* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Climatic Change
Climate change in Tuvalu wikipedia , lookup
Attribution of recent climate change wikipedia , lookup
Scientific opinion on climate change wikipedia , lookup
Media coverage of global warming wikipedia , lookup
Climate change in the United States wikipedia , lookup
Climate change and poverty wikipedia , lookup
Effects of global warming on human health wikipedia , lookup
Public opinion on global warming wikipedia , lookup
IPCC Fourth Assessment Report wikipedia , lookup
Surveys of scientists' views on climate change wikipedia , lookup
Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 DOI 10.1007/s10584-014-1253-6 LETTER Mapping the shadow of experience of extreme weather events Peter D. Howe & Hilary Boudet & Anthony Leiserowitz & Edward W. Maibach Received: 30 June 2014 / Accepted: 10 September 2014 / Published online: 13 October 2014 # Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014 Abstract Climate change will increase the frequency and/or intensity of certain extreme weather events, and perceived experience with extreme weather may influence climate change beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors. However, the aspects of extreme events that influence whether or not people perceive that they have personally experienced them remain unclear. We investigate (1) the correspondence of reported experience of extreme weather events with documented events, and (2) how characteristics of different extreme events shape the geographic area within which people are likely to report they have experienced it—the event’s perceived “shadow of experience.” We overlay geocoded survey responses indicating personal experience with hurricanes, tornadoes, and drought—from a 2012 nationally representative survey (N=1,008) of U.S. residents—on maps of recorded event impacts. We find that reported experiences correspond well with recorded event impacts, particularly for hurricanes and tornadoes. Reported experiences were related to event type, proximity, magnitude and duration. The results suggest locations where disaster preparedness efforts and climate change education campaigns could be most effective after an extreme weather event. 1 Introduction Extreme weather events—events that are rare for a particular place and time of year—can serve as teachable moments about climate change (Marx et al. 2007; Spence et al. 2011; Myers P. D. Howe (*) Department of Environment and Society, Quinney College of Natural Resources, Utah State University, 5215 Old Main Hill, Logan 84322 UT, USA e-mail: [email protected] H. Boudet Department of Sociology, School of Public Policy, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA e-mail: [email protected] A. Leiserowitz School of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA e-mail: [email protected] E. W. Maibach Center for Climate Change Communication, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, USA e-mail: [email protected] 382 Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 et al. 2013; Rudman et al. 2013). But what types and scales of events are the best teachers? And how near must a person be relative to the event for it to create a teaching opportunity? As climate change increases the frequency and/or intensity of certain extreme weather events (Coumou and Rahmstorf 2012; Peterson et al. 2012), these are important questions not only for climate educators trying to understand where to devote limited time and resources, but also for risk managers hoping to encourage disaster preparedness. Personal experience plays an important role in individual risk perceptions of future events as well as preparedness and mitigation behaviors, although the effect of experience is likely to be contingent on situation-specific aspects of the event (e.g., type, magnitude, distance), individual factors (e.g., knowledge, values, attitudes, emotions) and social context (e.g., media reports, peer influences, culture) (McGee et al. 2009; Zaalberg et al. 2009; Wachinger et al. 2013; Silver and Andrey 2014). Moreover, the effect of experience on perceptions and attitudes can create “windows of opportunity” for policy change (Tierney 2007; McGee et al. 2009). These findings from natural hazards research are similar to findings about the influence of weather experience on beliefs and actions related to climate change. Perceptions of changes in local weather (e.g., rising temperatures, flood experience) play an important role in beliefs, attitudes and behaviors related to climate change (Spence et al. 2011; Hamilton and Stampone 2013; Akerlof et al. 2013; Zaval et al. 2014) and vice versa (Goebbert et al. 2012; Howe and Leiserowitz 2013). Using direct measures of weather changes, studies have linked local weather and climate fluctuations to beliefs and attitudes, particularly for those with less entrenched beliefs (Egan and Mullin 2012; Goebbert et al. 2012; Howe et al. 2013; Howe and Leiserowitz 2013; Hamilton and Stampone 2013; Akerlof et al. 2013; Zaval et al. 2014). Recent research, however, has focused on variations in temperature and precipitation as opposed to extreme weather events, yet extreme weather events, particularly when they result in fatalities, may have a larger and longer term impact on climate change attitudes (Brody et al. 2008). Indeed, recent research has shown that personal experience with Hurricanes Irene and Sandy increased both pro-environmental beliefs and support for politicians who support climate change mitigation (Rudman et al. 2013). We analyze an important aspect of the relationship between exposure to extreme weather and people’s beliefs that they have personally experienced climate change: the characteristics of extreme weather events that influence how members of the public perceive that they have personally experienced extreme weather. To date, there has been limited research on how different extreme event types, magnitudes, durations or proximity influence public perceptions of extreme weather experience. Our goal is to investigate the geographic correspondence of subjective personal experience of extreme weather events with the recorded locations of events. In so doing, we will examine the effect of proximity on personal experience to characterize the events’ “shadows of experience.” While the concept of a risk perception shadow was originally created to map risk perceptions of technological hazards associated with siting hazardous facilities, we believe it useful in the context of extreme weather risk. Adapting a definition offered by Stone (2001), we define a shadow of experience as the geographic area within which a population perceives itself to have experienced an extreme weather event. This area may extend beyond the location where the event actually occurred, for example via indirect experience from friends or relatives, or social and environmental cues. Based on previous research (Stoffle et al. 1991) that has outlined the factors that may be important in mapping risk perception shadows, we analyze the type of event, proximity or directness of impacts, significance or magnitude of impacts, and duration of impacts on people’s tendency to report personal experience with the extreme weather event. Moreover, this analysis represents a novel application of spatial Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 383 relative risk mapping—a technique adapted from spatial epidemiology—and statistical models to characterize the relationship between extreme weather events and individual perceptions. 2 Methods Data on extreme weather perceptions were obtained from a national survey of U.S. adults (ages 18 or older) conducted in April 2012 using an online panel recruited by Knowledge Networks (N=1,008; completion rate=54.1 %; cumulative response rate=5.2 %). Response rate metrics for online panel surveys are still under development, and do not compare directly to telephone or mail surveys. A cumulative response rate may be calculated as the product of the panel’s recruitment rate, profile rate (the proportion of respondents who completed the initial profile survey to become panel members) and completion rate (Callegaro and DiSogra 2008). While the cumulative response rate appears low compared to telephone surveys, studies show that the accuracy of probability-based Internet surveys meets or exceeds that of telephone interviews, with the optimal combination of both sample composition and response accuracy (Chang and Krosnick 2009). Knowledge Networks recruits participants using random digit dialing and provides small incentives as well as a free netbook and Internet service to those without computers to ensure they are represented in the panel. Survey responses were based on a series of yes/no questions which asked the respondent: “In the past year have you personally experienced each of the extreme weather events or natural disasters listed below?” and included eleven types of extreme weather events (extreme snow storm, extreme cold temperature, extreme high winds, extreme rain storm, extreme heat wave, flood, wildfire, hurricane, drought, tornado, other unusual weather). Only those who provided an answer are included in the analysis. We focused our analysis on perceptions of tornadoes, hurricanes and droughts as examples of hazards with varying spatial extents, magnitudes, and rates of onset. We mapped the spatial pattern of perceived experience of each hazard by creating a relative risk surface for each survey item, depicting the probability density of each response relative to other responses and accounting for spatial inhomogeneity in the underlying population. Kernel density estimation was performed in R using the spatstat (Baddeley and Turner 2005) and sparr (Davies et al. 2011) packages, which use an isotropic kernel smoother with bandwidths selected by cross-validation. Positive concentrations of responses were identified after calculating asymptotic p-value surfaces, with positive concentrations at the 95 and 99 % confidence level represented as contours superimposed on the relative risk surface. We conducted a spatial nearest neighbor analysis to characterize the relationship between proximity to recorded extreme events and the probability that respondents would report personal experience with the event. We calculated the great-circle distance between each respondent and: (1) the nearest recorded tornado track of each category on the Enhanced Fujita scale between January 2011 and March 2012 and (2) the nearest record of property damage, injuries, or fatalities due to a hurricane between January 2011 and March 2012. Tornado tracks were obtained from the NOAA Storm Prediction Center (SPC 2013) and hurricane records were obtained from the National Climatic Data Center Storm Events Database (NCDC 2013). Records were included if they were categorized as a hurricane/ tropical storm event, or if they were categorized as a coastal flood, storm surge, or flood event related to a hurricane/tropical storm. We included events beginning in January 2011 because our survey question asked about events “in the past year,” which may have been interpreted as referring to events either within the last 12-month period or within the previous calendar year. The Storm Events Database georeferences hydro-meteorological events to the point, county, or weather forecast zone. In the case of events referenced only to a county or weather forecast 384 Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 zone, the events were assigned to the centroid of the county or zone. This method generates some locational uncertainty at the lower bound of the nearest-neighbor analysis. We counted the number of respondents within successive 1-km distance bands from the nearest event and calculated the proportion of respondents who reported experiencing a particular type of event within each distance band. Using a chi-square test we then tested the difference between the proportion of respondents reporting personal experience within each distance band and the national baseline proportion of respondents reporting personal experience with each type of event. To examine the relationship between perceptions and experiences of drought, we calculated the duration of severe drought (Category 2 or higher) at the county level during the 15 months prior to the survey. We then extracted these county-level data based on respondent locations for use in logistic regression analysis. 3 Results In a nationally representative survey conducted in April 2012, a large majority of Americans (82 %) said they had personally experienced one or more types of extreme weather or natural disasters in the past year (Leiserowitz et al. 2012). Here we focus on three hazards that differ in spatial scale, rate of onset, magnitude, and relationship to climate change: tropical cyclones/ hurricanes, tornadoes, and drought. Hurricanes are moderate-onset events (with a typical advance warning of days) that affect large areas and are projected to increase in severity as the climate changes, but possibly decrease in frequency (IPCC 2012). Tornadoes are fast-onset events (with a typical advance warning of minutes) that affect relatively small areas, but it remains unclear whether climate change will affect the frequency or severity of tornadoes (IPCC 2012). Droughts are slow-developing events, but affect large areas, are often longlasting, and are projected to increase in frequency and severity in the Western U.S. as a result of climate change (IPCC 2012). In April 2012, 16 % of Americans reported experiencing a hurricane, 21 % reported experiencing a tornado, and 34 % reported experiencing a drought in the prior year. The spatial distribution of recorded tropical cyclone activity, tornado tracks, and drought corresponded with the distribution of people’s reports of personal experience with each event (see Fig. 1). The analysis of hurricane experience was limited to one event, since Irene was the only hurricane to affect the contiguous U.S. during the study period, making landfall in North Carolina in August 2011 as a Category 1 hurricane and later making a second landfall in New Jersey as a tropical storm. Mapping the relative distribution of reported hurricane experience showed that respondents from eastern North Carolina through northern New England were significantly more likely than respondents elsewhere in the country to report experiencing a hurricane, with a rate ranging from 20 to 35 % (as compared to less than 5 % in the West). This region corresponds to the area affected by Hurricane Irene. Although rainfall from Tropical Storm Lee caused flooding in the Southeast through Mid-Atlantic regions in 2011, respondents in those areas did not show an elevated probability of reporting hurricane experience—possibly because the storm was never classified as a hurricane. The study period was an abnormally active tornado year in the U.S., with seven major tornado outbreaks, 1,691 confirmed tornadoes, 551 deaths, and over one billion dollars in damages (NCDC 2012). In contrast to the analysis of hurricane experience, the analysis of tornadoes includes many events with different characteristics. Mapping the relative distribution of reported tornado experience showed that respondents in the southern Midwest and 385 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 p < 0.05 Track of Hurricane (Tropical Storm) Irene Tornado-related disaster declarations p < 0.01 Yes No Presidential disaster declaration Personally experienced tornado (n=187) 1 0.6 0.4 0.2 p < 0.01 Personally experienced drought (n=316) Severe drought duration 0 0.8 p < 0.05 Tornado track 0 65 1 52 0.8 39 0.6 Weeks 26 0.4 13 0.2 1 0 p < 0.05 p < 0.01 Pr(hurricane experience) No Presidential disaster declaration Yes 1 Pr(tornado experience) Personally experienced hurricane (n=128) Hurricane-related disaster declarations Pr(drought experience) Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 0 Fig. 1 Spatial distribution of recorded extreme weather events (left) and reported personal experience (right) Southeast were significantly more likely than the national average to report that they had experienced a tornado, ranging from 25 to 35 % (as compared to less than 5 % in the Pacific Northwest). The region of reported experience of tornadoes corresponds with the region that suffered the highest rate of deaths and property damage from tornadoes in 2011. Drought also affected large areas of the U.S. in 2011 through spring 2012, with 56 % of the U.S. in abnormally dry conditions and 19 % of the U.S. in severe drought as of March 27, 2012. Drought was concentrated in the Southwest, Southern Plains, and Southeast. These regions corresponded with areas where respondents were significantly more likely than the national average to report that they had experienced a drought. For example, 75 % of respondents in southern Texas reported that they had experienced a drought, compared to less than 20 % of respondents in the Northeast. We compared the distances among respondents and the nearest recorded impacts of the different types of extreme weather events to identify the spatial extent of the shadow of experience—the area in which people are more likely to report experiencing an event. Figure 2a shows the probability that a respondent reported experiencing a hurricane versus their distance from locations affected by Hurricane/Tropical Storm Irene, as compared to the 386 Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 Probability of reported personal experience by event type, distance to nearest event, & magnitude Local probability (95% CI) b 1.0 0.8 0.4 0.2 0.0 0 800 50 100 150 200 250 0 Nearest tornado track, EF0-EF5 (km) e f Tornado (EF3-5) Pr (tornado experience) 0.6 0.4 Pr (tornado experience) 100 150 200 Nearest tornado track, EF2-EF5 (km) 250 200 250 Tornado (EF4-5) 0.2 0.2 0.0 0.0 50 150 0.8 0.8 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 0 100 Nearest tornado track, EF1-EF5 (km) 1.0 Tornado (EF2-5) 50 1.0 600 0.6 400 Tornado (EF1-5) 0.6 Pr (tornado experience) 0.4 0.6 Pr (tornado experience) 0.2 0.0 200 Nearest tropical cyclone record (km) Pr (tornado experience) c 0.8 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 Pr (hurricane experience) 0.2 0.0 0 d Baseline probability Tornado (EF0-5) 1.0 Hurricane 0.4 a 0 50 100 150 200 Nearest tornado track, EF3-EF5 (km) 250 0 50 100 150 200 250 Nearest tornado track, EF4-EF5 (km) Fig. 2 Probability of reported personal experience by distance to nearest event record, as compared to national baseline, for hurricanes (a) and tornadoes of magnitude EF0-5 (b), EF 1–5 (c), EF 2–5 (d), EF 3–5, and EF 4–5 (f). Note the difference in the x-axis between (a) and (b–e). Shaded areas represent 95 % confidence intervals of the proportion of respondents who reported personal within each distance band unweighted national baseline. The shadow of experience for Irene extended beyond the area directly affected by the storm: respondents within 820 km of locations with recorded impacts were significantly more likely (up to 3.3 times greater than the national baseline) to report that they had experienced a hurricane (p<0.05). However, even within areas directly affected by Irene, no more than 45 % of respondents reported that they had experienced a hurricane, possibly due to the fact that it weakened from a hurricane to a tropical storm just prior to its second landfall. Figure 2b–f depict the probability that a respondent reported experiencing a tornado as a function of their distance from the nearest tornado track for varying sets of tornadoes of increasing magnitude, as compared to the unweighted national baseline. For tornadoes, the shadow of experience grows with the magnitude of the event. For example, respondents within 71 km of an EF 0–5 tornado track were significantly more likely to report (p<0.05) that they had experienced a tornado, whereas the distance band increased to 121 km for an EF1-5 tornado track, 207 km for an EF 2–5 tornado track, 504 km for an EF 3–5 tornado track, and 1,150 km for an EF 4–5 tornado. To examine the effects of drought duration on perceived experience, we fit a logit model predicting perceived drought experience as a function of the duration of severe drought at the county level during the 15 months prior to the survey. As shown in Fig. 3, the probability of reported experience increases with the duration of exposure to drought (β=0.05, std. error= 0.01, p<0.001). On average, each week of severe drought increases the chance of perceived drought experience by about 1 %. However, only in areas exposed to at least 25 weeks of severe drought conditions were the majority of people (>50 %) likely to report that they had experienced a drought. Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 387 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 Pr (drought experience) 1.0 Perceived experience with drought 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 Number of weeks in drought category 2 or higher Fig. 3 Probability of reported personal experience with drought by duration of exposure to drought. Shaded area indicates 95 % confidence interval 4 Discussion and conclusions Our results suggest that the public tends to accurately recall and report experiences with extreme weather, particularly discrete events like hurricanes and tornadoes that tend to have large impacts and attract media coverage. The distributions of perceived experiences reported by survey respondents follow the same spatial patterns as the records of actual events. As one might expect, people located closer to recorded events are more likely to report experiencing them. This proximity effect may be explained by an increased likelihood of personally suffering harm or property damage as one approaches the site of the event, as well as environmental cues (e.g. dark clouds or high winds) and social cues (e.g. tornado sirens or warnings) that are more likely to be present closer to the event. We find that the shadow of experience—in terms of the distance within which people are more likely to report that they have experienced extreme events—increases as the magnitude of an event increases, as shown by our analysis of experience with tornadoes of different magnitudes. As disaster events increase in magnitude, they become more likely to cause not only direct damages and loss of life, but indirect damages through disruption of services, utilities, businesses, social networks, and local economies. These indirect effects are one likely cause for the tendency of people to report personally experiencing events even if they live many kilometers away and did not suffer direct personal damages. Our analysis of personal experiences with drought presents a contrast to the moderate- to fast-onset hazards of tornadoes and hurricanes. Drought is particularly challenging for individuals to perceive because it is a slow-onset hazard with a long time horizon, broad spatial extent and diverse set of conceptual and operational definitions. Indeed, only after 25 weeks of severe drought do a majority of people consider themselves to have personally experienced the drought. Our findings are consistent with previous research showing that perceptions are consistent with observations of phenomena such as seasonal temperature anomalies, average temperature change, and seasonal rainfall patterns and timing (e.g. Marin 2010; Hartter et al. 2012; Howe et al. 2013; Howe and Leiserowitz 2013). The correlation of proximity with personal experience is consistent with a study of perceptions of local extreme temperatures (Ruddell et al. 388 Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 2012), which found that perceptions were related to local rather than distant environmental factors. These findings have important implications for disaster preparedness efforts and efforts to educate people about climate change in the context of extreme weather events. By mapping the shadows of experience of extreme weather, we highlight where disaster preparedness and climate education efforts could be the most effective after an event, since people who did not previously accept the reality of climate change sometimes change their minds when they are personally affected by extreme events (Rudman et al. 2013). TV weathercasters, for example, can provide important climate change context for extreme weather events; many are interested in doing so (Maibach et al. 2011), and when they do, their viewers learn important climate information (Zhao et al. 2014). For slow-onset events like drought, communication efforts may need to focus on encouraging public recognition of the phenomenon as well as preparedness behaviors. We note that our data are limited to retrospective assessments of personal experiences over one year for several extreme weather events, and our findings depict only a rough map of the shadow of experience for these extreme weather events (Stone 2001). We used a simplified yes/no measure of personal experience, and future research should address the different ways in which people may experience extreme events. For example, the effect of proximity on personal experience may be due to a combination of different types of experience such environmental cues, social cues, indirect experience via social networks and media coverage, affective responses, and direct impacts. Future research should also investigate the contours of these shadows of experience in more detail, analyze their durability over time, explore how they aggregate across multiple events, and examine different types of hazards with different spatial and temporal characteristics to better understand the mechanisms that convert any one event into a memorable experience that shapes subsequent perceptions and behavior. References Akerlof K, Maibach EW, Fitzgerald D et al (2013) Do people “personally experience” global warming, and if so how, and does it matter? Glob Environ Chang 21:81–91. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2012.07.006 Baddeley A, Turner R (2005) Spatstat: an R package for analyzing spatial point patterns. J Stat Softw 12:1–42 Brody SD, Zahran S, Vedlitz A, Grover H (2008) Examining the relationship between physical vulnerability and public perceptions of global climate change in the United States. Environ Behav 40:72–95 Callegaro M, DiSogra C (2008) Computing response metrics for online panels. Public Opin Q 72:1008–1032. doi:10.1093/poq/nfn065 Chang L, Krosnick JA (2009) National surveys via RDD telephone interviewing versus the internet: comparing sample representativeness and response quality. Public Opin Q 73:641–678. doi:10.1093/poq/nfp075 Coumou D, Rahmstorf S (2012) A decade of weather extremes. Nat Clim Chang 2:491–496. doi:10.1038/ nclimate1452 Davies TM, Hazelton ML, Marshall JC (2011) Sparr: analyzing spatial relative risk using fixed and adaptive kernel density estimation in R. J Stat Softw 39:1–14 Egan PJ, Mullin M (2012) Turning personal experience into political attitudes: the effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. J Polit 74:796–809. doi:10.1017/S0022381612000448 Goebbert K, Jenkins-Smith HC, Klockow K et al (2012) Weather, climate and worldviews: the sources and consequences of public perceptions of changes in local weather patterns. Weather Clim Soc 4:132–144. doi: 10.1175/WCAS-D-11-00044.1 Hamilton LC, Stampone MD (2013) Blowin’ in the wind: short-term weather and belief in anthropogenic climate change. Weather Clim Soc 5:112–119. doi:10.1175/WCAS-D-12-00048.1 Hartter J, Stampone MD, Ryan SJ et al (2012) Patterns and perceptions of climate change in a biodiversity conservation hotspot. PLoS ONE 7:e32408. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032408 Howe PD, Leiserowitz A (2013) Who remembers a hot summer or a cold winter? The asymmetric effect of beliefs about global warming on perceptions of local climate conditions in the U.S. Glob Environ Chang 23: 1488–1500. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.09.014 Climatic Change (2014) 127:381–389 389 Howe PD, Markowitz EM, Lee TM et al (2013) Global perceptions of local temperature change. Nat Clim Chang 3:352–356. doi:10.1038/nclimate1768 IPCC (2012) Managing the risks of extreme events and disasters to advance climate change adaptation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge Leiserowitz A, Maibach E, Roser-Renouf C, Hmielowski JD (2012) Extreme weather, climate & preparedness in the American mind. Yale University and George Mason University, Yale Project on Climate Change Communication, New Haven, CT Maibach EW, Cobb S, Leiserowitz A et al (2011) A national survey of television meteorologists about climate change: education. George Mason University Center for Climate Change Communication, Fairfax Marin A (2010) Riders under storms: contributions of nomadic herders’ observations to analysing climate change in Mongolia. Glob Environ Chang 20:162–176. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2009.10.004 Marx SM, Weber EU, Orlove BS et al (2007) Communication and mental processes: experiential and analytic processing of uncertain climate information. Glob Environ Chang 17:47–58 McGee TK, McFarlane BL, Varghese J (2009) An examination of the influence of hazard experience on wildfire risk perceptions and adoption of mitigation measures. Soc Nat Resour 22:308. doi:10.1080/ 08941920801910765 Myers TA, Maibach EW, Roser-Renouf C et al (2013) The relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global warming. Nat Clim Chang 3:343–347. doi:10.1038/nclimate1754 NCDC (2013) Storm Events Database | National Climatic Data Center. In: Natl Ocean Atmospheric Adm. http:// www.ncdc.noaa.gov/stormevents/. Accessed 17 Sept 2013 NCDC (2012) State of the climate: annual 2011. National Climatic Data Center, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration Peterson TC, Stott PA, Herring S (2012) Explaining extreme events of 2011 from a climate perspective. Bull Am Meteorol Soc 93:1041–1067. doi:10.1175/BAMS-D-12-00021.1 Ruddell D, Harlan SL, Grossman-Clarke S, Chowell G (2012) Scales of perception: public awareness of regional and neighborhood climates. Clim Chang 111:581–607. doi:10.1007/s10584-011-0165-y Rudman LA, McLean MC, Bunzl M (2013) When truth is personally inconvenient, attitudes change: the impact of extreme weather on implicit support for green politicians and explicit climate-change beliefs. Psychol Sci 24:2290–2296. doi:10.1177/0956797613492775 Silver A, Andrey J (2014) The influence of previous disaster experience and sociodemographics on protective behaviors during two successive tornado events. Weather Clim Soc 6:91–103. doi:10.1175/WCAS-D-1300026.1 SPC (2013) Storm Prediction Center severe weather GIS (SVRGIS). In: Natl Ocean Atmospheric Adm Natl Weather Serv. http://www.spc.noaa.gov/gis/svrgis/. Accessed 17 Sept 2013 Spence A, Poortinga W, Butler C, Pidgeon NF (2011) Perceptions of climate change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nat Clim Chang 1:46–49. doi:10.1038/nclimate1059 Stoffle RW, Traugott MW, Stone JV et al (1991) Risk perception mapping: using ethnography to define the locally affected population for a low-level radioactive waste storage facility in Michigan. Am Anthropol 93: 611–635. doi:10.1525/aa.1991.93.3.02a00050 Stone JV (2001) Risk perception mapping and the Fermi II nuclear power plant: toward an ethnography of social access to public participation in Great Lakes environmental management. Environ Sci Pol 4:205–217 Tierney KJ (2007) From the margins to the mainstream? Disaster research at the crossroads. Annu Rev Sociol 33: 503–525. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.33.040406.131743 Wachinger G, Renn O, Begg C, Kuhlicke C (2013) The risk perception paradox—implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal 33:1049–1065. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x Zaalberg R, Midden C, Meijnders A, McCalley T (2009) Prevention, adaptation, and threat denial: flooding experiences in the Netherlands. Risk Anal 29:1759–1778 Zaval L, Keenan EA, Johnson EJ, Weber EU (2014) How warm days increase belief in global warming. Nat Clim Chang 4:143–147. doi:10.1038/nclimate2093 Zhao X, Maibach E, Gandy J et al (2014) Climate change education through TV weathercasts: results of a field experiment. Bull Am Meteorol Soc 95:117–130. doi:10.1175/BAMS-D-12-00144.1