* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Fulltext - Brunel University Research Archive
History of anthropometry wikipedia , lookup
Cultural ecology wikipedia , lookup
Forensic anthropology wikipedia , lookup
Social Bonding and Nurture Kinship wikipedia , lookup
Children's geographies wikipedia , lookup
American anthropology wikipedia , lookup
Spoilt Rotten wikipedia , lookup
Cross-cultural differences in decision-making wikipedia , lookup
Intercultural competence wikipedia , lookup
Political economy in anthropology wikipedia , lookup
Social sharing of emotions wikipedia , lookup
Ethnography wikipedia , lookup
Current Emotion Research in Anthropology Super Title: VIEW FROM A DISCIPLINE Running Head: CURRENT EMOTION RESEARCH IN ANTHROPOLOGY Current Emotion Research in Anthropology: Reporting the Field Andrew Beatty Brunel University, UK Correspondence to: Andrew Beatty Department of Anthropology School of Social Sciences Brunel University Uxbridge UB8 3PH UK Email: [email protected] 1 Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 2 Abstract An internal critique of anthropology in recent decades has shifted the focus and scope of anthropological work on emotion. In this article, I review the changes, explore the pros and cons of leading anthropological approaches and theories, and argue that—so far as anthropology is concerned—only detailed narrative accounts can do full justice to the complexity of emotions. A narrative approach captures both the particularity and the temporal dimension of emotion with greater fidelity than semantic, synchronic, and discourse-based approaches. Keywords: anthropological approaches, ethnography, translation, narrative Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 3 Introduction: Dialogue across Boundaries A review of what anthropologists get up to—how they think about emotion—will look a little different depending on whether one is talking en famille or to friendly neighbors over the fence. Not many anthropologists are trained in psychology or philosophy; few can hope to keep abreast of the booming interdisciplinary emotion literature. Those few specialists aside, we tend to take what we find useful, cheerfully rummaging among theories like Levi-Strauss’s bricoleur, the DIY enthusiast who picks out what fits his or her needs without always knowing what the original item was intended for. Theoretical innocence about emotion might detract from the usefulness of many anthropological studies for psychologists, though of course the difficulty of reading across disciplines cuts both ways. Anthropologists grumble at accounts of “culture” or “cultures” that don't register how contested these concepts have become in the last thirty years; they shrug at “ethnographic” reports that scant the deep immersion that traditionally characterizes anthropology. A further barrier is that terms of art have different meanings across boundaries, stemming from diverse intellectual traditions. Embodiment in psychology concerns the way in which emotion is embedded in physiology, in the production and recognition of facial expression, in the neurology underlying empathy, and so on. In anthropology, embodiment leaves physiology untouched, nerves unjangled. It denotes an anti-positivist perspective on “lived experience” and “being-in-the-world” that draws heavily upon phenomenology (Csordas, 2002). Phenomenology, in turn, means something rather different in anthropology and in experimental psychology (where it often means no more than subjective feeling). It was first used in anthropology by Hallowell (1955) to analyze the framing of human experience in what he called the “culturally constituted behavioral environment”. Nowadays phenomenological approaches in anthropology draw explicitly on Continental philosophy or join a parallel venture that one of its Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 4 exponents, following William James, champions as “radical empiricism” (Jackson, 1989). The emphasis here is on capturing the vivid immediacy of experience, from which meaning cannot be separated. Anthropologists working in these traditions do not usually show much interest in the broader currents of psychological and (analytical) philosophical writing on emotion. Readers will be aware that affect now has similarly diverse, even incommensurate, meanings across disciplines. These hazards notwithstanding, I shall defend the view that anthropology can maintain a dialogue with neighboring disciplines about emotion, providing not only stimulating evidence, as it always has done, but critical challenges to current thinking. The key, I shall suggest, is in keeping faith with the findings and insights of ethnography, which time and again have eluded neat theoretical formulation. Innocent or not, the anthropologist can be relied upon to turn up awkward and surprising facts. The problem is what to make of them, how to recognize them, and how to write about them. When it comes to emotions, anthropologists have often skimped these difficult questions, failing to capitalize on their greatest resource: the living evidence of the field. Leaving aside the vast and various findings of world ethnography, my concern in this article will be with unresolved conceptual and methodological issues. I want to suggest it is time for a rethinking. Whatever Happened to the Anthropology of Emotion? An overview of highlights in the anthropology of emotion would show that the major contributions have mostly been made some time ago. Briggs (1970), Levy (1973, 1984), Lutz (1988), M. Rosaldo (1984), R. Rosaldo (1989), Shweder (1991), White (1990), and Wikan (1990) staked out the key theoretical positions a generation ago and offered much of the best evidence up until the millennium. (Long-term contributors, such as Shweder and White, are Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 5 unusual.) So what has happened since? The apparent thinness of current emotion research is due to several factors. Paradigm Creep A shift of emphasis in psychological anthropology away from emotion in favor of “subjectivity”, “embodiment”, “personhood”, and “experience” has fruitfully complicated the issues, making emotion one of a set of interrelated problem-aspects rather than a distinct topic or explicit focus of interest (Biehl et al., 2007; Csordas, 2002; Jackson, 1989). The above litany needs quotes because the key terms refer not to transparent concepts or standard fields of interest—unlike, say, cognition (another growth area in anthropology)—but, in their fullest extension, to research paradigms with distinctive theoretical traditions (some retrospectively constructed), canonical authors, and specialized jargon. Cross-cutting these divergences, broadly assumed—if continuously revised—central theoretical concepts have been ditched or dismantled in the search for sharper, more manageable tools. “Culture” has given way to “cultural models”, “hegemony”, and “discourse”; “society” to “sociality” and “relatedness”. Dissolution of the “Subject” Postcolonial theory, work in political economy, the postmodern critique, and a trend toward a more political vision of the discipline have banished the representative cultural actor (The Balinese, The Nuer) from the scene as fictions and thrown the emphasis onto how people navigate within structured systems of power (Clifford & Marcus, 1986). Thanks to the influence of Bourdieu and Foucault, one might say actors have become politically instead of culturally generic (culture being an aspect of power). Theorists have found new ways of ignoring—more tendentiously, “abolishing”—the individual. The locus of emotion has evaporated. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 6 Absorption into Cognitive Science Largely a thing apart and at odds with the above developments, a resurgent anthropological interest in cognition joins interdisciplinary work in cognitive science and evolutionary psychology. Some of this work touches on emotion. For example, Whitehouse’s (2004) theory of “modes of religiosity”, though not centrally concerned with the nature of emotion, gives the ritual elicitation of violent emotions a causal role in the transmission of religious practices in non-literate societies. Globalization Globalization has shaken up the field, in both senses (the places we go, the things we do), forcing a rethinking of what anthropologists should be looking at, how their enquiries should be framed, and what methodologies to employ (Inda & Rosaldo, 2007). The old model of the researcher alone in a remote village documenting the culture no longer passes muster. “Cultures” as bounded, homogeneous isolates are gone; so too has the ethnographer as invisible recording angel. In line with critical, postcolonial, and postmodernist questionings of social scientific authority, the ethnographer is now a “positioned subject” (Rosaldo, 1989) in need of critical scrutiny, not a neutral, harmless presence. Ethnic identities are fluid, pragmatic, often plural; old distinctions between home and abroad, “us and them”, have disappeared. Increasingly, researchers speak of diasporas, borderlands, transnationalism, “ethnoscapes”, and “cultural flows” rather than peoples and cultures. As these heterogeneous factors indicate, in the swirl of theoretical realignments and global change, emotion research can no longer be surveyed as one among many areas of research like, say, ritual, migration, or markets. The last such comprehensive effort (Lutz & White, 1986) could not, I think, be repeated today. Emotion intersects with divergent research programs in Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 7 disparate ways, in different theoretical languages, frustrating the kind of cumulative, inclusive debate on common ground characteristic of this journal. Any anthropologist’s view is therefore bound to be limited and partial. If this sounds like a counsel of despair, we should remember that what most anthropological approaches have in common is a basis in and commitment to ethnographic fieldwork. It is from this common ground—and with a view back to it: a validation of fieldwork as the best source of anthropological insight—that I shall base my review. What Is Emotion? The Ethnographic Challenge Consider their diversity. Some emotions come with distinctive facial expressions (anger), some without (regret). Some respond instantly to an external stimulus (surprise), others follow introspection (remorse); some prompt action (disgust), others imply inaction (boredom); some have an evolutionary pay-off (fear, love), others lack adaptive advantage (nostalgia). Given this heterogeneity, it’s hardly surprising that a superordinate category of emotion has not been reported from many of the places where anthropologists have worked. For Russell (1991, p. 430), this is the principal challenge posed by ethnography. If a domain of emotion is unrecognized or unnamed, the comparative project of recording and contrasting exotic emotion terms seems threatened. For what is to count as an emotion? The very idea of an emotional domain can lead us to misjudge similarly named behaviors as equivalent. It is not always clear, for example, whether a reported instance of “shame” refers to a feeling, a form of etiquette, or an unemotional evaluation of a situation (Russell, 1991; Beatty, 2005a, 2005b). Still, emotion is the word we are stuck with. And if the historical question is why the category should have arisen in Europe, the scientific and philosophical question is whether its cultural specificity vitiates its general applicability. Can it be that our folk concept just happens to capture a human universal? Or are scientific definitions notably different? Are they culture-free? Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 8 A glance at the literature shows that, without being committed to a firm definition of the object of study or a shared view of its reality, theorists in the human sciences and philosophy are, indeed, able to agree on a rough area of discussion they call “emotion”. In such collections as Ekman and Davidson (1994), Goldie (2010), and Manstead et al. (2004), contributors appear to accept the usefulness of the English word to categorize certain socially embedded psychobiological processes without agreeing about how such processes cohere, or how much causal or definitional prominence should be given to such components as arousal, feeling, appraisal, or facial expression. For some theorists “emotion” denotes a class of distinctive processes conveniently labeled by the English word; for others it lacks any essential referent. But how coherent is the concept if “emotions” are not natural kinds (Barrett, 2006; Scarantino, 2012) and there is no agreement about what the word otherwise might mean? A compromise position between realism and skepticism can be found in Averill’s (1994) suggestion that emotion is a polythetic class, that is, a class composed of overlapping sets of members belonging in some loose grouping not by virtue of exclusive identity but by sporadic family resemblances. Needham (1975), from whom I have taken this definition, first explored the implications of polythetic classification for anthropology, pointing out that many of the categories used by anthropologists, such as religion, kinship, and marriage, have turned out, on critical inspection (or on the evidence of ethnographic variation), to be polythetic; which meant that generalizations based on the assumption that kinship—Needham’s field—was a homogeneous category were false. This led him to such radical pronouncements as “there is no such thing as kinship”. A trail of scholars in Needham’s wake announced the death of this or that category. Yet the anthropological ship sailed on. And after languishing in the doldrums, kinship stormed back onto the scene, albeit trimmed and tweaked into novel perspectives (e.g., Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 9 “relatedness”) that avoid some of the old essentialist assumptions. The key message was that rather than abandoning the kinship concept altogether, one respected its polythetic configuration and looked for regularities at a lower level, within a fuzzily-bounded field, while remaining alert to connections across the boundary with, say, politics. This, I take it, is what emotion skeptics like Shweder (1994) and Wierzbicka (1999) are advocating. Why else write a book called Emotions across Languages and Cultures when the English word “emotion” does not “carve nature at its joints” (Wierzbicka, 1999, p. 3)? Granted this polythetic definition, the anthropologist can get to work. According to Shweder (2004, p. 83), “emotion is a complex synthetic notion; and particular emotions (e.g., sadness, envy, guilt, and love) are derivatives of various combinations of wants, beliefs, feelings, and values.” The anthropologist’s job, on this view, would be to investigate such combinations without a prior commitment to that notional whole, “emotion”. In similar terms, Wierzbicka (1999, p. 24) proposes investigating “questions focusing on what people think, feel, want, know, say, and do; what happens in their bodies; how the thoughts, feelings, wants, and bodily events are linked… and what role the feelings…play in the stream of life”. This proposal, which encompasses a great deal while apparently taking little for granted, offers a robust basis for ethnographic research. Nonetheless, those who see emotion as having some theory-independent reality or integrity (e.g., Manstead et al., 2004; Mulligan & Scherer, 2012) might wonder at the rationale of a comparative project whose central organizing concept is in doubt. Fortunately for dialogue, both relativists and realists are interested in how appraisals, feelings, words, and actions are variably linked, however those linkages may be conceived. Shweder (1995, 2004) is prepared to suggest that the linkages might not point to something that we would call emotion. Indeed, to assume otherwise is to prejudge the case and rule out the Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 10 possibility that in other cultural settings appraisals, feelings, and behavior might not hang together in ways familiar to us. He argues that we must be open to the possibility—attested by ethnography (Levy, 1973, 1984)—that the death of a loved one may be experienced as fatigue or illness rather than “emotionalized” as “sadness”. This would not be altogether surprising given that, as Lutz (1988, p. 100) writes of Ifaluk (and as many ethnographers testify), “emotion, thought, and body are seen in ethnotheory as intimately linked through their roles in illness.” But a non-emotional response would depend on the possibility that cultural practices don’t merely shape experience but can override putatively universal processes. “Emotionalizing”, on this view, would consist in consciously dwelling on the personal dimension of loss, thinking about the feeling. It would be an open question which kinds of experience are emotionalized or not in a given cultural setting. Shweder is, I believe, onto something here, and his deconstructive method is useful for the fieldworker. It helps to makes sense of a puzzling episode in my Javanese fieldwork (Beatty, 2009, pp. 245–258) when the headman of my host village, publicly humiliated by Islamist critics in the mosque on the Prophet’s Birthday and unable to face them down, suddenly sank back in a faint—“dizzy”, he croaked—and fell mysteriously ill. He hadn’t emotionalized his response to an event that might otherwise have prompted expressions of shame or anger. Although I cannot precisely know the string of thoughts and feelings leading up to his collapse, seated next to him I was party to his deliberations as he whispered to me and passed me notes. He was paralyzed by hesitation—whether to respond or submit. A Javanese adage I often heard when conflict loomed goes: “Aja kalah, ngalah!” “Better to concede than be defeated!” Sometimes one can’t oppose brute force, stupidity, or craziness, so one yields, as a tree bends to the wind, and survives. The headman explicitly deliberated in this way but evidently felt the attack as overwhelming: he Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 11 could neither accept defeat nor shrug in face-saving concession. So how did he construe the assault—or, in Shweder’s preferred sequence, the feeling? In some sense, evidently, he did not emotionalize it; that is, he didn’t interpret the event or the feelings evoked in terms that entailed the locally predictable emotions. To put this in English terms (and setting aside the question of their cross-cultural validity), his social defeat was less a shaming disgrace or angering affront than something like a physical blow: so it materialized. Accordingly, his slow recovery was not effected through emotional introspection or catharsis (as we, in his place might have it), nor through angry revenge or a public expiation of shame. Instead, he outflanked his critics. Victory was the cure. I think also of the ploughman who one day welcomed me into his house shortly after finding his buffalo—his sole means of livelihood—poisoned in the stable. His perfect composure and broad smile gave nothing away. (I heard about the buffalo later.) This was not simply an instance of Javanese display rules, miraculous self-control, or even—I hope—of extreme ethnographic dullness on my part. The links between event, appraisal, feeling, and expression were not what I could have expected or could even have recognized, though we had been neighbors for two years and I knew the man fairly well. Again, something in the sequence of an emotion episode appears to have been short-circuited, truncated, or overcome: exhibiting less, implying more, than what might have been expected. In Shweder’s analysis (which in this respect is Jamesian), the emotion is an interpretation of the feeling, rather than an interpretation of the eliciting event. Emotions are “complex narrative structures that give shape and meaning to somatic and affective experiences” (1994, p. 37). I’m not persuaded that the sequence is necessarily so. My Javanese cases are equivocal; but so too is introspective knowledge. Sometimes we scan our feelings as a touchstone of what really Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 12 matters to us: “Do I really love her?” More often the touchstone is the affecting situation: “Was that really meant as an insult? Should I be angry?” But however the parts fit together, the challenge remains. Ethnography throws up examples that contradict our expectations of how emotions work, prompting doubts about whether they qualify as emotions, and therefore whether there are such things—cross-culturally and unambiguously—as emotions (as opposed to diverse combinations of appraisals, feelings, etc.). As ever, it is the exceptions that present the greatest challenge. I have mentioned two instances of my own already. A famous example that bears revisiting is that of Levy’s Tahitian informant, a man abandoned by his wife, who “interpreted his feelings about separation as some sort of vague sickness” (Levy, 1973, p. 304). On Shweder’s view (1994), one could not apply the concepts of “sadness” or “emotion” here because the feeling has not been emotionalized. In another Tahitian example, a man seen crying at the grave of his wife is assumed by witnesses to be feeling remorse for his infidelities rather than—as we should suppose—sadness for his recent loss (Levy, 1973, pp. 298, 301). This is a different kind of evidence from the self-reported malaise of the deserted husband. Nevertheless, in all such cases, the ethnographer argues, Tahitians “hypocognize” sadness (Levy, 1984, p. 227). Lacking an equivalent word, perhaps lacking the concept altogether, and without the cultural formulas and expectations of prolonged grief, they have no means of packaging their feelings and thoughts as anything like sadness and do not recognize or expect persisting sadness in others. Troublesome feelings following loss are interpreted, instead, as possession by the departed spirit (Levy, 1973, p. 299; 1984). For Levy, this implies misrecognition of the primary apprehension, a culturally imposed cognitive mistake rather than a simple absence of the (to us) predictable emotion. Like Shweder, Levy makes the interpretation contingent on the feeling. (The abandoned husband obsesses about his loss, so it’s Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 13 not the event or what Lazarus (1994) calls the core relational theme that is underrated, only the bad feeling.) Despite the wealth of cultural information presented in Tahitians, these much discussed examples are reported in scant terms, with minimal concrete observation, and mixed with hypothetical cases. We know nothing of the background of the individuals, the quality of their relationships, the history of separations, or how normal the individuals are. The absence of idiosyncratic particulars and the recourse to general cultural factors and folk psychology make it hard to compare among cases and across cultures. I cannot easily apply Levy’s analysis to my Javanese friends whose reactions, in any case, are not especially typical, even of the men themselves. To make sense of them you need a good deal of biographical background and narrative context. Levy’s Tahitians are presented as both typical and fully explained by immediate context; yet only in the general terms of cultural logic do the examples carry conviction. On a human level, as persons with particular relations and histories, they remain enigmatic, merely exotic. Indeed, the reader cannot help wondering whether the sharpening of the argument, the distillation of a generalization, exaggerates the difference. Is there, as in Ifaluk and Java, a close association between emotion and illness that might explain why a man haunted by his wife’s departure should experience “vague illness”? Or are there untold stories that explain the underrating of the feelings associated with loss? Something is missing, but we should not assume it is missing from the Tahitians. If I have labored an old example, it is because Levy’s ethnography has featured in many discussions of emotion, besides serving as the model for subsequent “person-centered ethnography”. It remains unsurpassed in its comprehensiveness, but the problems with its presentation of emotion have not been properly identified or overcome. And there is a further Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 14 lesson. If, as is often the case with cross-cultural comparison, compression, selection, and narrative omission enhance difference, so does the inverse stereotyping of “Euro-American culture”, the usual point of departure.1 We emotionalize, they somatize. We feel sadness, they feel fatigue. Yet anyone who has attended a modern English funeral cannot be surprised by Tahiti. The collective determination to put on a good face, the cheerful tributes celebrating the life of the deceased, and the sheer lack of solemnity would have seemed unaccountable fifty years ago. And we learn from the latest draft of DSM-V that grief lasting more than a few weeks is to be classed as pathological. We are all becoming “Tahitians”. Emotions in Translation Evan-Pritchard, in an apocryphal statement, described anthropology as “the translation of culture”. In her pathbreaking book, Unnatural Emotions, Lutz writes: “The process of coming to understand the emotional lives of people in different cultures can be seen first and foremost as a problem of translation” (1988, p. 8). Wisely, Lutz extends the problem of translation to “tears and other gestures, and…audience reaction to emotional performance”, but her emphasis is on language use, or “discourse”.2 This does not extend, however, to the inclusion of natural dialogues or ethnographic narrative; nor does discourse, in her presentation, have much historical resonance or time-depth. As with Levy, her examples are in the present and exist only in the present. They are mostly typifying and shorn of particularity. Indeed, an exclusion of idiosyncratic background (if one can call what is most significant for any individual background) is a matter of principle: “cultural and historical factors supersede the individual in explanatory import” (p. 229, n.1). What underpins Lutz’s approach is its strong sociologism, an attribute she shares with Michelle Rosaldo, another pioneer of constructionism in the anthropology of emotion. “Talk Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 15 about emotions is simultaneously talk about society,” she writes (1988, p. 6). In the same vein, Rosaldo (1984, p. 142) declares: “selves and feelings, shaped by culture, may be understood in turn as the creation of particular sorts of polities.” Lutz’s position is the ne plus ultra of constructionism, the “unnaturalness” of emotions consisting precisely in their cultural composition, their embedding in social processes. Accordingly, the causal nexus of Ifaluk emotion, in Lutz’s analysis, is functional. Ifaluk emotions take the shape they do because of the constraints of living in large, intimate households on a small, crowded island at the mercy of the elements. The great originality of the book, however, lies not in this functionalist thesis but in the exploration of “emotional meaning” and its pragmatic interweaving with social processes. In common with constructionists like Averill and Bedford, Lutz presents a view of emotions—or at least emotional discourse—as pragmatic ploys whose meaning and function can be explained almost entirely through situational context. All the more surprising, then, that the ethnography contains very few detailed firsthand accounts. In fact, there is only one episode witnessed by the author that extends over more than a couple of lines (Lutz, 1988, pp. 125–127), and this concerns the death of a child: not an event likely to be rich in emotional nuance or backstory. In fact, Unnatural Emotions is largely free of particular persons, histories, and narratives—just those things which, I suggest, give emotions their force and resonance, their quality as emotions, distinct from other kinds of opinions, moral judgments, and speech acts. It is free of “natural emotions” because it depends mostly on data obtained from interviews, selfreports, card-sorting classifications, and hypothetical examples. The picture that emerges of emotion as a function of social situations—and generic situations at that—is also a function of the methodology. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 16 Nevertheless, if we take emotions in an expanded sense (as ploys, ways of talking about society, etc.), Lutz marks a step forward, a means to understanding other kinds of sociality unimaginable within a Western individualist mindset. Her discourse-centered approach has much in common with that of White (1990, 2005), whose work on another Pacific society explores the political and moral role of emotional rhetoric. White shows how in the Solomon Islands, public debate enables the reframing of conflict situations by the indirect means of emotion talk or “disentangling”. Through participants’ narration of events, “anger”, which encodes a scenario of retribution, gives way to “sadness”, which implies a peaceful outcome. “Disentangling” is not mere social commentary: it has a pragmatic effect on the assembly, engendering pro-social emotions. White calls it an example of an “emotive institution”. Having read Lutz and White, I find I am better able to understand the impassioned oratory of Nias, a tribal society in Indonesia where I carried out fieldwork in the mid-1980s. A hundred years earlier (when James and Darwin were publishing their pioneering studies), a German missionary was similarly puzzling over Niasan emotion idioms. In preparation for his Bible translation, Sundermann (1887) recorded 88 terms referring to the heart, including such bizarre collocations as “my heart has an unhulled grain” (suspicious) and “feeling as though my heart has swallowed a ball of cat’s fur” (seriously disgruntled). Aside from “hot-hearted” (angry), “broken-hearted” (disappointed) and a few others with rough equivalents, most cardiac terms were not readily comprehensible. Even the simpler phrases could be misleading. “Painfulhearted” meant “feeling resentful/spiteful”; “clear-hearted” meant “content”. And there was an overlap with non-emotional feelings and thoughts: ebolo dödö “broad-hearted” = patient; aboto ba dödö “broken in the heart” = understand; other heart terms referred to thirst and tiredness. Sundermann’s approach depended on finding dictionary-like equivalences (he went on to write a Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 17 German-Nias dictionary), so naturally he drew many blanks. What did it mean to have a scorched or squeezed heart, or to “have a curly hair in the heart”? And how to explain the preponderance of negative terms? Niasan heart-speech seemed to map a vast region of discomfort and grievance unsuspected even by the grim Lutheran missionaries.3 A century later, I was similarly perplexed. The fact that most of the terms in use refer not to personality traits but to feelings is all the more puzzling, suggesting a cognitive clustering at the lower end of the emotional keyboard. (A man reported as aukhu dödö, “hot-hearted”, is allegedly angry rather than irascible.) Yet heart-speech in Nias is best understood as a mode of public oratory, not a native psychology. People declare their hearts at weddings and feasts when the intention is to win concessions in ceremonial exchange or fend off the other party’s demands for pigs and gold (Beatty, 2012). Although their speeches are often passionate, the pleas and rebuffs of “scorched” and “shriveled” hearts are pragmatic, even political, as speakers jockey for position. So the auditors do not look for signs of authentic feeling in a speaker parading a scorched heart (though they might anticipate a certain move); nor do they question his sincerity. In most cases heart terms do not describe actual feelings, either occurrent or past; nor do they index behavior. Away from the public stage, there is no cat’s fur feeling. In ordinary speech a much smaller set of terms is used for emotions—simple rather than metaphorical and with cognates in other Austronesian languages (e.g., ata’u = afraid; cf. Malay takut, Ifaluk metagu). Should the more outlandish heart idioms nevertheless qualify as emotion terms? By most definitions, they check all the boxes. They are announced and enacted with passion; they provoke an emotional response; they imply a “normative judgment about one’s situation” (Solomon, 1980, p. 258); and they imply action tendencies (of refusal, gratitude, threat, and evasion), which is why they have rhetorical force. In many respects, heart speech is enacted Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 18 emotion—otherwise it wouldn’t work. But the emotions actually experienced—by the speakers and the people they are talking about—are different from the affairs of the heart reported. There is a distance between the mixed, undeclared emotions animating the contending parties and the manipulative, positional soul-searching of the orators who represent them. Yet although the auditors recognize posturing for what it is, they are nevertheless moved. This, precisely, is the power of oratory. If the job is well done, the targets feel constrained to concede, to throw in an extra pig or grain of gold: their hearts are truly squeezed. What matters if the speaker’s heart is not really scorched? We gain little by qualifying his phrases as pretend emotions or psychological impossibilities. Far more is to be gained by recognizing the layered context of staged emotion, its background in interwoven histories, and its relation to unvoiced emotions, past and present. From Discourse to Narrative The language of the heart in Nias is performative, a tactic in the game of exchange. Emotion speech is at once a form of politics, a technique of micro-management, and a rhetoric of moral suasion: on this we can agree with the cultural constructionists. But there is a life beyond the stage, an emotional hinterland, both shared and intensely personal, that is not captured in talk alone and for which there are no stage directions, no simple cultural grammar. To make sense of it one needs time-depth and a thorough knowledge of plots and players, the people who inhabit the roles. A debate is a moving tableau of real persons locked into evolving stories, stories that motivate and overflow the confines of the debate. The “tactical uses of passion”, in Bailey’s phrase (1983), imply narrative structures and call, in turn, for narrative ethnography, not mere diachronic modeling. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 19 If this narrative claim is true of set-piece oratory, with its well-defined positions and tropes and its indirect relation to occurrent emotion, it applies all the better to everyday emotions. The reasons are twofold. Firstly, as diverse theorists have emphasized, emotions are nothing if not particular. Unlike values or norms, which by definition are widely shared, emotions refer compellingly to an individual’s or group’s situation. In Rosaldo’s (1984, p. 143) phrase, they are “embodied thoughts, thoughts seeped with the apprehension that ‘I am involved’”. Lutz (1988, p. 71) has pointed out that in the American value system, “the subjectivity of emotions…gives them a fundamental—even sacred—role in individuating the person…From this perspective, emotions are Me in a way that thoughts are not.” Yet even in places like Ifaluk, where emotions are not so conceived—where they are often predicated of “us”—they remain first-person experiences, felt in “our insides”; indeed, emerging from “our insides” (p. 95). Javanese like to say that at the level of rasa (feeling/consciousness) we are “all the same”; but they do not deny the individuality of the experiencing self. Solomon (1995, p. 191), in an East-West comparison, agrees: The personal aspect of an emotion derives from the individual’s “perspective, background, fears, and concerns,” not from the feeling. The peculiar egocentricity of emotion, which psychologists and philosophers have remarked upon (Barrett, 2006; Ben-Ze’ev, 2010; Roberts, 1988), is not contradicted by shared experiences or patterns of emotion that are culturally typical. The critical point is to recognize that our emotions are always more than cultural formulas and social structural positions require.4 My anger, hope, and regret make sense against a background of cultural expectations and social possibilities, but they take shape within the unique circumstances of a life, which is why they are mine alone. The me-focus of emotions—which transcends contrasts between individualist and Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 20 sociocentric ideologies—positively requires a descent to particulars. Only ethnographic narrative can capture this particularity. The second reason why emotion implicates narrative is again to do with how emotions are constituted. Pride, shame, contempt, even fear and anger, are not only tailored to individual experience, they unfold over time; they tell a story. This obvious fact is often neglected. A focus on emotions of short duration (of the kind easily produced in the lab) misleads about the role emotions play in our lives. Likewise, an ethnographic emphasis on stereotyped behaviors (avoidance, respect) tends to mask the temporal dimension that inflects even formalized relationships. Take the case of rural East Java, where fathers and adult sons make reluctant companions and don’t generally sit together—a mild avoidance recognized in seating arrangements at feasts. The slight awkwardness that prevails has its roots in deeply held values of respect, socialization practices, and ideas about personhood—structurally, sons “replace” their fathers and are felt to be in mystical competition with them (Beatty, 2009; H. Geertz, 1974). But the statistical average—the stereotyped emotion—doesn’t begin to capture the reality. Even in the cases that most conform to type there are always personal stories and previous emotional encounters that color the relation, often in contradictory ways, and give it its meaning and peculiar tone. It is those past encounters—culturally framed but underdetermined—that are most significant to any given relationship and help explain the course of an emotional episode. Of the objections that might be offered to a narrative approach, two are most challenging: that it gives an illusion of coherence, and that narrative accounts may be culturally inappropriate. In his study of the Yolmo of Nepal, Desjarlais forswears conventional narrative because “in conveying suffering to others, villagers tend not to tell sequential accounts of how they hurt or heal, but rather to ‘tell images’ that portray their plights: a witch’s bloody assault, the ‘casting’ of Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 21 grief from the body” (1992, p. 31). On this view, it is inappropriate to impose a narrative structure on emotion where none has been offered. To recall an earlier example, if the Tahitians don’t emotionalize certain feelings, nor would they make sense of them narratively. The narrator falsifies. But the point must be conceded only if we stick to articulated feelings. Few ethnographers would so limit themselves. Besides, we all know cases of individuals who seem unaware they are jealous, angry or in love. It’s precisely through narrative, not self-report, that we are able to construe their cases. The fitting together of actions, responses, expressions, and language in a temporal sequence that respects particularities—in a word, narrative—explains what the passionate actor herself cannot see or say. For the same reason, one should treat selfreported emotion and autobiographical narrative (especially as told to a cultural outsider) with caution. The shaping and revision involved distort the original experience. As White (1994, p. 231) puts it, if emotions are “fundamentally relational”, one cannot analyze them out of context. Taking his point strictly, we should insist on the living context: the remembered context is altogether a different thing. As to the question of whether narrative might be wholly alien to a certain people, the case remains to be proven. Even supposing an aversion to psychological explanations of behavior such as Mead (1972, p. 107) found in Samoa, or granted a dogma of the “opacity of other minds” such as is widely reported in the Pacific (Hollan & Throop, 2011), people everywhere need to be able to read behavior, fathom motives, assign significance, and apportion blame (Bruner, 1990). A sense of the evolving structure of persons-in-situations is therefore built into human sociality, whether the apprehensions are worked up into fully-fledged narratives, transformed into visual images, tactfully downplayed, or altogether denied. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 22 New Developments Boster (2005) lists empathy, alongside “mapping” and translation, as one of three methods open to the ethnographer in comparing “emotion systems”. As anthropologists have reconsidered the dynamics of fieldwork, pondering the epistemological quandaries of participant-observation and the basis of anthropological knowledge in the human encounter, the question of empathy has become increasingly salient. The revival of interest has a number sources, among them the rediscovery of Briggs’s (1970) classic account of fieldwork among the Inuit, Rosaldo’s (1989) still-influential essay on the anthropologist as “positioned subject”, and person-centered ethnographies such as Wikan’s (1990) Balinese study, with its zeitgeist-capturing title, Managing turbulent hearts. These works explored the researcher’s ambivalent positioning in the field and the possibility of knowing through feeling; in different ways they testified to common factors in human experience that challenged the prevailing relativism of the time. Recent work has reopened the debate (for a skeptical review of the topic, see Beatty 2005a, 2010). Hage’s (2009) focus, for example, is on the shared experience of hosts and guests. Do the researcher’s emotions in the field offer insight or illusion? That depends, says Hage, on the overlap not only in situation and cultural framing but in intentions, stakes, and personal formation. Hage’s interest is in “political emotions”. “Are there such things as emotions that are sui generis political, or are there merely emotions in general that we end up investing in various spheres of social life, the sphere of politics being one among many” (2009, p. 60)? A familiarity with the non-anthropological literature might have helped here, but to answer the question Hage considers how his own sentiments, as a secular Lebanese exile, differed from those of his Muslim hosts in confronting the Israeli assault on Lebanon in 2006. His friends could not feel hatred for an anthropomorphized state as Hage could. Their engagement—their political Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 23 emotions—required something different from the passive frustration of the intellectual, namely an emotive identification with the resistance that turned them into actors in the conflict, not mere witnesses or victims. They felt differently because they confronted a practical enemy, not an ideological adversary. Hage comes to see that his own vacillation between participation and observation—his semi-identification—limited the possibility of empathy. Drawing on the historian Rosenwein’s notion of “emotional communities”, Dureau (2012) found that the experience of her child falling sick in the field did not permit a privileged understanding of mothers in the Western Solomons. Their expressions of maternal love take shape against a background of food shortages, disease, and life chances starkly different from those of the Western researcher. “Against such claims of experiential knowing, I became increasingly aware that with better understanding of women’s lives I felt deeper sympathy but less emotional congruence, grasping taru [“love”] as I recognized the relative superficiality of our similarities”(p. 146). Examples pro and contra empathy-as-method could be multiplied; but only in longer works can the reader really judge what has been achieved. At novelistic length, there is enough circumstance, plot, and characterization to place and evoke emotions. Briggs (1970), ScheperHughes (1992), and Abu-Lughod (2000) offer the outstanding examples of insightful accounts informed by a self-conscious but unshowy ethnographic empathy. In each case, a scrutiny of the ethnographer’s motives and feelings offers insights into local constructions of emotion. Once again, narrative provides the necessary scaffold. Curiously, none of these works has much overlap with more conventionally empirical studies of empathy as an ethnographic topic, such as Hollan and Throop’s (2011) recent volume on Pacific societies, or Goluboff’s (2008) study of Azeri Jewish lamentation. The former looks at Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 24 empathy (or its denial) among people who hold to “the opacity of other minds”. The latter joins the mainstream of recent anthropological work on emotion in analyzing how specific emotions are generated, the work they do in social reproduction, and how they shape subjectivities. In Azerbaijan, the job of the paid mourner is to bring individual sorrows together in a common lament: “she transformed their personal sorrow into an empathetic statement of communal suffering” (p. 90). The bottom line is political: Weeping functions as an enactment of “female subjectivity firmly located in patriarchal kinship obligations”. A similar concern with the emotional fashioning of subjectivity—or for that matter the political fashioning of emotion—animates much recent work on trauma and memory, an expanding enterprise that crosses disciplinary lines and variously comments on and criticizes the trauma industry (see examples in Biehl et al., 2007). Beyond the scope of the present article, much of this work has its roots in Foucault (especially as refracted through Hacking), Critical Theory, postcolonial theory, and approaches from political economy. Other work seeks new emotional configurations in non-traditional settings. What, for example, is the nexus between emotion and social mobility in a globalizing world? What structures of feeling (nostalgia, homesickness, loss, hope) are created by migration or work in the modern industrial zone (Boelstorff & Lindquist, 2004)? Svašek and Skrbiš (2007) consider the “emotional dynamics of transnational family reunions” and ask how people adapt to the alien emotional climate of their home from home. They find that migrants’ distress is often medicalized in the West, “emphasiz[ing] the sharp edges of universalizing discourses of emotions in contexts of power inequality” (p. 375). Studies of this kind testify once again to the value of ethnography in extending our knowledge of how persons relate to contexts and how experience is shaped by broader forces. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 25 Conclusion Anthropology’s contribution to the study of emotion is at once comparative and critical. Its special method, ethnographic fieldwork, affords unique insights into cultural diversity and the many ways of being human. But if we are to realize those insights and continue to unsettle established categories in the human sciences, we must be faithful to the experience, thinking harder about what goes on in the field and about how we recognize, record, and write about emotion in other cultural settings. Interviews have their uses, but only detailed naturalistic observation that preserves the full narrative context can capture emotions in all their complexity. A major task for anthropology in the years ahead will be to rediscover the particularity of emotion and its temporal dimension which only narrative can adequately capture. For as Nussbaum (2001, p. 177) observes, “all human emotions are in part about the past, and bear traces of a history that is at once commonly human, socially constructed, and idiosyncratic”. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 26 References Abu-Lughod, L. (2000). Veiled sentiments: Honor and poetry in a Bedouin society.2nd ed. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Averill, J. (1994). In the eyes of the beholder. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 7–14). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Bailey, F. G. (1983). The tactical uses of passion: An essay on power, reason and reality. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 28– 58. Beatty, A. (2005a). Emotions in the field: What are we talking about? Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 11, 17–37. Beatty, A. (2005b). Feeling your way in Java: An essay on society and emotion. Ethnos, 70, 53– 78. Beatty, A. (2009). A shadow falls: In the heart of Java. London, UK: Faber. Beatty, A. (2010). How did it feel for you: Emotion, narrative, and the limits of ethnography. American Anthropologist, 112, 430–443. Beatty, A. (2012). The tell-tale heart: Conversion and emotion in Nias. Ethnos, 77, 1–26. Ben-Ze’ev, A. (2010). The thing called emotion. In P. Goldie (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion (pp. 41–62). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Biehl, J., Good, B., & Kleinman, A. (2007). (Eds.), Subjectivity: Ethnographic investigations. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Boellstorff, T., & Lindquist, J. (2004). Bodies of emotion: Rethinking culture and emotion through Southeast Asia. Ethnos, 69, 4. Special issue. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 27 Boster, J. S. (2005). Emotion categories across languages. In H. Cohen & C. Lefebvre, (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science, (pp. 187–222). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Briggs, J. (1970). Never in anger: Portrait of an Eskimo family. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Clifford, J., & Marcus, G.E. (Eds.), (1986). Writing culture: The poetics and politics of ethnography. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Csordas, T. J. (2002). Body, meaning, healing. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan. Desjarlais, R. (1992). Body and emotion: The aesthetics of illness and healing in the Nepal Himalayas. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Dureau, C. (2012). Translating love. Ethos, 40, 142–163. Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. (1994). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Geertz, H. (1974). The vocabulary of Javanese emotion. In R. A. LeVine (Ed.), Culture and personality (pp. 249–264). Chicago, IL: Aldine. Goldie, P. (Ed.). (2010). The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Goluboff, S. (2008). Patriarchy through lamentation in Azerbaijan. American Ethnologist, 35, 81–94. Hage, G. (2009). Hating Israel in the field: On ethnography and political emotions. Anthropological Theory, 9, 59–79. Hallowell, A. I. (1955). Culture and experience. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 28 Hollan, D., & Throop, C. J. (Eds.). (2011). The anthropology of empathy: Experiencing the lives of others in Pacific societies. New York, NY: Berghahn. Inda, X., & Rosaldo, R. (2007). (Eds.). The anthropology of globalization: A reader. Oxford, UK: Wiley/Blackwell. Jackson, M. (1989). Paths towards a clearing: Radical empiricism and ethnographic enquiry. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. Lazarus, R. (1994). Universal antecedents of the emotions. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 163-171). New York: Oxford University Press. Levy, R. I. (1973). Tahitians: Mind and experience in the Society Islands. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Levy, R. I. (1984). Emotion, knowing, and culture. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. Levine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 214–237). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and their challenge to Western theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lutz, C. A., & White, G. (1986). The anthropology of emotions. Annual Review of Anthropology 15, 405–436. Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N. H., & Fischer, A. (Eds.). (2004). Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Mead, M. (1972 [1928]). Coming of age in Samoa. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. Mulligan, K., & Scherer, K. R. (2012). Toward a working definition of emotion. Emotion Review, 4, 345–357. Needham, R. (1975). Polythetic classification. Man, 10, 349–367. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 29 Nussbaum, M. (2001). Upheavals of thought: The intelligence of emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Roberts, R. C. (1988). What an emotion is: A sketch. The Philosophical Review, 97, 183–209. Rosaldo, M. Z. (1984). Toward an anthropology of self and feeling. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. Levine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 137–157). Cambridge,UK: Cambridge University Press. Rosaldo, R. (1989). Culture and truth. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450. Scarantino, A. (2012). How to define emotions scientifically. Emotion Review, 4, 358–368. Scheper-Hughes, N. (1992). Death without weeping: The violence of everyday life in Brazil. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Shweder, R. A. (1994). “You’re not sick, you’re just in love”: Emotion as an interpretive system. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 7–14). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Shweder, R. A. (2004). Deconstructing the emotions for the sake of comparative research. In A S. R. Manstead, N. H. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 81–97). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Solomon, R. C. (1980). Emotions and choice. In A. Rorty (Ed.), Explaining emotions (pp. 251– 282). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 30 Solomon, R. C. (1995). Some notes on emotion, “East and West”. Philosophy East & West, 42, 171–202. Sundermann, H. (1887). Die Psychologie des Niassers. Allgemeine Missions-Zeitschrift, 14, 289– 302. Svašek, M., & Skrbiš, Z. (2007). Passions and powers: Emotions and globalisation. Identities: Global Studies in Culture and Power, 14, 367–383. White, G. M. (1990). Moral discourse and the rhetoric of emotion. In C. A. Lutz & L. AbuLughod (Eds.), Language and the politics of emotion (pp. 46–68). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. White, G. M. (1994). Affecting culture: Emotion and morality in everyday life. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 219–239). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. White, G. M. (2005). Emotive institutions. In C. Casey & R. Edgerton (Eds.), A companion to psychological anthropology (pp. 241–254). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and universals. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Wikan, U. (1990). Managing turbulent hearts: A Balinese design for living. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Wilce, J. M. (2009). Language and emotion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Current Emotion Research in Anthropology 31 Endnotes 1 For a systematic comparison, pitched at the level of ideology rather than experience, see Lutz, 1988. 2 For a comprehensive review of linguistic approaches to emotion, see Wilce, 2009. 3 White (1994, p. 226) sees in the high proportion of negative terms in emotion vocabularies “a reflection of the moral work that emotion words and expressions perform in ordinary discourse.” 4 This point does not depend on methodological individualism or an individualist ideology. The claim is not that individuals are the exclusive locus of emotions, that emotions are generated and contained within the self; only that they are personal and perspectival. The me-focus is often a we-focus, or, if predicated of others, a they-focus.