* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download An effect study of `Creative Expressive Arts Therapy` for sexual
Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup
Abnormal psychology wikipedia , lookup
Psychedelic therapy wikipedia , lookup
Factitious disorder imposed on another wikipedia , lookup
Child psychopathology wikipedia , lookup
Pyotr Gannushkin wikipedia , lookup
Controversy surrounding psychiatry wikipedia , lookup
An effect study of ‘Creative Expressive Arts Therapy’ for sexual abused children in South Africa and a possible moderation effect of social support. M. E. van Overstraten Kruijsse (3400654), Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands Supervisors: Prof. Dr. R. Kleber (The Netherlands) and Drs. N. van Westrhenen (in South Africa) General coordinators: Prof. Drs. T. Knijn. Date: 29 may 2014 Utrecht University Teddy Bear Clinic Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences Johannesburg Branch Heidelberglaan 1 13 Joubert Street 3584 CS Utrecht Parktown, Johannesburg The Netherlands South Africa Abstract Introduction: In this research a ‘Creative Expressive Arts Therapy (CEAT)’ is piloted for sexually abused children in South Africa, at the Teddy Bear Clinic for abused children. The effect of CEAT on Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) symptoms was evaluated and social support was measured as possible moderator. Methods: This study included 13 participants in the age from 8 to 12 years, 4 children were on a waiting list and 9 children participated in 10 sessions of CEAT. PTSD symptoms were measured with the Young Child PTSD Checklist (YCPC) and the PTSD section of the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children- Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL). The perceived social support was measured with the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS). Pre and post CEAT measurements were conducted and compared. Results: CEAT did not decrease the PTSD symptoms of the children, measured with the YCPC and K-SADS-PL, and results did not show significant differences between the therapy and control group. Neither did the analysis show evidence for social support as moderator between CEAT and PTSD symptoms. The presumption is made that the results are caused by the amount of methodological constraints. For parents did indicate to see a difference after CEAT in the children in the therapy group; they showed a decrease in aggressive behaviour, sadness, loneliness and nightmares. Conclusion: The results show no statistical evidence of a positive effect of CEAT, although qualitative data show a positive effect of CEAT on PTSD symptoms. This supports the use of CEAT with sexually abused children. Besides statistical results, this research found numerous recommendations for future research in the South African context. Keywords: Sexual abuse, Creative Expressive Arts Therapy, PTSD, Social Support, South Africa. 2 Samenvatting Introductie: Huidig onderzoek is een pilot onderzoek voor de toepassing van ‘Creative Expressive Arts Therapy (CEAT)’ voor seksueel misbruikte kinderen in Zuid-Afrika, in de ‘Teddy Bear Clinic’ voor misbruikte kinderen. Het effect van de CEAT, op Post Traumatische Stress Stoornis (PTSS) symptomen, is geëvalueerd en sociale steun is gemeten als mogelijke moderator. Methode: Aan huidig onderzoek namen 13 participanten deel in de leeftijd van 8 tot 12 jaar, 4 kinderen stonden op de wachtlijst en 9 kinderen zaten in de CEAT groep van 10 sessies. PTSS symptomen zijn gemeten met behulp van de ‘Young Child PTSD Checklist (YCPC) en de PTSS sectie van de Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for SchoolAge Children- Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL). De ervaren sociale steun is gemeten door middel van de Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS). Metingen voor en na de CEAT zijn afgenomen en met elkaar vergeleken. Resultaten: Het volgen van CEAT verminderde de PTSS symptomen niet, gemeten op de YCPC en de K-SADS-PL, en er zijn geen verschillen tussen de therapie en controle groep gevonden. Noch bleek sociale steun te functioneren als moderator tussen de CEAT en de PTSS symptomen. Het vermoeden bestaat dat het gebrek aan resultaten het gevolg is van verschillende methodologische beperkingen. Ouders gaven namelijk aan dat kinderen in de therapie groep wel degelijk verschillen lieten zien; zij lieten een vermindering in agressief gedrag, bedroefdheid, eenzaamheid en nachtmerries zien. Conclusie: De resultaten geven geen statistisch bewijs voor een positief effect van de CEAT, daarentegen wijzen kwalitatieve gegevens wel op positieve resultaten van de CEAT op PTSS symptomen zien. Dit ondersteunt de toepassing van CEAT op seksueel misbruikte kinderen, maar geeft ook het belang van vervolg onderzoek aan. Naast de statistische resultaten verschaft het huidig onderzoek talrijke aanbevelingen voor toekomstig onderzoek in de Zuid-Afrikaanse context. Sleutelwoorden: Seksueel misbruik, Creative Expressive Arts Therapy, PTSS, Sociale Steun, Zuid-Afrika. 3 ‘The thing no one understands about me is that I have a small heart when I am scared’ Child during the pre-measurements 4 Forword This thesis is written as an assignment for the master Clinical and Health Psychology, at the University of Utrecht. The thesis is written about sexually abused children in South Africa, Johannesburg, and the possible effect of a Creative Expressive Arts Therapy (CEAT). In February 2014 I arrived in South Africa, an unknown country to me. Therefore I expected some challenges, as we were in a different context than we were used to. But still I can say that I underestimated it. During these four months of research in South Africa, I learned more about the challenges of research than I could ever have learned in the Netherlands. There where weeks were we contacted Nadine (our supervisor in South Africa) almost every day, to discuss the necessary adaptations. Not only my research skills, but also my patience, flexibility and ‘thinking in solutions’ got refined. And last, but definitely not least, we got to make a lot of precious contacts with people in the Teddy Bear Clinic and the lovely children that participated in the research. I have to thank several persons for their effort and making my time in South Africa so amazing. First of all Drs. Nadine van Westrhenen, our supervisor in South Africa, she helped us whenever necessary and made time for us in her already busy schedule. Not only did she help us with the research, she also took us on trips and gave us tons of tips. I want to thank Prof. R. Kleber for his useful feedback and the long distant calls. The Teddy Bear Clinic for opening their doors for us and for letting us use their files, time, space and help. In particular the help of their social workers Charles and Ndumiso, who helped us more than we could have wished for. Not only their help but also their company during our months are so much appreciated. All the students who helped us conducting the questionnaires, without them we could have stopped our research in week 3. And many thanks go to the children and caregivers who participated in this research, it was not always easy to come to therapy, but we made it! Finally, thanks to everyone who helped us or made our time in South Africa even better than it already was. In this research I worked closely with 2 other students; Marloe Leermakers and Robbe Stolk. Al three of us had the same mean research question, but took a different sub research question to make our research more individual. Marloe Leermakers and I both took social support as an extra variable, therefore we worked 5 together most of the research and wrote parts together as well. This because of the great amount of overlap. I took social support as a moderator and Marloe Leermakers took social support as a mediation, parts about social support are therefore individual. There was a good distribution in subjects, everything was discussed carefully and both put in the same amount of effort in the end product. Therefore I want to thank Marloe Leermakers for the pleasant partnership. 6 Contents 1. Abstract English pp. 2 2. Abstract Dutch pp. 3 3. Forword pp. 5 4. Content pp. 7 5. Case study pp. 8 6. Introduction pp. 9 a. Cultural sensitive intervention: CEAT pp. 10 b. Social support pp. 12 c. Theoretical framework pp. 13 d. Goals of this study pp. 15 7. Method pp. 15 a. Participants pp. 15 b. Instruments pp. 16 c. Procedure pp. 17 d. Statistical analyses pp. 18 8. Results pp. 20 a. Reliability of questionnaires pp. 20 b. Development of the posttraumatic stress symptoms pp. 20 of the research group c. Comparison of the intervention and control group pp. 21 d. Qualitative findings pp. 22 e. Social support as a possible moderator pp. 22 f. The influence of the relation with the perpetrator pp. 23 on social support 9. Discussion pp. 23 a. General findings pp. 23 b. Insights & limitations pp. 25 i. Measurements of PTSD symptom pp. 25 ii. Instruments pp. 27 iii. This research pp. 29 10. Conclusion pp. 31 11. References pp. 32 7 Case study Dembe (pseudonym) is a skinny black boy, 12 years old, who lives in a safe house in Florida, Johannesburg. He does not know who his father is and he witnessed his mother die, which had a great impact on him. Therefore, he lived the major part of his life with foster parents. His foster father seriously abused him physically (beat him up and let him sleep in the field) and both his foster parents neglected him. For that reason he was put in a safe house. In the safe house he is abused, several times, by other boys who lived in the same house. ‘Here the older boys teach the younger boys how to rape.’ When we met him, he stuttered, did not have many social contacts, felt lonely, he was very aggressive and had intense nightmares (they had to tie him to the bed to keep him under control). Therapy Dembe participated in the Creative Expressive Arts Therapy (CEAT) and was actively present. He opened up, especially during the music and dance sessions, and he became a loyal group member. They showed empathy for each other and exchanged stories and emotions, helping them feeling less lonely. The social worker saw that they assisted each other through the activities. Dembe did not have to talk during therapy, which was an advantage for him, as talking was scary since he stuttered. Dembe loved coming to therapy, he would wait all day at the car so he would not miss it. Post therapy Dembe’s housemother told us that after the 10 CEAT sessions Dembe was less aggressive, which made him more social in a group. He still had nightmares, but instead of every day, they decreased to 3 nights a week. She noticed that he talked more often, without hesitation and less fear. She saw a happier child and was very grateful for the CEAT. Even though the questionnaires still showed PTSD, the CEAT helped Dembe process his trauma and gave him tools to help him in the future. 8 Introduction Sadly Dembe is not alone; South Africa has the world highest prevalence of community violence and levels of household abuse (Jewkes et al., 2006). Exposure to community violence (i.e. robbery, assault in- and outside the home) is associated with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms in AIDS orphaned South African children (Cluver, Gardner & Operario, 2008). Research shows high levels of PTSD (around 22-25%) amongst poor urban children in South Africa (Seedat, Nyamai, Njenga, Vythilingum & Stein, 2004). Child sexual abuse (CSA) can be seen as part of domestic and community violence, and is considered to be a major issue in South Africa (Jewkes & Abrahams, 2002; Jewkes, Penn-Kekana & Rose-Junius, 2005). A recent meta-analysis indicated that South Africa has the highest prevalence of CSA globally (Pereda, Guilera, Forns & Gómez-Benito, 2009). In the context of South Africa there is not enough help for all the victims, due to a lack of facilities. Furthermore, not all Western interventions, for example EMDR or Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, appear to be suitable for South African society. Therefore, this pilot study will develop and evaluate a cultural sensitive trauma intervention appropriate for South African society. The consequences of abuse can be severe and there is evidence that CSA is related to posttraumatic stress (Estes & Tidwell, 2002; Hall, Mathews & Pearce, 2002). CSA is defined as physical violation of a child’s body or being exposed to sexually inappropriate stimuli (Piercea & Bozalekb, 2004). CSA is ranked as the most serious abuse, in comparison with other kinds of abuse (physical maltreatment, societal abuse and non-physical maltreatment), by South Africans in the social sector and laypersons (Piercea & Bozalekb, 2004). PTSD can develop after experiencing, being involved in, or hearing an extreme traumatic stressor (APA, 2000, p.467), like CSA. PTSD is defined according to criteria as presented in the DSM-IV (APA, 2004). The DSM-IV is used in current study, as there are no instruments based on the DSM5 yet. One study found that CSA is the strongest predictor for developing PTSD in South African children, accounting for 25% of the psycho trauma cases (Seedat et al., 2004). In addition, PTSD among children has only been studied for the past 20 years and is still developing as a research field (Alisic, Jongmans, van Wesel & Kleber, 2011). Finally, there is a lack of quantitative research in the South African context in 9 the field of PTSD. This indicates the need for further research considering PTSD and interventions for children who experienced CSA. Cultural sensitive intervention: Creative expressive art therapy Looking for a cultural sensitive intervention in South Africa, going back to the roots of the society seems a logic choice. Dance, music and art are strongly embedded in the South African culture and therefore these activities seem suitable for a cultural sensitive intervention (Harris, 2009). Arts as a form of healing is probably as old as human society itself. Using arts as a professional therapy can be seen as a formal application of human tradition (Malchiodi, 1998). Naumburg (1950/1973) states that art therapy is a type of psychotherapy that uses art practices and interventions in combination with talking as a form of treatment. Nowadays, an increasing amount of evidence supports that creating art is a beneficial way to explore and control emotions and cognitions (Camic, 2008). The efficiency of creative expressive arts therapy (CEAT) with children is shown in multiple studies (Carolan, 2001; Douglass, 2001; Gilroy, 2006; Waller, 2006). However most of the studies concerning art therapy are case-studies, is qualitative research and have methodological constrains. Moreover, only one quantitative art therapy intervention study has been conducted in South Africa, and this study focussed on girls only (Pretorius & Pfeifer, 2010). Most research has been conducted in Western societies, and can therefore not automatically be implemented in South Africa. A need for further research that explores the effectiveness of arts-based methods is recommended by Camic (2008) in previous studies. Therefore, this study will develop and evaluate an art based trauma intervention, appropriate for the South African context. CEAT is a form of therapy used for self-expression and reflection by art. Each different form of therapy helps people to process their experiences in their own way; creation of art mediates reflection and personal exploration, dance allows for experiencing emotions through physical expression, and music has got a soothing capacity that demonstrates listening skills, and facilitates the increase of understanding and meaning (Malchiodi, 2008). The positive effect of art based interventions has been extensively discussed by research with traumatised children (Coholic, Lougheed & Cadell, 2009; DiSunno, Linton & Bowes, 2011; Kozlowska & Hanney, 2001; Lev-Wiesel & Liraz, 2007; Robarts, 2006; Rousseau et al., 2007). 10 CEAT is used in different styles of practice, from pure artistic expression to therapeutic tool in psychotherapy (Hogan, 2009). This research will use a ‘groupinteractive approach’, which not only uses art but also includes group interaction. There is no set protocol for CEAT, most therapists make their own. In this research the ‘Creative Expressive Arts Therapy Intervention Protocol for Traumatized Children’ (van Westrhenen, Fritz, Lemont & Oosthuizen, 2014) will be piloted. Pretorius and Pfeifer (2010) found positive results regarding anxiety and depression when implementing a group based art therapy intervention for sexually abused girls from 8 to 11 years old originating from South Africa. Even though most research on art based interventions is published in journals focussed on art therapy, there are enough indications to expect a positive effect of CEAT. While words are usually the strongest way to communicate among humans, sometimes words cannot describe the amount and complexity of feelings we experience (Harris, 2009). This is why art therapy can be an alternative way of expressing emotions and perceived experiences (Edwards, 2004). Expressing through art therapy gives an opportunity to explore the issues surrounding a traumatic event in a less threatening, nonverbal way, with some distance and control over disclosure (Backos & Pagon, 1999). Looking at the diversity of languages in South Africa, a barrier in communication can often occur. Moreover, cultural rites of art making that are healing and life enhancing already exist in South African culture (Harris, 2009). In addition children have difficulty using words as well when talking about feelings and thoughts (Hanney & Kozlowska, 2002), making CEAT appropriate for the South African context and for children. Or as seen in the case of Dembe, for children having trouble talking at all. Because of a lack of evidence for an appropriate therapy for (sexually) abused children in South Africa, this pilot study is an important addition to the current body of literature. The effect of CEAT as an intervention for traumatized children in South Africa on posttraumatic stress reactions will be explored in this research. The research question is ‘Does CEAT decrease PTSD symptoms in children who are sexually abused in South Africa?’ (Q1). Social support As stated before, there is a very high prevalence of abuse in South Africa, with negative psychological effects like PTSD (Carr, 2006). Nevertheless, there are many 11 victims of CSA who adapt and therefore function normally and effectively further in life (Farber & Egeland, 1997 in Hyman, Gold & Cottt, 2003). One factor that influences this recovery process is social support (Hyman et al., 2003). Social support is defined as perceived financial, physical, and emotional help from family, friends, and the community as a whole (Cluver, Fincham & Seedat, 2009). There are numerous studies that have looked at social support in combination with sexually abused children or adults (Cluver et al., 2009; Davidson, Inslicht & Baum, 2000; Runtz & Schallow, 1997) and as a moderator in different types of therapies (apart from CEAT) (Orgodniczuk, Piper, Joyce, McCallum & Rosie, 2002; Taylor et al., 2007; Thrasher, Power, Morant, Marks & Dalgleish, 2010). However, not one study has examined social support as a possible moderator between CEAT and its outcome in the South African population yet. This study is an attempt to fill this gap in literature. Perceived social support is related to a better recovery process and is seen as a buffering response to extreme stress and trauma (Cluver, et al., 2009; Dirkzwager, Bramsen, & van der Ploeg, 2003). Runtz and Schallow (1997) found that 55% of the variance in psychological adjustment in sexually or physically abused male and female college students could be attributed to social support. This is consistent with research that has indicated that social support has a significant positive effect on the recovery process after a traumatic event (Davidson et al., 2000). Family support in particular has been the focus of studies, showing a positive influence on psychological well-being after a traumatic event (Cohen & Mannarino, 2000; Grills-Taquechel, Littletonb & Axsomc, 2011). A lack of social support therefore can place a child at risk for developing posttraumatic stress symptoms, especially when the perpetrator is someone known to the child (Jewkes et al., 2005). Studies have shown that this actually happens in most cases, for instance Collings (2005) found that up to 87% of the male victims in their study did know their perpetrator. Tremblay, Hebert and Piche (1999) also found a higher occurrence of internalizing problems (e.g., anxiety, depression, somatic complaints and withdrawal) when children were abused by a related adult than by a more distant perpetrator. This indicates that when the perpetrator is someone the child knows this could harm the child’s perception of social support, which could predict a negative outcome. In conclusion, social support is a meaningful factor when it comes to CSA. 12 Cluver and colleagues (2009) hypothesized a moderation effect of social support on the relationship between trauma exposure and PTSD symptoms. According to Baron and Kenny (1986), a moderator is a variable that specifies if and under what conditions an independent variable predicts a dependent variable. Spaccarelli (1994) created a transactional model that indicates that environmental factors (e.g. family support) moderate relationships between responses of the victim and abuse stressors. In addition, a moderating role of social support following sexual abuse on the psychological maladjustment in children and adults was found in several studies (Hyman et al., 2003; Spaccarelli & Kim, 1995; Tremblay et al., 1999). In studies on the effect of post-trauma therapies other than CEAT, a positive effect of social support on the therapy outcome was found, for example in medical treatments for cancer (Taylor et al., 2007), in exposure therapy for PTSD (Thrasher et al., 2010), in cognitive restructuring therapy for PTSD (Thrasher et al., 2010) and in interpretive and supportive group therapy for complicated grief (Orgodniczuk et al., 2002). This creates the expectation that social support will act as a moderator on the CEAT outcome (reducing symptoms of PTSD). In this study the influence of perceived social support on the CEAT outcome will be measured. To revert to Baron and Kenny (1986), PTSD symptoms post CEAT is the dependent variable and PTSD symptoms prior CEAT is the independent variable in this study. The research questions are ‘Does social support function as a moderator between CEAT (PTSD symptoms prior to CEAT) and PTSD symptoms (post CEAT)?’ (Q2) and ‘Do children who know their perpetrator experience less social support than children who do not know their perpetrator?’ (Q3). Theoretical framework The theoretical lens in this study is predominantly derived from a neuroscience perspective. The neuroscience perspective explains the way the brain functions during and after a traumatic event in support of a creative approach to trauma treatment. Research shows that abused women with PTSD have a limited verbal declarative memory in comparison with women with early abuse without PTSD and non-abused women without PTSD (Bremner, Vermetten, Afzal & Vythilingam, 2004). This could be explained by research that indicates that the hippocampus, a brain area that is involved in the learning process and memory, has a lower activation level and a smaller volume in people who suffer from PTSD (Alastair, 2002; De Bellis, 2001; 13 Glaser, 2000). This is also found in abused women (Bremner, 2001) and abused children (Bremner et al., 2003) with PTSD compared to women and children without PTSD. Bremner and his colleagues (2003) hypothesized that a smaller and lower activated hippocampus, after experiencing abuse, can lead to false and fragmented memories. Although there is a growing body of evidence in favour of the framework, there is quite a lot of research that did not find differences in hippocampus volume or memory recall with abused women with PTSD (Pederson et al., 2004). In addition, research shows growing consensus that traumatic memories are stored in the right hemisphere of the brain and are nonverbal (Glaser, 2000; Klorer, 2005). Harris (2009) hypothesizes that during the traumatic event activity decreases in the left hemisphere of the brain, where the language and declarative memory is located, thereby diminishing verbal processing. This is supported by the findings of decreased activity in Broca’s area, in patients with PTSD (Alastair, 2002). In addition, research using Lycra cap (electro cap), PET scans and fMRI found that people with PTSD have an increased activity in their right hemisphere and a decreased activity in their left hemisphere in comparison to people without PTSD (Lanius et al., 2004; Rauch, van der Kolk, Fisler, & Alpert, 1996; Schiffer, Teicher, & Papanicolaou, 1995). These differences in brain activity in PTSD patients may explain the nonverbal nature of traumatic memories (Klorer, 2005; Lanius et al., 2004). These neuroscientific findings suggest that using a verbal based therapy may not be the best approach for people with PTSD. CEAT avoids verbalization and uses non-linguistic communication and expression (Harris, 2009; Malchiodi, 2003). As explained above, CEAT includes specific drawing exercises and questions to help the children after a trauma express their experiences, so the children can reframe their emotions and negative thoughts without words (Malchiodi, 2003). This can reduce the symptoms of posttraumatic stress, which makes it a suitable therapy for children who have experienced a traumatic event. Goals of this study The purpose of this study can be divided in two different goals. The first goal is to improve psychological wellbeing and support positive recovery of children after a traumatic event, through a creative expressive arts therapy intervention. The second goal is to add knowledge to the current state of literature on child trauma and 14 interventions. This study will explore the implementation of a creative expressive arts therapy intervention programme and its effects. The hypotheses are: 1. Children who will join the CEAT intervention will experience less posttraumatic stress symptoms after the intervention compared to prior to the intervention. 2. Children who will join the CEAT intervention will experience a greater decrease of posttraumatic stress after the intervention compared to the control group (which includes children on the waiting list). 3. Social support functions as a moderator between CEAT and PTSD symptoms, expecting that children who perceive high social support experience a greater decrease of posttraumatic stress after the intervention compared to children who perceive low social support, using the pre measurements of social support. 4. If the perpetrator is someone the child knows, this will predict less perceived social support and, it is therefore expected (see H3), less progression through CEAT. Method Participants The participating children have been selected from files available at the Teddy Bear Clinic for sexually abused children in South Africa, based on demographic and intake information available as part of the normal intake procedure in this clinic. Inclusion criteria were (1) age (8-12 years old), (2) experienced one or multiple events of sexually abuse between three months and two years, (3) show posttraumatic stress symptoms, and (4) English speaking. Exclusion criteria were (1) mental retardation, autistic disorder, and blindness, and (2) already had any form of previous trauma treatment. 31 children from two different branches of the Teddy Bear Clinic, one school in Gauteng and a safe house in Florida, Johannesburg, were approached to participate in this research. 24 children showed up for pre measurements; 11 in the Soweto branch and 13 in the Krugersdorp branch. When therapy started more children dropped out, leaving 4 children in the Soweto branch (4 girls, age; M = 11, SD = 0.82) and 5 in the Krugersdorp branch (2 boys and 3 girls, age; M = 10.60, SD = 1.95)). All the children experienced sexual abuse, and additionally two children indicated that they experienced physical abuse and one child saw his mother die. 15 These children were all participating in CEAT. For the control group, 2 children in both branches were used (1 boy and 3 girls, age: M = 10, SD = 1.63). Instruments PTSD symptoms were measured with the Young Child PTSD Checklist (YCPC) (Scheeringa, 2010), conducted with parents. This checklist consists of 42 items; 12 items indicate the absence (= 0) or presence (= 1) of several traumatic events during a lifetime and 30 items who rate the DSM-IV-TR characterized PTSD symptoms in the last two weeks. The scale uses a 4-point Likert scale, which ranges from ‘not at all’ or ‘hardly ever/none’ (scored 0) to ‘everyday’ (scored 3). Higher scores indicated more severe PTSD symptoms and more functional impairment due to the symptoms. The YCPC checklist was derived empirically from a series of studies over the past 20 years that determined validity of the items with diagnostic interviews (Scheeringa, 2011; Scheeringa, Zeanah & Cohen, 2011). No psychometric data are available yet and so far no studies in the South African context have reported to use this questionnaire. However, a comparable PTSD checklist, the Child PTSD Checklist (C-PTSD-C) (Amaya-Jackson, McCarthy, Chemey, & Newman, 1995), is used extensively in South African context (Cluver, Gardner, & Operario, 2007; Cluver et al., 2008; Fincham, Altes, Stein & Seesat, 2009; Seedat et al., 2004). The C-PTSD-C shows high reliability (α= 0.93) and the internal consistency is acceptable to good (Boyes, Cluver & Gardner, 2012). This comparable PTSD checklist was not available and therefore the YCPC was used in this study. To be able to diagnose the children, the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children- Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) (Kaufman et al., 2000) was used, conducted with parents. The KSADS-PL is a standardised clinician administered diagnostic interview, based on the DSM-IV. The K-SADS-PL consists of 82-items to use as a screening interview, to assess key symptoms for current and past episodes of 25 different diagnostic areas. This research only used the PTSD section. The K-SADS-PL has previously been used in South Africa (Suliman, Kaminer, Seedat & Stein, 2005) and has shown good reliability and validity (Perrin, Smith & Yule, 2000). Social support was measured with the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) (Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet & Farley, 1988). This questionnaire 16 has 12 items which measure perceived social support from family, friends, and a significant other (Zimet et al., 1988). Answers are given on a 7-point Likert-type scale (‘very strongly disagree’ to ‘very strongly agree’). Respondents’ scores can range from 12 to 84 points, the higher the score, the higher the level of perceived social support. The MSPSS has been previously used in South Africa (Bruwer, Emsley, Kidd, Lochner, & Seedat, 2008; Kuo, Fitzgerald, Operario, Casale, 2012; Myint, & Mash 2008) and is a psychometrically complete instrument that can be used in South Africa (Bruwer, et al., 2008). The entire scale has a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.86 (Bruwer, et al., 2008) and has excellent reliability (Zimet, et al.,1988). Procedure Participants were selected based on information in files from the clinic. Files with children matching all inclusion criteria were chosen. Unfortunately not enough children matched the criteria ‘English speaking’ and therefore children with different mother languages were included as well (Zulu, Tswana and English). In addition there was a low turn up of the individually chosen children at one of the two branches (Krugersdorp), therefore a new group of children was selected by social workers at a safe house for children in Florida, Johannesburg. Reasons given by the parents who did not show up were; they did not have money for transport (even though we would provide them the transport costs), they could not get the day off from work and they could not leave their other children unattended. For the control group more children were approached, however, only 2 children showed up. Therefore, 2 children who did not show up for CEAT were persuaded to come one last time to do postmeasurements. Social workers from the Teddy Bear Clinic made a first appointment with the children and their parents or caregivers. During the first appointment the researchers explained what CEAT is, what the goals of the research were and asked for assent/consent from children and parents or caregivers. Informed consent was followed by the administration of questionnaires, this research was part of a larger research project including 6 questionnaires. This research used 2 questionnaires, one for the children (the MSPSS) and one for the parents (the YCPC), and an interview (the K-SADS-PL). All questionnaires were conducted in English, on paper, with no time limit. During the data collection there was a social worker available the whole time to translate if necessary. Children and parents were separated while filling in the questionnaires, avoiding influencing each other. 17 Informal interviews were conducted and observations were made to supplement the quantitative measurements. Social workers gave the CEAT after they had received a two-day CEAT training from Lefika, an organisation for creative art therapy. The CEAT included 10 sessions of 1½ hours, twice a week. Due to logistical challenges, the planning differed from week to week. Each session had its own theme and activities, for a summary of general outlines of the therapy programme, see table 1. After 10 sessions the children and parents came in one last time, to repeat the questionnaires, the informal interviews and the observations. Table 1. The general outlines of the CEAT programme. Week Phase Session theme Activity 1 Activity 2 1 Creating a Introduction Group rules This is me 2 safe space Psycho-education Brave Bart Scribble to music Safe space Safety symbols Mirrored dancing 3 4 Telling the Emotion identification Name the feeling Emotional drama 5 story Trauma telling What happened Bubbles of hope Emotion regulation Mask Powerful/powerless 6 7 Returning to Strength finder Hero’s play My favorite things 8 community Community support Music group Helping hands 9 Meaning making Past/Present/Future Group photo shoot 10 Goodbye Memory box Certificate ceremony Statistical analysis The design of this study was an experimental pretest-posttest control group design, the two groups existed of children on the waiting list (control group) and children who participated in CEAT. For the processing and analysis of the research data, the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS, version 22.0, 2013) was used. First, descriptive statistics were used to summarize the demographic data. Thereafter, pre measurement scores of the YCPC, K-SADS-PL and MSPSS were compared to post measurement scores to test the effect of CEAT, using a paired-sample t-test. From the K-SADS-PL only the diagnose (‘PTSD’ or ‘no PTSD’) was used in the analyses. The 18 assumption of normality was analysed with histograms. The variables were not normally distributed, but the t-test is robust against violations of the assumption of normal distribution, therefore it was decided to continue and use the test. To compare the intervention group and the control group scores of the YCPC and K-SADS-PL were used in an independent sample T-test. In order to test the moderation effect of social support, as measured with the MSPSS, linear regression analyses was executed. Although the assumption of homogeneity was violated and corrections made no significant differences, it was decided to follow through with the analyses, using regression analyses with centralised scores. The model used for the moderator analyses is shown in Figure 1, showing three paths leading to the outcome variable (post CEAT PTSD symptoms measured with the K-SADS-PL and the YCPC). Path a; prior CEAT PTSD symptoms as a predictor. Path b; the impact of social support as a moderator. And Path c; the interaction of these two. Social support functions as a moderator when the interaction (Path c) is significant (Baron & Kenny, 1986). To test if the relation with the perpetrator has an influence on perceived social support, a between-subjects design is used; independent sample t-tests were executed. For all analysis a p < .05 was significant. Figure 1. Model of social support as moderator between prior CEAT PTSD symptoms and post CEAT PTSD symptoms. 19 Results Reliability of questionnaires Both the YCPC as the MSPSS showed good reliability (α = .86 and α = .72) at pre measurements. Additionally a good reliability was found during post measurements for the YCPC and MSPSS (α =.88 and α = .71). Development of posttraumatic stress symptoms of the therapy group The YCPC was completed for all 9 children of the therapy group who received the intervention. Table 2 shows scores at the YCPC prior and after the intervention as well as the results of the analyses that reflect the statistical significance of the changes. As can be seen in table 1 no significant results were found on each scale. During pre and post measurements the highest mean was found at the avoidance and numbing scale of the YCPC (M = 4.89, SD = 3.95 and M = 5.56, SD = 5.55). The effect that can be observed is a slight decrease of the total scale of the YCPC although not significant. Table 2: Comparison of scores per scale at the YCPC prior and after the intervention for the therapy group N Pre Post measurement measurement M SD M SD t P YCPC R 9 3.00 3.50 3.00 3.08 .00 1.000 YCPC AN 9 4.89 3.95 5.56 5.55 -.38 .358 YCPC IA 9 4.11 5.18 3.11 2.57 .56 .297 YCPC T 9 15.56 15.49 14.67 11.99 .21 .420 YCPC FI 9 2.11 2.71 2.44 4.75 -.22 .417 Note. YCPC = Young Child PTSD Checklist: R = Re-experiencing scale, AN = Avoidance and Numbing scale, IA = Increased Arousal scale, T = Total (R+AN+IA), FI = Functional Impairment. 20 According to the DSM-IV criteria in the K-SADS-PL 6 out of 13 children (therapy group N = 3 and control group N = 3) were diagnosed with PTSD during premeasurements and 4 out of 13 children showed a PTSD diagnosis during post measurements (intervention group N=4, and control group N=2). Despite the decrease of PTSD diagnosis it has to be noted that 4 participants (all therapy group) out of 6 diagnosed with PTSD at pre measurements showed no PTSD diagnose during post measurements while 2 participants (all intervention group) displayed a PTSD diagnose only at post measurements. Comparison of the intervention and control group Table 3 displays a comparison of results on the YCPC of the intervention and control group. Scores displayed are difference scores of the post-measurement and premeasurement per scale using an independent sample T-test. A significant result was found for the functional impairment scale of the YCPC (p = 0.042) with a positive mean of M = .33 (SD = 4.58) for the intervention group and a negative mean of M = 4.25 (SD = 1.71) for the control group. Meaning that the therapy group experienced more functional impairment during post measurements and the control group experienced less functional impairment during post measurements. Table 3: Comparison of difference scores (post-measurements minus premeasurement score) between intervention and control group Intervention Control N M SD N M SD T P R9 .00 2.45 4 4.00 9.49 -.83 .232 YCPC AN 9 .67 5.32 4 -.25 1.50 .33 .373 YCPC IA 9 -1.00 5.39 4 2.25 4.99 -1.02 .164 YCPC T 9 -.89 12.70 4 7.50 15.15 -1.04 .160 YCPC FI 9 .33 4.58 4 -4.25 1.71 1.90 .042* YCPC 21 Note. YCPC = Young Child PTSD Checklist: R = Re-experiencing scale, AN = Avoidance and Numbing scale, IA = Increased Arousal scale, T = Total (R+AN+IA), FI = Functional Impairment. * p < 0.05 Qualitative findings Although no significant evidence was found in favour of the effectiveness of the CEAT therapy a few qualitative findings were noticeable. During an informal interview after post measurements, parents of children indicated that symptoms of the children had been decreased, namely aggressive behaviour (e.g. less fighting with other children), sadness (e.g. children were happier), loneliness (e.g. children played more with other children) and nightmares. Social support as a possible moderator Figure 2 displays the results of a moderator analyses, with social support as moderator between prior CEAT PTSD symptoms and post CEAT PTSD symptoms. A significant result was found for the first step, prior CEAT PTSD in relation with post CEAT PTSD symptoms (β = 0.76, p = 0.036). No significant result was found for the second step, social support in relation with post CEAT PTSD symptoms (β = 0.08, p = 0.788). And neither for the third step a significant result was found, the interaction of prior CEAT PTSD symptoms and social support in relation with post CEAT PTSD symptoms ((β = 0.06, p = 0.845). Therefor social support does not function as a moderator between prior CEAT PTSD symptoms and post CEAT PTSD symptoms. 22 Figure 2. Results for social support as a moderator between prior CEAT PTSD symptoms and post CEAT PTSD symptoms. * p < 0.05. The influence of the relation with the perpetrator on social support The analysis of the relation between the child and their perpetrator and the possible influence on social support did not show a significant effect, t (11) = -.50; p = 0.314. Children who know their perpetrator however did show a higher mean score on the MSPSS (M = 69.13; SD = 11.53) than children who do not know their perpetrator (M = 65.20; SD = 17.12). Indicating that children who know their perpetrator experience more social support than children who do not know their perpetrator. Discussion General findings The purpose of this study was to examine the influences of a creative expressive art therapy (CEAT) intervention on posttraumatic stress symptoms among sexually abused South African children aged 8 to 12. PTSD symptoms prior to CEAT were compared to PTSD symptoms after CEAT. Furthermore this research has 23 focused on social support, expecting it to moderate the effect of CEAT and expecting it to be an influence, when the child knows the perpetrator. The results show that there was no significant effect of CEAT on PTSD symptoms. Therefore the first hypothesis, ‘children who will join the CEAT intervention will experience less posttraumatic stress symptoms after the intervention compared to prior to the intervention’, is rejected. In addition there were no significant differences found in PTSD symptoms after the CEAT between the therapy and control group. Therefore also the second hypothesis, ‘children who will join the CEAT intervention will experience a greater decrease of posttraumatic stress symptoms after the intervention compared to the control group’, is rejected. These findings are not consistent with recent literature, indicating that CEAT is an effective therapy for traumatised children (Coholic et al., 2009; DiSunno et al., 2011; Kozlowska & Hanney, 2001; Lev-Wiesel & Liraz, 2007; Robarts, 2006; Rousseau et al., 2007). It should be noted that only Rousseau and colleagues (2007) conducted a quantitative research and they solely found a small effect. The other studies were qualitative and have methodological constrains. Neither are these results support for the positive effect of the non-verbal expression during CEAT, which was expected from a neuroscience point of view (Alastair, 2002; De Bellis, 2001; Glaser, 2000; Harris, 2009; Klorer, 2005; Malchiodi, 2003). However, the qualitative results that were found are worth mentioning: caregivers experienced a decrease of aggressive behaviour, sadness, loneliness and nightmares. During informal conversations with the children after post measurements, the researchers also noted behavioural changes. Children seemed happier and were very enthusiastic about the therapy; they did not want the therapy to stop. Looking at the statistic results and the contradictory observations, several possible explanations will be discussed in the insight and limitation section of this article. Furthermore, results show no moderating role of social support between prior to CEAT PTSD symptoms and post CEAT PTSD symptoms. Therefore the third hypothesis, ‘children who perceive high social support experience a greater decrease of posttraumatic stress after the intervention compared to children who perceive low social support’, is rejected. Last, results show no influence of the relation between the child and his/her perpetrator. And therefore also the last hypothesis, ‘children who know their perpetrator will experience less social support than children who do not know their perpetrator (and if we had adopted the third hypothesis, therefore 24 experience less progression through CEAT)’, can be rejected. The results even show an opposite effect; children who know their perpetrator experienced more social support in comparison with children who did not know their perpetrator. These findings do not support the current body of literature, indicating that social support could function as a moderator between CEAT and PTSD symptoms (Cluver et al., 2008; Davidson et al., 2000; Thrasher et al., 2010), although this was the first study for moderation using CEAT. Other research that found the influence of the childs’ relation with the perpetrator (Jewkes et al., 2005; Tremblay et al., 1999) is also not supported by current research. A possible explanation is that children who are brought to the clinic have a high social support in comparison with children who are not brought to the clinic. That would mean that this research population is not a good sample for this research question. Further explanations will be discussed below. Current research started as a pilot study for the applicability of CEAT for sexually abused children in South Africa. However during the research another subject was found which contributes to the existing body of literature, namely ‘conducting research in South Africa’. Several insights were found in how to conduct or how not to conduct research in the South African context. This insights will be discussed alongside the limitations of this study. Insights & limitations There were several methodological insights and limitations in this study relating to the diverse cultural context in which this research took place. The two main limitations, besides the small research population, concerned the sensitivity of PTSD symptoms in the population under study and the cross-cultural validity of the instruments used. These two limitations and some other challenges that were encountered in this project are further discussed below. Measurement of PTSD symptoms This research aimed to measure PTSD symptoms in South African children. Results show low levels of posttraumatic stress, most children (7) did not show enough symptoms to be diagnosed with PTSD prior to the CEAT, even though they were exposed to horrific events and their files indicated several symptoms. A first possible explanation could be that PTSD is a Western cultural construction that has 25 little relevance in traditional societies (Summerfield, 2004), like most black societies of South Africa. In recent literature, especially as a reaction to the recent published DSM 5, there is a discussion about the cross-cultural validity of the disorder ‘PTSD’ (Friedman, Resick, Bryant, & Brewin, 2011; Hinton, & Lewis-Fernandez, 2011). Some research supports the universality of the disorder (Hinton & Lewis-Fernandez, 2011), other research emphasise the cultural insensitivity of the disorder (Herbert & Sageman, 2004; Renner, Salem & Ottomeyer, 2006; Summerfield, 2004). Hinton and Lewis-Fernandez (2011) indicate that PTSD is a universal construct, though there are variabilities in different areas of the construct, like a ‘relative salience of avoidance/numbing symptoms’. Consistently, current results show that the highest scores were found in the ‘avoidance & numbing scale’ of the YCPC (table 1). The findings of this research could be an indication of the cultural insensitivity of the disorder. Osterman and de Jong (2007) state that there is a need for a model that includes cultural factors when working with PTSD (culturally competent model of traumatic stress); this could be the subject of future research. A second possible explanation of the low levels of posttraumatic stress could be found in the complexity of trauma in most South African victims, as a result of early, ongoing and multiple traumas they experience (Friedman et al., 2011). Symptoms of the multiple traumas could get in the way of typical PTSD symptoms and give the appearance that PTSD symptoms are less prominent (Herman, 2001 in Edwards, 2005). Therefore PTSD symptoms could be more difficult to measure in children who experience ongoing trauma. It could even be questioned if it could be called PTSD, for the trauma is not over yet (post). Additionally, it is possible that children in South Africa ‘don’t know better than that it is normal’ due to their frame of reference and therefore report less symptoms. Most children in this sample experienced multiple traumatic experiences (like physical abuse, being abandoned by their parents, or being neglected), than merely a single incidence of sexual abuse. This emerged from conversations with the children, as they did not show this in the questionnaires. Several studies promote the use of the term ‘Complex Trauma (CT)’ or ‘Developmental PTSD (DP)’ (Cloitre et al., 2009; Roth, Newman, Pelcovitz, Van der Kolk & Mandel, 1997; Van der Kolk, 2005), referring to the experience of multiple, chronic, prolonged, developmentally and early-life onset trauma’s like physical, emotional, and educational neglect or abuse (Van der Kolk, 2005). Children 26 suffering from CT or DP show different symptoms, in addition to the PTSD symptoms, like ‘self-regulatory disturbances’ (Cloitre et al., 2009), ‘Loss of expectancy of protection by others’ and ‘Negative self-attribution’ (Van der Kolk, 2005). This research purely looked at PTSD symptoms instead of CT or DP symptoms, future research should take these symptoms into consideration. A third possible explanation for the low PTSD scores could be socially desirable responses. Social desirability is a personality characteristic which can affect measurements, and is especially likely to happen when dealing with sensitive issues as well as when data is collected in a less anonymous way (Johnson & van de Vijver, 2003; Johnson, Shavitt & Holbrook, 2011). In this study there was a high likelihood for social desirability seeing the sensitivity of the issue at hand and due to resource constraints, data had to be collected in a public space. An additional complication included the presence of a perpetrator during pre-measurements in Krugersdorp. Also, higher social desirability scores were found in Black South Africans compared to White (Edwards & Riordan, 1994), and current research population consisted entirely of Black South Africans. Another possible explanation could be that children did not dare to tell the truth to strangers and therefore answered as what they thought was desired. Social desirability lowers the validity of this study, which should be taken into account while interpreting the results as well as in future studies. Instruments Apart from the challenges measuring PTSD symptoms in this research population, another limitation was reliability of the instruments used in this research. Although numerous studies reported successful application of the selected instruments in the South African community, and it was therefore expected to be applicable in this context, several challenges were experienced. A first limitation jeopardizing the reliability of the instruments was the language barrier between respondent and researcher in this study, and between instrument and respondent. Different social workers and students from South Africa (no trained translators) were used to translate the English instructions and questions of the questionnaires and interview. Since there are multiple languages in South Africa, questionnaires and the interview had to be translated in different languages (Zulu and Tswana), possibly adapting the guidelines and questions slightly. Another difficulty 27 during translation is the use of abstract concepts like feelings (e.g. depression, motives). Van Eeden and Mantsha (2007) found that it is difficult for translators to describe words across languages in the same abstract manner. This is consistent with observations of social workers and students, during testing, that the vocabulary in different South African languages is not as diverse as the English vocabulary. Therefore, different English words became the same in the translated language. Also, control questions used for decreasing social desirability that were formulated slightly different in English became the same when translated in one of the African languages. This makes measuring social desirability difficult in other languages. The translation of questions and guidelines, language capacity and interpretation of words might cause a bias (Peters & Passchier, 2006; Van de Vijver & Leung, 2011; Van Eeden & Mantsha, 2007; Wild, Flisher, Bhana & Lombard, 2005), this should be kept in mind while interpreting the results. A second observation, made during the use of the instruments, is that children chose a lot of extreme answers. This could be explained by research that found that children experience difficulties using Likert-scales (Chambers & Johnston, 2002; Mellor & Moore, 2014). Chambers and Johnston (2002) reported that children, aged between 5 to 12 years, had difficulties filling in questions about abstract concepts, such as their emotional states, using 3-point and 5-point response formats. Consistent research found that answers of children using a 5-point Likert scale were inconsistent compared with dichotomous yes/no responses (Mellor & Moore, 2014). Their research suggested that younger children are more capable using a 5-point Likert scale when the anchors are meaningful to the child. However, even then the percentage of children that were able to use the Likert scale successfully remains low (Mellor & Moore, 2014). For children the easiest is a format that reflects frequency or behaviours/thoughts (i.e., ‘never’ to ‘always’) (Mellor & Moore, 2014).This is supported by the findings of this research; children had less difficulty filling in the Children’s Coping Strategies Checklist-R2 (CCSC-R2), which was conducted for a larger research. The CCSC-R2 is answered with a 5-point Likert scale from ‘never’ to ‘always’, compared to the questionnaires using a 5-point Likert scale from ‘totally agree’ to ‘totally disagree’. Therefore the use of abstract answer formats like ‘totally agree’ to ‘totally disagree’ lowers the reliability of the answers. Nevertheless there is research supporting the use of Likert-scales with children, children chose the Likert28 scale above the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) and the numeric VAS (Laerhoven, van der Zaag-Loonen & Derkx, 2004). During the last round of questionnaires the social workers made drawings of the scales, big for ‘totally agree’ and small for ‘totally disagree’, this worked much better. A possible solution would therefore be to use Likert-scales with meaningful and clear anchors, this should be taken in account in future research. The last observation made by the social workers about the use of the instruments is that children answer the question about recent events instead of the general past, while most questionnaires ask about the general past. This means, for example, that if children were asked about their relationship with their family and there brother or sister gave them a cookie last weekend, they would remember that and say they have a good relationship. This lowers the reliability and validity of the questionnaires. The overall conclusion that can be made on the use of instruments working with traumatized children in South Africa is that researchers should be aware of risks that accompany the use of these instruments. Instruments and their interpretation cannot be seen without the context (Lupele, Mwingi, Kinyanjui, Kimani & Kisamba, 2005). Research strongly advise researchers to include indigenous knowledge in the design and implementation of questionnaires in Africa (Owusu-Ansah & Mji, 2013). A start could be to modify current instruments and adapt them to be cultural equal and cultural sensitive (Friedman, 2011). When the use of a translator is necessary, a possible bias should be taken into account. Not only cultural differences should be noted, also working with children and additional necessary adaptions should be an important focus of future research. This research Apart from the sensitivity of PTSD symptoms in the population under study and the cross-cultural validity of the instruments used, some additional insights were experienced. These challenges have not been reported in previous studies, and are important in order to formulate recommendations for future research. 29 One of the observations that was made, was a difference in attitude between participants in the two branches, in specific between caregivers. In the Soweto branch caregivers seemed willing to put effort in the healing process of their children, where in the Krugersdorp branch caregivers were largely dependent on the clinic in terms of transport arrangements and even to feed their children. Working with the Krugersdorp branch caregivers was therefore more difficult and there was a lower commitment and turn up. It is recommended to carefully consider the social and financial status of participants and its implications when designing a program. Another observation were challenges with punctuality and time management between the researchers and the community involved. It was challenging making appointments with the group, for instance the arrival time could vary from 2 hours too early to 2 hours too late. Therefore it is recommended in future research to use participants close to the clinic or use groups (like the safe house) who only need one person to bring and collect them. Another solution would be to go to a place where all the children are (i.e. at school), this was not possible during this research due to all the public holidays. It was also challenging administering questionnaires with the house parents of the safe house. Housemothers/fathers were not constantly present to conduct all questionnaires at the same time and some housemothers only knew the child for a short while so they could not report on the child before the traumatic event. This lowered the validity of the questionnaires, as a result of a systematic bias of less accurate reporting for children in the safe house (Cluver et al., 2009). In addition a limitation of this study is the use of different caregivers (parents, housemothers, sisters, etc.). They all knew the child on a different level or in a different way, what made the comparison biased. Future research should use the same kind of caregivers for all children in their research. Another incident with the children from the safe house was that after session 3 in the Krugersdorp branch the researchers found out that one of the children in the group, who had a very dominant role, had abused at least 2 other children in the group. This could have happened due to missing information in the files of the children and lacking knowledge of the house mothers. Future research should conduct a separate intake with the children, to make sure there is no missing information. This child was present during the measurements prior to the therapy, influencing other 30 children. In the questionnaires this influence is found, the social desirability was high in children sitting close to this boy. This makes the results of these children understandable; questionnaires show no effect of the therapy, for they did not show symptoms prior to the therapy. This boy was taken out of the therapy and the first session was focused on the effect of his presents and acts, but the influence of his company during the first sessions could not be undone. Not only the children formed a social group, also the researchers became acquaintances with the children and caregivers. This was especially noticed at post measurements, children felt less scared to talk about their problems and feelings. This is probably the reason why some children show more PTSD symptoms after CEAT in comparison with before CEAT. This expectation corresponds with observations of the researchers, which show less symptoms instead of more. Future research should try to make a connection with the children before they conduct the research, so they will have equal measurements. The final limitation is that part of the control group (2) existed of children who were part of pre-measurements but did not show for therapy. This could be explained by a few factors; it could be that they were not motivated enough, or they had less symptoms what made them need therapy less than other children, or they had less social support what made the family not bring them to therapy. The consequence of a control group that is not randomly assigned is a bias in the results. This should be taken into account when interpreting the findings. Conclusion This study did evaluate the effect of CEAT and social support on PTSD symptoms in sexually abused children in South Africa. Although no evidence was found in the questionnaires for improved wellbeing in the children, probably due to the amount of methodological constraints, observations and feedback from those involved in the program were very positive. Referring back to the case study of Dembe, positive changes were seen in is his behavior and feelings. Also, many insights and knowledge was generated that can contribute to the current state of literature and benefit future research in this context to a great extent. Future research could for instance focus on the development of cultural sensitive instruments for the use in South Africa, making 31 use of indigenous knowledge. The most important finding is the serious need of more research in this context. References: Alastair, M. H. (2002). Neuroimaging findings post-traumatic stress disorder: Systematic review. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 102-110. Alisic, E., Jongmans, M. J., van Wesel, F. & Kleber, R. (2011). Building child trauma theory from longitudinal studies: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(5), 736-747. Amaya-Jackson, L., McCarthy, G., Newman, E. & Chemey, M. (1995). Child PTSD Checklist. Unpublished instrument. Durham, NC: Duke University Department of Psychiatry. American Psychological Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV-Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association (2004). Beknopte handleiding bij de diagnostische criteria van DSM-IV-TR. (Nederlandse vertaling: G.A.S. Koster van Groos). Amsterdam: Harcourt. Backos, A. K. & Pagon, B. E. (1999). Finding a Voice: Art Therapy with Female Adolescent Sexual Abuse Survivors, Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 16(3), 126-132. Baron, R. M. & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social, 51(6), 1173-1182. Boyes, M. E., Cluver, L. D. & Gardner, F. (2012). Psychometric properties of the child PTSD Checklist in community sample of South African children and adolescents. Plos one, 7(10), 1-8. Bremner, J. D. (2001). A biological model for delayed recall of childhood abuse. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 4(2), 165-183. Bremner, J. D., Vermetten, E., Afzal, N. & Vythilingam, M. (2004). Deficits in verbal declarative memory function in women with childhood sexual abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192(10), 643-649. 32 Bremner, J. D., Vythilingam, M., Vermetten, E., Southwick, S. M., McGlashan, T., Nazeer, A., Khan, S., Vaccarino, L. V., Soufer, R., Garg, P. K., Ng, C. K., Staib, L. H., Duncan, J. S. & Charney, D. S. (2003). MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(5), 924-932. Bruwer, B., Emsley, R., Kidd, M., Lochner, C. & Seedat, S. (2008). Psychometric propertie of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support in youth. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 49(2), 195–201. Camic, P. M. (2008). Playing in the mud: Health psychology, the arts and creative approaches to health care. Journal of Health Psychology, 13(2), 287-298. Carolan, R. (2001). Models and paradigms of art therapy research. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 18, 190-206. Carr, A. (2006). The handbook of child and adolescent clinical psychology: A contextual approach (Second edition). East Sussex: Routledge. Chambers, C. T. & Johnston, C. (2002). Developmental differences in children’s use of rating scales. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27(1), 27-36. Cloitre, M., Stolbach, B. C., Herman, J. L., Van der Kolk, B., Pynoos, R., Wang, J. & Petkova, E. (2009). A developmental approach to complex PTSD: Childhood and adult cumulative trauma as predicors of symptom complexity. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22(5), 399–408. Cluver, L., Fincham, D. S. & Seedat, S. (2009). Posttraumatic stress in AIDSorphaned children exposed to high levels of trauma: The protective role of perceived social support. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22(2), 106-112. Cluver, L., Gardner, F. & Operario, D. (2007). Psychological distress amongst AIDS orphaned children in urban South Africa. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(8), 755-763. Cluver, L. D., Gardner, F. & Operario, D. (2008). Effects of stigma on the mental health of adolescents orphaned by AIDS. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 410-417. Cohen, J. A. & Mannarino, A. P. (2000). Predictors of treatment outcome in sexually abused children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(7), 983–994. 33 Coholic, D., Lougheed, S. & Cadell, S. (2009). Exploring the helpfulness of artsbased methods with children living in foster care. Traumatology, 15(3), 6471. Collings, S. J. (2005). Brief research report sexual abuse of boys in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa: A hospital-based study. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 17(1), 23–25. Davidson, L. M., Inslicht, S. S. & Baum, A. (2000). Traumatic stress and posttraumatic stress disorder among children and adolescents. In Sameroff, A. J., Lewis, M. & Miller, S. M. (red.). Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 723-737). New York: Springer. De Bellis, M. D. (2001). Developmental traumatology: The psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 539–564. Dirkzwager, A., Bramsen, I. & van der Ploeg, H. (2003). Social support, coping, life events, and posttraumatic stress symptoms among former peacekeepers: A prospective study. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1545–1559. DiSunno, R., Linton, K. & Bowes, E. (2011). World Trade Center tragedy: Concomitant healing in traumatic grief through art therapy with children. Traumatology, 17(3), 47- 52. Douglass, L. (2001). Nobody hears: How assessment using art as well as play therapy can help children disclose past and present sexual abuse. In Murphy, J. (Ed.), Art therapy with young survivors of sexual abuse: Lost for words (pp. 50-65). East Sussex: Brunner-Routledge. Edwards, D. (2004). What is art therapy? In Edwards, D. (Ed.) Art Therapy (pp 117). London, England: Sage. Edwards, D. (2005). Critical perspectives on research on post-traumatic stress disorder and implications for the South African context. Journal of psychology in Africa, 15(2), 117-124. Edwards, D., & Riordan, S. (1994). Learned resourcefulness in black and white South African university students. The Journal of social psychology, 134(5), 665675. Estes, L. S., & Tidwell R. (2002). Sexually abused children’s behaviours: Impact of gender and mother’s experience of intra- and extra-familial sexual abuse. Family Practice, 19(1), 36–44. 34 Fincham, D. S., Altes, L. K., Stein, D. J. & Seedat, S. (2009). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in adolescents: risk factors versus resilience moderation. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50, 193-199. Friedman, M. J., Resick, P. A., Bryant, R. A. & Brewin, C. R. (2011). Considering PTSD for DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety, 9, 750-769. Gilroy, A. (2006). Art therapy, research and evidence-based practice. London: SAGE Publications. Glaser, D. (2000). Child abuse and neglect and the brain: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(1), 99–116. Grills-Taquechel, A. E., Littletonb, H. L. & Axsomc, D. (2011). Social support, world assumptions, and exposure as predictors of anxiety and quality of life following a mass trauma. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(4), 498–506. Hall, D. K., Mathews, F. & Pearce, J. (2002). Sexual behavior problems in sexually abused children: A preliminary typology. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 289– 312. Hanney, L. & Kozlowska, K. (2002). Healing traumatized children: creating illustrated storybooks in family therapy. Family Process, 41(1), 37-65. Harris, D. A. (2009). The paradox of expressing speechless terror: Ritual liminality in the creative arts therapies’ treatment of posttraumatic distress. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 36, 94–104. Herbert, J. D. & Sageman, M. (2004). “First do no harm:” Emerging guidelines for the treatment of post-traumatic reactions. In G.M.Rosen (Ed.), post-traumatic stress disorder: Issues and controversies (pp. 213–232). New York: Wiley Hinton, D. E. & Lewis-Fernández, R. (2011). The cross-cultural validity of posttraumatic stress disorder: Implications for DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 783-801. Hogan, S. (2009). The art therapy continuum: A useful tool for envisaging the diversity of practice in British art therapy. International Journal of Art Therapy, 14(1), 29-37. Hyman, S. M., Gold, S. N. & Cott, M. A. (2003). Forms of social support that moderate PTSD in childhood sexual abuse survivors. Journal of Family Violence, 18(5), 295-300. Jewkes, R. & Abrahams, N. (2002). The epidemiology of rape and sexual coercion 35 in South Africa: An overview. Social Science and Medicine, 55(7), 1231– 1244. Jewkes, R., Dunkle, K., Koss, M. P., Levin, J. B., Nduna, M., Jama, N. & Sikweyiya, Y. (2006). Rape perpetration by young, rural South African men: Prevalence, patterns and risk factors. Social Science & Medicine, 63, 2949–2961. Jewkes, R., Penn-Kekana, L. & Rose-Junius, H. (2005). "If they rape me, I can't blame them": Reflections on gender in the social context of child rape in South Africa and Namibia. Social Science & Medicine, 61(8), 180-1820. Johnson, T. P., Shavitt, S. & Holbrook, A. L. (2011). Survey response styles across cultures. In Matsumoto, D., & Van de Vijver, F. J. (2011). Cross-cultural research methods in psychology (pp. 130-178). New York, NY: Cambridge university press. Johnson, T. P. & Van de Vijver, F. J. (2003). Social desirability in cross-cultural research. In Harkness, J. A., Van de Vijver, F. J., & Mohler, P. P. (2003). Cross-cultural survey methods (pp. 193-202). Hoboken: Wiley Interscience. Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Ryan, N. & Rau, U. (2000). K-SADS-PL. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1208. Klorer, P. G. (2005). Expressive therapy with severely maltreated children: Neuroscience contributions. Art therapy, 22, 213-220. Kozlowska, K. & Hanney, L. (2001). An art therapy group for children traumatized by parental violence and separation. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6(1), 49-78. Kuo, C., Fitzgerald, J., Operario, D. & Casale, M. (2012). Social support disparities for caregivers of AIDS-orphaned children in South Africa. Journal of Community Psychology, 40(6), 631-644. Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Neufeld, R. W., Gati, J. S. & Menon, R. S. (2004). The nature of traumatic memories: A 4-T fMRI functional connectivity analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 36-44. Laerhoven, H., van der Zaag-Loonen, H. J. & Derkx, B. H. F. (2004). A comparison of Likert scale and visual analogue scales as response options in children’s questionnaires. Acta Paediatrica, 93, 830–835. Lev-Wiesel, R. & Liraz, R. (2007). Drawings vs. narratives: Drawing as a tool to 36 encourage verbalization in children whose fathers are drug abusers. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12(1), 65–75 Lupele, J., Mwingi, M., Kinyanjui, F., Kimani, J. & Kisamba, C. (2005). Methodological decisions in context: The dilemmas and challenges in novice African scholars. Southern African Journal of Environmental Education, 22, 46-58. Malchiodi, C. A. (1998). The art therapy sourcebook. Los Angeles: Lowell House. Malchiodi, C. A. (2003). Handbook of art therapy. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Malchiodi, C. A. (2008). Creative interventions with traumatized children. New York, NY: Guilford. Mellor, D. & Moore, K. A. (2014). The use of Likert scales with children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39(3), 369–379. Myint, T. & Mash, B. (2008). Coping strategies and social support after receiving HIV-positive results at a South African district hospital. South African Medical Journal, 98, 276–278. Naumburg, M. (1950/1973). Introduction to art therapy: Studies of the “free” art expression of behavior problem children and adolescents as a means of diagnosis and therapy. New York: Teachers College Press/Chicago: Magnolia Street. Orgodniczuk, J. S., Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., McCallum, M. & Rosie, J. S. (2002). Social support as a predictor of response to group therapy for complicated grief. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 65(4), 346-357. Osterman, J. E. & De Jong, J. T. V. M. (2007). Cultural Issues and Trauma. In Friedman, M. J., Keane, T. M. & Resick, P.A. (2007), editors. Handbook of PTSD: Science and Practice (pp. 425-446). New York: The Guilford Press. Owusu-Ansah, F. E. & Mji, G. (2013). African indigenous knowledge and research, African Journal of Disability, 2(1), 1-5. Pederson, C. L., Maurer, S. H., Kaminski, P. L., Zander, K. A., Peters, C. M., StokesCrowe, L. A. & Osborn, R. E. (2004). Hippocampal volume and memory performance in a community-based sample of women with posttraumatic stress disorder secondary to child abuse. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17(1), 37–40. 37 Pereda, N., Guilera, G., Forns, M. & Gómez-Benito, J. (2009). The prevalence of child sexual abuse in community and student samples: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 328-228. Perrin, S., Smith, P. & Yule, W. (2000). The assessment and treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(3), 277-89. Peters, M. & Passchier, J. (2006). Translating instruments for cross-cultural studies in headache research. Headache: The Journal of Head & Face Pain, 46, 82-91. Piercea, L. & Bozalekb, V. (2004). Child abuse in South Africa: An examination of how child abuse and neglect are defined. Child Abuse and Neglect, 28, 817-832. Pretorius, G. & Pfeifer, N. (2010). Group art therapy with sexually abused girls. South African Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 63-73. Rauch, S. L., van der Kolk, B. A., Fisler, R. E. & Alpert, N. M. (1996). A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(5), 380-387. Renner, W., Salem, I. & Ottomeyer, K. (2006). Cross-cultural validation of measures of traumatic symptoms in groups of asylum seekers from Chechnya, Afghanistan, and West Africa. Social Behavior and Personality, 34(9), 11011114. Robarts, J. (2006). Music therapy with sexually abused children. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 11(2), 249-269. Rousseau, C., Benoit, M., Gauthier, M. F., Lacroix, L., Alain, N., Rojas, M. V., Moran, A. & Bourassa, D. (2007). Classroom drama therapy program for immigrant and refugee adolescents: a pilot study. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12(3), 451-465. Roth, S., Newman, E., Pelcovitz, D., van der Kolk, B. & Mandel, F. S. (1997). Complex PTSD in victims exposed to sexual and physical abuse: Results from the DSM-IV field trial for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(4), 539-555. Runtz, M. G. & Schallow, J. R. (1997). Social support and coping strategies as mediators of adult adjustment following childhood maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21(2), 211-226. 38 Scheeringa, M. S. (2010). Young Child PTSD Checklist. New Orleans, LA: Tulane University. Scheeringa, M. S. (2011). PTSD in children younger than the age of 13: Toward developmentally sensitive assessment and management. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 4(3), 181-197. Scheeringa, M. S., Zeanah, C. H., & Cohen, J. A. (2011). PTSD in children and adolescents: toward an empirically based algorithma. Depression and Anxiety, 28(9), 770-782. Schiffer, F., Teicher, M. & Papanicolaou, A. (1995). Evoked potential evidence for right brain activity during the recall of traumatic memories. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 7, 169-175. Seedat, S., Nyamai, C., Njenga, F., Vythilingum, B. & Stein, D. (2004). Trauma exposure and post-traumatic stress symptoms in urban African schools. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 169–175. Spaccarelli, S. (1994). Stress, appraisal, and coping in child sexual abuse: A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 1-23. Spaccarelli, S. & Kim, S. (1995). Resilience criteria and factors associated with resilience in sexually abused girls. Child Abuse and Negect, 9, 1171–1182. Suliman, S., Kaminer, D., Seedat, S. & Stein, D. J. (2005). Assessing post-traumatic stress disorder in South African adolescents: using the child and adolescent trauma survey (CATS) as a screening tool. Annals of General Psychiatry, 4(2), 1-16. Summerfield, D. A. (2004). Cross-cultural perspectives on the medicalization of human suffering (pp. 233-245). In: Rosen G. M. (2004), editor. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Issues and Controversies. England: Wiley. Taylor, C. L. C., de Moor, C., Basen-Engquist, K., Smith, M. A., Dunn, A. L., Badr, H., Pettaway, C. & Gritz, E. R. (2007). Moderator analyses of participants in the active for life after cancer trial: Implications for physical activity group intervention studies. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33(1), 99-104. Thrasher, S., Power, M., Morant, N., Marks, I. & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Social support moderates outcomes in a randomized controlled trial of exposure therapy and (or) cognitive restructuring for chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 55(3), 187-190. 39 Tremblay, C., Hebert, M. & Piche, C. (1999). Coping strategies and social support as mediators of consequences in child sexual abuse victims. Child Abuse Neglect, 23, 929–945. Van der Kolk, B. A. (2005). Developmental trauma disorder: Toward a rational diagnosis for children with complex trauma histories. Psychiatric Annals, 35, 401–408. Van de Vijver, F. J. & Leung, K. (2011). Equivalence and Bias: A Review of Concepts, Models, and Data Analytic Procedures. In Matsumoto, D., & Van de Vijver, F. J. (2011). Cross-cultural research methods in psychology (pp. 1745). New York, NY: Cambridge university press. Van Eeden, R. & Mantsha, T. R. (2007). Theoretical and methodological considerations in the translation of the 16PF5 into an African language. South African Journal of Psychology, 37, 62-81. Van Westrhenen, N., Fritz, E., Lemont, S. & Oosthuizen, H. (2014). Creative Expressive Arts Therapy Intervention Protocol for Traumatized Children. Unpublished. Waller, D. (2006). Art therapy for children: How it leads to change. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 11, 271-282. Wild, L. G., Flisher, A. J., Bhana, A. & Lombard, C. (2005). Psychometric properties of the Self-Esteem Questionnaire for South African adolescents. South African Journal of Psychology, 35, 195-208. Zimet, G., Dahlem, N., Zimet, S. & Farley, G. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30–41. 40