Download Chronic WIN55,212-2 elicits sustained and conditioned increases in

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Drug interaction wikipedia , lookup

Pharmacognosy wikipedia , lookup

Stimulant wikipedia , lookup

NK1 receptor antagonist wikipedia , lookup

Theralizumab wikipedia , lookup

Cannabinoid receptor antagonist wikipedia , lookup

Neuropharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Psychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Bilastine wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) 114–118
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Behavioural Brain Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr
Research report
Chronic WIN55,212-2 elicits sustained and conditioned increases in intracranial
self-stimulation thresholds in the rat
Maria Mavrikaki a , Eirini Markaki a , George G. Nomikos b , George Panagis a,∗
a
b
Laboratory of Behavioral Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Crete, 74100 Rethymno, Crete, Greece
Takeda Global Research & Development Center, Inc., Deerfield, IL 60015, USA
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 22 December 2009
Received in revised form 14 January 2010
Accepted 18 January 2010
Available online 25 January 2010
Keywords:
Brain stimulation reward
Cannabinoid
Reinforcement
Reward
Conditioned anhedonia
a b s t r a c t
The present study sought to examine whether repeated administration of the CB1 receptor agonist
WIN55,212-2 affected intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) behavior and induced phenomena of tolerance
or sensitization, similar to typical addictive drugs. Rats received intraperitoneal injections of vehicle for
5 days, vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 0.3 or 1 mg/kg) for 20 subsequent days, and vehicle for 5 additional
days. Thresholds for ICSS were measured before and after each injection. The initial five injections of vehicle did not affect ICSS thresholds. WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) significantly increased ICSS thresholds from
the first day of administration, an effect that remained stable across the subsequent days of administration. During the 5 additional days, where WIN55,212-2 was substituted with vehicle, rats demonstrated a
conditioned increase in postinjection thresholds that was significant the first 3 days of this period. These
findings indicate that repeated WIN55,212-2 administration elicited a sustained increase in ICSS, i.e.,
phenomena of tolerance or sensitization were not observed. The present data demonstrate cannabinoidpredictive stimuli that may gain affective salience and play an important role in maintaining cannabinoid
administration.
© 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Dependence-producing substances with a high abuse potential that produce euphoric states in humans are believed to affect
reinforcement and reward mechanisms by actions on certain areas
and pathways of the brain, which are collectively known as brain
reward systems. In most cases, these drugs also yield reinforcing and rewarding effects in experimental animals, as assessed
by relevant animal models, such as self-administration, conditioned place preference and intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS)
[29].
Although marijuana is considered to be one of the oldest
and most widely used and abused drugs, our knowledge and
understanding of the manner it acts in the brain to exert its reinforcing/rewarding effects are far from complete. Animal studies using
several behavioral paradigms and under various experimental conditions have shown that 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol (9 -THC) and
other synthetic cannabinoid agonists can induce both appetitive
and aversive effects (for reviews, see [7,9,10,14,15,21,31,33]).
Intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) has been suggested to be
a valid approach for studying the rewarding/reinforcing proper-
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +30 28310 77546; fax: +30 28310 77548.
E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Panagis).
0166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.024
ties of various drugs of abuse [38]. Indeed, drugs with reinforcing
properties in animals and addictive potential in humans tend to
facilitate the reinforcing effects of brain stimulation in rats, i.e., to
lower thresholds for rewarding brain stimulation (ICSS thresholds),
shifting to the left the function that relates response strength to
stimulation strength. On the other hand, drugs that have a negative
impact on reward and reinforcement of behavior typically increase
ICSS thresholds shifting the function to the right [4,38].
Interestingly, there are contradictory reports on the effects of
cannabinoids on ICSS. Gardner and colleagues found that 9 -THC
decreases thresholds for ICSS primarily in Lewis rats [8,13], whereas
other studies have found no effect or increased thresholds for ICSS
following acute administration of 9 -THC or synthetic cannabinoid
agonists in other rat strains [1,2,12,32,34–36]. These discrepancies,
which are likely due to methodological differences, remain to be
resolved.
A possible explanation for the failure to find a clear reward
enhancing effect of cannabinoids in the ICSS model is that the
first exposure(s) to the cannabinoid may produce severe dysphoric
actions, which may mask the rewarding effects of the drug.
Interestingly, the effects of chronic cannabinoid administration
on ICSS have not been examined. Such a study would allow us to
track the cannabinoid effects over time, and this information might
provide insight into the mechanisms that contribute to the abuse
potential of cannabis preparations.
M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) 114–118
115
Fig. 1. Schematic representation of acquisition, training and experimental procedures for ICSS.
Hence, the purpose of the present study was to investigate
whether repeated administration of the CB1 receptor agonist
WIN55,212-2 affected ICSS behavior in the rat. Since acute
administration of WIN55,212-2 has been shown to increase ICSS
thresholds, our study was designed to assess not only the effects
of daily WIN55,212-2 injections on brain stimulation reward,
but also whether a conditioned stimulus associated with chronic
WIN55,212-2 would affect brain reward function. To this end, we
used a protocol invented by Markou and co-workers that assessed
the effects of repeated cocaine administration on ICSS and potential, associated, conditioned phenomena [11].
2. Methods
this method, plotting the responses of the animals against the various pulse frequencies yields a sigmoidal rate–frequency curve as shown in Fig. 2. Shifts in the lateral
position of the curve provide a selective measure of stimulation-produced reward,
as elegantly demonstrated by Edmonds and Gallistel [6], while vertical shifts provide
information on motor/performance capacity. Furthermore, this method offers quantitative scaling of drug-induced changes in reward [3] that is useful when comparing
the effects of different drugs. In other words, the rate–frequency method appears to
have reward selectivity that is required in psychopharmacological research [17,19].
2.3. Drugs and drug administration
WIN55,212-2 mesylate (Tocris, Westwoods Bus. Park Ellisville, U.S.A.) was dissolved in 5% dimethyl sulphoxide (DMSO) and 5% cremophor in 0.9% NaCl and
injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) at a volume of 3 ml/kg of body weight.
In previous studies, we had found that a single injection of WIN55,212-2 at the
3 mg/kg, but not at the 1 mg/kg, dose raised ICSS thresholds [34]. For this reason, we
2.1. Animals and surgery
Male Sprague-Dawley rats (n = 21) weighting 300–350 g at the time of surgery
were used. Before surgery they were housed in groups of three and maintained
on a 12 h light–12 h dark cycle with free access to food and water. The animals
were anaesthetized with intramuscular (i.m.) injection of ketamine hydrochloride (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg). Atropine sulphate (0.6 mg/kg, i.m.) was
injected to reduce bronchial secretion. The animals were implanted with a monopolar stimulation electrode aimed at the medial forebrain bundle (MFB) at the level of
lateral hypothalamus (2.56 mm posterior to bregma, 1.8 mm lateral from midsaggital suture, and 8.6 below the outer flat skull), according to Paxinos and Watson
[23]. The electrodes were constructed from 0.25 mm stainless-steel wire insulated
with Epoxylite except for the conically shaped tip. The anode was an uninsulated
stainless-steel wire connected to an amphenol pin. Five miniature skull screws, the
electrode and the anode were secured to the skull with acrylic dental cement. Following implantation and for the entire duration of the experiments, the animals
were housed individually.
Experiments were conducted in accordance with “Principles of laboratory animal care” (NIH publication No. 86-23, revised 1985).
2.2. Apparatus and procedures for self-stimulation
After one-week recovery, the rats were tested for self-stimulation in an operant chamber made of transparent Plexiglas (25 cm wide, 25 cm deep and 30 cm
high). Each chamber was equipped with a stainless-steel poke device (lever) 4 cm
wide and protruded 2 cm from the left side at a height of 4 cm from the bottom.
Each bar-press triggered a constant current stimulator (Med Associates, St. Albans,
VT) that delivered a 0.4-s train of rectangular cathodal pulses of constant duration
(0.1 ms) and intensity (250 (A) and variable frequency (15–100 Hz, i.e., 6–40 number of pulses/0.4 s). The pulse frequency, i.e., the number of pulses within a train,
was progressively increased up to 40 per stimulation train until the subject showed
vigorous self-stimulation. During the acquisition phase the animals were trained
to self-stimulate for at least three consecutive days (1 h daily), using stimulation
parameters that maintained near maximal bar-pressing rates. After self-stimulation
has been acquired and stabilized for a given pulse frequency, rats were trained to
self-stimulate using four alternating series of ascending and descending pulse frequencies. The pulse frequency was varied by steps of approximately 0.1 log units.
Each frequency was tested within trials of 60 s in duration, followed by an extinction
period of 30 s. For each trial, there was an initial “priming” phase during which the
animals received three trains of stimulation at the frequency which was available
for the specific trial. A rate–frequency determination session lasted about 45 min.
One rate–frequency curve was established daily, for 10–14 days, depending on the
period when the self-stimulation indices (i.e., curve-shift and threshold measure)
were stable. The stimulation parameters, intracranial self-stimulation sessions and
data collection were controlled by a computer. For a schematic representation of
acquisition and training schedules for ICSS, see Fig. 1.
Since cannabinoids seem to affect performance capacity in a dose-dependent
manner, the use of a reward selective measure, like the curve-shift, was requisite. In
Fig. 2. (A) Changes in self-stimulation thresholds after chronic administration of
vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg, i.p.) during the testing phase (days
1–20). *** p < 0.001 WIN55,212-2 1 mg/kg vs vehicle. (B) Changes in asymptotic rate
of responding after chronic administration of vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or
1 mg/kg, i.p.) during the testing phase (days 1–20).
116
M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) 114–118
conducted a pilot study under the present experimental conditions that includes 5
days of ICSS testing with all the animals receiving vehicle and found that even the
1 mg/kg dose administered on the 6th day raised ICSS thresholds. Adaptation to the
experimental conditions, such as that most likely occurring with animals receiving
vehicle for 5 days in the present setting, has been shown to affect the behavioral responses to the cannabinoid receptor agonists 9 -THC and WIN55,212-2. For
example, Pollissidis et al. [24] showed that low doses of these compounds increased
locomotion only under habituated conditions, whereas high doses decreased locomotion only under non-habituated conditions. To this end, we speculate that the 5
days of vehicle administration, ICSS testing and further habituation to the experimental procedures rendered the animals more susceptible to the anhedonic effects
of WIN55,212-2. Based on the present finding, we continued the study, using the
1 mg/kg dose as the highest dose for the chronic regimen, given that we sought to
investigate if changes in ICSS thresholds occurred in either direction after chronic
administration.
2.4. Experimental procedure
Rats were trained in the ICSS procedure until stable thresholds were achieved.
The criterion for stability was met when the frequency thresholds did not vary by
more than 0.1 log units. Rats were then tested daily for 5 consecutive days in order
to evaluate the baseline threshold. Immediately after the first ICSS session (preinjection) the animals were removed from the ICSS chamber and injected with vehicle.
Rats were returned to the ICSS chamber and thresholds were assessed 10 min later
(postinjection threshold). The mean of the first 4 days of vehicle administration
(pre- and post-injection) was considered as baseline, while the response on the 5th
day was considered as control (B5). After these 5 days, animals were injected with
vehicle [n = 5] or WIN55,212-2 (0.1 [n = 5], 0.3 [n = 5] or 1 mg/kg [n = 6]), returned
to the ICSS chamber and thresholds were assessed 10 min later. During this phase,
pre- and post-injection thresholds were assessed for 20 consecutive days (testing
phase). Thereafter, WIN55,212-2 was substituted with vehicle and thresholds were
assessed for 5 more days (washout phase). For a schematic representation of the
experimental procedures, see Fig. 1.
2.5. Data analysis and statistics
The analysis was performed on two aspects of data obtained from the
rate–frequency curve: the intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) threshold and the
maximum rate of responding or asymptote. [18]. Threshold and asymptote values at B5, testing and washout phases were expressed as percentage of baseline
values. Data were initially analyzed using two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA)
with repeated measures followed whenever appropriate by one-way ANOVAs or
one-way ANOVAs with repeated measures and correlated t-test using Bonferroni’s
adjustment for multiple comparisons.
2.6. Histology
At the end of the experiment, the animals were given a lethal dose of sodium
pentothal. The location of the terminal stimulation site was then marked according
to the following procedure: a direct anodal current of 0.1 mA and 15 s duration was
passed through the electrode tip. The animals were perfused intracardially with
saline 0.9%, which was followed by a 50 cc solution of potassium ferrocyanide (3%),
potassium ferricyanide (3%), and trichloroacetic acid (0.5%) in 10% formalin. The
brains were then removed and stored in 10% formalin for 3 days, and 2 days in a
30% sucrose solution. Finally, the brains were sliced in a cryostat microtome and the
sections containing the electrode tract were mounted on slides and stained with
cresyl violet. Only the rats in which tracks from the electrode were verified to be
located in the MFB were included in this study (n = 21).
3. Results
In the testing phase (Fig. 2), two-way ANOVA with repeated
measures performed on the changes in self-stimulation thresholds showed a statistical significant drug effect [F(3,17) = 14.848,
p < 0.001], but neither significant interaction of time and drug
nor time effect, indicating neither tolerance nor sensitization.
Furthermore, paired-samples t-test indicated statistical significant difference between the groups receiving 1 mg/kg and vehicle
(p < 0.001) across all 20 days of administration. In contrast, 0.1 and
0.3 mg/kg of WIN55,212-2 did not induce statistically significant
effects. To the contrary with the changes in self-stimulation thresholds, two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on the asymptotic
rate of responding data showed no statistically significant drug,
time or interaction of drug and time effects (p > 0.05).
In the washout phase (Fig. 3), two-way ANOVA with repeated
measures on the changes in ICSS thresholds showed a statis-
Fig. 3. (A) Changes in self-stimulation thresholds after administration of vehicle
during the washout phase (days 1–5) in animals that received vehicle or WIN55,2122 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg) during the testing phase. B5: last day of 5 consecutive days
of vehicle administration before the testing phase. * p < 0.05 (B) Changes asymptotic
rate of responding after administration of vehicle during the washout phase (days
1–5) in animals that received vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg) during
the testing phase. B5: last day of 5 consecutive days of vehicle administration before
the testing phase.
tical significant interaction of time and drug [F(15,85) = 1.800,
p < 0.01]. One-way ANOVA indicated no differences between groups
at B5. One-way ANOVAs on days 1–3 indicated significant differences between groups [F1(3, 17) = 5.658, F2(3, 17) = 14.519, F3(3,
17) = 13.637, p < 0.01 for each day]. Paired-samples t-tests indicated
statistical significant differences only between the groups administered with 1 mg/kg and vehicle on days 1–3 (p < 0.05 for each day).
One-way ANOVAs on days 4 and 5 indicated no significant differences. However, two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on the
asymptotic rate of responding showed no statistically significant
drug, time or interaction of drug and time effects (p > 0.05).
Fig. 4 depicts rate–frequency functions from representative animals obtained before and after administration of WIN55,212-2 and
vehicle from the days 1 and 20 of the testing phase and the day 1 of
the washout phase. As indicated in the figure WIN55,212-2 administration produced a parallel curve-shift to the right, indicating a
clear decrease in the reinforcing efficacy of the stimulation.
4. Discussion
ICSS thresholds remained stable and unaltered when measured
before each daily injection of vehicle or WIN55,212-2 over the
M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) 114–118
Fig. 4. Rate–frequency functions taken from representative animals. Each plot represents data from a single animal under pre-drug (filled circles) and drug (open
circles) conditions. Rate–frequency functions were obtained by logarithmically
decreasing the frequency of the stimulation pulses from a value that sustained
maximal lever pressing to one that failed to sustain lever pressing.
30 consecutive days of testing. Furthermore, there was no difference between pre- and post-injection ICSS thresholds during the
first 5 consecutive days of the baseline phase. However, postinjection ICSS thresholds were increased after each WIN55,212-2
injection during the testing phase. This effect was significant for
the highest dose of WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg), and the magnitude of
this increase did not vary significantly across the 20 consecutive
days of drug administration. These findings indicate that the CB1
receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 transiently increased ICSS threshold after each daily injection, whereas ICSS thresholds returned to
preinjection baseline the next day, and there was no evidence of
tolerance or sensitization to this effect over a course of administration.
These findings clearly indicate that chronic WIN55,212-2 did
not facilitate ICSS, but rather increased the ICSS thresholds, a finding that is in agreement with several previous reports utilizing
acute regimens [1,2,12,32,34–36]. Our results differ from those of
previous studies by Gardner’s group [8,13], whereby acute 9 THC decreased ICSS threshold. These contrasting results could be
attributed to procedural differences. For example, Lepore et al. [13]
found the most pronounced action of 9 -THC in Lewis rats, compared to Sprague-Dawley rats, in which the effect was marginal,
whereas in the Fisher strain of rats there was no effect. The same
117
group in their previous studies also used Lewis rats, which in general appear to be more sensitive and vulnerable to the effects of
addictive drugs [8]. Although one can speculate that the reward
facilitating effect of cannabinoids on ICSS may only be obtained
in certain strains of rat, suggesting an important genetic component in this action, other studies have shown that 9 -THC produces
aversion in the conditioned place preference paradigm in the same
strain of rats [22]. Secondly, in the study by Lepore et al. [13], 9 THC could only shift the rate–frequency function to the left by
approximately 0.05 log units. Interestingly, these authors adopt a
very strict criterion of stable responding (i.e., 0.01 log units for three
consecutive days), which increases the likelihood that the observed
effect of 9 -THC in their study may reflect normal baseline variation over days. To the contrary, in our study the criterion of stable
responding was defined within 0.1 log units over three consecutive days, in agreement with most other studies utilizing the ICSS
paradigm (see, e.g., [2]).
Although in the present study we did not assess the role of
CB1 receptors in the inhibitory effects of WIN55,212-2 on ICSS,
we have previously shown that CB1 receptors do indeed mediate
the observed increases in ICSS thresholds after acute WIN55,2122 [34]. It appears worthwhile to examine the role of specific CB1
receptor stimulation in the sustained and conditioned anhedonia
in response to repeated administration of a non-selective cannabinoid receptor agonist, such as the WIN55,212-2, that is observed
here for the first time.
According to our results, vehicle- and WIN55,212-2administered rats did not present any statistical significant
differences in the maximum rate of responding, and chronic
administration of WIN55,212-2 did not affect the asymptotic
rate of responding (see Fig. 2). Thus, the increased postinjection
ICSS thresholds observed after each WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg)
injection during the testing phase seem to be independent of
motor impairment, and the effects of chronic WIN55,212-2 on
ICSS thresholds were not confounded by performance effects. This
is consistent with previous reports on homologous findings with
acute administration of CB1 receptor agonists [34,35].
Pharmacological studies have shown that chronic administration of cannabinoids, such as 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol and
WIN55,212-2, produces cannabinoid receptor desensitization and
down-regulation, as well as tolerance to some of the cannabinoidmediated effects, such as hypoactivity, antinociception and
hypothermia [20,26–28,30,37]. Our behavioral observations are not
congruent with the aforementioned behavioral and biochemical
findings. That is, ICSS thresholds increased after the first injection
of WIN55,212-2 and remained stable across the 20 days of drug
treatment. We speculate that the effects of chronic cannabinoids
on brain reward function may be independent from changes in
sensitivity, number or function of central cannabinoid receptors.
Alternatively, downstream effects elicited by chronic administration of cannabinoids may counteract any ensuing change in
cannabinoid receptors resulting in a sustained decrease in ICSS
reward.
Interestingly, in the washout phase, in which the WIN55,212-2
injection was substituted with a vehicle injection and thresholds
were assessed for 5 more days, the postinjection threshold was
significantly increased compared with preinjection threshold. This
effect was statistically significant for the days 1, 2 and 3 of the
group that received 1 mg/kg WIN55,212-2 during the testing phase.
This finding demonstrates that after being repeatedly paired with
WIN55,212-2 administration, a previously neutral vehicle injection increased ICSS threshold in a manner similar to that induced
by WIN55,212-2 administration. A similar phenomenon in the
washout (withdrawal) phase, although in the opposite direction
considering the testing phase, has been described by Markou and
co-workers after repeated cocaine administration [11].
118
M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) 114–118
The conditioned increase in ICSS thresholds observed in the
present study probably reflects an aversive conditioning phenomenon (i.e., conditioned anhedonia). It has become increasingly
apparent that some drug effects can be elicited by environmental
cues or neutral stimuli that have been repeatedly paired with drug
administration. That is, certain drug effects can be conditioned. The
contribution of such drug-predictive environmental cues or neutral stimuli can be explained by the Pavlovian conditioning model.
Thus, in our study, cannabinoid-associated conditioned stimuli can
inhibit brain reward function, as indicated by the increased ICSS
thresholds. Interestingly, exogenous cannabinoid ligands, such as
9 -tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), have previously shown to exert
conditioned place avoidance responses (see, e.g., [5,16,25]). The
present data demonstrate cannabinoid-predictive stimuli that may
gain affective salience and plausibly act as discriminative stimuli
and affect/drive subsequent cannabinoid administration or relapse
to drug-taking behavior.
In conclusion, repeated administration of CB1 receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 did not increase the reinforcing efficacy of
the stimulation. To the contrary, the highest dose increased
ICSS threshold, indicating a clear anhedonic effect, which did
not undergo tolerance or sensitization but appeared stable with
chronic administration. The increase in ICSS thresholds produced
by cannabinoid-predictive stimuli may play an important role in
maintaining cannabinoid self-administration in a subpopulation of
cannabis-dependent individuals, by acting as discriminative stimuli.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by a grant from the Research Committee (KA 2303) and the Department of Psychology of the University
of Crete. Maria Mavrikaki was supported by a scholarship from
Alexander S. Onassis Public Benefit Foundation.
References
[1] Antoniou K, Galanopoulos A, Vlachou S, Kourouli T, Nahmias V, Thermos K, et al.
Behavioral pharmacological properties of a novel cannabinoid 1 ,1 -dithiolane
delta8-THC analog, AMG-3. Behav Pharmacol 2005;16(5–6):499–510.
[2] Arnold JC, Hunt GE, McGregor IS. Effects of the cannabinoid receptor agonist
CP 55,940 and the cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR 141716 on intracranial
self-stimulation in Lewis rats. Life Sci 2001;70(1):97–108.
[3] Campbell KA, Evans G, Gallistel CR. A microcomputer-based method for physiologically interpretable measurement of the rewarding efficacy of brain
stimulation. Physiol Behav 1985;35(3):395–403.
[4] Carlezon Jr WA, Chartoff EH. Intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) in rodents to
study the neurobiology of motivation. Nat Protoc 2007;2(11):2987–95.
[5] Cheer JF, Kendall DA, Marsden CA. Cannabinoid receptors and reward
in the rat: a conditioned place preference study. Psychopharmacology
2000;151(1):25–30.
[6] Edmonds DE, Gallistel CR. Parametric analysis of brain stimulation reward in
the rat: III. Effect of performance variables on the reward summation function.
J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974;87(5):876–83.
[7] Gardner EL. Addictive potential of cannabinoids: the underlying neurobiology.
Chem Phys Lipids 2002;121(1–2):267–90.
[8] Gardner EL, Paredes W, Smith D, Donner A, Milling C, Cohen D, et al. Facilitation
of brain stimulation reward by delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Psychopharmacology 1988;96(1):142–4.
[9] Gardner EL, Vorel SR. Cannabinoid transmission and reward-related events.
Neurobiol Dis 1998;5(6 Pt B):502–33.
[10] Justinova Z, Goldberg SR, Heishman SJ, Tanda G. Self-administration of cannabinoids by experimental animals and human marijuana smokers. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 2005;81(2):285–99.
[11] Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Conditioned facilitation of brain reward function
after repeated cocaine administration. Behav Neurosci 2003;117(5):1103–7.
[12] Kucharski LT, Williams JE, Kornetsky C. The effects of levonantradol on
rewarding brain stimulation thresholds in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
1983;19(1):149–51.
[13] Lepore M, Liu X, Savage V, Matalon D, Gardner EL. Genetic differences in delta
9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced facilitation of brain stimulation reward as
measured by a rate-frequency curve-shift electrical brain stimulation paradigm
in three different rat strains. Life Sci 1996;58(25):PL365–72.
[14] Maldonado R. Study of cannabinoid dependence in animals. Pharmacol Ther
2002;95(2):153–64.
[15] Maldonado R, Rodriguez de Fonseca F. Cannabinoid addiction: behavioral models and neural correlates. J Neurosci 2002;22(9):3326–31.
[16] Mallet PE, Beninger RJ. Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol, but not the endogenous
cannabinoid receptor ligand anandamide, produces conditioned place avoidance. Life Sci 1998;62(26):2431–9.
[17] Markou A, Koob GF. Construct validity of a self-stimulation threshold
paradigm: effects of reward and performance manipulations. Physiol Behav
1992;51(1):111–9.
[18] Mavrikaki M, Nomikos GG, Panagis G. Effects of mood stabilizers on brain
reward processes in rats: studies using the intracranial self-stimulation
paradigm. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;19(3):205–14.
[19] Miliaressis E, Rompre PP, Laviolette P, Philippe L, Coulombe D. The curve-shift
paradigm in self-stimulation. Physiol Behav 1986;37(1):85–91.
[20] Oviedo A, Glowa J, Herkenham M. Chronic cannabinoid administration alters
cannabinoid receptor binding in rat brain: a quantitative autoradiographic
study. Brain Res 1993;616(1–2):293–302.
[21] Panagis G, Vlachou S, Nomikos GG. Behavioral pharmacology of cannabinoids
with a focus on preclinical models for studying reinforcing and dependenceproducing properties. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 2008;1(3):350–74.
[22] Parker LA, Gillies T. THC-induced place and taste aversions in Lewis and
Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav Neurosci 1995;109(1):71–8.
[23] Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. sixth ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2007.
[24] Polissidis A, Chouliara O, Galanopoulos A, Rentesi G, Dosi M, Hyphantis T,
et al. Individual differences in the effects of cannabinoids on motor activity,
dopaminergic activity and DARPP-32 phosphorylation in distinct regions of
the brain. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;27(November):1–17.
[25] Quinn HR, Matsumoto I, Callaghan PD, Long LE, Arnold JC, Gunasekaran N,
et al. Adolescent rats find repeated Delta(9)-THC less aversive than adult
rats but display greater residual cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal protein expression following exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology
2008;33(5):1113–26.
[26] Rodriguez de Fonseca F, Gorriti MA, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ, Palomo T, Ramos
JA. Downregulation of rat brain cannabinoid binding sites after chronic
delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol treatment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
1994;47(1):33–40.
[27] Romero J, Berrendero F, Garcia-Gil L, Ramos JA, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ. Cannabinoid receptor and WIN-55,212-2-stimulated [35S]GTP gamma S binding and
cannabinoid receptor mRNA levels in the basal ganglia and the cerebellum
of adult male rats chronically exposed to delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol. J Mol
Neurosci 1998;11(2):109–19.
[28] Romero J, Garcia-Palomero E, Castro JG, Garcia-Gil L, Ramos JA, Fernandez-Ruiz
JJ. Effects of chronic exposure to delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on cannabinoid receptor binding and mRNA levels in several rat brain regions. Brain Res
1997;46(1–2):100–8.
[29] Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R. Behavioural assessment of drug reinforcement
and addictive features in rodents: an overview. Addict Biol 2006;11(1):2–38.
[30] Sim-Selley LJ, Martin BR. Effect of chronic administration of R-(+)[2,3-Dihydro-5-methyl-3-[(morpholinyl)methyl]pyrrolo[1,2,3-de]-1,4-b
enzoxazinyl]-(1-naphthalenyl)methanone mesylate (WIN55,212-2) or
delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol on cannabinoid receptor adaptation in mice. J
Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002;303(1):36–44.
[31] Solinas M, Goldberg SR, Piomelli D. The endocannabinoid system in brain
reward processes. Br J Pharmacol 2008;154(2):369–83.
[32] Stark P, Dews PB, Cannabinoids I. Behavioral effects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther
1980;214(1):124–30.
[33] Tanda G, Goldberg SR. Cannabinoids: reward, dependence, and underlying
neurochemical mechanisms—a review of recent preclinical data. Psychopharmacology 2003;169(2):115–34.
[34] Vlachou S, Nomikos GG, Panagis G. CB1 cannabinoid receptor agonists
increase intracranial self-stimulation thresholds in the rat. Psychopharmacology 2005;179(2):498–508.
[35] Vlachou S, Nomikos GG, Panagis G. WIN 55,212-2 decreases the reinforcing
actions of cocaine through CB1 cannabinoid receptor stimulation. Behav Brain
Res 2003;141(2):215–22.
[36] Vlachou S, Nomikos GG, Stephens DN, Panagis G. Lack of evidence for appetitive effects of Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in the intracranial self-stimulation
and conditioned place preference procedures in rodents. Behav Pharmacol
2007;18(4):311–9.
[37] Whitlow CT, Freedland CS, Porrino LJ. Functional consequences of the repeated
administration of Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol in the rat. Drug Alcohol Depend
2003;71(2):169–77.
[38] Wise RA. Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward. Annu Rev Neurosci
1996;19:319–40.