the school 4GBs doing your own work helping old ladies having loads of fruit carrying someone’s shopping listening in assembly listening to your parents don’t bully people going to church taking turns sharing listening respecting your elders to walk away when someone is mean mum & dad following the rules winning read a book work together a good teacher learn and get a job everyone listens in this school work /school telling the truth attempt to agree manners reader of the week to have shelter listening to parents grateful love going to bed at the right time when you give someone a gift when you finish your work good telly sleep not leaving someone out being friends having friends don’t blame or lie being able to swim don’t leave someone out playing with each other treat people the same no matter how different they are be nice to people who are a bit different doing stuff you don’t want to do working together to go to school fairness birthday communication assembly math’s books school exercise not arguing good attitude getting questions right homework listening to others ideas be grateful say sorry be obedient human rights free country independence honesty knowledge food and drink clean water not to be wasteful school being free think before you speak listen making yourself useful reading a book brushing your teeth getting sleep getting a job to watch TV not be bullied being grateful for what you get tick means right 18 read the bible having rules in your house Olympics life be nice to people even if you don’t like them include people say you like peoples work or it might hurt their feelings learning corrections be eco-friendly respect others respect the world freedom to have breakfast education using manners being polite recycling to have people who care for us and look after us teachers Queen and other royals to look after the world don’t litter be sensible seeing family have help to be kind give help stand up for yourself watch what you say don’t go back on your word think of others before yourself being healthy and safe holidays having medicine and hospitals being kind to animals helping Mum and Dad keep the world clean Learning otherwise we would be dumb shelter for safety It is a fascinating insight into the minds of the children! The range and diverse nature of their ideas was far wider and deeper than had been anticipated. It became clear immediately that the children had strong opinions and many of 19 them. The overall results broadly fitted with Kohlberg views of moral development and maturity as seen in his six stages above, though the researcher was excited not just by the breadth and depth of thought but the young age that some of these opinions were exhibited and at a much younger age than the Kohlberg stages advocated. Another issue that came to light was in respect of the range of ideas on the term ‘right’ and how it was not always used within the ‘moral’ context. Raising the question as to the children’s understanding and clarity of thought on what constitutes moral lines and boundaries. When reduced to a number picture the results are fascinating, not clearly defined and not closely matching the Kohlberg’s six stages. Obviously the year 5 & 6 cohort have more views and opinions overall and would seem to be moving comfortably into the 4th stage. They advocate maintaining the social order and show a concern for society with an emphasis on duty and fitting the social norms, however they do seem to be a little short on clearly defined ideas related to respecting laws and authority, so they are not necessarily clear what is right by law. The results from the Year 3&4 cohort are interesting particularly when looking at the number of ‘me’ views, which raise an interesting point about strength of character maybe something to watch in the future! Also interesting is the complete nil score from the 20 youngest children on rules supporting ‘right’ behaviours – do they have any positive rules regarding behaviour and boundaries or is it all ‘woolly’ & fuzzy? The chart below is an analysis of the children’s mind maps and notes on whom or what has influenced them with regards to the term ‘right’. Again the range and diversity of views is fascinating, and the complete lack of influences such as the church and religion is very noticeable. The researcher is convinced that if this piece of research had been done 50-60 years ago then the results would have be markedly different and the influence of church would have been noticeably more. It is obvious that the family is a huge factor in the opinion forming of the children and recognized as such by all the children, which of course, though not surprising, is very important when thinking of the capacity of the school in this process of influence. Looking at the data the older pupils do seem to recognise the influence of school, though whether they would lean towards family or school if there was a contest is another question. It is also interesting that the older pupils have a much wider range of recognizable influences, some of which we might not want them to have i.e. the media or friends. We must keep in mind that the different levels of influence mentioned by the children are going to be related to an actually understanding of what an influence might be obviously there is a relationship to the children’s own levels of maturity and social development. . 21 The following section is related to the second part of the research day when the question was asked of the children’s opinions on ‘wrong’, and who or what has influenced them and helped them to have that opinion. ‘This creature makes me think of wrong things.’ Year 2 WRONG a) Rule means it is wrong b) Feels wrong to me c) Wrong because it fits the social norm Children’s ideas of wrong taken from quotes from the art days. Rule means its wrong Y1&2 Y3&4 Y5&6 buses crashing war fighting litter burning stuff Food rubbish in the wrong bin stealing a toy that don’t belong to you knocking trees over shooting and killing guns bombs breaking into a house knives not working killing drinking gun knife rape jail don’t steal weapons poaching killing murder people fighting black mailing drinking driving and getting into trouble from the police animal abuse stealing abuse racism drugs using phone when driving breaking the law violence police at your door graffiti playing with fire setting a fire hurting someone 22 Feels wrong to shouting in school not sharing food me not fair falling in gym things in the wrong place rain feel wrong to me putting ideas on somebody else doing what you are told not to saying I hate you breaking things being mean telling tales when someone talks to you don’t walk away giving up if you get it wrong kisses getting stuck in the toilet not shutting the toilet door and I could see them! My sister pushing me off the bike no chocolate left rain on a special day feeling angry and sad school on my birthday computer stopped working rain stopping me play feeling sad When something goes wrong early bed time lost toys went to cubs with no tea can’t play football sister wont let me have the TV remote football ref! Rained at football training having a bad dream banned toys stuck indoors guilty scared petrified unfair ill upset death sadness Nan dying just before her 103 birthday being mean really bad threatening kidnapping terrorism making dogs fight riots carrying bombs gangs hacking 9/11 shop lifting pirated movies phone hacking speeding mugging guns child labour men stealing little children selling drugs calling the police when you don’t need to not wanting to learn getting a question wrong so called friends bullies and bigmouths Dad forgetting things cheating on loved ones leaving someone out saying your work is bad the opposite to what you believe in Mum and Dad breaking up taking the Mickey computers being mean daddy & mummy being sad being naughty upsetting people arguing with parents breaking things not having fun attitude answering back some TV blaming others breaking promises fur coats zoo being lost junk food bad computer games being a loan shark 23 Wrong because running indoors it fits the social getting sums wrong kicking and punching norm I want a boy but it’s a girl arguing swearing sticking your tongue out snatching toys from your brother wasting paper throwing paper having a tantrum not obeying the rules boasting calling people rude names being silly wandering off interrupting throwing stones teasing calling out picking and killing flowers not listening to the teacher bad day not being the favourite feeling sorry illness being mean to my mum and dad being selfish moaning silly mother shouting at you losing something rain unfair behaviour vegetables whispering telling family you hate them death laughing at someone pranks being badly treated skin cancer making fun of people lashing out and hitting silly things sadness people may deserve better than they have using someone to feel better about yourself taking it out on others talking behind someone’s back not taking the blame / not telling the truth illness parents hating each other hurting someone’s feelings not sharing Dad hitting his thumb with a hammer hurting my arm hit my brother shouting fall out with friends broken bicycle sink at swimming ball through a window being unfriendly fighting bullying calling names swearing rudeness death Mum & Dad fighting failing matches fire being naughty not listening rude arguments mistakes nasty people danger rip up book being rude answering back to an adult swearing interrupting people jealousy telling tales making fun of people who are over weight envy affecting others in a bad way litter farting and burping people who do not have a home should get one from the government being homeless bullying not listening talking when your teacher is talking not doing as you are told smoking war war interrupting people 24 slamming doors not eating your vegetables blowing a cigarette danger bad films cold when the sun is out being horrid fighting friends people being upset fighting with friends people ignoring you wikapedia cutting down trees calling people names backing out of a deal lying committing suicide being sinful selfishness travelers being disrespectful fighting in the playground disease poverty government weather forecast pollution house prices 10 years olds playing a 18+ game arguing being cruel The range and diverse nature of their ideas on ‘wrong’ was again far wider and deeper than had been anticipated. It became clear immediately that the children had strong opinions and many of them, and for some i.e. the youngest group, far more for wrong than right. Again, as with the data from the ‘right’ session, the overall results broadly fitted with Kohlberg’s views of moral development and maturity. The researcher was not surprised, though rather concerned, at the depth of knowledge of ‘wrong’ behaviours that the children had, especially within the list from the older group. The issue with respect to this range of ideas on the term ‘wrong’ is very telling when looking at the influences the children have and where they are getting their information. Again, as with the ‘right’ ideas, the researcher believes that if this study had been undertaken 50 years ago the media influence, material available to children, sorts of games played, in fact the ‘protection’ from all such information would have been very different. 25 When seen pictorially the results again align overall with Kohlberg’s moral development views, but with some interesting highlights. Understandably the year 5 & 6 cohort have more knowledge and many more views in all three sub sections than the younger children, but it is very interesting to look at the numbers in the ‘feels wrong to me’ column, their personal opinions being very strong. What was even more interesting was the number in this ‘feels wrong to me’ section for the year 3 & 4 cohort, as noted in the ‘right’ section, this would seem to be a very opinionated group who do not seem to understand, relate or base their ideas on society rules or social influences, a cohort to watch! The youngest group fit Kohlberg’s hierarchy perfectly with the influence of society being paramount in their views, and the egocentric nature of their maturity showing in their results. The chart below is an analysis of the children’s mind map drawings and notes on whom or what has influenced them with regards to the term ‘wrong’. We must remember again that the different levels of influence mentioned by the children are going to be related to an actually understanding of what an influence might be, obviously there is a relationship to the children’s own levels of maturity and social development. Again the range of views is of paramount interest, and the complete lack of influences such as the church and religion is very noticeable. It is obvious that the family is again a big factor when forming the opinions of the children, 26 which, of course, is hugely important when looking at the capacity and influence of the school. As with the data from the ‘right’ section the older pupils do seem to recognise the influence of school, though again whether they would turn towards family or school if there were issues or disputes is debatable. It is also interesting that the older pupils have a wider range of recognizable influences, the media or friendships numbers are particularly strong; something that both parents and education may need to monitor more closely and factor into any moral education plan more directly than possibly is done at present. The Year 3 & 4 cohort fitted into Kohlberg’s moral ‘pre-conventional’ development stage as seen by their number of ‘government and law’ and ‘dangers’ as influences, unlike the year 5 & 6 cohort who seemed to have moved into Kohlberg’s ‘post conventional’ stage of development where it would seem they are becoming more regulated by mutual respects, i.e. friends, family and school. The analysis of the free art sessions that followed the data collection sections of the day was beyond the skills of this researcher. However, from the 27 school’s point of view the art sessions were very interesting on many levels. It was fascinating to see how the children approached a completely free art session, those who excelled in the freedom of art choices and those who struggled with imaginative ideas and did not have the skills to exhibit their ‘right and wrong’ views in this way. ‘Red & black make me think of wrong things that happen in the world.’ Year 6 ‘Bits of wrong that are all over good and right things.’ Year 2 ‘The zig-zags are right and wrong and they are fighting each other.’ Year1 ‘This is a beautiful garden but it has bits of dark that feel wrong.’ Year 2 ‘I think life is full of right and wrong together, but right is green and all around.’ Year 4 ‘Right is light colours and in order. Wrong is black and like lightening.’ Year 4 ‘Right is like flowers in the garden and it grows good.’ Year 2 ‘I think right and wrong are two parts of our life that we have to find out about.’ Year 5 ‘Wrong is out of order and a mess. We have to try and sort it out.’ Year 2 28 ‘WRONG’ ARTWORK from Year 1&2 ‘RIGHT’ ARTWORK from Year 1&2 The researcher was particularly interested to see the different types of artwork and the interpretation of the ‘right and wrong’ theme and the ‘inhibitions’ with the art that developed with some of the older children. The more imaginative and vibrant art coming from the youngest cohort of children. The older children (Years 5 & 6) used the art to set scenes and describe right and wrong, they had faces and people in action situations; much of their art was drawn and detailed. The middle age group used a lot of words and symbols, poster types of artwork as well as a more varied use of art forms such as clay and sculpture; though some got so into the material they lost their way with the art idea! The youngest group (years 1& 2) went for pattern, colour and design to show their right and wrong ideas, many mixed their right and wrong together in their pictures and explained which colours and patterns stood for right and wrong. ‘zig-zags and black means war the lines are opposite like in a war.’ (HW year1) 29 From the point of view of this piece of research the process involved in the art sessions was a bonus, the major data results coming from the first half of the day. However, the experience of working in this way and the opportunity for discussion on ‘right and wrong’ with the children was an added factor which was almost immeasurable and that the researcher had not anticipated. All the adults commented about this chance to ‘chat’ and question about these moral issues without any awkwardness. c) As an educational establishment, how much can the primary school hope to influence those opinions? The researcher found the work of Brown and Brown (1997) very useful as a foundation to her thinking on the school’s religious education process and how that impacts on its moral and ethical influences. They see cultural development as involving a whole range of values, beliefs, attitudes and customs which are taken on board and amalgamated by an individual to form a person’s identity; and also the basis for cohesive community and social groups. For the Browns’ there is the everyday community culture in terms of theatre, music, dance, museums, etc., but also culture as an ‘anthropological’ term for the whole attitude, belief system and world view. How then can the school be part of that process? They see religion not as a set of rules, like rules in sport that may change year on year. For them morality is an attitude 30 of mind that occurs within a framework of beliefs and commitments. A person’s duty, to God, country, family, in fact for them the social group is the essential element in defining and sorting moral behaviour. So RE in schools should teach that there is an open dialogue with morality, neither constraining nor instead of the religious education. They also firmly advocate that other subjects can and should support the education of morality. For them there is a hugely important skill in this moral education process and that is how children apply their knowledge and understanding of morality, wherever it is gained and whatever it is, to ethical and moral issues in their own lives. That it must affect behaviour or it is valueless. This is a big question that has not been properly defined, in the view of this researcher, throughout the whole period of her educational career both as a pupil and as a teacher. How can schools give their pupils opinions, skills and moral codes of conduct, what moral framework is to be used? If it is a way of coping with life, a means of responding to profound questions about the nature of life and the universe, which religion or ideas and views does a school focus on, or does it endeavour to do a ‘muddled mixture’ of all and actually give nothing clear or helpful for the children as a result? Even if the school experience is on the whole good, and children are supported to make right decisions and helped to decide when actions are inappropriate; even if their school education forms a clear basis of information it will not constitute the total experience of pupils. We must remember, as seen in the data analysis, that the family and media will also play an important part in exploring a range of social and cultural issues. What is clear is that we must give children the skills to be able to make connections between their formal school learning and their life experiences and hopefully the right decisions will be made. The school uses the Essex Agreed Syllabus as the foundation to its specific RE studies. The work is either done through the termly or half termly themes, or as specific blocks or units of work if the subject matter is not suitable for the theme. In the introduction to the syllabus it is clear that the syllabus expects 31 the school to… ‘develop pupils’ knowledge and understanding about the diversity of national, regional, religious and ethnic identities in the United Kingdom and the need for mutual respect and understanding; enabling pupils to think about topical spiritual, moral, social and cultural issues including the importance of resolving conflict fairly…’ However it has been interesting for the researcher to discover that actually there are very few references, except in one Early Years unit, to specific ‘moral’ teaching and there is almost no guidance on how to teach it. The references are vague and unhelpful and open to huge differences in interpretation from adults of different background themselves. Although much would seem to come under ‘common sense’ interpretations we are relying, it would seem to the researcher, a great deal on innate knowledge’s of right and wrong. There is an overarching similarity in moral views that may not always be the case amongst the adults and the children, and very definitely not the case between school and home. Assembly themes are chosen by the school to highlight important ethical and moral issues and although this is felt to be an important learning process, it is limited in its impact. Next steps! The results from the research were intriguing. Patterns emerged which shed light on the attitudes of the pupils to right and wrong…and on what they value. Far from being lacking in a sense of right and wrong pupils emerged as deeply engaged in working out for themselves what right and wrong might be – and in thinking how they would know. This study has shown the children’s thinking and views through the mind mapping process and the works of art. So the question is, where the school should and can go from here? Bigger and Brown (1999) believe that the school environment may count as a place of ‘social cohesion’, but is that enough? ‘Social cohesion has enabled the development of skills and culture yet continuing cooperation cannot be taken for granted: altruism is weaker than the instinct for personal survival. Given a choice between giving aid 32 and being killed, most prefer to stay alive.’ (10:1999) So although the children may have the moral knowledge we know from the experience with a previous year’s cohort that putting that ‘right’ knowledge into ‘right’ action is another matter. For Mcghee (2001) there would not seem to be a single or perfect way for attaining knowledge on the questions of life but that there must be different ‘perspectives on knowledge’. (6:2001) Then from Lee (2001) we come across views and questions related to the problems of the rapidly changing society and life styles that have to be negotiated by the children in their preparation for adult life. ‘...As we enter the 21st Century, the experience of adult life is a lot less stable than it used to be. With regards to being ‘grown up’, we have entered an age of uncertainty, an age when adult life is newly unpredictable and in which stabilities we manage to produce cannot be expected to last our whole lives.’ (7:2001) Lee firmly believes that the changes to perceptions of adulthood and what should or can be expected from adulthood now have and are having a huge affect on adult life views, ‘Change and incompleteness have entered adulthood as principles of living that replace stability and completeness…The implications of this destabilization of adulthood are vitally important for understanding contemporary relationships of authority and power between adults and children.’ (8:2001) Much has been written on how we can help our young people to guide their own behaviour, in the end all of them will have to make their own choices about right and wrong in many and varied situations both in and out of school, and as they become teenagers and then adults. Lindon (2001) has views and ideas on how to support children’s development, and moral education seemed to the researcher to be both thought provoking but also sensible and manageable. To have, where possible, a consistency from adults as personal identity and social & cultural influences shape children’s views of what is right behaviour. So any differences within communities of ethnicity, culture, faith, etc., need to be celebrated and similarities fostered. Children will need to continue to learn different sets of rules for different situations and it is up to 33 the adults to help to make it clear which rules are for, where and why. Maybe for the school in question there would seem to be a need for clearer messages on guidelines and rules and, where possible, a more cohesive approach with the community on moral approaches. That the children have a very good understanding on the principles behind the moral guidelines so that they are adhering for the right reasons, not just so that they get a reward! The school in question has always felt it advocated a ‘firm but fair’ attitude and that any action as a consequence of wrong behaviour was not just for the punishments sake but as a learning process. This is hard to guarantee for every school staff adult, especially those who do not have good discipline skills and this maybe a skill the school could develop for both its staff and parents so that children have a more even response to behaviours. The school does involve its youngsters in discussions, processes and decisions during assemblies, circles times, class and school councils and lessons, but again this may be an area that needs reinvigorating, re-training, resourcing and supporting by senior members of staff or even outside agencies. Supporting the children to learn alternative strategies on how to solve problems rather than through unacceptable behaviours is a key learnt skill. The idea of not always just telling an adult but trying to work out the right and wrong actions and following them through acceptable processes, like not crowding each other, not having a cross face, respecting each other and listening to other views. Helping the children to think up the rules in classrooms and playground has always been a part of this school but it needs constant revision and re-visiting. The whole process of research has been both fascinating and extremely helpful for this head teacher. Having time to focus on a single theme instead of the multitude that is the norm of headship has been a privilege. It is clear that the school in question is not failing in its children’s moral education; rather it would seem that for many it is giving them the knowledge and skills that are required to survive in this changing and often seeming ‘immoral’ society. However, the research has highlighted the importance of a common vision and moral purpose and the need to include all of the school community in the process of devising and revisiting any vision and purpose. 34 35 References & Bibliography Aristotle (1962) Nichomachean Ethics, tr. M. Ostwald, Indianapolis: Liberal Arts Press Bandura, A. (1997) Self-Efficacy The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company Bigger,S. & Brown, E. (1999) Spiritual, Moral, Social and Cultural Education Exploring Values in the Curriculum. Trowbridge: Fulton Pub. Brown, A. & Brown, E. (1997) Religious Education in the Primary School. London: National Society. Crain,W.C.(1985). Theories of development. Prentice-Hall, pp118-136 Gross, R. & Rolls, G. (2004) Essential A2 Psychology. Oxford: Hodder Education Halstead, J.M., Pike, M.A. (2006) Citizenship and Moral Education. London: Routledge Knowles, J. Gary & Cole, (2008) A. Handbook of the ARTs in Qualitative Research. California: Sage pub. Langer,S.K. (1957) Problems of Art: Ten philosophical lectures. New York: Scribner. Lee, N. (2001) Childhood and Society Growing up in an age of uncertainty. Buckingham: Open University Press Levine, L. & Munsch, J. (2011) Child Development An Active Learning Approach. London: Sage Pub Inc. Lindon, J. (2001) Understanding Children & Young People Development from 5-18 years. London: Hodder Mcghee, P. (2001) Thinking Psychologically. Hampshire: Palgrave. McNiff, S. (10/2/2007) 3. Art-Based Research. Knowles (Handbook)=45358.qxd online Raths, L.E., Harmin, M. and Simon, S.B. (1966) Values and Teaching: Working with Values in the Classroom, Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill. 36