* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download View - OhioLINK ETD
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
QUANTITATIVE GENETICS OF ZEBRAFISH ONTOGENY UNDER CHANGING ENVIRONMENTAL CONDITIONS A Dissertation Presented to The Graduate Faculty of The University of Akron In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy Chris Marks May, 2012 i QUANTITATIVE GENETICS OF ZEBRAFISH ONTOGENY UNDER CHANGING ENVIRONMENTAL CONDITIONS Chris Marks Dissertation Approved: Accepted: ______________________________ Advisor Brian Bagatto ______________________________ Department Chair Monte Turner ______________________________ Co-Advisor Francisco B.-G. Moore ______________________________ Dean of the College Chand Midha ______________________________ Committee Member Kevin P. Kaut ______________________________ Dean of the Graduate School George R. Newkome ______________________________ Committee Member Andrea Case ______________________________ Date ______________________________ Committee Member Mark Kershner ii ABSTRACT Quantitative genetics has provided a proximate tool for making evolutionary predictions based on genetic and environmental sources of variation. Unfortunately, quantitative models have failed to address the evolutionary consequences of development in complex, changeable environments. This type of knowledge can only be achieved by tracking unique genotypes across all possible combinations of changing environmental factors. The first part of my research focuses on the consequences of changing environmental oxygen on zebrafish cardiovascular development. I found that cardiac output at the 48th hour of development in a given oxygen environment was conditional upon oxygen conditions during the first 24 hours of development. These conditional responses varied across genotypes, resulting in interactions between genotype, early, and later environments (G xExE). The second part of my research focused on the consequences of changing oxygen on zebrafish morphology. I found that body shape in zebrafish in a given oxygen environment for days 6-90 was conditional upon oxygen conditions for days 0-6. These conditional responses also varied across genotypes. The third part of my research focused on the consequences of changing oxygen on zebrafish behavior, size, and physiology. I found that size and iii behavior in zebrafish was dictated by oxygen conditions for days 0-30. Physiology, however, was influenced most by oxygen conditions for days 30-90. The fourth part of my research focused on the consequences of changing food rations of zebrafish size and swimming ability. I found that fish raised under all possible combinations of high and low food treatments achieved the same body size at 60 days. Swimming performance, however, was conditional on interactions between feeding treatments. I also detected significant family-level variation for these responses, indicating at least some heritable variation. These studies comprise some of the first quantitative genetic studies to track developmental outcomes across more than one instance of environmental change. My work has shed light on the evolutionary potential in complex, changeable environments as well as provided a tractable tool for partitioning the proximate sources of development across discrete ontogenetic periods. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES .............................................................................................. viii LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................. x CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................. 1 Quantitative genetics in changing environment .............................................. 2 Predictability and plasticity ............................................................................. 8 Evolutionary predictions in leiu of multiple instances of environmental Change........................................................................................................... 8 Environmental change and past selection ...................................................... 9 Incorporating G: GxExE ............................................................................... 10 From theory to practice: zebrafish under changing environmental conditions ..................................................................................................................... 11 Chapters II-VI: a roadmap ............................................................................ 14 Hypothesis ................................................................................................... 16 II. A QUANTITATIVE GENETIC ANALYSIS ON THE RELATIVE INFLUENCES OF GENOTYPE, EARLY, AND LATER ENVIRONMENTS ON THE DEVELOPING ZEBRAFISH CARDIOVASCULAR SYSTEM ............................ 17 Introduction .................................................................................................. 17 Methods ....................................................................................................... 23 Results ......................................................................................................... 27 v Discussion…………………….........................................................................36 Conclusions and evolutionary implications ................................................... 40 III. ONTOGENETIC PROGRAMMING IN BODY SHAPE OF ZEBRAFISH: THE ROLE OF CHANGING OXYGEN DURING DEVELOPMENT IN SHAPING THE P MATRIX........................................................................................................ 46 Introduction .................................................................................................. 46 Methods ....................................................................................................... 49 Results ......................................................................................................... 56 Discussion .................................................................................................... 70 Conclusions and evolutionary implications ................................................... 77 Acknowledgments ........................................................................................ 77 IV. ONTOGENETIC OXYGEN CHANGES ALTER ZEBRAFISH BEHAVIOR, PHYSIOLOGY, AND ORPHOLOGY ............................................................... 79 Introduction .................................................................................................. 79 Methods ....................................................................................................... 83 Results ......................................................................................................... 90 Discussion .................................................................................................. 105 Conclusions and evolutionary implications ……….…..………………………116 Acknowledgments ...................................................................................... 116 V. THE INFLUENCE OF ONTOGENETIC DIETARY CHANGES ON ZEBRAFISH SIZE AND SWIMMING PERFORMANCE ..................................................... 118 Introduction ................................................................................................ 118 Methods ..................................................................................................... 120 Results ....................................................................................................... 123 Discussion .................................................................................................. 133 vi Conclusions and evolutionary implications ................................................. 136 Acknowledgments ...................................................................................... 136 VI. Conclusions................................................................................................ 138 Ramifications of environmental complexity ................................................ 138 Variation, selection, and evolution.............................................................. 139 Future directions ........................................................................................ 141 LITERATURE CITED ...................................................................................... 143 vii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 2.1 Analysis of variance for vessel structure and cardiac performance in developing zebrafish embryos ................................................................ 30 3.1 Multivariate analysis of variance for zebrafish morphology and body shape ................................................................................................................ 58 3.2: Analysis of variance results for univariate traits (body area, maximum depth, eye area, operculum height, eye-operculum distance, PC1, and PC2)....................................................................................................... 60 3.3 Results of pair wise Flury comparisons for all half sib families pooled by developmental treatment ........................................................................ 62 3.4 Results of pair wise Flury comparisons for all half sib families divided among all four developmental treatments ............................................... 63 3.5 Pairwise Procustes distances between environmental treatments. The first row and column represents the treatment............................................... 67 3.6 Pairwise Procustes distances between all sires and environmental treatments ............................................................................................... 71 4.1 Mixed model repeated-measures ANOVA analyzing the effects of trial phase, sire, early oxygen (E0-30), later oxygen (E30-90), and all possible interactions on behaviors ........................................................................ 92 4.2 Mixed model ANOVA analyzing the effects of sire, early oxygen (Env0-30), later oxygen (Env30-90), and all possible interactions on blood glucose, PC1 (size), and latency to shoal (latency).............................................. 93 5.1 ANOVA results for total length (TL), standard length (SL), maximum depth (MD), and swim velocity .............................................................. 125 5.2 Total length (mm) for zebrafish at 30 days under high and low food rations ................................................................................................... 126 5.3 Maximum depth and standard length (mm) for zebrafish at 60 days under viii all combinations of high and low food rations ...................................... 127 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1.1 Hypothetical reaction norm showing the response of three genotypes to one instance of environmental change ..................................................... 3 1.2 Hypothetical reaction norms representing phenotypic responses to two instances of environmental change during development .......................... 5 1.3 Hypothetical reaction norms representing phenotypic responses of three unique genotypes to environmental change later in development ............ 7 2.1 Various strains of zebrafish..................................................................... 25 2.2 The influence of developmental oxygen on stroke volume ..................... 31 2.3 The influence of developmental oxygen on cardiac output for half-sib families A-K............................................................................................. 32 2.4 The influence of developmental oxygen on arterial diameter .................. 33 2.5: The influence of developmental oxygen on arterial heart rate ................ 34 2.6 The influence of developmental oxygen on venous diameter ................. 35 2.7 Cardiac output of all half sib families ...................................................... 42 2.8 Cardiac output of all half sib families raised under early normoxic conditions................................................................................................ 43 2.9 Cardiac output of all half sib families raised under early hypoxic conditions................................................................................................ 44 2.10 Coefficient of variation due to all four combinations of oxygen Environments .......................................................................................... 45 3.1 Landmarks and morphological measurements on photographed zebrafish ................................................................................................. 53 3.2 The influence of developmental oxygen on PC1 ..................................... 65 x 3.3 The influence of developmental oxygen on PC1 for half sib families A-D.......................................................................................................... 66 3.4 Effect of developmental environment (HH, HN, NH, NN) on the canonical variate scores ........................................................................................ 68 3.5 Comparison of body shape of HH vs NN fish.......................................... 69 3.6 Variance due to all four combinations of oxygen environments .............. 76 4.1 Overview of behavioral arena ................................................................. 85 4.2 Percentage time spent swimming before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments .............. 94 4.3 Percentage time spent on the shoaling side before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments .......................................................................................... 95 4.4 Percentage time shoaling before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments .................................. 97 4.5 Number of crosses between shoaling and non-shoaling sides of the arena before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments .................................................................. 99 4.6 Percentage time spent hiding before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments ............ 100 4.7 Blood glucose levels (mg/dl) as a function of four developmental environments ........................................................................................ 102 4.8 Principal component 1 (body size) as a function of four developmental environments ........................................................................................ 104 4.9 Variance due to all four combinations of oxygen environments ............ 117 5.1 The influence of diet for days 0-30 on total length in four full-sib families.................................................................................................. 124 5.2 The influence of changing diet on standard length in four full-sib families.................................................................................................. 128 5.3 The influence of changing diet on maximum depth in four full-sib xi families.................................................................................................. 129 5.4 Swimming velocity as a function of four nutritional environments ......... 131 5.5 The influence of changing diet on swimming velocity in four full-sib families.................................................................................................. 132 5.6 Variance due to all four combinations of feeding treatments ............... 137 xii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Within a population, phenotypic variation provides the raw material for natural selection to optimize phenotypes. Thus, understanding the proximate causes of phenotypic variation are critical to understanding evolutionary processes. Ultimately, phenotypic variation is reduced to genetic variation (G), environmental variation (E), and their interaction (GxE). The genetics of phenotypic variation has been extensively explored. Additive, dominant, and epistatic interactions have been pinpointed as discrete subcomponents contributing to overlying phenotypic variation (Atchley and Zhu 1997; Lynch and Walsh 1998; Wang et al 2006). Unfortunately, studies addressing the nature of environmental variation have not kept pace. Traditional studies examining the influence of environmental variation on resulting phenotypic variation typically consider the influence of environmental change at one discrete point in ontogeny. This approach has been criticized as a gross oversimplification (Sih 2004). As the environment can change multiple times during ontogeny, there arises the need to consider multiple changes at different points during developmental time. Studies examining the consequences of such changes thus offers not only a deeper understanding the of proximate causes of variation, but also allows for 1 better informed predictions on evolutionary processes in changeable environments. Qunatitative genetics in changing environments The environment plays a critical role in development (Pigluicci 1998). It can permanently alter developmental trajectories at critical ontogenetic stages (Burggren and Reyna 2011). For example, insulin sensitivity can be programmed in utero based on maternal nutrition status (review in Jones and Ozanne 2009). Thus, the entire ontogenetic history of the developing organism becomes critical in determining adult fitness. If we partition ontogeny into discrete stages (i.e. early and later), we can apply quantitative genetic approaches to weigh the relative influences of environmental conditions during these stages in determining phenotypic outcomes. Under this approach, phenotypic outcomes can be due to early environment (Eearly), later environment (Elate), or their interaction (Eearly x Elate). This approach not only elucidates which conditions are most critical in shaping phenotypes, but also the consequences of applying these conditions at various points of ontogeny. The above approach helps elucidate the role environmental change can play throughout development, however, the potential for evolutionary change remains unclear. Genetic variation for plasticity (i.e. G x E interactions) predicts that plasticity can evolve. Traditional reaction norms plot the slope of the responses of separate genotypes to one instance of environmental change (Fig. 1.1) (Via and Lande 1985). Such plots can be modified to account for two instances of environmental change. Under this modified approach, 2 Fig. 1.1. Hypothetical reaction norm showing the response of three genotypes to one instance of environmental change. 3 rather than representing genotypes, individual lines represent early environmental states. The x axis, which traditionally represented the single instance of environmental change in traditional reaction norms, now represents a later environmental change. Examples of such figures are shown in figure 1.2. On this hypothetical figure, line type (dashed vs solid) represents two possible early environmental states (Eearly; A vs B). The x axis represents two possible later environmental states (Elate; A vs B). Thus, the responses of individuals to all four possible combinations of subsequent environmental changes are visibly quantified (AA vs AB vs BB vs BA). Figure 1.2A represents the case where early environmental conditions alter the directional response to later environmental conditions. Quantitatively, this would be an Eearly x Elate interaction. Figure 1.2B represents the case where variation is due to only to the early environment. Quantitatively, Eearly would be the only significant source of variation. Both early and later environmental conditions influence variation in figure 1.2C. Therefore, both Eearly and Elate would be significant sources of variation. Figure 1.2D represents a special case. Similar to figure 1.2A, the direction of the responses to multiple instances of environmental change differ. While this would also represent a significant Eearly x Elate interaction, unlike figure 1.2A, the population variance structure in environments A and B differ. 4 Fig. 1.2. Hypothetical reaction norms representing phenotypic responses to two instances of environmental change during development. Line type (dashed vs solid) represent unique environmental conditions at an early point during development. The x axis (A vs B) represents unique environmental conditions experienced during later development. Each panel represents hypothetical responses of four different genotypes. 5 While these figures display variance contributed by both environments, genotypic variance is difficult to visualize. We can divide the reaction norms by early developmental conditions (A vs B) with the response of each genotype to later environmental conditions (A vs B). Figure 1.3 depicts the response of genotypic variants to later environments A and B. However, figure 1.3A represents variants exposed to early environment A and 1.3B represents the same variants exposed to early environment B. These are therefore similar to traditional reaction norms, however, the influence of subsequent developmental environments is apparent. Visualizing genetic variation is essential to making informed evolutionary predictions. The change in variance expressed in each environment influences that rate at which selection removes unfit variants from a population (Gupta and Lewontin 1982, Remold and Lenski 2001). The variance structures in figure 1.3 are altered by early environmental conditions. In figure 1.3A, there is no change in variance structure across later environments A and B. The change in rank order across later environments A and B is also important in maintaining genetic variation within variable populations. This is because there is no superior genotype as the environments changes from A to B (Gupta and Lewontin 1982, Remold and Lenski 2001). Whereas early environment A alters the rank order of 2 of the 3 genotypes, early environment B does not. The influence of early environment B also differs in that the genetic variance structure differs in later environments A and B. 6 Fig. 1.3. Hypothetical reaction norms representing phenotypic responses of three unique genotypes to environmental change later in development. Line type (dashed vs solid) represent unique environmental conditions at an early point during development. The x axis (A vs B) represents the later environment. 7 Predictability and plasticity Theory predicts that adaptive plasticity can occur if environmental change is predictable (Levins 1968, Via et al. 1995 Schlichting and Pigliucci 1998, Reed et al. 2010). The basic tenant of this dissertation is that environmental change is not always predictable. Although some factors are certainly predictable (photo cycle, seasons, high and low tides), many factors are more stochastic. Factors such as food availability, oxygen levels, temperature, pH, and predators can vary spatiotemporally across an organism’s ontogeny. Phenotypic responses in unpredictable environments (i.e. where the early environment fails to predict the later environment) have thus far been characterized at the population level (Relyea 2003, Marks et al. 2005, Hoverman and Relyea 2007, Monaghan 2008, Kotrschal and Taborsky 2010). One shortcoming of these studies is that no evolutionary inferences can be made. In order to make evolutionary predictions, genetic variation for responses in unpredictable environments must be characterized. The work in this dissertation embodies the first series of experiments that charactrize genotypic variance in a model organism exposed to changing, reversible conditions during ontogeny. By partitioning genotypic variation across different conditions during different periods of development, this work will shed light on how evolution proceeds in complex enviornments. Evolutionary predictions in leiu of multiple instances of environmental change Variation in a given environment provides potential insight into evolutionary dynamics. As the classic breeder’s equation predicts, the response to selection (R) equals the product of the heritability of a trait (h 2) and the 8 selection differential (S) (Lynch and Walsh 1998). Numerous studies have noted an increase in the heritability of traits under environmental change (Sgro and Hoffman 1998, Bubily et al 2001, Charmantier and Garant 2005, Ibarra and Famula 2008). As heritability and phenotypic variance increases, the response to selection would increase proportionally. Thus, the amount of observed variance under a set of subsequent environmental conditions provides insight into the rate of evolution under such environmental changes. In figures 1.2A-C, the variance in both later environments remains the same regardless of early environmental conditions. Therefore, the rate of evolution would be the same under environmental sequences AA and AB as BB and BA. Figure 1.2D, however, provides a unique example. While this figure is quantitatively identical to figure 1A (i.e. Eearly x Elate interactions), the observed variance changes and is conditional on the particular order of sequential environments. Individuals from sequential environments AA and BA exhibit limited phenotypic variance. Individuals from sequential environments AB and BB exhibit increased variance. Thus, the response to selection in populations where these two particular combinations of environmental states potentially occur would be accelerated. Environmental change and past selection Variance not only provides insight into the rate of evolution, but can also provide information on past selection. As it optimizes phenotypic responses, selection would by definition reduce phenotypic variance (Lynch and Walsh 1998). In figure 1.2D, variance is reduced under subsequent environmental 9 states AA and BA. Thus, these particular combinations of environments may have predominated in this population’s evolutionary history. Incorporating G: G x E x E Statistically, the unique responses of separate genotypes to one instance of environmental change contributes to G x E interactions (Via 1985). Variation in the magnitude and direction of responses of separate genotypes across the four possible combinations of subsequent environmental changes would contribute to a significant G x E x E interaction. Figures 1.2A-D represent cases where there is variation in the quality of responses to four possible combinations of sequential environments. These collective responses embody the three way interaction between genotype and subsequent environmental states (G x E x E). Thus, in a complex environment, where states can be A, B, switch from A to B, and from B to A, the most optimal response to these collective combinations of sequential environmental states would be selected. For example, perhaps the highest fitness would be achieved if a larger body size is expressed when exposed to early environmental state A regardless of later environmental state (A or B). And assume in the same population it would also be advantageous to express a small body size when exposed to early environmental state B regardless of later environmental state (A or B). This is the scenario presented in figure 1.2B. In this scenario, the fitness of genotype B would be superior to genotypes A, C, and D. Thus, genotype B would thrive under changes between these environmental states. A significant GxExE response at the population level thus indicates the 10 presence of multiple genotypic strategies to respond to changes between environmental states. From theory to practice: zebrafish under changing environmental conditions Why zebrafish? Zebrafish are a popular vertebrate model of development (Grunwald and Eisen 2002). They are native to Southeast Asia and inhabit a wide variety of habitats including rivers, streams, and stagnant rice fields (Engeszer et al. 2007b). The wide range of environments they encounter makes them a tractable model for studying the effects of changing ecological conditions on development. The zebrafish genome has also been extensively studied, making them a promising model for linking ecological impacts with developmental end evolutionary outcomes (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/guide/zebrafish/). Why use a developmental framework? Development played a major role in evolutionary theory before and after Darwin’s major contributions (Gould 1977). Prior to the rediscovery of Mendel’s particulate theory of inheritance (De Vries 1889, Corren 1900), embryonic development was viewed as the primary target for evolution and taxonomic diversity (Richards 1982). However, the rise of Mendelian genetics catalyzed the demotion of developmental biology to the backwaters of evolutionary thought. The modern synthesis emphasized genetic mutation and recombination as the primary target for evolutionary novelty. The computational utility offered by population (Haldane 1924, Wright 1932) and quantitative genetic (Fisher 1930) models yielded predictive potential not previously offered by developmental 11 models. The molecular underpinnings of phenotypic expression coupled with theories of selection and drift have become the dominant paradigm for evolutionary biology over the past 80 years. Following the modern synthesis, biologists have returned to development to explain more rapid diversification both in the fossil record (Gould 1977) and extant species (Waddington 1952). Developmental plasticity and genetic assimilation gained momentum as more parsimonious explanations for macroevolutionary patterns (Waddington 1953, West-Eberhard 2003). The recent discovery of homologous developmental regulatory genes has unlocked the potential for studying development in a population and quantitative genetic framework (Carroll 1995). Quantitative genetic models have begun to incorporate a developmental framework into their models (Cheverud 1983, Atchley 1987, Wang 2006). These studies demonstrate the usefulness of quantitative genetics in making evolutionary predictions by partitioning development into discrete stages through time. What is necessary to complete this picture is a further understanding of the role plasticity plays at various stages of development and its potential to confound or redirect ontogenetic trajectories. Particularly, do early environmental conditions promote a plastic response that is canalized (as in Fig. 1.2B)? Or do they alter the manner in which later plastic responses are expressed (as in Figs. 1.2A, D)? Collective figures such as 1.2A-D have yet to be established in any know biological system. This dissertation’s analysis of reactions to subsequent environmental changes across discrete stages of 12 development among separate genotypes represents a novel contribution to the fields of development, evolution, and quantitative genetics. Why hypoxia? The majority of my studies examine the effects of changing oxygen levels during zebrafish development. Dissolved oxygen is an aquatic environmental factor that can change seasonally, monthly, or even daily according to other conditions. Factors such as light, temperature, turbidity, and depth can alter oxygen availability (Boutilier 1990). Hypoxia via eutrophication is becoming increasingly common as humans continue to impart their influence on their surroundings (Diaz and Rosenbergt 1995; Rabalais and Turner 2001).Hypoxia is a common stressor in fish and is known to alter gene expression, physiology, and behavior (Wu 2002). The ability to extract oxygen from the environment is critical for organisms to maintain routine metabolic functions. Evolutionarily, fish have acquired physiological adaptations to hypoxia such as increased gill surface area (Nilsson 2007), altered hemoglobin binding efficiency (Jensen and Weber 1982), respiration (Saint-Paul 1984), and lowered tissue oxygen demands (Hopkins and Powell 2001). Fish have also adopted numerous behavioral strategies to cope with hypoxia (reviewed in Pollock et al 2007). Hypoxia is therefore a particularly important factor in shaping fish evolution. Why different windows of development? As indicated above, the following studies analyze the effects of changing environmental conditions at varying points during ontogeny. While the timing of these changes was chosen somewhat arbitrarily, they each represent a snapshot 13 of the potential ramifications of switching environmental conditions during development. While the likelihood of zebrafish encountering constant hypoxia for the first 30 days of development followed by constant normoxic conditions for 60 days (as an example) is unlikely, this approach explores a level of environmental complexity that is not well established in current plasticity studies. Of all four studies, chapter III uses perhaps the most ecologically relevant approach. Oxygen regimes were switched at day 6 of development. Zebrafish embryos remain sessile for approximately 5 days after development, after which they can seek more optimal conditions. Given the aforementioned stagnant conditions in which they reside coupled with their ability to tolerate anoxia (Padilla and Roth 2001), it is therefore likely that they encounter hypoxia early in development. Their developmental regime therefore makes it likely that larval and adult oxygen environments can differ. Chapters II-VI: a roadmap Given the usefulness of zebrafish as a vertebrate model as well as their dynamic environmental history, the following chapters analyze the evolutionary potential on the effects of changing environmental conditions during their ontogeny. Chapter II analyzes the effects of changing oxygen conditions (hypoxia vs normoxia) on cardiovascular development at 48 hours. This developmental window (particularly the first 24 hours) has not been well studied with regard to the developing zebrafish cardiovascular system. We examine the impact of 14 switching oxygen conditions at 24 hours to determine the effect on subsequent cardiovascular development at hour 48. Chapter III analyzes the effects of changing oxygen on zebrafish morphology and body shape at day 90. As zebrafish remain sessile for the first 56 days of development, we utilize this ecologically relevant time period to analyze the impact of switching oxygen conditions during this particularly early window of development on later adult body shape and morphology. Chapter IV analyzes the effects of changing oxygen on zebrafish behavior, size, and physiology at day 90. As organisms are comprised of suites of traits that can be functionally linked in many ways (Roff 2000), we examine the influence of switching hypoxia and day 30 of development on zebrafish behavior, morphology, and physiology. Many environmental factors can contribute to developmental variation. Therefore, rather than using oxygen concentration, chapter V analyzes the effects of changing food rations on zebrafish size and swimming ability at day 60. We switched food rations between high and low treatments after 30 days to examine the influence on zebrafish size and swimming performance. Chapter VI discusses the consequences of each chapter’s findings on making predictions of zebrafish evolution under changing environments. Each chapter utilizes the modified reaction norm approach described earlier. In general, the variance expressed in each later environment as a function of each early environmental state will allow for predictions on which stages (early vs later) are most important in making evolutionary predictions. Finally, chapter VI will discuss the potential ramifications of the specific findings of each chapter. 15 At the end of each chapter, comparisons of selected traits will be made to the modified reaction norms established in this chapter and the evolutionary implication will be discussed. All chapters use breeding designs to estimate the genetic contribution to plasticity as a function of changing developmental environments. Since dams donate both genotypic and maternal effects, sire variance is typically used as a proxy to estimate additive genetic variance in a population (Lynch and Walsh 1998). Although maternal effects have important implications on measuring selection (reviewed in Robinson 1981, Lynch and Walsh 1998), they were not within the scope of this study. Therefore, although dam effects were included in every analysis, only results including sire and subsequent interactions are presented. All models have therefore accounted for dam variance. Hypothesis The following chapters test the overall hypothesis that early environmental conditions will constrain phenotypic variation in later environments. By tracking separate genotypes across subsequent developmental environments, we further hypothesize that there is significant additive genetic variation in the quality of these environmentally-mediated responses. 16 CHAPTER II A QUANTITATIVE GENETIC ANALYSIS ON THE RELATIVE INFLUENCES OF GENOTYPE, EARLY, AND LATER ENVIRONMENTS ON THE DEVELOPING ZEBRAFISH CARDIOVASCULAR SYSTEM Introduction Understanding the relationship between phenotypic variation, its underlying causes, and evolutionary change remains one of the primary goals of evolutionary biology. Recently, the reintegration of development and evolution has revitalized the synthetic field of evolutionary developmental biology (EDB) (Hall et al 2004). EDB views evolution as a change in developmental trajectories (Atchley 1987, Carroll 1995, Carroll 2005). In recent decades, a more developmental framework has been incorporated in quantitative genetic models. These models demonstrate the utility of quantifying the underlying genetic variation of developmental events occurring at discrete stages through time. One finding is that the underlying genetic structure of traits (dominance, epistasis, additive genetic variance) is dependent on the developmental stage studied (Atchley and Zhu 1997, Wang et al 2006). As the environment changes, however, developmental trajectories may be contingent on past conditions. It is therefore necessary to examine the role plasticity plays at various stages of development and its potential to constrain or redirect developmental trajectories. 17 Seminal to this effort is an understanding of how ecological change regulates developmental trajectories. While the current environment can provide immediate clues to the developing system (Schlichting and Smith 2002), the effects of past environments can remain due to previous epigenetic reprogramming (Ellison 2010). Modeling the underlying quantitative genetics of development therefore requires rigorous approaches that track all possible combinations of environmental conditions experienced during development. This stems from awareness that development is a sequential process in which early components induce and alter the development of later components (Cowley and Atchley 1992). As the early developmental environment mitigates cell, hormone, and protein activity, there lies the potential for altering the development of future phenotypes. Hypoxia, for example, induces a cascade of responses with altered cell signaling and protein expression (Ton et al 2003). Quantitative genetic variation in these events provides one proximate source of variation in the quality and timing of developmental events. As the environment changes, previous courses of development may confound or redirect the expression of future phenotypes. Phenotypic development proceeds as a function of genotype, environment, and their interaction (Scheiner 1993, Lynch and Walsh 1998, Pigliucci 2001). While the genotype provides the basic instructions for development, the environment can alter these instructions, resulting in alternative phenotypes. This ability, termed developmental plasticity (West-Eberhard 2003), underscores an organism’s ability to buffer the demands of environmental 18 change. Adaptive plasticity is believed to occur when environmental change is predictable (Levins 1968, Via et al. 1995 Schlichting and Pigliucci 1998, Reed et al. 2010). In a specific example, Sih and Moore (1993) found that salamanders (Ambystoma texanum) delayed hatching in the presence of predators, resulting in larger, more physically developed larvae. These larvae experienced less predation than their non-exposed, smaller, less physically developed conspecifics. Studies partitioning the effects of environmental variation across genotypes have revealed the presence of genetic variation for plastic responses (Pigliucci 2001). Thus, plasticity has been considered a trait which selection can optimize. Fisher (1918) first decomposed phenotypic variance (V P) into basic causal components of genetic variance (VG), environmental variance (VE), and their interaction (VGXE). This partitioning has provided that basis for modeling the relative impacts of genotype and environment on phenotypic evolution and development as well as making evolutionary predictions. More recently, Scheiner and Goodnight (1984) defined plasticity variance (VPL) as the summation of environmental variance (VE), and genotype by environment variance (VGXE). Thus, plasticity can also be considered a quantitative trait that is reducible to environmental (VE) and genetic (VGxE) influences and is capable of evolution. While quantitative genetic studies have further partitioned genetic variance into the effects of additive (VA), dominance (VD) and epistasis (VI) (Lynch and Walsh 1998), there has been continued emphasis on only one level of environmental variance (VE). The conventional partitioning of plasticity or 19 phenotypic variance across one level of environmental variance has not been without criticism. Sih (2004) has argued that traditional GxE studies have disregarded the true complexity of the environment. Indeed the environment is likely to change during development. Variations in early environmental conditions can occur at any time during development. Early conditions can have immediate, lasting impacts on the course of the developing organism. For example, maternal nutrition status can irreversibly influence offspring insulin sensitivity in a number of animal models (Jones and Ozanne 2009). In sticklebacks, increasing maternal stress resulted in offspring that later exhibited increased shoaling and antipredator behavior (Giesling et al. 2010). Vascular, endocrine, and metabolic pathways are also established during early development (Hocher et al 2001) with lasting effects on adult behavior and disease susceptibility in a number of animal models (reviewed in Ellison 2010). These results suggest the need to partition environmental influences beyond the current paradigm into early and later environments. The cardiovascular system is one of the first organ systems to develop in vertebrates. This makes it particularly susceptible to early environmental influences. In zebrafish (Danio rerio) cardiac output has been shown to increase at 3 days post fertilization (d.p.f.) in response to chronic hypoxia (Jacob et al 2002). Bagatto (2005) found that chronic hypoxia shifted andrenergic responses to occur earlier in zebrafish development. Moore et al (2006) found that cardiac venous diameter, arterial diameter, heart rate, stroke volume, and cardiac output in zebrafish at 4 d.p.f. was altered by hypoxia differentially across families. Moore 20 et al. were the first to establish genotype-specific plastic responses in cardiovascular development in zebrafish. While more rigorous designs of the quantitative genetics of zebrafish cardiovascular development have yet to be performed, this result indicates at least some level of genetic variation in this animal model. Collectively, these studies suggest that the early developing cardiovascular system in zebrafish provide a tractable system to partition the effects of genetic and environmental variation. Here, we test the flexibility of the early zebrafish cardiovascular system to changing oxygen conditions (i.e. all possible combinations of hypoxia and normoxia). With the aid of a rigorous quantitative genetic design, we partition the relative impacts of genotype (among sire variation), early (oxygen for hours 024), and later (oxygen for hours 24-48) conditions, as well as their inclusive interactions. Our approach expands on traditional quantitative genetic studies by partitioning the effects on developmental plasticity across discrete developmental environments. Our approach allows for additive as well as interactive effects of sequential developmental environments. In quantitative genetic terms, our approach is as follows: Traditional approach: VPL = VE + VGxE Where VPL = variance in plasticity, VE = variance due to the developmental environment, and VGxE = variance due to genotype by environment interaction. Our (expanded) approach: VPL = VE1 + VE2 +VGxE1 + VGxE2+VE1xE2 + VGxE1xE2 21 Where VPL = variance in plasticity, VE1 = variance due to the first developmental environment, VE2 = variance due to the second developmental environment, VGxE1 = variance due to interaction between genotype and the first environment, VGxE2 = variance due to interaction between genotype and the second environment, VE1xE2 = variance due to interactions between both environments, and VGxE1xE2 = variance due to the interactions between both environments and genotype. What knowledge can be gained from partitioning these sources? While previous plasticity studies characterize genetic variation associated with one instance of environmental change, they yield no predictive value on the importance of early vs later environments in shaping developmental trajectories. If early oxygen (hours 0-24) alters the response to later oxygen (hours 24-48), we anticipate significant environment – environment (ExE) interactions between early and later oxygen environments. Such results have been previously demonstrated in a few models (Marks 2005; Kotrschal and Taborsky 2010). In this study, however, we partition the responses to changing oxygen across separate genotypes (sires). This will shed light on how early conditions alter or constrain responses to later conditions across genotypes. If there is variation in these responses, we anticipate significant GxExE interactions. We analyzed particularly early windows of development (hours 0-24 and 24-48). This early window of development has yet to be explored with respect to cardiovascular development. Oxygen sensors controlling cardiac performance have been shown to be competent as early as 2-3 d.p.f. (Jacob et al 2002, 22 Pelster et al 2003). Hypoxia has also been shown to alter gene expression and suppress morphological development at 48 h.p.f. (Kajimura et al. 2005). Although the heart is not fully functional at 24 h.p.f., environmental cues have been shown to later the onset of critical events in development (reviewed in Spicer and Burggren 2003). Furthermore, hypoxia has been proven to influence primordial germ cell migration which can influence the development of later organ systems (Lo et al. 2011). We therefore hypothesize that oxygen changes during the first 24 hours of development will alter cardiovascular development. Methods Animals Zebrafish (Danio rerio) are freshwater cyprinids native to India, Bangladesh, and Myanmar (Barman 1991). They occupy a variety of habitats ranging from stagnant, hypoxic ditches and rice fields to fully oxygenated waters such as ponds and streams (Talwar and Jhingran 1991). They are an important vertebrate model organism in the fields of development and genetics (Vascotto et ali 1997, Grunwald and Eisen 2002). Their established usefulness in genetic and developmental assays coupled with their exposure to a wide range of habitats makes them an ideal candidate for the study of oxygen-induced developmental plasticity. Adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) were obtained from Aquatica Tropicals (Plant City, FL). There are a number of strains (Fig. 2.1). In this study, we used strains of wildtype (Fig. 1 WT) and albino (Fig. 1 A). and populations were maintained 23 according to standard procedures (Westerfield 1994). Adults were maintained and bred at 26 °C with a 14L : 10D light cycle. Breeding design Wildtype males (n=11) were randomly selected and mated to randomly selected albino females (n=55) in a nested mating design. Albino females were used for their ease of sexing, large egg production, and established minimal genetic variation. Such designs are typically used with among sire variance used to estimate genotypic variance (Lynch and Walsh 1998). Each male was mated with 5 unique females. After successfully mating, each female was removed from the design. Successful mating was determined if at least 80 fertilized eggs were acquired. Mating pairs were placed in 2 L containers supplied with a common water source (Z-Mod housing system, Marine Biotech, Beverly, MA). Developmental treatments Eighty eggs from each clutch were removed before the 8 cell stage and randomly divided between two developmental treatments. One treatment consisted of normoxia (dissolved oxygen > 6.0 mg O2 l-1) and the other hypoxia (dissolved oxygen 1.0 ± 0.2 mg O2 l-1). The apparatus for maintaining developmental treatments is described in Marks et al. (2005). Following 24 hours, half of the eggs from each treatment were randomly placed in the opposite environment. The remaining eggs were left to develop in the same environment. This resulted in four treatment groups: larvae raised in hypoxia for 48 hours (HH), normoxia for 48 hours (NN), normoxia for 24 hours followed by hypoxia for 24 hours (NH), and hypoxia for 24 hours followed by normoxia for 24 hours (HN). 24 Figure 2.1. Various strains of zebrafish. Taken from http://www.pnas.org/content/vol105/issue3/images/large/zpq0010889800001.jpeg 25 Measurements At 48 hours of development, high speed digital recording was used to capture images of cardiovascular architecture. Since breeding designs were staggered, only one clutch per day was measured. Measurements were made between 47-50 hours of development. Measurements were randomized across treatments to avoid a time bias. Larvae were recorded on a temperature controlled stage (Harvard Apparatus) under and inverted microscope (Leica DMIRB). The temperature controlled stage consisted of a thermal insert attached to a dual channel bipolar temperature controller. The thermal insert surrounded a small 3 cm x 2 cm x 2cm chamber filled with water from the subject’s most recent treatment. A detailed schematic of the apparatus is available online at: http://www.harvardapparatus.com/Live_Cell_Imaging_Brochure.pdf. Digital video was recorded at 250 frames sec-1 with a mounted high speed digital video camera (Red Lake MASD, San Diego, CA). Heart rate (beats/min), stroke volume (nl) cardiac output (nl/min) , and caudal arterial and venous diameters (mm) were measured according to Moore et al. (2006). All of these parameters showed family-specific plasticity to hypoxia in Moore et al. (2006). Stroke volume was calculated as the product of heart rate and cardiac output. Due Statistics A three-way nested ANOVA utilizing the proc glm procedure in the SAS software package (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) was used to test which sources contributed significantly to variation in cardiovascular architecture and performance. Early environment, later environment, and their interaction were 26 treated as fixed factors. Sire and all inclusive interactions were treated as random factors. All results conformed to normality assumptions. We therefore used raw, untransformed data for all analyses. Results Sire All measured variable varied significantly across sires (table 2.1). This indicates that there is significant additive genetic variance for the traits measured. Eearly Stroke volume and cardiac output both exhibited variance do to early oxygen conditions (table 2.1). Fish raised in early hypoxia had higher overall stroke volume and cardiac output. These parameters were highest on average when the second developmental environment was hypoxia (Figs. 2.2, 2.3L) Elate Stroke volume, cardiac output, arterial diameter, and heart rate were significantly altered by later oxygen. With the exception of heart rate, all parameters increased on average when the second oxygen environment was hypoxia (Figs. 2.2, 2.3L, 2.4, 2.5). EearlyxElate Arterial diameter was the only trait in which the response to later oxygen was altered by early oxygen. This resulted in a significant interaction between subsequent oxygen environments (table 2.1). While fish raised in early normoxia 27 increased arterial diameter when switched to hypoxia, fish raised in early hypoxia decreased arterial diameter when switched to normoxia (Fig. 2.4). Genotype-dependent plasticity to each environment: GxEearly and GxElate Arterial diameter, venous diameter, and stroke volume showed variation in responses to early oxygen that varied significantly across sires (Fig. 2.1). This indicates significant additive genetic variance to the manner in which early oxygen constrains or alters these parameters. The response to arterial diameter to later oxygen also varied across sires (Fig. 2.1). This indicates significant additive genetic variance to the manner in which later oxygen constrains or alters arterial diamater. Genotype-dependent conditional plasticity: GxEearlyxElate Heart rate, stroke volume, and cardiac output all showed significant interactions between sire, early, and later oxygen environments (table 2.1). This indicates that the nature of the interplay between subsequent developmental environments dependent on the genetic context. Cardiac output will be explored as an example. While cardiac output generally increases in response to developmental hypoxia for hours 24-48 (Fig. 2.3L), the responses vary across families (Figs. 2.3A-K). While some genotypes remain flexible to oxygen changes, others are heavily influenced by early oxygen conditions. Families B, C, D, E, and K, for example, express higher cardiac output by individuals raised in early hypoxia regardless of the later rearing conditions (Fig. 2.3). Families A and F are also constrained by early oxygen conditions, however, individuals raised in early normoxia consistently express higher cardiac output (Fig. 2.3). Families I 28 and J remain flexible in their responses to later oxygen. The direction of these changes, however, is genotype specific. For family I, individuals tend to increase cardiac output when switched to the opposite oxygen environment. Family J shows the opposite trend, with individuals decreasing cardiac output (Fig. 2.3). It is important to note that these assessments are strictly qualitative. We preformed no post-hoc test to determine which environmental sources were significant across sires (ex: reaction norms in fig. 2.3). However, clearly the differences in direction and magnitude of responses seen across sires contributed to a significant interaction between sire and subsequent developmental environments. It is this higher-order interaction which was within the scope of this study. 29 Table 2.1 Analysis of variance for vessel structure and cardiac performance in zebrafish embryos at 48 h.p.f Sources of variation included sire, developmental oxygen for hours 0-24 (Eearly), developmental oxygen for hours 24-48 (Elate), and all possible interactions. For each variable, the first line represents the mean square error and the second line represents the significance level. Source Sire Eearly Sire x Eearly Elate Sire x Elate Eearly x Elate Sire x Eearly x Elate DF Stroke Volume (nl) 37 0.1024 <.0001 1 0.2516 0.0028 10 0.1178 <.0001 1 1.7414 <.0001 10 0.0427 0.1227 1 0.0088 0.5752 10 0.2092 <.0001 Cardiac Output (nl/min) 3354.67 <.0001 4498.97 0.0058 2538.58 <.0001 31063.66 <.0001 1209.85 0.0255 492.40 0.3600 4759.73 <.0001 Arterial Diameter (mm) 4.63 <.0001 9.91 0.2989 2.39 0.0041 1.58 <.0001 1.19 0.2270 4.01 0.0368 1.13 0.2658 Heart Rate (beats/min) 3310.86 <.0001 202.76 0.4441 488.50 0.1700 2053.91 0.0150 1172.23 0.0002 563.83 0.2020 1130.80 0.0004 Venous Diameter (mm) 7.20 <.0001 -5 -5 -6 -5 1.34 0.4267 -5 -5 4.60 0.0181 30 -4 -6 3.74 0.8249 -5 -5 2.53 0.2940 -5 -7 6.90 0.8565 -5 -5 1.48 0.7293 Stroke Volume (nl) 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 Normoxia Hypoxia Fig. 2.2. The influence of developmental oxygen on stroke voulme. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for hours 0-24, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Error bars represent standard errors. 31 40 20 100 80 I 60 40 20 Normoxia Hypoxia 20 100 80 F 60 40 20 100 80 J 60 40 20 Normoxia Hypoxia C 80 60 40 20 100 80 G 60 40 20 100 80 K 60 40 20 Normoxia Cardiac Output (nl/min) 40 Cardiac Output (nl/min) 60 100 Cardiac Output (nl/min) 60 80 Cardiac Output (nl/min) E B Cardiac Output (nl/min) Cardiac Output (nl/min) 80 100 Cardiac Output (nl/min) 20 100 Cardiac Output (nl/min) 40 Cardiac Output (nl/min) 60 Cardiac Output (nl/min) A 80 Cardiac Output (nl/min) C aCr d a ir ad ci a cOOu ut pt pu ut t (( nn ll // m m iInn) ) Cardiac Output (nl/min) 100 Hypoxia 100 D 80 60 40 20 100 80 H 60 40 20 100 80 L 60 40 20 Normoxia Fig. 2.3. The influence of developmental oxygen on cardiac output for half-sib families A-K. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for hours 0-24, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Error bars represent standard errors. Figure 5L represents the mean values across all families. 32 Hypoxia Arterial Diameter (mm) 0.0210 0.0205 0.0200 0.0195 0.0190 0.0185 0.0180 Normoxia Hypoxia Fig. 2.4. The influence of developmental oxygen on arterial diameter. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for hours 0-24, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Error bars represent standard errors. 33 Heart Rate (beats/min) 162 160 158 156 154 152 150 148 Normoxia Hypoxia Fig. 2.5 The influence of developmental oxygen on heart rate. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for hours 0-24, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Error bars represent standard errors. 34 Venous Diameter (mm) 0.0246 0.0244 0.0242 0.0240 0.0238 0.0236 0.0234 0.0232 0.0230 Normoxia Hypoxia Fig. 2.6. The influence of developmental oxygen on venous diameter. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for hours 0-24, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Error bars represent standard errors. 35 Discussion Phenotypic development is a dynamic, hierarchical process involving the genesis of cells, hormones, tissues, and organs (Hall 2003). Environmental input can occur at any stage of development. A specific stage of development may be critical in determining the nature of the physiological response. For example, Chan and Burggren (2005) showed that hypoxia altered chick morphological development (eye size, beak length) as well as yolk mass differentially across three stages of development. Slatkin (1987) proposed the first model integrating quantitative genetics and development. This model examined the effects of selection given quantitative genetic variation in growth rates and transition times between developmental stages (Slatkin 1987). The quantitative genetics of heterochrony provides a tractable means of partitioning the relative effects of genetic and environmental variation in evolutionary developmental studies. While genetic information may be lacking, environmental factors can be controlled and manipulated at various critical windows of development. The partitioning of developmental stages into discrete events provides the ability to partition the relative effects of environments experienced throughout the organism’s ontogeny. Recent studies have emphasized the need to examine developmental plasticity within a more ecological framework (Gilbert 2001, Hoverman and Raylea 2007). This arises from an awareness of the environment’s ability to alter the course of development at any time during an organisms’ ontogeny. Specific interest has focused on the consequences changing ecological factors have on 36 the course of developmental trajectories. Pigliucci (1998) suggested environmental change can constrain developmental plasticity in two ways. One is through ontogenetic drift, in which a maladaptive resource allocation pattern is maintained after the developmental environment changes (Pigliucci 1998). A second process involves a mismatch between developmental windows and environmental cues (Pigliucci 1998). Developmental windows involve the onset of functional regulatory systems which can competently respond to environmental change. Adaptive plasticity would therefore require the onset of these critical points in development. If early developmental conditions are capable of altering the timing of these developmental windows, this may alter the ability of an organism to competently respond to later environmental change in an adaptive manner. Our work shows that the early developing cardiovascular system of zebrafish responds to environmental change in manners dependent on early oxygen conditions. This suggests that early oxygen can alter the timing or nature of regulatory events in cardiovascular development and therefore mediate later responses to environmental change. Switching oxygen environments during early zebrafish development clearly alters cardiovascular architecture and performance. These changes are not simply a function of one particular oxygen environment. Switching oxygen environments constrains and redirects cardiovascular performance uniquely across genotypes. In this manner, cardiovascular performance is a function of the interaction between early and later oxygen environments. The cardiovascular system is essential in supplying developing tissues with adequate oxygen and 37 nutrients. Thus, the heart is the first organ to develop. Proper cardiac responses are therefore essential for proper development and survival (Clark 1990). Under normoxic conditions, bulk diffusion is adequate to supply developing zebrafish tissues with oxygen for the first 2 weeks of development (Pelster and Burggren 1996). Under hypoxic conditions, however, convective transport may become necessary much earlier to compensate for oxygen deficits (Jacob et al 2002). Previous studies imposing hypoxia through incubation (Jacob et al 2002) and exercise (Pelster et al 2003) have confirmed the presence of oxygen sensors controlling cardiac performance as early as 2-3 d.p.f. Our research indicates that the oxygen conditions during the first 24 hours of development can modulate cardiac responses over subsequent days. Although the heart is not functioning or fully developed at 24 h.p.f., conditions during the first few hours of development can be critical in shaping early cardiac performance. Our study revealed variation in the direction and magnitude of response to oxygen changes. This genotypic variation spanning subsequent developmental environments may provide the raw material for natural selection to shape plasticity-induced developmental trajectories. Spicer and Burggren (2003) termed plasticity in the timing of crucial developmental events as heterokairy. Oxygen has displayed the capacity to both up regulate and down regulate development. Although hypoxia has a tendency to slow most developmental events (Pelster 1997, Padilla and Roth 2001), it can accelerate others. Spicer and El-Gamal (1999) showed that hypoxia accelerated the onset of regulated respiration in brine shrimp. Bagatto (2005) found that chronic hypoxia shifted andrenergic 38 responses to occur earlier in zebrafish development. Our study shows that early oxygen conditions can delay or alter cardiovascular responses in developing zebrafish. Since the cardiovascular system is not functioning at 24 h.p.f., there have been no previous attempts to assess the impact of this early window of development on cardiovascular performance. This is the first study, to our knowledge, to show that conditions during the first 24 hours of development can influence subsequent physiological development in zebrafish. Our study elucidates the evolutionary potential within species when early developmental conditions switch between environmental states (i.e. hypoxia vs normoxia). Switching oxygen during development constrained and redirected physiological trajectories in ways that were conditional on the quality of environments experienced throughout ontogeny. Variation across genotypes in the quality of environmentally constrained and redirected responses highlights the potential for selection to shape the fittest developmental trajectory in light of changing environmental conditions. Current models do not account for potential variation within populations. Models such as the ‘silver spoon’ (Grafen 1988) and ‘environmental matching’ (Monaghan 2008) consider average responses at the population level. While such models may apply at larger scales, our results underscore the potential for interspecific variation in the nature of environmentally mediated responses within populations. Therefore, the most beneficial developmental pathway resulting from to environmental change seen at the population or species level may be selected. Specifically, adaptive plasticity can occur in light of subsequent environmental changes. 39 Conclusions and evolutionary implications The overreaching goal of this chapter was to characterize variation across genotypes (sires) in response to changing oxygen condition during early zebrafish development. Indeed, we found that the nature of the constraint imposed by early oxygen conditions in the ability to respond to later oxygen change varied significantly across genpotypes. In the example of cardiac output, this resulted in a significant Sire x Eearly x Elate (G xE x E) interaction. While figure 2.3 is informative for visualizing the effects of developmental conditions for each genotype, it is difficult to visualize the degree of genetic variance. Therefore, the evolutionary implications are not tractable from this figure. The modified approach of splitting these figures by early environmental conditions addresses this issue. The following figures plot cardiac output in hypoxia and normoxia at 48 h.p.f. as a function of early hypoxia and normoxia. Figure 2.7 plots all genotypes and early oxygen states on the same figure. Overall, it appears that variance increases when later oxygen is normoxia. Which early environment contributes to this variance? Figures 2.8 and 2.9 plot only fish exposed to early normoxia and hypoxia, respectively. Figure 2.10 summarizes the variance structure across all four treatments. They indicate that fish exposed to early hypoxia experience increased variance when switched to later normoxia. Thus, early developmental hypoxia appears to influence the amount of variance expressed in a later environment. Selection would therefore have more variants with which to select from when early conditions switch from hypoxia to normoxia. Interestingly, fish exposed to consistent hypoxia display the lowest amount of variance (Fig. 2.10). 40 Indeed, zebrafish are likely to encounter such conditions (see chapter I). Given they are tolerant of anoxia during early development (Padilla and Roth 2001), it is likely they have adapted to hypoxia during early development. This suggests that selection has strongly influenced development in early hypoxia. Overall, these findings provide novel insight into the mechanisms of variance expression during early development in changing environments. 41 Fig. 2.7. Cardiac output of all half sib families. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for hours 0-24, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Each color represents each genotype. Error bars represent standard errors. 42 Fig. 2.8. Cardiac output of all half sib families raised under early normoxic conditions (hours 0-24). The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Each color represents each genotype. Error bars represent standard errors. 43 Fig. 2.9. Cardiac output of all half sib families raised under early hypoxic conditions (hours 0-24). The x axis represents the oxygen environment for hours 24-48. Each color represents each genotype. Error bars represent standard errors. 44 Coeffecient of Variation (cardiac output) 2.2 2.1 2.0 1.9 1.8 Later oxygen Early oxygen H N H H N N Fig. 2.10. Coefficient of variation due to all four combinations of oxygen environments. 45 CHAPTER III ONTOGENETIC PROGRAMMING IN BODY SHAPE OF ZEBRAFISH: THE ROLE OF CHANGING OXYGEN DURING DEVELOPMENT IN SHAPING THE P MATRIX Introduction A fundamental issue in evolutionary biology is the interplay between genotype and environment in determining phenotypic outcomes. Phenotypic plasticity allows organisms with a given genotype to respond to environmental change (Bradshaw 1965; Via 1984). Correlations between traits (i.e. phenotypic correlations) have been targeted as important players in affecting the expression and evolution of plastic responses (Schlichting 1986; Pigliucci et al. 1995). Although plasticity studies have revealed that phenotypic integration can be altered by environmental change (Gianoli and González-Teuber 2005; Tonsor and Scheiner 2007), the genetics of development in changeable ontogenetic environments remains unclear. In lieu of genetic variation, phenotypic plasticity can evolve as an adaptive response to changeable environments (Van Tienderen 1991). Developmental plasticity has been proposed as a source of evolutionary novelty (West-Eberhard, 2003, Carroll 2005). Knowledge of critical windows of development that are particularly sensitive to environmental change reveal that the timing of change 46 can be critical in determining the expression of plastic responses (Pigliucci, 2001). These critical periods, particularly early in development, can result in alternative developmental trajectories (Chan and Burggren 2005), which can have lasting effects on development (Relyea 2001). These trajectories, however, are likely to proceed under subsequent environmental change. The adaptive value of developmental plasticity must therefore be considered in more complex developmental environments. As development proceeds, organisms are likely to encounter environmental variation. An altered developmental trajectory can be adaptive only if the early environment predicts future conditions (Dudley and Schmitt 1996). The influence on development can occur relatively soon, or can manifest itself in delayed effects not seen until later stages of development (Chen et al. 1996; Buckley et al. 2005). If plastic results are delayed, changing environmental conditions can offset otherwise adaptive responses. Recent work has begun to characterize the consequences of plastic responses within populations when the environment changes during ontogeny (Relyea 2001; Hoverman and Relyea 2007; Criscuolo et al. 2008; Monaghan 2008). While these studies underscore the importance of environmental variance, the adaptive meaning of developmental plasticity in complex environments can only be assessed by addressing genetic variance. Organisms are composed of suites of traits which can be genetically, developmentally, and/or functionally linked (Cheverud 1996). These links result in covariances between traits and have been proposed as a source of evolutionary 47 constraint (Arnold 1992; Murren 2002). Quantitative genetics models the multivariate response to selection as Δ z =Gβ, where z is the vector of change in mean trait values following selection, G is the matrix of additive genetic variances and covariances, and β is the vector of selection gradients (Lande 1979). Since multivariate trait correlations can be modulated by environmental change (Pigluicci et al. 1995; Tonsor and Scheiner 2007), plasticity in these correlations may provide the raw material for selection to overcome evolutionary constraints. Here, we test whether oxygen changes during development alter overall morphology and phenotypic integration in zebrafish. Although hypoxia has been shown to alter body shape in fish (Chapman et al 2008, Crispo and Chapman 2011), no study to date has analyzed if the influence of hypoxia is maintained after switching oxygen environments. Our study is the first to characterize the effect of changing developmental oxygen on body shape and integrated morphology (P) in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. We track separate genotypes (sire offspring) across multiple windows of environmental change during development to help elucidate the relative impact of sequential developmental environments as well as the potential for genotype-specific responses. The ability to detect the genetic contribution in this study will further aid in understanding the potential for populations to adapt to heterogeneous conditions during development. Since hypoxia is known to influence body shape, we hypothesize that fish exposed to early hypoxia will experience modified body shapes, however, these modifications will be dependent on later oxygen conditions. 48 Methods We analyzed the effects of changing developmental oxygen on phenotypic integration in 409 zebrafish resulting from 4 paternal half sib and 4 maternal half sib families. Fish were raised in hypoxia for 90 days, normoxia for 90 days, hypoxia for 6 days followed by normoxia for 84 days, or normoxia for 6 days followed by hypoxia for 84 days. While we generally chose our times somewhat arbitrarily, we chose 6 days in this study to represent an ecologically relevant scenario. Following fertilization, zebrafish embryos remain sessile for approximately 5 days, after which they can seek more optimal conditions. Their developmental regime therefore makes it likely that larval and adult oxygen environments can differ. The early developmental window utilized in this experiment (days 0-6) provides an ecologically relevant example of the impact of opposing larval and adult oxygen environments. We used MANCOVA and Flury matrix comparisons to partition the effects of genotype, early, and later developmental environments on overall morphology and phenotypic integration, respectively. Animals Adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) were obtained from Aquatica Tropicals (Plant City, FL) and populations of wild-type and albino were maintained according to standard procedures (Westerfield, 1994). Adults were maintained and bred at 26 °C with a 14L : 10D light cycle. Breeding design 49 Approximately 50 eggs from each clutch were removed before the 8 cell stage and equally divided between developmental treatments. Following 6 days of development, half of the eggs from each treatment were randomly selected and placed in the opposite environment. The remaining eggs were left to develop in the same environment. The larvae were raised under these conditions for the remainder of the experiment (up to 90 days). This resulted in four treatment groups: larvae raised in hypoxia for 90 days (HH), normoxia for 90 days (NN), normoxia for 6 days followed by hypoxia for 84 days (NH), and hypoxia for 6 days followed by normoxia for 84 days (HN). Wildtype males were randomly selected and mated to randomly selected albino females in a full factorial North Carolina II breeding design. Sixteen clutches resulted from 4 paternal half sib and 4 maternal half sib families. Albino females were used due to their large egg production, ease of sexing, and lower genetic variance. The later detail allowed us to contribute most of the genetic variation in this study to sires. Mating pairs were placed in 2 L containers supplied with a common water source (Z-Mod housing system, Marine Biotech, Beverly, MA). Developmental treatments Treatments consisted of normoxic water (dissolved oxygen > 6.0 mg O2 l1 ) and hypoxic water (dissolved oxygen 1.0 ± 0.2 mg O2 l-1). Fish were raised in 154 x 35 x 23 cm tanks divided into 16 chambers. System water was supplied to each chamber with screen covered holes connecting neighboring chambers. This ensured homogenous environmental conditions with the exception of treatments 50 being maintained using a partially separated sump system that allowed mixing of water between treatments. The apparatus for maintaining developmental treatments is further described in Marks et al. (2005). Data collection At 90 days post fertilization, fish were euthanized with an overdose of ms222, weighed, and photographed on each side with a length standard in each picture. Photography was conducted with a Nikon D300 camera with a standard reference scale under standard lighting conditions with a Kodak 18 percent reflectance grey background. Phenotypic measurements Integrated morphology - To characterize integrated morphology, we measured 5 morphometric characters (Fig. 3.1). These included body area, maximum depth, eye area, operculum height, and the distance from the anterior eye socket to the posterior portion of the operculum (eye-operculum distance). We chose these traits as fair representatives of the variety of morphological measurements found in previous studies (Pankhurst et al. 1993; Machiels and Wijsman 1996; Webb et al. 2001; Monna et al. 2011). Measurements were taken 5 times on each side and averaged across both sides. This removed any potential effect of changing asymmetry. Since sex could not always reliably be determined visually (i.e. large males vs small females), sexes were pooled. Measurements were made in imageJ (Version 1.42, NIH). Body shape - On the left side of each subject, 14 landmarks were scored using the software tpsDig2 (Rohlf 2001). We used a generalized least square 51 Procustes fit to correct for variation in scale, position, orientation, and centroid size (Rohlf and Slice 1990). To account for any influence of size on shape, we regressed Procustes coordinates on centroid size and used the residuals for subsequent analysis. To characterize variation in body shape among and between treatment groups, we performed a canonical variate analysis (CVA) and discriminate function analysis (DFA) on regression residuals. These are common ordination techniques used to assess multi-dimensional variation (Klingenberg et al. 2003). These analyses reduce the data to fewer dimensions and allow for simpler, 2-dimensional analysis. These analyses were performed in MorphoJ v. 1.01c (Klingenberg 2008). We used the software tpsRelw v. 1.49 (Rohlf, 2010) to calculate weight matrices consisting of partial warp scores. These weight matrices were used for a multivariate analysis (described below). 52 Fig. 3.1. Landmarks and morphological measurements on photographed zebrafish. Landmarks: (1) Anterior tip of upper jaw; (2) anterior base of dorsal fin; (3) posterior base of dorsal fin; (4) dorsal base of caudal fin; (5) medial spot of caudal fin(aligned with midpoint of medial stripe); (6) ventral base of caudal fin; (7) posterior base of anal fin; (8) anterior base of anal fin; (9) base of left pelvic fin; (10) ventral tip of operculum; (11) distal tip of angular; (12) posterior tip of operculum; (13) posterior portion of eye socket; (14) anterior portion of eye socket. Morphological measurements: (A) Eye area; (B) distance from dorsal to ventral operculum opening(operculum height); (C) distance from anterior portion of eye socket (Eye-operculum (EO) distance); (D) maximum depth; (E) body area (excluding fins). 53 Analysis of means Integrated morphology - A three-way MANCOVA was performed to test for effects on integrated morphology. Sire, early environment (Eearly, days 0-6), later environment (Elate, days 6-90), and all possible interactions were included as potential sources of variation. Post hoc univariate ANOVAs tested the effects on individual traits. Sire, dam, and all inclusive interactions were treated as random factors. Early environment, later environment, and their interaction were treated as fixed factors. To remove the effect of size, body length was included as a covariate. Assumptions of normality were assessed and variables were log transformed as needed. Body shape - A three-way MANOVA was performed on weight matrices to test for effects on body shape. Sire, early environment (Eearly, days 0-6), later environment (Elate, days 6-90), and all possible interactions were included as potential sources of variation. Both of these tests were performed in SAS software package version 9.0 using PROC GLM (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). For both tests, if early environment affects variation in adult phenotype, we expect to find significant Eearly components. If later environment influences adult phenpotype, we expect to find significant Elate effects. If there is genetic variation for these environmentally mediated responses, we expect to find significant G x Eearly and/or G x Elate interactions. Another possibility is that later responses to developmental oxygen are altered by early oxygen conditions. In this case, we expect to find significant Eearly x Elate interactions. If there is genetic variation in 54 the manner in which these cross-environmental responses manifest, we expect to find significant G x Eearly X Elate interactions. Matrix comparisons In addition to analyzing means, we analyzed the effect of subsequent developmental environments and genetic variance on phenotypic covariance matrices (P). Numerous studies have found correlations between P (phenotypic) and G matrices (Cheverud 1988; Reusch and Blanckenhorn 1998; Waitt and Levin 1998). We therefore use the P matrix as a reasonable estimator of the G matrix in this study. While there are many matrix comparison methods available, the Flury method remains one of the most widely used (Phillips and Arnold 1999). Whereas most methods compare matrices for equality, Flury considers a hierarchy of possible differences. It compares the matrices based on five possible levels of similarity: 1) equality; in which the matrices share eigenvectors and eigenvalues; 2) proportionality, in which the matrices share eigenvectors and all their eigenvalues differ by a proportional constant; 3) common principle components, in which the matrices share eigenvectors but do not share proportional eigenvalues; 4) partial common principal components, in which matrices share some eigenvectors; and 5) unrelatedness, in which none of the above criteria apply (Flury, 1988). We used this method to compare P matrices of full-sib half-sib groups reared in all possible environmental combinations (Table 2). For example, AHN represents the covariance matrix for the half-sibs of sire A raised in hypoxia for days 0-6 and normoxia for days 6-90. We also compared the matrices for all families pooled in each combination of oxygen environments 55 (HH, HN, NH, NN). This tested the average effect of changing oxygen conditions across all families. P matrices were calculated in JMP 8.0. Flury analysis was performed with the program CPC (available at http://darkwing.uoregon.edu/~pphil/programs/cpc/cpc.htm). Phillips and Arnold (1999) present three methods for determining the relationship between matrices in the Flury hierarchy. One is the step up approach, in which the model is compared to the next lowest model in the hierarchy and a move up the hierarchy is made only if the result is not significant. Another is the jump up approach. This is similar to the step up, except that each model is compared to an unrelated structure rather than the next lowest one. The final method is the Akaike information criterion (AIC). Similar to a log likelihood model, AIC adjusts the goodness of fit according to the numbers of parameters estimated (Flury 1988). All three models have their advantages and disadvantages. Phillips and Arnold (1999) recommend the jump up approach because it is most appropriate for testing a single hypothesis and provides the simplest interpretation of results. Results Matings resulted in 16 full-sib, half-sib families consisting of 94 individuals raised completely in hypoxia (HH), 107 individuals raised in hypoxia followed by normoxia (HN), 100 individuals raised in normoxia followed by hypoxia (NH), and 108 individuals raised completely in normoxia (NN). Integrated morphology Many factors contributed significantly to variance in integrated phenotype. Significant sire component indicates the presence of genetic variation for overall 56 morphology. Significant Eearly and sire x Eearly components indicate there is plasticity in overall morphology due to early oxygen conditions and these plastic responses are genotype specific. The same pattern holds true for Elate and sire x Elate. While there was not a significant Eearly x Elate interaction, there was genetic variation for cross-environmental responses (sire x Eearly x Elate) (Table 3.1). 57 Table 3.1. Multivariate analysis of variance for zebrafish morphology and body shape. Sources of variation included sire, developmental environment for days 06 (Eearly), developmental environment for days 6-90 (Elate), and all possible interactions. The first two rows depict the numerator and denominator degrees of freedom. Sire x Sire x Eearly x Length Sire Eearly Eearly Elate Elate Elate Num/ 69 23 69 23 69 23 Den DF 965.81 323 965.81 323 965.81 323 <.0001 0.1344 0.0576 <.0001 0.1744 0.1879 Shape Num/ 5 15 5 15 5 15 5 Den DF 338 933.47 338 933.47 338 933.47 338 Morphology <.0001 <.0001 0.0006 <.0001 <.0001 0.0002 0.0823 58 Sire x Eearly x Elate 69 965.81 0.0063 15 933.47 0.0454 Univariate means All univariate traits showed significant genetic variation in the form of sire effects (Table 3.2). Eye-operculum distance and PC2 were the only variables influenced solely by early oxygen. Maximum depth, operculum size, eye-operculum distance, and PC2 showed genetic variation in plastic responses due to early oxygen. Body area, max depth, eye area, and PC2 exhibited plasticity due to later oxygen (Table 3.2). Body area, maximum depth, operculum size, and PC2 exhibited significant genotypic specific variation in plastic responses due to later oxygen (Table 3.2). Averaging across genotypes, maximum depth, eye area, and PC1 showed plastic responses to later oxygen which were conditional on early oxygen (Table 3.2). In all 3 instances, fish raised in early normoxia showed little or no response to switching to hypoxia. For example, in early hypoxia, however, increased PC1 when switched to later normoxia (Fig. 3.2). PC1 also showed genotype-specific variation in altered responses across early and later oxygen environments (sire x Eearly x Elate). Individuals from sires A and D reared in early normoxia showed an increase in PC1 when switched to hypoxia. Individuals from sire B showed a decrease. Individuals from from sire C remained unresponsive. Individuals from sires A, C, and D reared in early hypoxia showed an increase in PC1 when switched to later normoxia. Individuals from sire B showed the opposite trend (Fig. 3.3). 59 Table 3.2. Analysis of variance results for univariate traits (body area, maximum depth, eye area, operculum height, eye-operculum distance, PC1, and PC2). Sources of variation included sire, dam, developmental environment for days 0-6 (Eearly), developmental environment for days 6-90 (Elate), and all possible interactions. Where appropriate, the effect of size was removed by including length as a covariate Log transformations were applied prior to analysis for all variables with the exceptions of PC1 and PC2. DF Body Area Max Depth Eye Area Operc. Ht. EO Dist. PC1 PC2 Elate 1 Sire x Elate 3 Eearly x Elate 1 Sire x Eearly x Elate 3 Length 1 Sire 3 Eearly 1 Sire x Eearly 3 <.0001 0.0022 0.2904 0.4141 0.0002 0.0190 0.6926 0.0892 <.0001 <.0001 <.0001 <.0001 0.3077 0.0983 0.0269 0.4495 <.0001 0.0350 0.0045 0.4264 0.0369 0.0060 0.5638 0.0404 <.0001 <.0001 <.0001 0.0284 0.0107 <.0001 0.4251 0.0134 0.6962 0.0109 0.0005 0.0029 0.2133 <.0001 0.5858 0.0501 0.1754 0.0491 0.0068 0.1497 0.6038 0.0209 0.2899 0.9880 0.0281 0.2660 0.5733 0.0683 0.0209 0.1193 60 Phenotypic covariances For pooled families, the eigenstructure varied across developmental treatments (Table 3.3). P matrices for NN fish were equal to HH and NH fish (P = 0.1970, 0.0649 respectively). NN fish also shared 3 common principal components with HN fish (P = 0.0554). P matrices for NH fish were equal HH fish (P = 0.5006). However, they shared only one principal component with HN fish (P = 0.0701). HN fish shared common principal components with HH fish (P = 0.0665). Partitioning across all half sib families, there were a total of 120 matrix comparisons, 40 which showed no shared structure (Table 3.4). This matrix provides insight into which specific factors contributed to unequal variance structure in this study. For example, individuals from sire C exposed to continual hypoxia (CHH) equaled individuals from the same sire exposed to hypoxia followed by normoxia (CHN). However, They shared no common variance structure with individuals from the same sire raised under normoxia followed by hypoxia (CNH). Thus, the first 6 days of oxygen proved more influential in altering this particular genotype’s variance structure. 61 Table 3.3. Results of pair wise Flury comparisons for all half sib families pooled by developmental treatment. The first row and column represents the treatment. HH = hypoxia for 90 days, HN = hypoxia for days 0-6 and normoxia for days 690, NH = normoxia for days 0-6 and hypoxia for days 6-90, and NN = normoxia for 90 days. For comparison results, E = matrix equality, C = common principal components, and 1-3 = the number of shared principal components. HN NH NN HH C E E HN NH 1 3 E 62 Table 3.4. Results of pair wise Flury comparisons for all half sib families divided among all four developmental treatments. The first row and column represents the sire, the second row and column represents oxygen conditions for days 0-6, the third row and column represents oxygen for days 6-90, and the fourth row and column represents the number of fish sampled. For comparison results, E = matrix equality, P = proportionality, C = common principal components, 1-4 = the number of shared principal components, and N = no shared structure. A A A B B B B C C C C D D D D H N N H H N N H H N N H H N N N H N H N H N H N H N H N H N 23 20 24 25 31 31 30 23 23 24 29 27 28 27 25 A H H 19 2 E E N N 2 1 N 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 A H N 23 A N H 20 A N N 24 B H H 25 B H N 31 B N H 31 B N N 30 C H H 23 C H N 23 C N H 24 C N N 29 D H H 27 D H N 28 D N H 27 1 C 1 1 1 N E E N C 1 N C 1 E 1 C E E 1 1 N N E N 1 N E C P E C E N E E C 4 N 2 1 E 1 E N N 1 N 1 N E E E 2 N N 1 N C N E E 1 N E 1 N N N E C N C 1 C C C E N E N C E C N E N 2 1 N N N N N N E E E N N P N N N 1 63 Shape Multivariate analysis revealed that body shape showed significant genotypic variation (sire). Shape also showed plasticity to due later oxygen E late. There was also significant genotype-specific cross-environmental responses to oxygen changes (Table 3.1). Procustes distances comparing individuals pooled by environments revealed that HH fish differed in shape compared to HN and NN fish. NH fish also significantly differed in shape compared to HN and NN fish (Table 3.5). In a specific comparison, NN fish differed from HH fish by dorsal transpositions in landmarks 2, 3, and 12 (Fig. 3.5). 64 0.8 0.6 PC1 0.4 0.2 0.0 -0.2 -0.4 -0.6 Normoxia Hypoxia Fig. 3.2. The influence of developmental oxygen on PC1. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for days 0-6, respectively. The x axis represents the oxygen environment for days 6-90. Error bars represent standard errors. 65 1.5 1.0 PC1 0.5 Sire A Sire B Sire C Sire D 0.0 -0.5 -1.0 -1.5 Normoxia Hypoxia Fig. 3.3. The influence of developmental oxygen on PC1 for half sib families A-D. Dashed and solid lines represent fish raised in hypoxia and normoxia for days 06, respectively. The horizontal axis represents the oxygen environment for days 6-90. Error bars represent standard errors. 66 Table 3.5. Pairwise Procustes distances between environmental treatments. The first row and column represents the treatment. HH = hypoxia for 90 days, HN = hypoxia for days 0-6 and normoxia for days 6-90, NH = normoxia for days 0-6 and hypoxia for days 6-90, and NN = normoxia for 90 days. Asterisks indicate level of significance (* = <0.05, † = <0.01). HH HN NH HN 0.0098* NH 0.0081 0.0107† NN 0.0117† 0.0062 0.0128† 67 0.8 NH CV2 (16.1%) 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 HN NN -0.2 HH -0.4 -0.6 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 CV1 (76.8%) Fig. 3.4. Effect of developmental environment (HH, HN, NH, NN) on the canonical variate scores. Mean + SE of the canonical scores are shown. 68 Fig. 3.5. Comparison of body shape of HH vs NN fish. Vectors depict the components of shape change based on a discriminant analysis. 69 Procustes distances comparing individuals pooled by sire and environments revealed sire, Eearly, Elate, and all combinations of factors contributed significantly to variation in body shape. Of the 120 comparisons, 82 differed in shape (Table 3.6). Variation in shape due to Elate was evident along the first canonical axis (76.8% explained variance). Variation along the second canonical axis (16.1% explained variation) was due to Eearly (Fig. 3.4). Landmark comparisons reveal more specific effects of developmental treatments. Across treatments, shape variation was mainly attributed to landmarks 2, 3, 8, and 9. When Elate was switched to normoxia, individuals tended to produce deeper profiles. This is evident in HH vs. HN, HH vs. NN, and NH vs. NN comparisons. HH vs. NH and HN vs. NN comparisons reveal that the effects of different early environments were not as pronounced (appendix). Completely opposite developmental regimes (HN vs. NH) resulted in ventral transpositions at landmarks 8 and 9 and a slight dorsolateral transposition in landmark 3. 70 71 Discussion The changing oxygen conditions applied during this experiment are within those experienced during the ontogeny of zebrafish. Zebrafish are subjected to variation in a number of environmental factors including oxygen, temperature, and turbidity (Talwar and Jhingran 1991). Their association with rice fields (Spence at al., 2008) makes them likely to experience anthropogenic hypoxia. Laboratory tests using native soils as substrate have shown that conditions reach anoxia with 96 hours (Strecker 2011). Padilla and Roth (2001) demonstrated that zebrafish embryos can survive and recover from anoxia for 24 hours of development. These findings make it reasonable to assume that zebrafish larvae are adapted to developmental hypoxia. Following fertilization, zebrafish embryos remain sessile for approximately 5 days, after which they can seek more optimal conditions. Their developmental regime therefore makes it likely that larval and adult oxygen environments can differ. The early developmental window utilized in this experiment (days 0-6) therefore provides an ecologically relevant example of the impact of opposing larval and adult oxygen environments. Ontogenetic oxygen changes clearly alter body shape and phenotypic integration in zebrafish. For many of these changes, the response to later developmental oxygen is contingent upon the oxygen condition early during development. These results are seen in significant Eearly x Elate interactions for eye area, maximum depth, and PC1 (Table 3.2). Furthermore, numerous traits show significant genetic variation for these responses to oxygen changes in the form of sire x Eearly x Elate interactions (Tables 3.1 & 3.2). For zebrafish, which 72 inhabit a wide variety of habitats (Talwar and Jhingran 1991), this may have profound implications on morphological evolution. Oxygen is known to have direct effects on fish morphology. Hypoxia has been shown to alter gill morphology in sea bass (Rinaldi et al. 2005), carp (Sollid et al., 2003), African cichlids (Chapman et al. 2008), and sailfin mollies (Timmerman and Chapman 2004). Chapman et al (2008) found skull variation correlated with gill plasticity in African cichlids. Although zebrafish gills are not fully developed by day 14 (Vulesevic and Perry 2006), neural epithelial cells in primordial gill filaments are actively expressed at 5 d.p.f. with associated hyperventilatory responses to hypoxia (Jonz and Nurse 2005). We found variation in jaw and gill covering morphology across treatments (Fig. 3.5; landmarks 10, 11, 12, & 13). While we did directly assess skeletal morphology, our results indicate the presence of oxygen-mediated morphological plasticity. The influence of oxygen, however, was contingent on the unique combination of oxygen environments. On average, variation in body shape was more influenced by later oxygen (Table 3.1). Hypoxia has been shown to decrease feeding in bass (Pichavant et al. 2001) salmon (Jobling, 1994) and turbot (Pichavant et al. 2000). Metabolic alterations including reduced aerobic respiration (Mohamed and Kutty, 1983; Pincetich et al. 2005), reduced protein synthesis (Storey 1988), and reduced lipolytic rates (Van Raaij et al. 1996) are common responses to hypoxia across species. While early oxygen did not solely significantly alter body shape (Table 3.1), partitioning across genotypes reveals that some genotypes proved more 73 sensitive to early oxygen conditions. Individuals from sires A and D differed in body shape despite sharing a common later oxygen environment (AHH vs ANH, DHH vs DNH) (Table 3.5). The significant sire x Eearly x Elate interaction for body shape (Table 3.1) highlights the importance of both early and later oxygen environments in determining body shape. The sire component, however, indicates genetic variation for these responses. In other words, some genotypes were more influenced by early than later oxygen. This is evident in maximum depth, where the direction and magnitude of the response to switching environments were genotype specific (Fig. 3.6; appendix). Flury comparisons revealed a number of P matrices that shared no common structure (Table 3.4). These inequalities resulted from all sources of variation, including their various combinations. These results indicate that phenotypic (and likely genetic) covariance structures within populations can be altered by numerous sources of input throughout ontogeny. Plasticity in character correlations such as these may provide the raw material for natural selection to optimize integrated phenotypes. G matrices have been found to be fairly stable across closely related species and different at higher taxanomic levels (Reviewed in Steppan et al. 2002). The observed variation covariance structure in our study provides novel insight into the role of ecology and development in guiding micro and macroevolutionary patterns. Ecological input, as a guiding force of development, can redirect or canalize integrated phenotypic development in manners dependent upon the contexts of both early and later developmental conditions 74 contingent upon specific genotypes. PC1, for example, displays both plastic and canalized responses (Fig. 3.7). While individuals raised in early normoxia exhibit no response when switched to hypoxia, individuals raised in early hypoxia increase eye area when switched to hypoxia. Partitioning across genotypes, however, reveals that the level of canalization varies across genotypes (Fig. 8). Given the diverse habitats of zebrafish as well as their wide distribution (Talwar and Jhingran 1991), our results indicate the potential for G and P to evolve as developmental plasticity varies within populations. Recent studies have addressed the consequences early developmental conditions have on adult fitness (Hales and Barker 2001; Relyea 2003; Hoverman and Relyea 2008). Plastic responses to early conditions have been shown to increase adult fitness when the inducing environment predicts future conditions (Moran 1992; Kingslover and Huey 1998; Relyea 2011). Recent interest has focused on the ramifications of mismatches between juvenile and adult developmental environments (Monaghan 2008). Hypotheses have been proposed modeling fitness outcomes when these conditions prevail. The silver spoon hypothesis predicts that individuals raised in optimal conditions will always have a fitness advantage regardless of later conditions (Grafen 1988). The environmental matching hypothesis predicts that individuals will always perform best in the environment in which they were raised and do poorly in the opposite environment (Monaghan 2008). While these models may apply to populations in general, they do not account for potential variation within populations. Our results demonstrate that genotypes respond uniquely to mismatches between juvenile 75 and adult oxygen environments. For instance, while PC1 of some genotypes is more canalized by early oxygen, for others PC1 responds to later oxygen. The direction and magnitude of plastic response, however, is contingent upon unique combinations of subsequent developmental environments (Fig. 3.3). This indicates that knowledge of both early and later developmental environments is critical in determining proximate causes of observed phenotypic outcomes. Most importantly, genotype can dictate the how canalized or plastic development in changeable environments can be. We found that zebrafish body shape and integrated morphology are modified by changes in developmental oxygen during development and the nature of these modifications is contingent on the organism’s genotype. The interaction between sire, early, and later oxygen environments results in one of the most early noted examples of G x E x E interactions. Genetic variation therefore plays a critical role in determining the level of canalization within a population during environmental change. Methodologically, we highlight the importance of incorporating dynamic environmental histories into quantitative genetic studies. Variation in the way genotypes respond to environmental change during development provides critical insight into the evolution of phenotypes in complex environments. While this study analyzes morphological responses, further studies should seek to test hypotheses regarding fitness and adaptive responses. 76 Conclusions and evolutionary implications Figure 3.3 (PC1; overall morphology) represents a modified reaction norm described in chapter 1. While it demonstrates that that nature of interactions between subsequent developmental environments varies across sires, it also yields insight into the evolutionary potential of zebrafish morphology under changing oxygen conditions. Since the reaction norms are largely crossing with changes in rank order across environments, selection would have just as much variation in both later hypoxia and normoxia with which to select optimal morphologies. While the variance structures of PC1 look qualitatively similar in figure 3.5, they are actually quite different (Fig. 3.6). Plotting them indicates that variance is highest when oxygen switches from hypoxia to normoxia. This result is similar to chapter II, in which variance in cardiac output was highest when the environment switched from hypoxia to normoxia. Also similar to chapter II, the lowest variance is expressed in consistent hypoxia. This again implies that hypoxia has been a major force in shaping morphology in zebrafish. Acknowledgements We than Tim Astrop for his help with analyzing morphology data. 77 Variance (PC1) 2.2 2.1 2.0 1.9 1.8 Later oxygen Early oxygen H H N H N N Figure 3.6. Variance due to all four combinations of oxygen environments. 78 CHAPTER IV ONTOGENETIC OXYGEN CHANGES ALTER ZEBRAFISH BEHAVIOR, PHYSIOLOGY, AND MORPHOLOGY Introduction In a given environment, the fitness of an organism is determined by the manner in which it budgets many potential behaviors in response to environmental conditions. Behavioral requirements such as predator avoidance, grouping, foraging, and mating all present unique combinations of potentially conflicting external factors that require context-specific assessment strategies (Lima and Dill 1990; Vainikka et al. 2005). Behavioral responses are the result of the underlying physiological properties of an organism’s sensory, cognitive, and motor abilities (Alais et al. 2010). Examining the factors that influence the development of these modules is critical to further understanding biological complexity. Development proceeds from cells to tissues to organs to systems, all of which underlie the prerequisites for complex behaviors. While genetics provides the basic instructions for this unfolding, environmental input may alter the course of development (Pigliucci 1998). Developmental plasticity provides one mechanism for organisms to buffer environmental change (West-Eberhard 2003). Genotype-environment interactions provide the basis for natural selection to 79 optimize the most appropriate plastic responses (Schlichting and Pigliucci 1998). Environmental conditions can potentially change at any point during development. Recent studies have begun to characterize phenotypic development as a consequence of changing environments. While some traits display seemingly infinite responsiveness (Sollid and Nilsson 2006) , others may be permanently “programmed” according to early developmental conditions (Jones and Ozanne 2009). Both of these findings suggest a dynamic relationship between environment and development. Similar developmental mechanisms across vertebrates pose tractable sources of investigation for studying genetic and environmental sources of both physiological development and evolution (Burggren 2000). Organisms are comprised of suites of traits which may be correlated (Gould and Lewontin 1979). These correlations can be due to genetic, developmental, and functional links (Pigliucci 2003). Underlying physiological mechanisms provide a proximate target for understanding the integrated nature of biological development. Hormones, for example, may co-regulate the physiology, morphology, and behavior of developing organisms (Ketterson et al. 2009). Hormone action can be modulated through external programming during early development. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA), for example, can be irreversibly programmed via early nutrition, alcohol exposure, or stressful conditions (Hellemans et al. 2010; Jones and Ozanne 2009; Tanaka et al. 2010). While the implications of early epigenetic reprogramming on adult behavior and 80 physiology have been well documented (Kapoor et al. 2006), the role of genetic variation in these mechanisms requires further elucidation. Hypoxia is a ubiquitous environmental factor affecting the development of a wide range of fish species. In fish, hypoxia has been shown to alter genetic, physiological, and behavioral outcomes (Wu 2002). Genetic responses include the induction of glycolytic genes and the down regulation of structural and metabolic proteins (Ton et al. 2003). Physiological responses include reduced ATP concentration, increased androgen production, increased blood glucose, and altered Na+-K+ concentrations (Smit and Hattingh 1978; Wu 2002; Silkin and Silkina 2005). Behaviorally, fish reduce foraging, alter swimming behavior, and reduce aggressive encounters (Bejda et al. 1987). Chronically hypoxic fish also tend to be smaller and experience delayed development (Shang and Wu 2004). While the overall effect of hypoxia has been well documented, the developmental consequences of changing oxygen have only recently been explored. Developmentally hypoxic zebrafish, for example, spend more time hiding in the presence of a mirror image regardless of the oxygen environment they are tested in (Marks et al. 2005). Given the far-reaching effects of hypoxia on the development, it is important to consider multiple traits spanning many biological hierarchies. Studies examining the role of changeable environments in directing phenotypic development have only recently begun. On average, individuals can compensate for poor early developmental conditions by accelerating development if conditions become more favorable. The costs of these responses, 81 however, remain poorly understood (Metcalfe and Monaghan 2001). Qualitative models predicting the consequences of such changes have been proposed (Monaghan 2008), however, the evolutionary consequences of these models cannot be examined until the underlying genetics of these responses have been explored. One promising way to explore these relationships involves studying the effects of shifting environmental conditions across separate genotypes within a population. While a few studies have examined the potential for interactive effects across developmental environments (Yang 1993; Marks et al. 2005; Taborsky 2006; Kotrschal and Taborsky 2010) , none have included a genetic component. Genotype-environment studies have provided quantitative predictions on the evolution of plasticity. Recent environment-environment studies, however, have provided only qualitative predictions about plasticity. If there is genetic variation for environment-environment responses, this would provide natural selection with the raw material to select the response with the lowest cost. It is therefore essential to add a genetic component to the growing literature of environment-environment interactions. In this study, we examine the effects of changing developmental oxygen on zebrafish behavior, physiology, and morphology. We utilize a modified quantitative genetic approach to assess the relative contributions of genotype, and early and later oxygen conditions on integrated phenotypic development. Zebrafish are native to Southeast Asia and inhabit a wide variety of habitats including rivers, streams, and stagnant rice fields (Engeszer et al. 2007b). The wide range of oxygen concentrations they encounter makes them a tractable 82 model for studying the effects of shifting oxygen conditions on development. Zebrafish respond genetically and physiologically to hypoxia early in development ( Ton et al. 2003; Bagatto 2005; Moore et al. 2006;). These alterations can have lasting effects on adult physiology and physical performance (Widmer et al. 2006). Our approach yields insights into role that changing ecological parameters can play in shaping integrated adult morphology, physiology, and behavior, as well as the underlying genetic contributions. Materials and Methods Animals Adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) were obtained from Aquatica Tropicals (Plant City, FL) and populations of wildtype and albino strains were maintained according to standard procedures (Westerfield, 1994). Adults were maintained and bred at 26 ± 0.5°C with a 14L : 10D light cycle. Breeding design Four wildtype males were randomly selected and mated to four randomly selected albino females in a 4x4 full factorial design. Albino females were used due to their large egg production and ease of sexing. Mating pairs were placed in 2 L containers lined with a marble substrate and supplied with a common water source (Z-Mod housing system, Marine Biotech, Beverly, MA). Developmental treatments Approximately 50 eggs from each clutch were removed before the 8 cell stage and randomly divided between developmental treatments. One treatment consisted of normoxic water (dissolved oxygen > 6.0 mg O2 l-1) and the other 83 consisted of hypoxic water (dissolved oxygen 1.0 ± 0.2 mg O2 l-1). The apparatus for maintaining developmental treatments is described in Marks et al. (2005). Fish were raised in 154 x 35 x 23 cm tanks divided into 16 chambers. System water was supplied to each chamber with screen covered holes providing homogeneous conditions between neighboring chambers. Since food could not travel among the chambers or reach the tank’s outflow, multiple daily feedings were found to be excessive. Fish were therefore fed once daily with a custom diet of ground standard zebrafish food with nutrient additives. Excess food was siphoned from the bottom of each chamber daily. Following 30 days of development (E0-30), half of the eggs from each treatment were randomly selected and placed in the opposite environment. The remaining eggs were left to develop in the same environment. Fish were left under these conditions for the remainder of the experiment (90 days; E30-90). This resulted in four treatment groups: fish raised in hypoxia for 90 days (HH), normoxia for 90 days (NN), normoxia for 30 days followed by hypoxia for 60 days (NH), and hypoxia for 30 days followed by normoxia for 60 days (HN). Data collection Behavioral assay – We combined previous behavioral paradigms which assessed fish shoaling (Godin and Morgan 1985; Krause and Godin 1994) and novel stimulus response (Frost et al. 2007; Sneddon et al. 2003; Wright et al. 2003). At 90 days, fish were subjected to a behavioral assay which measured their swimming and shoaling behavior before, during, and after the presentation of a novel stimulus. 84 Fig. 4.1. Overview of behavioral arena. The starting area consists of a 5 cm diameter PVC tube. After a one minute accommodation period, each subject was released to explore the arena. The upper half represents the shoaling half with shoaling tank containing 5 conspecifics. Lower half contains a plastic plant for refuge. 85 For each behavioral session, subjects were randomly netted and placed in a 5 cm PVC tube which was immersed vertically in the center of a 30.5 x 30.5 x 30.5 cm white, opaque polyethylene tank. At one side of the tank was a smaller 16 x 7.5 x 7.5 cm clear polyethylene tank containing 5 randomly selected conspecifics. The conspecifics were chosen randomly each day from a separate system raised under standard conditions. Along the opposite wall was a plastic aquarium plant (see Fig. 4.1). Following a 1 minute accommodation period, the PVC tube was lifted and the subject was allowed to freely explore the arena. The behavioral experiment was conducted in 3 phases: 1) pre-stimulus, 2) stimulus introduction, and 3) stimulus removal. Behavior was recorded with a Samsung SC-HMX10C digital camcorder that was mounted 50 cm above the tank. An LED light illuminated the tank from below. The arena was continuously supplied with system water from the subject’s most recent developmental environment. Flow was adjusted to supply fresh water to the arena without disturbing the behavior of the fish. Phase 1: Pre-stimulus - Positive shoaling behavior was initiated once the subject spent 3 or more consecutive seconds within a 3 cm perimeter of the conspecifics’ tank (see Fig. 4.1). If the subject did not shoal within 3 minutes of filming, the experiment was terminated and the subject was labeled a nonshoaler. If the subject initiated shoaling, the subject was allowed to shoal or explore the arena for 30 seconds and experiment proceeded to phase 2. Phase 2: Stimulus introduction – Thirty seconds after shoaling was initiated, a novel stimulus was introduced to the shoaling area. Stimuli of various 86 shapes and colors have been used as novel fright stimuli in previous fish studies (Frost et al. 2007; Sneddon et al. 2003). Our stimulus consisted of five 14.2 g lead weights wrapped in orange rubber, and was suspended just above the water line prior to delivery. The experimenter manually released the weights, which were all tied to a common releasing mechanism, resulting in simultaneous deployment into the tank. The releasing mechanism was outside the tank and not in the field of view of the subject. The weights were allowed to free fall and designed to rest 2 cm above the arena floor. The weights were distributed evenly around the perimeter of the shoaling tank, with 3 in the front and 1 on each side. Following 30 seconds, the experiment proceeded to phase 3. Phase 3: Stimulus removal - The experimenter slowly returned the weights above the water line via the external mechanism. The subject was recorded for another 60 seconds, at which time the behavioral assay was terminated. Video analysis - On the video screen, a 3 cm perimeter was drawn around the conspecific tank and defined as the shoaling area (see Fig. 4.1). A horizontal line was drawn at the midpoint of the arena, dividing the arena into two sides. The shoaling side was defined as the side with the shoaling tank and the other side contained the plastic plant. Recorded dependent variables included whether the subject shoaled (phase 1), latency to shoal (phase 1), percentage time swimming (phases 1, 2, 3), percentage time on shoaling side of arena (phases 1, 2, 3), percentage time shoaling (phases 1, 2, 3), number of re-entries (per second) to the shoaling side of the arena (phases 1, 2, 3), percentage time hiding in the plant (phases 1, 2, 3), and reaction to the stimulus (phase 2). We defined 87 reaction to the stimulus as any sudden burst swimming (i.e. suddenly exiting of the shoaling area) that occurred immediately following deployment of the stimulus. Re-entry to the shoaling side of the arena was recorded if the entire body length of the subject crossed the midpoint line and entered the plant side of the arena followed by re-entry to the shoaling side. We define this behavior herein as “crossing”. Morphological measurements – Immediately following the behavioral assay, the subject was euthanized with MS-222( 300 mg/ml tricaine methane sulfonate buffered to a neutral pH with sodium bicarbonate), weighed, and photographed on the subject’s right side with a length standard in each picture. Photography was conducted with a Nikon D300 camera under standard lighting conditions with a Kodak 18 percent reflectance grey background. We measured standard length from most anterior point to the base of the hypural plate at caudal flexion. Maximum depth was measured as the maximum dorsal-ventral distance measured along the flank. All measurements were made using imageJ (Version 1.42, NIH). Measurements were made 5 times and the mean was recorded. Blood glucose – Glucose measurements were made with a Nova Max blood glucose monitor (Bedford, MA) according to manufacturer’s instructions. Immediately following photography, the subject was bisected through the pectoral girdle and the exposed dorsal artery at the posterior portion of the head was applied to the test strip. 88 Statistics Behavior – A three way mixed model repeated measures ANOVA was performed to test for effects on overall shoaling behavior. Two individuals were removed from this analysis due to failure of the alarm stimulus to properly deploy. For purposes of this study, we were interested in quantifying genetic variation for cross-environmental responses. Since sires donate purely genotypic effects and dams donate both genetic and maternal effects, we dropped dam from all models. This allowed us to assess purely additive genetic variance. Sire, early oxygen for days 0-30 (E0-30), later oxygen for days 30-90 (E30-90), and all possible interactions were included as potential sources of variation. Post hoc TukeyKramer analyses provided adjusted p values estimating the significance of all factors at each stage of the experiment. Residuals were analyzed for normality and transformations were applied as needed. We used chi-squared tests to determine associations between both stimulus reactivity and shoaling tendency and developmental treatments. Morphology – To assess the effects of genetic and environmental variation on overall size, we reduced the dimensionality of the data using principal components analysis. Mass, maximum depth, and body length were included as dependent variables. Subsequent scores from the first principal component were used in a three way mixed model ANOVA. Sire, early oxygen for days 0-30 (E030), later oxygen for days 30-90 (E30-90), and all possible interactions were included as potential sources of variation. 89 Blood Glucose – Blood glucose did not differ between shoaling and nonshoaling fish (T-test P=0.28). For shoaling fish, no behavioral variables exhibited a significant correlation with blood glucose. We therefore pooled shoaling and non-shoaling fish for blood glucose analysis. We regressed all continuous dependent variables (including mass) on blood glucose. Blood glucose correlated positively with mass (P <0.0001) and negatively with percentage time spent swimming (P=0.0006). These two factors were therefore included as covariates. This removed the effects of mass and swimming on blood glucose in determining genetic and environmental sources of variation. A three way mixed model ANCOVA was used to estimate the effects of sire, early oxygen for days 0-30 (E030), later oxygen for days 30-90 (E30-90), and all possible interactions on blood glucose. All data analyses were performed in SAS software package (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) using PROC MIXED (ANOVAs) and PROC FREQ (chisquared). Results Survival After starting with equal egg numbers across treatments, we ended with 100 fish in HH, 100 fish in HN, 88 fish in NH, and 101 fish in NN. (Chi-squared test revealed no association between treatments and mortality (chi-square = 1.86, P = 0.602) Behavior Shoaling tendency - Of the 389 fish in the study, 60% initiated shoaling behavior. Of these, 28% were raised under consistent hypoxia (HH), 28% under 90 hypoxia followed by normoxia (HN), 23% under normoxia followed by hypoxia (NH), and 21% under consistent normoxia (NN). Developmental oxygen significantly altered shoaling tendency (chi-square = 8.76, P = 0.0326), with early developmentally hypoxic fish (HH) shoaling most often. Latency to shoal was not affected by developmental oxygen, however, we did detect significant variation among genotypes (Table 4.2). Swimming - Swimming significantly increased over time (see ANOVA Table 4.1, Fig. 4.2) and there was variation in overall swimming time across genotypes (Table 4.1). Early oxygen (E0-30) altered overall swimming time (Table 4.1), with early developmentally normoxic fish (NN & NH) swimming more than early developmentally hypoxic fish (HH and HN) (refer to Fig. 4.2). Post hoc Tukey-Kramer comparisons revealed significant effects of developmental oxygen between and within phases of the experiment. Both early (E0-30) and later (E30-90) oxygen altered swimming behavior. Overall, NH fish swam more than HN fish (P=0.0203; Fig. 4.2). Both early normoxic (NN & NH) and early hypoxic (HH & HN) fish increased swimming in presence of the stimulus (P=0.0018, 0.0279; Fig. 4.2). Later developmentally hypoxic fish (HH & NH) also increased swimming in the presence of the stimulus (P=0.0067; Fig. 4.2). Later normoxic fish (NN, HN) also increased swimming in response to stimulus presence (P=0.0098; figure 4.2). 91 Table 4.1. Mixed model repeated-measures ANOVA analyzing the effects of trial phase, sire, early oxygen (E0-30), later oxygen (E30-90), and all possible interactions on behaviors. Dependent variables included percentage time spent swimming (swim), percentage time spent on shoaling side of the arena (shoalside), percentage time spent shoaling (shoal), number of crosses between shoaling and non-shoaling side of the arena per second (cross), and percentage time spent hiding (hide). Since there were no significant interactions with other factors, only the overall effect of trial phase is presented. Degrees of freedom (numerator/denominator) are listed below each factor. F values are presented for each factor. Significance level is indicated by notation; *<.05, **<.01, ***<.0001. DF (N/D) Swim ShoalSide Shoal Cross Hide Phase 2/18 21.10*** 7.91** 10.46** 2.5 1.56 Sire 3/23 5.54** 1.03 0.92 2.95 3.77* E0-30 1/9 8.25* 0.24 0.00 9.44* 4.67 92 Sire x E0-30 3/20 2.52 1.03 1.09 3.05 1.78 E30-90 1/8 6.26* 0.46 0.05 1.91 4.32 Sire x E30-90 3/16 2.8 1.79 3.81* 0.89 1.36 E0-30 x E30-90 1/8 0.15 0.00 0.02 2.78 0.60 Sire x E0-30 x E30-90 3/11 1.57 1.37 1.53 2.15 0.32 Table 4.2. Mixed model ANOVA analyzing the effects of sire, early oxygen (Env030), later oxygen (Env30-90), and all possible interactions on blood glucose, PC1 (size), and latency to shoal (latency). Covariates for blood glucose included mass and total time spent swimming (swimT). Degrees of freedom (numerator/denominator) are listed below each factor. F values are presented for each factor. Significance level is determined by notation; *<.05, **<.01, ***<.0001. Sire x Sire x Sire x E0-30 x E0-30 x Source Mass SwimT Sire E0-30 E0-30 E30-90 E30-90 E30-90 E30-90 DF (N/D) 1/184 1/184 3/184 1/184 3/184 1/184 3/184 1/184 3/184 Glucose 21.39*** 4.91* 2.70* 1.68 1.35 19.37*** 0.63 1.68 0.65 DF (N/D) 3/372 1/372 3/372 1/372 3/372 1/372 3/372 PC1 0.82 32.38*** 0.80 23.82*** 0.30 0.02 0.42 DF (N/D) 3/214 1/214 3/214 1/214 3/214 1/214 3/214 2.95* 0.70 0.38 0.81 2.09 0.45 0.57 Latency 93 Fig. 4.2. Percentage time spent swimming before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments. Solid circles represent fish raised in early and later hypoxia (HH), open circles represent fish raised in early hypoxia followed by later normoxia (HN), solid triangles represent fish raised in early normoxia followed by later hypoxia (NH), and open triangles represent fish raised in early and later normoxia (NN). For environmental factors, early developmental oxygen (E0-30) is represented by shapes (circles = hypoxia, triangles = normoxia). Shading indicates later developmental oxygen (E 30-90; solid = hypoxia, open = normoxia). Error bars represent one standard error. Overall differences between developmental oxygen are noted below symbol legend. Significant differences between various combinations of developmental oxygen treatments between phases are noted. Significance level is determined by notation; *<.05, **<.01, ***<.0001. 94 Fig. 4.3. Percentage time spent on the shoaling side before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments. Solid circles represent fish raised in early and later hypoxia (HH), open circles represent fish raised in early hypoxia followed by later normoxia (HN), solid triangles represent fish raised in early normoxia followed by later hypoxia (NH), and open triangles represent fish raised in early and later normoxia (NN). For environmental factors, early developmental oxygen (E0-30) is represented by shapes (circles = hypoxia, triangles = normoxia). Shading indicates later developmental oxygen (E30-90; solid = hypoxia, open = normoxia). Error bars represent one standard error. Significant differences between specific phases of the trial are noted. Significance level is determined by notation; *<.05, **<.01, ***<.0001. 95 Shoaling - Developmental oxygen did not influence latency to shoal, however, genotype did effect shoaling tendency (Table 4.1). As noted in table 4.1 and represented in figures 4.3 and 4.4, fish significantly increased the time spent on the shoaling side of the arena and within the shoaling zone throughout the 3 phases of the experiment. E0-30 had no overall effect on the time spent on the shoaling side during the experiment, yet it was noted that E30-90 altered time spent shoaling as a function of genotype (see Table 1). 96 Fig. 4.4. Percentage time shoaling before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments. Solid circles represent fish raised in early and later hypoxia (HH), open circles represent fish raised in early hypoxia followed by later normoxia (HN), solid triangles represent fish raised in early normoxia followed by later hypoxia (NH), and open triangles represent fish raised in early and later normoxia (NN). For environmental factors, early developmental oxygen (E0-30) is represented by shapes (circles = hypoxia, triangles = normoxia). Shading indicates later developmental oxygen (E30-90; solid = hypoxia, open = normoxia). Error bars represent one standard error. Significant differences between specific phases of the trial are noted. Significance level is determined by notation; *<.05, **<.01, ***<.0001. 97 Reaction to stimulus – Of the fish that shoaled, 58% reacted to the stimulus. Chi- squared results indicated no association between developmental oxygen and reactivity. In other words, fish from all 4 developmental oxygen environments were just as likely to react to the stimulus. Crossing – Averaged across all treatment groups, crossing behavior did not change throughout the 3 phases of the experiment (Table 4.1, Fig. 4.5). However, E0-30 influenced the tendency to cross between shoaling and nonshoaling sides (Table 4.1), with early developmentally normoxic fish (NN & NH) crossing more than early developmentally hypoxic fish (HH & HN) (Fig. 4.5). For later developmentally normoxic fish (NN & HN), only those exposed to early hypoxia decrease in crossing behavior (Tukey-Kramer adjusted P=0.047; Fig. 4.5). In response to the stimulus introduction, early developmentally normoxic fish (NN & NH) crossed more than early developmentally hypoxic fish (HH, HN) (Tukey-Kramer adjusted P=0.0386; Fig. 4.5). 98 Fig 4.5. Number of crosses between shoaling and non-shoaling sides of the arena before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments. Solid circles represent fish raised in early and later hypoxia (HH), open circles represent fish raised in early hypoxia followed by later normoxia (HN), solid triangles represent fish raised in early normoxia followed by later hypoxia (NH), and open triangles represent fish raised in early and later normoxia (NN). For environmental factors, early developmental oxygen (E0-30) is represented by shapes (circles = hypoxia, triangles = normoxia). Shading indicates later developmental oxygen (E30-90; solid = hypoxia, open = normoxia). Error bars represent one standard error. Overall differences between developmental oxygen are noted below symbol legend. Significant differences between developmental oxygen treatments within specific phases of the trial are noted. Significance level is determined by notation; *<.05, **<.01, ***<.0001. 99 Fig. 4.6. Percentage time spent hiding before, during, and after stimulus introduction as a function of four developmental environments. Solid circles represent fish raised in early and later hypoxia (HH), open circles represent fish raised in early hypoxia followed by later normoxia (HN), solid triangles represent fish raised in early normoxia followed by later hypoxia (NH), and open triangles represent fish raised in early and later normoxia (NN). For environmental factors, early developmental oxygen (E0-30) is represented by shapes (circles = hypoxia, triangles = normoxia). Shading indicates later developmental oxygen (E30-90; solid = hypoxia, open = normoxia). Error bars represent one standard error. 100 Hiding – Overall hiding behavior was not influenced by developmental oxygen (Table 4.1, Fig. 4.6). We did, however, detect significant genetic variation in overall hiding behavior (Table 4.1). Blood glucose Blood was successfully extracted from 202 fish. Following the behavioral assay, later oxygen (E30-90) significantly altered blood glucose levels. Later developmentally normoxic fish (NN & HN) had on average higher glucose levels than later developmentally hypoxic fish (HH & NH) (Table 4.2, Fig. 4.7). Although early developmentally normoxic fish (NN & NH) maintained higher overall blood glucose regardless of later oxygen conditions (Fig. 4.6), the effect of E0-30 was not significant (Table 4.2). Blood glucose levels did not differ between fish that shoaled and fish that did not shoal (T-test; P=0.28). 101 Fig. 4.7. Blood glucose levels (mg/dl) as a function of four developmental environments. Solid symbols and line represent fish raised in early normoxia. Open symbols and dashed line represent fish raised on early hypoxia. The x axis represents later oxygen conditions. Error bars represent one standard error. 102 Morphology Mass, maximum depth, and body length all loaded positively in the first principal component (PC1). PC1 accounted for 90% of the variation. Early developmentally normoxic (NN & NH) fish were significantly larger regardless of E30-90 (Table 2, Fig. 4.8). Later developmentally normoxic (NN & HN) fish were also significantly larger than later developmentally hypoxic (HH & NH) (Table 4.2, Fig. 4.8). 103 Fig. 4.8. Principal component 1 (body size) as a function of four developmental environments. Solid symbols and line represent fish raised in early normoxia. Open symbols and dashed line represent fish raised on early hypoxia. The x axis represents later oxygen conditions. Error bars represent one standard error. 104 Discussion Environmental conditions during early development have been shown to permanently alter developmental outcomes. Environmental mismatches (i.e. when developmental conditions fail to accurately predict future conditions) can yield pathological results. One excellent illustration lies in early metabolic reprogramming. Fetuses exposed to malnutrition have been shown develop reduced insulin sensitivity, and upon exposure to improved nutrition later in life, reduced insulin action leads to physiological pathologies such as obesity and diabetes (Jones and Ozanne 2009). Numerous examples have been outlined in both human and animal models (review in Jones and Ozanne 2009). While the environmental and physiological aspects of such responses are well-studied, the underlying genetics of these responses remain poorly understood. Specifically, are these responses the same across all genotypes? Are some genotypes more constrained by early conditions than others? Here, we have attempted to address these questions by analyzing the effects of early and later developmental conditions on phenotypic development across genotypes. Shoaling In fish, shoaling offers many advantages, including but not limited to avoiding predators and optimizing food intake (Godin and Morgan 1985; Pitcher et al. 1982). Since zebrafish inhabit a wide variety of habitats, it is likely that shoaling behavior has been highly selected upon across a wide range of oxygen environments (Engeszer et al. 2007b). While shoaling steadily increased throughout the experiment across all treatments, we found little influence of 105 genotype or developmental oxygen on shoaling behavior. This concurs with previous findings in zebrafish. Wright et al. (2003) also found no variation in shoaling behavior of zebrafish across different populations. We did, however, find significant genotype-environment responses to shoaling behavior resulting from E30-90. These results indicate the presence of genetic variation in plastic responses to more recent oxygen conditions. In this sense, E0-30 plays no role in shaping shoaling behavior. E30-90, however, effect shoaling differently across genotypes. This indicates shoaling behavior is not constrained by early environment and exhibits plasticity to later conditions. While zebrafish begin shoaling during the early larvae stage, shoaling preferences remain plastic until later development (Engeszer et al. 2007a). Our results confirm the existence of later windows of plasticity for shoaling behavior. Swimming Fish exposed to early normoxia spent the most time swimming, most noticeably during stimulus introduction. Interestingly, early normoxic fish exposed to later hypoxia (NH) spent the most time swimming both during and after stimulus introduction. Hypoxia has been shown to increase swimming activity in a number of species (Bejda et al. 1987; Domenici et al. 2000; Parsons and Carlson 1998), which might explain in part why NH fish maintained the highest swimming levels during the experiment. Increased swimming in this group correlates with acute responses to hypoxia in previous studies (Bejda et al. 1987; Domenici et al. 2000). 106 Crossing Crossing behavior was used as an index of responsiveness to the novel stimulus. Shoaling, darting, and freezing are all intraspecific behaviors occurring in fish in response to alarm stimuli (Brown and Godin 1999). Therefore, during stimulus introduction, we expected variance in the tendency to retreat (cross) or remain shoaling during stimulus presence. While crossing behavior remained steady across treatments before stimulus introduction, crossing tendencies became more divergent as the trial progressed. Developmentally normoxic fish (NN) exhibited significantly increased crossing behavior in the presence of the novel stimulus. This group did so while maintaining shoaling times consistent with the other three groups during this phase. These results indicate quite a dynamic behavioral regimen. As with increased swimming, the increased crossing activity seen in developmentally normoxic fish would also be metabolically costly. The underlying physiologies of developmentally normoxic vs hypoxic fish may provide insight into these divergent behaviors, as discussed later Hiding Curiously, developmental oxygen did not alter hiding behavior. We previously demonstrated that fish raised in chronic hypoxia spent more time hiding than those from chronic normoxia regardless of the oxygen environment which they were tested in (Marks et al. 2005). The fish from the current experiment, however, were switched to the opposite oxygen environment much earlier during development. These results indicate that timing of environmental 107 change may be critical in determining the nature of adult phenotypes. In our previous study (2005), we hypothesized that fish raised in chronic hypoxia engaged in more hiding behavior as an energy saving strategy. Concurrent with our lab’s findings that fish raised in chronic hypoxia attain lower swimming speeds and produce lower lactate levels (Widmer et al. 2006), we hypothesized that hypoxia elicits metabolic depression in zebrafish. In our current study, however, hiding time did not differ across treatments. Also, hypoxic fish that began development in normoxia (NH) spent the most time swimming during the experiment. These results indicate the potential for early favorable environments, such as normoxia, to impart optimal developmental trajectories regardless of later environmental conditions. One such theory, the silver spoon hypothesis, states that individuals raised in optimal conditions maintain fitness advantages throughout life, regardless of later conditions (Grafen 1988). Compared with our previous findings, our current results highlight the potential for such responses to be contingent upon timing of environmental change. Proximate mechanisms of developmental plasticity Phenotypic variation underlies the basic target for evolution by natural selection to optimize traits within populations (Darwin 1859). More recently, selection on environmentally-induced variation has gained notoriety (Hall 2001; Schlichting and Smith 2002; Lande 2009). Zebrafish inhabit a wide range of habitats with numerous variable ecological factors (Engeszer et al. 2007b). Ecological factors such as oxygen content, community structure, and habitat complexity can influence brain size in fish (Chapman and Hulen 2001; Pollen et 108 al. 2007; Shumway 2008). Furthermore, variation in hypoxia-induced brain size plasticity among populations of cichlids has been detected (Chapman et al. 2008). Links between brain morphology and behavior in fish are well documented (Kotrschal et al. 1998; Healy and Rowe 2007). For example, trout with larger telencephalons exhibit more active foraging strategies (Wilson and McLaughlin 2010). Links between physiology and behavior have also been studied in fish (Scott and Sloman 2004). In a specific example, bold carp exhibit lower glucose, lactate, and metabolic rates than timid carp (Huntingford et al. 2010). While we did not assess brain morphology, physiological variation may provide one proximate mechanism for the observed behavioral variation. In our study, developmental oxygen altered stimulus responsiveness (crossing behavior) and time spent swimming. Inter-population variation in responsiveness to novel stimuli have been observed in zebrafish (Wright et al. 2003). Such differences among populations would require genetic or environmentally-induced variation for selection to act upon. The observed ability of developmental oxygen to alter behavior in this study embodies one such proximate example. Zebrafish, like many other fish species, utilize external fertilization. Developing embryos are therefore subject to environmental insults such as hypoxia. We routinely raise zebrafish embryos in oxygen conditions as low as 1.5 mg/L (approximately 17% dissolved oxygen at 25˚C and 1000 m above sea level) without severe pathologies (Marks et al. 2005; Widmer et al. 2006). Although little is known about the natural conditions and their associated variation during zebrafish development, laboratory tests using native zebrafish sediments as 109 substrate revealed conditions nearing anoxia within 96 hours post fertilization (hpf) (Strecker et al. 2011). Additionally, zebrafish embryos have been shown to be able to survive anoxia for up to 24 hours (Padilla and Roth 2001). These findings suggest that zebrafish could be experiencing extremely low oxygen levels during development. Chronic hypoxia has been shown to elicit a two-fold increase in whole-body cortisol concentrations in larval trout (Fuzzen et al. 2011). In sticklebacks, elevated cortisol exposure during development elicited lasting effects on later metabolism and behavior (Giesing et al. 2010). Early developmentally hypoxic fish (HH & HN) displayed significantly different behavior than early normoxic fish (NN & NH). In addition to fewer crossings between sides of the arena, they also consistently swam less time than NN and NH fish. Given previous studies, we feel the relationship between developmental hypoxia, cortisol production, and their influence adult behavior can provide promising insight into proximate causes of animal behavior. While the overreaching effect of developmental stressors has been established in a number of animal models (McArdle et al. 2006), the role genetic variation plays during development has received little attention. In this study, we used crossing behavior as an indicator of reactivity to novel stimulus introduction. We found that early developmentally hypoxic fish (HH & HN) crossed among arena sides less often and were thus more less to the stimulus. E 0-30 thus altered the reactivity of fish to a novel stimulus in this study. Our work highlights the potential for early developmental conditions to alter adult behavioral phenotypes. 110 Developmental oxygen clearly altered circulating glucose concentrations in this study. The overall mean glucose was 65 mg/dl. A previous study reported means between 60-73 mg/dl in untreated zebrafish fish (Eames et al. 2010). Increased blood glucose is a reliable indicator of stress in fish (Iwama et al. 1995). In our study, shoalers and non-shoalers did not differ in blood glucose levels. Non-shoalers were not exposed to the novel stimulus during the behavioral trials. Therefore, we deduce that the novel stimulus did not elicit a significant stress response. The only correlates with glucose in this study were mass (positive) and total time spent swimming (negative). E30-90 significantly altered blood glucose levels, with glucose increasing in normoxia. Despite the effect of E30-90, HN fish did not catch up to NN fish in terms of blood glucose (figure 7). This suggests that E0-30 fundamentally altered glucose metabolism. Upon acute exposure to hypoxia, fish typically mobilize more blood glucose for anaerobic ATP production (Chippari-Gomes et al. 2005; MacCormack and Driedzic 2007; O’Connor et al. 2011). Chronic hypoxia in this study, however, lowered blood glucose. Chronic developmental hypoxia in zebrafish has been shown to alter physical performance and underlying physiology. Developmentally hypoxic zebrafish not only attain lower swimming velocities than their normoxic counterparts, but they also produce less lactate (Widmer et al. 2006). These collective findings (lower glucose, lower lactate, lower stimulus reactivity) indicate unique physiological responses contrary to previous studies (Chippari-Gomes et al. 2005; MacCormack and Driedzic 2007; O’Connor et al. 2011) and require furtther elucidation. While shoaling as much as their developmentally normoxic 111 counterparts, hypoxic fish exhibited lowered responsiveness to the novel stimulus. Therefore, while routine shoaling and swimming behaviors may be physiologically compliant, the reactivity to potential stressors such as the novel stimulus may be too energetically costly. Morphologically, NN and NH fish were significantly larger than HH and HN fish. Fish exposed to later normoxia (NN & HN) were also on average larger than fish exposed to later hypoxia (HH & NH). These results indicate a steadily steeper developmental trajectory defined by both E0-30 and E30-90. Small body size may be an adaptive feature of hypoxic survival. Smaller individuals have been shown to be more tolerant to hypoxia than their larger counterparts in both bass and sea bream (Burleson et al. 2001; Cerezo and García García 2004). Indeed the general consensus is that smaller fish are more tolerant to hypoxia (Doudoroff 1970; Lowe-Jinde and Niimi 1983). This is due to a number of reasons. One factor is that increasing ventilation is more costly for larger fish (Jones 1971). Secondly, studies have noted an inverse relationship between body size and hematocrit (Lowe-Jinde and Niimi 1983; Zanuy and Carrillo 1985). Smaller fish also have more relative gill area vs body weight than larger fish (Hughes 1984). Smaller fish are also observed to be outcompeted by larger fish for more favorable habitats (Coutant 1987). Smaller fish may also be forced to less favorable waters to avoid predation (Werner and Hall 1988). These collective factors indicate that the smaller overall size of HH and HN fish in this study may have been a result of past selection for adaptive developmental responses to hypoxia. Whether smaller body size in this case is due to selection 112 (i.e. adaptive) or physiological constraint (i.e. non-adaptive) is not within the scope of this study and warrants further investigation. It is worth noting that the effect of E0-30 on size remained, with HH and HN fish never “catching up” to NN and NH fish in terms of size. This was true despite plentiful food (daily excess food removal was required for all treatments). Fish have been shown to fully or even overcompensate for growth deficits when switched from unfavorable to favorable conditions (Noltie and Wang 1997). Periods of rapid growth, however, can be energetically costly, reduce fitness, and may therefore be selected against (Ali et al. 2003). The tendency to retain a steady growth pattern in this study may likely be a function of changing conditions during development. While we characterize development as a function of one environmental change, the reality is environments can change frequently during development. Dissolved oxygen can fluctuate between normoxia and hypoxia due to daylight photosynthesis and night time respiration (Tyler 2007). If individuals starting out in hypoxia began increasing growth when switched to normoxia, the response would be unfavorable if the environment changed back to hypoxia. Simply put, it may be costly to continually respond to environmental change. A general developmental trajectory, established during early development, may be the best strategy. While shrimp respond to actuate hypoxia, gene expression is shown to return to pre-hypoxia levels upon exposure to cyclic hypoxia (Brown-Peterson et al. 2008). This may provide direct evidence for the costs associated with repeated reactions to environmental change. 113 Fish size may have also influenced shoaling behavior. Across species, fish are observed to shoal with similar sized fish in both lab and field populations (Hoare et al. 2000). Since conspecific shoaling fish were chosen at random from lab stocks each day, we had no way to quantify the relationship between their size and test subject size. Also, since different conspecifics were randomly chosen each day, any effect of size would have been balanced throughout the experiment. When we regressed mass against individual behavioral variables, however, mass showed no significant correlation with any behaviors. This indicates that, once shoaling behavior was initiated, fish size had no influence on subsequent behavior. With no observed covariation between mass and any of the measured variables, we are confident the variation in observed behaviors was influenced primarily by genotype and developmental oxygen. There was a significant positive correlation between mass and blood glucose. Larger individuals had higher blood glucose values. Larger fish typically have higher glycogen stores, particularly in white muscle tissue (Ferguson et al. 1993) . This provides larger fish with an increased capacity to engage in anaerobically supported burst swimming (Kieffer 2000). In an endurance study, developmentally nornoxic fish produced more lactate than their hypoxic counterparts, suggesting an increased capacity for anaerobic respiration for normoxic fish (Widmer et al. 2006). Reduced feeding may have also influenced body size and blood glucose. Hypoxia has been shown to decrease feeding in bass (Pichavant et al. 2001) salmon (Jobling, 1994) and turbot (Pichavant et al. 2000). These responses, however, tend to occur with acute exposure to hypoxia. 114 Since fish exposed to early normoxia (NN and NH) were significantly larger than HH and HN fish, it is likely that the observed correlation between high glucose and high body mass is driven primarly by E0-30 . Conclusion – We have provided insight into the consequences of changing oxygen during zebrafish development. Our unique design incorporating all possible combinations of developmental environments reveals the relative impact of sequential environmental conditions in shaping developmental outcomes. Furthermore, by considering the effect of environmental fluctuation across separate genotypes, we add insight into the evolutionary potential of behavioral, physiological, and morphological plasticity in complex environments. Our approach offers the flexibility to explore the consequences of shifting environmental factors at any point during development. This can address important questions regarding critical windows of development. We found morphological (body size) and behavioral (time spent swimming) factors to be constrained by early oxygen conditions. These results are indicative of the numerous studies highlighting the lasting effects of early developmental conditions (McArdle et al. 2006). Epigenetic reprogramming, for example, is believed to occur as a result of certain stimuli occurring during a critical point in development (Clark 1997). Despite early conditions, shoaling behavior and blood glucose levels remained reactive to E30-90. This insight was available due to the partitioning of the developmental environment into discrete components. We propose the advancement of this technique to analyze the influence of early 115 developmental conditions on further plastic responsiveness in other traits and systems. Conclusions and evolutionary implications Contrary to the previous chapters, we found little evidence of interactions between subsequent developmental environments. Variation in time spent swimming, stimulus reactivity, and size was constrained by early oxygen conditions. Fish raised in early normoxia, for example, maintained larger body sizes regardless of later oxygen conditions. This implies the early environment can be critical in determining zebrafish size and behavior. Furthermore, with no significant interaction between sire and early oxygen, this indicates that there is little genotypic variation in plastic responsiveness due to early oxygen. Given that zebrafish are likely to encounter changing oxygen during development, this indicates that selection has likely optimized plasticity in these traits. The variance structures across all four treatments confirms this observation. Figure 4.9 shows little change in variance due to later oxygen. Rather, early oxygen contributes to the changes in variance, with variance highest in early normoxia. Similar to chapters II and III, hypoxia contributes to decreased variance. This suggests that hypoxia is also a strong selective force in shaping zebrafish size. Acknowledgements We thank Kevin P. Kaut for commenting on drafts of this manuscript. 116 2.0 Variance (PC1) 1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2 1.0 0.8 0.6 Later oxygen Early oxygen H H N H N N Figure 4.9. Variance due to all four combinations of oxygen environments. 117 CHAPTER V THE INFLUENCE OF ONTOGENETIC DIETARY CHANGES ON ZEBRAFISH SIZE AND SWIMMING PERFORMANCE Introduction Phenotypic flexibility is critical in determining fitness. As conditions change during ontogeny, continued responsiveness is necessary to meet the demands of the environment. Phenotypic responses can be irreversibly sculpted during critical points of ontogeny (Burggren and Reyna 2011). For example, we previously that long term constraints on aggression, swimming performance, and lactate production are a product of early oxygen environment (Marks et al. 2005; Widmer et al. 2006). We found that fish tested in normoxia displayed phenotypes that were altered or constrained by development in early hypoxia. Studies demonstrating genetic variation for responses to environmental change are ubiquitous (DeWitt and Scheiner 2004). This variation is an important factor in shaping evolutionary dynamics (West-Eberhard 2005). Much of our current understanding comes from studies tracking genotypes across one instance of environmental change. As the environment can change multiple times during ontogeny, it becomes increasingly important to characterize the role genetic variation plays in more complex environments. 118 Food availability is a critical factor in shaping animal development. Food manipulation studies have demonstrated significant effects of dietary change on fish metabolism and swimming performance (Alsop and Wood 1997; Beamish et al. 1989). Food type and availability vary and such changes are associated with seasons (Wu and Culver 1992), presence of competitors (Osenberg et al. 1992), and microhabitat use (García-Berthou 1999). Ontogenetic dietary shifts have been demonstrated to increase cognitive performance in the cichlid Simochromis pleurospilus (Kotrschal and Taborsky 2010). Fish that were switched from high ration to low ration and low ration to high ration diets outperformed conspecifics maintained on steady high or low ration diets. While dietary change clearly influences developmental outcomes in fish, the role genetic variation plays in influencing these altered responses in more complex environments remains unclear. To elucidate the role genetic variation plays in determining responses to ontogenetic dietary changes, we examined body size and swim performance in the zebrafish Danio rerio. Four full sib families were fed either consistent high or low food rations, or a combination of the two for 60 days. We analyzed sources of variation on phenotypic outcomes as functions of family (F), early diet (days 030; Diet0-30), later diet (days 30-60; Diet30-60), interactions between dietary environments (Diet0-30 x Diet30-60), genetic variation to either dietary environment (F x Diet0-30, F x Diet30-60), and genetic variation in interactions between dietary environments (F x Diet0-30 x Diet30-60). With many potential sources of variation, we made no a priori hypotheses on these sources. 119 Methods Animals This experiment performed under approval by The University of Akron’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) were obtained from Aquatica Tropicals (Plant City, FL) and populations of wildtype strains were maintained according to standard procedures (Westerfield, 1994). Adults were maintained and bred at 26 ± 0.5°C with a 14L:10D light cycle. Breeding design Males and females were randomly paired resulting in four full-sib families. Mating pairs were placed in 2 L containers lined with a marble substrate and supplied with a common water source (Z-Mod housing system, Marine Biotech, Beverly, MA). Treatments Siblings were raised together in 2 L containers with a common water source (Z-Mod housing system, Marine Biotech, Beverly, MA) and were maintained at 26 ± 0.5°C with a 14L : 10D light cycle for the duration of the experiment. Food consisted of pulverized Zeigler™ adult zebrafish diet supplemented with equal parts of <100 and 100-150 micron Zeigler™ larval diet (1:1:1). After 30 days, the <100 and 100-150 micron supplements were replaced with 150-250 and 250-450 micron supplements. For all feedings, 500 mg of food was mixed with 250 ml of system water. From this solution, fish were fed at 0.1 ml/fish (low ration treatment) and 0.2 ml/fish (high ration treatment). We chose these rations based on a standard dry food recipe from a protocol available at the 120 Zebrafish International Resource Center (http://zebrafish.org/zirc/documents/protocols/pdf/Fish_Feeding/Flake_Food/Dry_ Food_Recipes.pdf). We assigned 0.1 ml/fish to the low ration treatment since it was the minimum designated amount according to this protocol. Therefore, the terms “high” and “low” are relative and apply only to the confines of this particular study. Feedings were conducted once daily and excess food was removed before each feeding. After two weeks, we noted all food was being consumed within 24 hours. After 30 days, half of the individuals from each treatment were switched to a separate 2 L tank and subjected to the opposite feeding treatment for the remainder of the experiment. This resulted in four feeding treatments: high food rations for 60 days (HH), low food rations for 60 days (LL), high food rations for 30 days followed by low food rations for 30 (HL), and low food rations for 30 days followed by high food rations for 30 (LH). Measurements Body size - To ensure feeding treatments had an initial effect, we measured the total length (TL) of each subject at 30 days. Fish were placed individually in a small petri dish filled with system water. A standard length was included in each picture. Photography was conducted with a Nikon D300 camera under standard lighting conditions. We measured total length from most anterior point to the posterior point of the caudal fin. We observed no damage to caudal fins at any point in the study. At 60 days, each subject was euthanized with MS222 (300 mg/ml tricaine methane sulfonate buffered to a neutral pH with sodium bicarbonate) and photographed on the subject’s right side with a length standard 121 in each picture. We measured standard length (SL) from most anterior point to the base of the hypural plate at caudal flexion. Maximum depth (MD) was measured as the maximum dorsal-ventral distance measured along the flank. All measurements were made using imageJ (Version 1.42, NIH). Measurements were made 5 times on each subject and the mean was recorded. Swim velocity - Prior to terminal measurements (above), swimming velocity was measured according to Widmer et al (2006). Briefly, individual fish were placed in a clear acrylic flume (44.7 mm inner diameter by 30 cm long) which drew system water. Baffles placed at the anterior portion of the chamber maintained consistent laminar flow throughout the length of the flume. With an initial flow rate of 3.8 l/min, flow (Blue-White Industries, Huntington Beach, CA, U.S.A.; flow rate meter F-1000-RB) was incrementally increased by 1.9 l/min until the fish spent >50% of the time increment touching the back mesh of the chamber (Brett 1964). Maximum swim velocity was calculated based on the inner diameter of the tube and the final flow measurement, and was determined based on the standard length of the fish tested. Statistics To test for the effects of feeding for days 0-30 on TL, we used a two way ANOVA. Family, treatment, and their interaction were included as sources of variation. To test for the effects of feeding throughout the experiment on SL and swim velocity, we used a three way ANOVA. Family, food treatment for days 030 (Diet 0-30), food treatment for days 30-60 (Diet 30-60), and all possible 122 interactions were included as potential sources of variation. TL and SL were log transformed to meet normality assumptions. Results Survival Ninety-three subjects survived the experiment. Chi-squared tests revealed that survival shared no contingencies with feeding treatments (X2=0.56, P=0.91) or families (X2=2.91, P=0.41). Body size At 30 days, family (F) and diet were both significant factors in influencing TL (Table 5.1). Individuals from the high food treatment were significantly longer (Fig. 5.1). At 60 days, however, the only significant source of variation on SL and MD was family (Table 1; Figs. 5.2 & 5.3). Untransformed means are presented in tables 2 and 3. 123 Fig. 5.1. The influence of diet for days 0-30 on total length in four full-sib families. Error bars represent standard errors. 124 Table 5.1. ANOVA results for total length (TL), standard length (SL), maximum depth (MD), and swim velocity. For TL, factors included family, diet for days 0-30 (Diet0-30), and their interaction. For SL, MD, and swim velocity, factors included family, diet for days 0-30 (Diet0-30), diet for days 30-60 (Diet30-60), and all possible interactions. TL, SL, and MD were log transformed for analyses. Variable Total length Source DF MS Family 3 16.4850 Diet0-30 1 19.8648 Family x Diet0-30 3 0.7649 Error 128 2.1122 Standard length Family 3 3.4353 Diet0-30 1 0.0123 Family x Diet0-30 3 0.0017 Diet30-60 1 0.0424 Family x Diet30-60 3 0.0013 Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 1 0.0003 Family x Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 3 0.0078 Error 77 0.0140 Maximum depth Family 3 2.6945 Diet0-30 1 0.0066 Family x Diet0-30 3 0.0055 Diet30-60 1 0.0370 Family x Diet30-60 3 0.0043 Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 1 0.0009 Family x Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 3 0.0074 Error 77 0.0111 Swim velocity Family 3 491.9777 Diet0-30 1 7.1416 Family x Diet0-30 3 5.0020 Diet30-60 1 51.2372 Family x Diet30-60 3 28.1016 Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 1 56.8658 Family x Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 3 28.3586 Error 77 9.0594 125 F P 7.8045 <0.0001 9.4046 0.0026 0.3621 0.7805 245.04 <0.0001 0.87 0.3526 0.12 0.9477 3.02 0.0860 0.09 0.9645 0.02 0.8861 0.56 0.6438 242.74 <0.0001 0.60 0.4426 0.50 0.6858 3.33 0.0718 0.39 0.7612 0.08 0.7724 0.67 0.5728 54.31 0.79 0.55 5.66 3.10 6.28 3.13 <0.0001 0.3774 0.6482 0.0199 0.0315 0.0143 0.0304 Table 5.2. Total length (mm) for zebrafish at 30 days under high and low food rations. Data are presented as untransformed arithmetic mean ± SEM. H L Total Length 4.68±0.20 3.87±0.17 126 Table 5.3. Maximum depth and standard length (mm) for zebrafish at 60 days under all combinations of high and low food rations. Data are presented as untransformed arithmetic mean ± SEM. HH Maximum Depth HL LH LL 5.86±0.87 6.13±0.90 5.84±0.79 5.64±0.82 Standard Length 6.20±0.96 5.32±0.79 7.17±0.97 6.10±0.94 127 Fig. 5.2. The influence of changing diet on standard length in four full-sib families. Line type (open vs solid) represents diet ration for days 0-30. The x axis represents food ration for days 30-60. Error bars represent standard errors. 128 Fig. 5.3. The influence of changing diet on maximum depth in four full-sib families. Line type (open vs solid) represents diet ration for days 0-30. The x axis represents food ration for days 30-60. Error bars represent standard errors. 129 Swimming velocity Many factors contributed to variation in swimming velocity. Besides variation among families (F), diet for days 30-60 also contributed to variation in swimming velocity (Diet30-60) and this effect varied significantly across families (F x Diet30-60; Table 5.1). Fish fed low rations for days 30-60 attained higher velocities on average than those fed high rations (Fig. 5.4). Early diet (Diet0-30) also contributed to variation in swimming performance through its interaction with later diet (Diet0-30 x Diet30-60; Table 5.1). Fish raised on low rations for the duration of the experiment (LL) maintained similar velocities to those switched from low to high rations (LH; Fig. 5.4). Interestingly, fish switched from high to low rations (HL) attained higher swimming velocities than thoses maintained on high rations (HH; Fig. 5.4). The interaction between family and both dietary periods indicates that the quality of interactions between dietary treatments varied across families. This resulted in a three way interaction between family, early, and later food treatments (F x Diet30-60 x Diet30-60; Table 5.1). Variation in swimming performance for families A and B remained consistent across diet treatments. Families C and D, however, showed variation due to both early (Diet 0-30) and later (Diet30-60) food treatments. Fish from these families raised on low rations for the duration of the experiment (LL) maintained similar velocities to those switched from low to high rations (LH). Fish switched from high to low rations (HL), however, attained higher swimming velocities than this maintained on high rations (HH; Fig. 5.5). 130 Fig. 5.4. Swimming velocity as a function of four nutritional environments. Open lines and symbols represent low food rations days 0-30 and solid lines and symbols represent high food rations for days 0-30. The x axis represents food ration for days 30-60. Error bars represent standard errors. 131 Fig. 5.5. The influence of changing diet on swimming velocity in four full-sib families. Line type (open vs solid) represents diet ration for days 0-30. The x axis represents food ration for days 30-60. Error bars represent standard errors. 132 Discussion Low diet fish were significantly smaller than high diet fish at 30 days. This confirms our feeding treatment significantly altered development prior to switching feeding treatments. Interestingly, neither nutritional environment contributed to variation in SL or MD at the end of the experiment. In other words, while early diet initially influenced size, subsequent nutritional change resulted in equal sizes across all four treatments. This indicates some compensatory growth mechanism for fish exposed to low food rations for days 0-30. Compensatory growth is generally not without cost. Following exposure to suboptimal conditions, compensatory growth has been show to be associated with costs in a number of physiological, morphological, life history, and performance traits (reviewed in Metcalfe and Monaghan 2001). The performance trait addressed in this study was maximum swim velocity. Although they exhibited compensatory growth, fish raised on early low food rations maintained similar swimming velocity regardless of later dietary rations. This indicates negligible cost of compensatory growth on swimming performance in our study. Previous studies have demonstrated a tradeoff between accelerated growth and physical performance in salmon (Farrell et al. 1997) and sticklebacks (Álvarez and Metcalfe 2007). In the case of sticklebacks, however, the associated tradeoff was present in stream rather than pond populations. This result indicates that local selective pressures can alter tradeoff trajectories among populations. Zebrafish inhabit a wide variety of habitats ranging from active streams to stagnant rice fields (Spence et al. 2008). It is 133 therefore likely that ecological variation has shaped tradeoff trajectories in this species. Interestingly, swim velocity was highest in fish switched from high to low food rations. Thus, dietary change enhanced swimming performance, but only for fish started on high rations. Fish started on low rations did not increase swimming velocity when switched to high rations. Thus, although HL and LH fish attained similar size, their physical abilities differed. Phillips (2004) performed a similar study with mussels and found similar results. While mussels switched from high to low rations equaled those switched from low to high in terms of shell size, they differed in terms of lipid content. This result suggests that dietary order can be more critical in shaping physiological rather than morphological outcomes. Thus, the underlying physiologies of the subjects in our study may have been affected. Specifically, fish switched from high to low rations proved physiologically superior to fish from other treatments. Swimming ability is a critical trait in fish. Its implications on prey capture, predator avoidance, and social interactions are evident (Domenici and Blake 1997). Thus, as swimming ability is sensitive to environmental change, the ontogenetic history of fish becomes critical in shaping their fitness. At the population level, variation in environmentally-altered developmental trajectories provides the raw material for natural selection to optimize fitness in changing environments. Zebrafish inhabit a wide variety of habitats throughout Southeast Asia (Spence et al. 2008). Their association with a number of different habitats throughout seasonal changes makes it likely that factors such as temperature, 134 oxygen, and food availability can vary during their ontogeny. Thus, it is likely that there is some ecological component to variation in swimming ability in zebrafish. Given their small size, zebrafish are prolific swimmers that display remarkably low associated physiological costs (Plaut and Gordon 1994). The selective factors that have shaped these abilities require further elucidation. The role environmental complexity plays in shaping ontogenetic trajectories is receiving increasing attention (Monaghan 2008). Few studies to our knowledge have quantitatively demonstrated significant interactions between subsequent ontogenetic periods of development (Kotrschal and Taborsky 2010; Marks et al. 2005). Even less clear is the role genetic variation plays in determining the quality of phenotypic outcomes under complex conditions. Our study not only demonstrates that subsequent dietary conditions can interact in shaping zebrafish physical performance, but the quality of these effects is family specific. This result indicates at least some role of genetic variation in shaping plastic responses under complex conditions. This variation provides critical insight into how populations evolve in complex environments. In summary, we found a significant interaction between dietary environments (Diet0-30 x Diet30-60) for swimming velocity. Overall, fish switched from high to low food rations attained the highest swimming velocity. Fish started on low food rations attained similar swimming velocities regardless of later food rations. The quality of responses to dietary change varied across families, resulting in a significant Family x Diet0-30 x Diet30-60 interaction. Although early food rations influenced size at the midway point of the experiment, fish achieved 135 equal sizes across all food treatments at the end of the experiment. These results suggest that plastic responsiveness to subsequent environmental changes can be trait specific and vary significantly within populations. The specific order of environmental conditions can also be critical in determining performance outcomes. Conclusions and evolutionary implications Figure 5.5 depicts a modified reaction norm similar to the one described in chapter I and employed in chapters II and III. While it also demonstrates that the quality of interaction between subsequent environments is conditional on genotype, it also yields potential evolutionary implications. This reaction norm demonstrates that variance in swimming velocity for fish exposed to later low food rations is highest when fish start on high food rations. Figure 5.6 confirms this assertion. Therefore, fish exposed to these subsequent conditions would exhibit more phenotypic variants with which selection can act upon. Swim velocity is undoubtedly an important trait. Faster swimming fish would be more likely to escape predators and would therefore incur a fitness advantage. This result therefore demonstrates that changes in dietary conditions can have consequences on fitness. Acknowledgements We thank Choose Ohio First Tiered Mentoring Program for funding this project. We also thank Steven Lombardo and Kristie Formanik for their help collecting data. 136 Coeffecient of Variation (swim velocity) 0.70 0.68 0.66 0.64 0.62 0.60 0.58 0.56 Later food Early food H H L H L L Figure 5.6. Variance due to all four combinations of feeding treatments. 137 CHAPTER VI CONCLUSIONS We tested the overall hypothesis that the early environment can constrain or alter later phenotypic responses in a given environment. We also explored the role of genetic variation in this process. The previous chapters clearly demonstrated that environmental modulation of phenotypic variance in a given environment is conditional on early environmental conditions. In chapter II, for example, cardiovascular performance at 48 h.p.f. was not only conditional on the oxygen level of the first 24 hours of development, but the quality of these conditional responses varied across genotypes. In chapter III, the variance of integrated morphology showed the same trend. Chapter V showed that variation in swim velocity was greatest in fish exposed to a low ration diet, but only for fish started out on a high ration diet. However, in chapter IV we found no variation in the manner in which the early environment altered zebrafish shoaling, stimulus reactivity, and size. Thus, while our hypothesis was confirmed in many cases, these responses were specific to the trait being studied. Ramifications of environmental complexity With the likelihood that organisms experience changing conditions during development, it is therefore likely that the degree of observable phenotypic variance within any population is a function of early developmental conditions. 138 Therefore, when assessing fitness and evolution within natural populations, it is critical to account for the entire environmental history. The environment is extremely complex with nearly infinite combinations of various biotic and abiotic factors that can vary spatiotemporally throughout ontogenetic time. These methods could therefore be adapted to explore the influence of changing conditions in any model. However, caution should be used in over-factorializing studies. Just one more subsequent environmental change in our study would have further halved sample sizes in each treatment. Maintaining sample sizes large enough to execute multifactorial studies would be difficult to employ. However, while these collective studies represent relatively simple scenarios, they still provide insight into the consequences of changing environmental conditions during development. The current paradigm analyzes plasticity as a function of one environmental change. We have demonstrated that this view is not only overly simplistic, but provides an incomplete picture of the proximate causes of phenotypic variance in populations. We have demonstrated that early conditions can permanently constrain zebrafish size and behavior regardless of later conditions (chapter IV). We have also demonstrated that early conditions can alter responses to later ones and that the nature of these conditional responses vary significantly across genotypes (chapters II, III, V). Variation, selection, and evolution The factors that promote and maintain phenotypic variation in the natural world are of central importance to biologists. It is this variation that continually provides the raw material for natural selection to optimize fitness and promote 139 speciation. Our collective studies demonstrate that the degree of variation expressed in a population can be a function how variants from a particular early environment respond to later conditions. Thus, the context of subsequent developmental environments can be critical in determining the rate at which unfit variants are removed. In other words, the tempo of evolution in any given population can be conditional on the series of conditions its individuals experience. Plotting the variance structures yielded interesting insight into the environmental factors shaping phenotypic variance. In chapter II, III, and IV, the variance in all examples (Cardiac output, morphology, and overall size) are lowest in fish exposed to consistent hypoxia (Figs. 2.10, 3.6, 4.9). This reduced variance relative to other combinations of subsequent environments suggests that hypoxia has been a major force in shaping the evolution of these traits in zebrafish. This not surprising given the conditions of their native habitat discussed in chapter I. Future directions Proximate mechanisms of plasticity: quantitative epigenetics Biologists have qualitatively known that early conditions can shape developmental trajectories and determine phenotypes regardless of later environmental conditions. Barker (1998) noted that adult phenotypes such as disease susceptibility could be correlated with maternal nutrition, drug use, and stress levels. He speculated that these stressors irreversibly altered the course of development. Thus, he coined the term ‘fetal programming’. Since then biologists 140 have pinpointed the proximate mechanisms of this epigenetic phenomenon (Reviewed in Mulder et al 2002). One well-documented case involves the rearchitecturing of the developing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. During instances of maternal stress, corticoid receptor density is permanently down-regulated, resulting in offspring with compromised negative feedback systems (reviewed in Mulder et al 2002). Thus, while the epigenetics of the influence of early development has been elucidated, genetic variation in this trait has not been characterized. I advance the hypothesis that there is variation in the quality of epigenetic remodeling and this may contribute to interactions between subsequent developmental environments. For example, while maternal stress may cause receptor density to be down regulated on average within a population, some genotypes may not respond accordingly. Some genotypes may develop the same receptor density regardless of maternal stress levels. They would develop a normal negative feedback system. Alternatively, other phenotypes may increase receptor density. This would promote an individual with an increased negative feedback system. Therefore, a quantitative genetic assessment of epigenetic phenomenon would shed critical insight into the proximate mechanisms of environmental modulation of phenotypic outcomes. Quantitative genetics and the future of evolutionary biology Pigliucci and Muller emphasized the need to advance the work of the modern synthesis by incorporating aspect of development and ecology (Pigliucci and Muller 2010). As these ideas to develop, the need will arise to further synthesize ecology and development into a more refined theory of evolution. 141 Quantitative genetics has proved a powerful tool for making evolutionary predictions (Roff 2007). Our results clearly demonstrate the utility of quantitative genetics in addressing the evolution of complex, developmentally plastic traits in heterogeneous environments. Our approach offers the flexibility of manipulating ecological factors while exploring specific windows of development. Development is a dynamic, hierarchical process involving the genesis and interaction of cells, hormones, tissues, and organs (Hall 2003). The multifactorial framework we employ can provide a powerful tool in exploring the complex relationship between genes, developing systems, phenotypic variation, and dynamic environments. Subsequent studies should not only focus on elucidating proximate mechanisms of plasticity, but also the relative impacts of sequential environmental conditions of more fitness related traits (i.e. fecundity). 142 LITERATURE CITED Alais D., Newell F., and Mamassian, P. 2010. Multisensory Processing in Review: from Physiology to Behaviour. Seeing and Perceiving 23: 3-38. Alderman S. L., and Bernier N.J. 2009. Ontogeny of the corticotropin-releasing factor system in zebrafish. Gen Comp Endocrinol 164: 61-69. Ali M., Nicieza, A., and Wootton, R. J. 2003. Compensatory growth in fishes: a response to growth depression. Fish and Fisheries 4: 147-190. Alsop D., and Wood, C. 1997. The interactive effects of feeding and exercise on oxygen consumption, swimming performance and protein usage in juvenile rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). J Exp Biol 200: 2337-2346. Álvarez D., and Metcalfe, N. B. (2007). The tradeoff between catch-up growth and escape speed: variation between habitats in the cost of compensation. Oikos 116: 1144-1151. Arnold S. J. 1992. Constraints on phenotypic evolution. Am Nat 140: S85–S107. Atchley W. R. 1987. Developmental quantitative genetics and the evolution of ontogenies. Evolution 41:316-330. Atchley W. R., and Zhu J. 1997. Developmental quantitative genetics, conditional epigenetic variability and growth in mice. Genetics 147: 765-776. Bagatto B. 2005. Ontogeny of cardiovascular control in zebrafish (Danio rerio): effects of developmental environment. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol 141: 391-400. Barman R. P. 1991. A taxonomic revision of the Indo-Burmese species of Danio rerio. Record of the Zoological Survey of India Occasional Papers 137:1– 91. Bejda A. J., Studholme A. L., and Olla B. L. 1987. Behavioral responses of red hake, (Urophycis chuss), to decreasing concentrations of dissolved oxygen. Environ Biol Fish 19: 261-268. 143 Beamish F. W. H., Howlett J. C., and Medland T. E. 1989. Impact of diet on metabolism and swimming performance in juvenile lake trout, Salvelinus namaycush. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 46: 384-388. Boutilier, R.G. 1990. Respiratory gas tensions in the environment. In: Advances in comparative and environmental physiology. Boutilier, R.G. (ed.). Springer-Verlag. Chapter 1. Bradshaw A. D. 1965. Evolutionary significance of phenotypic plasticity in plants. Adv Genet 13: 115–155. Brett J. R. 1964 The respiratory metabolism and swimming performance of young sockeye salmon. J Fish Res Board Can 21: 1183–122. Brown-Peterson N.J., Manning C. S., Patel, V., Denslow, N. D., and Brouwer, M. 2008. Effects of cyclic hypoxia on gene expression and reproduction in a grass shrimp, (Palaemonetes pugio). Biol Bull 214: 6-16. Brown G. E. and Godin, J.-G. J. 1999. Chemical alarm signals in wild Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Can J Zool 77: 562-570. Bubliy O. A., Loeschcke V., and Imasheva, A. G. 2001. Genetic variation of morphological traits in Drosophila melanogaster under poor nutrition: isofemale lines and offspring-parent regression. Heredity 86:363–369. Buckley C. R., Michael S. F. and Irschick, D. J. 2005. Early hatching decreases jumping performance in a direct-developing frog, Eleutherodactylus coqui. Funct Ecol 19: 67–72. Burggren W.W. 2000. Developmental physiology, animal models, and the August Krogh principle. Zool Anal Complex Syst 102: 148-156. Burggren W. W., and Reyna K. S. 2011. Developmental trajectories, critical windows and phenotypic alteration during cardio-respiratory development. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 178: 13-21. Burleson M. L., Wilhelm, D. R., and Smatresk, N. J. 2001. The influence of fish size size on the avoidance of hypoxia and oxygen selection by largemouth bass. J Fish Biol 59: 1336-1349. Carroll S. B. 1995. Homeotic genes and the evolution of arthropods and chordates. Nature 376:479-485. Carroll. S.B. 2005 Endless Forms Most Beautiful: The New Science of Evo Devo and the Making of the Animal Kingdom. W.W. Norton, New York. 144 Cerezo J., and García García, B. 2004. The effects of oxygen levels on oxygen consumption, survival and ventilatory frequency of sharpsnout sea bream (Diplodus puntazzo Gmelin, 1789) at different conditions of temperature and fish weight. J Appl Ichthyol 20: 488-492. Chan T., and Burggren, W. W. 2005. Hypoxic incubation creates differential morphological effects during developmental critical windows in the embryo of the chicken (Gallus gallus). Resp Physiol Neurobi 145:251-263. Chapman L. J., Albert J., and Galis, F. 2008. Developmental Plasticity, Genetic Differentiation, and Hypoxia-induced Trade-offs in an African Cichlid Fish. The Open Evolution Journal 2: 75-88. Chapman L.J., and Hulen, K.G. 2001. Implications of hypoxia for the brain size and gill morphometry of mormyrid fishes. J Zool 254: 461-472. Charmantier A., and Garant D. 2005. Environmental quality and evolutionary potential: lessons from wild populations. Proc Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 272:1415 –1425. Chen D. S., Dykhuizen, G. V., Hodge J., and Gilly W. F. 1996. Ontogeny of Copepod Predation in Juvenile Squid (Loligo opalescens). Biol Bull 190:69 81. Cheverud, J. M., Rutledge, W., and Atchley, W. R. 1983. Quantitative genetics of development: Genetic correlations among age specific trait values and the evolution of ontogeny. Evolution 37:895-905. Cheverud J. M. 1988. A comparison of genetic and phenotypic correlations. Evolution 42: 958–968. Cheverud J. M. 1996. Developmental Integration and the Evolution of Pleiotropy Amer Zool 36: 44-50. Chippari-Gomes A.R., Gomes L. C, Lopes N.P., Val A. L., and Almeida-Val V. M. F. 2005. Metabolic adjustments in two Amazonian cichlids exposed to hypoxia and anoxia. Comp Biochem Physiol B 141: 347-355. Clark E. B. 1990. Growth, morphogenesis and function: The dynamics of cardiac development. In: Moller, J.H., Neal, W., and Lock, J. eds. Fetal, neonatal and infant heart disease. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Clark P.M. 1997. Programming of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis and the fetal origins of adult disease hypothesis. Eur J Pediatr 157: S7-S10. 145 Correns, C. G. 1900. Mendel's Regel über das Verhalten der Nachkommenschaft der Rassenbastarde. Berichte der deutschen botanischen Gesellschaft 18:158–168. Coutant C. 1987. Thermal preference: when does an asset become a liability? Environ Biol Fish 18: 161-172. Cowly D. E., and Atchley W. R. 1992. Quantitative genetic models for development, epigenetic selection, and phenotypic evolution. Evolution 46: 495-518. Criscuolo F., Monaghan, P., Nasir L., and Metcalfe N. B. 2008. Early nutrition and phenotypic development: ‘catch-up’ growth leads to elevated metabolic rate in adulthood. P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 275: 1565-1570. Crispo, E. and L. J. Chapman. 2011. Hypoxia drives divergence in cichlid body shape. Evol Ecol 25:949-964. Darwin C. 1859. On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: J. Murray. De Vries, H. 1889. Intracellulare Pangenesis. Jena. Gustav Fisher. DeWitt T. J., and Scheiner S. M., eds. 2004 Phenotypic Plasticity. Functional and Conceptual Approaches. New York: Oxford University Press. Diaz R. L., and Rosenberg R., 1995. Marine benthic hypoxia: a review of its ecological effects and the behavioural responses of benthic macrofauna. Oceanogr. Mar Biol Ann Rev 33:245–303. Domenici P., and Blake R. 1997. The kinematics and performance of fish faststart swimming. J Exp Biol 200:1165-1178. Domenici P., Steffensen, J. F., and Batty, R. S. 2000. The effect of progressive hypoxia on swimming activity and schooling in Atlantic herring. J Fish Biol 57: 1526-1538. Doudoroff P. S., and Shumway D. L. 1970. Dissolved oxygen requirements of freshwater fishes. FAO Technical Paper 86. Dudley, S. A., and Schmitt, J. 1996. Testing the adaptive plasticity hypothesis: density-dependent selection on manipulated stem length in Impatiens capensis. Am Nat 147:445–465. 146 Eames S. C., Philipson, L. H., Prince, V. E., and Kinkel, M. D. 2010. Blood Sugar Measurement in Zebrafish Reveals Dynamics of Glucose Homeostasis. Zebrafish 7: 205-213. Eidietis L., Forrester T. L., and Webb, P. W. 2002. Relative abilities to correct rolling disturbances of three morphologically different fish. Can J Zoolog 80: 2156-2163. Ellison P. T. 2010. Fetal programming and fetal psychology. Infant Child Dev 19: 6-20. Engeszer R. E., Da Barbiano L., Ryan M., and Parichy, D. 2007a. Timing and plasticity of shoaling behaviour in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Anim Behav 74: 1269-1275. Engeszer R.E., L.B. Patterson, A.A. Rao, and D.M. Parichy. 2007b. Zebrafish in the wild: a review of natural history and new notes from the field. Zebrafish 4: 21-40. Espmark Å.M., Eriksen, M. S., Salte R., Braastad B. O., and Bakken, M. 2008. A note on pre-spawning maternal cortisol exposure in farmed Atlantic salmon and its impact on the behaviour of offspring in response to a novel environment. Applied Anim Behav Sci 110: 404-409. Farrell A. P., Bennett W., and Devlin R. H. 1997. Growth-enhanced transgenic salmon can be inferior swimmers. Can J Zool 75: 335-337. Ferguson R. A., Kieffer J. D., and Tufts B. L. 1993. The effects of body size on the acid–base and metabolite status in the white muscle of rainbow trout before and following exhaustive exercise. J Exp Biol 180: 195–207. Fisher R. A. 1918. The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian Inheritance. T Royal Soc Edin 52: 399-433. Fisher, R.A. 1930. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Clarendon Press, Oxford. Flury, B. 1988. Common Principal Components and Related Multivariate Models. New York: Wiley and Sons.. Frost A. J., Winrow-Giffen A., Ashley P. J., and Sneddon L. U. 2007. Plasticity in animal personality traits: does prior experience alter the degree of boldness? Proc Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 274: 333-339. Fuzzen M. L. M., Alderman S. L., Bristow E. N., and Bernier N. J. 2011. Ontogeny of the corticotropin-releasing factor system in rainbow trout and 147 differential effects of hypoxia on the endocrine and cellular stress responses during development. Gen Comp Endocrinol 170: 604-612. García-Berthou E. 1999. Food of introduced mosquitofish: ontogenetic diet shift and prey selection. J Fish Biol 55: 135-147. Gianoli E., and González-Teuber M. 2005. Environmental heterogeneity and population differentiation in plasticity to drought in Convolvulus chilensis (Convolvulaceae) Evol Ecol 19: 603-613. Giesing E. R., Suski C. D., Warner R. E., and Bell A. M. 2010. Female sticklebacks transfer information via eggs: effects of maternal experience with predators on offspring. Proc Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 278: 1753-1759. Gilbert S. F. 2001. Ecological developmental biology: Developmental biology meets the real world. Dev Biol 233:1-12. Glover V. 2011. Prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: an evolutionary perspective. J Child Psychol Psych 52: 356-367. Godin J.-G. J., and Morgan M. J. 1985. Predator avoidance and school size in a cyprinodontid fish, the banded killifish (Fundulus diaphanus Lesueur). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16: 105-110. Gould S. J., and Lewontin R. C. 1979. The Spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Paradigm: A Critique of the Adaptationist Programme. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 205: 581-598. Gould, S. J. 1977. Ontogeny and Phylogeny. The Belknap Press, Cambridge. Grafen A. 1988. On the uses of data on lifetime reproductive success. In: Reproductive Success. Clutton-Brock T. editor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Grunwald, D. J., and Eisen, J. S. 2002. Headwaters of the zebrafish – emergence of a new model vertebrate. Nat Rev Genet 3:717-724. Gupta A. P., and Lewonti, R. C. 1982 Evolution (Lawrence, Kans.) 36:934–948. Haldane, J. B. S. 1924. A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection. Part I. Trans Camb Phil Soc 23:19-41. Hales C. N., and Barker D. J. P. 2001. The thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Br. Med. Bull. 60:5–20. 148 Hall B. K. 2001. Organic Selection: Proximate Environmental Effects on the Evolution of Morphology and Behaviour. Biology and Philosophy 16: 215237. Hall, B. K. 2003. Evo-Devo: Evolutionary developmental mechanisms. Int J Dev Biol 47: 491-495. Hall B. K., Pearson R. D.,and Müller G. B. 2004. Environment, development, and evolution. Cambridge: MIT Press. Healy S. D., and Rowe C. 2007. A critique of comparative studies of brain size. Proc Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 274: 453-464. Hellemans K. G. C., Sliwowska J.H., Verma P., and Weinberg J. 2010. Prenatal alcohol exposure: Fetal programming and later life vulnerability to stress, depression and anxiety disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34: 791-807. Hoare D. J., Krause J., Peuhkuri N., and Godin J.-G. J. 2000. Body size and shoaling in fish. J Fish Biol 57: 1351-1366. Hopkins S. R., and Powell F. L. 2001. Common themes of adaptation to hypoxia: insights from comparative physiology. Adv Exp Med Biol 502:153–167. Hocher B., Slowinksi T., Bauer C., and Halle H. 2001. The advanced fetal programming hypothesis. Nephrol Dial Transplant 16: 1298-1299. Hoverman J. T., and Reylea R. A. 2007. How flexible is phenotypic plasticity? Developmental windows for trait induction and reversal. Ecology 88:693705. Hoverman J. T., and Reylea R. A. 2008. Temporal environmental variation and phenotypic plasticity: a mechanism underlying priority effects. Oikos 117:23-32. Hughes G. M. 1984. Measurement of gill area in fishes: practices and problems. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 64: 637-655. Huntingford F.A., Andrew G., Mackenzie S., Morera D., Coyle S. M., Pilarczyk M., and Kadri S. 2010. Coping strategies in a strongly schooling fish, the common carp (Cyprinus carpio). J Fish Biol 76: 1576-1591. Ibarra A. M., and Famula, T. R. 2008. Genotype by environment interaction for adult body weights of shrimp Penaeus vannamei when grown at low and high densities. Genet Sel Evol 40:541–551. 149 Iwama G.K., Morgan J.D., and Barton B.A. 1995. Simple field methods for monitoring stress and general condition of fish. Aquaculture Research 26: 273-282. Roff, D. A. 2007. A centennial celebration for quantitative genetics. Evolution 61, 1017-1032 (2007).Jobling, M. 1994. Fish Bioenergetics. Volume 13: 1-44. London: Chapman and Hall. Jensen F. B. and Weber R. E. 1982. Respiratory properties of tench blood and hemoglobin: adaptation to hypoxichypercapnic water. Mol Physiol 2:235– 250. Jones D. R. 1971. Theoretical analysis of factors which may limit the maximum oxygen uptake of fish: The oxygen cost of the cardiac and branchial pumps. J Theor Biol 32: 341-349. Jones R. H., and Ozanne S. E. 2009. Fetal programming of glucose-insulin metabolism. Mol Cell Endocrinol 297: 4-9. Jonz M. G., and Nurse C. A. 2005. Development of oxygen sensing in the gills of zebrafish. J Exp Biol 208:1537-1549. Kajimura, S., Aida, K., and Duan, C. 2005. Insulin-like growth factor-binding protein-1 (IGFBP-1) mediates hypoxia-induced embryonic growth and developmental retardation. Proc Nat A Sci USA 102, 1240 –1245. Kapoor A., Dunn E., Kostaki A., Andrews M. H., and Matthews S.G. 2006. Fetal programming of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal function: prenatal stress and glucocorticoids. J Physiol 572: 31-44. Ketterson E. D., Atwell J. W., and McGlothlin J. W. 2009. Phenotypic integration and independence: Hormones, performance, and response to environmental change. Integr Comp Biol 49: 365-379. Kieffer J. D. 2000. Limits to exhaustive exercise in fish. Comp Biochem Phys A 126: 161-179. Kingsolver J. G., and Huey R. B. 1998. Evolutionary analyses of morphological And physiological plasticity in thermally variable environments. Amer. Zool. 38:545- 560. Klingenberg C. P., Barluenga M., and Meyer A. 2003. Body shape variation in cichlid fishes of the Amphilophus citrinellus species complex. Biol J Linn Soc 80:397–408. Klingenberg, C. P. 2008. MORPHOJ. Faculty of Life Sciences, University of 150 Manchester, UK. http://www.flywings.org.uk/MorphoJ_page.htm. Kotrschal K., Van Staaden M. J., and Huber R. 1998. Fish Brains: Evolution and Environmental Relationships. Rev Fish Biol Fish 8: 373-408. Kotrschal A., and Taborsky B. (2010). Environmental change enhances cognitive abilities in fish. PLoS Biol 8, e1000351. Krause J., and Godin J.-G. J. 1994. Shoal Choice in the Banded Killifish (Fundulus diaphanus, Teleostei, Cyprinodontidae): Effects of Predation Risk, Fish Size, Species Composition and Size of Shoals. Ethology 98: 128-136. Lande R. 1979. Quantitative genetic analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain:body size allometry. Evolution 33:402-416. Lande R. 2009. Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation. J Evol Biol 22: 1435-1446. Levins, R. 1968 Evolution in Changing Environments. Princeton University Press, Princeton. Lima S. L., and Dill L. M. 1990. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68: 619-640. Lo K. H., Hui M. N., Yu R. M., Wu R. S., and Cheng S. H. 2011 Hypoxia Impairs Primordial Germ Cell Migration in Zebrafish (Danio rerio) Embryos. PLoS One 6:e24540 Lowe-Jinde L., and Niimi A. J. 1983. Influence of sampling on the interpretation of haematological measurements of rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri). Can J Zool 61: 396-402. Lynch M., and Walsh B. 1998. Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits. Sunderland: Sinauer. MacCormack T. J., and Driedzic W. R. 2007. The impact of hypoxia on in vivo glucose uptake in a hypoglycemic fish (Myoxocephalus scorpius). Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 292: R1033-R1042. Machiels, M. A. M., and Wijsman J. 1996. Size-selective mortality in an exploited perch population and the reconstruction of potential growth. Ann Zool Fenn 33: 397-401. Marks, C., West, T. N., Bagatto, B., and Moore, F. B-G. 2005. Developmental environment alters conditional aggression in zebrafish. 2005: 901-908. 151 McArdle H. J., Andersen H. S., Jones H., and Gambling L. 2006. Fetal Programming: Causes and Consequences as Revealed by Studies of Dietary Manipulation in Rats – A Review. Placenta 27, Supplement: 56-60. Metcalfe N. B., and Monaghan, P. 2001. Compensation for a bad start: grow now, pay later? Trends Ecol Evol 16: 254-260. Mohamed, M. P., and Kutty M. N. 1983. Influence of hypoxia on metabolism and activity in Puntius sarana (Hamilton) (Pisces: Cyprinidae). Proc Indiana Acad Sci Anim Sci 92: 215-220. Monna, F., Camizuli E., Revelli P., Biville C., Thomas C., Losno R., Scheifler R., Bruguier O., Baron S., Chateau C., Ploquin A., and Alibert P. 2011. Wild brown trout affected by historical mining in the C vennes National Park, France. Envir Sci Tech 45: 6823-6830. Moore F. B.-G., Hosey M., and Bagatto B. 2006. Cardiovascular system in larval zebrafish responds to developmental hypoxia in a family specific manner. Front Zool 3: 4. Monaghan P. 2008. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental change. Philos Trans R Soc B 363: 1635-1645. Moran, N. A. 1992. The evolutionary maintenance of alternative phenotypes. Am Nat 139: 971-989. Murren C. J. 2002. Phenotypic integration in plants. Plant Spec Biol 17:89–99. Nilsson, G. E. 2007. Gill remodeling in fish: a new fashion or an ancient secret? J Exp Biol 210:2403–2409. Noltie D. B., and Wang N. 1997. Use of compensatory growth to double hybrid sunfish growth rates. T Am Fish Soc 126: 316-322. O’Connor E. A., Pottinger T. G., and Sneddon L. U. 2011. The effects of acute and chronic hypoxia on cortisol, glucose and lactate concentrations in different populations of three-spined stickleback. Fish Biochem Physiol 37: 461-469. Osenberg C. W., Mittelbach G. G., and Wainwright P. C. (1992). Two-stage life histories in fish: the interaction between juvenile competition and adult performance. Ecology 73: 255-267. Padilla P.A., and Roth M.B. 2001. Oxygen deprivation causes suspended animation in the zebrafish embryo. Proc Nat A Sci 98: 7331-7335. 152 Pankhurst, P. M., Pankhurst N. W., and Montgomery J. C. 1993. Comparison of behavioural and morphological measures of visual acuity during ontogeny in a teleost fish, Forsterygion varium, tripterygiidae (Forster, 1801). Brain Behav Evolut 42: 178-188. Parsons G. R., and Carlson J. K.. 1998. Physiological and behavioral responses to hypoxia in the bonnethead shark (Sphyrna tiburo): routine swimming and respiratory regulation. Fish Biochem Physiol 19: 189-196. Pelster B., and Burggren, W. W. 1996. Disruption of hemoglobin oxygen transport does not impact oxygen-dependent physiological processes in developing embryos of zebra fish (Danio rerio). Circ Res 79:358–362. Pelster, B. 1997. Oxygen, temperature, and pH influences on the development of nonmammalian embryos and larvae. In: Development of Cardiovascular Systems. Burggren W. W., and Keller B. B. eds. , Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pelster B., Sanger A. M., Siegele M., Schwerte T. 2003. Influence of swim training on cardiac activity, tissue apillarization, and mitochondrial density in muscle tissue of zebrafish larvae. Am J Physiol 285: R339-R347. Phillips P. C., and Arnold S. J. 1999. Hierarchical comparison of genetic variance-covariance matrices. I. Using the Flury hierarchy. Evolution 53: 1506-1515. Phillips N. E. 2004. Variable timing of larval food has conseuqnces for early juvenile performance in a marine mussel. Ecology 85: 2341-2346. Pichavant K., Person-Le-Ruyet J., Le Bayon N., Severe A., Le Roux A., Quemener L., Maxime V., Nonnotte G., and Boeuf G. 2000. Effects of hypoxia on growth and metabolism of juvenile turbot. Aquaculture 188:103-114. Pichavant K., Person-Le-Ruyet J., Le Bayon N., Severe A., Le Roux A., Quemener L., Maxime V., Nonnotte G., and Boeuf G. 2001. Comparative effects of long-term hypoxia on growth, feeding and oxygen consumption in juvenile turbot and European sea bass. J Fish Biol 59: 875-883. Pigliucci M., Whitton J., and Schlichting C. D. 1995. Reaction norms of Arabidopsis. I. Plasticity of characters and correlations across water,nutrient and light gradients. J Evol Biol 8: 421–438. 153 Pigliucci M. 1998. Developmental phenotypic plasticity: Where internal programming meets the external environment. Curr Opin Plant Biol 1: 8791. Pigliucci M. 2001. Phenotypic Plasticity: Beyond Nature and Nurture. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Pigliucci M. 2003. Phenotypic integration: studying the ecology and evolution of complex phenotypes. Ecol Lett 6: 265-272. Pincetich, C. A., Viant M. R., Hinton D. E., and Tjeerdema R. S. 2005. Metabolic changes in Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes) during embryogenesis and hypoxia as determined by in vivo P-31 NMR. Comp Biochem Physiol C Toxicol Pharm 140: 103-113. Pigliucci, M. & Muller, G. (Eds.) 2010. Evolution: The Extended Synthesis. MIT Press, Cambridge. Pitcher T. J., Magurran A. E., and Winfield I. J. 1982. Fish in larger shoals find food faster. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10: 149-151. Plaut I., and Gordon M. 1994. Swimming metabolism of wild-type and cloned zebrafish Brachydanio rerio. J Exp Biol 194: 209-223. Pollen A. A., Dobberfuhl A. P., Scace J., Igulu M. M., Renn S. C. P., Shumway C. A., and Hofmann H. A. 2007. Environmental Complexity and Social Organization Sculpt the Brain in Lake Tanganyikan Cichlid Fish. Brain Behav Evol 70: 21-39. Pollock M. S., Clarke L. M. J, and Dubé, M. G. 2007. The Effects of Hypoxia on Fishes: From Ecological Relevance to Physiological Effects. Environ Rev 15: 1–14. Rabalais, N. N., and Turner R. E. (eds) Coastal hypoxia: consequences for living resources and ecosystems. American Geophysical Union, Washington, DC. Reed, T. E., Waples, R. S., Schindler, D. E., Hard, J. J., and Kinnison, M. T. 2010. Phenotypic plasticity and population viability: the importance of environmental predictability. Proc of the Roy Soc B Biol Sci 277:3391 – 3400. Reylea, R. A. 2001. The lasting effects of adaptive plasticity: Predator-induced tadpoles become longer-legged frogs. Ecology 82:1947-1955. 154 Relyea, R. A. 2003. Predators come and predators go: The reversibility of predator-induced traits. Ecology 84: 1840-1848. Relyea, R. A. 2011. Adaptive plasticity in predator-induced defenses in a Common freshwater snail: altered selection and mode of predation due to prey phenotype. Evol Ecol 25: 189-202. Remold, S. K., Lenski, R. E. 2001. Contribution of individual random mutations to genotype-by-environment interactions in Escherichia coli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:11388-11393 Reusch T., and Blanckenhorn W. U. 1998. Quantitative genetics of the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea: Cheverud’s conjecture revisited. Heredity 81: 111–119. Richards, R. 1992. The Meaning of Evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Rinaldi L., Basso P., Tettamanti G., Grimaldi A., Terova G., Saroglia M. and DeEguileor M. 2005. Oxygen availability causes morphological changes and a different VEGF/Flk-1/HIF-2 expression pattern in sea bass gills. Ital J Zool 72: 103-111. Robison, O. W. 1981. The influence of maternal effects on the efficiency of selection: A review. Livest Prod Sci 8:121-137. Roff, Derek. 2000. The Evolution of the G Matrix: Selection or Drift? Heredity 84:135–142. Roff, D. A. A. 2007. A centennial celebration for quantitative genetics. Evolution 61:1017-1032. Rolf F. J., and Slice D. 1990. Extensions of the Procustes method for the optimal superimposition of landmarks. Syst Zool 39: 40-59. Rohlf, F. J. 2001. tpsDig2: a program for landmark development and analysis. http://life.bio.sunysb.edu/morph. Rohlf, F. J. 2010. tpsRelw, version 1.49. Stony Brook, NY: Department of Ecology and Evolution, State University of New York. Saint-Paul, U. 1984. Physiological adaptation to hypoxia of a neotropical characoid fish Colossoma macropomum, Serrasalmidae. Environ Biol Fish 11: 53–62. 155 Scheiner, S. M.1993. Genetics and evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 24:35–68 Schlichting, C. D. 1986. The evolution of phenotypic plasticity in plants. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 17: 667–693. Schlichting C. D., and Pigliucci M. 1998. Phenotypic evolution : a reaction norm perspective. Sunderland: Sinauer. Schlichting C. D. and Smith H. 2002. Phenotypic plasticity: linking molecular mechanisms with evolutionary outcomes. Evol Ecol 16: 189-211. Scott G. R., and Sloman K. A. 2004. The effects of environmental pollutants on complex fish behaviour: integrating behavioural and physiological indicators of toxicity. Aquat Toxicol 68: 369-392. Sgro, C. M., and Hoffmann, A. A. 1998. Effects of stress combinations on the expression of additive genetic variation for fecundity in Drosophila melanogaster. Gen Res 72:13-18 Shang E. H. H., and Wu R. S. S. 2004. Aquatic Hypoxia Is a Teratogen and Affects Fish Embryonic Development. Environ Sci Technol 38: 4763-4767. Shumway C. A. 2008. Habitat Complexity, Brain, and Behavior. Brain Behav Evol 72: 123-134. Sih A., and Moore R. 1993. Delayed hatching of salamander eggs in response to enhanced larval predation risk. Am Nat 142: 947-960. Silkin Y. A., and Silkina E. N. 2005. Effect of Hypoxia on PhysiologicalBiochemical Blood Parameters in Some Marine Fish. J Evol Biochem Phys 41: 527-532. Slatkin M. 1987. Quantitative genetics of heterochrony. Evolution 41: 799-811. Smit G. L., and Hattingh J. 1978. The effect of respiratory stress on carp haemoglobin. Comp Biochem Physiol A Physiol 59: 369-374. Sneddon L. U., Braithwaite V. A., and Gentle M. J. 2003. Novel object test: examining nociception and fear in the rainbow trout. J of Pain 4: 431-440. Sollid J., De Angelis P., Gundersen K., and Nilsson G. E. 2003. Hypoxia induces adaptive and reversible gross morphological changes in crucian carp gills. J Exp Biol 206: 3667–3673, 156 Sollid J., and Nilsson G. 2006. Plasticity of respiratory structures - Adaptive remodeling of fish gills induced by ambient oxygen and temperature. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 154: 241-251. Spence R., Gerlach G., Lawrence C., and Smith C. 2008. The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol Rev 83: 13-34. Spicer J. L., and El-Gamal M. M. 1999. Hypoxia accelerates the development of respiratory regulation in brine shrimp – but at a cost. J exp Biol 202: 3637– 3646. Spicer J. L., and Burggren, W. W. 2003. Development of physiological regulatory systems: altering the timing of crucial events. Zoology 106:91–99. Stamatiou D., and Liew C. 2003. Gene expression profile of zebrafish exposed to hypoxia during development. Physiol Genomics 13: 97-106. Steppan S. J., Phillips P. C., and Houle D. 2002. Comparative quantitative genetics: evolution of the G matrix. Trends Ecol Evol 17: 320–327. Storey, K. B. 1988. Suspended animation-the molecular basis of metabolic depression. Can J Zool 66: 124-132. Strecker R., Seiler T.-B., Hollert H., and Braunbeck T. 2011. Oxygen requirements of zebrafish (Danio rerio) embryos in embryo toxicity tests with environmental samples. Comp Biochem Phys C 153: 318-327. Taborsky B. 2006. The influence of juvenile and adult environments on lifehistory trajectories. Proc Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 273: 741-750. Talwar P. K., and Jhingran A. G. 1991. Inland fishes of India and adjacent countries. Calcutta.: Oxford & I. B. H. Publishing. Tanaka K., Osako Y., and Yuri K. 2010. Juvenile social experience regulates central neuropeptides relevant to emotional and social behaviors. Neuroscience 166: 1036-1042. Timmerman, C. M., and Chapman L. J. 2004. Hypoxia and interdemic variation in the sailfin molly (Poecilia latipinna). J Fish Biol 65: 635-650 Ton C., Stamatiou D., and Liew C. C. 2003. Gene expression profile of zebrafish exposed to hypoxia during development. Physiol Genomics 13: 97-106. Tonsor S. J., and Scheiner S. M.. 2007. Plastic trait integration across a CO2 Gradient in Arabidopsis thaliana. Am Nat 169: E119–E140. 157 Tyler R. M., and Targett T. E. 2007. Juvenile weakfish, (Cynoscion regalis), distribution in relation to diel-cycling dissolved oxygen in an estuarine tributary. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 333: 257-269. Vainikka A., Jokelainen T., Kortet R., and Ylönen H. 2005. Predation risk allocation or direct vigilance response in the predator interaction between perch (Perca fluviatilis L.) and pike (Esox lucius L.)? Ecol Freshw Fish 14: 225-232. Van Tienderen, P. H. 1991. Evolution of generalists and specialists in spatially heterogeneous environments. Evolution 45: 1317–1331. Van Raaij, M. T. M., Van den Thillart G., Vianen G. J., Pit D. S. S., Balm P. H. M., and Steffens B.. 1996. Substrate mobilization and hormonal changes in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss, L.) and common carp (Cyprinus carpio, L.) during deep hypoxia and subsequent recovery. J Comp Physiol B 166: 443-452. Via S. 1984. The quantitative genetics of polyphagy in an insect herbivore. 1. Genotype-environment interaction in larval performance on different host plant species. Evolution 38: 881–895. Via S., and Lande R. 1985. Genotype-Environment interaction and the evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Evolution 39: 505-522. Via S., Gomulkiewicz R., De Jong G., Scheiner S. M.., Schlichting, CD. and Van Tienderen, P. H. 1995. Adaptive phenotypic plasticity: consensus and controversy. Trends Ecol Evol 10:212–217. Vascotto S. G., Beckham Y., and Kelly G. M. 1997. The zebrafish’s swim to fame as an experimental model in biology. Biochem Cell Biol 75:479–485. Vulesevic B., and Perry S. F. 2006. Developmental plasticity of ventilatory control in zebrafish, Danio rerio. Resp Physiol Neurobi 154: 396-405. Waddington, C. H.1952. Selection of the genetic basis for an acquired character. Nature 169:278. Waddington, C. H. 1953. Genetic assimilation of an acquired character. Evolution 7:118-126. Waitt D. E., and Levin D. A. 1998. Genetic and phenotypic correlations in plants: a botanical test of Cheverud’s conjecture. Heredity 80:3 10–319. Wang C., Li, S., Liu, Z. Xiang S., Wang J., Pang Z., Duan J. 2006. Developmental quantitative genetic analysis of body weight and 158 morphological traits in red common carp, Cyprinus carpio L. Aquaculture 251: 219-30. Webb P.W., and Fairchild A. G. 2001. Performance and maneuverability of three species of teleostean fishes. Can J Zool 79: 1866-1877. Werner E.E., and Hall D.J. 1988. Ontogenetic Habitat Shifts in Bluegill: The Foraging Rate-Predation Risk Trade-off. Ecology 69: 1352-1366. West-Eberhard, M. J. 1989. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 20:249-278. West-Eberhard M. J. 2003. Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press. West-Eberhard M. J. 2005. Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. 102: 6543-6549. Westerfield M. 1994. The Zebrafish Book. Eugene: University of Oregon Press Widmer S., Moore F. B-G., and Bagatto B. 2006. The effects of chronic developmental hypoxia on swimming performance in zebrafish. J Fish Biol 69: 1885-1891. Wilson A. D. M., and McLaughlin R. L. 2010. Foraging behaviour and brain morphology in recently emerged brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64: 1905-1914. Wright, S. 1932. The roles of mutation, inbreeding, crossbreeding and selection in evolution. Proc 6th Int Cong Genet 1:356–66. Wright D., Rimmer L. B., Pritchard V. L., Krause J., and Butlin R. K. 2003. Inter and intra-population variation in shoaling and boldness in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Naturwissenschaften 90: 374-377. Wu L., and Culver D. A. 1992. Ontogenetic diet shift in Lake Erie age-0 yellow perch (Perca flavescens): a size-related response to zooplankton density. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 49: 1932-1937. Wu R. S. S. 2002. Hypoxia: from molecular responses to ecosystem responses. Mar Pollut Bull 45: 35-45. Yang A. P. 1993. The influence of changing resources on life-history patterns of allocation and plasticity in female guppies. Ecology 74: 2011-2019. Zanuy S., and Carrillo M. 1985. Annual cycles of growth, feeding rate, gross 159 conversion efficiency and hematocrit levels of sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax L.) adapted to two different osmotic media. Aquaculture 44: 11-25. 160