Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Cognitive psychology Cognitive psychology • • 2.1 Models of memory Recently, Schachter et al. (2000) added another LTM store – the perceptual representation system (PRS), which is related to perceptual priming, i.e. the increased likelihood of recognising something when you have recently seen something similar. After studying this section you should be able to: • • • describe the multi-store model of memory, including the concepts of encoding, capacity and duration give strengths and weaknesses of the multi-store model describe the working memory model and give strengths and weaknesses of this memory model describe the levels of processing approach and give strengths and weaknesses of this approach model. LEARNING SUMMARY • An empirical study is a study (such as an experiment or an interview) where data has been collected through direct observation or experience. U1 U2 Memory is the process by which we encode, store and retrieve information. It includes sensory memory, shortterm memory and longterm memory. Atkinson and Shriffrin (1968) proposed the multi-store model of memory. The model identifies three stores (see diagram below) and explains how information is transferred between these stores. Information enters sensory memory (SM). If attention is paid to the information, it is transferred to short-term memory (STM) where it is stored briefly. Verbal rehearsal maintains information in STM. Continued rehearsal creates long-term memories (LTM). Material may be retrieved from LTM via STM for recall or further processing. An example of a stimulus used in Sperling’s study: 7 I V F X L 5 3 B 4 W 7 MULTI-STORE MODEL The multi-store model of memory aims to describe the structure of memory and explain how information is transferred from STM to LTM. rehearsal sensory memory attention short-term memory transfer long-term memory retrieval A familiar example of sensory memory is when you still ‘hear’ information 1 or 2 seconds after it was first heard. The information is very briefly held in sensory memory. This explains why sometimes, when you say ‘Pardon?’, you are simultaneously aware of what was said. People have poor recall of events from childhood (called infantile amnesia) but particularly good recall for events from adolescence and early adulthood (reminiscence bump) and also for events from the last few years (recency effect). 24 retrieval In these studies of memory, what kind of LTM memory is being tested – semantic, procedural or episodic? How does this affect the ecological validity of the findings? The structure of memory 1. Sensory memory (SM) – The sensory form of a stimulus remains unaltered in the mind for a brief time. This could be an auditory or visual trace. It is rapidly lost through spontaneous decay (i.e. the physical trace disappears). 2. Short-term memory (STM) – Information receives minimal processing. It is relatively limited in capacity (it holds about seven items) and rapidly decays unless it is maintained through rehearsal. It may be held in a visual or auditory form (code), though it is mainly the latter. 3. Long-term memory (LTM) – Relatively permanent storage that has unlimited capacity. Different kinds of long-term memory have been identified: • Procedural memory – knowing how. Our knowledge of how to do things, and skills such as riding a bicycle. • Declarative memory – knowing that. Memory for specific information or facts, which is subdivided into semantic or episodic memory. Research evidence related to the MSM The evidence for separate memory stores comes from empirical studies of duration, capacity and encoding. Evidence also comes from the serial position effect, case studies of brain damage, brain scans and understanding of forgetting. 1. Duration Duration refers to how long a memory lasts in storage. Short-term memories last a short time – between 15 and 30 seconds, if not rehearsed. Long-term memories may last forever. The multi-store model (MSM) AQA A AQA B Semantic memory – storage for language, other cognitive concepts and general knowledge. It is well organised, usually is not forgotten, and does not disappear in cases of amnesia. It is this kind of memory that is tested in experimental work. Episodic memory (also called autobiographical memory) – memory for personal events and people, i.e. the episodes of your life. Episodic memory is reconstructed as an evolving process of a person’s history. A person’s episodic memory may not be reliable because of memory distortions. Miller suggested that the magic number 7±2 might explain why things so often come in sevens – like seven days in the week or seven wonders of the world. Think of your postcode. How many bits of information are there in it? How many chunks of information are there? Phone numbers are also presented in chunks, for example, 0181 654 3462. • Sensory memory (SM) – Sperling (1960) presented a display (like the one on the left) to trained participants for 50 milliseconds. After the display was presented, participants were told to report the whole array or just one row. In the whole array condition, they typically recalled 4 out of 12 items (about 33% recall). In the one row condition participants could recall 3 out of 4 items (75% recall), suggesting information decays quickly after presentation. • Short-term memory (STM) – Peterson and Peterson (1959) studied the recall of trigrams (i.e. consonant triplets of letters that had no meaning). If participants had to wait three seconds before recalling the trigram they could remember 80%. But if they had to wait 18 seconds, recall was reduced to 10%. (Participants did an interference task – counting backwards – between presentation and recall to prevent rehearsal). • Long-term memory (LTM) – Bahrick et al. (1975) demonstrated the existence of very-long-term-memories (VLTM). Nearly 400 adults of various ages were shown photographs from their high school yearbooks, and were asked to identify individuals. Even after 34 years, ex-students were still able to name 90% of their classmates. This shows that people have accurate VLTMs. 2. Capacity Capacity refers to how much can be held in a memory store. STM has a very small capacity, whereas LTM is potentially unlimited. • STM – Miller (1956) suggested that the span of STM is limited, not by the bits of information, but by the chunks; people can remember the same number of 10-letter words as 5-letter ones. The number of chunks that can be remembered is 7±2 (i.e. between 5 and 9). However, Simon (1974) found that there is a limit beyond which, chunk size does have an effect: participants had a shorter memory span for larger chunks (e.g. 8-word phrases) than for smaller chunks. Chunking relies on LTM in order to determine meaningfulness. Bower and Springston (1970) showed that participants recalled meaningful chunks (e.g. FBI PHD TWA) better than they recalled meaningless chunks (e.g. FB IPH DWT A). Recent research (Cowan et al., 2001) suggests that STM may actually be limited to 4, rather than 7, chunks. 25 Cognitive psychology Cognitive psychology • LTM – Merkle (1988) estimated (using the number of synapses) that LTM may have a capacity of between one thousand and one million gigabytes. 3. Encoding Encoding describes the form or code used to store data in memory. This may be based on the sound of the information (an acoustic code), the way the information appears (a visual code) or may be in terms of meaning (a semantic code). STM tends to be stored acoustically, whereas LTM is more semantic. Some examples of the word lists used by Baddeley: Acoustically similar: cap, cab, can, mad Acoustically dissimilar: pit, few, cod Semantically similar: great, large, broad Semantically dissimilar: good, huge, hot • Conrad (1964) found that participants made mistakes when recalling words that sounded similar (acoustic similarity), when recall was immediate (i.e. when testing STM). • Baddeley (1966) investigated both STM and LTM by giving participants lists of words that were acoustically similar or dissimilar, or semantically similar or dissimilar. It is not reasonable to generalise from a sample of one person. But, other studies have also supported the importance of the hippocampus. For example, Baddeley (1990) described the same symptoms in a man, Clive Wearing, whose hippocampus was damaged by infection. In an experiment testing STM, he found that participants who were given words that were acoustically similar recalled about 55% of the words, compared to 75% recall in the other three conditions. In the experiment testing LTM, it was found that participants given words that were semantically similar again recalled about 55% of the words compared to 75% recall in the other three conditions. 4. Serial position effect Are the first words that are heard recalled best (primacy effect)? Or are the words that are heard most recently recalled best (recency effect)? Maguire et al. (2000) used MRI scans to show that taxi drivers in London, who use their memory in their work, have larger hippocampi than non-taxi drivers. Beardsley et al. (1997) showed that the pre-frontal cortex of the brain was active when participants were involved in an STM task. The hippocampus is a small structure found deep inside the brain. There is one in each hemisphere of the forebrain. Glanzer and Cunitz (1966) asked participants to recall word lists. If this was done immediately, there was both a primacy effect and a recency effect (early and later words were better recalled) due to STM and LTM effects. If there was a delay of 10 seconds or more, there was only a primacy effect – LTM alone was affected. 3 6 2 9 8 5 1 4 7 3 Above is a string of digits. Cover up all the digits except the first four and say the digits, then shut your eyes and recall them. Were you right? Now try it with five digits. Keep going until you don’t get them right. This is called the ‘digit span technique’ and is a way to assess the capacity of a person’s short-term memory. 26 Hippocampi in the brain Primacy is due to the fact that the first items are more likely to have entered LTM. Recency occurs because the last items on the list are still in STM. 5. Brain damage Brain injury to specific areas can affect STM. Shallice and Warrington (1970) studied K.F., who experienced memory losses after a motorbike accident. He performed poorly on STM tasks (e.g. digit span) but his LTM was intact. Brain injury may also affect LTM. In anterograde amnesia, permanent memories remain intact but sufferers cannot remember any new information for more than the normal STM span. This is probably because transfer from STM to LTM is lost. This case study suggests that the hippocampus may function as a memory ‘gateway’ through which new memories pass before being permanently stored in LTM. 6. Brain scans A more recent method of studying brain activity is the use of brain scans (see page 114). CAT (computerised axial tomography), MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) and PET (positron emission tomography) scans all produce images of the brain in action. These findings suggest that in STM, information tends to be acoustically coded (and that is why acoustically similar words were muddled up) and in LTM, information tends to be semantically coded (and that is why words with similar meanings tended to be muddled up). Other research has found that STM does not always use an acoustic code. This depends on whether verbal rehearsal is prevented and/or whether recall is tested in an acoustic manner. For example, Brandimonte et al. (1992) found that participants used visual encoding in STM if they were given pictures to remember (a visual task). Verbal rehearsal was prevented (they had to say ‘la la la’) and they were asked to recall the items by drawing them (a visual recall task). The serial position effect refers to whether the position of words in a list affects the likelihood of recall. What are some of the weaknesses with using evidence from braindamaged individuals? Examples include Korsakoff’s syndrome, which is due to severe alcohol poisoning, and the case study of H.M. (Milner, 1959). H.M. had an operation to remove both hippocampi from his brain to alleviate his severe epilepsy. H.M.’s personality and intellect remained intact, but his memory was affected. He suffered extensive anterograde amnesia, so he had no memory for events after the operation. His memory for events prior to the operation was reasonable, but not as good as it had been. He could still talk and recall all the skills he knew previously (semantic memory) but his memory did not incorporate new experiences. For many years he reported that his age was 27 and that the year was 1953. After a while he realised this was absurd and tried guessing. In other words, he tried to ‘reconstruct’ memories. He watched the news every night yet he had no recall for major events. He happily re-read magazines with no loss of interest. He couldn’t memorise lists of words or recall faces of people he met. 7. Forgetting Explanations for forgetting are different for STM and LTM. See pages 39–42. The scientific method revolves around theorytesting. Scientists produce theories to explain events in the world. In order to test the validity of such theories, research is conducted. A ‘good’ theory should be easy to test, i.e. experiments can be designed to test the specific predictions of the theory. Evaluation of the MSM Strengths • The distinction between STM and LTM is well-supported by research evidence (see pages 25-27). • The MSM has encouraged psychologists to conduct empirical research to test the model, which has increased our understanding of memory. • The MSM has encouraged other psychologists to provide alternative explanations of memory. 27 Cognitive psychology The MSM is no longer regarded as an adequate representation of memory processes. However, historically, it is an important theory. Cognitive psychology Weaknesses • The model is over-simplified – Research has since shown that there are more than three memory stores and more than two processes involved (see evidence on page 30). • More memory stores – As we have already seen, LTM is divided into several different stores (procedural, declarative, episodic and semantic). The working memory model (see page 29) identifies subdivisions within STM. • Processes – The levels of processing approach (see page 32) suggests that lasting memories are created through elaborative processing, rather than simply verbal or maintenance rehearsal, as suggested by the MSM. • STM and LTM are not as separate as the MSM suggests. For example, chunking requires that LTM is accessed in order to establish the meaning of the chunks. • The MSM presents a passive view of memory and cannot account for active processes such as reconstruction, that is, when memories are altered because of expectations (see accuracy of eyewitness testimony on page 35). • Validity – The research studies have tended to concern only semantic memory and, therefore, the results and the MSM may be relevant only to this kind of memory rather than explaining, for example, memory for riding a bicycle or memory for events in the past. The working memory (WM) model AQA A AQA B U1 U2 Baddeley and Hitch (1974) suggested that short-term memory should be subdivided. They used the phrase ‘working memory’ instead of short-term memory to reflect their view that this is the area of memory that is active when you are working on information. Working memory consists of the following parts: sensory input (from world around us) visuo–spatial sketchpad central executive (visual & spatial info.) phonological loop (acoustic & verbal info.) Progress check 1 Identify six distinguishing features of the MSM. 2 STM and LTM differ in terms of duration. Name one study that illustrates this. 3 List the six ways that STM can be distinguished from LTM. 4 Outline two weaknesses with the research evidence for the MSM. episodic buffer visual store long-term memory acoustic store 4 E.g. Only one kind of memory is usually tested (semantic); Experiments may use artificial stimuli; In studies of brain damage we cannot be sure of the cause of observed behaviours; Case studies cannot be generalised. 3 Capacity; Encoding; Serial position effect; Brain damage; Brain scans; Forgetting. 2 E.g. Peterson and Peterson; Sperling; Bahrick et al. 1 E.g. SM; Attention; STM; Rehearsal; LTM; Based on stores and processes. The working memory model The structure of working memory 1. Central executive – This component of working memory is modality-free (i.e. not visual or auditory). It is roughly the same as attention (the concentration of mental effort on sensory or mental events). The central executive allocates resources to other components. 2. Visuo-spatial sketchpad (or scratchpad) – This part is used to hold visual memories, such as people’s faces. The phonological store allows an individual to rehearse information acoustically. 3. Phonological loop – This deals with verbal material and preserves the order of information. Information passes round and round in a loop as it is rehearsed. The phonological loop consists of… • a phonological store, which allows an individual to rehearse information acoustically – a kind of inner ear • an articulatory process, which is used for words that are heard or seen – a kind of inner voice. 4. Episodic buffer – This has recently been added by Baddeley (2000) because there was nowhere for information that is both visual and acoustic to be stored. The episodic buffer integrates information from the central executive, the visuo-spatial sketchpad and the phonological loop. It has a limited capacity. 28 29