Download WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Pleonasm wikipedia , lookup

Lithuanian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Arabic grammar wikipedia , lookup

American Sign Language grammar wikipedia , lookup

Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Causative wikipedia , lookup

Inflection wikipedia , lookup

Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ojibwe grammar wikipedia , lookup

Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Zulu grammar wikipedia , lookup

French grammar wikipedia , lookup

Udmurt grammar wikipedia , lookup

Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Kannada grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup

Esperanto grammar wikipedia , lookup

Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Navajo grammar wikipedia , lookup

Malay grammar wikipedia , lookup

Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Chinese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Turkish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Georgian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup

Icelandic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup

Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup

English clause syntax wikipedia , lookup

Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Pipil grammar wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT
*
STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
Ana Vilacy GALUCIO
Ministério da Ciência e Tecnologia/Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi
RESUMO
Este artigo procura determinar se existe uma ordem básica de constituintes em Mekens. Usando
freqüência textual e outros critérios relevantes, argumenta que SOV deve ser a ordem básica na
língua, e analisa outras variantes como sendo o resultado marcado de processos morfossintáticos e/
ou pragmáticos, como cliticização e focalização.
ABSTRACT
This paper tries to determine whether there is a basic word order in Mekens. Using text frequency
and other relevant criteria, it argues that SOV is the basic word order in the language and analyzes
other variants as the marked result of morphosyntactic and/or pragmatic processes such as cliticization and focus.
PALAVRAS-CHAVE
Estrutura constituinte, ordem de palavras, línguas indígenas, Mekens
KEY-WORDS
Constituent structure, word order, Indigenous languages, Mekens
Introduction
This paper presents evidence for determining what the basic word
order is in Mekens. On the basis of text frequency and other relevant
constituent structure tests, I argue that SOV is the basic unmarked
word order in Mekens and that other variants found in the language
 Revista da ABRALIN, vol. 1, no 2, p. 51-73, dezembro de 2002.
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
result from morphosyntactic and/or pragmatic processes such as cliticization and focus.
Mekens or SakÆrabiat1 is one of the five surviving languages
from the Tupari subfamily (Tupi family), spoken by approximately
23 people in the state of Rondônia, in Northwestern Brazil. It
figures among the native Brazilian languages that are in great danger of disappearing (Rodrigues 1993, Moore, forthcoming), since
it has fewer than fifty speakers and is, generally, not being learned
by the younger generations.
The study of the languages of the Tupari family is still very much
in its beginnings. There are only a few partial descriptions (Alves
1991, 2002, Braga 1992, 2001, Galucio 1994, 1996, 2001, 2002,
Seki 2002). Aside from an article about the phonemic system of
Mekens (Hanke et al. 1958), the only other available materials for
this language are the outcome of my own research (Galucio ibid).
The data for this paper were collected by the author in the period
between 1994 and 1996, during four trips to the Área Indígena Rio
Mekens in Rondônia.
1 Morphosyntax Summary
There is no case marking in Mekens. The noun phrase (NP) grammatical relations of S, A, and O2 are marked through a fixed order of
the noun phrases in relation to their respective predicates. The nominal arguments precede the verb, which amounts to saying that SOV
is the preferred order of constituents; however, there are grammatical and pragmatic processes that may affect this ordering. In the case
of transitive verbs, since both arguments (A and O) precede the verb,
their linear order functions as the main criterion in distinguishing them.
Thus, in a transitive clause with two full noun phrases, the first NP
refers to the subject and the second to the object. Note in the examples (1a-b) below that changing the word order results in changes in
the meaning of the clause.
52
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
(1) a. Ameko
aose so-a-t
jaguar/dog
man see-Theme-past
‘The jaguar saw the man’
b. aose ameko
so-a-t
man jaguar/dog see- Theme-past
‘The man saw the jaguar’
There are three types of verb phrases (VP) in Mekens: intransitive, transitive, and non-inflectible or particle verbs. Intransitive VPs are composed of an intransitive verb stem and a
person prefix that cross-references number and person of the
clause subject.
(2) a. o-ib-a-t
sekwa
1s-return-Theme-past soon
‘I returned quickly’
ikãõ
Dem
b. Roque
se-er-a
i-toop
Roque
3c-sleep-Theme 3s-aux+lying+non.past
‘Roque is sleeping’
Transitive VPs are composed of a transitive verb stem and an
object. The object may be a noun phrase or a personal prefix, whereas the subject is realized as a full NP (noun or pronoun):
(3) a. ameko
e-so-a-t
dog/jaguar
2s-see-Theme-past
‘The dog saw you’
b. e-iape
seisa-t
2s-drink bring+Theme3-past
‘I brought your drink’
53
õt
I
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
Note in the above examples that whereas in the intransitive
VPs the personal prefix co-occurs with the subject noun phrase,
in the transitive VPs the personal prefix is in complementary distribution with the object NP. In addition, there is a person hierarchy regarding pronominal subjects in transitive clauses with respect to non-deletability. If the subject of a transitive verb is either a first or a second person, the subject pronoun must be
present. If it is a third person, the pronoun can be optionally omitted. Therefore, a zero subject marking in a transitive clause is interpreted as a third person singular subject. In the case of example (3b) above, if the pronoun õt ‘I’ were not present, the clause
would have an interpretation with a third person singular subject:
‘He/she brought your drink’. This ranking regarding the possibility of deleting a subject marking in transitive clauses does not
extend to plural subjects.
A few three-argument (ditransitive) verbs occur in Mekens. They
show the same inflection paradigm of transitive verbs for subject and
direct object. The indirect object is realized in an oblique postpositional phrase.
The third type of verb phrase, the uninflected or particle VP, is
composed of a verb which does not inflect for person and/or
number of its arguments. This class of VP is largely characterized
by onomatopoeic verbs but also includes lexical verbs. Uninflected verbs subcategorizing for one argument have only a subject,
realized as an NP (pronoun or noun) as in (4a). In the rare cases
where uninflected verbs have an explicit second argument, like a
notional object, this is either realized in an oblique phrase or omitted, but is never realized as a prefix. The default rule for this type
of VP is to have one or both of their arguments omitted under
identity with previously mentioned NPs. Third person subjects are
also generally omitted. The uninflected VPs are underlined in the
examples below:
54
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
(4) a. te
arop  s koboy-koboy
ebapita kwep
Foc then deer dive-Red
emerge climb
‘Then the deer dove, emerged, and came up (again)’
b. pˆp
pe=i-pap
throw
Obl=3s-dead
‘(They) threw away the dead (animal)’
Semantically, this class can be organized into three subgroups: (i)
verbs that express activity, including concepts generally expressed by
onomatopoeia, (ii) utterance verbs, which signal direct speech in the
discourse, and pro-sentence verbs, and (iii) a closed set of stance
verbs, such as standing, sitting, and lying. There is a series of arguments internal to the language that distinguishes uninflected verbs
from ideophones. The occurrence of this type of verb is a characteristic of other Tupian languages, consistent with data presented in
Moore (2002).
Mekens has a series of auxiliary verbs used in the progressive
aspect which mark person, number, and positional posture of the
clause subject through a series of personal prefixes (Galucio
1996b). In intransitive clauses, the personal prefix in the lexical
verb cross-references the person and number of the subject marked
in the auxiliary. In transitive clauses, the personal prefix in the
auxiliary is the only reference to the sentence subject, when it is
pronominal. This is clearly shown in the examples below. The
pattern of auxiliaries with intransitive (one-argument) verbs and
their inflectional paradigm reveals a nominative-accusative pattern since it always marks the clause subject, regardless of the
valence of the lexical verb.
(5) a. Ameko
se-er-a
i-toop
dog/jaguar
3c-sleep-Theme 3s-aux+lying+non-past.
‘The dog is sleeping’
55
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
b. terro
mõkwa
chicha
make
‘I am making chicha’
o-koop
1s-aux+mov+non-past.
On the other hand, pronominal marking with transitive and intransitive verbs shows an ergative pattern. This pattern is revealed in
a complementary distribution between free pronouns and personal
prefixes. The prefixes mark the verb’s S and O arguments, as shown
in (6a-b), while the pronouns mark the A argument (7a). That distribution becomes more complex once the auxiliary paradigm and other
sentence and functional types are considered, but this discussion is
beyond the scope of this paper.
(6) a. e-er-a-t
2s-sleep-Theme-past
‘(you) slept’
b. e-so-a-t
2s-see-Theme-past
‘He/she saw you’
(7) a. i-so-a-t
3s-see-Theme-past
‘You saw him/her/it’
(e)t) 4
you
(sete)
he/she/it
e) t
you
2 SOV as the basic order of constituents
Some of the criteria frequently used in the literature to determine the basic order of constituents are textual frequency, descriptive simplicity, morphological marking, and pragmatic value (Mithun 1992). On the basis of these criteria, I analyzed 113 simple and
complex sentences extracted from texts in order to determine the
basic constituent order in Mekens. The corpus included 62 transi56
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
tive clauses, 44 intransitive clauses, and 07 nominal clauses. Among
the transitive clauses, only 21 (34%) had both subject and object
overtly expressed with an NP. The general pattern for Mekens is to
have either one of the core arguments expressed by a verbal prefix
or left unmarked, as in the case of a third person singular A argument. This tendency explains the low percentage of overt NPs for
A and O. As pointed out by Siewierska (1988:10) ‘transitive clauses containing full nominal participants are uncommon in many languages in which the subject and object are either optionally or obligatorily indicated in the verb.’
In simple transitive sentences with both arguments expressed with
an overt NP, the predominant constituent order is Subject-ObjectVerb (SOV) when the subject is nominal and OVS when it is expressed with an independent pronoun. In intransitive clauses, SV is
the most frequent order with nominal subjects, while VS is the most
common with pronominal subjects. The order SOV/SV was found in
70% of the analyzed clauses with nominal subjects, but only in 29%
of the clauses with pronominal subjects; so the corpus of pronominal subject clauses showed 71% of clause final subject (OVS/VS).
I had initially entertained the hypothesis of two basic orders in
Mekens, one with full subject NPs, and the other with pronominal
subjects. The former would be subject initial, and the latter, subject
final. This type of distinction has been described for various languages of the world according to Givon (1984:217-18). Nonetheless, a
closer observation of all the clause final pronominal subjects revealed
that more than 90% of them occur with first and second singular
person pronouns–õt and e)t. The other pronouns had SOV/SV as the
common order. Thus, there is not a split between nominal and pronominal subjects, but rather a tendency for first and second singular
person pronouns to occur in final position following the verb.
It is interesting to note that these two pronouns are oscillating between the two orders given above. In all the textual examples that were
considered, simple clauses with first and second singular person pro57
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
nouns had final subject, as shown in (8a), whereas in the elicited examples the order varies between SOV/SV and OVS/VS, though the latter
is generally preferred. Changing the subject position does not seem to
entail any pragmatic or functional difference, though more research is
needed on this topic. Examples (9a-b) are elicited clauses.
(8) a. eba e-iape
seisa-t
evid. 2s-drink bring+Theme-past
‘Look, I brought your drink’
(9) a. kwaay
õpa-a-t
tapir
kill-Theme-past
‘I killed the tapir’
õt
I
õt
I
b. õt
kwaay
õpa-a-t
I
tapir
kill-Theme-past
‘I killed the tapir’
The behavior of these pronouns in two independent morphophonological processes presents evidence for analyzing them as clitics
as opposed to free pronouns. The first process involves sonorization of the voiceless stops /p, t, k/ when followed by a vowelinitial morpheme: /p, t, k/ become /b, r, g/, respectively. The second process involves nasalization in morpheme boundaries: nasalization may spread from a vowel to a morpheme final voiced stop,
causing an alternation between the voiced stop and its nasal counterpart. The spreading can be left or rightwards, and, both morpheme internally or intermorphemically (10a-b), but it does not span
word boundaries (10c-d).
(10) a./kp + e)po/
tree +
??
‘tropical creeper’
[kbe)mpo] or
58
[kme)mpo]
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
b. /o+ane)p+asi/
[oane)mbasi] or
1s-head-pain
‘I have a headache’
c. /opoep # ãkãy)/
[opoebãkãy)] but not
1s-possession burning.coal
‘My burning coal’
d. /Nwe)re)p # k/
[Nwe)re)mb k] but not
[oane)masi]
*[opoemãkãy)]
*[Nweremk]
finish water
‘Finish the water’
As we can see in (11a-b), the first person pronouns, õt and e)t,
may cause nasalization of the final consonant on a preceding word.
Therefore, they do not have the status of a phonologically free
word, since nasalization does not occur between word boundaries, as shown above. These clause-final pronouns are a syntactic
word, but not a phonological word, that is, they function as postverbal clitics in the language.
(11) a. /o+ebasi-r-e)p=õt/
1s-alive-rel-really=I
‘I am alive’
[webasire)bõn]
b. [o+akop=õt]
[o-akob-õn]
1s-hot/heat=I
‘I am hot’5 (lit: my heat I (have))
or
[webasire)mõn]
or
[o-akom-õn]
The fluctuation in the order of constituents shown in (9a) and (9b)
indicates that the reanalysis of õt and e)t as clitics is not yet completed,
but it is currently taking place in the language, thus explaining the current alternation between subject initial SOV/SV and subject final OVS/
VS word orders. As is well known, cross-linguistically clitic pronouns
are often subject to special positioning rules, which are not necessarily
related to their grammatical relations (Comrie 1989:89).
59
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
Therefore, we can maintain that the basic order of constituents in Mekens is subject initial. The subject-final variants that
were found in the corpus come either from phonological processes, such as the cliticization of pronouns at the end of verbs, as
seen above, or from morphosyntactic and/or pragmatic processes, such as focus and deverbalization.
Moreover, there is strong evidence that Mekens is not just a
subject-initial, but also a verb-final language. That is, both subject and object precede the verb, resulting in SOV as the basic
order. As it was previously mentioned, since there is neither
case marking in the nouns nor agreement on the verb, when
both NPs precede the verb, the linear order of constituents
functions as a decisive criterion in identifying the grammatical
function of the NPs.
2.1 The Object (O) + Verb (V) sequence
form a constituent
In addition to the linear precedence of the object in relation to
the verb, the data show that these two constituents form a syntactic
phrase in the language. There have been a number of contentions in
the typological literature arguing in favor of a cross-linguistic tendency for verb and object to form a syntactic unit (Comrie 1989,
Croft 1990). The data from Mekens confirms that tendency by revealing a well-established VP unit.
The existence of a verb phrase in Mekens is justified on different
syntactic grounds. Evidence for object and verb forming a syntactic
unit arises from tests involving the possibility of movement, coordinated structures, and from the fact that nothing may intervene between object and verb.
a) Nothing may intervene between the verb and its object. Even
when the object is extracted from the position immediately preceding
the verb, the OV sequence is maintained through affixation of the
60
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
object marker prefix (i-, s-) to the verb, which, then, functions as the
grammatical argument. Note in the following examples that the freely ordered particle eba may occur anywhere in the clause, except between object and verb.
(12) a. Eba
e-iape
seisa-t=õt
evid
2s-drink bring+Theme-past=I
‘Look, I brought your drink’
b. e-iape
seisa-t=õt eba
c. *e-iape
eba seisa+Theme-t=õt
(b) Some processes require the object and verb to move as a constituent to the left of the subject at the beginning of the clause. One
such process is focus, as shown in (13a) below:
(13) a. kpe
siit õ-a-t=te
tabisarã te-bõ
big.knife small. give-Theme-past=Foc chief
he/she-Dat
‘The chief gave a knife to him’ (lit. Give a knife to him is what the
chief did)
(c) The distribution of adverbial phrases in the established pattern of constituent order is consistent with the hypothesis of
object and verb forming a constituent. Four types of adverbial
phrases formally marked according to their syntactic function
have been identified: locative (N-ese), ablative (N-eri), dative
(N-bõ) and associative (N-ese p ). These adverbial phrases are
composed of a noun (phrase) and a postposition and function
grammatically as obliques. Adverbial phrases, including the time
adverbs, may occur in basically any position within the clause,
except between verb and object. Structures (14a-c) below are
possible, but (14d) is not.
61
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
(14) a.
NP
S
(14b)
AdvP VP
Subj
O
Pedro pp s
AdvP NP VP
V
Subj O
pp
miat
Pedro ontemveado matou
(14c)
S
NP
VP
S
pedro s
V
miat
ontem Pedro veado matou
*(14d)
S
AdvP
NP
O
AdvP
Subj O V
Adv
Subj Obj Adv
pedro s miat
pp
Pedros
pp
V
V
miat
Textual frequency is another strong argument favoring OV as the
basic order. Only two of the morphologically unmarked simple clauses
of the analyzed corpus did not have the object preceding the verb.
The only clauses that can have a notional object following the
verb are those in which the object has been syntactically and pragmatically demoted from its argument function and marked as an oblique. At the pragmatic level, the object demotion deemphasizes the
role of the object NP, functioning as a backgrounding antipassive,
according to the definition given in Foley and Van Valin (1985). Syntactically, it ceases to be the verbal internal argument (object), and
patterns with the oblique NPs, marked by an adposition. In such cases, the prefix i- (object marker) appears on the verb, and functions as
the O argument. A more comprehensive account of this construction in Mekens is given in Galucio (2002).
se-yãã
te pe-k
(15) a.Poret i-õ-a-t
then OM-give-Theme-past 3c-aux+sitting+nonpast foc obl-water
‘Then he gave water (to him)’
62
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
As shown above, the sequence of object and verb forms a syntactic unit. Furthermore, there is an asymmetric relationship between
subject and object in relation to the verb. Therefore, instead of a flat
structure having both NPs and the verb at the same level (16a), we
propose a branching structure (16b) for Mekens, having the subject
NP at the same level of the VP, and above the object NP.
First Approximation of a phrase structure for Mekens:
(16) a.
NP
*S
NP
(16b)
V
S
NP
Subj Obj
VP
Subj
NP
V
Obj
2.2 Nominal Clauses confirm SOV
as the basic word order
The ordering pattern shown in nominal clauses confirm SOV as
the basic order of constituents in the language. There is no copula
verb in Mekens. Predicative nominal clauses are formed through the
juxtaposition of two nominal phrases, as illustrated in (17a) below.
(17) a.o-top
kwãmõã
1s-father
shaman
“My father is a shaman”
In the analyzed corpus, there were seven nominal sentences, five
of which had the predicative NP following the subject NP. That is,
they presented the same ordering pattern found in verbal clauses, as
illustrated by (18a) below. . The only two examples that showed the
reverse order —subject NP following the predicative NP—are transcribed in (19a-b) below. The clause in (19a) has the clitic õt as sub63
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
ject, and, as it has already been shown, this pronoun tends to occur
post verbally. The clause in (19b) is a yes/no question, and as a
property of this type of clause, the predicative NP, which bears the
focus of the question, always occurs in initial position, thus preceding the subject NP.
taba i-paba
eteet kaat
(18) a.(...) paay
palm.tree leaf 3s-dry
really say
‘(...) the palm tree leaf was really dry, (he) said’
(19) a. Are)p are)bõ
o-tag-e)pit kpkat poor-apõõt=õt õt=te
then then-really 1s-son-?? tree old-neg+1s=I I=emph
‘Then (he said): As for me, I was not an old tree, my grandson’
b. Are)p sete i-so-a
pase pe=õt eyat-same
eyat-kera
then he/she OM-see-Theme all obl=I 2pl-good/well 2pl-non.assert
‘Then she looked at me, at every one (and asked) ‘Are you okay?’
2.3 Cases of postverbal NPs
The (O)VS ordering was found in 34% of the clauses that had an
explicit subject NP different from first and second person pronouns.
In general, all these cases can be explained through interaction with
other grammatical or pragmatic processes. In the following sections
we will consider two of these processes.
2.3.1 Deverbal construction with -pit
Mekens has a type of construction in which the focus of the clause
is placed on the situation described by the predicate, and the subjectundergoer is not pragmatically relevant or is unknown. This construction is employed, for instance, when the speaker does not know who
the undergoer is. This is not to be confused with an anticausative
construction, however, for the agentive argument might not be known,
but the causative component is clearly implied to be present. Anti64
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
causative constructions in Mekens involve a co-referential (reflexive) affix in the inchoative clause, similar to the constructions found
in French or Portuguese.
The Mekens deverbal construction is characterized as follows:
Firstly, a deverbal suffix -pit appears on the verb, deriving into an
adjective stem. Secondly, the NP corresponding to the subject in the
respective active clause is either omitted or treated as oblique with
respect to its distribution within the clause. Thirdly, the verbal phrase
may be divided, that is, the object may be separated from the verb,
and no additional morphological marking appears. This indicates at
least that the object NP of the active clause no longer forms a constituent with the verb. Fourthly, the object NP of the active clause is
not marked as an oblique, even when occurring outside the VP. These
two latter points indicate that the verb marked with -pit does not
subcategorize as a transitive verb. Examples (20a-b) below illustrate
this type of construction.
(20) a. korakora o-pãrãre pt-ka-a-t
chicken 1s-sieve
hole-verbz-Theme-past
‘The chicken pierced my sieve’
b. oe)p
pt-ka-pit
o-pãrãre
already hole-verbz-part. 1s-sieve
‘My sieve was pierced’/’My sieve has been pierced’
In previous versions of this paper, I had termed constructions
like those in (20b) passive-like constructions motivated by the fact
that the subject NP of a corresponding active clause, such as in
(20a), presents the behavior of an oblique NP in the clause, being
either omitted or freely ordered in the clause. Such properties are
typical of oblique NPs, not of core arguments. However, in spite
of the fact that the object NP of a corresponding active clause
does not form a syntactic unit with the verb in sentences like (20b),
65
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
as explained above, it does not show the morphosyntactic properties of a subject either. These characteristics have motivated my
adopting the term passive-like construction, in an attempt to acknowledge both the similarities and differences between this construction and real passives. However, to avoid further misinterpretation, I will simply refer to constructions, such as those in (20b)
and (21a), as deverbal constructions with –pit.
One of the sentences from my corpus that had the subject NP
following the verb (21a) fits the given characteristics of deverbal constructions with -pit in Mekens. The OVS ordering in this case is explained grammatically, by the oblique status conferred to the agentive argument in these constructions; therefore, it is not a counterexample for the analysis of SOV as the basic order.
(21) a. kare)p
kare)p
aisi õ-ã-pit
tabisarã
then
then
wife give-Theme-part.
chief
‘Then a wife was given by the chief ’/’Then the chief gave (him) a wife’
Regardless of translation, constructions such as those in (20b)
and (21a) involve deverbalization of the transitive verb, giving
rise to an adjective stem, and freeing the object NP of its VP
internal position. A more literal translation of a sentence like
(20b) would be ‘my by now pierced (by someone) sieve’, which
acknowledges the structure of the deverbal adjective and the notional NP object. Nonetheless, the AN order found in (20b) remains to be explained. The pattern of the language is NA, as seen
in (21a), but that question is out of the scope of the present paper
and will be discussed in future works.
2.3.2 Focus
The term focus has been generally used in the literature to
include nonpresupposed asserted new information as well as contrastive information (Payne 1992). Dahlstrom (forthcoming) gives
66
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
the following definition of focus found in Lambrecht (1994):
“typically new information asserted against the background of a
presupposed proposition”. This is the focus definition that we
are adopting here.
One example of a construction that is generally assumed crosslinguistically to involve focus is the question-word questions in
which the information in the proposition is presupposed, except for
the object of the interrogative word. Thus, the question word normally bears the focus.
Focus constructions in Mekens are syntactically and morphologically marked. First, the focused element occurs at the beginning of
the clause. Secondly, the clitic te appears to the right of the constituent in focus position, and before the rest of the clause. In the following sentence, the focus constituent is underlined.
(22) a.apo=te
who=foc
o-tek
1s-house
poga-b-eg-a
open-Nmlzr-house-Theme
ke
say
i-koop
ikãõ
tete
3s-aux+mov
that time ??
‘“Who opened my house?” She was saying at that time’
All question-word questions in Mekens are realized in this manner, that is, the interrogative word occurs in focus position at the
beginning of the clause, preceding the focus particle. In the same
way, in an answer to such questions, the non-presupposed new information generally comes in a focus construction.
Any constituent in a Mekens clause can bear the focus feature: subject, object, verb, verbal phrase, and oblique NPs. Therefore, focus constructions may give rise to alterations in the basic order of constituents, since any focus element will necessarily occur at the beginning of the clause. The following example
illustrates this point.
67
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
(23) a. serek=te kpmãkãy kweõp
=te
split=foc floor
climb+neg=foc
‘The floor split and the deer sank’
s
deer
Note that, in the two clauses in (23a), the subject occurs after the
verb, which is clause initial and followed by the clitic te. This particular example is extracted from a text in which the mythological hero,
Pasiare, is preparing himself to cause a flood. He uses a deer to verify
whether or not he is ready for the task. He throws the deer into the
water to see the depth, but since there is not much water at that
point, the deer struggled for a short moment and rose back easily.
This is said in the regular order SV, as shown in (24a). Then, the
second time, after having ingested a great quantity of water, Pasiare
experiments with his powers again, by expelling a lot of water and
throwing the deer into it. This time, the water level is really high
causing the floor to split into two sides and the deer to sink completely. It is this contrast between the first and the second time the
deer is thrown into water that is expressed by the focus construction
in (23a) above.
(24) a. Arop s koboy-koboy
ebapi-ta
kwep
then/that deer dive-red
front-?(emerge) climb+up
“Then the deer dove dove (into the water) and surfaced back”
It is worth mentioning that 85% of all the corpus examples of
reverse word order—subject NP following the verbal phrase (VP)—
are cases in which the VP is clause initial because it bears the focus
feature. Thus, focus constructions in Mekens are evidence for the
SOV basic word order precisely because they deviate from it, giving
rise to an OVS order. Therefore, placing a constituent in focus gives
rise to a syntactically marked structure, which deviates from the SOV
basic word order, as in (23).
Nonetheless, there is still the question of how the focus element
68
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
fits the phrase structure of the sentence. There are at least two possible hypotheses: the first is shown in (25a) where the focus element
is a daughter of the S node and stands in a symmetrical relation to the
others elements in the remainder of the clause. The second is (25b)
where the focus is a sister of S. The former structure would imply
that the non-focus elements do not form a constituent, whereas the
latter has the opposite implication.
(25a)
*S
Foc SN
(25b) S’
SV
Foc
SN
S
SV
Since there are coordinate sentences in Mekens in which a sole element in focus position refers simultaneously to anaphoric positions in
the two S’s, the second structure (25b) seems to be the right one. Considering that in this structure, the remainder of the sentence to the right of
the focus constituent forms a syntactic unit, the existence of coordinated structures that share exactly the focus element is predictable, since
there is nothing in the phrase structure blocking such constructions. On
the other hand, in a symmetric structure like (25a), in which the elements to the right of the focus do not form a constituent, such coordinate structures would not be allowed. Note in the example below that
the focus element s is coreferential to the object of both verbs.
(26) a. s tõe=te
Chico i-mi Rosalina i-perop-ka-t
deer non-afirm=foc Chico OM-kill Rosalina OM-food-TR-past
‘It looks like it was a deer that Chico killed and Rosalina cooked’
3 Final Remarks
This paper has attempted to show that SOV is the basic unmarked
order of constituents in Mekens and that other processes of the lan69
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
guage work to produce the alternative orderings identified in the corpus. These processes include the on-going cliticization phenomenon
of first and second person pronouns, denominalization of the verb
associated with de-emphasis on the role of the subject (deverbal constructions with –pit), and placement of the direct object in the initial
focus position with subsequent fronting of the constituent in question. The analysis proposed in this paper is a contribution to the typology of basic word order and possible pragmatic permutations on
the syntax of clauses in the languages of the Tupari family.
Abbreviations
1s: first person singular; 2s: second person singular; 3c: third person
coreferential; 3s:third person singular; 2pl: second person plural; aux:
auxiliary; aux+mov: auxiliary+moving; Dat: dative; Dem: demonstrative; Emph: emphasis; Evid: evidential; Foc: focus; Neg: negation;
Nmlzr: nominalizer; Non.-Afor,: non-afirmative; Non.Assert: nonassertive; NP: noun phrase; Obl: oblique; OM: object marker; Part:
participle; Red: reduplication; TR: transitivizer; Verbz: verbalizer; VP:
verbal phrase.
Recebido em
References
ALVES, Poliana M.. 1991. Análise fonológica preliminar da língua Tupari.
Dissertação de mestrado, Universidade de Brasília, DF.
____. 2001. Flexão Relacional em Tupari e em Tupi-Guarani. In
Cabral e Rodrigues (org.) Línguas brasileiras: fonologia, gramática e história,
Atas do I Encontro Internacional do Grupo de Trabalho sobre línguas
indígenas da ANPOLL, tomo I :269-273, editora universitária
UFPA:Belém, 2002.
70
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
BRAGA, A. 1992. A fonologia segmental e aspectos morfofonológicos da língua
Makurap (Tupi). Dissertação de mestrado, Universidade Estadual de
Campinas, SP.
____. 2001. A Estrutura Oracional, o Tópico e a Ordem dos
Constituintes em Makurap. Trabalho apresentado no I Encontro
Internacional do GTLI da ANPOLL/UFPA, Belém, October, 2001.
COMRIE, Bernard. 1989. Language Universals and Linguistic Typology.
nd
2 edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
CROFT, William. 1990. Typology and Universals. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
DAHLSTROM, Amy. (no prelo) Morphology and Syntax of the Fox
(Mesquakie) Language.
DIXON, Robert M.W. 1994. Ergativity. Cambridge University Press.
FOLEY and R.VAN VALIN JR. 1985. Information packing in the
clause. In T. Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, vol
1:282-364. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
GALUCIO, A. Vilacy. 1994. Fonologia Segmental da Língua Mekens.
In Anais do IX Encontro Nacional da Associação Nacional de Pós-Graduação
em Letras e Lingüística. Caxambu, MG, vol 1, p. 988-997.
_____. 1996a. Mekens Phonology. University of Chicago, IL. (ms.)
_____. 1996b. Mekens Syntax: a preliminary survey. Dissertação de
Mestrado. University of Chicago, IL.
_____. 2001. The Morphosyntax of Mekens (Tupi). Tese de
Doutorado, University of Chicago, IL.
_____. 2002. O prefixo i- em Tupi: morfema antipassivo vs. marcador
pronominal incorporado. In Cabral e Rodrigues (org.) línguas brasileiras:
fonologia, gramática e história, Atas do I Encontro Internacional do Grupo
de Trabalho sobre línguas indígenas da ANPOLL, tomo I :274-287,
editora universitária UFPA:Belém, 2002.
GIVON, T. 1984. Syntax: A functional typological introduction.
Amsterdam:John Benjamins.
HANKE, W., M. Swadesh, and A. Rodrigues. 1958. Notas da
fonologia Mekens. In Miscellanea Paul Rivet Octogenario. Dicata, vol. 2:
71
WORD ORDER AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE IN MEKENS
187-217. México.
LAMBRECHT, Knud. (1994). Information Structure and Sentence Form:
Topic, Focus, and the Mental Representation of Discourse Referents.
Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.
MITHUN, M. (1992). “Is basic word order universal?” In Payne, ed.
Pragmatics of word order flexibility. 15-61.
MOORE, D. 2002. Verbos sem flexão. In Cabral e Rodrigues (org.)
Línguas brasileiras: fonologia, gramática e história, Atas do I Encontro
Internacional do Grupo de Trabalho sobre línguas indígenas da
ANPOLL, tomo I :139-150, editora universitária UFPA:Belém, 2002.
________.(no prelo). Endangered Languages of Lowland Tropical
South America. Linguistic Diversity Endangered, ed. por Matthias
Brenzinger, Mouton.
PAYNE, D. 1992. “Nonidenfiable information and pragmatic order
rules in ‘O’odham”. In Payne, ed. Pragmatics of word order flexibility.
137-166.
SEKI, L. 2002. Aspectos da Morfossintaxe do Nome em Tuparí. In
Cabral e Rodrigues (org.) Línguas brasileiras: fonologia, gramática e história,
Atas do I Encontro Internacional do Grupo de Trabalho sobre línguas
indígenas da ANPOLL, tomo I :274-287, editora universitária
UFPA:Belém, 2002.
SIEWIERSLA, A. 1988. Word order rules. London: Croom Helm.
Endnotes
* A preliminary version of this paper was presented at the 1996 Conference on American Indian Languages, sponsored by the Society for the Study of the Indigenous
Languages of the Americas (SSILA) at the 95th Annual Meeting of the American
Anthropological Association, in San Francisco, California, November, 1996. I would
like to thank Amy Dahlstrom, Dennis Moore and two anonymous reviewers for
comments and suggestions on earlier versions of this paper, and Chris Corcoran for
helping with editing and proofreading.
1 Mekens is the name generally found in the literature, but the group has chosen
Sakrabiat as the self-identifying name.
72
ANA VILACY GALUCIO
2 S, O, and A are used here in the sense of Dixon (1994). S is the subject of an
intransitive verb, A and O are, respectively, subject and object of a transitive verb.
3 In this example, the final vowel of the verb stem –a fuses with the theme vowel –a.
4 The parenthesis in 6a indicates that a pronoun may optionally co-occur with the
personal prefix on the intransitive verb. In these cases, the pronoun may be omitted,
whereas the prefix may not. In 6b the parenthesis indicates the possibility of deletion
of a third person subject in transitive clauses, regulated by the person hierarchy
explained in section 1 above.
5 The syntactic structure of clauses like those in (11a-b) is that of a predicative clause
formed by two NP’s — an adjective stem modified by a personal prefix and a subject
pronoun. Although they are on the surface similar to an intransitive clause with
pronoun doubling, such as those shown in (6a-b) above, they have different morphological and syntactic structures (Galucio 2001). Those predicative clauses based on
an adjective stem are formed in that manner.
73