Download Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and Psychological

Document related concepts

Antisocial personality disorder wikipedia , lookup

Personality disorder wikipedia , lookup

International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems wikipedia , lookup

History of psychiatry wikipedia , lookup

Spectrum disorder wikipedia , lookup

Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup

Generalized anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Child psychopathology wikipedia , lookup

History of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup

Pyotr Gannushkin wikipedia , lookup

Classification of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders wikipedia , lookup

Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup

Narcissistic personality disorder wikipedia , lookup

Abnormal psychology wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine
DigitalCommons@PCOM
PCOM Psychology Dissertations
Student Dissertations, Theses and Papers
2004
Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and
Psychological Disorders Across Diagnostic Axes
Bradley Michael Rosenfield
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine, [email protected]
Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.pcom.edu/psychology_dissertations
Part of the Clinical Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation
Rosenfield, Bradley Michael, "Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders Across Diagnostic Axes"
(2004). PCOM Psychology Dissertations. Paper 119.
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Dissertations, Theses and Papers at DigitalCommons@PCOM. It has been
accepted for inclusion in PCOM Psychology Dissertations by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@PCOM. For more information, please
contact [email protected].
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine
Department of Psychology
THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN COGNITIVE DISTORTIONS AND
PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS ACROSS DIAGNOSTIC AXES
By Bradley M. Rosenfield
Submitted in Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree of
Doctor of Psychology
December 2004
PHILADELPHIA COLLEGE OF OSTEOPATHIC MEDICINE
DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY
Dissertation Approval
th
This is to certify that the thesis presented to us by Bradley Rosenfield on the 5 day of
October 2004, in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for the degree of Doctor of
Psychology, has been examined and is acceptable in both scholarship and literary quality.
Committee Members' Signatures:
Robert A. DiTomasso, Ph.D., ABPP, Chairperson
Michael Ascher, Ph.D.
Carrie L. Yurica, Psy.D.
Robert A. DiTomasso, Ph.D., ABPP, Chair, Department of Psychology
iv
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Sincere thanks to all of those who have encouraged, mentored and otherwise supported
my academic and professional development leading up to and throughout the dissertation
process. Specifically, Dr. Robert DiTomasso, Committee Chairperson, has provided invaluable
professional, didactic, and technical guidance. Beginning in my very first class at PCOM and
continuing throughout the entire internship and dissertation process, I have been grateful for his
selfless support, enthusiasm, and supervision. He has been incredibly helpful to me and to my
fellow students despite his ever-increasing responsibilities.
Equal gratitude is extended to my committee members: Dr. Carrie Yurica, for her
pioneering research into cognitive distortions, which served as the foundation for the present
study, and for her selfless guidance and support whenever it was most needed. I would also like
to thank Dr. Michael Ascher for his professionalism, guidance, support, and good sense of
humor.
Special thanks to Dr. Arthur Freeman for his keen ability to identify and nurture each
student’s unique and often undiscovered talents. Dr. Freeman has always gently guided and
encouraged many students’ professional development, while keeping them on the right academic
and professional path. He has always gone well beyond the norm in making himself cheerfully
accessible to hard-working students.
Sincere appreciation is also extended to study collaborators and participants, who have
brought this research to fruition and furthered science, in general.
Finally, deepest thanks to my parents for their efforts, sacrifice, and sincere love for all of
their children. They are the two most caring and, yet competent and dynamic people with whom
v
a child could be blessed. They have both always modeled the deepest concern for others, and the
highest ethical behavior. Of course, neither I, nor the present study, would be here without them.
vi
Abstract
The Relationship Between Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders Across Diagnostic
Axes
The purpose of this study was twofold: First, the study sought to determine whether or
not cognitive distortions correlated with the number and severity of psychological disorders
across diagnostic axes. Second, the study endeavored to assess further the validity and reliability
of a promising new self-report measure, The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions (ICD), by
correlating this instrument with clinical diagnoses as determined by the Millon Clinical
Multiaxial Inventory-III. The sample was selected from a heterogeneous adult outpatient
population. Participants meeting inclusion criteria presented for psychological treatment or
assessment. Excluded were participants who had less than an eighth grade education, or who met
diagnostic criteria for a number of neurological disorders. The study was correlational in design.
Results supported the reliability and validity of the ICD as an assessment instrument measuring
cognitive distortions, because the ICD determined that approximately half of the variance in both
the number and severity of psychological dysfunction, on both Axis I and Axis II, was accounted
for by the frequency of cognitive distortions.
vii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Prevalence of Mental Health Disorders …………………………………………
Rise of the Cognitive Model …………………………………………………….
Future Directions for Cognitive Therapy ……………………………………….
Future Directions for Cognitive Therapy ……………………………………….
Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorder: Statement of the Problem ….
1
2
6
8
9
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
Beck’s Original Model of Cognitive Therapy …………………………………..
Contemporary Models of Cognitive Therapy …………………………………...
Contemporary Models of Cognitive Behavior Therapy ………………………...
Varieties of Cognitive Distortions ………………………………………………
Cognitive Distortions: Origins of Terms ………………………………………..
Cognitive Processing Models of Cognitive Distortions …………………………
Evolutionary Models of Cognitive Distortions ………………………………….
Developmental Theories, Cognition, and Cognitive Distortions ………………..
The Efficacy of Cognitive Therapy ……………………………………………...
Cognitive Therapy for Depression ……………………………………………….
Depression Complicated by Personality Disorders ………………………….
Cognitive Therapy for Anxiety Disorders ……………………………………….
Anxiety Complicated by Personality Disorders ………………………………
Demonstration of Treatment Outcome ………………………………………….
Outcome and Measures of Cognitive Distortion ………………………………..
Measuring Cognitive Distortions ………………………………………………..
Limitations of Existing Measures of Cognitive Distortion ……………………...
The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions …………………………………………
Outcome and Measures of Psychopathology ……………………………………
The Minnesota Multipahasic Personality Inventory-2 ……………………….
The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III …………………………………
Limitations of the MCMI—III ………………………………………………
Cognitive Distortions in Personality Disorders and Clinical Syndromes ……….
Rationale for the Study ………………………………………………………….
The Purpose of the Study ………………………………………………………..
Research Hypotheses ……………………………………………………………
Definition of Terms ……………………………………………………………...
14
15
19
20
21
22
26
30
34
36
38
43
45
48
48
49
51
52
55
55
56
66
67
73
75
78
86
viii
CHAPTER 3
METHODOLOGY
Participants ………………………………………………………………………
Inclusion Criteria ………………………………………………………………..
Exclusion Criteria ……………………………………………………………….
Participant Recruitment …………………………………………………………
Research Design …………………………………………………………………
Measures: The Million Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-Third Edition ……………
MCMI—III reliability ……………………………………………………….
MCMI—III validity ………………………………………………………….
Measures: The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions ……………………………..
ICD Psychometrics ………………………………………………………….
89
90
90
91
91
92
93
94
94
95
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
Descriptive Statistics …………………………………………………………….
Relationship to Demographic Variables ………………………………………...
The Relationship between the MCMI-III and the Total ICD Scores ……………
98
99
99
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION
Demographic Characteristics ……………………………………………...
Major Findings: Presence of psychopathology ………………………………….
Axis I Clinical Syndromes: Number of Axis I diagnoses ……………………….
Axis I Clinical Syndromes: Severity of Axis I clinical syndromes ……………..
Axis II Personality Disorders: Number of personality disorders ………………
Axis II Personality Disorders: Severity of personality disorders ……………….
Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Disorders …………………………………………
Number of comorbid clinical syndromes and personality disorders ……………
Total severity of all psychological dysfunction …………………………………
The Relationship between Cognitive Distortions and Individual Disorders ……
Limitations of the Study …………………………………………………………
Recommendations for Future Research …………………………………………
Conclusions ……………………………………………………………………...
107
107
108
109
110
111
111
112
113
114
120
121
122
REFERENCES ……………………………………………………………………...
124
ix
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1. Summary of meta-analytic findings ……………………………………….
Table 2. Hypothesis Summary ……………………………………………………...
Table 3. Analysis of Variance for Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Conditions ………
Table 4. Number and Severity of Psychological Disorders across Axis I
and Axis II and the Relation to Total Frequency of
Cognitive Distortion ………………………………………………...
Table 5. The Relationship between Frequency of Cognitive Distortion and
Individual Psychological Disorders ………………………………...
35
78
102
103
105
x
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1. Illustration of interpersonal dimensions ………………………………….
62
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Prevalence of Mental Health Disorders
Psychological disorders are surprisingly common in the United States and abroad.
According to The U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1999) approximately 22% of
American adults over the age of 18 years—or over 44 million people—suffer from a diagnosable
mental disorder in a given year. The most common maladies are the anxiety and depressive
disorders, which affect 16.4 and 7.1%, respectively, of the American population each year.
Personality disorders (PDs) are believed to cause distress for 2.1% of the American population
and for many of those with whom they interact. It is also estimated that schizophrenia affects
1.3% of the U.S. population. Comorbidity among the various disorders, even across clinical axes
is also common (Reiger, Narrow, Pae, Manderscherd, Lockem & Goodwin, 1993; Kessler,
McGonagle, Zhoa, Nelson, Hughes, Eshleman et al, 1994 ; Weissman, Bland, Canino, Faravelli,
Greenwald, Hwa et al, 1997; Millon, 1999).
It is logical that millions of those suffering from these disorders would seek treatment to
alleviate their distress and to improve their functioning. Empirically supported psychological
treatments have found increasing acceptance among professionals, given the pressures arising
from managed care, from fiscal constraints, from the public’s growing psychological
sophistication, and from potential accusations of malpractice should therapy be ineffectual, or
worse, cause harm (e.g, American Psychological Association [APA], 2004; Hawley & Weisz,
2
2002). Cognitive therapy (CT) is in the vanguard of evidenced-based psychological treatments,
both in the U.S. and, increasingly, around the world (Dobson & Khatri, 2000).
The Rise of the Cognitive Model
CT evolved during a tumultuous time in the development of effective psychotherapy. During
this period two competing, dominant factions, the psychoanalytic (e.g., Freud, 1899/1962;
Sulloway, 1979) and behavioral schools (e.g., Skinner, 1958; Wolpe, 1958) were competing with
a popular nonconformist theory: the client-centered humanistic approach (Rogers, 1951).
While yet a practicing psychoanalyst, Beck (1961) adroitly discerned that his depressed
patients were often vaguely aware of certain negative thoughts that they frequently failed to
report during free association. These negative thoughts composed part of a constellation of
cognitions, which he described as “verbal and pictorial events in the stream of consciousness…
based on attitudes, assumptions,” expectations, and schema (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979,
p. 4). Because these cognitions resided mainly outside of conscious control and occurred without
volition, they were labeled automatic. As a trained psychoanalyst, Beck compared these
automatic thoughts to Freud’s notion of the preconscious. This original cognitive model of
depression proposed that patients may not have been aware of the automatic thoughts, yet they
could become acutely aware of the negative affect that such cognitions could engender. These
automatic thoughts were subject to specific types of logical errors or cognitive distortions, which
were labeled “selective abstraction, overgeneralization, dichotomous thinking, and exaggeration
(of the negative aspects of their experiences)” (Beck, 1991, p. 368). One’s cognitions, arising
from past experience, become habitual patterns of thought about the self, the world, and the
3
future, which profoundly influence one’s affect and behavior (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et
al., 1979; Beck, Freeman, Davis, & Associates, 2004; Beck Wright, & Newman, 1993; Ellis &
Geiger, 1977).
Moreover, both Ellis (1958, 1962, 1973) and Beck (1961, 1964, 1967, 1976) recognized that
neither excavation of early childhood trauma nor modification of stimulus-response patterns was
sufficient to produce enduring change in human emotion and behavior. They similarly viewed
the non-directive humanistic approach as inefficient and without empirical support. Instead,
these theorists discerned a number of patterns of dysfunctional thinking among their patients. For
example, Ellis (1958, 1962, 1973) proposed 11 irrational beliefs that he believed predisposed
individuals to negative emotional and behavioral consequences.
Similarly, Beck (1967) coined the term “cognitive therapy” after discovering a correlation
between the negative thinking and the moods of his depressed patients. By identifying,
exploring, and modifying these frequently inaccurate cognitions, Beck (1964, 1967, 1979)
witnessed a distinct improvement in his patients’ moods. Consequently, Ellis, Beck and their
adherents viewed dysfunctional cognitive processes as primary targets for psychotherapy.
Beck further proposed that automatic thoughts, which arise from interpretations of events,
are themselves based on a network of secondary beliefs, assumptions, formulas, and rules that
are often connected to relevant memories. At a still deeper level are more absolute, dysfunctional
beliefs about the self, or schemas, such as “I am unlovable.”; “I am a loser.”; “I am worthless.”
These cognitive structures develop early in life from personal experiences, from identification
with others, and from reinforcement (Beck, 1967; Beck & Weishaar, 1995). Thus persistent
patterns of generalizing, deleting, and otherwise distorting internal and external stimuli foster a
confirmatory bias which supports a constellation of beliefs that may not accurately reflect one’s
4
current environment (Alford & Beck, 1997; Yurica, 2002). When this occurs, cognition is said to
be distorted.
For example, one’s belief system can shape experience in ways that are analogous to
optical lenses. Prescription glasses that are carefully crafted to help a child to see the world more
clearly may need to be adjusted as the child matures. Lenses that are not properly altered may
later distort events so drastically that they impair functioning and cause great distress.
Similarly, schemas or subjective beliefs about one’s own self, shaped by early childhood
experience may foster rules, assumptions, and expectations that no longer suit the real world.
When this occurs, cognitive distortions may arise that systematically misrepresent reality, cause
impairment and distress, and prevent individuals from resolving their own problems. Thus,
cognitive distortions play a fundamental role in the onset, maintenance, and ultimately, the
amelioration of all manner of psychological dysfunction, be they Axis I and II disorders (e.g.,
Beck et al., 2004). Beck et al. (1979) proposed that individuals distort reality in six distinct
ways. Since then, many types of cognitive distortions have been postulated (Burns, 1980, 1999;
Freeman & Oster, 1999; Yurica, 2002), all of which represent inaccuracies and misattributions in
interpreting interpersonal and contextual information (Giancola, Mezzich, Clark, & Tarter,
1999).
From the original theoretical cognitive distortions, Yurica (2002) provides empirical
support, via factor-analysis, for 11 cognitive distortions, including 1) Externalization of SelfWorth, 2) Fortune-Telling, 3) Magnification, 4) Labeling, 5) Perfectionism, 6) Comparison with
Others, 7) Emotional Reasoning, 8) Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusions, 9)
Minimization, 10) Mind-Reading, 11) Emotional Reasoning and Decision-Making.
5
Cognitively based therapies have been conceptualized in a variety of ways. Ellis (1962)
originally po sited the ide a that irrational beliefs foster emotional distress and functional
impairment. Alternately, Meichenbaum (1975) suggested that self-talk, that is, one’s internal
dialogue is the fundamental mediator of affect and behavior. Finally, it has been postulated that
both emotional disorders (Beck, 1967, 1976) and PDs (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al.,
2004) result from maladaptive cognitive processing, manifested by dysfunctional core beliefs
and associated cognitive distortions.
CT and cognitive-behavior therapies (CBT) have subsequently evolved to include CT (Beck,
1967; Beck et al., 1979), rational emotive behavioral therapy (Ellis & Grieger, 1977), problemsolving therapy (D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1990; Nezu, 1986), narrative therapy, (e.g., White & Epston,
1989), and self-instructional therapy (Meichenbaum, 1993).
Moreover, cognitively oriented therapies have gained growing acceptance throughout the
mental health community and, more generally, across the entire healthcare industry
(Meichenbaum & Turk, 1987; Robins, Gosling, & Craik, 1999). In fact, cognitively oriented
psychotherapy has gained such prominence that Robins, et al. (1999) determined this modality to
be the fastest growing choice for treatment throughout the last four decades, as measured by the
subject matter of published dissertations, leading psychological journals, and subdisciplinary
journals. Consequently, according to these objective measures, the preeminent psychotherapy
models today appear to be the cognitive and cognitive behavioral. And, common to these schools
of thought is the recognition of the fundamental role of cognitive distortion in psychopathology.
6
Future Directions for Cognitive Therapy
Progress in psychotherapy has been predicated upon a number of important variables. First,
the science and profession of clinical psychology has become increasingly concerned with
incorporating scientifically supported treatment into progressively more evidence-based clinical
practice. Thus, the synthesis of the scientist-practitioner (Raimy, 1950) and practitioner-scholar
models has allowed the field to advance, incrementally and systematically, by incorporating
empirical findings and scholarly theories directly into clinical practice (Belar & Perry, 1992).
Next, legitimate empirical research is itself based upon the use of standardized procedures in
order to allow consistent application of procedures and to facilitate replication. This requires
valid and reliable instruments to measure results (Kazdin, 1998). Consequently, the validation of
psychometrically sound instruments is a prerequisite for the advancement of empirically
validated treatments for wide range of psychological disorders.
Finally, pressures from managed care and from distressed patients seeking relief have
increased the need for optimally efficient and effective therapy (McDaniel, 1995). This has
generated increasing demand for manualized treatment approaches with proven efficacy, which
can be further augmented by incorporating empirically validated, standardized assessment
measures (Gilson & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004). Moreover, it is incumbent upon both
scientists and practitioners to demonstrate successful treatment outcome objectively. This
requires the measurement of cognitive and behavioral variables (Dobson & Khatri, 2000).
Beck (1993) asserted that CT has gained preeminence because of its reliance on scientific
method and ability to assimilate empirical findings. Evidence for effectiveness of CT and CBT
has been demonstrated for the treatment of the following disorders: major depressive disorder,
7
panic disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, eating disorders,
post-traumatic stress disorder, sexual dysfunction, substance abuse, and PDs (Agras, Rossiter,
Arnow, Schneider, Telch, Raeburn, et al., 1992; Butler & Beck, 2000; Beck, & Freeman, 1990;
Chambliss & Olendick, 2001; Deblinger, Steer & Lipman, 1999; DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph,
1999; Woody, McLellan, Luborsky, & Obrien, 1990; Young, 1990/1999). Cogntive therapy is
showing promise even in the treatment of schizophrenia (Beck, 2000) and bipolar disorder
(Newman, Leahy, Beck, Reilly-Harrington, & Gyulai, 2004).
There is also empirical support for the association between cognitive distortions and a
number of other maladaptive social and clinical conditions, including sexual assault (Baumeister,
Catanese, & Wallace, 2002), pathological gambling (Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan,
2002), adolescent anxiety and depressive disorders (Kendall, Kortlander, & Brady, 1992),
violence and anger in marital relationships (Ekhardt, Barbour, & Davison, 1998), and adolescent
depression and anxiety (Kolko, Brent, Baugher, Bridge, Birmaher, 2000). To the extent that
dysfunctional cognition and its corollaries contribute to these problems, there is hope—and
evidence—for the effectiveness of CT in their amelioration.
Given the future trends in the evolution of psychological research, clinical practice, and CT,
this ability to provide empirical validation for a promising new measure of cognitive dysfunction
could provide further support for the cognitive model, provide clinicians with a valuable
assessment instrument, and further advance the field. Moreover, identifying the extent to which
cognitive distortions contribute to psychological dysfunction across Axis I and Axis II should
add practical knowledge to aid professionals when assessing and treating these troubling
disorders.
8
Current Clinical Practice
Identifying cognitive distortions in current clinical practice among cognitive therapists
generally involves the use of the various thought records, such as the Dysfunctional Thoughts
Record (Beck, et al., 1979), Thought Record (Persons, Davidson, & Thompkins, 2001), and
Burns (1999) Daily Mood Log. Although these instruments may illuminate distorted cognition,
they fail to identify specific cognitive distortions; instead, this task is left to the clinician.
Moreover, even after completing various thought records, the process of identifying cognitive
distortions is generally neither standardized nor quantitative (Yurica, 2002).
Results of randomized clinical trials supported the efficacy of such thought records for
modifying dysfunctional thinking in depressed outpatients (Craighead, Craighead, & Ilardi,
1998; DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph, 1998). However, there are a number of impediments to using
thought records. For instance, Persons et al., (2001) found that the following factors were
impediments to proper application: (a) difficulty eliciting patients’ automatic thoughts, (b)
patients’ reluctance to use the instruments both in and outside of treatment sessions, (c)
noncompliance with homework assignments, (d) patients’ beliefs that the procedure would not
be helpful.
In addition, it appears that clinicians use indirect and non-standardized methods for
identifying specific types of cognitive distortions because the various assessment tools intended
to assess dysfunctional thinking are neither widely known nor employed in the field and
inadequately explain specific varieties of cognitive distortions independent of clinical diagnosis;
some of these assessment tools include: the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS; Weissman &
9
Beck, 1978; Weissman, 1979), Cogntive Errors Questionnaire (CEQ; Lefebvre, 1981), and
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Hollon & Kendall, 1980) In practice, this makes
identifying, modifying, documenting, and predicting cognitive distortions problematic and
unnecessarily time-consuming (Yurica, 2002).
Cognitive Distortions and Psychological Disorders:
Statement of the Problem
Although the cognitive model recognizes the fundamental role of cognitive distortion in the
genesis and maintenance of all manner of psychological disturbance, only one recently validated
measure exists that identifies the frequency and categorical use of cognitive distortions by mental
health patients. Although promising, this instrument, the ICD, has been validated only for
anxious and depressed populations. Moreover, a review of the literature revealed that the DAS
was useful in distinguishing individuals with either Axis I or Axis II conditions from controls;
however, there was no support for discriminant validity of the DAS in identifying specific PDs
or Axis II Clusters, and there was little research into how individuals with specific Axis I and
Axis II conditions differ in their tendency to engage in cognitive distortion (Ilardi & Craighead,
1999).
The overall purpose of the present study was twofold: First, the study sought to determine
whether or not cognitive distortions correlated with the number and severity of psychological
disorders across Axis I and Axis II. Second, the study intended to assess further the validity and
reliability of a promising new self-report measure of cognitive distortions, the ICD and was to
correlate this instrument with clinical diagnoses as determined by the MCMI-III.
10
A literature review revealed no research addressing the contribution of the frequency of both
cognitive distortions in a heterogeneous adult outpatient sample meeting criteria for a wide range
of DSM-IV disorders across both Axis I and Axis II. Most studies were limited to investigations
of circumscribed diagnoses, such as depression or anxiety, with the possibility of another added
variable, that is, comorbid PDs (e.g., Ilardi and Craighead, 1999). Moreover, no studies were
found assessing the relative contribution of the frequency of cognitive distortions both to the
number and the severity of disorders. This lacunae is problematic; because if, as cognitive theory
and research have indicated, cognitive distortions contribute to psychological disorders, an
additional understanding is needed of the many patients, especially those with PDs, who present
with multiple diagnoses across both axes (Beck et al., 2004; Millon & Davis, 1996).
Consequently, providing further validation for the ICD, a brief, portable, and objective
measurement of these key cognitive processes should be beneficial both to research and to
practice in a number of important respects. First, cognitive therapists can provide convincing
post-treatment evidence of successful treatment outcome by using the ICD to establish objective
baselines and regularly monitor behavior and mood utilizing empirically valid and reliable
instruments. Although such measures are helpful in diagnosis and treatment planning for a
variety of disorders, there is a paucity of empirically supported measures designed to assess
cognitive distortions directly—conceptualized as one of the fundamental cognitive
dysfunctions—that have been validated for the a wide range of clinical diagnoses. Accurately
identifying specific patterns of cognitive distortions early in the course of treatment might assist
the clinician to target more efficiently dysfunctional thought patterns that maintain and
exacerbate problems. Moreover, an instrument which can provide regular assessment of
cognitive distortions throughout treatment can provide an objective measure of progress because
11
cognitive distortions have been shown to correlate with a number of Axis I disorders; for
example, depression, (e.g., Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erdbaugh, 1961; Butler & Beck,
2000; Hollon & Kendall, 1980; Yurica, 2002) anxiety (Ross, Gottfredson, Christensen, &
Weaver, 1986; Yurica, 2002) and PDs (Ilardi and Craighead, 1999).
Second, it has been shown that treatment occurring earlier in the course of psychological
disorders correlates with a number of therapeutic benefits, including positive treatment outcome,
fewer office visits to physicians, and reduced health care costs (Bruns, 1998; Kaplan, 2000).
Thus, early identification of cognitive distortions might allow the therapist and patient to
collaboratively and more quickly illuminate and modify the underlying secondary beliefs and
schemas, which maintain those same cognitive distortions.
Third, the instrument could facilitate psychoeducational processes regarding patients’
particular patterns of cognitive distortions. If specific distorted thought patterns are noted, the
therapist may teach more adaptive cognitive and behavioral skills explicitly targeting those
distortions. For example, if the patient exhibits rigid patterns of perfectionism, it might be more
efficient to use decentering or scaling techniques as a first line of treatment (Beck & Freeman,
1990).
Fourth, there may emerge characteristic patterns of distortions that delineate particular
Axis I versus Axis II conditions. If the ICD supports this difference, its use may increase
diagnostic accuracy, which, in turn, may lead to more effective treatment selection.
Fifth, demonstrating a correlation between the amelioration of cognitive distortions and the
improvement of clinical symptoms for Axis I and Axis II conditions would lend further empirical
support to the cognitive model of psychological disorders on both diagnostic axes.
12
Sixth, the ICD has already demonstrated efficacy in correlating the chronicity of cognitive
distortions with depression and anxiety. If the same pattern emerges with PDs, the ICD will be
able to provide researchers and clinicians with an elegant tool to gauge quickly and accurately
the severity of pathology (Yurica, 2002). Furthermore, because the ICD directly assesses
cognitive processes that are central to maladaptive functioning and treatment, ascertaining the
severity of distortions may provide a roadmap to guide appropriate treatment planning by
allowing the clinician to adapt to the patient’s level of cognitive functioning.
Seventh, this study also sought to expand further the utility of the ICD beyond the
evaluation of anxiety and depression (Yurica, 2002) for use with additional Axis I and Axis II
conditions. It is hoped, also, that a sufficiently validated ICD will allow for greater flexibility in
empirical research by providing an additional assessment tool for the design and standardization
of empirical procedure, which can be employed to measure pathology and progress, aiding as
well in the replication of empirical research.
Finally, in a review of 136 research studies, Grove and Meehl (1996) determined that
empirically based personality assessment instruments are consistently equal to or superior to
less-structured clinical interview methods; these instruments increase the efficiency of the
assessment process, understand patients more fully, establish rapport, formulate an accurate
diagnosis, develop insight, plan optimum empirically guided treatment planning, and predict the
course of treatment (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Consequently, a brief self-report measure, such as
the ICD, administered early in the treatment process can efficiently increase diagnostic accuracy
and improve treatment planning; this may facilitate these benefits for clinicians and patients
alike. Such an instrument might also lessen the burden on the psychological and medical
communities by reducing the course of treatment and, by extension, health care costs.
13
Stated more psychometrically, the goals of the study are to 1) evaluate the construct
validity of the ICD in a sample of Axis I and Axis II patients, 2) determine the relationship
between the ICD with specific Axis I and II disorders, 3) establish the relationship between the
ICD and the MCMI—III, a well-established, multidimensional, valid and reliable measure of
Axis I and Axis II conditions, 4) further confirm the internal consistency of ICD content by
confirming the acceptable alpha coefficient levels originally found by Yurica (2002), and 5)
provide an instrument that will assist clinicians to target specific maladaptive cognitive
distortions regardless of diagnosis.
In summary, the purpose of this study is to further assess a promising instrument, the ICD,
for the measurement of cognitive distortions, as defined by Beck (1976), Beck et al., (1979),
Burns (1980, 1999), Freeman & Oster (1999), Gilson & Freeman, (1999), and Yurica (2002) as
these occur in various psychological disorders. It is hoped that this instrument will aid in
cognitive-behavioral treatment for adult clinical populations.
The subsequent literature review offers an empirical and theoretical basis for the further
validation of the ICD, as supported by relevant literature regarding 1) the original model of
cognitive therapy. 2) contemporary models of cognitive therapy 3) contemporary models of
cognitive behavior therapy 4) varieties of cognitive distortions 5) cognitive processing models of
cognitive distortions 6) evolutionary models of cognitive distortions 7) developmental theories
cognition and cognitive distortions, 8) efficacy of cognitive therapy 9) outcome and measures
and 10) limitations of existing measures of cognitive distortion.
14
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
Beck’s Original Model of Cognitive Therapy
Beck (1976; Beck et al. 1979) originally posited the theory that one’s affect and behavior are
powerfully influenced by one’s cognition, including one’s past, present, and future
interpretations of oneself, the world, and one’s future. Perception and experience were believed
to be active processes based on the interpretation of both internal and external stimuli. Further,
individuals were said to manifest certain trait-like cognitive patterns that left them vulnerable to
emotional distress (Beck, 1976; Beck et al., 1979). These cognitions did not necessarily
correspond to reality but were thought to be “based on attitudes or assumptions (schema),
developed from previous experiences.” (Beck et al., 1979, p.3). In other words, cognition may be
corresponding to previous experience rather than extant reality and may, therefore, be distorted.
Consequently, CT is founded on the premise that individuals can learn to recognize and
correct their distorted thinking and that more realistic thinking can result in amelioration of
clinical symptoms.
Consistent with the cognitive model of psychopathology, CT is designed to be structured,
directive, active, and time-limited, with the express purpose of identifying, reality-testing, and
15
correcting distorted cognition and underlying dysfunctional beliefs (Beck, 1967, 1976, Beck et
al., 1979).
Although modern CT employs interventions, which may be cognitive, behavioral,
interpersonal, or imaginal, (e.g., Beck, 1996; McMullin, 2000), the ultimate goal is to disconfirm
maladaptive beliefs and to correct cognitive distortions (Beck et al., 1979). Assumptions
underlying CT include the following:
1.
Perception and experience are active processes involving both inspection and introspection.
2.
Cognitions represent a synthesis of internal and external stimuli.
3.
The way one views an event is evident in one’s cognitions (thoughts and visual images).
4.
Cognitions—reflecting on oneself, on one’s past and future, and on other people—constitute
one’s stream of consciousness or phenomenological field.
5.
Modifications in cognition will alter affect, mood, and behavior.
6.
Psychotherapy allows the patient to become aware of cognitive distortions.
7.
Correcting these dysfunctional cognitions can lead to amelioration of clinical disorders.
Contemporary Models of Cognitive Therapy
There are a several competing theoretical models within the field of CT today. Despite
their differences, there are a number of common assumptions among cognitively oriented
therapists which still unite the field. For example, modern cognitively oriented therapists respect
the patient’s subjective experience. In addition, cognitively oriented therapy is active, directive,
empirical, and behavioral. Cognitive models generally draw on Beck's general schema theory
and the notion of automatic thoughts (Beck, 1976; Beck et al., 1979). This theory holds that core
16
beliefs and the content of schema are responsible for a myriad of cognitive, emotional, and
behavioral interactions—some of which may be dysfunctional. The maladaptive cognitive
processes, which may cause symptoms that arise to the level of clinical significance, include
cognitive distortions and maladaptive assumptions, rules, and other secondary beliefs (Alford &
Beck, 1997; Beck, 1996; Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004). Additionally, psychological
disorders may be maintained by schema- driven processes, particularly schema-related avoidance
and maladaptive compensatory strategies (Young, 1990/1999). To complicate matters,
conflicting and interacting beliefs and unhelpful coping strategies are involved in maintaining
certain disorders (Wells, 1997). In other words, the patients’ subjective perceptions are the
central focus of CT.
CT requires an active approach, with the therapist and patient collaboratively defining
problems, identifying goals, and negotiating the tactics and strategies to achieve those goals
(Beck et al, 1979, J. Beck, 1996; Freeman & Oster, 1979). Patients’ active collaboration is
essential for identifying, exploring, and modifying distorted cognitions and maladaptive
behavior.
Another salient characteristic of modern CT is the wide use of behavioral interventions.
However, as opposed to conditioning models, CT theorists attribute resulting change in emotion
and behavior to change in “verbal behavior” (Alford & Beck, 1997, p.60). In other words, the
cognitive model asserts that the ultimate goal of even the most behavioral interventions is
reducing distorted cognition and increasing rational thinking. It is then essential to generalize
these changes to important areas of the patient’s life (Beck et al, 1979; Freeman & Oster, 1999).
To facilitate the generalization, patients are oriented to the cognitive model and encouraged
to engage actively in between-session homework assignments (Beck et al., 1979; Freeman &
17
Rosenfield, 2002). Contemporary CT models are empirical. For instance, both intrasession CT
and intersession homework allow patients to test and to modify, empirically, the idiosyncratic
beliefs that maintain various clinical disorders on both Axis I and Axis II (Ilardi & Craighead,
1999; Alford & Beck, 1997, Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004; Freeman & Rosenfield,
2002). Homework activities may include, but are not limited, to activity scheduling, completing
thought records, self-monitoring, breathing retraining, graded task performance, mastery and
pleasure tasks, social skill building, and relaxation.
The original cognitive model has evolved as Alford and Beck (1997) synthesized the
subsequent two decades of research into their modified cognitive theory to include 10 “formal
axioms” (p. 15). First, schemas are conceptualized as meaning-making structures, through which,
individuals interpret given contexts in relation to the self.
Second, assigning meaning controls behavior, emotion, attention, and memory. Meaning
assignment can occur both at the automatic and deliberative levels. Third, there is a reciprocal
influence between cognition, affect and behavior. Four, “cognitive content specificity” (p. 16)
occurs when meaning is translated into particular patterns of attention, memory, emotion, and
behavior.
The fifth axiom, which is particularly germane, states that because meaning is constructed
by the individual, it may be incorrect in relation to specific contexts and goals. The authors state,
“When cognitive distortion or bias occurs, meanings are dysfunctional or maladaptive” (p. 16).
Cognitive distortions may include errors in interpretation and cognitive processing, or both. Six,
individuals are predisposed to particular cognitive distortions, leaving them predisposed to
specific syndromes. This is termed cognitive vulnerabilities. Seven, psychopathology is said to
result from cognitive distortions related to a slightly modified cognitive triad, reflecting
18
maladaptive interpretations “regarding the self, the environmental context (experience), and the
future (goals)” (p.16). Eight, events are interpreted on two levels: The Public or objective level
and a private level. The latter incorporates generalizations, implications, and significance. Nine,
“Three Cognitive Systems” provide for three levels of cognition, including (a) the automatic
level (unintentional/preconscious), (b) the conscious level, and (c) the realistic, adaptive,
rational, or metacognitive level. Changes occurring at the conscious level primarily account for
progress in CT.
Finally, the tenth axiom posits the idea that individuals’ schemas have evolved to assist them
to adapt to their environments. Schemas become maladaptive only when they are out of context
with the general social or physical environment.
In summary, Alford & Beck (1997) asserted that cognitive distortions arise from “patterns”
and “predispositions”, indicating that this maladaptive pattern of thinking may be trait-like rather
than state-like. This is a theory that has received some support, particularly with Axis II patients.
For example, Ilardi & Craighead, (1999) found that Axis II patients’ distortions, as measured by
the DAS, seemed to persist beyond amelioration of Axis I syndromes. However, in individuals
with only Axis I diagnoses, cognitive distortions receded as depressive symptoms remitted.
Thus, the tendency to engage in cognitive distortion was more trait-like in individuals with Axis
II disorders, whereas this propensity was more state-like in those with Axis I conditions. Once
again, theory indicates that cognitive distortions play a pivotal role in the genesis, maintenance,
and eventual amelioration of psychological disorders.
19
Contemporary Models of Cognitive Behavior Therapy
CT has always included cognitive and behavioral interventions in its repertoire (Beck et al.
1979; Ellis, 1962). In, perhaps, one of the most comprehensive reviews of cognitive-behavior
therapy (CBT), Brewin (1996) contended that CBT had evolved to a point at which it
encompassed diverse sets of theories, terms, and procedures. Historically, Brewin asserted that
the cognitive and behavioral camps suffered from an “absence of an explicit role for conditioning
in cognitive therapies and the absence of a role for verbal mediation in behavior therapies (p.
34)”; this led to a period of estrangement and disparagement between the two factions.
Today, various CBT schools are distinguished by their targeting of circumscribed disorders
or by targeting various generalized disorders. Additionally, some CBT theorists suggest that
interventions should be aimed at modifying conscious beliefs and representations rather than
modifying unconscious representations in memory. Interventions aimed at altering consciously
accessible beliefs subscribe to the theoretical notion of appraisal theories of emotion and
cognitive theories of emotion and motivation. Interventions aimed at modifying unconscious
representations are related to their theoretical bases in learning theory and in findings from
experimental cognitive psychology (e.g. Chambless & Gillis 1993, Dobson 1989, Hollon,
Shelton, & Davis, 1993). Regardless of theoretical underpinning, CBT is focused on cognitive
change, behavioral change, or both.
A comprehensive review of the literature (DeRubeis & Crits-Cristoph, 1997), indicated
differential results for interventions that were principally cognitive versus those that were
predominantly behavioral. For instance, predominantly behavioral interventions have
demonstrated good outcome with certain Axis I conditions, such as specific phobias, social
20
phobia, panic disorder, PTSD and agoraphobia. On the other hand, both PDs and depressive
disorders responded better to predominantly cognitive interventions. Leichsenring & Leibing
(2003) also demonstrated efficacy for CBT in treating PDs. Other studies have found that both
cognitive and behavioral interventions were effective (DeRubeis & Crits-Cristoph, 1997;
Rachman, 1999).
These facts indicate that there are cognitive and behavioral components both in Axis I and
Axis II disorders which can be ameliorated through either predominantly cognitive or mainly
behavioral means. However, one cannot discount the fundamental role of cognitive change
resulting from even the most behavioral interventions. Although both modern CT and CBT
approaches employ interventions which may be cognitive, behavioral, interpersonal, or imaginal
(e.g., McMullin, 2000), the ultimate theoretical goals are to disconfirm maladaptive beliefs and
to correct cognitive distortions (Beck et al., 1979; Butler & Beck, 1998). Consequently, it would
be beneficial both for CT and CBT practitioners to be able to assess these cognitive components,
which are pivotal correlates of treatment success; this is possible with valid, reliable instruments
for the myriad of disorders and comorbidities with which they are confronted. A literature review
related to cognitive distortions is considered next.
Varieties of Cognitive Distortions
The term cognitive distortion first appeared in the literature in Beck’s (1967) discussion of
depression. At that time, Beck theorized that his patients frequently engaged in cognitive
processing patterns that were systematic but erroneous. He recognized that such faulty
information processing predictably produced maladaptive emotion and behavior.
21
Cogntive Distortions: Origins of Terms
A number of leading CT and CBT theorists have proposed a variety of cognitive distortions
Beck (1967) originally postulated six varieties of distortion, including the following: (1)
absolutistic/dichotomous thinking, (2) arbitrary inference (3) minimization and magnification,
(4) overgeneralization, (5) personalization, (6) selective abstraction.
In an effort to make Beck’s concepts even more comprehensible to the average patient, and
borrowing from Ellis & Grieger (1986), Burns (1990, 1999) offered 10 cognitive distortions,
including: (1) all-or-nothing thinking (2) discounting the positive, (3) emotional reasoning, (4)
jumping to conclusions, (5) labeling, (6) magnification, (7) mental filter, (8) overgeneralization,
(9) blaming and personalization, (10) should-statements.
Additional cognitive distortions added important interpersonal and subjective dimensions,
such as (1) comparison, (2) externalization of self-worth, and (3) perfectionism (Freeman &
DeWolf, 1990; Freeman & DeWolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).
Later, Gilson & Freeman (1999) suggested that, in addition to distorted cognitive
processing, individuals often engage in fallacious thinking, such as: (1) fallacy of change, (2)
fallacy of worrying, (3) fallacy of fairness, (4) fallacy of ignoring, (5) fallacy of being right, (6)
fallacy of attachment, (7) fallacy of control, and (8) heaven’s reward fallacy.
Other investigators have suggested disorder-specific cognitive distortions that may
manifest in particular disorders. For example, in pathological gamblers: reframed losses and the
illusion of control over luck, (Toneatto, 1999); in incarcerated teenagers: self-serving and selfdebasing distortions (Barriga, Landau, Stinson, Liau, & Gibbs, 2000), and in PTSD patients:
22
preoccupation with danger and self-blame (Briere, 2001; Briere, Runtz, Giancola, Mezzich,
Clark, & Tarter, 1993).
.
Najavits (1993; Najavits, Gotthardt, Epstein, 2004) proposed a series of distortions
specifically related to substance use disorders, including, but not limited to: (1) The escape:
Inability to tolerate feelings or solve problems. The only escape appears to be drugs, self-cutting,
sleep, food, etc. (2) Dangerous permission: Patients give themselves permission for selfdestructive behavior. (3) Time warp: It feels as if negative feelings will go on forever. (4) Shortterm thinking: Focus on the short-term only, how one feels for a few minutes. (5) The good old
days: Remembering only the wonderful highs from a drug or an abusive relationship.
A survey of the above catalog of cognitive distortions, though not complete, illustrates the
fact that these processing errors may affect virtually every aspect of individuals’ lives. The
following review of cognitive processing models more fully illustrates the cognitive mechanisms
involved in these maladaptive thought patterns.
Cognitive Processing Models of
Cognitive Distortions
The concept of cognitive distortions has enjoyed wide spread acceptance in clinical
practice and considerable validation in empirical research (e.g., Najavitis et al., 2004; McGrath
& Repetti, 2002; Yurica, 2002). Kelly’s Personal Construct Theory (1955) proposed that people
develop anticipatory cognitive attitudes that results from past experience. A portion of this
cognitive organization revolves around schemas, which Kelly theorized were mental
organizations of information developed from past experience that are used to categorize new
events. Schemas facilitate encoding new events, orienting individuals to novel events, suggesting
23
where to look for more information, and providing default information to fill gaps in perception
and interpretation. These constructs direct, filter, encode, and evaluate new experience. In
addition, schema can influence memory, both proactive and retroactive. For instance, beliefs,
such as “others cannot be trusted” may lead to expectations and misinterpretation of events in
ways that are congruent with that belief. Additionally, language and the particular labeling of
events can also transform experience, so that a man with “shifty eyes” will be experienced in a
way that may be much different from perceiving that same man to be “cautious, observant, or
thoughtful” (Andersson, 1994; Chiu, Krauss, & Lau, Whorf, 1956). Beck (1963, p. 120)
recognized that one of the deleterious effects of “inexact labeling” was that it contributed to
distortion. Consequently, expectations and language can distort appraisals of objective reality.
There are a number of extant cognitive processing models (see Ingram, Miranda & Segal,
1998 for a review), which at the most fundamental level, attempt to explain how individuals
process information and perform cognitive tasks that affect all aspects of behavior and emotion.
However, Kendall’s cognitive taxonomy model is the most relevant to the present research
(Ingram & Kendall, 1986; Kendall, 1992). Kendall theorized that human cognition consisted of
the following four fundamental elements:
1.
Cognitive products or content: Thoughts, beliefs, and images. Self-referent speech
composed of stored and organized memories.
2.
Cognitive processes: Attention, encoding, and retrieval procedures.
3.
Cognitive structures. Mental templates that attend to or filter certain stimuli.
4.
Cognitive products: Thoughts, assumptions, beliefs.
Kendall (1985, 1992) further delineated faulty cognitive processing into active, cognitive
distortion rather than cognitive deficiencies; the latter is postulated as the result from de ficits in
24
cognitive functioning. According to the model, dysfunctional cognition is the predictable
byproduct of maladaptive cognitive processing, manifested as cognitive distortions. However,
according to Yurica (2002) it is also possible that the failure to think more actively or mindfully
(Teasdale, Segal, Williams, Ridgeway, Soulsby, & Lau, 2000) distorts one’s experience.
Social and cognitive psychologists alike have observed that cognition occurs on two
levels, one deep and effortful, and the other automatic and superficial (Chaiken & Trope, 1999;
Petty & Cacioppo, 1984; 1986). When individuals engage in more superficial thinking, they are
more liable to make errors in judgment based on emotion or on salient, but not necessarily
important aspect of the situation.
Consistent with this theory, there is increasing evidence that humans have at least two
partially independent ways of reaching decisions about important (often social) events. The first
uses an experiential system, employs heuristics, takes mental short cuts to reach judgments
quickly, resorts to emotional reasoning, is automatic, seems to rely more on conditioning, and is
congruent with early childhood schemas (Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Gilbert, 1998; Petty &
Cacioppo, 1984).
The second mode of cognitive processing involves a volitional, rational system, which
operates more slowly as it integrates stored information from memory and knowledge. This
system has the ability to incorporate logical deductive and abstract forms of reasoning in a more
conscious way and without undue influence of past experience and conditioning (Chaiken &
Trope, 1999; Power & Brewin, 1991; Gilbert, 1998; Petty & Cacioppo, 1984).
Investigators have determined that it is possible to reduce superficial processing by
increasing the perception of personal relevance (Petty & Caciappo, 1984) and allowing more
time for deliberation (Ratneswar & Chaiken, 1991). For example, students are more likely to
25
reduce rational decision-making strategies regarding the time needed to study for an upcoming
exam when they believe that will be exempt. Most relevant to the cognitive model of
psychotherapy, personal experience fosters cognitive processes, cognitive structures, and
cognitive products that confirm expectancies whether those expectations are rational or are
distorted. This complicated cognitive interaction is consistent with the notion of cognitive or
confirmatory bias (Beck et al., 1979). In CT, patients learn to identify instances of heuristic,
distorted thinking and then they are encouraged to engage actively in more rational, mindful
thinking (e.g., Teasdale, Moore, Hayhurst, Pope, Williams, Segal, 2002).
Interestingly, it is also possible that cognitive distortions can be beneficial and,
conversely, that promoting more accurate thinking may produce distress. In other words, there
are instances when cognitive distortions may be adaptive and that accurate thinking may
correlate with maladaptive behavior and distress. For example, it was determined that although
depressed patients engaged in more negative cognition than nondepressed individuals, the
depressed patients were actually more accurate in their thinking (Alloy & Abramson, 1988).
Support for this depressive realism was also demonstrated in more accurate thinking among
depressed breast cancer patients, as compared with their nondepressed counterparts; this type of
realism motivated the depressed individuals to treatment adherence (Keller, Lipkus, & Rimer,
2002). Opponents of this theory caution that the phenomenon has been demonstrated in severely
limited settings and suffers from lack of empirically derived criteria for the term accuracy (e.g.,
Pacini, Muir, & Epstein, 1998; Vazquez, 1987).
In addition, comparing oneself favorably with those perceived to be less fortunate, that is,
downward social comparison (Wood, Taylor, & Lichtman, 1985), increased self-esteem and
reduced stress among breast cancer patients. Similarly, the mere perception of control allowed
26
enhanced performance, inspired confidence, and produced higher self-esteem (Tafarodi, Milne,
& Smith, 1999).
Thus, the research is not without controversy regarding the costs and benefits of distorted
thinking. However, there is little debate that cognitive distortions are complex phenomena that
occur outside of conscious awareness and correlate with a host of psychological disorders,
dysfunctional behavior, and emotional problems.
Evolutionary Models of
Cognitive Distortions
In seeking to gain a deeper understanding of personality, Millon (1990) believed that it
was necessary to explore the common elements of nature and science beyond the unnecessarily
restrictive realm of human psychology. Evolution, Millon postulated, was the fundamental
process that explained and unified nature, the sciences, and, more specifically, emotion,
behavior, and cognition. Personality and PDs could be explained as attempts at adaptation and as
a product of phylogenetic (specific to genus or group) as well as ontogenetic (specific to the
individual) evolutionary progression.
Millon (1990; Millon et al, 1996) further asserted that personality was the individual’s
distinctive attempt at adapting to a given environment. PDs were the result of maladaptive and
deficient functioning. Such dysfunction was said to occur because of “protective constriction”
(akin to repression and avoidance) and cognitive distortion that restricted prospects for new
learning, misconstrued essentially benign events, and provoked undesirable reactions from
others; these problems foster a self-defeating vicious cycle of personal problems and new
27
interpersonal predicaments that further magnify distress and drive people further away from an
unbiased appraisal of reality (Millon et al, 1996, p. 15).
Gilbert (1998) viewed cognitive distortions as an evolutionarily adaptive response to the
perception of threat, rather than mere maladaptive thinking. Gilbert posited the idea that
“humans beings evolved to think adaptively, not rationally” (p. 448). Consistent with the
previously cited cognitive psychological research, Gilbert also asserted that negative thinking,
even when rational, might be dysfunctional at times.
For example, because the environment bombards people with so much data that it is
impossible to perceive, attend to, analyze, interpret and draw conclusions about every aspect of
all stimuli, humans are necessarily forced to make heuristic judgments about many events
(Kahneman & Tversky, 1972). This is especially true in the face of emergency situations—or
even the mere perception of threat—when there is little time to cogitate. For example, natural
selection might favor an individual who responds to a surprise encounter with a shadowy figure
in a darkened alley with the heuristic judgment of threat and a subsequent fight or flight
response; it might, therefore, favor another individual less who attempts logical discourse and
careful analysis during such an event (Cosmides, 1989). In this case, distorted thinking in the
form of a threat bias might prove to be adaptive. Thus, there are advantages to acting quickly
and in favor of a threat bias.
There is also evidence that moderate positive cognitive bias is a variety of cognitive
distortion that may be adaptive. For example, Krebs and Denton (1997) determined that when
individuals operate in groups, self-serving attributions, overestimation of personal abilities,
illusion of control, and increased risk-taking behavior correlated with greater self-esteem,
improved affect, and more adaptive behavior. Gilbert (1998) offered the following evolutionary
28
explanation and some empirical support for seven cognitive distortions that are frequently cited
in the literature:
1.
Selective Abstraction. Beck et al. (1979) referred to a tendency to focus on
negative details out of context. Gilbert hypothesized that this tendency resulted from
attentional biases. There is now much evidence that mood states do affect attentional
mechanisms on tasks such as the Stroop Test (Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996) and
that attentional biases and processing can occur without conscious awareness (Power &
Brewin, 1991) and that the perception, for example, of threat can be adaptive.
2.
Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusions (Beck et al., 1979). Emergency
situations require categorical thinking when determining whether or not an event poses a
threat. Decision-making is much more rapid when the number of possible choices is reduced,
for instance, to threat or no threat (Epstein, Lipson, Holstein, & Huh, 1992) . The principle of
“better safe than sorry” when dealing with threat will likely result in jumping to conclusions
and, according to Gilbert (1997; 1998) is the most salient cognitive distortion.
3.
Dichotomous/All-or-Nothing Thinking (Beck et al., 1979). Similar to jumping to
conclusions, dichotomous thinking illustrates rapid categorical thinking. Once the brain
perceives threat, it will resort to more categorical processing to reduce response time and
risk, which will result in action that may reduce the threat (Epstein et al., 1994; Gilbert,
1998).
4.
Emotional Reasoning (Burns, 1990). According to Gilbert (1998, p. 457), “… for
millions of years animals and humans have relied on fast track affect (emotional reasoning)
to make decisions about actions when under threat.” Once again, this is fast track thinking
29
that may increase genetic fitness in life and death situations by increasing the probability of
erring on the side of safety (Nesse, 1998).
5.
Disqualifying the Positives (Burns, 1990). Downplaying one’s own attributes, as
manifested in the form of modesty, is appropriate and even admired in certain contexts,
cultures, and in moderation (Gilbert, 1998). Numerous studies support the hypothesis that
modest people are better liked than those who boast (Baumeister & Jones, 1978; Rosen,
Cochran, & Musser, 1990; Wosinka, Dabul, Whetston-Dion, & Cialdini, 1996). Of course,
being liked enhances one’s attractiveness and increases opportunities to mate and pass on
those modest traits (Langlois, & Musselman, 1995).
6.
Social Comparison (Festinger, 1954). Both upward and downward social
comparison can enhance self-esteem, boost confidence, reduce stress, and encourage
perseverance on difficult tasks (Gibbons & Gerrard, 1989; 1991; Gilbert, 1998). It has long
been recognized that people compare themselves socially with others because it would be
adaptive to identify who is one’s superior or inferior before determining the most
advantageous way to interact with him or her. Comparison occurs even when this makes one
unhappy or depressed. Gilbert, (1998) posited the theory that social comparison is probably
one of the oldest forms of social cognition.
7.
Personalization and Blame (Beck et al., 1979). Attributions of self-blame may be
adaptive in a number of ways. First, it may offer some illusion of control, because negative
events that are perceived to result from one’s own negligence should be controllable. Second,
blaming oneself avoids attacks on others as well as the resultant retaliation. As Gilbert (1998)
suggested, “Even if not expressed, the desire to counter-attack when blaming others (nonsubmissive attitude) could be detected by the other and escalate the conflict” (p. 450). In
30
support of the adaptive nature of personalization of blame, Andrews & Brewin (1990), found
that abused women blamed themselves more for abuse while still in the abusive relationship,
but blamed their partners more after separation; that is, when the women were removed to a
safe distance, the risk of counter-attacks from the partners were reduced. Self-blame may
also elicit secondary reinforcement in the form of social support (Driscoll, 1988).
Gilbert (1998) concluded that evolutionary explanations for cognitive distortions have
implications for CT. For example, framing such distortions as the product of normal adaptations
that, with treatment may be modulated, normalizes the patient’s responses, provides insight, and
increases mindfulness. Although these seemingly automatic cognitive mechanisms may have
been adaptive throughout our evolutionary history, they can, however, cause immense problems
today, in creating, maintaining, and exacerbating psychopathology. The question remains, how
do cognitive distortions arise on the ontogenetic level?
Developmental Theories, Cognition, and
Cognitive Distortions
Yurica (2002) considered many of the following developmental theories when
formulating the ICD. Head and Holmes (1911) were among the first to use the term schema.
These investigators were interested in patients’ spatial assessment of their own bodies, referring
to their perceptions as the postural schema. Later, Piaget (1952) conceptualized the idea that the
human mind organized schema as internalized regularities (operations) into dynamic cognitive
structures, which he termed schema. Schema represented the relationships between perceived
environmental regularities, or concepts. With experience, humans develop associations and
31
perceive relationships among concepts and develop a rich variety of schema, which encompass
virtually every aspect of life.
Erikson (1963) proposed that schema or unconditional core beliefs develop when
individuals are infants and that children progress through four critical psychosocial stages of
development, occurring at circumscribed junctures from infancy through puberty. Layden,
Newman, Freeman, & Morse, (1993) produced a seminal work on the development of borderline
personality disorder patients by integrating Erikson and Piaget’s theories into a cognitive
framework. Synthesizing Erikson’s theory into a cognitive model of schema development,
Layden et al. (1993) suggested that failure at specific Eriksonian stages predisposes individuals
to acquiring maladaptive schemas. Success or failure at each of these stages results in the
following, respective outcomes:
Stage 1: Ages birth to 18 months. Trust versus mistrust. Mistrust and abandonment
schemas develop in response to failure to develop loving and trusting relationships
Stage 2: Ages: 18 months to 3 years. Autonomy versus shame and doubt. Failure to
develop physical control, such as in walking, grasping, and rectal-sphincter response may result
in schemas regarding competence which are related to shame and doubts.
Stage 3: Ages 3 to 6 years. Initiative versus guilt: Failure to take initiative or being
unusually assertive may lead to schemas related to dependence and emotional deprivation
Stage 4: Ages 6 to 12 years. Industry versus inferiority: Inability to deal with the growing
demands associated with new skills may lead to schemas of incompetence. Additional negative
schemas of unlovability and defectiveness may result at any stage (Layden et al., 1993).
Although infants begin with scant cognitive processing skills, these abilities, along with
reasoning, and problem-solving skills develop as infants interact with their new social and
32
physical environment (Piaget, 1952). As skills develop at certain phases of development, so too
may inaccurate thinking patterns or cognitive distortions (Layden et al., 1993). Layden et al.
further posited that cognitive distortions may also develop at discrete stages of cognitive
development as proposed by Piaget (1952), as follows:
Stage 1: Ages birth to 2 years. The Sensory Motor Stage. Consistent with Piaget’s notion
of egocentrism and a lack of object permanence occurring at this phase, Layden et al. (1993)
propose that the cognitive distortion of mind reading, in addition to entitlement and lack of
empathy develop in BPD patients at this stage.
Stage 2: Ages 2 to 7 years. The Preoperational Stage. During this stage, children think
concretely but are unable to engage in abstract logic. However, many cognitive distortions may
develop at this stage. For instance, when decentration or the ability to see the whole in
relationship to the parts, fails to develop adequately, Layden et al. (1993) proffer the idea that the
cognitive distortion of overgeneralization can result as individuals categorize similar objects on
the basis of only one or of unimportant characteristics. Additionally, the cognitive distortions of
emotional reasoning, dichotomous thinking, catastrophization, and perfectionism are thought to
develop during this phase.
Stage 3: Ages 7 to 11 years. The Concrete Operations Stage. With the accumulation of
physical experience, the children begin to conceptualize, creating logical structures to explain
physical experiences. This plants the seed for abstract reasoning, such as in arithmetic equations.
Stage 4: Ages 11 to 15 years of age. The child becomes capable of abstract thought, and
meatacognition—the capacity to think about thinking—allowing them to see wide varieties of
possible solutions for increasingly complex problems.
33
Layden et al. (1993) asserted that individuals with BPD exhibit a lack of cognitive
development during Stages three and four. This produces deficits in metacognition, theory
building, generation of alternatives, imaginal hypothesis testing, and a lack of deductive and
inductive reasoning skills; these may, in turn, distort reality, prevent adaptive functioning, and
produce immense interpersonal difficulties.
To summarize, the synthesis of the cognitive model with the Erikson’s and Piaget’s
developmental theories provides a cogent explanation for the generation of maladaptive schemas
and cognitive distortions. Moreover, the deficits in cognitive adaptation from these earlier stages
of cognitive development are quite appropriate for reciprocal CT techniques intended to reduce
the distortions, including “… teaching patients alternative ways of thinking (e.g., examining the
evidence, weighing the evidence, generating alternative explanations, imagining hypothetical
scenarios” and self-monitoring (Yurica, 2002, p. 37).
The above models, cognitive processing, evolutionary, and developmental, provide
elegant and convincing evidence for the genesis, maintenance, process, and amelioration of
cognitive distortions. The following is an explication of how these maladaptive thought patterns
are expressed in psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II; an explation of treatment
procedures is also included.
34
The Efficacy of Cognitive Therapy
CT is based on the principle that human emotion and behavior are heavily influenced by
cognition. Thus, consistent with cognitive theory, personality and clinical pathology correlate
with cognitive distortion (e.g., Beck, 1976; Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004; Yurica,
2002).
In research and practice, CT enjoys considerable empirical support for its efficacy in treating
many frequently occurring disorders, not only in a broad array of populations, but also in all
formats, such as individual, couples, group and family, and in both inpatient and outpatient
populations (Beck, 1993). Butler and J. Beck (2000) evaluated 14 meta-analyses, examining the
efficacy of CT in 325 studies with a total of 9,138 participants. Meta-analysis is a statistical
strategy that permits researchers to aggregate the results of several studies and to translate them
into standard units known as an effect sizes. In their review, these investigators examined how
CT outcomes compared with the outcomes of various control groups in terms of their effect
sizes, including the percentage of those participants’ responses that was superior to a variety of
no-treatment controls. Table 1 clearly illustrates the efficacy of CT for a host of Axis I
conditions.
35
Table 1
Summary of meta-analytic findings: Cognitive therapy vs. no-treatment, wait list, and placebo
controls (Butler & Beck , 2000).
SUMMARY OF META-ANALYTIC FINDINGS
Comparisons of cognitive therapy to no-treatment, wait list, and placebo controls.
Disorder
Adult unipolar depression
Adolescent unipolar depression
Generalized anxiety disorder
Panic disorder with or without agoraphobia
Social phobia
Childhood depression and anxiety disorders
Marital distress
Anger
Childhood somatic disorders
Chronic pain (not headache)
Average effect
size
% of CT
patients
superior to
controls
.82
1.11
1.04
.91
.93
.90
.71
.70
.47
.46
79%
87%
85%
82%
82%
82%
76%
76%
68%
68%
Note. Empirical evidence for the efficacy of CT over controls in magnitude of effect and
percentage of superior response. Reprinted with the permission of Judith Beck. Source:
http://www.beckinstitute.org/beck.html
36
Cognitive Therapy for Depression
The efficacy of CT for depression is now widely accepted. Empirical studies have
demonstrated the relationship between positive CT outcome and cognition. For example, Oei and
Sullivan (1999) found that participants who had recovered from depression, operationalized as
BDI scores below 10, engaged in less frequent negative automatic thoughts and reported higher
activity levels than their non-recovered counterparts.
In their comprehensive meta-analysis, Butler and Beck (2000) demonstrated the fact that
79% of adult unipolar patients responded to CT for depression with an average effect size of .82.
Even better results were found for adolescents, with 87% responding and an average effect size
of 1.11. This meta-analysis also determined that CT was superior to antidepressant medication
with an average effect size of .38 for adult unipolar depression. Perhaps the greatest benefit that
CT offered as being superior to medication alone was the fact that CT patients experienced less
than half the relapse rate of medicated patients at one-year follow up, 30% vs. 60%, respectively.
Additional mounting evidence also suggests that CT reduces relapse and recurrence of
depressive episodes. For example, a number of studies found that outpatients who recovered
from major depression following CT experienced less subsequent relapse or perceived need for
treatment than patients who recovered using pharmacotherapy; they are subsequently withdrawn
from antidepressant medication (Blackburn, Eunson, & Bishop, 1986; Evans, Hollon, DeRubeis,
Piasecki, Grove, Garvey et al, 1992; Shea, Elkin, Imber, Sotsky, Watkins, Collins, et al., 1992;
Simons, Murphy, Levine, & Wetzel, 1986). The utilization of CT following recovery with
37
pharmacotherapy also reduced subsequent relapse and recurrence of depression (Fava, Grandi,
Zielezny, Rafanelli, Canestrari, 1996; Fava, Rafanelli, Grandi, Conti, & Belluardo, 1998). Even
for patients responding only partially to antidepressant medication, the addition of CT to clinical
management and continuing antidepressant medication significantly reduced rates of relapse
(Paykel, Scott, Teasdale, Garland, Moore, Jenaway et al., 1999).
Additional evidence also suggests that CT reduces relapse and recurrence of depressive
episodes. For example, outpatients who recovered from major depression following CT
experienced less subsequent relapse or perceived need for treatment than patients who recovered
using pharmacotherapy and then were subsequently withdrawn from antidepressant medication
(Blackburn, et al., 1986; Evans, Hollon, DeRubeis, Piasecki, Grove, Garvey, & Tuason, 1992;
Shea, Elkin, Imber, Sotsky, Watkins, Collins et al, 1992; Simons, Murphy, Levine, & Wetzel,
1986). CT following recovery from pharmacotherapy also reduced subsequent relapse and
recurrence of depression (Fava, Grandi, Zielezny, Rafanelli, & Canestrari, 1996; Fava, Rafanelli,
Grandi, Conti, & Belluardo, 1998).
In a controlled clinical trial, Teasdale, Moore, Hayhurst, Pope, Williams, Segal (2002)
determined that the prevention of relapse from depression was mediated through changes in an
all-or-none, absolutistic thinking style, operationalized as an extreme dichotomous negative or
positive response set to a number of self-report instruments; these included the Attributional
Style Questionnaire (Peterson et al., 1982), Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Weissman & Beck,
1978), and the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1979). Dichotomous distortions were
inferred when questions received responses similar to the following: Causes of negative
outcomes "will never again be present." Interestingly, such extreme responding has been
determined as a marker of rapid, seemingly automatic processing, which is also consistent with
38
negative correlations between attitude extremity and response latency, as previously reported
(e.g., Bargh et al., 1992). Teasedale (et al., 2002) affirmed the pivotal role of such automatic
cognitive distortions as predicted by the cognitive model of depression. Also supported was the
hypothesis that relapse into depression was correlated with the sum total of dichotomous
cognitive distortions.
Depression Complicated by Personality Disorders
Millon, Davis, & Millon (1997) conceptualize Axis I clinical symptoms as “extensions
or distortions of the patient’s basic personality pattern... waxing and waning over time depending
on the impact of stressful situations.” (p. 22). The multiaxial model posits an interaction between
Axis I and Axis II symptomatology in which personality is defined as the “…overall capacity to
perceive and to cope with our psychosocial world…” (Millon et al., 1997, p.120). Personality is
further conceptualized as analogous to an immune system, protecting the individual from the
vicissitudes of external stressors. However, Axis I symptoms may occur because the PD patient’s
dysfunctional “immune systems” fails to protect the individual from stress. When this occurs,
Axis I clinical syndromes occur in addition to the preexisting PD symptoms. This increases the
total severity of the individual’s dysfunction (Millon et al., 1997) . In other words, all other things
being equal, an individual who meets the diagnostic criteria for both major depressive disorder
and dependent personality disorder would be expected to evince more impairment and/or distress
than another person with a diagnosis on either Axis I or Axis II.
According to Hirschfeld and Shea’s (1992) vulnerability model, like a compromised
immune system which fails to insulate one from disease, the distorted cognition associated with
39
PD’s may leave one vulnerable to the stressors that are noted on Axis IV. As Millon et al. (1997,
p. 124) assert:
Faced with recurrent failures, anxious about old and unresolved conflicts reemerging, and
unable to recruit new adaptive strategies, they (the personality disordered) may revert to
pathological ways of coping, to less adequate control over emotions, and ultimately, to
increasingly subjective and distorted perceptions of reality and to the production of clinical
symptoms.
This leads to a pattern in which cognitive distortion can constrict behavior in a fashion that
may actually exacerbate existing difficulties, leading to vicious circles of cognitive distortion,
distress, and interpersonal impairment. When this occurs, one’s cognition may be further
impaired by the resulting Axis I clinical syndromes. Prolonged Axis I symptoms can, in turn,
exacerbate personality dysfunction.
This reciprocal interaction between Axis I and Axis II explains why Millon et al. (1997)
posited the notion that the presence of Axis I pathology supports the presence of personality
pathology. Consequently, it is expected that individuals with greater total pathology, independent
of whether this pathology arises from Axis I or Axis II, will experience more frequent cognitive
distortions. Cognitive distortions have been reported in PDs (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al.,
2004) and account even for an overlap between some PDs, such as borderline and schizotypal
(Rosenberger & Miller, 1989).
Further evidence for the additive effects of depression and Axis II syndromes is offered
by studies in which depressed patients with at least one PD were found to have greater frequency
of previous episodes of depression, higher levels of distress, and more severe symptomatology
(Diguer, Barber, & Luborsky, 1993; Farmer & Nelson-Gray, 1990; Shea et al., 1987). Other
40
evidence suggests that personality disordered inpatients tend to suffer from earlier initial onset of
illness, longer duration of episode, and more suicidal thoughts and/or suicide attempts than those
without Axis II comorbidity (Black, Bell, Hulbert, & Nasrallah, 1988).
Moreover, Skodol, Gundserson, MacGlashan, & Dyck, (2002) recently conducted the first
study to document and quantify the extent of functional impairment experienced by patients with
varying types of PDs in contrast to patients having only an impairing Axis I disorder. This study
supported Millon et al.s’ (1997) contention that schizotypal and borderline personality disorders
are more severe, because they produced greater dysfunction on virtually every measure of
impairment than patients with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder or major depressive
disorder alone produced, even after controlling for comorbid Axis I psychopathology and
regardless of whether the assessment was interview-based or by patient self-report. Avoidant
personality disorder impairment was intermediate. No gender effects were found. It should be
noted schizotaypal, borderline, and to a lesser extent, avoidant personality disordered patients
were significantly more impaired at work, in social relationships, and at leisure than patients with
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder or major depressive disorder. These authors
convincingly concluded “Personality disorders are a significant source of psychiatric morbidity,
accounting for more impairment in functioning than major depressive disorder alone.”
Most patients with PDs enter treatment, seeking relief for an Axis I disorder. There have
been a number of studies evaluating treatment response in those meeting criteria for comorbid
Axis I and Axis II conditions. Ilardi and Craighead (1999) found that, in formerly depressed
patients, rates of relapse into depression occurred 7.4 times faster for patients with comorbid PDs
than for those without Axis II personality pathology. Additionally, Axis II pathology accounted
for approximately 29% of the variance in dysfunctional attitudes (as measured by the DAS) and
41
14% of the variance in maladaptive negative event attributions (as measured by the ASQ-N),
regardless of whether or not the patient relapsed into depression. This dysfunctional thinking
may suggest depressogenic vulnerability in individuals with Axis II disorders. Interestingly, for
each Axis II criterion met, there was an 8% decrease in expected survival of remission.
Moreover, there was a significant interaction between specific PD Clusters and outcome with
Clusters B and C associated with shorter remission times. Surprisingly, the presence of Cluster A
diagnoses was actually associated with longer remission. Otherwise, analysis of Axis II clusters
was largely consistent with a hypothesized general personality pathology factor associated with
dysfunctional cognitions. This study illustrates the fact that more effective identification and
amelioration of dysfunctional cognitions associated with comorbid PDs may provide great
benefit not only for predicting outcome, but also for combating both personality pathology and
depression. Thus, more effective identification and amelioration of dysfunctional cognitions
associated with PDs and depression may provide great benefit for predicting outcome and
combating depression.
Barber and Muenz (1996) found CT to be significantly more effective than interpersonal
therapy (IPT) in treating depression in Avoidant PD patients. Although IPT and CT were both
effective in treating PD patients with comorbid depression, those with obsessive personality
disorder (OCPD) responded significantly better when treated with IPT rather than CT. However,
CT was significantly superior to IPT for depressed patients with comorbid avoidant PD. It was
hypothesized the IPT differentially addressed perfectionistic, internal coping strategies
characteristic of OCPD, whereas CT was more effective in ameliorating the externalized coping
strategies, such as the need for approval, more common to avoidant PD. Nevertheless, common
to both of these effective treatment modalities is that both CT and IPT, in part, ameliorate
42
dysfunctional cognition (e.g., Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004; Klerman, Weissman,
Rounsaville, & Chevron, 1984).
Additional evidence for the interactive and additive nature of Axis I and Axis II
symptomatology arises from a naturalistic study demonstrating an important fact; in a
comparison of depressed patients without a personality disorder and of patients with depression
and PDs, the latter reported more severe depressive symptoms at intake and more residual
problems at the conclusion of CT. However, there was no evidence that patients with depression
and PDs benefited less from CT than depressed patients without a PD. Rather, treatment outcome
was negatively influenced by avoidant and paranoid beliefs whether or not a PD was diagnosed.
Specifically, avoidant beliefs predicted changes in self-reported depressive symptoms and
paranoid beliefs predicted changes in therapist-rated general functioning (Kuyken, Kurzer,
DeRubeis, Beck, & Brown, 2001).
Similarly, Hardy, Barkham, Shapiro, Stiles, Rees, & Reynolds, (1995) found that patients
with depression, which was complicated by Cluster C PDs also presented with more severe
symptomatology than patients with uncomplicated depression. Interestingly, there was an
interaction between the presence of severe PD and treatment modality in outcome. At
posttreatment and one-year follow-up, there was no significant difference between complicated
and uncomplicated depressed patients when treated with CBT. However, severely depressed PD
patients who were treated with psychodynamic—interpersonal psychotherapy continued to
maintain more severe symptomatology at posttreatment and at 1-year follow-up. This lends
further support to the central role of dysfunctional cognition, targeted, specifically by CBT, in
the maintenance and treatment of Axis I and Axis II conditions.
43
Cognitive Therapy for Anxiety Disorders
Butler and Beck (2000) included many of the following studies in their meta-analysis of
treatment outcome for CT with anxiety disorders:
Panic Disorder
CT research has been particularly impressive in regard to panic disorder. For example,
Sokol, Beck, Greenberg, Wright, & Berchick (1989) found CT to be significantly superior to
supportive therapy in eliminating panic attacks. Another study found CT to be significantly more
effective than behavior therapy, imipramine, and placebo control in ameliorating panic attacks at
the end of treatment, and at 1-year follow-up (Clark, 1991). Another important advantage of CT
was far lower attrition rates, as compared with behavior therapy (often employing aversive
exposure methods) or wait-list controls.
Gould et al. (1995) conducted a meta-analysis comparing cognitive interventions with
more behavioral interventions based on exposure treatments and various control conditions. The
most effective treatments were those that combined cognitive restructuring with interoceptive
exposure (ES = 0.88), the mainstay of the traditional CT approach (e.g., Barlow, 1988; Taylor,
2000). CT was superior to pharmacological interventions (ES = 0.47) or combination treatments
(ES = 0.56). One-year follow indicated excellent resiliency of treatment effects, with virtually no
slippage in effect size. Another advantage for CT was a significantly lower attrition rate, with
only 5.6% of those treated with CT relapsing within the first year after treatment, compared with
44
19.8% of those treated pharmacologically. Finally, Oei et al. (1999) demonstrated that CT
reduced panic symptoms to levels near or below those found in the general population.
Social Phobia
Butler & Beck (2000) also reviewed meta-analyses related to CT for social phobia (Feske
& Chambless, 1995; Gould, Buckminster, Pollack, Otto, Yap, 1997). Gould et al. (1997) found
that CT was superior to exposure interventions without cognitive restructuring (ES = 0.89) and,
surprisingly, the combination of the two interventions (ES = .80) Consistent with studies on
numerous other Axis I disorders, CT was also superior to wait-list and placebo attention controls
(ES = 0.93) for treating social phobia.
Generalized Anxiety Disorder
CT has also been found to be effective in treating Axis I conditions, such as generalized
anxiety disorder (Butler Fennell, Robson, & Gelder, 1991). In a meta-analysis of studies
comparing CT with controls and pharmacotherapy for treating generalized anxiety disorder,
Gould, Otto, Pollack, and Yap (1997) determined that CT was significantly more effective than
nondirective therapy, no-treatment controls, wait list controls, or placebo controls. However, CT
combined with drug therapy, the latter consisting mainly of Benzodiazepines, were not
statistically different at post-treatment.
45
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
According to (Butler & Beck, 2000) exposure and response prevention is often the
frontline psychological treatment of choice for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; e.g., Riggs
& Foa, 1993). However, Abramowitz (1997) found no significant differences in effect sizes
when comparing the exposure and response prevention with CT for treating OCD. Simailarly,
Van Balkom, van Oppen, Vermeulen, van Dyck, Nauta, & Vorst, (1994) determined, via metaanalysis, that both cognitive and cognitive-behavioral treatments resulted in substantial
reductions in OCD symptoms.
Anxiety Complicated by Personality Disorders
Although PDs are subject to increasing investigation (Reich, 2003; Ruegg & Allen, 1995),
relatively few studies have focused on comorbid anxiety and PD, as compared with comorbid
depression and Axis II disorders. This issue has important ramifications for assessment and
treatment, as well, because of a growing emphasis on the use and cost of mental health services
(Phillips, Shea, Warshaw & Dyck, 2001).
Three naturalistic, prospective, longitudinal studies of panic disorder (Faravelli & Albanesi,
1987; Noyes, Reich, Christiansen, Suelzer, Pfohl & Coryell, 1990; Pollack, Otto, Rosenbaum,
Sachs, O'Neil, Asher et al., 1990) reported that the presence of PDs was associated with poor
treatment outcome for panic disorder symptoms and functioning.
Moreover, another longitudinal study linked panic disorder and comorbid PDs with poor
treatment outcome for panic disorder symptoms and functioning (Pollack, Otto, Rosenbaum,
46
Sachs, O'Neil, Asher, Meltzer-Brody, 1990; Pollack Otto, Rosenbaum, & Sachs 1992). However,
it should be noted that this study was limited by the use of self-report PD instruments and a
briefer follow-up of only 1 year. Although convincing, the empirical data regarding the
contribution of PDs to anxiety disorder outcome is limited, inconsistent, and contradictory.
Other studies have shown that the presence of PDs decreases treatment effectiveness for
specific anxiety disorders. For example, in a multisite, prospective, longitudinal, naturalistic
study assessing PDs over a five-year period, Massion, Dyck, Shea, Phillips, Warshaw, & Keller
(2002) found that the presence of a personality disorder predicted a 39% lower likelihood of
social phobia remission and a 30% lower likelihood of remission GAD, even after controlling for
the effects of major depression. However, contrary to previous naturalistic studies, these
researchers determined that PDs had no effect on remission form panic disorder remission. But
the effects of PDs on anxiety were differential. Specifically, dependent personality disorder
predicted a 41% lower probability of remission from GAD; avoidant personality disorder was
associated with a 34% reduced chance for remission in GAD, and a 14% reduced remission rate
in social phobia. Surprisingly, the presence of borderline personality disorder failed to predict
negative outcome.
In other studies of generalized anxiety disorder, Yonkers, Warshaw, Massion, & Keller
(1996) reported an interaction effect between personality and clinical disorders. These
investigators demonstrated the fact that the presence of cluster B or C PDs were differentially
negative treatment indicators. Avoidant personality disorder predicted a poor outcome for both
generalized anxiety disorder and social phobia. It is interesting to note that the presence of a PD
diagnosis has deleterious treatment effects even in pharmacotherapy (Versiani, Nardi, Mindim,
Pinto, Saboya, & Kovacs, 1996; Versiani & Amrein, 1997). Accurately assessing Axis II
47
pathology is crucial because another study found that fully 37% of individuals meeting criteria
for social phobia also met criteria for two PDs, namely, avoidant and OCPD (Turner, Beidel,
Borden, Stanley, Jacob, 1991).
In summary, it appears that when Axis I disorders are complicated by PDs, outcome is often
less favorable. The literature has long reflected that the presence of PDs may correlate with
clinical syndromes to a degree that is far more than anticipated and that such comorbidity may
impede treatment success (see Reich, 2003 for review). This is consistent with the notion that
there may be an additive effect when comorbidity is present whether or not the disorders appear
on either of the two DSM-IV-TR diagnostic axes (Millon et al, 1997). It should be noted that
most patients with PDs do benefit from treatment, but not always as much as those without Axis
II pathology (Beck et al., 2004). Additionally, cognitive distortions have demonstrated a
correlation with a number of Axis I and Axis II conditions and CT has been shown to be
effective in ameliorating these distorted thinking patterns and subsequent dysphoria and
impairment (Beck et al., 2004). These facts also illustrate the need for adequate assessment prior
to treatment planning. In research and in practice, CT enjoys considerable empirical support for
treating many frequently occurring disorders across Axis I and Axis II, in a broad array of
populations and formats (Beck, 1993).
48
Demonstration of Treatment Outcome
Outcome and Measures of Cognitive Distortion
According to Pickstone (2001), the contemporary revolution in healthcare, in general, has
been stunning. Clinicians have come under increasing pressure to demonstrate treatment
effectiveness in the modern cost-accountable and resource-limited world. Forces militating for
efficiency include managed care, rising costs, demands of distraught patients, and therapists’
own desires to alleviate distress. Clinicians are under increasing pressure to demonstrate the fact
that their methods are not only effective, but also efficient. Pressure arises, in large part, from
managed care insurers who may limit the number of sessions for which they reimburse clinicians
for specific disorders. This has spurred the need for clinicians to provide more effective therapy
in less time (McDaniel, 1995). This is especially true of CT and CBT practitioners, who
endeavor to provide effective, brief therapy (Beck, 1996). As a consequence, although
motivation may vary, insurers, patients and clinicians ultimately share the same goal: salubrious
outcome provided in an optimally expeditious manner (Budman, & Gurman, 1988).
Demonstrating treatment effectiveness requires objective, psychometrically sound
measures. According to Kazdin (1998), self-report instruments provide cost- and time-efficient
tools to establish baseline data, assess current levels of functioning, supply a basis to modify
treatment strategies, and substantiate treatment outcome. Although the foundation of CT and
CBT is the exploration and modification of cognition (e.g., Beck, 1979; Burns, 1999), relatively
few instruments have been validated to detail adequately one of the most fundamental cognitive
process involved in psychological dysfunction, specifically, cognitive distortions (Yurica, 2002).
49
At this time, in fact, the literature reflects only five instruments with acceptable
psychometric properties that profess to measure some aspect of cognitive distortions. In
chronological order of publication, these measures include: The Cognitive Errors Questionnaire
(CEQ; Lefebvre, 1981), The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS-A, DAS-B; Weisman, 1979),
The Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Hollon & Kendall, 1980), The Cognitive
Distortions Scale (CDS; Briere, 2001), and The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions (ICD; Yurica
& DiTomasso, 2002. In research, measures of cognitive distortion have been used to document
levels of distress and psychopathology (e.g., Yurica, 2002), response to psychotropic medication
(Meyer, Kenedy, Korman, Brown, DaSilva, et al., 2003), response to CT and CBT (DeRubeis,
Evans, Hollon, Garvey, Grove & Tuason, 1990), and even as predictors by organizational and
consumer psychologists (Netmeyer, Williamson, Burton, & Biswas, 2002). A review of the
professional literature at this time indicates that the DAS and, to a lesser extent the CEQ, have
become used increasingly for the measure of cognitive distortion in research. However, no data
exists regarding their application in clinical practice. The question arises: How can one best
measure cognitive distortion?
Measuring Cognitive Distortions
Cognitive distortions are generally assessed via self-report inventories. A number of such
instruments have been produced purporting to measure features of cognition and behavior
relating to specific Axis I disorders. Examples of such instruments include the Beck Depression
Inventory—II (BDI—II; Beck, Steer, & Brown 1996) the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck,
& Steer, 1990), the Panic Attack Cognition Questionnaire (PAC; Clum, Boyles, Bordin, &
50
Watkins, 1990) the Agoraphobia Cognitions Questionnaire (ACQ; Chambless, Caputo, Bright, &
Gallagher, 1984), Social Anxiety Thoughts Questionnaire (SAT; Hartman, 1984), and the Fear of
Negative Evaluation Scale (Watson & Friend, 1983). Although these instruments assess distorted
cognitions, they are limited because they identify only cognitions related to specific disorders or
syndromes.
On the other hand, additional instruments have been developed to assess the quantity and
content of positive and negative cognitions, such as the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire
(ATQ; Hollon & Kendall, 1980) the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire—Revised (ATQ-R;
Kendall, Howard, & Hayes, 1989), and the Cognitive Triad Inventory (CTI; Beckham, Leber,
Watkins, Boyer, & Cook, 1986), Another instrument, the Young Schema Questionnaire-Long
Form, 2 nd ed. (YSQ-L2; Young & Brown, 1990), assesses cognition for purposes of identifying
underlying schema. Finally, several inventories have been developed to measure irrational
beliefs and attitudes such as the Rational Behavior Inventory (RBI; Shorkey & Whiteman, 1987),
the Irrational Values Scale (IVS; MacDonald, 1972) the Irrational Beliefs Scale (IBS; Malouff &
Schute, 1986) and the General Attitude and Belief Scale (GABS; DiGiuseppe, Leaf, Exner, &
Robin). Although these instruments assess constructs related to cognitive distortion, they cannot
claim to quantify specific distortions or their severity.
A literature review revealed five instruments specifically designed to assess the construct of
cognitive distortions in a clinical context. These instruments are as follows: The Dysfunctional
Attitude Scale (DAS; Weissman & 1979), the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ, Hollon
& Kendall, 1980), the Cognitive Error Questionnaire (CEQ; Lefebvre, 1981), the Cognitive
Distortion Scale (CDS; Briere, 2000), and the Inventory of Cognitive Distortions (Yurica &
DiTomasso, 2002). Of these measures, only the ICD assesses the frequency of 11 factor-
51
analyzed cognitive distortions culminating in a Total Cognitive Distortion score—the dependent
variable of interest in the present study.
However, most of these instruments suffer from a number of critical limitations.
Limitations of Existing Measures of Cognitive Distortion
Many of the existing measures of cognitive distortion are lacking in a number of ways that
are relevant to the present research. Specifically, the ATQ (Hollon & Kendall, 1980), DAS
(Weissman & 1979), and CEQ (Lefebvre, 1981) were designed for assessing cognitive
distortions attendant to depression. Furthermore, the DAS yields only a total distortion score.
This is problematic in research because the DAS fails to illuminate a clear picture about which
specific distortions correlate with particular disorders. Moreover, in assessment and clinical
practice, the DAS is designed to assess dysfunctional beliefs and fails to identify many important
distortions that may be targeted in treatment.
Second, of the existing measures of cognitive distortion, only the ICD measures 11 different
cognitive distortions on separate subscales, thereby offering a significant advantage over both the
DAS and CEQ, because the DAS is limited to only six types of distortions (arbitrary inference,
overgeneralization, selective abstraction, magnification or minimization, dichotomous reasoning,
and personalization), whereas the CEQ is further limited to only four varieties of distortion
(overgeneralization, arbitrary inference, selective abstraction, and magnification or
minimization).
Third, although the CDS (Brierre, 2000) is more useful for identifying specific cognitive
distortions, it is still limited because it yields only the following five distortions: helplessness,
52
hopelessness, self-criticism, self-blame, and preoccupation with danger. Consequently, the
instrument fails to identify a number of important theoretical cognitive distortions, such as those
identified by Yurica (2002).
Third, as Yurica (2002) explicated, all four instruments lack specificity in their definitions of
the term cognitive distortions, and suffer from “… poor consensus in definition, variable
measurement across instruments, limited applicability, and outdated, limited scope of the
measurement of cognitive distortions….” (p. 57). However, of all of these measures, only the
ICD assesses the frequency of Yurica’s (2002) 11 factor-analyzed cognitive distortions and
yields a total score of cognitive distortions—the dependent variable of interest in the present
study.
The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions
The ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002) provides the latest and most comprehensive,
structured, psychometrically validated self-report instrument for measuring cognitive distortions
in a heterogeneous, adult, clinical, outpatient population. The ICD is a 69-item self-report
inventory designed specifically to measure the frequency of distorted cognitions in an outpatient
clinical population. The instrument consists of single sentence items, answered on a five-point
Likert scale. The ICD provides scores on 11 factors/cognitive distortions. According to Yurica
(2002), the ICD was specifically designed to assess “self-statement cognitive products
representative of particular types of cognitive distortions in differing mental health disorders” (p.
103). CT experts and factor analysis have established good content validity. Three cognitive
therapy experts came to a 100% independent agreement on the original 69 items.
53
Yurica (2002) explicitly designed the ICD to validate the 16 theorized cognitive distortions
(Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980, 1999; Freeman & DeWolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).
However, of the original theorized cognitive distortions, factor analysis revealed 11 fundamental
factors that closely resembled 10 theory-derived cognitive distortion subscales (Externalization
of Self-Worth, Fortune-Telling, Magnification, Labeling, Perfectionism, Comparison to Others,
Emotional Reasoning, Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusion, Minimization, MindReading), in addition to one important new cognitive distortion (Emotional Reasoning and
Decision-Making). It should be noted that the ICD did not retain the theoretical construct of
overgeneralization because this was the only distortion that failed to receive a 100% agreement
among independent cognitive therapy experts.
Despite convincing theoretical and empirical evidence for the fundamental role of cognitive
distortions in both the cognitive model and cognitive-behavioral therapies, there has been
surprisingly little effort to objectively quantify the frequency with which these cognitive events
occur. In fact, to date, only two instruments, the ICD and the CDS, have been empirically
validated via factor-analysis of the theoretical cognitive distortions to test the frequency of
cognitive distortions with a clinical sample of heterogeneous, adult, mental health outpatients.
ICD also offers several advantages over other measures of cognitive distortion. First, it provides
the latest factor analysis of the original theorized cognitive distortions. Second, it identifies and
evaluates a larger number of cognitive distortions than any of the other competing instruments.
Third, the distortions identified by the ICD have been demonstrated to span diagnostic
categories, rather than being restricted to a particular diagnosis (Yurica, 2002).
Yurica (2002) determined that the ICD significantly and positively correlated with widely
accepted measures of depression, the (BDI-II) anxiety (BAI), and dysfunctional attitudes (DAS).
54
Comparing scores of outpatient psychiatric patients (n=122) with a comparison control group
(n=66), the ICD demonstrated impressive internal consistency, having a total scale estimate of
internal reliability measuring a Coefficient alpha of .998 (N=28), indicating good homogeneity
of item content. Additionally, the ICD total scale scores produced test-retest reliability of r = .98,
indicating good stability. Patterns of cognitive distortion appeared to be stable and enduring,
even after a cognitively and emotionally traumatic event of historic proportion, the attacks of
September 11, 2001. Such an enduring pattern of cognition is more consistent with trait- versus
state-like thinking. However, it is also possible that such an unprecedented mass trauma may
have artificially affected ICD reliability.
Nonetheless, such reliability is consistent with Alford and Beck’s (1997) revised cognitive
theory, in which cognitive distortion was postulated to include both errors in content/meaning,
on the one hand, and cognitive processing/meaning elaboration, on the other hand. It was
proposed that predisposition to specific cognitive distortions or cognitive vulnerabilities leaves
individuals susceptible to specific disorders. This proposal is also supported by research of the
interaction of parenting and children’s coping styles, as well as developmental theories relating
to cognitive distortions (Yurica, 2002).
In summary, Yurica, (2002) found a robust and significantly positive correlation between
cognitive distortions on the one hand, and anxiety, depressive symptoms and dysfunctional
attitudes, on the other hand. A review of the literature revealed no similar research examining the
relationship between the frequency of the 11 factor analyzed cognitive distortions and Axis II
conditions. Thus, this study endeavors to expand further the utility of this promising instrument
into additional specific clinical syndromes, and beyond that, into the personality disorder
domain.
55
Outcome and Measures of Psychopathology
The Minnesota Multipahasic Personality Inventory-2
A number of widely accepted, standardized self-report measures exist for
psychopathology outcome measurement. These instruments are also employed for the diagnostic
classification of research participants and clinical patients. Such measures are valuable because
they provide an indication both of the number and severity of clinical disorders, mainly along
accepted nosology. Since Hathaway & McKinley (1943) originally developed The Minnesota
Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), it has become the most widely used and researched
clinical assessment inventory, with over 10,000 published research references (Groth-Marnat,
1999). Despite its enormous value, however, its most recent incarnation, the MMPI-2 (Butcher,
Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989), has its limitations. Most significantly,
according to Groth-Marnat (1999), the MMPI-2 may be prohibitively time-consuming; it is, in
fact, with 567 items, the lengthiest of such measures. Additional MMPI-2 limitations include
high item overlap, resulting in high intercorrelations among various scales, the use of obsolete
diagnostic labels (such as, Psychasthenia), ineffectiveness in diagnosing either Axis I or Axis II
conditions, including the criteria of the current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association
[APA]; 2000).
56
The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III
Another widely accepted self-report measure is the Millon Clinical Multiaxial InventoryIII (MCMI-III; Millon, 1994). The MCMI-III purports to “help the clinician assess DSM-IVrelated personality disorders and clinical syndromes” and provide information relating to
emotional adjustment, and attitude toward taking the test (Millon, 1994, p. 17). A review of the
literature reveals that various versions of the MCMI have been used as outcome measures in
hundreds of studies, for such disorders as post-traumatic stress disorder (Allen, Coyne, &
Huntoon, 1998), PDs (Alnaes & Torgensen, 1989; Bayon, Hill, Svrakic, Przybeck, & Cloninger,
1996; Joiner & Rudd, 2002; de Groot, Franken, van der Meer, & Hendriks, 2003), personality
cluster traits (Bender, Farber, & Geller, 2001; Espelage, Mazzeo, Sherman, & Thompson, 2002),
narcissistic personality disorder (Auerbach, 1984), forensic patients (Blackburn, 1998; Rogers,
2003)., substance abusers (Craig, 1997a, 1997b; Craig, & Olson 1998), depression (Davis &
Hayes, 1997; Libb, Stankovic, Sokol, Freeman, Houck, & Switzer, 1990), schizophrenia (del
Rosario, McCann, & Navarra, 1994), bipolar disorder (Turley, Bates, Edwards, & Jackson,
1992), anxiety disorders (Freeman, Kablinger, Rolland, & Brannon, 1999), aggressiveness (Holt,
Meloy, & Strack, 1999), and for all manner of psychiatric outpatients (Piersma & Boes, 1997).
The MCMI-III is also used extensively in clinical practice (Groth-Marnat, 1999).
The MCMI—III was normed on a heterogeneous sample of 998 males and females
representing a wide variety of diagnoses and demographic variables. The sample included
patients seen in independent practices, in mental health centers, in clinics, in residential settings,
57
and in hospitals. Because norms were based exclusively on a clinical sample, the instrument is
not appropriate for use with nonclinical populations due to a high frequency of diagnostic falsepositives (Millon & Davis, 1996).
The MCMI-III has become a frequently used measure of clinical syndromes and PDs and
has good empirical validation (e.g., Choca & VanDenburg, 1997; Craig, 1993, 1997a; Hsu,
2002). The MCMI—III PD scales include all of the PDs contained in the DSM—IV plus four
additional, nonofficial PDs (depressive, passive—aggressive, self-defeating, and sadistic).
Although these additional diagnoses are no longer in the official nosology, Millon et al (1997)
maintain that these disorders, nonetheless “exist in nature,” despite having been deleted from the
DSM. The MCMI—III is widely used by clinicians and researchers, and there is an extensive
literature reporting favorable psychometric properties (Craig & Weinberg, 1993; Millon &
Davis, 1996; Retzlaff, 1995). More generally, studies comparing a variety of self-report and
interview forms of diagnostic assessment have provided little evidence that one test or method is
superior to others, particularly in evaluating PDs (Widiger & Sanderson, 1995).
In discussing the assessment and quantification of psychological disorders, Millon (1999.
p. 4) philosophically observed the following:
The natural world is complex and intricate, and its phenomena are not easy to classify.
Our concepts and categories are only tools to guide observation, interpret its results, and
solve problems. Alternative concepts may help us to understand a single subject of
inquiry… In psychology and other soft sciences, we sometimes have to impose arbitrary
systems that suggest more clarity and coherence than the imprecise nature of the subject
may warrant…. Debates on these issues may sometimes degenerate into semantic
arguments and theoretical hair-splitting, but conceptual distinctions and the assumptions
58
they reflect are a necessary part of scientific decision-making. However, clinicians and
researchers are ultimately concerned with the welfare of real people, holistic collections
of inextricably interconnected configurations of cognition, behavior, and affect.
Millon (1999) acknowledges this reality by advocating a “psychosynergistic” assessment
that integrates Axis I clinical syndromes with Axis II PDs into a single dynamic psychological
profile, which recognizes that, “Each person becomes a synthesized and substantive whole that is
greater than the sum of its multiaxial parts.” (p. 4). Thus, impairment and distress may be
manifested as an Axis I syndrome, an Axis II disorder, or in problems across both axes. To this
end, the MCMI—III, does not proceed through a serial interpretation of individual scales; rather,
it transforms their meanings into a holistic profile—a profile that Millon (1999) posited can be
legitimately explained by behavioral, psychodynamic or cognitive perspectives.
According to Millon et al. (1997, p. 124) “the greater the number of scales elevated above
BR 75, the greater the personality pathology” and the higher the BR scale score elevation above
BR 75, the more severe the disturbance. Although all MCMI-III BR scores above 75 are
interpretable, a score of BR 75 is expected to indicate the presence of a clinically significant
personality “style” or “syndrome”; BR 85, however, is indicative of symptoms sufficient to
warrant a diagnosis of PD (p. 120). Thus, for purposes of assessing Axis II pathology, the sum
of the positive distance from the BR 75 cutoff score for any of the specific MCMI-III Clinical
Personality Pattern and/or Severe Personality Pathology scales should capture the total pathology
on Axis II.
Millon specifically intended the original Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (Millon &
Davis, 1977) to assist in diagnosing PDs as well as clinical syndromes (Groth-Marnat, 1999;
Millon & Davis, 1977). The instrument has undergone a number of revisions in order to
59
accommodate relevant empirical findings. The most recent version, the MCMI—III (Millon,
1994) is a self-report measure consisting of 175 true or false statements, rendering scores on 28
scales in the following categories: three Modifying Indices, one Validity Scale, three Severe
Personality Pathology scales, 11 PD scales, seven clinical syndrome scales, and three additional
Severe Clinical Scales.
Clinical Syndrome Scale.
Separate MCMI—III categories distinguish Axis I from more enduring Axis II disorders.
Within clinical axes, scales are further categorized according to severity. Thus, three Severe
Clinical Syndromes (Thought Disorder, Delusional Disorder, and Major Depression) are
assessed independently of more moderate or “neurotic” Clinical Syndromes Scale scores,
composed of the following seven subscales: Anxiety, Somatoform, Bipolar Manic, Dysthymia,
Alcohol Dependence, Drug Dependence, and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (Millon & Davis,
1996, p. 3). The instrument also provides data regarding psychosocial stressors and their
therapeutic implications. It should be noted that the presence of certain Axis I conditions, such as
particular dysthymic and anxiety states can elevate or depress certain PD scales (Shea et al,
1987). However, the MCMI automatically compensates for the influence of this state (Axis I)—
trait (Axis II) interaction (Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis, 1996).
60
Personality Disorder Scales.
The MCMI-III consists of two personality disorder scales, both consisting of a total of 14
theorized PDs. The first scale, Clinical Personality Pattern Scales, contains the following 11
personality subscales: Schizoid, Avoidant, Depressive, Dependent, Histrionic, Narcissistic,
Antisocial, Aggressive/Sadistic, Compulsive, Passive-Aggressive (negativistic and SelfDefeating (Masochistic). The second personality scale, Severe Personality Scales consists of the
following three subscales: Schizotypal, Borderline, and Paranoid (Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis,
1996).
Axis II PDs are scored in a manner similar to Axis I, with BR scale scores of 75 to 84 on
Axis II suggestive of personality traits; however, scores of 85 or greater provide strong empirical
support for the presence of personality disorder(s), and according to Millon, resultant Axis I
clinical pathology as the personality disorder deficits exacerbate distress and interpersonal
difficulties (Millon & Davis, 1996) .
In a recent study conducted with a combined sample of clinical and nonclinical populations,
O’Connor & Dyce (2001) demonstrated empirically that the MCMI—III (Millon, 1994)
differentiated individuals with PDs from those failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. Normal
and disordered personalities were found to coexist in a variety of regions of the multivariate
space, based on the 5-factor model (FFM) of personality (Widiger, 2000). Examination of the
interpersonal dimensions chart (Figure 1) reveals that within regions, the profiles of normal and
disordered personalities were very similar in characteristic configuration or vector but notably
different in vector length. Moreover, this difference was detected by the MCMI-III. Those with
PDs appear to be “out there” in inner experience, interpersonal behavior, and in this case,
61
geometric configuration. Significantly, although length of vector predicted the presence of Axis
II pathology, it failed to predict any specific personality disorder. Consequently, it is predicted
that the severity of cognitive distortion will predict the presence and severity of Axis II
conditions. Although the severity of cognitive distortion may be able to predict the extremity of
the vector (severity) and presence of PD(s) or clinical disorder(s), distorted thinking is not,
however, considered capable of predicting the direction of the vector, that is, specific personality
disorder.
62
Figure 1. Illustration of interpersonal dimensions.
Note. O’Connor & Dyce (2001) empirically demonstrated that the MCMI—III and, to a lesser extent, the MMPI—2
differentiated individuals with PDs from those failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. Although normal and
disordered personalities were found to coexist in a variety of regions of the multivariate space based on the 5-factor
model (FFM) of personality (Widiger, 2000), those with PDs appear to be “out there” in inner experience,
interpersonal behavior, and in this case, geometric configuration. Source: O'Connor, B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (2001).
Rigid and extreme: A geometric representation of PDs in five-factor model space. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 81, 1119-1130.
63
Personality-disordered individuals tended to be located in the perimeters or outer regions of
the FFM space, as indicated by their longer vector lengths. This indicates that the presence and
severity of a personality disorder was manifested in a difference in the quantity, rather than the
quality of four of the five FFM dimensions: Neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and
conscientiousness were differentially more extreme in individuals with PDs. Specifically, those
reaching the threshold for significance on MCMI-III, with BR scores equal to or greater than BR
75 or BR 85, exhibited significantly more extremity and rigidity on these dimensions.
In summary and in support of the Cognitive Model, evidence exists suggesting that as the
severity or the number of Axis I or Axis II conditions increases, there will be a correspondent
increase in pathology. In other words, there should be a positive correlation between measures of
pathology and measures of cognitive distortion.
MCMI Scoring Methods
Scale scores have been established by actuarial base-rate-transformed and base rate-adjusted
PD scale scores reflecting the prevalence of the syndromes or characteristics in the general
population, as described in the MCMI—III manual (Millon & Davis, 1996). Empirical evidence
suggests that BR scores offer more diagnostic accuracy than T scores (Duthie & Vincent, 1986).
Derived from a standardization sample of 1079 clinical mental health patients, Millon & Davis
(1997) recommended cutoff scores are as follows:
•
35 = median score for “normal”/nonpsychiatric groups.
64
•
60 = median score for psychiatric groups.
•
75 = presence of features.
•
85 = definite characteristics.
The scales were explicitly designed to measure the official DSM—IV diagnostic criteria and
Millon’s evolutionary theory of PDs (Millon, 1990, Millon et al., 1996). In addition, with a
completion time of approximately 25 minutes, the MCMI—III is shorter and thus more practical
and appealing than comparable instruments for patients and clinicians (Millon & Davis, 1996).
The three-stage validation process is an additional strength of the MCMI.
Evidence for the validity of the scales has been provided in the form of correlations with
ratings by clinicians, correlations with “collateral tests measuring of identical constructs”, and
strong diagnostic efficiency statistics (Millon, 1994, p. 30). The instrument has been validated
with a variety of clinical samples including inpatients, outpatients and drug and alcohol center
patients (Hsu, 2002; Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis, 1996). The MCMI—III is widely used both
by clinicians and researchers, resulting in an extensive literature reporting favorable
psychometric properties (e.g., Craig & Weinberg, 1993; Retzlaff, 1995).
The MCMI-III has demonstrated high test-retest validity with a median for all scales of .91
(Millon, 1994). Test-retest reliability reflected high stability, ranging from a low of .82 for
debasement to a high of .96 for Bipolar: Manic with a median stability coefficient of .91. It
should be noted, though, that the delay between test and retest was only 5 to 14 days. These
results indicate high stability, at least over short periods of time. However, Overholser, (1989)
found .69 and .67 test-retest validities for the personality scales and clinical scales, respectively,
with retests occurring at a mean interval of 379 days.
65
Strong internal consistency was also confirmed with alpha coefficients surpassing .80 for 20
scales, ranging from a high of .90 for Depression and a low of .66 for the Compulsion scale
(Millon, 1994). Correlations between the MCMI-I and MCMI-III scales are moderately high
(Groth-Marnat, 1999). Later versions of the MCMI have also demonstrated little difference
between stability on personality scales versus clinical scales (Groth-Marnat, 1999). In fact, the
MCMI-III manual (Millon et al., 1997) actually indicates a slightly lower mean of .89 for the
personality scales versus .91 for the clinical scales.
Concurrent validity.
The MCMI-III also demonstrated good concurrent validity, with the Major Depression and
Dysthymia scales correlating with the Beck Depression Inventory, .74 and .71, respectively
(Millon & Davis, 1996). However, although the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI;
Spielberger, 1983) correlated more highly with the Major Depression scale (.59), the Anxiety
scale still correlated moderately with state (.55) and trait (.58) anxiety. Consequently, caution
might be appropriate regarding discriminant validity; for example, the BDI also correlated .63
with the PTSD and .62 with the Thought Disorder scales (Millon et al, 1997) .
Predictive validity.
The aggressive personality disorder scales and several of the neurotic scales correlated with
future institutional violence in prison populations (Retzlaff, Stoner, & Kleinsasser, 2002).
Discriminant validity.
The MCMI—III has demonstrated both convergent and discriminant validity as a measure
of sleep disturbance (Allen, Console, Brethour, 2000).
66
Limitations of the MCMI—III
Because the MCMI-III data is based exclusively on individuals who were undergoing
psychotherapy, psychological assessment, or on those who were evincing emotional or
interpersonal problems, the instrument is not appropriate for use in nonclinical populations.
Moreover, the instrument provides valuable data regarding personality and psychopathology;
however, scores are best regarded as a cross-sectional snapshot of the individual. Consequently,
it might be ideal to augment the instrument with extensive corroborating information from
collateral sources and biographical interviews of the type that are prohibitive to this sort of
experimental procedure (Kazdin, 1998; Millon & Davis, 1996). A lack of discriminant validity
has been noted between clinical and nonclinical populations (Boyle & Loick, 2000). Millon &
Davis, (1996) recommend a multimethod approach in which MCMI results are evaluated in the
light of independent clinical evidence such as other psychological tests, observed behavior, case
history, and data gathered through clinical interviews.
Claims of “on the mark” validity for the MCMI-III range from 55 to 65% with “off-target”
errors in 10 to 15% of cases indicating a quantitative range of five to six times greater than
chance (Millon & Davis, 1996, p. 7). The instrument is most accurate for individuals in the
moderate range of disturbance. This means that more acutely disturbed or psychotic individuals
may be underdiagnosed, whereas those experiencing milder adjustment disorders or transient
stressors are more liable to be overdiagnosed (Millon & Davis, 1996).
The MCMI shares limitations of many other self-report measures, including vague items and
language, cultural assumptions, and state-bias (Hsu, 2002).
67
Although, studies comparing various self-report and interview forms of PD assessment have
provided little evidence that one test or method is superior to the other (Widiger & Sanderson,
1995), the relative brevity, diagnostic breadth, and psychometric validity make the MCMI-III an
appealing instrument.
Summary
Overwhelming empirical evidence supports the cognitive model of psychological disorders,
which posits the idea that cognitive distortions may precipitate, attend, maintain, and exacerbate
a wide range of clinical problems. To measure these problems, the MCMI—III provides a
practical, psychometrically sound instrument for assessing psychological disorders on both Axis
I and Axis II. Similarly, the ICD appears to be the most comprehensive, psychometrically
validated measure of cognitive distortion to date.
Cognitive Distortions in
Personality Disorders and Clinical Syndromes
Personality Disorders have long represented one of the most perplexing and frustrating
facets of psychopathology confronting clinicians and researchers (Nigg & Goldsmith, 1994). The
lives of the personality disordered are often unusually stressful for themselves and their families.
The impact of PDs can range from annoyance to increased sucidality for both patients and for
those with whom they interact. In fact, high stress combined with concurrent PD diagnosis has
68
been correlated with elevated risk of suicidal behavior (Zimmerman, Pfohl, Stangl, & Coryell,
1985).
According to Beck, Butler, Brown, Dahlsgaard, Newman and Beck (2001, p. 1214), “…the
essence of a personality disorder is revealed in the dysfunctional beliefs that characterize and
perpetuate it.” Cognitive distortions (Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980; 1999; Freeman & Oster
1999) contributing to those dysfunctional beliefs may foster and maintain an enduring, rigid, and
pervasive pattern of inner experience and behavior that deviates markedly from what is expected
by one’s culture (Beck & Freeman, 1990). When this occurs, one may experience subjective
distress, interpersonal problems, and/or deficits in impulse control. Once these symptoms are of
sufficient quantity, chronicity, and/or severity, the diagnosis of a PD may be appropriate. Thus,
cognitive distortions can cause severe and chronic problems for PD patients (Beck, & Freeman,
1990; Beck et al., 2004; Freeman & Oster 1999).
The following illustration, typical of many patients diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive
PD (OCPD), illustrates virtually all of Yurica and DiTomasso (2001) empirically validated
cognitive distortions (distortions italicized): Perfectionism, characteristic of many OCPD patients
(Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004), can lead to avoidance of personal and occupational
tasks for fear that they will be unable to live up to unobtainable idealistic standards. When
complicated by dichotomous/black and white thinking, this can lead OCPD patients to expect
inevitable and abject failure in everything perceived to be significant. Even if they attempt an
activity, magnification of minor errors and minimization of success maintains a negative
confirmatory bias, making the individual liable to make arbitrary inferences/jump to conclusions
that they have failed before they have ever given themselves a chance to succeed. In this way,
externalization of self-worth occurs as the individual’s self-esteem becomes conditional on the
69
quality and quantity of his or her work product. Because the patient inevitably fails to meet these
impossible standards, he or she may engage in chronic upward social comparison to others. The
OCPD patient, then, may come to label him- or herself as defective, helpless, or unlovable,
leading to a further magnification of the perceived problem. Thus, it is easy to understand how
this interlocking knot of cognitive distortions may lead to shame, frustration, anger, and a host of
other dysphoric emotions as well as maladaptive behavior, which may maintain dysfunctional
schema at the core of the process (Young, 1999). These processes are theorized to be similar
across the official DSM-IV-TR nosology (APA, 2000).
There is evidence that cognitive distortions, as measured by the Dysfunctional Attitude
Scale (Weissman & Beck, 1978), remit in most patients when their depression abates, rendering
their cognition indistinguishable from nondepressed controls (Blackburn, Eunson, & Bishop,
1986). These results support Beck’s (1976) notion that depression results from dormant
maladaptive schema(s), which are often activated by negative life events.
However, it has also been demonstrated empirically that the cognitive distortions of patients
diagnosed with PDs remain intact even after depression remits (Ilardi & Crgaihead, 1999).
Moreover, individuals with comorbid PDs and clinical depression exhibited higher levels of
dysfunctional attitudes, operationalized as DAS scores, than do depressed patients without PDs
(Evans & Graighead, 1995). These results lend additional empirical support to the conclusion
that individuals manifesting PDs engage in more distorted thinking than controls or Axis I
patients without PDs. Moreover, the cognitive distortions of Axis II patients are trait-like,
because they are stable and fail to remit when Axis I conditions abate. Such dysfunctional
cognitive style may carry the prediction of leaving them more vulnerable to future episodes of
Axis I disorders (Ilardi & Crgaihead, 1999; Millon, 1994).
70
DSM-III (APA, 1980) originally delineated PDs along a separate axis to encourage the
consideration of enduring characterlogical problems in the context of comorbid and more
immediate mood, anxiety, and psychotic disorders (Frances, 1980; Spitzer, Williams, & Skodol,
1980). Although distinguishing PDs from other clinical syndromes diagnostically can be useful
and valid, the demarcation often remains problematic and illusory whether or not diagnoses are
placed on separate axes (Widiger & Shea, 1991).
Cognitive patterns exhibited in PDs (Axis II) differ from so-called clinical disorders on
Axis I in a number of interesting ways. First, in Axis I conditions, such as depressive, anxiety,
and psychotic disorders, the frequency and intensity of dysfunctional automatic thoughts are
expected to subside as the Axis I syndrome abates. Axis I conditions are expected to be more
episodic, meaning that patients typically return to premorbid levels of cognitive function as
symptoms remit. Conversely, dysfunctional beliefs and distorted thinking patterns exhibited in
PDs are more systematically “built into the ‘normal’ cognitive organization” and therefore more
persistent and less amenable to change (Beck & Freeman, 1990, p. 58). Thus, if PDs originate in
adolescence or early adulthood and are, by definition, pervasive and inflexible, then one might
expect the dysfunctional beliefs and associated cognitive distortions to be more chronic in PD
patients. An additional clinical problem arises from the fact that Axis II patients are more prone
to “resistance” in the form of treatment nonadherrence or “noncollaboration” (Beck & Freeman,
1990, p. 67).
This noncollaboration may stem from lack of motivation, secondary ga in, rigidity of
beliefs and behavior, vague or unrealistic treatment goals, and/or fears of the effects of change.
All of these obstacles to change can be maintained, reinforced, and exacerbated by cognitive
distortions (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 2004). Accordingly, correctly identifying and
71
treating patterns of cognitive distortions may illuminate underlying maladaptive beliefs, facilitate
treatment effectiveness, improve patient level of functioning, and ameliorate distress for
individuals with recalcitrant Axis II conditions.
Distorted cognitive style also has been associated with aggressive behavior, such as
conduct disorder, reactive aggression, and commission of violent crimes in a sample of highly
aggressive juvenile offenders (Dodge & Newman, 1981; Dodge, Price, Bachorowski, &
Newman, 1990). To compound matters, early aggressive behavior, associated with cognitive
distortions, has been found to be a developmental precursor of later drug use (Brook, Whiteman,
Finch, & Cohen, 1996; Lynskey & Fergusson, 1995).
There is also empirical support for the association between cognitive distortions and a
number of other troubling social and clinical conditions, including sexual assault (Baumeister,
Catanese, & Wallace, 2002), pathological gambling (Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan,
2002), adolescent anxiety and depressive disorders (Kendall, Kortlander, & Brady, 1992),
violence and anger in marital relationships (Ekhardt, Barbour, & Davison, 1998), and adolescent
depression and anxiety (Kolko, Brent, Baugher, Bridge, Birmaher, 2000).
Moreover, there is evidence of a relationship between comorbidity and cognitive
distortions. For example, Najavits, Gotthardt, Weiss, & Epstein (2004) recently demonstrated the
fact that individuals diagnosable with a dual Axis I diagnosis (PTSD and substance use
disorders) had a significantly greater tendency to engage in 10 out of 20 cognitive distortions, as
measured by the Cognitive Distortion Scale, than did individuals who only met criteria for one
Axis I diagnosis (PTSD).
Thus, consistent with the cognitive model, dysfunctional beliefs and their associated cognitive
distortions appear to be a unifying feature of psychological disorders, regardless of axis
72
taxonomy or biopsychosiocial manifestations. Yet it is only recently that a single measure was
developed to quantify the frequency of specific cognitive distortions independent of diagnosis
(Yurica, 2002; Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002).
Cognitive distortions serve a pivotal role in the maintenance of emotional disorders (Beck et
al, 1979; Beck, Wright, & Newman, 1993; Ellis & Grieger, 1977) and PDs (Beck & Freeman,
1990; Beck et al., 2004). Cognitive distortions can become habitual patterns of thinking that are
evoked by certain stimuli and are supported by underlying intermediate beliefs composed of
conditional assumptions, beliefs and rules (Beck, 1996). At the foundation of these dysfunctional
cognitions are maladaptive core beliefs. These core beliefs are related to the most vulnerable
aspects of an individual’s self-concept (e.g., I am unlovable, helpless, worthless, vulnerable, or
incompetent) and maladaptive views of others (e.g., People are rejecting, demeaning, or hostile)
(Beck, 1996). Cognitive distortions may also prevent reality testing of thoughts, thus maintaining
the entire dysfunctional system of cognition, emotion, and behavior. When core beliefs are
activated, as in times of psychological stress, cognitive distortions may become increasingly
prominent and severe (Beck & Weishaar, 1989).
According to the cognitive model of psychopathology, CT “…techniques are designed to
identify, reality-test and correct underlying distorted conceptualizations and the dysfunctional
core beliefs (schemas) underlying those cognitions.” (Beck et al., 1979, p. 43). Thus, CT seeks to
reduce cognitive distortions by promoting more accurate thinking.
Consequently, if cognitive distortion occupies such a central role in the cognitive model and
patients present with a myriad of psychological disorders and permutations of comobidites, then
it would behoove therapists to use the most accurate means to assess the presence and severity
73
these cognitive distortions. This illustrates the need for a brief, portable, psychometrically sound
instrument, such as the ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002).
For the assessment of psychological disorders, the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory
(MCMI—III; Millon, 1994) provides a comprehensive self-report questionnaire to assess
personality and emotional adjustment according to DSM-IV criteria and Millon’s evolutionary
theory of personality. In addition, the MCMI—III also provides a means to assess the validity of
the test-taker’s responses. The instrument has also demonstrated good reliability and validity
(Groth-Marnat, 1999).
Rationale for the Study
The influence that cognition exerts on emotion and behavior has been postulated since the
beginning of classical civilization. Similarly, CT is based on the principle that human emotion
and behavior are heavily influenced by cognition (e.g., Beck, 1976). Thus, according to cognitive
theory, clinical disorders (e.g., Beck, 1976) and personality pathology (Beck & Freeman, 1990;
Beck et al, 2004) should correlate with distorted cognition.
When Beck originally conceptualized depression as a “thinking disorder” in 1964 (p.13); he
defined it as a disorder in which individuals viewed the world in a subjective manner largely
determined by idiosyncratic cognitions. Consistent with Bartlett (1932) and Piaget’s vocabulary
(1947/1950) and Kelly’s (1955) cognitive constructs, Beck further postulated that the activation
of particular schemas were fundamental to cognitive, affective, and behavioral symptoms found
in various psychological disorders (Beck, 1964, 1976; 1996; Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1985;
Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck, Wright, Newman, & Liesse, 1985).
74
Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002 provide the latest and most comprehensive psychometric selfreport instrument for measuring cognitive distortions in an adult clinical po pulation. The ICD is
a 69-item self-report inventory designed to measure distorted cognitions in an outpatient clinical
population and in normal controls. The instrument consists of single sentence items answered on
a five-point Likert scale (Likert, 1932). The ICD provides scores on 11 factors/cognitive
distortions and a total score reflecting the total frequency of cognitive distortion. Possible scores
range from 69 to 345.
Yurica & DiTomasso (2001) designed the ICD explicitly to validate theorized cognitive
distortions (Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980, 1999; Freeman & DeWolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster,
1999). However, of the original theorized cognitive distortions, factor analysis revealed 11
fundamental factors that closely resembled 10 theory-derived cognitive distortion subscales
(Externalization of Self-Worth, Fortune-Telling, Magnification, Labeling, Perfectionism,
Comparison to Others, Emotional Reasoning, Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusion,
Minimization, Mind-Reading), in addition to one new cognitive distortion (Emotional Reasoning
and Decision-Making).
Despite convincing theoretical and empirical evidence for the fundamental role of cognitive
distortions in both the cognitive model and cognitive-behavioral therapies, there has been
surprisingly little effort to quantify objectively the frequency with which these cognitive events
occur. In fact, to date, only one instrument, the ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso, 2002), has been
empirically validated via factor-analysis of the theoretical cognitive distortions with a clinical
sample of heterogeneous outpatient mental health patients to test the frequency of these
particular cognitive distortions (Yurica, 2002). Thus, this study endeavored to further validate
the ICD and to expand the utility of this promising instrument into specific Axis I conditions
75
and, beyond that, into the Axis II domain by correlating cognitive distortions (Total ICD scores)
with psychological disorders (MCMI-III subscale scores).
The Purpose of the Study
This study sought to determine the relationship between the frequency of cognitive
distortions and the number and severity of psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II.
Further, the study endeavored to further determine the validity and reliability of the ICD, a
promising new self-report measure of cognitive distortions, by correlating this instrument with
clinical diagnoses as determined by the MCMI-III, a widely accepted measure of psychological
disorders. Providing further validation for the ICD, a brief, portable, and objective measure of
these key cognitive processes should be beneficial to both research and practice in the following
ways:
1.
Accurately identifying specific patterns of cognitive distortions early during the course of
treatment might assist the clinician to target more efficiently dysfunctional thought patterns
that maintain and exacerbate social and psychological problems. It has been shown that
treatment occurring earlier in the course of psychological disorders correlates with a number
of therapeutic benefits, including positive treatment outcome, fewer office visits to
physicians, and reduced health care costs (Bruns, 1998; Kaplan, 2000). Thus, using the ICD
for early identification of cognitive distortions might allow the therapist and patient to
illuminate collaboratively and more quickly the underlying secondary beliefs and schemas
which maintain those same cognitive distortions.
2.
An instrument that allows for regular assessment of cognitive distortions throughout
treatment, such as the ICD, can provide an objective measure of progress, guiding treatment
76
planning because cognitive distortions have been shown to correlate with a number of Axis I
and Axis II disorders (e.g., Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, &
Erdbaugh, 196; Hollon & Kendall, 1980; Ross, Gottfredson, Christensen, & Weaver, 1986).
3.
The instrument could facilitate psychoeducational processes regarding patients’ particular
patterns of cognitive distortions, as well as the cognitive and behavioral skills explicitly
targeting those distortions (Beck & Freeman, 1990).
4.
There may be an emergence of characteristic patterns of distortions that delineate particular
Axis I versus Axis II conditions. If the ICD supports this difference, its use may increase
diagnostic accuracy, which may, in turn, lead to more effective treatment selection.
5.
Demonstrating a correlation between the amelioration of cognitive distortions and the
improvement of clinical symptoms for Axis I and Axis II conditions would lend further
empirical support to the cognitive model of psychological disorders on both axes.
6.
In a review of 136 research studies, Grove and Meehl, (1996) determined that empirically
based personality assessment instruments are consistently equal to or superior to lessstructured clinical interview methods for increasing the efficiency of the assessment process,
for understanding patients, establishing rapport, formulating an accurate diagnosis, for
developing insight, developing optimum empirically guided treatment planning, and for
predicting the course of treatment (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Consequently, a brief self-report
measure administered early in the treatment process might also lessen the burden on the
psychological and medical communities by reducing the course of treatment and, by
extension, healthcare costs.
7.
The ICD has been validated only in an anxious and depressed sample. This study also
sought to further expand the utility of the ICD beyond the evaluation of anxiety and
77
depression (Yurica, 2002) for use with additional Axis I and Axis II conditions. Additionally,
it is hoped that a sufficiently validated ICD will allow for greater flexibility in empirical
research by providing an additional assessment tool for the design and standardization of
empirical procedure, which can be employed to measure pathology and progress for a
broader range of psychological disorders, as well as aiding in the replication of empirical
research. In addition, the ICD should also provide researchers and clinicians with an elegant
tool to gauge quickly and accurately guage the severity of pathology and aid in treatment
planning.
8.
Psychometrically, the goals of the study are to a) evaluate the construct validity of the ICD
in a sample of Axis I and Axis II patients, b) determine the relationship between the ICD
with specific Axis I and II disorders, c) establish the relationship between the ICD and the
MCMI—III, a well-established, multidimensional, valid and reliable measure of Axis I and
Axis II conditions, and d) further confirm the internal consistency of ICD content by
confirming the acceptable alpha coefficient levels originally found by Yurica (2002).
78
Research Hypotheses
The study included eight research hypotheses. Table 2 presents a summary of hypotheses,
rationale for individual hypotheses, and respective operational definitions.
Table 2
Table 2. Hypothesis Summary
Hypothesis
Rationale
Formula
H 1: Individuals with a greater
number of diagnosable Axis I
clinical disorders and/or Axis
II PDs will report a higher
frequency of cognitive
distortions than will
individuals with fewer
diagnosable psychological
disorders or who are otherwise
free of diagnosable
psychological disorders.
If frequency of cognitive
distortion correlates with the
manifestation of clinical and
personality disorders, then
individuals experiencing more
frequent cognitive distortions
should manifest a greater
number of psychological
disorders.
The number of significant
clinical disorder and
personality disorder scales r
Total ICD scores
H2: The number of PDs for
which an individual may be
diagnosed will positively
correlate with the reported
frequency of cognitive
distortions.
If frequency of cognitive
distortion correlates with the
manifestation of personality
disorders, then individuals
experiencing more frequent
cognitive distortions should
manifest a greater number of
personality disorders.
The number of significant
MCMI—III Clinical
Personality Pattern scale
scores and/or Severe
Personality Pathology scale
scores r Total ICD scores
H3: The number of Axis I
clinical syndromes for which
an individual can be diagnosed
will po sitively correlate with a
person’s reported frequency of
cognitive distortion.
If frequency of cognitive
distortion correlates with the
manifestation of clinical
syndromes, then individuals
experiencing more frequent
cognitive distortions should
manifest a greater number of
clinical syndromes.
The number of significant
MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes
and/or Severe Clinical
Syndromes r Total ICD
scores.
H4: Individuals with a greater
number of comorbid Axis I
and Axis II disorders will
report more frequent cognitive
Individuals with comorbid
Axis I and Axis II conditions
engage in more frequent
cognitive distortions than
The number of significant
MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes
and/or Severe Clinical
Syndromes r Total ICD
79
distortion than will individuals
meeting criteria for only one
Axis, that is either personality
disorder(s) or for clinical
syndrome(s).
individuals who are
diagnosable on only one axis.
scores.
H5: The severity of Axis I
conditions will po sitively
correlate with the reported
frequency of cognitive
distortions.
Individuals with more severe
Axis I pathology engage in
more frequent cognitive
distortions than those with less
severe Axis I conditions.
The sum of all MCMI—III
Clinical Syndromes and
Severe Clinical Syndromes
scale BR score points of 75 or
greater r Total ICD scores.
H6: The severity of personality
disorders will po sitively
correlate with the reported
frequency of cognitive
distortions.
Individuals with more severe
personality disorder pathology
will engage in more frequent
cognitive distortions than
those with less severe Axis II
conditions.
The sum of all MCMI—III
Clinical Personality Pattern
and Severe Personality
Pathology BR scale score
points of 75 or greater r Total
ICD scores
H7: The total severity of
psychological dysfunction,
independent of whether the
diagnoses occur on Axis I or
Axis II, will positively
correlate with the reported
frequency of cognitive
distortions.
Individuals with more severe
psychological dysfunction will
engage in more cognitive
distortion, regardless of
whether or not impairment is
diagnosable on Axis I or Axis
II.
The sum of all MCMI—III
BR scale score points of 75 or
greater on both Axis I and
Axis II r Total ICD scores
H8: There will be a positive
correlation between reported
cognitive distortions and each
clinical diagnosis for which an
individual tests positive.
The frequency of cognitive
distortion will correlate with
individual diagnosis.
Individual MCMI-III scale
score points of BR 75 or
greater r Total ICD scores.
Note: Psychological disorders will be operationalized as MCMI—III scale scores, whereas
cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores. Individuals must score at least
BR 75 or greater for inclusion on a minimum of one MCMI—III personality or clinical disorder
scales.
Hypothesis 1: Individuals with a greater number of diagnosable Axis I clinical disorders
and/or Axis II PDs will report a higher frequency of cognitive distortions than will individuals
with fewer diagnosable psychological disorders or who are otherwise free of diagnosable
80
psychological disorders. Diagnosable disorders will be operationalized as a BR of 75 or greater
on any scale in the following MCMI-III categories: Clinical Syndromes (Axis I), Severe Clinical
Syndromes (Axis I), Clinical Personality Patterns (Axis II), Severe Personality Pathology (Axis
II). This will yield a score of between 0 for no disorders to 24 if all personality scales and clinical
disorder scales are scored BR 75 or greater.
Cognitive distortion will be operationalized as Total ICD scores. Participants will be judged
as manifesting a “diagnosable” disorder by meeting a threshold of BR 75 or greater on one or
more scales of either Axis I or Axis II pathology. Although an MCMI—III score of BR 85
indicates the prominence of a clinical syndrome or the presence of a personality disorder
(Millon, 1994, Millon & Davis, 1996), a BR of 75 indicates the presence of a style or syndrome.
It should be noted that this criterion applies to all of this study’s hypotheses (See Table 2 for
summary of hypotheses, rationale, and formulae.)
Hypothesis 2: The number of PDs for which an individual may be diagnosed will positively
correlate with the reported frequency of cognitive distortions. The number of PDs will be
operationalized as the sum of all significant MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern scales and
Severe Personality Pathology scale that score BR 75 or greater. This will yield a score of
between 0 for no PDs to 14 if all personality scales scored BR 75 or greater. Cognitive
distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores.
Rationale for H1 and H2: According to Millon et al. (1997, p. 124), “the greater the number
of scales elevated above BR 75, the greater the personality pathology” and the higher the BR
scale score elevation above BR 75, the more severe the disturbance. Thus, the number of
significant MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern and/or Severe Personality Pathology scales
should correlate with total pathology on Axis II, as measured by the ICD. Although all MCMI-
81
III BR scores above 75 are interpretable, a score of BR 75 is expected to indicate the presence of
a clinically significant personality “style” or “syndrome”; however, BR 85 is indicative of
symptoms sufficient to warrant a diagnosis of PD (Millon & Davis, 1996, p. 120).
Hypothesis 3: The number of Axis I clinical syndromes for which an individual can be
diagnosed will po sitively correlate with a person’s reported frequency of cognitive distortion.
This means that an individuals who meet criteria for only one Axis I diagnosis is expected to
engage in less cognitive distortion than individuals with a greater number of clinical syndromes.
These effects are expected to be incremental, with the tendency to engage in cognitive distortion
increasing as the number of Axis I clinical syndromes increases.
The number of Axis I clinical syndromes will be operationalized as the total number of
MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes (Axis I), Severe Clinical Syndromes (Axis I) that are elevated
above BR 75. This will yield a potential score of 0 for no diagnosis to 10, if an individual scores
equal to or greater than BR 75 for all of the MCMI-III Axis I scales.
Hypothesis 4: Individuals with a greater number of comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders
will report more frequent cognitive distortion than will individuals meeting criteria for only one
Axis; that is it meets the criteria either for personality disorder(s) or clinical syndrome(s). In
other words, individuals meeting diagnostic criteria for both Axis I and Axis II conditions will
engage in more cognitive distortions than an individual who is diagnosable only on Axis I or
Axis II. Additionally, these effects are predicted to be incremental, increasing as the number of
disorders increases.
The number of Axis I and Axis II disorders will be operationalized as above, with a possible
range of scores of 0 for no diagnosis to 24 for an individual scoring greater than or equal to BR
75 for all of the Axis II scales (n=14) and Axis I scales (n=10).
82
Hypothesis 5: The severity of Axis I conditions will positively correlate with the reported
frequency of cognitive distortions. The severity of Axis I conditions will be operationalized as
MCMI-III scale scores of BR 75 or greater on for any of the Clinical Syndromes and/or Severe
Clinical Syndrome scales. Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores.
The severity of Axis I conditions will be quantified as the sum of all BR points for Clinical
Syndromes and Severe Clinical Syndrome scale scores of BR 75 or greater for any of the Axis I
scales. A ceiling of 115 is imposed for a valid score (Millon & Davis, 1996). The scope of
possible Axis I severity scores will range from 0 to a possible score of 400 for an individual
scoring 115 for all 10 scales. For example, an individual scoring BR 95 for scale A and 85 for
scale D would yield an Axis I severity rating of 20 +10 = 30.
Rationale for H3, H4, and H5: Axis I clinical syndromes may appear independent of
personality pathology. (Millon & Davis, 1996). Empirical evidence lends strong support to the
cognitive model of emotional disorders for various Axis I clinical syndromes (Beck, 1967,
1976), which posits that schema-driven “cognitive errors” powerfully influence affect and
behavior (Beck et al., 1979, p. 10). Perhaps at the most fundamental level, one of the principal
goals of CT is to reduce these characteristic patterns of distorted thinking. Even schema-focused
approaches ultimately aim to replace maladaptive core beliefs with more adaptive cognitions,
which maintain, and are perpetuated by, one’s cognitive distortions (Young, 1999). Thus, it is
expected that the frequency of cognitive distortion will correlate with the severity and number of
psychological disorders.
Hypothesis 6. The severity of PDs will positively correlate with the reported frequency of
cognitive distortions. Axis II personality severity will be operationalized as the sum of all
MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern and Severe Personality Pathology scales scores of BR 75
83
or greater, indicating a threshold of BR 75 for significance. The MCMI—III imposes a ceiling of
BR 115 (Millon & Davis, 1996). Thus, the scope of possible Axis II severity scores will range
from 0 to a possible score of 560 for an individual scoring the maximum 115 for all 14 Axis II
pathology scales. Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total ICD scores.
Rationale for H6: In a recent study conducted with a combined sample of clinical and
nonclinical populations, O’Connor & Dyce (2001) demonstrated empirically that the MCMI—III
was able to differentiate the individuals with PDs from those individuals failing to meet Axis II
diagnostic criteria. Normal and disordered personalities were found to coexist in a variety of
regions of the multivariate space based on the 5-factor model (FFM) of personality (Widiger,
2000). Examination of the interpersonal dimensions chart (Figure 1) reveals that within regions,
the profiles of normal and disordered personalities were very similar in characteristic
configuration or vector but notably different in vector length. Moreover, this difference can be
detected by the MCMI-III. Those with PDs appear to be “out there” in inner experience,
interpersonal behavior, and in this case, geometric configuration. Significantly, length of vector
predicted the presence of Axis II pathology, but it failed to predict any specific personality
disorder. Consequently, it is predicted that the severity of cognitive distortion will predict the
presence and severity of Axis II conditions. Although cognitive distortion may be able to predict
the extremity of the vector and presence of personality disorder(s), it is not predicted to be able
to predict the direction of the vector, that is, specific personality disorder.
Personality-disordered individuals tended to be located in the perimeters or outer regions of
the FFM space, as indicated by their longer vector lengths; this is indicative of the fact that the
presence and severity of a personality disorder was manifested in a difference in the quantity,
rather than the quality of four of the five FFM dimensions: Neuroticism, extraversion,
84
agreeableness, and conscientiousness, which were differentially more extreme in individuals
with PDs. Specifically, those reaching the threshold for significance on MCMI-III, with BR
scores equal to or greater than BR 75 or BR 85, exhibited significantly more extremity and
rigidity on these dimensions.
Hypothesis 7. The total severity of psychological dysfunction, independent of whether
diagnoses occur on Axis I or Axis II, will positively correlate with reported frequency of
cognitive distortions.
Total severity of psychological dysfunction scores will be operationalized as the sum of all
relevant MCMI-III scales of BR 75 or greater within the following categories: Clinical
Syndromes (Axis I), Severe Clinical Syndromes (Axis I), Clinical Personality Patterns (Axis II),
Severe Personality Pathology (Axis II). Cognitive distortions will be operationalized as Total
ICD scores. This indicates a threshold for significance of BR 75. The scope of possible total
severity scores will range from 0 to a possible score of 960 for an individual scoring 115 for all
24 clinical scales.
Rationale for H7: Millon & Davis, (1997) conceptualize Axis I clinical syndromes as
“extensions or distortions of the patient’s basic personality pattern... waxing and waning over
time depending on the impact of stressful situations”(p. 22).
The multiaxial model po sits an interaction between Axis I and Axis II symptomatology in
which personality is defined as the “…overall capacity to perceive and to cope with our
psychosocial world…” (Millon & Davis, 1996, p. 120). Personality is further conceptualized as
analogous to an immune system, protecting the individual from the vicissitudes of external
stressors. However, Axis I symptoms may occur because the PD patients dysfunctional “immune
systems” fails to protect the individual from stress. When this occurs, Axis I clinical syndromes
85
arise in addition to the preexisting PD symptoms. This increases the total severity of the
individual’s dysfunction (Millon & Davis, 1996). In other words, all other things being equal, an
individual meeting the diagnostic criteria for both Major Depressive Disorder and Dependent
Personality Disorder is expected to evince more impairment and/or distress than another person
meeting diagnostic criteria for only one of these axes.
Comorbid diagnoses across axes may lead to a pattern in which cognitive distortion can
constrict behavior in a fashion that may actually exacerbate existing difficulties, leading to
vicious circles of cognitive distortion, distress, and interpersonal impairment. When this occurs,
one’s cognition may be further impaired by the resulting Axis I clinical syndromes. Prolonged
Axis I symptoms can, in turn, exacerbate personality dysfunction.
Because of this reciprocal interaction between Axis I and Axis II (Millon et al., 1996; Millon
& Davis, 1996), it is expected that individuals with greater total pathology, independent of
whether this pathology arises from Axis I or Axis II, will experience more frequent cognitive
distortions. This explains why Millon & Davis, (1997) posit the idea that the presence of Axis I
pathology supports the presence of personality pathology.
Finally, Beck (1976) and Lefebvre (1981) suggest that a total distortion score might be of
more practical value than differentiating individual categories of distortions. Similarly, the DAS
(Weisman & Beck, 1978) was designed to yield a single total score of dysfunctional attitudes.
Consequently, this study will correlate ICD total scores with the number of diagnosable clinical
disorders, with the severity of total psychological dysfunction, and with various individual
disorders—all of which will be determined by the MCMI—III scale scores.
Hypothesis 8: There will be a positive correlation between reported cognitive distortions and
individual clinical diagnosis scores for which an individual tests positive. Cognitive distortions
86
will be operationalized as ICD scores; a positive diagnosis will be operationalized as BR scores
greater than 75 on any MCMI-III scale on either Axis I or Axis II. The 24 MCMI-III BR scale
scores plus a single ICD score will yield 25 x 25 multi-correlational matrix correlating cognitive
distortions with individual disorders.
Rationale for H8: According to the cognitive model, cognitive distortions will positively
correlate with psychological disorders as assessed by the MCMI-III on either Axis I or Axis II.
However, it is not expected that any differential pattern of frequency of cognitive distortion to
specific disorder will emerge.
Definition of Terms
Diagnoses: Operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any of the MCMI-III
subscales.
Axis II Personality Disorder: Operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any
MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern Scale(s) or any Severe Personality Pathology Scale(s).
Axis I Clinical Syndrome(s): Operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any
MCMI-III Clinical Syndrome Scale(s) or Severe Clinical Syndrome Scale(s).
The 11 theoretical factors or cognitive distortions, as defined verbatim by Yurica (2002, p.
60), are composed of the following:
Cognitive Distortions:
1.
Externalization of Self-Worth: Refers to the development and maintenance of self-worth
based almost exclusively on how the external world views oneself (Freeman & DeWolf, 1992 ;
Freeman & Oster, 1999).
87
2.
Fortune-Telling: The process of foretelling or predicting a future event or events and
believing that this prediction is absolutely true for oneself (Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).
3.
Magnification: The tendency to exaggerate or magnify either the positive or negative
consequences of some personal trait, event, or circumstance (Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).
4.
Labeling: The cognitive process of labeling oneself using derogatory names (Burns, 1980,
1989, 1999; Freeman & Dewolf, 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).
5.
Perfectionism: Refers to a constant striving to live up to some internal or external
representation of perfection without examining the evidence for the reasonableness of these
perfect standards, often to avoid the subjective experience of failure (Freeman & DeWolf, 1990;
Freeman & Dewolf, 1992 ; Freeman & Oster, 1999).
6.
Comparison to Others: The tendency to compare oneself to others whereby the outcome
typically results in concluding that they are inferior or worse off than others (Freeman, &
DeWolf 1992; Freeman & Oster, 1999).
7.
Emotional Reasoning: Refers to the predominant use of an emotional state to form
conclusions about oneself, others, or situations (Beck et al., 1979; Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).
8.
Arbitrary Inference/Jumping to Conclusion: Refers to the process of drawing a negative
conclusion in the absence of specific evidence to support that conclusion (Beck et al., 1979;
Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).
9.
Minimization: Refers to the process of minimizing or discounting the importance of some
event, trait, or circumstance (Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).
10.
Mind-Reading: Refers to one’s arbitrary conclusion that someone is reacting negatively, or
thinking negatively towards him or her, without specific evidence to support that conclusion
(Burns, 1980, 1989, 1999).
88
11.
Emotional Reasoning and Decision-Making: The tendency to rely on emotions to make
decisions (Yurica, 2002).
89
CHAPTER 3
METHODOLOGY
Participants
A total of 168 adult outpatients presenting at the Center for Brief Therapy at the
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine for psychological treatment or evaluation were
offered the opportunity to participate in the present study of cognition. Participants were also
recruited from various patients in the care of clinicians in private practice in the Philadelphia
area.
Participation in this project was voluntary for all subjects and they retained the right to
withdraw at any time without explanation. All participants remained completely anonymous. No
identifying information was ever collected on any subjects, other than basic demographic data.
Clinicians presented the participants with a packet containing the following:
1.
Letter of informed consent.
2.
Brief Demographic Questionnaire
3.
Millon Clinical Multliaxial Inventory-III
4.
Inventory of Cognitive Distortions
5.
A stamped, self addressed return envelope to the investigators
Completion time was estimated to be no more than 50 minutes.
90
Inclusion Criteria
Participants were required to meet the following conditions to be included in this study.
Individuals were between 18 to 88 years of age (Millon & Davis, 1996) and obtained a score of
BR 75 or greater on at least one MCMI-III subscale on either Axis I or Axis II. Participants
currently undergoing pharmacological care were included in the study and identified by a yes or
no format on the Brief Information Sheet (BIS) to determine whether or not they were currently
taking psychotropic medication (Yurica, 2002). Participants verified a minimum of an eighth
grade education on the MCMI-III questionnaire sheet.
Exclusion Criteria
Participants younger than 18 or older than 88 years of age were excluded. Additionally
excluded were participants who met diagnostic criteria for traumatic brain injury, mental
retardation, pervasive developmental disorders, tic disorders, delirium, dementia, amnestic
disorders, schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders (Yurica, 2002), or individuals who
appeared to be intoxicated. These diagnoses were made by the treating clinician. Also excluded
were participants who reported an educational level below eighth grade either to the clinician or
on the MCMI-III.
91
Participant Recruitment
Participants were recruited from the Center for Brief Therapy, an outpatient mental
healthcare clinic associated with the Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine and from
individual clinicians in private clinical practice. Study clinicians included therapists at the
Master’s level, doctoral students, doctoral candidates, as well as licensed psychologists. All
graduate-level therapists were supervised by an experienced licensed psychologist.
Participants were requested to review an Agreement to Participate in Research Study of
Cognition form prior to participating in this study. They were then given a packet including the
BIS, one copy of the ICD, and one copy of the MCMI—III. The anticipated time for completion
of both instruments was approximately 50 minutes. Participation was completely voluntary and
participants could have withdrawn at any time without explanation.
All participant data remained completely anonymous. Only demographic data, such as age
race, gender, and a yes/no response about current psychotropic medication use was retained. No
other identifiable participant information was collected or stored.
Research Design
A correlational design was employed to compare the relationship between cognitive
distortions, as measured by the ICD, and psychological disorders, operationalized as scores on
the MCMI-III. Additionally, the study was intended to assess the psychometric properties of the
ICD. A series of Pearson Product Moment Correlations was conducted to compare ICD scores of
92
those with significant clinical and/or PDs, as measured by the MCMI—III, versus those who
failed to meet diagnostic criteria for any Axis I or Axis II condition.
Measures
The Million Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-Third Edition
Test materials consisted of the MCMI—III (Millon, 1994), the ICD (Yurica & DiTomasso,
2002), and a brief demographic questionnaire.
MCMI—III.
The MCMI—III (Millon, 1994) is a 175-item self-report test with a total of 27 scales.
Estimated administration time is approximately 25 minutes. The test is divided into five sections
with scales measuring both PDs and clinical syndromes comprising: (a) Modifying Indices (3
validity scales), (b) Clinical Personality Patterns (11 basic personality disorder scales on Axis II),
(c) Severe Personality Pathology (3 severe personality disorder scales on Axis II), (d) Clinical
Syndromes (7 moderate psychopathology scales on Axis I), and (e) Severe Syndromes (3 severe
psychopathology scales on Axis I). The MCMI—III raw scores are translated into BR scores.
The BR method of scoring is used on the MCMI—III rather than the more common normalized
standard t-score transformations. The rationale for the use of BR scores is to assure that the
proportion of individuals who score above each scale's cutoff point approximates, as closely as
possible, the actual prevalence rates of the disorder in a clinical population. A BR score of 0 is
the minimum, and a BR score of 115 corresponds to the maximum valid BR score for each scale.
93
A BR score of 60 corresponds to the median BR score for each scale in a psychiatric population
(Millon, 1994; Millon & Davis, 1996).
MCMI—III reliability.
Millon & Davis (1997) reported that the MCMI—III has demonstrated acceptable
psychometric properties. The instruments internal consistency, or the degree to which scale items
intercorrelate, ranged from a low of Cronbach’s alpha 0f .66 for the Compulsive scale to a high
of .90 for Major Depression.
MCMI—III validity.
The MCMI-III also demonstrated good concurrent validity with the Major Depression
and Dysthymia scales correlating with the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Steer, 1987) .74
and .71, respectively (Millon & Davis, 1996). The State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI;
Spielberger, 1983) correlated more highly with the Major Depression scale (.59) than the
Anxiety scale, the MCMI—III; however, the Anxiety scale still correlated moderately with state
(.55) and trait (.58) anxiety (Millon et al., 1997). Consequently, caution might be appropriate
regarding discriminant validity as, for further example, the BDI also correlated .63 with the
PTSD and .62 with the Thought Disorder scales (Millon & Davis, 1996).
94
Predictive validity.
The aggressive personality disorder scales and several of the neurotic scales correlated with
future institutional violence in prison populations (Retzlaff, Stoner, & Kleinsasser, 2002).
Discriminant validity.
The MCMI—III has demonstrated both convergent and discriminant validity as a measure
of sleep disturbance (Allen, Console, & Brethour, 2000).
The MCMI-III has also demonstrated high test-retest validity with a median for all scales of
.91 (Millon & Davis, 1996).
The Inventory of Cognitive Distortions
The ICD, a 69-item self-report inventory, is composed of short sentences reflecting 11
factor-analyzed cognitive distortions. The ICD was designed for and validated with an adult
clinical population with symptoms of anxiety and/or depression (Yurica, 2002). Items are scored
on a five-point Likert (1932) scale ranging from 1 = Never to 5 = Always. Total possible ICD
scores range from 69 to 345, with lower scores reflecting less frequency of cognitive distortions
than higher scores. The instrument has attained satisfactory construct and content validity, with
experts achieving 100% agreement that all inventory items reflected each specific distortion
construct. Only items with 100% expert agreement were retained for final factor-analysis. The
intent of the ICD is to provide a total score of cognitive distortion, with higher scores signifying
95
more severe patterns of distortion and employment of more types of distortion. The scale also
yields subscale scores of distortion. Yurica (2002) demonstrated good psychometric properties
for the ICD, as follows:
ICD Psychometrics
ICD Reliability.
The initial validation study found an impressive test-retest reliability with a test-retest
reliability coefficient for total ICD scores of .998 (n = 28, p <, 001).
ICD validity.
The ICD also demonstrated good concurrent validity, as Total ICD scores correlated
significantly and positively with other widely accepted measures of psychopathology, such as
dysfunctional attitudes, the DAS (r = .70, N = 159, p < . 0001); depression, the BDI-II (r = .70,
N = 161, p < .0001); and anxiety, the BAI (r = .59, N = 161, p < .0001). In the same study,
Yurica (2002) also found impressive criterion validity because the Total ICD scores
differentiated clinical outpatients from non-patient controls (F = 15.2, df = 169, p < .0001).
Procedure
The present study included a heterogeneous population of adult outpatients ranging in age
from 18 to 88 years; these patients presented to the Center for Brief Therapy at the Philadelphia
96
College of Osteopathic Medicine or specified local clinicians for psychological treatment or
evaluation. Excluded were those meeting diagnostic criteria for mental retardation, pervasive
developmental disorders, amnestic disorders, schizophrenia or any of the other psychotic
disorders, or those appearing to be intoxicated. Confirmation of inclusionary and exclusionary
criteria was performed by the treating clinicians (study collaborators) and through analysis of the
BIS and MCMI-III. Study collaborators included therapists at the Master’s level, do ctoral
students, doctoral candidates, as well as licensed psychologists. All graduate level therapists
were supervised by experienced, licensed psychologists.
Placement of the instruments into packets varied randomly to control for sequence and order
effects, with the MCMI—III preceding the ICD in only some cases (Kazdin, 1998). The random
order for each packet will be determined by coin toss.
Participants were informed in writing about the purpose and procedures involved in the
study. All participants had the right to withdraw from the study at any time without explanation.
Participation in the study was completely voluntary. Descriptive data were gathered including
age, sex, marital status, educational level, and a yes-no response as to current use of psychotropic
medication. Participants were informed that their responses would remain anonymous and
confidential.
97
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
A correlational research design with a sample of a heterogeneous group of adult outpatients
was used to ascertain the relationship between psychometric variables encompassed by the ICD
(frequency of cognitive distortions) and the MCMI-III (psychological diagnoses). In addition,
results were analyzed to test the psychometric properties of the ICD. Data gathered from these
individuals comprised the entire sample used for statistical computation and analyses. Study
collaborators recruited clients to volunteer from among their patients; the collaborators included
practicing psychologists, counselors, practicum students or interns in the Greater Philadelphia
area, recruited clients to volunteer from among their patients. Both study collaborators and the
MCMI-III screened participants for a minimum of eight years of education.
All psychological outpatients were combined to form one outpatient sample for the purposes
of this study. The sample (N = 168) included those meeting objective criteria for a clinical
diagnosis either on Axis I and/or Axis II.
Diagnosis was operationally defined as a score of 75 or greater on any of the ten MCMI-III
Axis I Clinical Syndrome Scales or any of the 14 Personality Scales. This provided objective
clinical diagnosis on a widely accepted measure of psychopathology. A number of variables
were analyzed, including participant demographics, diagnostic variables, frequency of cognitive
distortions, and the intercorrelation of these factors.
98
Descriptive Statistics
Age
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 86 years of age. The mean age in both groups was 42.8
with a standard deviation of 18.1.
Gender
The gender distribution for the total sample (N = 168) consisted of 48 males (29.3%) and
116 females (70.7%).
Marital Status
Marital status was distributed in the total sample as 65 single (67.3%), 69 married (42.1%),
and 29 (17.7%) endorsing “other”, the latter was further defined by the MCMI-III as separated,
divorced, widowed, or cohabitating.
99
Use of Psychotropic Medication
Of the 154 participants responding, 114 (67.9%) reported not using psychotropic
medication, whereas 39 (23.2%) reported current medication use.
Internal Consistency
Cronbach’s Alpha for the ICD scale was .97 for all ICD items, indicating good internal
consistency and homogeneity of item content.
Relationship to Demographic Variables
The Relationship between the MCMI-III and the Total ICD Scores
Statistical Package for Social Sciences version 10.0 (SPSS 10.0) was used in the data
analysis. A number of Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients, one-tailed were
gathered to test the relationship between the number (operationalized as the number MCMI-III
subscale scores of 75 or greater) and severity (operationalized as the cumulative sum of MCMIIII BR scores of 75 or greater) of clinical syndrome(s) and/or personality disorder(s) on the one
hand, and the total frequency of cognitive distortion on the other hand (hypotheses 1, 2, 3, 5, 6,
100
and 7). A one-way ANOVA was conducted to determine if there was a significant difference in
Total ICD scores between individuals diagnosable on only one clinical axis versus those
diagnosable with comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders (hypothesis 4). Finally, a series of
Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients was also conducted to illustrate the
relationship between the presence of specific Axis I and Axis II disorders on the one hand
(operationalized as MCMI-III scores of 75 or greater on any subscale), and the frequency of
cognitive distortions (operationalized as total ICD scores) on the other hand (hypothesis 8).
Hypothesis 1
It was hypothesized that the number of Axis I and Axis II disorders (MCMI-III BR scores of
75 or greater on any Axis I or Axis II scale) for which an individual met criteria would correlate
with the total ICD score. The results demonstrated a significantly positive Pearson correlation
coefficient (r = .662, r2 = .438, p <.001, one-tailed) on this dimension. This illustrates a coeffient
of determination, indicating that almost 44% of the variance in the number of Axis I and Axis II
disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was attributable to differences in Total
ICD scores.
Hypothesis 2
It was hypothesized that the number of PDs for which an individual may be diagnosed
(MCMI-III BR scores of 75 or greater on any Axis II scale) would positively correlate with the
total ICD score. A significant and moderate Pearson correlation coefficient (r = .563, r2 = .312, p
< .001, one-tailed) was obtained on this variable. Consequently, it appeared that 31.2% of the
variance in the number of Axis II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was
attributable to the differences in Total ICD score.
101
Hypothesis 3
It was hypothesized that the number of Axis I clinical syndromes for which an individual
could be diagnosed (MCMI-III BR scores of 75 or greater on any Axis I scale) would positively
correlate with the total ICD score. Again, a significantly positive Pearson correlation coefficient
(r = .638, r2 = .407, p <.001) was obtained, indicating that over 40% of the variance in the
number Axis I disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was attributable to the
differences in Total ICD score.
Hypothesis 4
It was hypothesized that the number of comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders (MCMI-III
BR scores of 75 or greater on both Axis I and Axis II scales) would report more frequent
cognitive distortion (greater total ICD scores) than will individuals meeting criteria for only one
Axis, that is for either personality disorder(s) or clinical syndrome(s). One-way ANOVA results
(see Table 3) demonstrated a significant difference between individuals diagnosable on only one
clinical axis versus those diagnosable with comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders, including total
ICD scores (F = 74.778, df = 123, p < .001). It should be noted that only five participants met
diagnostic criteria for only Axis I without PD pathology, making comparison with that group
superfluous.
102
Table 3
Analysis of Variance for Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Conditions
Source
Df
F
MS
p
Between groups
1
74.78
83333.3
.001
Within groups
122
1114.40
Note. Individuals with a larger number of comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders experienced a
significantly greater frequency of cognitive distortion than did those who met criteria for fewer
comorbid conditions. (Participants meeting only Axis I criteria were excluded do to a small n (n
= 5).
Hypothesis 5
It was hypothesized that the severity of Axis I conditions (the cumulative sum of MCMI-III
BR scores of 75 or greater on Axis I scales) would positively correlate with total ICD scores.
Pearson correlation results indicated that there was a significant, positive relationship between
these variables (r = .693, r2 = .48. p <.001, one-tailed). Thus, almost half of the variance in
severity of Axis I syndromes was attributable to differences in total ICD scores.
Hypothesis 6
It was hypothesized that the severity of PDs (the cumulative sum of MCMI-III BR scores of
75 or greater on all Axis II scales) would positively correlate with total ICD scores. Once again,
Pearson correlations indicated a highly significantly positive relationship on this dimension (r =
.757, r2 = 573, p <.001, one-tailed). In other words, 57 percent of the variance in severity of
personality disturbance was attributable to differences in total ICD scores.
103
Hypothesis 7
It was hypothesized that the total severity of psychological dysfunction, independent of
whether or not the diagnoses occurred on Axis I or Axis II (the cumulative sum of all MCMI-III
BR scores of 75 or greater on all Axis I and Axis II scales), would positively correlate with total
ICD scores. Again, results of Pearson correlations suggested a strong and highly significant
relationship in this regard (r = .748, r2 = .56, p <.001, one-tailed). This means that, once again,
well over half of the variance in severity of psychological dysfunction, across clinical axes, was
attributable to differences in total ICD scores.
Table 4 presents correlation coefficients for frequency of cognitive distortions in relation
both to the severity and the number of disorders.
Table 4
Number and Severity of Psychological Disorders across Axis I and Axis II and the Relation to
Total Frequency of Cognitive Distortion.
Table 4
Axis I
Axis II
Both
(Axes I and I)
Number of Disorders
.638**
.563**
.662**
Severity of Disorders
.693**
.757**
.748**
104
Note. Pearson coefficients, when comparing the total frequency of cognitive distortion either
with the number or the severity of psychological disorder, significant correlations were found for
both Axis I and Axis II disorders, severally and when comorbid. **p < .001.
Hypothesis 8
A series of Point-Biserial Correlations were conducted to determine the validity of the
hypothesized positive correlation between individual clinical diagnostic scores on either Axis I
and/or Axis II for which an individual tested positive (MCMI-III BR scores of 75 or greater on
any Axis I and/or Axis II scale[s]) and total ICD scores. The diagnostic variables were
dichotomous, meaning that individuals either met or did not meet operational criteria for each
specific disorder. For example, a participant with a score of 87 on schizoid and 64 on narcissistic
met criteria for the former, but not the latter. This positive relationship was confirmed by
multiple, significant Pearson correlations for most Axis I and Axis II disorder, on the one hand,
and Total ICD scores, on the other hand (see Table 5). However, a number of exceptions were
discovered.
First, although correlated in a positive direction, both bipolar disorder (r = .104, p < .109,
one-tailed) and drug dependence (r = .063, p <, 228, one-tailed) failed to reach statistical
significance. Conversely, three Axis II disorders were significantly but negatively correlated with
frequency of cognitive distortion, specifically, narcissistic (NPD), histrionic (HPD), and
compulsive PDs (OCPD) which correlated negatively with the total ICD scores, as follows: NPD
(r = -.214, r2 = .046, p < .05, one-tailed); HPD (r = -.266, r2 = .071, p < .001, one-tailed); and
OCPD (r =-.343. r2 = .118, p < .001).
105
Table 5
The Relationship between Frequency of Cognitive Distortion (Total ICD score) and Individual
Psychological Disorders.
Axis II
Axis I
Schizoid
.233**
Anxiety
.613***
Avoidant
.494***
Somatoform
.247*
Depressive
.491***
Bipolar
.104
Dependent
.518***
Dysthymia
.436***
Histrionic
-266***
Alcohol Dependence
-
Narcissistic
-.214**
Drug Dependence
.063
Antisocial
.188*
PTSD
.300***
Aggressive/Sadistic
.154*
Thought Disorder
.266***
Compulsive
-.343***
Major Depression
.344
Passive-Aggressive
.437***
Delusional Disorder
.334***
Self-Defeating
.513**
Shizotypal
.348***
Borderline
.384***
Paranoid
.354***
Note. Most Axis I and Axis II conditions were positively and significantly correlated with
frequency of cognitive distortion. Exceptions included bipolar disorder, drug dependence, NPD,
HPD, and OCPD. *p <.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
106
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was twofold: First, the study sought to determine whether or
not the frequency of cognitive distortions correlated with the number and severity of
psychological disorders across Axis I and Axis II. Second, the study endeavored to further assess
the validity and reliability of a promising new self-report measure of cognitive distortions by
correlating the ICD with clinical diagnoses as determined by the MCMI-III in a heterogeneous
adult clinical outpatient sample. Significant positive findings indicate that these goals were
achieved. This chapter summarizes the study, expands on relevant findings and discusses the
implications of the results in light of the existing literature. Study limitations and directions for
future research are also explored.
Cronbach’s analysis of all ICD items indicated strong homogeneity of item content
(Cronbach’s Alpha = .97). This finding indicates that 97% of Total ICD scores was due to shared
variance in the concept being measured, that is, cognitive distortions. Such internal consistency
provides valid support for using the Total ICD scores as a measure of the frequency of cognitive
distortion. In this manner, the study examined the relationship between the frequency of
cognitive distortions and the number and/or severity of psychological disorders.
Participants completed comprehensive assessment instruments for Axis I and II disorders,
as well as cognitive distortions; this was done to evaluate the impact of cognitive distortions on
co-occurring disorders. Specifically, the MCMI-III and the ICD were measured to determine the
relationship between cognitive distortion and the number and severity of psychological disorders
along Axis I and Axis II. Results supported all of the eight initial hypotheses.
107
Demographic Characteristics
Study participants included an ethnically diverse sample of clinical outpatients with a wide
variety of clinical diagnoses across the entire adult age span. Participants ranged in age from 18
to 86 years of age. The mean age was 42.8 years with a standard deviation of 18.1.
The gender distribution for the total sample (N = 168) consisted of 48 males (29.3%) and
116 females (70.7%). Marital status was distributed in the total sample as 65 single (39.9%), 69
married (42.3%), and 29 (17.8%) separated, divorced, widowed, or cohabitating. Of the 154
participants responding, 114 (67.9%) reported not using psychotropic medication, whereas 39
(23.2%) reported current medication use.
Major Findings
Presence of Psychopathology
As was hypothesized, individuals meeting criteria for any diagnosis (BR of 75 or greater on
any MCMI-III Axis scale[s]), on Axis I clinical disorders or on Axis II PDs, reported a higher
frequency of cognitive distortions than those who were free of diagnosable psychological
disorders. Moreover, individuals meeting criteria for a greater number of diagnoses, be they on
Axis I and/or Axis II PDs, reported a higher frequency of cognitive distortions than did
individuals with fewer diagnosable psychological disorders or than who were otherwise free of
diagnosable psychological disorders (r = .662, r2 = .438, p <.001, one-tailed). This illustrates the
108
fact that almost 44% of the variance in the ICD was attributable to the number of Axis I and Axis
II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria—the more disorders, the more
frequently they engaged in cognitive distortion.
These findings are consistent with the cognitive model of emotional disorders (Alford &
Beck, 1997; Beck, 1967; Beck et al., 1979), which predicts that dysfunctional cognition
correlates with psychopatholgy—in this case—generally across Axis I clinical syndromes and
Axis II PDs.
Axis I Clinical Syndromes.
Number of Axis I diagnoses.
The study found that as the number of clinical disorders for which an individual met criteria
increased (the total number of MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes, Severe Clinical Syndromes
elevated above BR 75), so too did their tendency to engage in cognitive distortion. A
significantly positive Pearson correlation coefficient (r = .638, r2 = .407, p <.001) was obtained,
indicating that over 40% of the variance in the ICD was attributable to the number Axis I
disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria. This means that an individual who met
criteria for only one Axis I diagnosis engaged in less frequent cognitive distortion than
individuals with a greater number of clinical syndromes. These effects were incremental, with
the tendency to engage in cognitive distortion increasing as the number of Axis I clinical
syndromes increased.
109
These results are consistent with the empirical findings of Najavitis et al. (2004), indicating
that individuals diagnosed with comorbid clinical syndromes exhibit greater maladaptive
cognition. This is also consistent with Beck’s (1967; Beck et al., 1979) cognitive model of
emotional disorders which predicts that dysfunctional cognition correlates with psychopatholgy --in this case—generally across Axis I clinical syndromes than those meeting criteria for only
one disorder.
Severity of Axis I clinical syndromes.
The severity of Axis I conditions (cumulative sum of all MCMI-III scale scores of BR 75
or greater on any of the Clinical Syndromes and/or Severe Clinical Syndrome scales) correlated
significantly and positively with the frequency of cognitive distortions (r = .693, r2 = .48. p
<.001, one-tailed). This indicates that almost half of the variance in severity of Axis I syndromes
was attributable to the frequency of cognitive distortion. These results are also consistent with
Najavits et al. (2004), who found that individuals meeting criteria for two comorbid Axis I
syndromes (PTSD and substance use disorders) were more liable to engage in more frequent
cognitive distortions than those meeting criteria for only one of those disorders, operationalized
as Cognitive Distortion Scale scores.
This finding lends additional empirical suppo rt to the cognitive model of emotional
disorders for various Axis I clinical syndromes (Beck, 1967, 1976), which posits that schemadriven “cognitive errors” powerfully influence affect and behavior (Beck et al., 1979, p. 10).
110
Perhaps at the most fundamental level, one of the principal goals of CT is to reduce these
characteristic patterns of distorted thinking. Even schema-focused approaches ultimately aim to
replace maladaptive core beliefs with more adaptive cognitions, which maintain, and are
perpetuated by, one’s cognitive distortions (Young, 1999). Thus, as was expected, the severity
and number of clinical disorders correlated with the frequency of cognitive distortion.
These findings also lend support to Millon’s assertions (Millon & Davis, 1996; Millon et al,
1997) that higher MCMI-III clinical syndrome scale scores reflect more severe Axis I severity.
As measured by the total frequency of cognitive distortion, the present study supports Millon’s
contention.
Axis II Personality Disorders
Number of personality disorders.
The number of PDs for which an individual met criteria (the number of all significant
MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern scales and Severe Personality Pathology scale that scored
BR 75 or greater) correlated significantly with the frequency of cognitive distortions (r = .563, r2
= .312, p < .001, one-tailed). Consequently, it appears that 31.2% of the variance in the number
Axis II disorders for which an individual met diagnostic criteria was attributable to the total
frequency of cognitive distortions.
A literature review revealed that studies investigating dysfunctional thinking in individuals
with comorbid psychological diagnoses have focused on the influence of PDs on various Axis I
conditions. No studies were found that examined the relationship between the number of
multiple PD diagnoses and cognitive distortion. This is surprising because the correlation
111
between PD diagnoses have been shown to be significant. For example, Beck et al. (2001)
determined that 32% of 756 outpatients with a primary Axis II diagnosis also met criteria for a
secondary PD. These authors also posited the fact that although the Personal Belief
Questionnaire (PBQ; Beck & Beck, 1991) differentiated five separate PDs (avoidant, dependent,
obsessive–compulsive, narcissistic, and paranoid PDs), the PBQ subscales were moderately to
strongly intercorrelated. Beck et al. (2001) proposed the idea that this corroborates the cognitive
model of personality disorders and, perhaps, a general distress factor that may be a cause, a
correlate, or a product of the inner experience and interpersonal functioning of individuals with
PDs (APA, 2000; Beck et al., 2001; Millon, 1999). It is likely that cognitive distortions
contribute to this general distress factor.
.
Severity of personality disorders.
There was a significant and positive relationship between the severity of PDs (cumulative
sum of all MCMI-III Clinical Personality Pattern and Severe Personality Pathology scales scores
of BR 75 or greater) and the frequency of cognitive distortions (r = .757, r2 = .573, p <.001, onetailed). This means that 57 percent of the variance in severity of personality disturbance was
explained by differences in total ICD scores.
These findings are consistent with a recent study conducted by O’Connor & Dyce (2001),
which demonstrated the fact that the MCMI—III was able to differentiate those individuals with
PDs from those individuals failing to meet Axis II diagnostic criteria. These investigators
determined that disordered personalities were very similar in characteristic but notably different
in the degree from which they varied from the norm. Moreover, as in the present study, this
112
difference was detected by the MCMI-III. Also similar to the present study, although the degree
of variance from “normal” predicted the presence of Axis II pathology, it failed to predict any
specific personality disorder.
Consequently, the ICD successfully predicted presence and severity of Axis II conditions.
Although the ICD predicted the extremity of the disorder and presence of personality disorder(s),
it did not, however, predict the diagnosis.
Comorbid Axis I and Axis II Disorders
Number of comorbid clinical syndromes and personality disorders.
As was predicted, the ANOVA revealed that individuals with a greater number of comorbid
Axis I and Axis II disorders engaged in more cognitive distortion than did individuals meeting
diagnostic criteria for fewer disorders (F = 74.778, df = 123, p < .001).
This finding offers support for the cognitive model of emotional disorders (Beck et al.,
1979) and the cognitive model of PDs (Beck et al., 2004), as well as the evolutionary model of
psychopathology (Millon et al, 1996), which respectively assert that there may be an additive
effect when comorbidity is present whether or not the disorders appear on either of the two
DSM-IV-TR diagnostic axes. Moreover, the greater the number of diagnoses, the more severe
the psychopathology (Diguer, Barber, & Luborsky, 1993; Farmer & Nelson-Gray, 1990; Shea,
Glass, Pilkonis, Watkins, & Docherty, 1987), operationalized in the present case, as an increased
frequency of cognitive distortions.
113
Total severity of all psychological dysfunction.
The total severity of psychological dysfunction across Axis I and Axis II (cumulative scores
of 75 or greater on all MCMI-III Clinical Syndromes, Severe Clinical Syndromes, Clinical
Personality Patterns, Severe Personality Pathology) positively correlated with cognitive
distortions (r = .748, r2 = .56, p <.001, one-tailed). This means that well over half of the variance
in severity of psychological dysfunction, across clinical axes, was attributable to differences in
total ICD scores. Moreover, the ICD was able to predict severity on both Axis I and Axis II.
Millon et al. (1997) conceptualized Axis I clinical syndromes as “extensions or distortions
of the patient’s basic personality pattern... waxing and waning over time depending on the
impact of stressful situations”(p. 22) . The multiaxial model po sits an interaction between Axis I
and Axis II symptomatology in which personality is defined as the “…overall capacity to
perceive and to cope with our psychosocial world…” (Millon et al., 1996, p. 120). Personality is
further conceptualized as analogous to an immune system, protecting the individual from the
fluctuating external stressors. However, Axis I symptoms may occur because the PD patients
dysfunctional “immune systems” fails to protect the individual from stress. When this occurs,
Axis I clinical syndromes arise in addition to the preexisting PD symptoms. This increases the
total severity of the individual’s dysfunction (Millon et al., 1996) . In other words, all other things
being equal, an individual meeting the diagnostic criteria for both major depressive disorder and
dependent personality disorder is expected to evince more impairment and/or distress than
another person meeting diagnostic criteria for a disorder on only one of these axes. This was the
case, as measured by the frequency of cognitive distortions.
114
Comorbid diagnoses across axes may lead to a pattern in which cognitive distortion can
constrict behavior in a fashion that may actually exacerbate existing difficulties, leading to
vicious circles of cognitive distortion, distress, and interpersonal impairment. When this occurs,
one’s cognition may be further impaired by the resulting Axis I clinical syndromes. Prolonged
Axis I symptoms can, in turn, exacerbate personality dysfunction (Millon et al., 1996; Millon &
Davis, 1996).
Because of this reciprocal interaction between Axis I and Axis II, it was expected that
individuals with greater total pathology would experience more frequent cognitive distortions,
independent of whether or not this pathology arose from Axis I or Axis II. This explains why
Millon & Davis, (1997) posit the idea that the presence of Axis I pathology supports the presence
of personality pathology. Our findings supported this theory.
The Relationship between Cognitive Distortions and Individual Disorders
A series of Point-Biserial Correlations confirmed the fact that the hypothesized positive
correlation between most of the MCMI-III individual clinical diagnostic scores and total ICD
scores was significant (see Table 4). According to the cognitive model, cognitive distortions
were expected to correlate positively with psychological disorders as assessed by the MCMI-III
both on Axis I or Axis II. This was the case for most Axis I and Axis II disorders. The major
findings of the present study were as follows: First: the results lend strong support for the
cognitive model. Regardless of whether or not disorders occur on Axis I or Axis II, the severity
of the disturbance was significantly attributable to the frequency of cognitive distortions. As the
frequency of cognitive distortions increased, so too did the severity of disturbance. Second, as
115
the frequency of cognitive distortions increased, so too did the number of disorders for which an
individual met criteria. In short, the more disturbed the patients were, the more frequently they
engaged in distorted thinking. This occurred regardless of the specific diagnosis.
However, a number of exceptions were discovered. First, although correlated in a positive
direction, both bipolar disorder (r = .104, p < .109, one-tailed) and drug dependence (r = .063, p
<, 228, one-tailed) failed to reach statistical significance. Lack of correlation between cognitive
distortions and these particular two Axis I syndromes may be partially explained by the
empirically demonstrated environmental, physiological, and genetic components attendant to
these syndromes. In the case of bipolar disorder, for example, this may explain why CT alone
has been shown as insufficient in ameliorating bipolar disorder and is, rather, recommended as
an adjunct to, but never a substitute for pharmacotherapy (e.g., Scott, 2001; Tsai, Chen, Kuo,
Lee, Lee, & Strakowski, 2001). If CT ameliorates cognitive distortions in bipolar disorder, but
other factors account for a greater proportion of dysfunction and distress than such dysfunctional
thinking, it would be understandable that CT would not be a sufficient treatment for this
disorder.
Similarly, the lack of significant correlations between cognitive distortions and drug
dependence may result from a significant contribution of po werful physiological and genetic
influences, as well as significant behavioral and interpersonal factors that impact substance
abusers (e.g., Higgins, Heil, Lussier, 2004; Robinson & Berridge, 2003).
Additionally, three Axis II disorders were significantly but negatively correlated with
frequency of cognitive distortion. Specifically, NPD (r = -.214, r2 = ...046, p < .05, one-tailed);
HPD (r = -.266, r2 = .071, p < .001, one-tailed); and OCPD (r =-.343. r2 = .118, p < .001).
116
However, a review of the data and the literature should help to explain the unexpected
negative correlations between reported frequency of cognitive distortions and the presence of
HPD, OCPD, and NPD. Although these results were surprising, they were not without precedent
or explanation.
First, participants meeting the operational definition for the three PD diagnoses, HPD,
OCPD, and NPD reported being significantly less likely to engage cognitive distortion; these
were the only participants whose MCMI Validity Scales were questionable on one very
important item—disclosure. In fact, for these individuals, the MCMI-III Modifying Index X or
Disclosure Scale was as follows: HPD (r = -.252. p <. 002, two-tailed), OCPD (-.284, p < .001,
two-tailed), and although not statistically significant NPD (r = -.088, p < ...277, two-tailed).
The Disclosure Scale was designed to detect the extent to which people are honest and
revealing, with low scores (below 34) indicating possible withholding of information (Millon,
1987). The tendency to withhold appeared to be represented in the negative ICD correlations.
Although the Disclosure Scale was originally designed to be neutral as to simulating
psychopatholgy (Millon, 1987), it has demonstrated the ability to detect both faking good and
faking bad and is generally highly intercorrelated with the other two MCMI-III validity scales
(Bagby, Gillis, Toner, & Goldberg, 1991).
Significantly, in a recent study, Shoenberg, Dorr, Morgan and Burke (2004) demonstrated
remarkably similar findings, because three MMPI-2 validity scales Infrequency (F), Back-Page
Infrequency (Fb), and Dissimulation (F-K) were highly correlated with the MCMI-III Disclosure
scale. Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation between these three MMPI-2
validity scales and the same three PD scales that correlated negatively with the MCMI-III
Disclosure scale: NPD, HPD, and OCPD. Thus, the present study replicated the findings that
117
individuals meeting criteria for NPD, HPD, and OCPD diagnoses disclose less information that
is likely to be interpreted as dissimulation and, in the present case, report less cognitive
distortions than individuals meeting criteria for other Axis I and Axis II conditions.
Additionally, the Disclosure Scale is a Modifying index, which allows for adjustment,
increasing the probability of still attaining an accurate diagnosis. Thus, participants may have
disclosed a sufficient amount to be properly diagnosed on the three PDs in question, yet their
tendency to withhold may have been reflected in their ICD responses (Millon et al., 1997). In
other words, with sufficient disclosure, scores of participants who met MCMI-III criteria for
NPD, HPD and OCPD may have been more positively correlated with the Total ICD.
This begs the question: Why would individuals with these three PDs be less likely to
disclose? In the case of NPD, grandiose self-importance, hypersensitivity to evaluation and need
for admiration might understandably make it difficult to disclose or even recognize certain
symptoms (APA, 2000). This failure to disclose could simultaneously reduce the MCMI-III
Disclosure Scale and decrease these patients’ tendencies to divulge cognitive distortions.
Consequently, as the NPD score is compensated by the Disclosure Scale Modifying index, and
the ICD score is depressed by lack of disclosure, a negative correlation may be produced
between NPD scores and Total ICD scores.
It has been demonstrated in the case of HPD that individuals meeting criteria for HPD are
exquisitely concerned with self-presentation, experience higher autonomic nervous system
arousal, and are easily influenced by other people and circumstances. This may make these
individuals less likely to disclose their histrionic symptoms because doiong so would be
incongruent with their constant impression management—both public and private—or,
alternately, they may not even be conscious of the information that they should disclose due to
118
their exteroceptive focus (APA, 2000; Beck et al., 2004; Gorenstein & Newman, 1980;
Hamburger, Lilienfeld, & Hogben, 1996).
Finally, why would individuals scoring high on the MCMI-III Compulsive Scale disclose
less than other PDs? The answer is likely to be the perfectionism, indecision, and rigidity that
may make them unaware of or unwilling to admit to internal thoughts and emotions (Beck et al.,
2004) that may be detected by the MCMI-III Disclosure Scale. Also, Barber and Muenz (1996)
found that patients with comorbid OCPD and depression responded significantly better when
treated with interpersonal therapy rather than CT; this could indicate that there may be some
mechanism other than cognitive distortions, such as interpersonal or behavioral components that
account for a large part of the variance in this disorder. However, Barber and Muenz
hypothesized that the interpersonal therapy differentially addressed perfectionistic, internal
coping strategies characteristic of OCPD, whereas CT, as operationalized in their study, was
more effective in ameliorating the externalized coping strategies.
Of course there are other possible explanations for the negative correlations; one of these is
that HPD, OCPD, and NPD patients actually engage in less cognitive distortions than other
clinical patients. Another alternative account is that the Disclosure Scale actually invalidated the
MCMI-III profiles and that this data should be discarded. However, this would not explain why
only one of three highly intercorrelated MCMI-III validity scales would correlate negatively with
only these three PDs. Similar results were also obtained by Schoenberg et al. (2004), who
showed that MCMI-III profiles were indeed valid on both the MCMI-III and MMPI-2 Validity
Scales. Thus, the characteristics of HPD, OCPD, and NPD likely account for the fact that these
three PDs correlated negatively with Total ICD scores.
119
In support of this conclusion, T. Millon (personal communication, September 20, 2004)
posited the concept that individuals scoring higher on the three PD subscales in question, NPD,
OCPD, and HPD, “tended to be closer to normal on the continuum of personality style. These
individuals may or may not be pathological and this may reflect less cognitive dysfunction. They
also have a tendency not to communicate as much as others regarding their problems and this is
reflected in the negative correlation on the Dsclosure Scale” with the Total ICD scores. “It is not
surprising that they (NPD, OCPD, and HPD patients) would engage in less cognitive
dysfunction” than other PD types.
In summary, the study provided further evidence for the cognitive model; that is, that
cognition, specifically cognitive distortion, significantly predicted the severity of dysfunction,
including the number of disorders on both diagnostic axes. Additionally, there is evidence of an
additive effect, with maladaptive thinking intensifying as severity and the number of diagnoses
increases. This may explain why it is more difficult for clinicians to treat individuals with more
severe and/or comorbid conditions.
Additionally, the study provided good psychometric support for the ICD in the form of
concurrent validity with the MCMI-III. Conversely, the study demonstrated that the MCMI-III is
sensitive to differences in cognition. As a consequence, the clinical value of the ICD and the
MCMI-III, when used as tools for assessment and treatment of psychological disorders should
not be underestimated.
The ICD is an efficient, practical instrument that can provide clinicians with important data,
accounting for about half of the variance in symptomatology for both clinical syndromes and
PDs. Individual ICD items can also inform the clinician about which specific maladaptive
120
thoughts and cognitive distortions to target as treatment progresses. Similarly, the MCMI-III has
proven, once again, to be a valid assessment instrument for clinical research.
Limitations of the Study
The ICD appears to offer promise as an instrument, providing a cohesive and efficient
means to assess cognitive distortions relevant to both Axis I and Axis II disorders. However,
additional examination of the psychometric properties of this new measure is urged. Areas
requiring additional investigation for the ICD might include additional test–retest reliability and
convergent validity, comparing the ICD to other existing cognitive distortion scales; it might also
involve larger sample sizes of participants with particular disorders to study the relative
influence of individual cognitive distortions on those specific disorders (Schoenberg et al.,
2004).
Another potential limitation of the study is the fact that severity was operationally
defined with a novel formula, the cumulative score of all MCMI-III scores of 75 or above.
However, this formula was consistent with Millon’s (et al., 1996) theory and his personal
communication of October 5, 2004.
The MCMI-III is one of the most widely accepted measures of psychopathology and
perhaps the most widely used measure of personality; it was, in fact, designed for diagnostic
screening and assessment of clinical populations in clinical research and practice. However,
others have reported its limitations. These include questions regarding reductions in construct
and convergent validity from the earlier versions of the MCMI, as well as discriminant
correlations that were higher than convergent validities on a number of scales. This means that
121
some scales correlated more highly with measures of other disorders than the ones they were
intended to assess (Rogers, Salekin, & Sewell, 1999). These findings and others have led Rogers
et al. to claim that the MCI, MCMI-II, and the MCMI-III are really separate measures requiring
further validation.
Finally, the study employed two self-report instruments: the ICD and the MCMI-III.
There are a number of potential limitations attendant on all such instruments, including
idiosyncratic differences in the way participants interpret individual items, the effect of
participants’ current affective state on responses, the reactive nature of such instruments, and the
influences of social desirability and self-presentation (Kazdin, 1998). Additionally, the use of
forced choices may limit and distort responses (Birelson, Hudson, Buchanan, & Wolff, 1987) .
Limitations of all psychometric instruments argue for a multitrait, multimethod approach to
assessment (Kenny & Kashy, 1992).
Recommendations for Future Research
In future research, it would be beneficial to explore whether or not particular distortions
on the ICD are more relevant to specific disorders. Longitudinal research could also investigate
whether or not there is an immediate relationship between symptom remission and the abatement
of cognitive distortions or whether or not there is some lag time on either factor. It would also be
interesting to determine whether or not the negative correlations between HPD, OCPD, and NPD
were replicable in a larger sample and with other measures of cognitive distortion.
Furthermore, Beck et al. (2001) determined that specific dysfunctional beliefs attendant
on PDs (Beck & Freeman, 1990), as measured by the Personal Beliefs Questionnaire (Beck &
122
Beck, 1991), could predict specific PDs. It is recommended that the data collection begun in the
current study be extended to obtain a sufficiently large sample; this would demonstrate whether
or not individuals with specific disorders have a greater tendency to engage in particular
cognitive distortions. Moreover, efforts should be made to replicate Yurica’s (2002) impressive
test-retest findings to ensure that extraneous events, such as the terrorist attacks of September 11,
2001, did not unduly influence the ICD’s reliability results (Yurica, 2002).
Finally, while the ICD may explain up to half of the variance in psychological disorders,
other factors must also contribute to psychological dysfunction. These factors may include
dynamics related to an interaction of influences, be they biological, social, genetic, neurological,
genetic, physiological, or aspects of cognition and behavior other than cognitive distortion.
These factors should be the subject of future research.
Conclusions
Despite the above limitations, there is proof that the ICD has once again provided a valid
and reliable measure of cognitive distortion underlying a wide range of psychological disorders
across both Axis I and Axis II. The current study provided further support for the psychometric
properties of the ICD and validated its application beyond the realm of Axis I and into PDs in a
heterogeneous, adult, outpatient sample. The ICD also appears to be valid in older populations
because the present sample ranged in age from 18 to 86 years. The ICD appears to be a valuable,
psychometrically sound instrument for the evaluation and treatment of clinical syndromes as well
as PDs by illuminating treatable cognitive distortions that correlate so highly with dysfunction. .
123
This study has also demonstrated that reduced frequency of cognitive distortions correlates
with diminution in the severity and number of diagnosable disorders. Consequently, to the extent
that clinicians successfully attend to and reduce this variety of dysfunctional thinking, they
should see a commensurate reduction in pathology because the present study has demonstrated
the fact that the frequency of cognitive distortions accounts for up to 50% of the variance in the
number and severity of many psychological disorders on both diagnostic axes. Thus, using a
tool, such as the ICD, that illuminates these troubling tendencies may greatly aid in establishing
crucial baseline data, advance the evaluation of treatment response, and thereby improve
treatment outcome.
124
References
Abramowitz, J. S. (1997). Effectiveness of psychological and pharmacological treatments
for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a quantitative review. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 44-52.
Agras, W. S., Rottister, E. M., Arnow, B., Schneider, J. H., Telch, C. S., Raeburn, S. D., et al.
(1992). Pharmacologic and cognitive-behavioral treatment for bulimia nervosa: A controlled
comparison. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 82-87.
Allen, J. G., Console, D. A., Brethour, J. R. (2000). Screening for trauma-related sleep
disturbance in Women Admitted for Specialized Inpatient Treatment. Journal of Trauma &
Dissociation. 1, 59-83.
Allen, J., Coyne, L. & Huntoon, J. (1998). Complex posttraumatic stress disorder in women from
a psychometric perspective. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70(2), 277-298.
Alford, B.A., & Beck, A.T. (1997). The integrative power of cognitive therapy. New York:
Guilford.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In
Alloy L. B. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford
Press.
Alnaes, R. & Torgensen, S. (1989). Personality and personality disorders among patients with
major depression in combination with dysthymic or cyclothymic disorders. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 79(4), 363-369.
Alnaes, R. & Torgersen, S. (1997). Personality and personality disorders predict
development and relapses of major depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 95, 336-342.
Retrieved November 25, 2002, from OVID Database.
125
American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (Text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychological Association, (2004) Testimony of the American Psychological
Association Submitted for the record House Appropriations Subcommittee on Labor,
Health and Human Services and Education regarding funding for fiscal year 2004.
Andersson, T. 1994. Conceptual Polemics - Dialectic Studies of Concept Formation. Ph.D.
thesis, Cognitive Science Dept. Lund: Lund University.
Andrews, B. & Brewin, C.R. (1990). Attributions of blame for marital violence: A study of
antecedents and consequences. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 757-767.
Auerbach, J. (1984). Validation of two scales for narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 48(6), 649-653.
Bagby, R. R., Gillis, J. R., Toner, B. B., Goldberg. J. (1991). Detecting fake-good and fake-bad
responding on the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-II. Psychological Assessment
3, 496-498. Retrieved December 5, 2003, from PsycARTICLES Database.
Barber, J. P., & Muenz, L. R. (1996). The role of avoidance and obsessiveness in matching
patients to cognitive and interpersonal psychotherapy: Empirical findings from the treatment
for depression collaborative research program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 64, 951—958. Retrieved December 5, 2003, from PsycARTICLES Database.
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R. & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic
attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 893-912.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. New
126
York: Cambridge University Press.
Barratt, E. S. (1983). The biological basis of impulsiveness: the significance of timing and
rhythm disorders. Personality and Individual Differences 4, 387-391.
Barriga, A. Q., Landau, J. R., Stinson, B. L., & Liau, A. K., & Gibbs, J. C. (2000). Cognitive
disortion and problem behaviors in adolescents. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27 (1), 3656.
Baumeister, R. F. Catanese, K, R., & Wallace, H. M. (2002). Conquest by force. A
narcissistic reactance theory of rape and sexual coercion. Review of General Psychology, 6,
92-135. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO Database.
Baumeister, R. F., & Jones, E. E. (1978). When self-presentation is constrained by the target's
knowledge: Consistency and compensation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36, 608-618. Retrieved December 3, 2003, from PsycARTICLES Database.
Bayon, C., Hill, K., Svrakic, D., Przybeck, T. & Cloninger, C. (1996). Dimensional assessment
of personality in an out-patient sample: relations of the systems of Millon and Cloninger.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30(5), 341-352.
Beck, A. T. (1961). A systematic investigation of depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
2, 163-170.
Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression: Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions,
Psychological Bulletin, 91,3-26.
Beck, A. T. (1964). Thinking and depression: II. Theory and therapy. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 10, 561-571.
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New
York: Harper & Row.
127
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York:
International Universities Press.
Beck, A. T. (1991). Cognitive therapy: A 30-year retrospective. American Psychologist, 46, 368375. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO Database.
Beck, A. T. (1993). Cognitive therapy: Past, present, and future. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 61, 194-198. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO Databases.
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology.
In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 1—25). New York:
Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T. (2000). Member's Corner: Cognitive approaches to schizophrenia: A
paradigm shift? Based on the 1999 Joseh Zubin award address. Psychopathology
Research, 10(2), 3-10.
Beck, A. T., & Beck, J. S. (1991). The Personality Belief Questionnaire. Unpublished
assessment instrument. The Beck Institute for Cognitive Therapy and Research, Bala
Cynwyd, Pennsylvania.
Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Berchick, R. J., Stewart, B. & Steer, R. A. (1990). Relationship
between hopelessness and ultimate suicide: A replication with psychiatric outpatients.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 190-195.
Beck Institute (2004). Meta-Analysis Shows the Effectiveness of Cognitive Therapy. Retrieved
August 35, 2004 from http://www.beckinstitute.org/beck.html
Beck, J. (1996). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford.
128
Beck, A. T., Butler, A. C., Brown, G. K., Dahlsgaard, K. K., Newman C. F., Beck, J. S. (2001).
Dysfunctional beliefs discriminate personality disorders. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 39, 1213-1225.
Beck, A. T.,Emery, G. W. & Greenberg, R. I (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias. New
York: Basic Books.
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G. & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring
clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 56, 893—897.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A. and Associates (1990). Cognitive Therapy of Personality
Disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Beck, A.T., Freeman, A., Davis, D. D., & Associates (2004). Cognitive Therapy of Personality
Disorders (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F. & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of
depression. New York: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T. & Steer, R. A. 1990. Beck Anxiety Inventory: Manual. The Psychological
Corporation. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc, San Antonio.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory
for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Beck, A. T. & Weishaar, M. E. (1995). Cognitive therapy. In R. J. Corsini & D.
Weddings (Eds.), Current psychotherapies (pp. 229—261). Itasca, IL: F. E.
Peacock.
129
Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F. & Liesse, B. S. (1993). Cognitive therapy of
substance abuse. New York: Guilford Press.
Beckham, E. E., Leber, W. R., Watkins, J. T., Boyer, J. L. & Cook, J. B. (1986).
Development of an instrument to measure Beck's cognitive triad: The Cognitive
Triad Inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 566-567.
Belar, C.D. & Perry, N.W. (1992). The national conference on scientistpractitioner education and training for the professional practice of
psychology. American Psychologist, 47, 71-75.
Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1997). Use of personality assessment instruments in empirically
guided treatment planning. Psychological Assessment, 9, 361-367. Retrieved January
16, 2003, from PsychINFO database.
Bender, D., Farber, B.A. & Geller, J.D. (2001). Cluster B personality traits and attachment.
Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 29(4), 551-563.
Birelson, P., Hudson, I., Buchanan, D.G., & Wolff, S. (1987). Clinical evaluation of self-rating
scale for depressive disorder in childhood. (Depression self-rating scale). Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 43-60.
Black, D. W., Bell, S., Hulbert, J. & Nasrallah, A. (1988). The importance of Axis II in
patients with major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 14, 115-122.
Blackburn, R. (1998). Relationship of personality disorders to observer ratings of interpersonal
style in forensic psychiatric patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(1), 77-85.
Blackburn, I. M., Eunson, K. M. & Bishop, S. (1986). A two-year naturalistic follow-up
of depressed patients treated with cognitive therapy, pharmacotherapy, and a
combination of both. Journal of Affective Disorders, 10, 67-75. January
130
10, 2003, from PsychINFO database.
Boyle, G. & Loick, D. (2000). Discriminant validity of the illness behavior questionnaire
and Millon clinical multiaxial inventory-Ill in a heterogeneous sample of psychiatric
outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 79-791 Abstract retrieved December 6,
2002, from EBSCO database.
Brewin, C. R., (1996). Theoretical foundations of cognitive-behavior therapy for anxiety and
depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 33-57.
Briere, J. (2001).The Cognitive Distortion Scale. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources, Inc.
Briere, J., Runtz, M., Giancola, P., Mezzich, A., Clark, D., & Tarter, R. (1993). Cognitive
distortions, aggressive behavior, and drug use in adolescent boys with and without a
family history of a substance use disorder. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8(3), 312–
330.
Brook, J., Whiteman, M., Finch, S. & Cohen, P. (1996). Young adult drug use and
delinquency: Childhood antecedents and adolescent mediators. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1584-1592.
Brown, G. K., Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Grisham, J. R. (2000). Risk factors for suicide in
psychiatric outpatients: A 20-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology. 68, 371-377. Retrieved December 28, 2002, from PsychINFO
database.
Bruns, D. (1998, October). Psychologists as primary care providers: A paradigm shift.
APA [Electronic version]. Health Psychology Division Newsletter. Retrieved June 8,
2002, from http://www. healthpsych.com/primarycare1.html.
131
Budman, S., & Gurman, A. (1988). Theory and practice of brief therapy. New York: The
Guilford Press.
Burns, D. D. (1980). Feeling good: The new mood therapy. New York: Signet.
Burns, D. D. (1990). The feeling good handbook. New York: William Morrow.
Burns, D. D. (1999). Feeling good: The new mood therapy. Avon Books, New York.
Butcher, J.N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989).
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI-2). Manual for administration
and scoring. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Butler, A. C., & Beck, J. S. (2000). Cognitive therapy outcomes: A review of metaanalyses. Journal of the Norwegian Psychological Association, 37, 1-9.
Butler, G., Fennell, M., Robson, P. & Gelder, M. (1991). Comparison of behavior therapy and
cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 167-175.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.), (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York:
Guilford Press.
Chambless, D., Caputo, G., Bright, P. & Gallagher, R. (1984). Assessment of fear in
agoraphobics: The Body Sensations Questionnaire and the Agoraphobia Cognitions
Questionnaire. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1090-1097.
Chambless, D. & Gillis, M. (1993). Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety Disorders. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 248-260.
Chambless, D. L. & Olendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported psychological interventions:
Controversies and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685-716.
132
Chick, D., Sheaffer, C., Goggin, W. & Sison, G. (1993). The relationship between MCMI
personality scales and clinician- generated DSM-III-R personality disorder diagnoses.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 61(2), 264-276.
Chiu, C. Y., Krauss, R. M. & Lau, I. Y. M. (2003). Some cognitive consequences of
communication." In S.R. Fussell & R.J. Kreuz (eds.), Social and Cognitive
Psychological Approaches to Interpersonal Communication. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum
Associates.
Choca, J. P., & VanDenburg, E. (1997). Interpretive guide to the Millon Clinical Multiaxial
Inventory (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Clark, D. M. (1991, September). Cognitive therapy for panic disorder. (Paper presented
at the National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference on the
Treatment of Panic Disorder). Bethesda, MD.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of
temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990.
Clum, G. A., Boyles, S. E., Borden, J. W. & Watkins, P. L. (1990). Validity and
reliability of the Panic Attack Symptoms and Cognitions Questionnaires. Journal
of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 233-245.
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans
reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187-276.
Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice:
The NEO personality inventory. Psychological Assessments, 4, 20—22 5-13. Retrieved
January 16, 2003, from PsychINFO databases.
Craig, R. J. (Ed.). (1993). The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: A clinical and
133
research information synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Craig, R. J. (1997). A selected review of the MCMI empirical literature. In T. Millon (Ed.), The
Millon inventories: Clinical and personality assessment. (pp. 303—326). New York:
Guilford.
Craig, R. J. (1997). Sensitivity of MCMI-III Scales T (drugs) and B (alcohol) in detecting
substance abuse. Substance Use and Misuse, 32(10), 1385-1393.
Craig, R.J. & Olson, R. (1998). Stability of the MCMI-III in a substance-abusing inpatient
sample. Psychological Reports, 83(3), 1273-1274.
Craig, R. J. & Weinberg, D. (1993). MCMI: Review of the literature. (In R. J. Craig
(Ed.), The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: A clinical research information synthesis
(pp. 23—70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Craighead, W. E., Craighead, L. W., & Ilardi, S. S., (1998). Psychosocial treatments for major
depressive disorder. In P. E. Nathan & J. M. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work
(pp. 226-239). New York: Oxford University Press.
Dagan, Y., Sela, H., Omer, H., Hallis, D., & Dar, R. (1996). High prevalence of personality
disorders among circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSD) patients. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 41(4), 357-363.
Davis, S.E. & Hays, L.W. (1997). An examination of the clinical validity of the MCMI-III
Depressive Personality scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53(1), 15-23.
Deblinger, S., Steer, R.A., Lippmann, J. Two-year follow-up study of cognitive behavioral
therapy for sexually abused children suffering post-traumatic stress symptoms. Child
Abuseand Neglect, 23, 1371-1378.
134
de Groot, M.H., Franken, I.H.A., van der Meer, C.W. & Hendriks, V.M. (2003). Stability and
change in dimensional ratings of personality disorders in drug abuse patients during
treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 24(2), 115-120.
Del Rosario, P.M., McCann, J.T. & Navarra, J.W. (1994). The MCMI-II diagnosis of
schizohrenia: operating characteristics and profile analysis. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 63(3), 438-452.
DeRubeis, R. J., Crits-Christoph P. (1998) Empirically supported individual and group
psychological treatments for adult mental disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 66:37-52.
DeRubeis, R. J., Evans, M. D., Hollon, S. D., Garvey, M. J., Grove, W. M., & Tuason, V. B.
(1990). How does cognitive therapy work? Cognitive change and symptom change in
cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 58, 862-869. Retrieved December 3, 2002 from PsycINFO
database.
Devanand, DP. (2002). Comorbid psychiatric disorders in late life depression. Biological
Psychiatry, 52(3), 236-42. Retrieved January 26, 2003, from Science Direct
database.
DiGiuseppe, R., Leaf, R., Exner, T. & Robin, M. W. (1988, September). The development
of a measure of irrational/rational thinking. (Paper presented at the World Congress
on Behavior Therapy, Edinburgh, Scotland).
Diguer, L., Barber, J. P. & Luborsky, L. (1993). Three concomitants: Personality disorders,
psychiatric severity and outcome of dynamic psychotherapy of major depression. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1246-1248. Retrieved December 23, 2002 from PsycINFO
135
database.
Dobson, K.S. (1989). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of cognitive therapy for depression.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 3, 414-419.
Dobson, K. S., & Khatri, N. (2000). Cognitive therapy: Looking backward, looking forward.
Journal of Clinical Psychology. Special Issue: Advances in clinical psychology, 56, 907923. Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child
Development, 51, 162—170.
Dodge, K. & Newman, J. (1981). Biased decision-making processes in aggressive boys.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 375-379.
Dodge, K., Price, J., Bachorowski, J., & Newman, J. (1990). Hostile attributional biases
in severely aggressive adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 385-392.
Driscoll, R. (1988). Self-condemnation: A conceptual framework for assessment and treatment.
Psychotherapy, 26, 104-111.
Duthie, B. & Vincent, K. R. (1986). Diagnostic hit rates for high point codes for the
Dianostic Inventory of Personality and Symptoms using random assignment, base
rates, and probability scales. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 612-614.
Dyce, J.A., O'Connor, B.P., Parkins, S.Y. & Janzen, H.L. (1997). Correlational structure of the
MCMI-III personality disorder scales and comparisons with other data sets. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 69(3), 568-582.
D'Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1990). Development and preliminary evaluation of the Social
Problem-solving Inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 2, 156-163.
136
Eckhardt, C. I., Barbour, K. A., Davison, G. C. (1998). Articulated thoughts of maritally violent
and nonviolent men during anger arousal. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology,
66, 259-269.
Elkin, I., Shea, M. T., Watkins, J. T., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, S. M., Collins, J. F., eta al. (1989).
National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research
Program:
General effectiveness of treatments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 971-982.
Ellis, A. (1958). Rational psychotherapy. Journal of General Psychology. 59, 35-49. Reprinted:
New York: Institute for Rational-Emotive Therapy.
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emtion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart.
Ellis, A. (1973). Humanistic psychotherapy: The rational-emotive approach. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Ellis, A., & Grieger, R. (1977). Handbook of rational-emotive therapy. New York:
Springer Publishers.
Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C. & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes:
Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,
328-339.
Erikson, E.H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.), New York: Norton.
Espelage, D.L., Mazzeo, S.E., Sherman, R. & Thompson, R. (2002). MCMI-II profiles of women
with eating disorders: A cluster analytic investigation. Journal of Personality Disorders,
16(5), 453-463.
Evans, M. D., Hollon, S. D., DeRubeis, R. J., Piasecki, J. M., Grove, W. M., Garvey, M.
J. et al. (1992). Differential relapse following cognitive therapy and
pharmacotherapy for depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 802-808.
137
Retrieved December 23, 2002 from PsychINFO database.
Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, S. B. J. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality
Questionnaire. Hodder & Staughton: Sevenoaks, Kent.
Fairburn, C., Jones, R., Peveler, R., Carr, S., Solomon, R., O'Connor, M. et al. (1991). Three
psychological treatments for bulimia nervosa: A comparative
trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 463-469.
Faravelli C, Albanesi G. 1987. Agoraphobia with panic attacks: 1-year prospective follow-up.
Comprehensive Psychiatry 28, 481-487.
Farmer, R. & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1990). Personality disorders and depression.
Hypothetical relations, empirical findings, and methodological considerations.
Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 453-476.
Fava, G. A., Grandi, S., Zielezny, M., Rafanelli, C. & Canestrari, R. (1996). Four-year
outcome for cognitive behavioral treatment of residual symptoms in major
depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 945-947. Retrieved December 23,
2002 from PsychINFO database.
Fava, G. A., Rafanelli, C., Grandi, S., Conti, S. & Belluardo, P. (1998). Prevention of
recurrent depression with cognitive-behavioral therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry,
55, 816-820.
Faravelli C, Albanesi G. (1987). Agoraphobia with panic attacks: 1-year prospective followup.
Comprehensive Psychiatry;28, 481-487.
Feske, U. and Chambless, D. L. (1995). Cognitive behavioral versus exposure only treatment for
social phobia: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 26, 695-720.
138
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human relations. 7,
114-140.
Frances, A. J. (1980). The DSM—III personality disorders section: A commentary.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1050-1054.
Freeman, A. & DeWolf, R. (1990). Woulda, coulda, shoulda. New York: William Morrow.
Freeman, A. & DeWolf, R. (1992). The 10 Dumbest mistakes smart people make and
how to avoid them. New York: HarperCollins.
Freeman A., & Fusco, G. M. (2003). Borderline Personality Disorder: A Therapist's Guide to
Taking Control. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co.
Freeman, A.M., III, Kablinger, A.S., Rolland, P.D. & Brannon, G.E. (1999). Millon multiaxial
personality patterns differentiate depressed and anxious outpatients. Depression and
Anxiety, 10(2), 73-76.
Freeman, A. & Oster, C. (1999). Cognitive behavior thereapy. In M. Herson & A. S.
Bellack (eds.). Handbook of interventions for adult disorders, 2nd edition, (pp. 108138). New York: Wiley and sons.
Freeman, A. & Rosenfield, B. (2002). Modifying therapeutic homework for patients with
personality disorders. In Session: Psychotherapy in Practice, 58, 513-524.
Freud, S. (1899, 1962). Screen memories. In J. Strachey (Ed.) The Standard Edition of the
Complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 3. London: The Hogarth Press,
1962.
Giancola, P., Mezzich, A., Clark, D. & Tarter, R. (1999). Cognitive distortions,
aggressive behavior, and drug use in adolescent boys with and without a family
history of a substance use disorder. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 22-32.
139
Retrieved January 6, 2003, from PsychINFO database.
Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1989). Effects of upward and downward social comparison on
mood states. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 14-31.
Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1991). Downward comparison and coping with threat. In J.
Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social Comparison Processes: Contemporary theory and
research. (pp. 317 - 345). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Gilbert, P. 1997 . T he e volution o f s ocial a ttractiveness a nd its r ole in shame, hu miliation, gu ilt
and therapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology 70, 113-147.
Gilbert, P . (1998). The e volved basis a nd adaptive functions for c ognitive d istortions. British
journal of Medical Psychology,12, 447-263.
Gilson, M., & Freeman, A. (1999). A cognitive therapy approach for taming the
depression beast: Client workbook. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
Gorenstein, E. E., & Newman, J. P. (1980). Disinhibitory psychopathology: A new perspective
and a model for research. Psychological Review, 87, 301-315.
Gould, R. A., Buckminster, S., Pollack, M. H., Otto, M.W., & Yap, L. (1997). Cognitivebehavioral and pharmacological treatment for social phobia: A meta-analysis. Clinical
Psychology – Science & Practice, 4(4), 291-306.
Gould, R. A., Otto, M. W., & Pollack, M. H. (1995). A meta-analysis of treatment outcome for
panic disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 15(8), 819-844.
Gould, R. A., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., & Yap, L. (1997). Cognitive behavioral and
pharmacological treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: A preliminary meta-analysis.
Behavior Therapy, 28, 285-305.
140
Grossman, L.S. & Craig, R.J. (1995). Comparison of MCMI-II and 16PF validity scales. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 64(2), 384-389.
Grove, W. M. & Meehl, P. E. (1996). Comparative efficiency of informal (subjective,
impressionistic) and formal (mechanical, algorithmic) prediction procedures: The
clinical—statistical controversy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2, 293-323.
Groth-Marnat, G. (1996). Handbook of psychological assessment. New York: Wiley & Sons.
Groth-Marnat, G. & Edkins, G. (1996). Professional psychologists in general health care
settings a review of the financial efficacy of direct treatment interventions.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 27, 161-174. Retrieved June 6, 2002, from
PsycARTICLES database.
Haaga, D. A. F., Dyck, M. J. & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory of
depression. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 215-236.
Hamburger, M. E., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Hogben, M. (1996). Psychopathy, gender, and gender
roles: Implications for antisocial and histrionic personality disorders. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 10, 41-55.
Hardy, G. E., Barkham, M., Shapiro, D. A., Stiles, W. B., Rees, A. & Reynolds, S.
(1995). Impact of Cluster C personality disorders on outcomes of contrasting brief
psychotherapies for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63,
997-1004. Retrieved December 22, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.
Hartman, L. M. (1984). Cognitive components of anxiety. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 40, 137-139.
Hathaway, S. R. & McKinley, J. C. A. (1943). MMPI Manual. Psychological
Corporation: New York.
141
Hawley, K. M., Weisz, J. R., (2002). Increasing the relevance of evidence-based treatment:
Review to practitioners and consumers. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 9(2),
225-230. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from Proquest database.
Head, H., Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34,
102-254.
Heath, A. C., Cloninger, C. R. & Martin, N. G. (1994). Testing a model for the genetic structure
of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66, 762-775.
Higgins, S. T., Heil, S. H., & Lussier, J. P. (2004). Clinical implications of reinforcement as a
determinant of substance use disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 431-461.
Hirschfeld, R. M. A. & Shea, M. T. (1992). Personality. In E. S. Paykel (Ed.), Handbook
of affective disorders (2nd ed., pp. 185—194). New York: Guilford Press.
Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of
an automatic thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 4, 383-395.
Hollon, S. D., Shelton, R. C., & Davis, D. D. (1993). Cognitive therapy for depression:
Conceptual issues and clinical efficacy. Journal of Consulting and Clininical Psychology.
61, 270-75.
Holt, S.E., Meloy, J.R. & Strack, S. (1999). Sadism and psychopathy in violent and sexually
violent offenders. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 27(1),
23-32.
Hsu, L. M. (2002). Diagnostic validity statistics and the MCMI—III. Psychological Assessment,
14, 410-422.
Hyer, L., Braswell, L., Albrecht, B., Boyd, S., Boudewyns, P. & Talbert, S. (1994). Relationship
of NEO-PI to personality styles and severity of trauma in chronic PTSD victims. Journal
142
of Clinical Psychology, 50(5), 699-707.
Ilardi, S. S. & Craighead, W. E. (1999). The Relationship Between Personality Pathology and
Dysfunctional Cognitions in Previously Depressed Adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
108, 51-57. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from Proquest database.
Ilardi, S. S., Craighead, W., Edward, & Evans, D. D. (1997). Modeling relapse in
unipolar depression: The effects of dysfunctional cognitions and personality
disorders. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 65(3) 381-391. Retrieved January
3, 2003, from Proquest database.
Ingram, R. E., & Kendall, P. C., (1986). Cognitive clinical psychology: Implications of an
information processing perspective. in R. Ingram (Ed.), Information Processing
Approaches to Clinical Psychology, CA: Academic Press, Inc.
Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New
York: Guilford Press.
Joffe, R. T. & Regan, J. J. (1988). Personality and depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research
22, 279-286.
Joiner, T. E. & Rudd, M. D. (2002). The incremental validity of passive-aggressive personality
symptoms rivals or exceeds that of other personality symptoms in suicidal outpatients.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 79(1), 161-170.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of
representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.
Kaplan, R. M. (2000). Two pathways to prevention. American Psychologist, 55, 382-396.
Retrieved June 12, 2002, from PsycARTICLES database.
Kay, J. H., Altshuler, L. L.,Ventura, J., & Mintz J. (2002). Impact of axis II comorbidity on the
143
course of bipolar illness in men: a retrospective chart review. Bipolar Disorders. 4(4), 237242.
Kazdin, A. E. (1998). Research design in clinical psychology. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Keller, A. P., Lipkus, I, M., and Rimer, B. K. (2002). Mood, framing and health-related
persuasion. Paper presented at the Association for Consumer Research Conference,
Joan Meyers-Levy and Mary Gilly (Chairpersons).
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.
Kendall, P. C. (1985). Toward a cognitive behavioral model of shild psychotherapyand a
critique related to interventions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 357-372.
Kendall, P. C. (1992). Healthy thinking. Behavior Therapy, 23, 1-11.
Kendall, P. C., Howard, B., & Hays, R. (1989). Self-referent speech and psychopathology: The
balance of positive and negative thinking. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13, 583-598.
Kendall, P. C., Kortlander, E., Chansky, T. E. & Brady, E. U. (1992). Comorbidity of
anxiety and depression in youth: Treatment implications. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 60, 869—880. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsychINFO
database.
Kenny, D. A., & Kashy, D. A. (1992). Analysis of the multitrait-multimethod matrix by
confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 165-172.
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle K. A., Zhoa, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., & Eshleman S et
al.(1994). Lifetime, 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United
States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8-19.
Kiesler, D. J. (1986). The 1982 interpersonal circle: An analysis of DSM-III personality
disorders. In T. Millon & G. L. Klerman (Eds.), Contemporary directions in
144
psychopathology: Toward the DSM-IV (pp. 571-597). New York: Guilford.
Klerman, G. L., Weissman, M. M., Rounsaville, B. J., & Chevron, E. S. (1984). Interpersonal
psychotherapy of depression. New York: Basic Books.
Kolko, D. J., Brent. D. A., Baugher, M., Bridge, J., & Brimaher, B. (2000). Cognitive and family
therapies for adolescent depression treatment specificity, mediation, and moderation.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 603-614. Retrieved November 20, 2002,
from PsychINFO database.
Krebs, D. L. & Denton, K. (1997). Social illusions and self-deception: The evolution of biases in
person perception. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kendrick (Eds), Evolutionary Social
Psychology, pp. 21-47. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kuyken, W., Kurzer, N., DeRubeis, R. J., Beck, A.T., Brown, G. K. (2001). Response to
Cognitive therapy in depression: The role of maladaptive beliefs and personality
disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 560-566.
Langlois, J. H., & Musselman, L. (1995). The myths and mysteries of beauty. In D. R. Calhoun
(Ed.), 1996 Yearbook of Science and the Future (pp. 40-61). Chicago: Encyclopaedia
Britannica, Inc.
Layden, M. A., Newman, C. F., Freeman, A., & Morse, S. B. (1993). Cognitive Therapy of
Borderline Personality Disorder. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Lefebvre, M. F. (1981). Cognitive distortion and cognitive errors in depressed
psychiatric and low back pain patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 49, 517-525.
Leichsenring, F., & Leibing, E. (2003). The effectiveness of psychodynamic therapy and
145
cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of personality disorders: A meta-analysis.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(7), 1223-1232.
Libb, J.W., Stankovic, S., Sokol, R., Freeman, A., Houck, C. & Switzer, P. (1990). Stability of
the MCMI among depressed psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment,
55(1-2), 209-218.
Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives of Psychology, 140, 155.
Livesley, W. J. (ed.) (1995). The DSM-IV Personality Disorders. Guilford Press: New York.
Lochman, J. E. & Dodge, K. A. (1994). Social-cognitive processes of severely violent,
moderately aggressive, and nonaggressive boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 62, 366-374. Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsycINFO database.
Lynskey, M. & Fergusson, D. (1995). Childhood conduct problems, attention deficit
behaviors, and adolescent alcohol, tobacco, and illicit drug use. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 23, 281-302. Retrieved February 14, 2003, from PsycINFO database.
Looper, K. J. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2002). Behavioral Medicine Approaches to
Somatoform Disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 70. 810-827.
Retrieved January 3, 2003, from PsycINFO database.
MacDonald A. P. (1987). The Irrational Values Scale. In K. Corcoran & J. Fischer.
Measures for clinical practice: A sourcebook. (pp. 206-208). New York: Free press.
Malouff, J. M. & Schulte, N.S. (1986) In Sourcebook of adult assessment: Applied clinical
psychology. (pp. 432-435). New York: Plenum Press.
McCrae, R.R. (1991). The five-factor model and its assessment in clinical settings. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 57(3), 399-14.
146
McDaniel, S. (1995). Collaboration between psychologist and family physicians:
Implementing the biopsychosocial model. Professional Psychology: Research and
Practice, 26, 117-122. Retrieved June 6, 2002, from PsycARTICLES database.
MacDonald, A. P. (1972). In: K. Corcoran & J. Fischer (2000). Measures for clinical practice:
A sourcebook. 3rd Ed. New York: Free Press.
McGrath, E. P., Repettie, R. L., (2002). A longitudinal study of children's depressive symptoms,
self-perceptions, and cognitive distortions about the self. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology 111, 7-87. Retrieved July 10, 2004, from PsyhcARTICLES Database.
McKee, G.R. & Klohn, L.S. (1994). MCMI profiles of pretrial defendants. Psychological
Reports, 74(3), 77-87. Retrieved August 19, 2004, from PsyhcInfo Database.
McMullin, R.E. (2000). The new handbook of cognitive behavior therapy techniques. New
York: Norton.
Massion, A. O., Dyck, I. R.. Shea, M. T., Phillips, K. A., Warshaw, M. G., Keller, M. B. (2002).
Personality disorders and time to remission in generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia,
and panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 59(5), 434-40.
Meichenbaum, D. H. (1975). A self-instructional approach to stress management: A proposal for
stress inoculation training. In C. D. Spielberger & I. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety
(Vol. 2, pp. 237-264). New York: Wiley.
Meichenbaum, D. & Turk, D. C. (1987). Facilitating treatment adherence: A practitioner's
guidebook. New York: Plenum.
Meichenbaum, D.H. (1993). Stress inoculation training: A twenty year update. In R. L.
Woolfolk, & P. M. Lehrer (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed.)
(pp.373-
406. New York: Guilford.
147
Melartin T. K., Rytsala, H. J., Leskela, U. S., Lestela-Mielonen, P. S., Sokero, T. P., Isometsa, E.
T. (2002). Current Comorbidity of Psychiatric Disorders Among DSM-IV Major Depressive
Disorder Patients in Psychiatric Care in the Vantaa Depression Study. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 63.126-134.
Meyer, J. H., Kennedy, S. H., Korman, L., Brown, G. M., DaSilva, J. N., et al. (2003).
Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT (2) receptors during depression and self-harm. The
American Journal of Psychiatry. 160(1), 90-100.
Millon, T. (1987). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-II: Manual for the MCMI-II.
Minneapolis, MN, National Computer Systems,Inc.
Millon, T, (1990). Current comorbidity of psychiatric disorders Among DSM-IV Major
depressive disorder patients in psychiatric care in the vantaa depression study. Toward a
new personology. New York: Wiley.
Millon, T. (1994). Manual for the MCMI-III (2nd ed.) .Minneapolis, MN: National
Computer Systems.
Millon, T. (1999). Clinical Syndromes and Personality Disorders. Harvard Mental HealthLetter,
5, 4-7.
Millon, T. & Davis, R. D. (1996). Disorders of personality.DSM—IV and beyond (2nd
ed.). New York: Wiley.
Millon, T., Davis, R. & Millon, C. (1997). MCMI-III manual (2nd ed.). Minneapolis,
MN: National Computer Systems.
Millon, T., Davis, R. D., Millon, C. M., Wenger, A., Van Zuilen, M. H., Fuchs, M., Mulder, R.
T., & Joyce, P. R. (1994). The relationships of the Tridimensional Personality
148
Questionnaire to mood and personality measures in depressed patients. Psychological
Reports 75, 1315-1325.
Morgan, C., Shoenberg, M.R., Dorr, D. & Burke, M.J. (2002). Overreport on the MCMI-III:
Concurrent validation with the MMPI-2 using a psychiatric inpatient sample. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 78(2), 288-300.
Mulder, R. T.; Joyce, P. R.; Sullivan, P. F. (1999). The Relationship Among Three
Models of Personality Psychopathology: DSM-III-R Personality Disorder, TCI
Scores and DSQ Defenses. Psychological Medicine. 29, 943-951. Retrieved
December 3, 2002, from OVID Database.
Najavits, L. M. (1993). Cognitive Distortions Scale. Unpublished measure. Harvard Medical
School/McLean Hospital, Boston, MA
Najavits, L. M., Gotthardt, S., Weiss, R. D., Epstein, M. (2004). Cognitive distortions in the
dal dagnosis of PTSD and substance use disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
28(2), 159–172.
Narrow W. E. (1998). One-year prevalence of depressive disorders among adults 18 and over in
the U.S.: NIMH ECA prospective data. Population estimates based on U.S. Census
estimated residential population age 18 and over on July 1, 1998. Unpublished table.
Retrieved August 9, 2004, from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/numbers.cfm#5.
Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature, 1, 261-289.
Netmeyer, R. G., Williamson, D. A., Burton, S., Biswas, D. (2002). Psychometric properties of
shortened versions of the automatic thoughts questionnaire Educational and
Psychological Measuremen. 62, 111-130.
149
Newman, C. F., Leahy, R., Beck, A. T., Reilly-Harrington, N., & Gyulai, L. (2004). Bipolar
Disorder: A Cognitive Therapy Approach. Washington, D.C., American Psychiatric
Press.
Nigg, J. T. & Goldsmith, H. H. (1994). Genetics of Personality Disorders Perspectives From
Personality and Psychopathology Research. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 346-380.
Noyes, R., Reich, J., Christiansen, J., Suelzer, M., Pfohl, B., & Coryell, W.A. (1990). Outcome
of panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 809-818.
O'Connor, B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (2001). Rigid and extreme: A geometric representation of
personality disorders in five-factor model space. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 81, 1119-1130.
Oei, T. & Sullivan, L. (1999). Cognitive changes following recovery from depression in a group
cognitive-behaviour therapy program. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
33, 407-415.
Overholser, J. C. (1999, August). Temporal stability of the MCMI personality disorder scales.
Paper presented at the 97th annual convention of the American Psychological
Association. New Orleans, LA.
Pacini, R., Muir, F., & Epstein, S. (1998). Depressive realism from the perspective of
cognitive-experiential self-theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74( 4),
1056-1068.
Paykel, E. S., Scott, J., Teasdale, J. D., Johnson, A. L., Garland, A., Moore, R. et al. (1999).
Prevention of relapse in residual depression by cognitive therapy: A controlled trial.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 829-835. Retrieved December 23, 2002, from
PsychINFO Database.
150
Persons, J. B., Davidson, J., & Thompkins, M. A. (2001). Essential components of cognitive
behavior therapy for depression. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I. & Seligman, M. E.
(1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 6, 287300. Retrieved December 23, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.
Petrocelli, J., Glaser, B., Calhoun, G. & Campbell, L. (2001). Early maladaptive schemas of
personality disorder subtypes. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(6), 546-559.
Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity
and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of personality and social
psychology, 46, 69-81.
Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1986). Communication and persuasion: The central and peripheral
routes to attitude change. Spring-Verlag: New York.
Phillips, K. A., Shea, T. Warshar, M., Dyck, I., (2001). The relationship between comorbid
personality disorders and treatment received in patients with anxiety disorders. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 15, 157-168.
Piaget, J. P. (1947/1950). The psychology of intelligence. New York: Hourcourt Brace.
Piaget, J. P. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press, New
York.
Pickstone, J. V. (2001). Ways of Knowing: A New History of Science, Technology and
Medicine. Chicago, Ill, University of Chicago Press.
Piersma, H.L. & Boes, J.L. (1997). MCMI-III as a treatment outcome measure for psychiatric
inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53(8), 825-831.
Pollack M. H., Otto M.W., Rosenbaum, J.F., Sachs G.S. (1992). Personality disorders in patients
151
with panic disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33. 78-83. Retrieved December 4, 2002,
from OVID Database.
Pollack, M. H., Otto, M. W., Rosenbaum, J. F., Sachs, G. S., O'Neil, C., Asher, R. et al.
(1990).Longitudinal course of panic disorder: Findings from the Massachusetts
General Hospital naturalistic study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1990, 12-16.
Retrieved December 20, 2002, from OVID Database.
Pollack, M. H., Otto, M. W., Rosenbaum, J. F., & Sachs, G. S. (1992). Personality disorders in
patients with panic disorder: Association with childhood anxiety disorders, early trauma,
comorbidity and chronicity. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33, 78-83.
Power, M. & Brewin, C. R. (1991). From Freud to cognitive science: A contemporary account
of the unconscious. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 289-310.
Rachman, S. (1999). Commentary. Rapid and not-so-rapid responses to cognitive behavioral
therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 293-301.
Raimy, V. C. (1950). Training in clinical psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Ratneswar, S. & Chaiken, S. (1991). Comprehension’s role in persuasion: The case of its
moderating effects on source cues. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18, 52-62.
Reich, J. (2003). The effect of Axis II disorders on the outcome of treatment of anxiety and
unipolar depressive disorders: A review. Journal of Personality Disorders17, 387. Retrieved
August 26, 2004 from PschInfo Database.
Reiger, D. A., Narrow, W. E., Pae, D. S., Manderscherd, R. W., Locke, B. Z., & Goodwin, F.
K. (1993). The de facto U.S. mental and addictive service system. Epidemiologic catchment
area prospective 1–year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Archive of General
Psychiatry, 50, 84-95.
152
Retzlaff, P. D. (1995). Tactical psychotherapy of the personality disorders: An MCMI-III-based
approach. (Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Retzlaff, P.D., Ofman, P., Hyer, L. & Matheson, S. (1994). MCMI-II high-point codes: severe
personality disorder and clinical syndrome extensions. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
50(2), 228-234.
Retzlaff, P, Stoner, J, Kleinsasser, D. (2002). The use of the MCMI-III in the screening and
triage of offenders. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology. Special Issue: Psychological Testing in Forensic Settings, 46(3), 319-332).
Ruegg, R. & Allen, F. (1995). New Research in Personality Disorders. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 9(1), 1-48.
Riggs, D . S ., & F oa, E . B . ( 1993). O bsessive compulsive d isorder. I n D . H . B arlow ( ed.).
Clinical handbook of psychological di sorders (2nd ed.) (pps. 189 -239). N ew Y ork: T he
Guilford Press.
Robins, R. W., Gosling, S. D., & Craik, K. H. (1999). An empirical analysis of trends in
psychology. American Psychologist, 54, 117-128.
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C., (2003). Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 25-53.
Rogers, C. R. (1951). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rogers, R. (2003). Forensic use and abuse of psychological tests: Multiscale inventories.
Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 9(4), 316-320.
Rogers, R., Salekin, R.T., & Sewell, K.W. (1999). Validation of the Million Clinical Multiaxial
Inventory for axis II disorders: Does it meet the Daubert standard? Law and Human
Behavior, 23(4), 425-443.
153
Rosen, S., Cochran, W., & Musser, L. M. (1990). Reactions to a match versus mismatch between
an applicant's self-presentational style and work reputation. Basic and Apllied Social
Psychology, 11, 117-129.
Rosenberger, P. H. & Miller, G. A. (1989). Comparing borderline definitions. DSM-III
borderline and schizotypal personality disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 161169.
Ross, S. M., Gottfredson, D. K., Christensen, P. & Weaver, R. (1986). Cognitive
self-statements in depression: Findings across clinical populations. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 10, 159-166.
Rossi, G., Hauben, C., Van den Brande, I. & Sloore, H. (2003). Empirical evaluation of the
MCMI-III personality disorder scales. Psychological Reports, 92(2), 627-642.
Rossi, G., Van den Brande, I., Tobac, A., Sloore, H. & Hauben, C. (2003). Convergent validity
of the MCMI-III personality disorder scales and the MMPI-2 scales. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 17(4), 330-340.
Samuels, J. F., Nestadt, G., Romanoski, A. J., Folstein, M. F. & McHugh, P. R. (1994). DSM-III
Personality Disorders in the Community. American Journal of Psychiatry 151, 1055-1062.
Scott, J. (2001). Cognitive Therapy as an adjunct to medication for bipolar disorder. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 164-168.
Shea, M. T., Elkin, I., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, F. M., Watkins, J. T., Collins, J. F.et al. (1992).
Course of depressive symptoms over follow-up: Findings from the NIMH treatment of
depression collaborative research program. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 782-787.
Retrieved December 23, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.
Shea, M. T., Glass, D. R., Pilkonis, P. A., Watkins, J. & Docherty, J. P. (1987). Frequency and
154
implications of personality disorders in a sample of depressed outpatients. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 1, 27-42.
Schoenberg, M.R., Dorr, D. & Morgan, C.D. (2003). The ability of the Millon Clinical
Multiaxial Inventory, Third Edition to detect malingering. Psychological Assessment,
15(2), 198-204.
Shorkey, C. T. & Whiteman, V. C. (1987). The Rational Behavior Inventory. In K. Corcoran &
J. Fischer (Eds.). Measures for clinical practice: A sourcebook.(pp. 270-273). New York: Free
Press.
Simons, A. D., Murphy, G. E., Levine, J. L. & Wetzel, R. D. (1986). Cognitive therapy and
pharmacotherapy for depression: Sustained improvement over one year. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 43, 43-50. Retrieved December 4, 2002, from PsychINFO Database.
Skinner, B. F. (1958). Teaching machines. Science, 128: 969-977.
Skodol, A., Gunderson, J. G., McGlashan, T. H., Dyck, I. (2002) Functional impairment in
patients with schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive-compulsive personality
disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 2, 276-283. Retrieved January 2, 2002, from
ProQuest databases.
Sokol, L., Beck, A. T., Greenberg, R. L., Wright, F. D. & Berchick, R. J. (1989). Cognitive
therapy of panic disorder: A nonpharmacological alternative. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 177, 711-716.
Soyguet, G., Nelson, L. & Safran, J. (2001). The relationship between interpersonal schemas and
personality characteristics. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 15(2), 99-108.
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form V). Palo Alto,
155
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W. & Skodol, A. E. (1980). DSM—III: The major achievements
and an overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 151-164.
Steenbergh, T. A., Meyers, A.W., May, R.K., & Whelan, J.P. (2002). Development and
validation of the Gamblers' Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16,
143-149.
Sulloway, F.J. (1979). Freud: Biologist of the mind. Beyond the psychoanalytic legend. New
York: Basic Books.
Svrakic, D. M., Whitehead, C., Przybeck, T. R. & Cloninger, C. R. (1993). Differential diagnosis
of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Archives
of General Psychiatry 50, 991-999.
Tarafodi, R. W., Milne, A. B., Smith, A. J. (1999). The confidence of choice: Evidence for an
augmentation effect on self-perceived performance. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 25, 1405-1417.
Taylor, S. (2000). Understanding and treating panic disorder: Cognitive-behavioural
approaches. New York: Wiley.
Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., Segal, Z. V. (2002).
Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression empirical evidence.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 275-287.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Ridgeway, V., Soulsby, J., & Lau, M. (2000).
Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615-623.
Toneatto, T. (1999). Cognitive psychopathology of problem gambling. Substance use & misuse,
156
34(11), 1593-1604.
Torgersen, S. & Alnaes, R. (1990). The relationship between the MCMI personality scales and
DSM-III, axis II. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(3-4), 698-707.
Tsai, S. M., Chen, C., Kuo, C., Lee, J., Lee, H., Strakowski, S. M. (2001). 15-year outcome of
treated bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 63, 215-220
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., Borden, J. W., Stanley, M. A., & Jacob, R. G. (1991). Social
phobia: Axis I and II correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 102-106.
Turley, B., Bates, G.W., Edwards, J. & Jackson, H.J. (1992). MCMI-II personality disorders in
recent-onset bipolar disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48(3), 320-329.
Tyrer, P. (1995). Are personality disorders well classified in DSM-IV? In The DSM-IV
Personality Disorders (ed. W. J. Livesley), pp. 29-45. New York: Guilford Press.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Mental Helath: A Report of the
Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Van Balkom, A. J. L. M., van Oppen, P., Vermeulen, A. W. A., van Dyck, R., Nauta, M. C. E.,
& Vorst, H. C. M. (1994). A meta-analysis on the treatment of obsessive-compulsive
disorder: A comparison of antidepressants, behavior, and cognitive therapy. Clinical
Psychology Review, 14, 359-381.
Vazquez, C. (1987). Judgment of contingency: Cognitive biases in depysphoric and
nondepressed students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 419-431.
Versiani, M., Amrein, R., Montgomery, R. A. (1997). Social phobia: long-term treatment
outcome and prediction of response: A moclobemide study. International Clinical
Psychopharmacology,12:239-254.
157
Versiani, M., Nardi, A. E., Mindim, F. D., Pinto, S., Saboya, E., Kovacs, R. (1996). The longterm treatment of social phobia with moclobemide. International Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 11 83-88.
Watson, D. & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 447-448.
Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: A practice manual and conceptual
guide. New York: Wiley
Weissman, A. N. (1979). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale validation study. Dissertation
Abstracts (3-b), 1389-1390.
Weissman, A. N. & Beck, A. T. (1978, November). The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. (Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior
Therapy, Chicago).
Weissman, M. M., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S., Hwu, H. G., et al.
(1997). The cross-national epidemiology of panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry,
54:305–309.
Wetzel, R. D., Knesevich, M. A., Brown, S. L., Wolff, H. A., Horn, C. J. & Cloninger, C. R.
(1992). Correlates of Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire scales with selected
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales. Psychological Reports 71, 1027-1038.
Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders. Chichester: Wiley.
Weissman, A. (1979). The dysfunctional attitudes scale. Philadelphia: Center for Cognitive
Therapy.
Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation of the
Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
158
Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Chicago, IL.
Weissman, A. N., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S. Husa, H. G. et al.,
(1997). The Craoss-rational epidemiology of panic disorder, Archives of General Psychiatry,
54, 305-309.
White, M & Epston, D, 1989 Literate Means to Therapeutic Ends. DCP: Adelaide.
Widiger, T. A. (2000). Personality disorders in the 21st century. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 14, 3-16.
Widiger, T. A. & Sanderson, C. J. (1995). Assessing personality disorders.(In J. N. Butcher
(Ed.), Clinical personality assessment (pp. 380—394). New York: Oxford University Press.
Widiger, T. A. & Shea, T. (1991). Differentiation of Axis I and Axis II disorders. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 100, 399-406.
Widiger, T. A., Williams, J. B. W., Spitzer, R. L. & Frances, A. (1985). The MCMI as a measure
of DSM—III. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 366-378.
Widiger, T. A., Williams, J. B. W., Spitzer, R. L. & Frances, A. (1986). The MCMI and DSM—
III: A brief rejoinder to Millon (1985). Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 198-204.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and
psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.
Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G. & Agras, W. S. (1997). Cognitive—behavioral therapy for
bulimia nervosa.(In D. M. Garner & P. Garfinkel (Eds.), Handbook of treatment for eating
disorders (pp. 67—93). New York: Guilford Press.
Wise, E. (2001). The comparative validity of MCM-II and MMPI-2 personality disorder scales
with forensic examinees. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(3), 275-279.
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
159
Wood, J. V., Taylor, S.E., & Lichtman, R. R. (1985) Social comparison in adjustment to breast
cancer. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49(5), 1169-1183.
Woody, E. E., McClellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., & O’Brien, C. P. (1990). Psychotherapy and
counseling for methadone-maintained opiate addicts: Results of research studies. U.S.:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. .
Wosinska, W., Dabul, A. J., Whetstone-Dion, R., & Cialdini, R. B. (1996). Self-presentational
responses to success in the organization: The costs and benefits of modesty. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 18(2), 229–242
Yonkers, K., Warshaw, M., Massion, A., Keller, M. (1996). Phenomenology and course of
generalised anxiety disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168(3): 308-313.
Young, J.E. (1990/1999). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused
approach (third edition). Professional Resource Press, Sarasota, Florida.
Young, J. E. & Brown, G. (1990). Young Schema Questionnaire-Long Form, 2nd ed. New York:
Cogntive Therapy Center of New York.
Yurica, C. & DiTomasso, R. (2002). Inventory of Cogntive Distortions. In C. Yurica, C. (2002).
Inventory of Cogntive Distortions: Validation of a Psychomatric Test for the
Masurement of Cognitive Distortions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine.
Yurica, C. (2002). Inventory of Cogntive Distortions: Validation of a Psychomatric Test for the
Masurement of Cognitive Distortions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine.
Zimmerman, M., Pfohl, B., Stangl, D., & Coryell. W. (1985), The validity of DSM-III Axis IV.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1437-1441.