* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Extending the stressgradient hypothesis is competition among
Introduced species wikipedia , lookup
Occupancy–abundance relationship wikipedia , lookup
Island restoration wikipedia , lookup
Biological Dynamics of Forest Fragments Project wikipedia , lookup
Pleistocene Park wikipedia , lookup
Plant defense against herbivory wikipedia , lookup
Storage effect wikipedia , lookup
Perovskia atriplicifolia wikipedia , lookup
Ecological fitting wikipedia , lookup
Latitudinal gradients in species diversity wikipedia , lookup
Oikos 000: 001–008, 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2012.00355.x © 2012 The Authors. Oikos © 2012 Nordic Society Oikos Subject Editor: Dustin Marshall. Accepted 13 November 2012 Extending the stress-gradient hypothesis – is competition among animals less common in harsh environments? I. C. Barrio, D. S. Hik, C. G. Bueno and J. F. Cahill I. C. Barrio ([email protected]), D. S. Hik, C. G. Bueno and J. F. Cahill, Dept of Biological Sciences, Univ. of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, T6G 2E9 Canada. ICB also at: Inst. de Investigación en Recursos Cinegéticos (CSIC-UCLM-JCCM), Ronda de Toledo s/n, Ciudad Real, ES-13071 Spain. The role of positive interactions has become widely accepted as a mechanism shaping community dynamics. Most empirical evidence comes from plant communities and sessile marine organisms. However, evidence for the relative role of positive interactions in organizing terrestrial animal communities is more limited, and a general framework that includes positive interactions among animals is lacking. The ‘stress gradient hypothesis’ (SGH) developed by plant ecologists predicts that the balance between positive and negative interactions will vary along gradients of biotic and abiotic stress, with positive interactions being more important in stressful environments. Paralleling the SGH, stress gradients for terrestrial herbivores could be equated to inverse primary productivity gradients, so we would expect positive interactions to prevail in more stressful, low productivity environments. However, this contradicts the typical view of terrestrial animal ecology that low primary productivity systems will foster intense competition for resources among consumers. Here we use alpine herbivores as a case study to test one of the predictions of the SGH in animal communities, namely the prevalence of positive interactions in low productivity environments. We identify potential mechanisms of facilitation and review the limited number of examples of interspecific interactions among alpine herbivores to assess the role of positive and negative interactions in structuring their communities. A meta-analysis showed no clear trend in the strength and direction of interactions among alpine herbivores. Although studies were biased towards reporting significant negative inter actions, we found no evidence of competition dominating in harsh environments. Thus, our results only partially support the SGH, but directly challenge the dominant view among animal ecologists. Clearly, a sound theoretical framework is needed to include competition, positive and neutral interactions as potential mechanisms determining the structure of animal communities under differing environmental conditions, and the stress-gradient hypothesis can provide a solid starting point. Interactions among organisms can be a major force structuring biotic communities. Species interactions are often defined based on their outcome for each of the interacting species, in terms of growth, reproduction and/or survival (Arthur and Mitchell 1989). In this sense, neutral interactions are those that have no net adverse effect on the fitness of the interacting species; negative interactions have detrimental effects on one of the species, while positive ones are those that benefit at least one of the species involved without negatively affecting the other (Bertness and Callaway 1994). Early ecologists recognized the importance of both, positive and negative interactions as organizing processes in communities (Clements 1916), but competition was often considered the primary mechanism involved in structuring both plant (Schoener 1983) and animal communities (Menge and Sutherland 1987). The role of positive interactions is now commonly recognized in some fields of ecology, such as plant ecology, and it is mostly accepted that the structure of biotic communities is the result of the interplay between positive and negative interactions (Holmgren et al. 1997, Bruno et al. 2003, Callaway 2007, Gross 2008). However, although positive interactions among animals have been described in some systems (Van de Koppel and Prins 1998, Huisman and Olff 1998, Arsenault and Owen-Smith 2002), animal ecologists have not embraced the potential role of positive interactions in structuring communities as readily as plant ecologists (Gross 2008). Most research on positive, facilitative interactions has been developed for plants (reviewed by Brooker et al. 2008) and marine sessile organisms (Kawai and Tokeshi 2007). Evidence of facilitation among plants has come mostly from harsh environments, such as deserts, salt marshes, and arctic and alpine habitats (Brooker et al. 2008), and led to the formulation of the ‘stress-gradient hypothesis’ (SGH; Bertness and Callaway 1994). This hypothesis predicts that the balance between positive and negative interactions will vary along gradients of biotic and abiotic stress and highlights the importance of positive interactions in harsh environments, where amelioration of EV-1 EV-2 Alpine Net effect of interactions stressful environmental conditions has greatest effect (Crain and Bertness 2006). These predictions have stimulated research in plant ecology for the last decade leading to recent refinements of the hypothesis that incorporate different types of stressors (resource, non-resource and biotic) and allow for different shapes of the stress-facilitation relationship (Maestre et al. 2009, Malkinson and Tielbörger 2010, Holmgren and Scheffer 2010). Broadly, these modifications keep the basic framework of the SGH but recognize that facilitation, under some circumstances, may not be as important at the extreme ends of the stress gradient, allowing for a hump-shaped relationship between the importance of facilitation and stress (Malkinson and Tielbörger 2010, Holmgren and Scheffer 2010). As proposed by the original formulation of the SGH, the importance of facilitation will increase in more severe environments through ameliorating stressors, particularly when stress is non-resource driven (Maestre et al. 2009). However, facilitating species are also competitors and, when stress gradients are determined by resources, competition for the shared resource can overrule facilitation in very harsh environments (Holmgren et al. 1997). Some attempts have been made to extend the SGH to other organisms, mainly aquatic invertebrates (Kawai and Tokeshi 2007, Daleo and Iribarne 2009, Fugère et al. 2012), but a parallel body of evidence seems to be lacking for other animals. Animal ecologists certainly recognize that abiotic factors and/or the availability of resources can determine the strength and direction of interactions among species within a trophic level (Dunson and Travis 1990, Chesson and Huntly 1997, Pringle et al. 2007). However, most research has focused on the importance of competition alone, without explicitly considering the potential for positive interactions (but see Abrams and Nakajima 2007). Despite the long-standing debate that interactions would be less important in determining community structure in harsher conditions, because harsher conditions would lead to reduced population numbers and less chances for interactions, Chesson and Huntly (1997) theoretically demonstrated that harshness alone does not limit the role of interactions (competition) in structuring communities. More recently, empirical evidence showed that productivity can affect the strength of indirect interactions, with the strongest interactions predicted to occur in low-productivity environments (Pringle et al. 2007). Still, there is no general framework in animal ecology relating the balance of positive and negative interactions to gradients in environmental conditions, and how the SGH may be applied to terrestrial animals remains to be tested. SGH studies usually define stress (sensu Grime 1977) as those environmental conditions that limit producers’ ability to convert energy into biomass (Callaway 2007, Maestre et al. 2009). Equivalent gradients for animals would be those of biotic or abiotic factors, such as primary producti vity (McNaughton et al. 1989), that affect consumers’ growth, i.e. the conversion of plant to animal biomass in the case of primary consumers. In this sense, resource gradients can be equated to inverse stress gradients (Fugère et al. 2012), with environments with lower net primary productivity being more stressful to herbivores than highly productive ones (Fig. 1). The SGH then, when applied to + a SGH 0 Implied understanding Productivity Stress Figure 1. Predictions on the net effect of animal interactions along a resource-driven stress gradient (productivity gradient). The stress gradient hypothesis (SGH; solid line) developed by plant ecologists, predicts that positive interactions (positive net effects) would dominate in stressful conditions. Although explicit predictions about facilitation and productivity are lacking, the implied knowledge of animal ecologists (dotted line) suggests that com petitive interactions (negative net effects) will dominate in low productivity environments, while the occurrence of positive interactions among animals has been mostly recognized in high pro ductivity environments (e.g. tropical savannas, depicted by point a). The box shows where alpine environments would be found, and the contrasting expectations of each view. herbivores, would predict that positive interactions will prevail in less productive systems. Indeed, some examples of positive interactions among herbivores have been described in low productivity, severe ecosystems such as salt-marshes (Van der Wal et al. 2000, Stahl et al. 2006) or deserts (Edelman 2012). However, most references on facilitation among herbivores come from productive ecosystems (Van de Koppel and Prins 1998, Arsenault and Owen-Smith 2002), and the current view of animal eco logists that competition for resources will dominate in less productive areas (e.g. desert grassland, Heske and Campbell 1991; sagebrush steppe, Cheng and Ritchie 2006) or when conditions are harsher (Odadi et al. 2011b), directly contradicts the predictions of the SGH (Fig. 1). Owing to the scarcity of examples in the animal literature investigating animal interactions across stress gradients explicitly (but see Lu et al. 2009), we focus here on one of the specific predictions of the SGH, namely, that positive interactions will prevail among herbivores in more stressful (low productivity) environments, using as a case study vertebrate herbivores in alpine areas. Alpine environments are particularly suited to address this question because of the highly seasonal conditions that determine a simpler trophic structure associated with low net annual primary productivity (Oksanen et al. 1981). Further, these are the systems driving much of the plant-related research in the stress-gradient hypothesis (Choler et al. 2001, Callaway et al. 2002), and thus they are a logical system to consider the predictions of the SGH for animals. In the following sections we examine potential positive interactions among alpine animals and review the literature for examples of interspecific interactions among alpine vertebrate herbivores. We conducted a meta-analysis on the available studies to assess the (relative) role of positive and negative interactions in structuring alpine communities and evaluate the applicability of the SGH to terrestrial vertebrates. We then discuss future directions for research that will help in defining a framework to include positive, negative and neutral interactions as potential drivers structuring animal communities under differing environmental conditions. When, where and how can we expect facilitation among alpine herbivores? Herbivores may facilitate each other in a number of ways (Table 1), probably through similar mechanisms as those of plants (Callaway 2007). Broadly, facilitation among terrestrial herbivores can be mediated by the environment, when one species directly benefits from the neighbour’s effect on the environment, or by interactions with other species, either predators or prey. Given the potential mechanisms by which alpine herbivores may facilitate each other and based on the current understanding of the SGH, we may be able to develop a predictive framework for the occurrence of positive interactions among terrestrial animals. However, detailed predictions are difficult without a mechanistic understanding of all the elements involved: the effects of the facilitating species on the stressor, its nature and magnitude, and the life histories and tolerance to stress of both interacting species (Maestre et al. 2009). In harsh abiotic environments the role of habitatmodifying species that ameliorate environmental stress can be the main form of facilitation (Crain and Bertness 2006). Indeed, among alpine plants, stress is mainly driven by non-resource abiotic factors, such as temperature, and studies in these areas generally support the predictions of the SGH (Maestre et al. 2009). Similar to nurse plants (Holmgren et al. 1997), many animals can ameliorate environmental conditions to others. The role played by such habitat-modifying species has received special attention through the development of the ‘ecosystem engineer’ concept (Jones et al. 1997). Indeed, many alpine animals have Table 1. Examples of positive interactions among both plants and herbivores in alpine areas. Shaded boxes reflect the most likely mechanisms of facilitation for each group. Positive interactions among plants Abiotic amelioration of conditions temperature, wind and soil instability at high elevations1; nurse plants2 Resources increased soil moisture5; N-fixation6 Predators protection from herbivory by association with an unpalatable neighbour9 Positive interactions among herbivores use of other species’ burrows as temporary or permanent shelter3,4 extended access to high quality forage through grazing7,8 eavesdropping on heterospecifics’ antipredator behaviour10,11; reduced predator pressure though co-occurring prey12 1 Callaway et al. 2002; 2Holmgren et al. 1997; 3Zeng and Lu 2008; 4Murdoch et al. 2009; 5Wied and Galen 1998; 6Thomas and Bowman 1998; 7Mysterud et al. 2011; 8Odadi et al. 2011b; 9Callaway et al. 2000; 10Trefry and Hik 2009; 11Blumstein and Armitage 1997; 12Bêty et al. 2002. been described as ecosystem engineers (Aho et al. 1998, Zhang et al. 2003), and one of the clearest examples is burrowing. Digging burrows is a widespread strategy among alpine animals that allows them cope with environmental extremes. Other species can benefit from these burrows directly, using them as temporary or permanent shelter (Zeng and Lu 2008, Murdoch et al. 2009), or indirectly through the changes burrows induce in plant communities (Wesche et al. 2007, Van Staalduinen and Werger 2007). However, alpine herbivores seem to be more constrained by resource availability (i.e. primary productivity; Oksanen et al. 1981, McNaughton et al. 1989) than by abiotic factors themselves. In this sense, species that regulate the availability of feeding resources to others are expected to have a greater impact as facilitators (Table 1). Herbivores can induce changes in resource quality, when grazing by one species enhances the nutritional quality of forage for another species, or in resource availability, when foraging by one species makes resources more accessible to another species (Arsenault and Owen-Smith 2002, Odadi et al. 2011b). Vertebrate herbivores in alpine systems can have local effects on vegetation (Jefferies et al. 1994, McIntire and Hik 2005, Mysterud et al. 2011), with cascading effects on other species. As well, foraging activities of alpine herbivores, and thus their acquisition of resources, are known to be limited by predation risk (Morrison et al. 2004). Therefore, relying on other species’ antipredator behaviours can be advantageous. For example, it has been shown that an alpine-dwelling lagomorph, the collared pika Ochotona collaris, responds to alarm calls of heterospecifics by increasing vigilance, which likely improves its success in antipre dator responses (Trefry and Hik 2009). A similar response has been reported for other alpine herbivores (Blumstein and Armitage 1997, Shriner 1998), suggesting this might be a common phenomenon. However, population consequences of such mechanisms remain to be tested. Differences between alpine plants and animals in the nature of the main stressor may lead to slightly different predictions. If stress gradients for alpine herbivores are mainly resource-driven (i.e. productivity gradients), according to the SGH, we would expect positive interactions to be the dominant interaction type at moderately stressful conditions, but not at the extreme ends of the stress gradient (Maestre et al. 2009, Holmgren and Scheffer 2010). Life-history strategies of alpine herbivores to cope with (or even avoid) harsh winter conditions, like hibernation, food hoarding or seasonal migration, may imply that these species co-occur only during the more productive season, when competition for resources is less likely to be the dominant interaction type (Van de Koppel and Prins 1998). Therefore, according to the SGH, in alpine environments we would expect to find facilitation among herbivores except under exceptionally harsh conditions, when limited availability of resources can lead to facilitation being overruled by increased competition, resulting in a net negative effect (Odadi et al. 2011b). Is there evidence for facilitation among alpine herbivores? To evaluate how common positive interactions are in alpine communities we conducted a meta-analysis of published EV-3 experimental studies (see Supplementary material Appendix 1 for details). An initial search retrieved 1467 studies, from which less than one hundred were relevant to our question; a comparable raw search on plants (substituting all animalrelated search terms with plant*) retrieved nearly twice as many studies (2711). Most of the relevant studies on animal interactions were observational (53 out of 74; 71.6%), which is not surprising given the difficulties associated with conducting manipulative experiments on wild animals in the field. Observational studies can provide a valuable first step to understanding the potential mechanisms underlying species interactions (Darmon et al. 2012), but experimental evidence is needed to infer particular mechanistic models (Novak and Wootton 2010). We could only find nine experimental studies to include in our meta-analysis that fulfilled our criteria and from which data could be extracted (Supplementary material Appendix 1), and thus we suggest our results are to be used to promote inquiry, rather than representing a strong conclusion. The studies reported changes in abundance of one species when cooccurring with another species and when occurring alone; therefore our effect size, the standardized mean difference, indicates positive or negative effects of a certain species on the abundance of the focal one. The experimental design used in this type of studies, analogous to neighbour removal experiments in plant ecology (Callaway 2007), should equally detect positive and negative interactions. The experiments we found do not specifically test the balance between positive and negative interactions across productivity gradients, and therefore lack specific controls Reference Vial et al. 2011 Steen et al. 2005 Vial et al. 2011 Forsyth and Hickling 1998 Bêty et al. 2001 Forsyth and Hickling 1998 Vial et al. 2011 Steen et al. 2005 Rüttiman et al. 2008 Loe et al. 2007 Austrheim et al. 2007 Heske and Steen 1993 Bêty et al. 2001 Galindo and Krebs 1985 Heske and Steen 1993 in benign environments; however, we can still evaluate the prevalence of positive or negative interactions in stressful, low productivity environments. We found no clear trend in the direction of the inter actions among alpine herbivores (p 0.790; Fig. 2). The test for homogeneity indicated that studies were significantly heterogeneous (Q 39.8, df 14, p 0.001), but we found a high proportion of the observed variance reflecting real differences among studies (I2 80.9%). This is surprising given the theoretical framework under which these experiments were conducted, with most studies assuming that competition among herbivores would be the primary (if not the only) type of interaction in alpine environments, due to their low primary productivity. To detect this potential publication bias in our data set we used the trim-and-fill method (Duval and Tweedie 2000), that estimates the number of missing studies that might exist in a meta-analysis based on the symmetry of the observed studies in a funnel plot. Although the subset of studies included in the meta-analysis were biased towards reporting on significant competitive interactions (Supplementary material Appendix 2), our results did not support the prevalence of negative interactions among alpine herbivores. Another potential source of bias are the life-histories of the interacting species, which are known to affect the outcome of their interactions (Maestre et al. 2009). Within herbivore guilds, niche partitioning leading to co-existence can be determined by body size and digestive ability (du Toit 2011). Since this would lead to the expectation of more competitive interactions among herbivores of similar size or Focal sp Interacting sp Blicks grass rat bank vole giant mole rat tahr greater snow goose large colony chamois brush−furred mouse field vole chamois willow grouse rodents voles greater snow goose small colony singing vole lemmings livestock sheep livestock chamois lemmings tahr livestock sheep sheep sheep sheep lemmings lemmings tundra vole voles Effect size [95% CI] −1.13 [−2.85, 0.59] −0.93 [−2.12, 0.26] −0.66 [−2.30, 0.98] −0.54 [−1.16, 0.07] −0.43 [−0.70, −0.16] −0.35 [−1.16, 0.46] −0.14 [−1.74, 1.46] −0.09 [−1.22, 1.04] 0.05 [−0.44, 0.53] 0.10 [−1.50, 1.70] 0.30 [0.02, 0.58] 0.40 [−1.00, 1.80] 0.40 [0.11, 0.70] 0.46 [−0.60, 1.52] 1.62 [0.04, 3.19] RE Model −0.04 [−0.31, 0.24] −5 0 Effect size 5 Figure 2. Forest plot for the meta-analysis of experimental studies on herbivore interactions in alpine environments. Nine studies, reporting 14 independent comparisons, i.e. the focal species with and without the interacting species, are included. Sizes of points are proportional to sample size of each study. Negative effect sizes (standardized mean differences) indicate competition, while positive ones indicate facilitation. Studies are ordered by increasing effect size. The overall effect size estimated using a random effects meta-analysis (RE Model) is shown. EV-4 among those using the same feeding strategy, we tested for the effect of differences in sizes between the focal and the interacting species, and of taxonomic family. However, we found no effects of these variables on our results (difference in size: Q 0.260, p 0.610; taxonomic family: Q 0.158, p 0.691). A possibility that cannot be ruled out is that facilitative interactions are occurring among alpine herbivores because few studies have been designed to look for facilitation. From the available studies, two found significant positive interactions, whereas only one found competitive inter actions (Fig. 2). Although recent theoretical development (Abrams and Nakajima 2007, Gross 2008) and some empirical evidence (Austrheim et al. 2007, Loe et al. 2007) suggest the role of positive interactions in organizing animal communities, more research is clearly needed. An alternative explanation is that, overall, the strength and direction of herbivore interactions is actually zero, and neutral interactions are more prevalent than previously thought. Neutral interactions are those that do not have measurable effects on the fitness of the interacting species, and as such, have been regarded as a lack of interaction (Arthur and Mitchell 1989). However, this ‘lack of inter action’, particularly the lack of strong competitive inter actions, represents a mechanism through which species can coexist (Tokeshi 1999). Future directions – how can we bridge the gap? We found no evidence of competition being the main interaction type among herbivores in low productivity alpine environments; on the contrary, neutral or positive interactions may prevail among terrestrial herbivores inhabiting harsh environments. Our results thus, only partially support the SGH, but interestingly, directly challenge the dominant view of animal ecologists of competition dominating in harsh environments (Fig. 2). However, how irreconcilable are these views? Maybe part of the solution comes from a closer scrutiny of the assumptions of the studies that have supported those views, under the light of the recent developments of the SGH. For example, the recent work by Odadi et al. (2011b) studied the balance of species interactions in two periods of contrasting ‘harshness’ (dry vs wet season) using cattle as a focal species. They found that wild ungulates compete with cattle in the dry season, while facilitation prevails in the wet, more productive season, and this result led them to challenge the SGH. However, other studies have reported positive interactions in the same system during the dry season; zebras are facilitated by elephants in the presence of cattle (Young et al. 2005), and cattle and donkeys facilitate each other when herded at low stocking densities (Odadi et al. 2011a). In this case, the identity of the interacting species may play an important role, even determining opposite outcomes in highly stressful situations (Maestre et al. 2009). As well, the relative densities of each herbivore type are known to affect the net effect of interactions (Odadi et al. 2011a, du Toit 2011), probably altering the perception of stress of the interacting species; low resource supply coupled with low population densities of consumers might not be as stressful as if it were coupled to higher numbers of consumers (Chesson and Huntly 1997). Maybe this is the reason why many studies working with livestock, that usually occur at artificially high densities, might be biased towards competitive effects, unless occurring in very productive environments (Mishra et al. 2004, Bagchi et al. 2004, Odadi et al. 2011b). A revision of results under the recent developments of the SGH will help reconcile both views, and guide future research of animal ecologists addressing this topic (Table 2). Table 2. Suggestions (research questions) for future research on the balance of positive and negative animal interactions along stress gradients, and predictions based on the current knowledge of the SGH. Topic Stress gradient Identity of interacting species Length of the stress gradient Measures of performance Biotic stress Multiple stressors Potential research questions Predictions based on the SGH How does each species respond to the stress Animal species will respond differently to each interacting gradient? Does this vary across and/or within species; facilitation will be more frequent between trophic guilds? different trophic guilds. Is the net effect of species interactions among When stress is resource-driven (e.g. primary productivity), animals non-linear along stress gradients? responses will be non-linear, with facilitation being more Does this hold for both resource and important when stress is high but not extreme. For non-resource driven stress gradients? non-resource gradients (e.g. abiotic constraints), facilitation will dominate at high stress levels. Do different measures of performance reflect Some measures (e.g. population size) will reflect longer-term changes in the strength and direction of effects of interactions while others (e.g. reproduction, interactions similarly? mortality) may better indicate shorter-term effects. How does predation affect the balance Other mechanisms of facilitation may arise when predation between positive and negative interactions is accounted for (e.g. apparent mutualism), leading to between animals? facilitation at low resource stress levels. How do stressors interact? How does this affect When considering multiple stressors (e.g. predation and the outcome of interactions among animals? productivity), interactions among stressors alter the net outcome of interactions expected based on a single stressor. Mechanisms of interaction Indirect interactions How prevalent are indirect interactions among animals? Extension to community How frequent are positive interactions among animals along stress gradients? Indirect positive interactions are the most common type of facilitation among animals. Positive interactions among animals will be more common in species-rich communities. EV-5 Overall, more basic empirical evidence is needed on the balance between positive and negative interactions among terrestrial animals along environmental gradients. One of the key aspects that needs to be carefully addressed in future studies is the nature of the stress gradient and how it is perceived by the interacting species (Table 2). Here, we have focused on productivity as the main stressor for primary consumers, but other sources of stress, including those driven by abiotic conditions, need to be considered too. A detailed understanding of the mechanistic cause of stress and the physiological responses of the interacting species to a specific stressor may help in defining predictions and experimental designs (Maestre et al. 2009). For example, a species may not respond in the same way to interacting species of different size or feeding strategy. As well, experiments need to be conducted across well defined stress gradients. Even if in the first assessments a rough distinction between high and low stress levels might still be valid, empirical studies should incorporate the fullest possible range because non-linear responses are likely to occur along stress gradients (Maestre et al. 2009, Malkinson and Tielbörger 2010). In addition, experiments should ideally account for different measures of species’ performance and of interaction strength (Chase et al. 2002), because these are likely to have an impact on the results. Another source of stress that has received less attention by plant ecologists when developing the SGH, is the biotic stress incurred by consumers (Smit et al. 2009). Predation can affect competitive outcomes between species (Chase et al. 2002) and although theoretical development on the interaction between predation and competition in animal communities has been extensive (Chase et al. 2002, Chesson and Kuang 2008), its relationship with positive interactions still needs to be addressed. Our approach has explicitly excluded consumer pressure as a source of stress, but future studies need to address this point. In this case, gradients would be defined by varying predation rates (or other suitable surrogates for predation pressure), and interactions among prey species would be expected to vary along these gradients. Some specific mechanisms may lead to positive interactions when predation risk is the main stressor. Similar to associational defence in plants (Atsatt and O’Dowd 1976), when two animal species share a common predator, increases in the abundance of one species can lead to a reduction of predation rates on the other less preferred prey, at least in the short-term, leading to an apparent mutualism (Abrams and Matsuda 1996). As well, it is likely that biotic and abiotic stress factors interact, which ultimately affects the outcome of species interactions (Smit et al. 2009). Therefore, consideration of multiple simultaneous stress gradients can alter the balance between interactions (Kawai and Tokeshi 2007). The specific mechanisms of interaction have to be accounted for as well in future studies (Table 2). While plants and animals might be similar in some respects, the differences between them may determine specific particularities in the ways they interact with each other. For instance, plants might be more directly affected by their neighbours, whereas mechanisms of facilitation among animals might be predominantly indirect. In addition, positive interactions among animals can be deferred in space EV-6 or time, and thus might be more difficult to detect if studies are not conducted at the appropriate scale (Karban et al. 2012). Scaling-up studies on pair-wise interactions to community levels may reveal more such indirect inter actions. For example, indirect facilitative effects in plant communities have been hypothesized to be more frequent in species-rich communities or in communities with several co-occurring limiting factors (Brooker et al. 2008). Recent work on animal communities demonstrates that the strongest indirect effects (interaction cascades) are likely to occur in low-productivity systems (Pringle et al. 2007). Assessing how prevalent indirect interactions are will greatly improve our understanding of the organization and dynamics of animal communities. Conclusions Despite recent claims from other disciplines on the need to include positive interactions into ecological theory (Bruno et al. 2003), the animal ecology literature seems far from reflecting this trend. Given the lack of a well defined theoretical framework, borrowing (and testing) hypotheses from other disciplines might be a good starting point. In this sense, the stress-gradient hypothesis developed by plant ecologists can provide a solid framework that might be applicable to other organisms, and its specific predictions need to be tested for terrestrial vertebrates using manipulative experiments in the field. This is likely to be a promising avenue for research in animal ecology, since the few attempts to explicitly test the SGH on other model systems, like detritivorous invertebrates (Fugère et al. 2012) or herbivorous crabs (Daleo and Iribarne 2009), have been successful. Furthermore, animal ecologists can build on plant ecologists’ experience and avoid some mistakes committed in the past, to design their experiments in a more effective way. Although there is a long way to go, we believe this is a promising field of research towards the integration of different ecological disciplines. Acknowledgements – A. Mysterud kindly provided data for the meta-analysis. We are very grateful to F. Maestre for constructive criticism of previous versions of this work. Funding was provided by NSERC Discovery Grants to DSH and JFC, awarded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (Canada). ICB was supported by a postdoctoral fellowship provided by the Consejería de Educación, Ciencia y Cultura (JCCM, Spain) and the European Social Fund; CGB is beneficiary of a postdoctoral grant from the AXA Research Fund and a Killam fellowship from the Univ. of Alberta. References Abrams, P. A. and Matsuda, H. 1996. Positive indirect effects between prey species that share predators. – Ecology 77: 610–616. Abrams, P. A. and Nakajima, M. 2007. Does competition between resources change the competition between their consumers to mutualism? Variations on two themes by Vandermeer. – Am. Nat. 170: 744–757. Aho, K. et al. 1998. Pikas (Ochotona princeps: Lagomorpha) as allogenic engineers in an alpine ecosystem. – Oecologia 114: 405–409. Arsenault, R. and Owen-Smith, N. 2002. Facilitation versus competition in grazing herbivore assemblages. – Oikos 97: 313–318. Arthur, W. and Mitchell, P. 1989. A revised scheme for the classification of population interactions. – Oikos 56: 141–143. Atsatt, P. R. and O’Dowd, D. J. 1976. Plant defense guilds. – Science 193: 24–29. Austrheim, G. et al. 2007. Interactions between sheep, rodents, graminoids, and bryophytes in an oceanic alpine ecosystem of low productivity. – Écoscience 14: 178–187. Bagchi, S. et al. 2004. Conflicts between traditional pastoralism and conservation of Himalayan ibex (Capra sibirica) in the Trans-Himalayan mountains. – Anim. Conserv. 7: 121–128. Bertness, M. D. and Callaway, R. 1994. Positive interactions in communities. – Trends Ecol. Evol. 9: 191–193. Bêty, J. et al. 2002. Shared predators and indirect trophic inter actions: lemming cycles and arctic-nesting geese. – J. Anim. Ecol. 71: 88–98. Blumstein, D. T. and Armitage, K. B. 1997. Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: I. The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls. – Anim. Behav. 53: 143–171. Brooker, R. W. et al. 2008. Facilitation in plant communities: the past, the present and the future. – J. Ecol. 96: 18–34. Bruno, J. F. et al. 2003. Inclusion of facilitation into ecological theory. – Trends Ecol. Evol. 18: 119–125. Callaway, R. M. 2007. Positive interactions and interdependence in plant communities. – Springer. Callaway, R. M. et al. 2000. Facilitation by unpalatable weeds may conserve plant diversity in overgrazed meadows in the Caucasus Mountains. – Oikos 89: 275–282. Callaway, R. M. et al. 2002. Positive interactions among alpine plants increase with stress. – Nature 417: 844–848. Chase, J. M. et al. 2002. The interaction between predation and competition: a review and synthesis. – Ecol. Lett. 5: 302–315. Cheng, E. and Ritchie, M. E. 2006. Impacts of simulated livestock grazing on Utah prairie dogs (Cynomys parvidens) in a low productivity ecosystem. – Oecologia 147: 546–555. Chesson, P. and Huntly, N. 1997. The roles of harsh and fluctua ting conditions in the dynamics of ecological communities. – Am. Nat. 150: 519–553. Chesson, P. and Kuang, J. J. 2008. The interaction between predation and competition. – Nature 456: 235–238. Choler, P. et al. 2001. Facilitation and competition on gradients in alpine plant communities. – Ecology 82: 3295–3308. Clements, F. E. 1916. Plant succession; an analysis of the development of vegetation. – Carnegie Inst. Crain, C. M. and Bertness, M. D. 2006. Ecosystem engineering across environmental gradients: implications for conservation and management. – BioScience 56: 211–218. Daleo, P. and Iribarne, O. 2009. Beyond competition: the stress gradient hypothesis tested in plant–herbivore interactions. – Ecology 90: 2368–2374. Darmon, G. et al. 2012. Spatial distribution and habitat selection in coexisting species of mountain ungulates. – Ecography 35: 44–53. du Toit, J. T. 2011. Coexisting with cattle. – Science 333: 1710–1711. Dunson, W. A. and Travis, J. 1990. The role of abiotic factors in community organization. – Am. Nat. 138: 1067–1091. Duval, S. and Tweedie, R. 2000. Trim and fill: a simple funnelplot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. – Biometrics 56: 455–463. Edelman, A. J. 2012. Positive interactions between desert granivores: localized facilitation of harvester ants by kangaroo rats. – PloS One 7: e30914. Fugère, V. et al. 2012. Testing the stress-gradient hypothesis with aquatic detritivorous invertebrates: insights for biodiversity-ecosystem functioning research. – J. Anim. Ecol. 81: 1259–1267. Grime, J. P. 1977. Evidence for the existence of three primary strategies in plants and its relevance to ecological and evolutionary theory. – Am. Nat. 111: 1169–1194. Gross, K. 2008. Positive interactions among competitors can produce species-rich communities. – Ecol. Lett. 11: 929–936. Heske, E. J. and Campbell, M. 1991. Effects of an 11-year livestock exclosure on rodent and ant numbers in the Chihuahuan Desert, southeastern Arizona. – Southwestern Nat. 36: 89–93. Holmgren, M. and Scheffer, M. 2010. Strong facilitation in mild environments: the stress gradient hypothesis revisited. – J. Ecol. 98: 1269–1275. Holmgren, M. et al. 1997. The interplay of facilitation and competition in plant communities. – Ecology 78: 1966–1975. Huisman, J. and Olff, H. 1998. Competition and facilitation in multispecies plant–herbivore systems of productive envi ronments. – Ecol. Lett. 1: 25–29. Jefferies, R. L. et al. 1994. Vertebrate herbivores and northern plant communities: reciprocal influences and responses. – Oikos 71: 193–206. Jones, C. G. et al. 1997. Positive and negative effects of organisms as physical ecosystem engineers. – Ecology 78: 1946–1957. Karban, R. et al. 2012. Facilitation of tiger moths by outbreaking tussock moths that share the same host plants. – J. Anim. Ecol. 81: 1095–1102. Kawai, T. and Tokeshi, M. 2007. Testing the facilitationcompetition paradigm under the stress-gradient hypothesis: decoupling multiple stress factors. – Proc. R. Soc. B 274: 2503–2508. Loe, L. E. et al. 2007. Positive short-term effects of sheep grazing on the alpine avifauna. – Biol. Lett. 3: 110–112. Lu, X. et al. 2009. Reproductive ecology of two sympatric Tibetan snowfinch species at the edge of their altitudinal range: response to more stressful environments. – J. Arid Environ. 73: 1103–1108. Maestre, F. T. et al. 2009. Refining the stress-gradient hypothesis for competition and facilitation in plant communities. – J. Ecol. 97: 199–205. Malkinson, D. and Tielbörger, K. 2010. What does the stressgradient hypothesis predict? Resolving the discrepancies. – Oikos 119: 1546–1552. McIntire, E. J. B. and Hik, D. S. 2005. Influences of chronic and current season grazing by collared pikas on above-ground biomass and species richness in subarctic alpine meadows. – Oecologia 145: 288–297. McNaughton, S. J. et al. 1989. Ecosystem-level patterns of primary productivity and herbivory in terrestrial habitats. – Nature 341: 142–144. Menge, B. A. and Sutherland, J. P. 1987. Community regulation: variation in disturbance, competition, and predation in relation to environmental stress and recruitment. – Am. Nat. 130: 730–757. Mishra, C. et al. 2004. Competition between domestic livestock and wild bharal Pseudois nayaur in the Indian Trans-Himalaya. – J. Anim. Ecol. 41: 344–354. Morrison, S. et al. 2004. Forage selection by collared pikas, Ochotona collaris, under varying degrees of predation risk. – Can. J. Zool. 82: 533–540. Murdoch, J. D. et al. 2009. The endangered siberian marmot Marmota sibirica as a keystone species? Observations and implications of burrow use by corsac foxes Vulpes corsac in Mongolia. – Oryx 43: 431–434. Mysterud, A. et al. 2011. Plant quality, seasonality and sheep grazing in an alpine ecosystem. – Basic Appl. Ecol. 12: 195–206. Novak, M. and Wootton, J. T. 2010. Using experimental indices to quantify the strength of species interactions. – Oikos 119: 1057–1063. Odadi, W. O. et al. 2011a. Facilitation between bovids and equids on an African savanna. – Evol. Ecol. Res. 13: 237–252. EV-7 Odadi, W. O. et al. 2011b. African wild ungulates compete with or facilitate cattle depending on season. – Science 333: 1753–1755. Oksanen, L. et al. 1981. Exploitation ecosystems in gradients of primary productivity. – Am. Nat. 118: 240–261. Pringle, R. M. et al. 2007. Herbivore-initiated interaction cascades and their modulation by productivity in an African savanna. – Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104: 193–197. Schoener, T. W. 1983. Field experiments on interspecific competition. – Am. Nat. 122: 240–285. Shriner, W. M. 1998. Yellow-bellied marmot and goldenmantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. – Anim. Behav. 55: 529–536. Smit, C. et al. 2009. Inclusion of biotic stress (consumer pressure) alters predictions from the stress gradient hypothesis. – J. Ecol. 97: 1215–1219. Stahl, J. et al. 2006. Subtle interplay of competition and facilitation among small herbivores in coastal grasslands. – Funct. Ecol. 20: 908–915. Thomas, B. D. and Bowman, W. D. 1998. Influence of N2fixing Trifolium on plant species composition and biomass production in alpine tundra. – Oecologia 115: 26–31. Tokeshi, M. 1999. Species coexistence. Ecological and evolutionary perspectives. – Blackwell. Trefry, S. A. and Hik, D. S. 2009. Eavesdropping on the neighbourhood: collared pika (Ochotona collaris) responses to playback calls of conspecifics and heterospecifics. – Ethology 115: 1–11. Supplementary material (available online as Appendix oik00355 at www.oikosoffice.lu.se/appendix ). Appendix 1–2. EV-8 Van de Koppel, J. and Prins, H. H. T. 1998. The importance of herbivore interactions for the dynamics of African savanna woodlands: an hypothesis. – J. Trop. Ecol. 14: 565–576. Van der Wal, R. et al. 2000. On facilitation between herbivores: how brent geese profit from brown hares. – Ecology 81: 969–980. Van Staalduinen, M. A. and Werger, M. J. A. 2007. Marmot disturbances in a Mongolian steppe vegetation. – J. Arid Environ. 69: 344–351. Wesche, K. et al. 2007. Habitat engineering under dry conditions: the impact of pikas (Ochotona pallasi ) on vegetation and site conditions in southern Mongolian steppes. – J. Veg. Sci. 18: 665–674. Wied, A. and Galen, C. 1998. Plant parental care: conspecific nurse effects in Frasera speciosa and Cirsium scopulorum. – Ecology 79: 1657–1668. Young, T. P. et al. 2005. Competition and compensation among cattle, zebras, and elephants in a semi-arid savanna in Laikipia, Kenya. – Biol. Conserv. 122: 351–359. Zeng, X. and Lu, X. 2008. Interspecific dominance and asymmetric competition with respect to nesting habitats between two snowfinch species in a high-altitude extreme environment. – Ecol. Res. 24: 607–616. Zhang, Y. et al. 2003. Burrowing rodents as ecosystem engineers: the ecology and management of plateau zokors Myospalax fontanierii in alpine meadow ecosystems of the Tibetan Plateau. – Mamm. Rev. 33: 284–294.