* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Polyandry and sex-specific gene expression
Lesbian sexual practices wikipedia , lookup
Human male sexuality wikipedia , lookup
History of human sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Human female sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Body odour and sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup
Rochdale child sex abuse ring wikipedia , lookup
Slut-shaming wikipedia , lookup
Sexual ethics wikipedia , lookup
Age disparity in sexual relationships wikipedia , lookup
Female promiscuity wikipedia , lookup
Human mating strategies wikipedia , lookup
Sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup
Designer baby wikipedia , lookup
Sexual reproduction wikipedia , lookup
Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 Polyandry and sex-specific gene expression Judith E. Mank1, Nina Wedell2 and David J. Hosken2 rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Review Cite this article: Mank JE, Wedell N, Hosken DJ. 2013 Polyandry and sex-specific gene expression. Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0047 One contribution of 14 to a Theme Issue ‘The polyandry revolution’. 1 Department of Genetics, Evolution and Environment, University College London, The Darwin Building, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK 2 Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Cornwall Campus, Tremough, Penryn TR10 9EZ, UK Polyandry is widespread in nature, and has important evolutionary consequences for the evolution of sexual dimorphism and sexual conflict. Although many of the phenotypic consequences of polyandry have been elucidated, our understanding of the impacts of polyandry and mating systems on the genome is in its infancy. Polyandry can intensify selection on sexual characters and generate more intense sexual conflict. This has consequences for sequence evolution, but also for sex-biased gene expression, which acts as a link between mating systems, sex-specific selection and the evolution of sexual dimorphism. We discuss this and the remarkable confluence of sexual-conflict theory and patterns of gene expression, while also making predictions about transcription patterns, mating systems and sexual conflict. Gene expression is a key link in the genotype– phenotype chain, and although in its early stages, understanding the sexual selection –transcription relationship will provide significant insights into this critical association. Subject Areas: evolution, genomics Keywords: sexual conflict, sexual selection, gene expression evolution, mating system Author for correspondence: Judith E. Mank e-mail: [email protected] 1. The role of mating and mating system in gene expression Gene expression represents an important step in the genome–phenotype relationship in both the short- and long-term. Over the short-term, expression of many genes is a dynamic response to stimuli, and this is the way that the static and predefined genome of an individual interacts with changing environments and ecologies over the course of a single lifetime [1,2]. Gene expression also evolves over time [1,3–5], and adaptive changes in gene expression represent an important route to phenotypic evolution [6 –8]. Mating system largely defines sex-specific selection, which in turn affects gene expression patterns in males and females in several different ways. Differences in mating system can generate different intensities of sexual selection and sex-specific selection, which in turn can generate sexual conflict when males and females experience different fitness optima for shared traits. It is clear that sexual conflict often plays out at the genetic level through gene expression differences between males and females [9,10], as described in table 1. Conflict can also lead to parent-of-origin genomic imprinting [19,22] when antagonism between the parents over growth and provisioning plays out through the developing offspring. Mating system and sex-specific selection also play a major role in the evolution of sexually dimorphic and sexually selected phenotypes, and many sexually dimorphic traits are encoded by different expression levels of the same genes rather than by different genes in males and females [12,23,24], a phenomenon referred to as sex-biased gene expression. In some extreme cases, expression may be completely absent in one sex leading to sex-limited gene expression; however, this is relatively rare for genes not linked to sex-limited Y or W chromosomes [14]. In addition to expressing different amounts of the same gene, females and males may express different structural arrangements of the same gene, splicing or skipping different exons to create different protein isoforms [16]. Sex-specific alternate splicing is common in many animals, and is key to the sex determination pathway in many insects [15,25,26]. & 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 Table 1. Mechanisms of gene expression differences between males and females. 2 manifestation prevalence mechanism sex-biased genes expressed in both very common, may encompass .50% of sex-biased genes can be regulated by the gene expression sex-limited gene females and males, but at different levels genes expressed only in one sex expression splicing imprinting all Y-linked and W-linked genes are by definition sex-specific; however, sex-limited expression is rare aside from these chromosomes [14] males and females express the same genes, but alter exon order or skip different exons either the paternal or sex determination pathway [12] or sexhormone receptors [13] for loci present in both sexes, regulation by Y or W loci could produce sexlimited expression key element in the sex determination can occur when splicing is regulated by pathway in some insects [15]; also prevalent in mammalian sex determination pathway or by sexhormone receptors transcriptomes [16] involved in sex determination pathway in presumably, methylation patterns in the maternal allele is some insects [17], some medical parental gametes are maintained in the expressed in the offspring conditions [18]; may be common in mammals [19,20] but this remains zygote; alternatively, imprinting requires some sort of sensing contentious [21] mechanism to identify parent of origin Here, we examine how mating and mating system, with particular emphasis on polyandry, affect gene expression. Our ultimate aim is to understand the relationship between the genome, the transcriptome (the sum output of all expressed genes in an individual) and the phenotype. The complete attainment of this goal is somewhat distant, as the connections linking genome, transcriptome and phenotype remain hazy in many ways; however, initial results have suggested that gene expression studies may be a powerful way to study the effects of sexual conflict and sex-specific selection within the genome. We therefore also extrapolate from phenotypic data in order to predict how polyandry might affect gene expression evolution. 2. Short-term gene expression dynamics in response to courtship and mating Gene expression is the primary route by which the genome of an individual responds to environmental, ecological and behavioural stimuli, and so it is perhaps not at all surprising that mating behaviours and mating itself have large effects on gene expression. The crucial nature of reproduction, both in terms of physiology and evolutionary fitness, suggests that it should factor heavily in gene expression dynamics. Reproductive maturity is associated with a large number of gene expression changes in both sexes [27–29]. However, sexual maturity is quite a different physiological state than actual reproduction, particularly for females in internally fertilizing species that must assemble either fertilized ova into layableeggs or provision developing embryos. Because courtship and mating always precede reproduction, these can be thought of as cues that initiate the final physiological changes required for successful breeding. From this perspective, courtship- and mating-induced changes in gene expression are the route by which an individual begins to transition physiologically from adult to parent. There are multiple targets and mechanisms by which courtship behaviour and mating induce transcriptomic changes, and although most of the work in this regard involves Drosophila, findings there have been corroborated, albeit with limited evidence from other animals. There are distinct changes in gene expression that occur in the brain of males and females related to mating, and these can be divided into changes relating to mating behaviours and changes arising owing to mating itself. Visual, auditory and other courtship cues are alone sufficient to cause gene expression changes in the female brain, as observed in both Drosophila melanogaster [30] and songbirds [27,31]. In many ways, this mirrors recently identified changes in cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) bouquets that occur as a consequence of variation in the social environment and mating status of male D. melanogaster. CHCs are complex sexual signals in Drosophila [32], and these plastic expression responses mean that trait values depend not just on the genotype of focal individuals, but also on the genes being expressed in individuals that make up the social environment [33,34]. Mating itself causes further gene expression changes in both male and female brains [35–37]. In all cases, these transcriptomic changes are subtle, and transcriptomic studies in a range of species suggest sex differences in the brain are relatively modest [13,38 –40]. This may be because small changes are sufficient to cause large behavioural differences between the sexes. Alternatively, behaviours are genetically controlled within relatively confined areas of the brain, and because these regions are difficult to isolate, studies that have used whole-brain homogenates almost certainly underestimate local expression differences [4,40]. Beyond the brain, mating elicits a huge response in gene expression in the female reproductive tract [37,41,42]. Part of this is due to the female’s innate transcriptomic preparations for embryo provisioning [43]. In addition to this, the female reproductive system stages an immune response Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 alternative genes in the genome [11] rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org type Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 Specific mating systems are associated with different intensities of sex-specific selection. Selection can alter gene expression or coding sequence to enact phenotypic change [8,48], and if the effect of sex-specific selection in coding sequence is anything to go by [49], then sex-specific selection should also act on gene expression of loci involved in sexual selection and reproduction. Polyandrous mating systems can experience increased levels of male-specific selection, particularly in regard to sperm competition, and this may act on the spermatogenesis transcriptome in several different ways. Gene expression differences in males under monandrous versus polyandrous mating systems are largely unknown, but we can extrapolate somewhat from the potential for male-specific selection. Given that polyandry is frequently associated with increased sperm competition, we might expect some transcriptomic shift from the production of somatic sexually selected indicators of male quality in monandrous systems [50] to a greater investment in sperm production in order to win fertilizations in polyandrous systems [51,52]. This would manifest as higher expression levels over evolutionary time for genes involved in spermatogenesis, as the sperm production and assembly mechanisms increase their output in polyandrous versus monandrous systems. This is not to say that males in polyandrous species will forgo investment in sexually selected somatic traits that function in mate choice and competition, but they also face post-mating competition that requires transcriptional investment to win. There are two general ways this can be achieved, either by trade-offs between sexual traits, or by trading-off sexual traits against other characters. As males must mate to obtain fitness, we would expect the trade-off to be with non-sexual traits, and hence polyandrous systems might display reductions in gene expression levels that have little obvious connection with sexual fitness. This is consistent with experimental evolution studies in yellow dung flies 3 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 3. Long-term gene expression evolution in response to mating system showing that higher investment in sexual characters such as testis size under polyandry comes at a cost to immune function [53 –55]. We might also expect elevated levels of purifying selection on both the gene sequence and gene expression patterns related to sperm production [56,57]. In mating systems with no or very little sperm competition, males need only produce a few viable sperm for each fertilization event. When the sperm competition that results from polyandry conforms to a lottery, where sperm are tickets and fertilizations the prize, males will not only be selected to produce more sperm, but they could also be under selection to produce more high quality sperm to ensure they successfully fertilize ova [58– 60]. This is clear from experimental evolution studies in mice that show an increase in sperm quality under polyandrous conditions compared with monogamy [61]. This increase in sperm quality may manifest in gene expression as lower transcriptional variance for spermatogenesis genes, as variance can be taken as a measure of the strength of purifying selection on gene expression [4]. The haploid nature of sperm offers the possibility that at least some genes associated with sperm function are subject to haploid selection [62]. This would make the effects of purifying selection associated with increased sperm competition even more powerful, especially for recessive or partially recessive variation in expression level. However, in metazoans, the scope for haploid selection appears to be relatively limited [63–65]. This is because DNA compaction during spermatogenesis means very few genes are expressed solely by the haploid sperm cell. Instead, expression more often seems to originate from the testis during sperm formation; therefore each sperm cell, although carrying a haploid genome, is composed of proteins from the diploid genotype, making the sperm population a mix of gene products from both alleles present in the testes. The effects of female-specific selection in gene expression in polyandrous systems are more difficult to predict. Polyandry may result in both post-copulatory and pre-copulatory mechanisms of female choice in some systems [59], adding transcriptomic mechanisms of female choice to the reproductive tract. In support of this, a recent study found significant differences in expression levels of ovary and brain genes in female D. melanogaster mated to either preferred or unpreferred males, indicating the involvement of both pre- and post-copulatory female choice mechanisms [66]. Beyond that, female-specific selection to reduce polyspermy may drive the evolution of gamete-recognition proteins on the ovum [67– 69]. As with males, polyandry could cause shifts in the focus of female choice to the post-copulatory arena at a cost to pre-copulatory mechanisms of choice, but selection could also favour reductions in investment elsewhere while retaining pre- and expanding post-copulatory choice mechanisms. This may also depend on how easy it is for females to avoid multiple mating when it is not in females’ interests. Again, experimental evolution has shown that females’ ability to circumvent male interests evolves under polyandry and that this comes at a cost to non-sexual immunological fitness [52,70]. Further support for the suggestion that females may divert their investment towards post-copulatory mechanisms comes from evidence that sperm storage can represent a cost to females, again in terms of compromised immune response. In Atta columbiana ants, for example, sperm storage is traded off rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org to the foreign proteins contained in the male’s seminal fluid [43– 45]. Finally, part of the changes in the female reproductive tract is in response to manipulative proteins from the male transcriptome in the form of accessory gland proteins (ACPs) [41,46,47], which stimulate female oogenesis and ovulation, as well as impede female remating [47]. ACPs appear to be involved in inter-locus sexual conflict, which involves interactions between antagonistic alleles at two or more loci in males and females, and so they are also discussed in §6. Most studies of gene expression in relation to mating compare mated and unmated individuals, and so therefore cannot differentiate the effects of monandry from polyandry in the female reproductive tract. Initial evidence suggests that female transcriptomic responses to multiple mating are not cumulative, rather the majority of changes occur with the transition from virgin to non-virgin, and additional matings after the first do little to change the female transcriptome further [45]. However, there may be a temporal aspect to polyandryinduced changes to the female transcriptome. Single-mated females experience a transcriptomic cycle extending for several hours after mating [42], and polyandrous females may experience this cycle multiple times. Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 Although polyandry can potentially reduce the variance in male reproductive success, and thereby reduce the intensity of sexual selection [73 –75], in many cases the evolution of polyandry also brings with it greater potential for interlocus and intra-locus sexual conflict between females and males compared with monogamy. This sexual conflict can play out between the parents directly as males and females seek to manipulate each other’s reproduction. Inter-locus conflict, which involves antagonistic alleles at multiple loci, includes maternal–foetal conflict over resource provisioning in species with internal gestation. Intra-locus conflict, where alleles at a single locus influence male and female fitness in opposite directions can affect a large proportion of the genome [76], and influences gene expression in several ways, ranging from transcriptional differences between the sexes [9,77– 79] to parent-of-origin imprinting in the developing offspring [80,81]. At this point, there are few direct connections between gene expression and sexual conflict, although it is becoming increasingly possible to test the existing predictions [77,81,82] with transcriptome data [19,78,79]. What is lacking most is the direct connection between transcriptome data and sex-specific phenotypes, but that will no doubt emerge in time. 5. Mechanisms of differential gene expression Phenotypes are ultimately composed of protein, and many phenotypic differences are attributable to differences in protein levels [8,48]. The same is true of sex-specific phenotypes, many of which are the result of differential gene expression between males and females, and ultimately many of these must result from conflicting selection between the sexes. Transcription of RNA is a direct predictor of protein translation levels, and because protein chemistry is much more difficult to work with than RNA chemistry, it is often easier to use RNA levels as a proxy for protein levels. Although the same nuclear genome is present in every somatic cell of an organism, the transcriptome is far more variable, and can vary throughout the body and over a life cycle. This is particularly true for sex-specific transcriptomes, which differ during development [29,83] and among different tissues in the body [84– 86]. Transcriptome studies must therefore target the correct part of the body and the correct developmental stage. Developmental stage is particularly important in this context, because although sexual dimorphisms and sexually selected phenotypes are most evident in adults, a significant proportion of these traits are the product of developmental 4 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 4. Polyandry and the effects of increased sexual conflict processes that amplify into adult phenotypes. For example, the horn in adult male horned beetles is the product of larval differences between the sexes that increase over development [23,87,88]. Similarly, the transcriptomic basis of the sword in male swordtails is largely complete by the time the sword manifests fully in the adult phenotype [24]. In cases such as these, it is crucial to sample the individual at the stage at which sexual dimorphisms are being programmed in the phenotype, rather than the adult stage when the dimorphism is simply most evident. There are several routes by which males and females can express the same gene in different ways and therefore achieve sex-specific phenotypes (table 1). Sex-biased expression is the most commonly assayed form of gene expression differences, and it is clear that many, if not most genes, are expressed at different levels between the sexes at some point in development or in some part of the body [89]. Aside from genes on sex-limited Y or W chromosomes, addressed later, relatively few of these differences are due to sex-limitation, where a gene is completely shut off in one sex or the other. Rather, most sex-biased gene expression is the product of different transcription rates of genes expressed to some degree in both males and females. The rarity of sex-limited expression may be due to the fact that selection to shut off expression progressively weakens as gene expression decreases and therefore produces little phenotypic effect. Theoretically, the difference in transcription between females and males is due to sexual conflict over optimal protein levels [9], and this conflict can be resolved when the correlation in expression between the sexes is broken down. It is, however, not always possible to resolve sexual conflict, especially when there are ontogenetic [90] or pleiotropic [91] constraints acting on a sexually antagonistic locus. It may be that in these cases, recent gene duplicates take on sex-specific expression patterns [92,93]. Gene duplication may therefore represent a route to the resolution of conflict over genes that perform multiple functions [82,92], but this may also require modifications to genetic architecture [94], in addition to dominance, recombination, sex-linkage and population size [82]. Although there are reasons to think that this might not be an easy or common solution to sexual conflict [95], gene expression studies, particularly of the sex chromosomes, have demonstrated that gene duplication, and duplicationmediated relocation, can provide a route for sexually antagonistic loci to transfer to regions of the genome with beneficial sex-specific selection regimes. The best illustrations come from the sex-limited W and Y chromosomes, which, due to their sex-limited nature, experience only femalespecific (in the case of the W) or male-specific (in the case of the Y) selection. Female-benefit genes that cause harm to males relocating to the W chromosome, or present on newly emergent W chromosomes, may experience the immediate resolution of this conflict as these genes are no longer expressed in males, and the same is true for malebenefit genes on Y chromosomes [96]. However, relocation to the Y or W chromosome, associated with loss of the original autosomal, X- or Z-linked locus, will resolve conflict only for some types of genes, particularly those with narrow sex-specific functions, and will not resolve conflicts over optimal expression for genes that function in both sexes. For the latter class of genes, resolution can occur via duplication to the Y and W chromosomes followed by sex-specific rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org against the ability to mount an immune response, and this effect is exacerbated when the ejaculates from different males are stored, possibly indicating an increased cost of storing and processing genetically different ejaculates [71]. In Drosophila pseudoobscura, a species with polymorphic sperm, nonfertile sperm function to protect the fertile sperm against female-mediated sperm death [72]. Female spermicide in turn may have evolved as a mechanism to reduce the cost of storing sperm from several males. Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 (a) (b) relocation 5 duplication rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org autosomes X or Z chromosome Y or W chromosome autosomes X or Z chromosome Figure 1. Duplication or relocation to the Y or W chromosome can resolve sexual conflict. (a) For genes that do not affect phenotypes in both sexes, relocation to the Y or W chromosome leads to sex-limited expression, there is no mutation accumulation in the non-expressing sex, and expression from the parent locus ceases after relocation. (b) In those cases where a sexually antagonistic gene is involved in both male and female phenotypes, duplication to the Y or W chromosome can lead to sex-specific divergence of the daughter locus and the parent locus can be retained. It is currently not clear, however, how long it takes for gene associations (genetic architecture) involving the parent gene to disconnect from the daughter locus. specialization of the W- or Y-linked daughter locus, in concert with retention of the parental copy (figure 1). This route may be more common than relocation for genes with many network connections or multiple functions. Selection for sexual conflict resolution may drive the evolution and turnover of sex chromosome systems [97,98], as well as gene gain by existing sex chromosomes, as evidenced by the fact that the rate of gene acquisition on the Drosophila Y chromosome is nearly ten times greater than the rate of gene loss [99]. We discuss the more complex pattern expected and observed for Z and X chromosomes in §7. It is not clear at this point how common sexual conflict is at the level of the exon versus the entire transcribed gene; however, there is growing evidence that males and females do not just express genes at different levels, but they also splice many genes differently, combining exons in different order or combinations. Sex-specific alternate splicing is common for many genes [16], and although alternate splicing is key to the sex determination pathway in many insects [15,25,26], it is not yet clear how alternative splicing contributes to somatic phenotypic sexual dimorphisms or sexual conflict in a broader sense. Genomic imprinting occurs when maternally or paternally inherited copies of a gene are expressed differently in the offspring, presumably due to methylation of genes or other regulatory control. Imprinting of single loci is important in the sex determination pathways of some haplodiploid insects [17,100], and recent assessments have suggested that imprinting affects a large proportion of mammalian genes [19,20], although the veracity of these results is currently under intense debate [21]. If this mechanism is common, it may represent a means of resolving sexual conflict that plays out in the developing offspring [81]. 6. Inter-locus sexual conflict Inter-locus sexual conflict can generate an arms race of adaptation and counter-adaptation in males and females that can result in severe physiological costs [101], as well as spurring phenotypic evolution [102]. Inter-locus sexual conflict is somewhat difficult to study from a gene expression perspective without some prior knowledge of the loci involved, although work on ACPs and imprinted loci provide some ideas about how the transcriptome may be affected. It may be that increasing levels of sexual conflict under polyandry mean that a greater proportion of loci become entangled in inter-locus arms races between males and females compared with monogamy. However, it may be some time yet before a comprehensive catalogue of the genomic distribution of loci involved in this type of conflict is available. In the meantime, there are several illustrative examples. Given their role in sexual conflict, the genes encoding ACPs represent potential loci for male benefits in the sexually antagonistic coevolution that can be associated with polyandry. When present in an ejaculate, ACPs act to manipulate female reproduction in the male’s favour and impede female remating [47]. They are therefore subject to particularly strong male-specific selection, which may explain why ACPs are among the most rapidly evolving proteins in the genome [103,104]. Females show a profound transcriptomic response to ACPs [41,46,47], and female-specific selection could act to make females resistant to male manipulation that is harmful to female fitness. Because of this, we might predict that ACPs would evolve more rapidly under polyandry than both monandry and monogamy. Although polyandry is associated with increased rates of evolution of seminal fluid proteins in primates [105], this pattern is not Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 Y or W chromosome Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 Intra-locus sexual conflict occurs when an allele at a single locus moves one sex away from its fitness optima but the other sex towards its sex-specific fitness peak [116], and has been documented in many traits and species [117]. However, while inter-locus sexual conflict can produce unresolvable arms races, the potential for resolution may be greater for 6 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 7. Intra-locus sexual conflict and the evolution of sex-biased gene expression intra-locus sexual conflict. Resolution of intra-locus sexual conflict was thought to occur when the genetic correlation between males and females was broken down, allowing for the evolution of phenotypic sexual dimorphism [77,118]. However, it has become apparent that things are not as straightforward as this [119], primarily because genes do not exist in isolation of other genes. The network and interactive structure of genetic pathways means that selection on a sex-limited gene can still ripple through the genome, and mutations will still accumulate in the sex lacking expression because they are neutral, and these could keep the expressing sex from reaching a fitness optimum [81,94]. For example, recent artificial selection work on the broad-horned flour beetle has shown how selection on a male-limited trait alters female fitness, because the male sex-limited trait is genetically correlated to other traits, and these in turn are genetically correlated between the sexes [90]. As a result, selecting on the male-limit trait, the mandibles, reverberates throughout the genetic network, ultimately affecting female fitness, and although females never develop mandibles, it is this sex-limited trait that lies at the heart of the sexual conflict. Intra-locus sexual conflict over gene expression may therefore not be resolved simply by the evolution of sex-biased or sex-limited gene expression. Furthermore, while it is now possible to measure transcriptomic differences between the sexes to identify a measure of intra-locus sexual conflict within the genome [78], it is not clear whether these differences completely or only partially resolve conflicts. Despite these concerns, sex-biased gene expression represents possibly the most direct connection linking sexual conflict, mating system and the evolution of sexual dimorphism with the genome, and although work on this front has been possible for little more than a decade, it is surprising how well sex-biased gene expression patterns conform to the predictions from intra-locus sexual conflict theory. For example, the greatest degree of sex-bias is observed in gonad transcriptomes [85,86,89], as is expected given the profound degree of conflict between optimal male and female gamete production and provisioning strategies. Male and female gonad formation and gametogenesis involve many of the same genes used in different ways, and this makes the testis and the ovary the site of maximal sexual conflict within the body. Sex-biased genes, as the encoding loci underlying sex-specific phenotypes, experience the accelerated rates of evolution predicted by sexual selection [11,120,121]. Furthermore, gene expression patterns on the X and Z sex chromosomes show evidence of sex-specific selection [10,79,122], as predicted by their uneven inheritance between males and females [77,123]. Although the pattern is complicated by the effects of sex chromosome regulation related to meiotic sex chromosome inactivation and dosage compensation [122], a general pattern emerges. X chromosomes are more often present in females than males, and therefore more often selected for female-specific effects. This has been tested with gene expression data with the assumption that female-biased genes benefit females and male-biased genes benefit males. Under this framework, the finding that the X chromosomes in both the mouse and Drosophila harbour a combination of excessive female-biased genes and fewer than expected male-biased genes [124] fits with sexual conflict predictions. These observations are supported by a recent population genetic model, showing that female-biased gene duplications preferentially accumulate on the X chromosome [77]. rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org universal, and polyandry does not explain rates of sequence evolution for ACPs in Heliconius butterflies [106,107]. It may be that polyandry does not affect sequence evolution as much as expression level of ACPs because increasing female resistance may select for increased ACP dosage. Alternatively, inter-locus sexual conflict may result in high turnover of ACPs. This would occur if males do not refine their existing toolkit with which they try to manipulate females, but instead exploit entirely new mechanisms that result in male fitness benefit and female cost. This may explain the vast number of ACPs in D. melanogaster (more than 100). Finally, it may be that other aspects of the ejaculate are responsible for suppressing female receptivity. In butterflies, for example, non-fertile sperm inhibit female remating by filling the female’s spermstorage organ [108], and male harming of females during copulation is in theory another way to suppress remating [109]. Finally, ACPs may also be beneficial to females when acting as nutrient provisioning. This can still generate sexual conflict over female receptivity, and there is evidence in butterflies that there is extensive genetic variation and genetic correlations between the sexes for ability to suppress female receptivity in males and to withstand manipulation in females [110], although whether this is due to differences in expression level or gene sequences remains to be examined. The increased level of inter-locus sexual conflict associated with polyandry may in some cases target alleles based on their parent of origin. There is a large body of evolutionary theory that predicts that sexual conflict will foster genomic imprinting [80,81,111–113], where either the maternal or paternal allele at a given locus is expressed in the offspring rather than normal bi-allelic expression. Imprinting of this form allows conflict between the parents to play out in the offspring, and has been shown to be particularly important in foetal growth [18] and murine mating behaviour [114], and underlie sex determination in scale insects [115]. Given the increased potential for inter-locus sexual conflict under polyandry, we might expect to observe a greater proportion of imprinted loci within the genome in polyandrous lineages compared with monogamous species. Until recently, however, surveying whole genomes for imprinted loci was difficult, and although the recent development of methods to scan for allelic imbalance in transcriptomic data may facilitate genome-wide surveys [19,20], this method is not without serious concern [21]. Additionally, there are methods that search RNA data for imprinting by looking for any statistical deviation from bi-allelic expression no matter how slight, and it is unclear how much of this effect is due to variation in cis-regulatory regions that affect expression versus conflict-driven selection for parent-of-origin imprinting. Most importantly, it is not clear how biologically relevant slight deviations from bi-allelic expression are, or how they might manifest phenotypically. Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 8. Conclusions The recent exponential advances in sequencing capability and reduced cost make the genome and the transcriptome approachable scientific frontiers for the study of model and non-model organisms alike. At the moment, there is a 7 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 greater magnitudes of expression difference. Whether greater levels of sexual dimorphism are due to more sexbiased genes or a few genes with greater magnitude of sex-bias depends on how much of the genome is subject to sexual conflict, and to a lesser extent, how much conflict can be resolved via differential expression levels. Similar predictions might be expected to hold for sex-specific alternate splicing, as this is really an exon-specific form of sex-biased gene expression. If the elevated rates of sequence evolution observed for sexbiased genes [89,120,121] really are the product of sex-specific selection, then we might expect the relative rates of evolution for sex-biased genes to be higher in polyandrous than monogamous systems. Relative rates of evolution for sex-biased genes under different mating systems may need to be determined in adults and embryos separately, as evidence from birds suggests that male-specific selection is strongest on adultexpressed genes while female-specific selection acts maximally on late-embryonic-expressed genes [29]. This is also consistent with studies at the phenotypic level in Drosophila where there is evidence of ontogenetic conflict between males and females [132]. Additionally, if sex chromosomes really are a hotspot of sexually antagonistic selection [10,77,123], then the increased conflict that results from those forms of polyandry might be expected to feminize X chromosomes and masculinize Z chromosomes to a greater degree than in monogamous or monandrous species. If this is true, it would potentially be demonstrated via greater levels of female-biased expression on X chromosomes and male-biased expression on Z chromosomes, and faster rates of gene exodus for malebiased genes from the X and female-biased genes from the Z chromosomes. At this point, these predictions are simple hand-waving based on vague assumptions, but hopefully they will soon be tested critically by studies that span clades with varying mating systems. A phylogenetic approach will also help determine the time-scale of sex-specific transcriptome evolution. Shortterm sex-specific experimental evolution in Drosophila produced little change in sex-biased expression [78]; however, sex-bias varies in both domestic animal breeds that have been subject to sex-specific selection regimes for more than 100 generations [14], and among closely related species [131]. This suggests that transcriptional evolution can occur for many genes over medium- and long-term evolutionary distances, and a phylogenetic study of closely related species with different mating systems will help quantify the rate of sex-specific transcriptomic evolution in response to defined sex-specific selection. This will help answer questions regarding the response mechanisms as well. For example, does the elevated level of sexual conflict in polyandrous systems mean that sex-biased expression evolves more rapidly than in monogamous species? Does polygamy produce more sex-biased genes, or does it produce a greater magnitude of sex-bias in a relatively small number of genes? rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Several objections to the approach of employing sexbiased gene expression to study sexual conflict have been voiced [94,125], including potential limitations of sex-specific regulatory mechanisms and differential male and female allometry. The first concern arises because very few genes lie directly in the sex determination pathway or are proximately located to sex-hormone receptors. This might suggest that there is very little scope for sex-specific gene regulation. However, there is clear evidence that sexual dimorphism of somatic traits can result when gene expression of the trait is ultimately regulated by the sex determination pathway [12,126,127]. Additionally, there are several thousand oestrogen and testosterone receptors in the average metazoan genome [128,129], and the interactive nature of the genetic network implies that even if only a few hundred genes are under direct control of sex hormones, they can transmit sexbiased expression through to thousands of other genes under their regulatory control. This suggests that the potential for sex-specific gene regulation is extensive, if not limitless. It also suggests that the complete resolution of sexual conflict may be difficult because of these extensive gene networks. Additionally, initial studies of sex-biased gene expression were performed on whole animals [120,121], leading to concerns that sex-biased expression was a result of different sex-specific allometry of constituent body parts. However, the opposite has been observed, as whole-body and aggregate transcriptome studies actually underestimate the degree of sex-bias within constituent parts [40,85,86]. This is particularly problematic for studies of brain transcription: although fine scale analysis of gene expression in the brain has revealed profound sex-bias in limited areas that control sex-specific behaviours [130], whole-brain or partial-brain homogenates reveal very little overall sex-bias [39,40] because sex-specific expression of different genes in different regions is averaged, and localized differences are therefore diluted. This suggests that the initial estimates of whole-body sexbias in roughly half of all genes are in fact underestimates, rather than overestimates, and that finer-scale dissections will reveal more pervasive patterns of local sex-biased gene expression. With these concerns allayed, we can now move on to discussing how polyandry influences sex-biased gene expression in light of intra-locus sexual conflict. The short answer is that we have no idea, and this is because no one has yet directly compared monogamous, monandrous and polyandrous species in a phylogenetically controlled study. At this point, we know that sex-biased expression varies among species [131], but this has not yet been put into the context of mating system. Simply doing so, ideally comparing related species with different mating systems, would be a big step. In the absence of any solid data, we can only speculate wildly. Sex-specific selection and sexual conflict may be stronger in polyandrous than monogamous systems. This is because even under polyandry, a few males more or less monopolize paternity. We therefore expect that phenotypic sexual dimorphism will either be greater in magnitude, or possibly just evolve more quickly, under polyandry compared with monogamy. If protein abundance, and therefore transcription level, holds the key to sexually dimorphic phenotypes, then we might expect either sex-biased gene expression to be prevalent in a larger proportion of genes under polyandry, or alternatively, a limited number of genes will show Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 sexual conflict, mating system and the evolution of sexual dimorphism with the genome. The rapid expansion of the research frontier in sexual selection and sexual conflict is exciting, and it creates opportunities for both theoretical frameworks for gene expression evolution, as well as empirical tests in a range of organisms with interesting mating systems. Stay tuned for forthcoming developments. References 1. Brawand D et al. 2011 The evolution of gene expression levels in mammalian organs. Nature 478, 343–345. (doi:10.1038/nature10532) 2. Tirosh I, Reikhav S, Levy AA, Barkai N. 2009 A yeast hybrid provides insight into the evolution of gene expression regulation. Science 324, 659 –662. (doi:10.1126/science.1169766) 3. Enard W et al. 2002 Intra- and interspecific variation in primate gene expression patterns. Science 296, 340–343. (doi:10.1126/science.1068996) 4. Harrison PW, Wright AE, Mank JE. 2012 The evolution of gene expression and the transcriptomephenotype relationship. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 23, 222–229. (doi:10.1016/j.semcdb.2011.12.004) 5. Khaitovich P, Hellmann I, Enard W, Nowick K, Leinweber M, Franz H, Weiss G, Lachmann M, Paabo S. 2005 Parallel patterns of evolution in the genomes and transcriptomes of humans and chimpanzees. Science 309, 1850 –1854. (doi:10.1126/science.1108296) 6. Carroll SB. 2008 Evo-devo and an expanding evolutionary synthesis: a genetic theory of morphological evolution. Cell 134, 25 –36. (doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.06.030) 7. Chan YF et al. 2010 Adaptive evolution of pelvic reduction in sticklebacks by recurrent deletion of a Pitx1 enhancer. Science 327, 302 –305. (doi:10.1126/science.1182213) 8. Manceau M, Domingues VS, Mallarino R, Hoekstra HE. 2011 The developmental role of agouti in color pattern evolution. Science 331, 1062 –1065. (doi:10.1126/science.1200684) 9. Connallon T, Knowles LL. 2005 Intergenomic conflict revealed by patterns of sex-biased gene expression. Trends Genet. 21, 495– 499. (doi:10.1016/j.tig.2005. 07.006) 10. Mank JE. 2009 Sex chromosomes and the evolution of sexual dimorphism: lessons from the genome. Am. Nat. 173, 141– 150. (doi:10.1086/595754) 11. Ellegren H, Parsch J. 2007 The evolution of sexbiased genes and sex-biased gene expression. Nat. Rev. Genet. 8, 689–698. (doi:10.1038/nrg2167) 12. Kopp A, Duncan I, Carroll SB. 2000 Genetic control and evolution of sexually dimorphic characters in 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. Drosophila. Nature 408, 553–559. (doi:10.1038/ 35046017) Reinius B, Saetre P, Leonard JA, Blekhman R, Merino-Martinez R, Gilad Y, Jazin E. 2008 An evolutionarily conserved sexual signature in the primate brain. PLoS Genet. 4, e1000100. (doi:10. 1371/journal.pgen.1000100) Moghadam HK, Pointer MA, Wright AE, Berlin S, Mank JE. 2012 W chromosome expression responds to female-specific selection. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA (doi:10.1073/pnas.1202721109) Bell LR, Maine EM, Schedl P, Cline TW. 1988 Sexlethal, a Drosophila sex determination switch gene, exhibits sex-specific RNA splicing and sequence similarity to RNA-binding proteins. Cell 55, 1037– 1046. (doi:10.1016/0092-8674(88)90248-6) Blekhman R, Marioni JC, Zumbo P, Stephens M, Gilad Y. 2010 Sex-specific and lineage-specific alternative splicing in primates. Genome Res. 20, 180 –189. (doi:10.1101/gr.099226.109) Verhulst EC, Beukeboom LW, van de Zande L. 2010 Maternal control of haplodiploid sex determination in the wasp Nasonia. Science 328, 620–623. (doi:10.1126/science.1185805) Reik W, Maher ER. 1997 Imprinting in clusters: lessons from Beckwith –Wiedemann syndrome. Trends Genet. 13, 330 –334. (doi:10.1016/S01689525(97)01200-6) Gregg C, Zhang JW, Butler JE, Haig D, Dulac C. 2010 Sex-specific parent-of-origin allelic expression in the mouse brain. Science 329, 682 –685. (doi:10.1126/ science.1190831) Gregg C, Zhang JW, Weissbourd B, Luo SJ, Schroth GP, Haig D, Dulac C. 2010 High-resolution analysis of parent-of-origin allelic expression in the mouse brain. Science 329, 643 –648. (doi:10.1126/ science.1190830) DeVeale B, van der Kooy D, Babak T. 2012 Critical evaluation of imprinted gene expression by RNAseq: a new perspective. PLoS Genet. 8, e1002600. (doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1002600) Reik W, Walter J. 2001 Genomic imprinting: parental influence on the genome. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2, 21 –32. (doi:10.1038/35047554) 23. Moczek AP. 2006 Pupal remodeling and the development and evolution of sexual dimorphism in horned beetles. Am. Nat. 168, 711–729. (doi:10.1086/509051) 24. Offen N, Blum N, Meyer A, Begemann G. 2008 Fgfr1 signalling in the development of a sexually selected trait in vertebrates, the sword of swordtail fish. BMC Dev. Biol. 8, 98. (doi:10.1186/ 1471-213X-8-98) 25. Beye M, Hasselmann M, Fondrk MK, Page RE, Omholt SW. 2003 The gene csd is the primary signal for sexual development in the honeybee and encodes an SR-type protein. Cell 114, 419 –429. (doi:10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00606-8) 26. Hasselmann M, Gempe T, Schiott M, Nunes-Silva CG, Otte M, Beye M. 2008 Evidence for the evolutionary nascence of a novel sex determination pathway in honeybees. Nature 454, 519–522. (doi:10.1038/ nature07052) 27. Avey MT, Phillmore LS, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. 2005 Immediate early gene expression following exposure to acoustic and visual components of courtship in zebra finches. Behav. Brain Res. 165, 247–253. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.002) 28. Kalinka AT, Varga KM, Gerrard DT, Preibisch S, Corcoran DL, Jarrells J, Ohler U, Bergman CM, Tomancak P. 2010 Gene expression divergence recapitulates the developmental hourglass model. Nature 468, 811– 814. (doi:10.1038/nature09634) 29. Mank JE, Nam K, Brunstrom B, Ellegren H. 2010 Ontogenetic complexity of sexual dimorphism and sex-specific selection. Mol. Biol. Evol. 27, 1570– 1578. (doi:10.1093/molbev/msq042) 30. Immonen E, Ritchie MG. 2012 The genomic response to courtship song stimulation in female Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 1359– 1365. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1644) 31. Mello CV, Vicario DS, Clayton DF. 1992 Song presentation induces gene expression in the songbird forebrain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 89, 6818– 6822. (doi:10.1073/pnas.89.15.6818) 32. Sharma MD, Hunt J, Hosken DJ. 2012 Antagonistic responses to natural and sexual selection and the sex-specific evolution of cuticular hydrocarbons in Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 Work in J.E.M.’s laboratory is supported by the BBSRC and the ERC (grant agreement no. 260233); N.W. by a Royal Society Wolfson Merit Award and NERC, and D.J.H. was supported by NERC. We thank the Editors for the invitation to participate in this special issue and the Royal Society for supporting the process. We also thank two anonymous referees for comments, which helped improve the manuscript. 8 rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org great deal of effort going into describing the natural history of the genome and transcriptome, and this is required before we can understand how these things interact to form the phenotype. Hopefully soon, this will progress to a more integrated grasp of the ways in which selection, acting on the phenotype, shapes the underling genes and gene expression patterns. The comparison of gene expression patterns associated with different mating systems provides a unique opportunity to begin to understand the evolution of sexual dimorphism, quantify the degree and loci of sexual conflict, and unravel the mechanisms by which this conflict can, or cannot be resolved. As such, gene expression differences between the sexes represent possibly the most direct connection linking Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 34. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. Firman RC, Simmons LW. 2010 Experimental evolution of sperm quality via postcopulatory sexual selection in house mice. Evolution 64, 1245–1256. 62. Joseph SB, Kirkpatrick M. 2004 Haploid selection in animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 592 –597. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.08.004) 63. Dorus S, Busby SA, Gerike U, Shabanowitz J, Hunt DF, Karr TL. 2006 Genomic and functional evolution of the Drosophila melanogaster sperm proteome. Nat. Genet. 38, 1440–1445. (doi:10.1038/ng1915) 64. Lenormand T, Dutheil J. 2005 Recombination difference between sexes: a role for haploid selection. PLoS Biol. 3, 396 –403. (doi:10.1371/ journal.pbio.0030396) 65. Pitnick S, Dobler R, Hosken DJ. 2009 Sperm length is not influenced by haploid gene expression in the flies Drosophila melanogaster and Scathophaga stercoraria. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 4029– 4034. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1208) 66. Bailey RI, Innocenti P, Morrow EH, Friberg U, Qvarnstrom A. 2011 Female Drosophila melanogaster gene expression and mate choice: the X chromosome harbours candidate genes underlying sexual isolation. PLoS ONE 6, e17358. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017358) 67. Berlin S, Qu L, Ellegren H. 2008 Adaptive evolution of gamete-recognition proteins in birds. J. Mol. Evol. 67, 488 –496. (doi:10.1007/s00239-008-9165-6) 68. Gavrilets S. 2000 Rapid evolution of reproductive barriers driven by sexual conflict. Nature 403, 886–889. (doi:10.1038/35002564) 69. Snook RR, Hosken DJ, Karr TL. 2011 The biology and evolution of polyspermy: insights from cellular and functional studies of sperm and centrosomal behavior in the fertilized egg. Reproduction 142, 779–792. (doi:10.1530/REP-11-0255) 70. Hosken DJ. 2001 Sex and death: microevolutionary trade-offs between reproductive and immune investment in dung flies. Curr. Biol. 11, R379– R380. (doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00211-1) 71. Baer B, Armitage SAO, Boomsma JJ. 2006 Sperm storage induces an immunity cost in ants. Nature 441, 872–875. (doi:10.1038/nature04698) 72. Holman L, Snook RR. 2008 A sterile sperm caste protects brother fertile sperm from femalemediated death in Drosophila pseudoobscura. Curr. Biol. 18, 292–296. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.048) 73. Shuster SM, Wade MJ. 2009 Mating systems and strategies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 74. Collet J, Richardson DS, Worley K, Pizzari T. 2012 Sexual selection and the differential effect of polyandry. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 8641– 8645. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1200219109) 75. Jones AG, Walker D, Kvarnemo C, Lindstrom K, Avise JC. 2001 How cuckoldry can decrease the opportunity for sexual selection: data and theory from a genetic parentage analysis of the sand goby, Pomatoschistus minutus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 9151 –9156. (doi:10.1073/pnas.171310198) 76. Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. 2003 Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18, 41 –47. (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(02)00004-6) 9 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 35. 46. cost of mating in Drosophila melanogaster females. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 964 –973. (doi:10.1111/j.14209101.2009.01708.x) McGraw LA, Clark AG, Wolfner MF. 2008 Postmating gene expression profiles of female Drosophila melanogaster in response to time and to four male accessory gland proteins. Genetics 179, 1395 –1408. (doi:10.1534/genetics.108.086934) McGraw LA, Gibson G, Clark AG, Wolfner MF. 2004 Genes regulated by mating, sperm, or seminal proteins in mated female Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 14, 1509–1514. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004. 08.028) Jones FC et al. 2012 The genomic basis of adaptive evolution in threespine sticklebacks. Nature 484, 55 –61. (doi:10.1038/nature10944) Meiklejohn CD, Parsch J, Ranz JM, Hartl DL. 2003 Rapid evolution of male-biased gene expression in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 9894 –9899. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1630690100) Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Harvey PH. 1980 Antlers, body size and breeding group size in the Cervidae. Nature 285, 565–567. (doi:10.1038/ 285565a0) Harcourt AH, Harvey PH, Larson SG, Short RV. 1981 Testis weight, body weight and breeding system in primates. Nature 293, 55 –57. (doi:10.1038/ 293055a0) Hosken DJ, Garner TWJ, Ward PI. 2001 Sexual conflict selects for male and female reproductive characters. Curr. Biol. 11, 489–493. (doi:10.1016/ S0960-9822(01)00146-4) Hosken DJ. 2003 Sperm biology: size indeed matters. Curr. Biol. 13, R355– R356. (doi:10.1016/ S0960-9822(03)00275-6) Hosken DJ, Taylor ML, Hoyle K, Higgins S, Wedell N. 2008 Attractive males have greater success in sperm competition. Curr. Biol. 18, R553 –R554. (doi:10. 1016/j.cub.2008.04.028) Hosken DJ, Ward PI. 2001 Experimental evidence for testis size evolution via sperm competition. Ecol. Lett. 4, 10 –13. (doi:10.1046/j.1461-0248.2001. 00198.x) Dorus S, Wasbrough ER, Busby J, Wilkin EC, Karr TL. 2010 Sperm proteomics reveals intensified selection on mouse sperm membrane and acrosome genes. Mol. Biol. Evol. 27, 1235–1246. (doi:10.1093/ molbev/msq007) Fitzpatrick JL, Baer B. 2011 Polyandry reduces sperm length variation in social insects. Evolution 65, 3006–3012. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011. 01343.x) Birkhead TR, Martinez JG, Burke T, Froman DP. 1999 Sperm mobility determines the outcome of sperm competition in the domestic fowl. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 1759–1764. (doi:10.1098/rspb. 1999.0843) Birkhead TR, Pizzari T. 2002 Postcopulatory sexual selection. Nat. Rev. Genet. 3, 262–273. (doi:10.1038/nrg774) Hunter FM, Birkhead TR. 2002 Sperm viability and sperm competition in insects. Curr. Biol. 12, 121 –123. (doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00647-9) rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org 33. Drosophila simulans. Evolution 66, 665–677. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01468.x) Kent C, Azanchi R, Smith B, Formosa A, Levine JD. 2008 Social context influences chemical communication in D. melanogaster males. Curr. Biol. 18, 1384 –1389. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.088) Krupp JJ, Kent C, Billeter JC, Azanchi R, So AKC, Schonfeld JA, Smith BP, Lucas C, Levine JD. 2008 Social experience modifies pheromone expression and mating behavior in male Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 18, 1373– 1383. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.089) Dalton JE, Kacheria TS, Knott SRV, Lebo MS, Nishitani A, Sanders LE, Stirling EJ, Winbush A, Arbeitman MN. 2010 Dynamic, mating-induced gene expression changes in female head and brain tissues of Drosophila melanogaster. BMC Genomics 11, 541. (doi:10.1186/1471-2164-11-541) Ellis LL, Carney GE. 2010 Mating alters gene expression patterns in Drosophila melanogaster male heads. BMC Genomics 11, 558. (doi:10.1186/14712164-11-558) Lawniczak MKN, Begun DJ. 2004 A genome-wide analysis of courting and mating responses in Drosophila melanogaster females. Genome 47, 900–910. (doi:10.1139/g04-050) Goldman TD, Arbeitman MN. 2007 Genomic and functional studies of Drosophila sex hierarchy regulated gene expression in adult head and nervous system tissues. PLoS Genet. 3, 2278– 2295. (doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.0030216) Mank JE, Hultin-Rosenberg L, Axelsson E, Ellegren H. 2007 Rapid evolution of female-biased, but not male-biased, genes expressed in the avian brain. Mol. Biol. Evol. 24, 2698 –2706. (doi:10. 1093/molbev/msm208) Naurin S, Hansson B, Hasselquist D, Kim YH, Bensch S. 2011 The sex-biased brain: sexual dimorphism in gene expression in two species of songbirds. BMC Genomics 12, 37. (doi:10.1186/1471-2164-12-37) Bono JM, Matzkin LM, Kelleher ES, Markow TA. 2011 Postmating transcriptional changes in reproductive tracts of con- and heterospecifically mated Drosophila mojavensis females. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 7878 –7883. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 1100388108) Mack PD, Kapelnikov A, Heifetz Y, Bender M. 2006 Mating-responsive genes in reproductive tissues of female Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 10 358– 10 363. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 0604046103) Kapelnikov A, Zelinger E, Gottlieb Y, Rhrissorrakrai K, Gunsalus KC, Heifetz Y. 2008 Mating induces an immune response and developmental switch in the Drosophila oviduct. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 13 912–13 917. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0710997105) Domanitskaya EV, Liu HF, Chen SJ, Kubli E. 2007 The hydroxyproline motif of male sex peptide elicits the innate immune response in Drosophila females. FEBS J. 274, 5659 –5668. (doi:10.1111/j.1742-4658. 2007.06088.x) Innocenti P, Morrow EH. 2009 Immunogenic males: a genome-wide analysis of reproduction and the Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 92. 93. 94. 96. 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. 122. Biol. Evol. 27, 2000–2013. (doi:10.1093/molbev/ msq092) Walters JR, Harrison RG. 2011 Decoupling of rapid and adaptive evolution among seminal fluid proteins in Heliconius butterflies with divergent mating systems. Evolution 65, 2855– 2871. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01351.x) Cook PA, Wedell N. 1999 Non-fertile sperm delay female remating. Nature 397, 486–486. (doi:10.1038/17257) Lessells CM. 2006 The evolutionary outcome of sexual conflict. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 301 –317. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1795) Wedell N, Wiklund C, Bergstrom J. 2009 Coevolution of non-fertile sperm and female receptivity in a butterfly. Biol. Lett. 5, 678– 681. (doi:10.1098/rsbl. 2009.0452) Frank SA, Crespi BJ. 2011 Pathology from evolutionary conflict, with a theory of X chromosome versus autosome conflict over sexually antagonistic traits. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 10 886–10 893. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 1100921108) Queller DC. 1994 Male-female conflict and parentoffspring conflict. Am. Nat. 144, S84–S99. (doi:10.1086/285654) Zeh JA, Zeh DW. 2008 Maternal inheritance, epigenetics and the evolution of polyandry. Genetica 134, 45 –54. (doi:10.1007/s10709-0079192-z) Swaney WT, Curley JP, Champagne FA, Keverne EB. 2007 Genomic imprinting mediates sexual experience-dependent olfactory learning in male mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 6084 –6089. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0609471104) Shuker DM, Moynihan AM, Ross L. 2009 Sexual conflict, sex allocation and the genetic system. Biol. Lett. 5, 682 –685. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0427) van Doorn GS. 2009 Intralocus sexual conflict. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1168, 52 –71. (doi:10.1111/j.17496632.2009.04573.x) Bonduriansky R, Chenoweth SF. 2009 Intralocus sexual conflict. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 280 –288. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.12.005) Lande R. 1980 Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection and adaptation in polygenic characters. Evolution 34, 292 –305. (doi:10.2307/2407393) Cox RM, Calsbeek R. 2009 Sexually antagonistic selection, sexual dimorphism and the resolution of sexual conflict. Am. Nat. 173, 176 –187. (doi:10. 1086/595841) Ranz JM, Castillo-Davis CI, Meiklejohn CD, Hartl DL. 2003 Sex-dependent gene expression and evolution of the Drosophila transcriptome. Science 300, 1742–1745. (doi:10.1126/science.1085881) Zhang Z, Hambuch TM, Parsch J. 2004 Molecular evolution of sex-biased genes in Drosophila. Mol. Biol. Evol. 21, 2130–2139. (doi:10.1093/molbev/ msh223) Bachtrog D, Kirkpatrick M, Mank JE, McDaniel SF, Pires JC, Rice WR, Valenzuela N. 2011 Are all sex chromosomes created equal? Trends Genet. 27, 350–357. (doi:10.1016/j.tig.2011.05.005) 10 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 95. the resolution of sexual antagonism in vertebrate gene expression. Am. Nat. 171, 35 –43. (doi:10.1086/523954) Gallach M, Betran E. 2011 Intralocus sexual conflict resolved through gene duplication. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 222– 228. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.02.004) Wyman MJ, Cutter AD, Rowe L. 2012 Gene duplication and the evolution of sexual dimorphism. Evolution 66, 1556–1566. (doi:10.1111/j.15585646.2011.01525.x) Hosken DJ. 2011 Gene duplication might not resolve intralocus sexual conflict. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 556 –557. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.004) Mank JE, Hosken DJ, Wedell N. 2011 Some inconvenient truths about sex chromosome dosage compensation and the potential role of sexual conflict. Evolution 65, 2133 –2144. (doi:10.1111/j. 1558-5646.2011.01316.x) Mank JE. 2012 Small but mighty: the evolutionary dynamics of W and Y sex chromosomes. Chromosome Res. 20, 21 –33. (doi:10.1007/s10577011-9251-2) Roberts RB, Ser JR, Kocher TD. 2009 Sexual conflict resolved by invasion of a novel sex determiner in Lake Malawi cichlid fishes. Science 326, 998–1001. (doi:10.1126/science.1174705) van Doorn GS, Kirkpatrick M. 2007 Turnover of sex chromosomes induced by sexual conflict. Nature 449, 909 –912. (doi:10.1038/nature06178) Koerich LB, Wang X, Clark AG, Carvalho AB. 2008 Low conservation of gene content in the Drosophila Y chromosome. Nature 456, 949–951. (doi:10.1038/nature07463) Hasselmann M, Gempe T, Schiøtt M, Nunes-Silva C, Otte M, Beye M. 2008 Evidence for the evolutionary nascence of a novel sex determination pathway in honey bees. Nature 419, 519 –522. (doi:10.1038/ nature07052) Wigby S, Chapman T. 2005 Sex peptide causes mating costs in female Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 15, 316–321. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005. 01.051) Arnqvist G, Rowe L. 1995 Sexual conflict and arms races between the sexes: a morphological adaptation for control of mating in a female insect. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 261, 123– 127. (doi:10.1098/ rspb.1995.0126) Swanson WJ, Clark AG, Waldrip-Dail HM, Wolfner MF, Aquadro CF. 2001 Evolutionary EST analysis identifies rapidly evolving male reproductive proteins in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 7375 –7379. (doi:10.1073/pnas.131568198) Swanson WJ, Vacquier VD. 2002 The rapid evolution of reproductive proteins. Nat. Rev. Genet. 3, 137– 144. (doi:10.1038/nrg733) Dorus S, Evans PD, Wyckoff GJ, Choi SS, Lahn BT. 2004 Rate of molecular evolution of the seminal protein gene SEMG2 correlates with levels of female promiscuity. Nat. Genet. 36, 1326–1329. (doi:10.1038/ng1471) Walters JR, Harrison RG. 2010 Combined EST and proteomic analysis identifies rapidly evolving seminal fluid proteins in Heliconius butterflies. Mol. rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org 77. Connallon T, Clark AG. 2010 Sex linkage, sex-specific selection and the role of recombination in the evolution of sexually dimorphic gene expression. Evolution 64, 3417 –3442. (doi:10.1111/j.15585646.2010.01136.x) 78. Innocenti P, Morrow EH. 2010 The sexually antagonistic genes of Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS Biol. 8, e1000335. (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio. 1000335) 79. Mank JE, Ellegren H. 2009 Sex-linkage of sexually antagonistic genes is predicted by female, but not male, effects in birds. Evolution 63, 1464 –1472. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00618.x) 80. Brandvain Y. 2010 Matrisibs, patrisibs, and the evolution of imprinting on autosomes and sex chromosomes. Am. Nat. 176, 511 –521. (doi:10.1086/656272) 81. Day T, Bonduriansky R. 2004 Intralocus sexual conflict can drive the evolution of genomic imprinting. Genetics 167, 1537 –1546. (doi:10.1534/genetics.103.026211) 82. Connallon T, Clark AG. 2011 The resolution of sexual antagonism by gene duplication. Genetics 187, 919–937. (doi:10.1534/genetics.110.123729) 83. Reinke V, Gil IS, Ward S, Kazmer K. 2004 Genomewide germline-enriched and sex-biased expression profiles in Caenorhabditis elegans. Development 131, 311–323. (doi:10.1242/dev.00914) 84. Mank JE, Hultin-Rosenberg L, Webster MT, Ellegren H. 2008 The unique genomic properties of sex-biased genes: insights from avian microarray data. BMC Genomics 9, 148. (doi:10.1186/14712164-9-148) 85. Parisi M et al. 2004 A survey of ovary-, testis-, and soma-biased gene expression in Drosophila melanogaster adults. Genome Biol. 5, R40. (doi:10.1186/gb-2004-5-6-r40) 86. Yang X, Schadt EE, Wang S, Wang H, Arnold AP, Ingram-Drake L, Drake TA, Lusis AJ. 2006 Tissuespecific expression and regulation of sexually dimorphic genes in mice. Genome Res. 16, 995–1004. (doi:10.1101/gr.5217506) 87. Kijimoto T, Andrews J, Moczek AP. 2010 Programed cell death shapes the expression of horns within and between species of horned beetles. Evol. Dev. 12, 449–458. (doi:10.1111/j.1525-142X.2010. 00431.x) 88. Snell-Rood EC, Cash A, Han MV, Kijimoto T, Andrews J, Moczek AP. 2011 Developmental decoupling of alternative phenotypes: insights from the transcriptomes of horn polyphenic beetles. Evolution 65, 231–245. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01106.x) 89. Ellegren H, Hultin-Rosenberg L, Brunstroem B, Dencker L, Kultima K, Scholz B. 2007 Faced with inequality: chicken do not have a general dosage compensation of sex-linked genes. BMC Biol. 5, 40. (doi:10.1186/1741-7007-5-40) 90. Harano T, Okada K, Nakayama S, Miyatake T, Hosken DJ. 2010 Intralocus sexual conflict unresolved by sex-limited trait expression. Curr. Biol. 20, 2036–2039. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.023) 91. Mank JE, Hultin-Rosenberg L, Zwahlen M, Ellegren H. 2008 Pleiotropic constraint hampers Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on June 17, 2017 130. Vu ET, Mazurek ME, Kuo YC. 1994 Identification of a forebrain motor programming network for the learned song of zebra finches. J. Neurosci. 14, 6924–6934. 131. Zhang Y, Sturgill D, Parisi M, Kumar S, Oliver B. 2007 Constraint and turnover in sex-biased gene expression in the genus Drosophila. Nature 450, 233–237. (doi:10.1038/nature06323) 132. Chippindale AK, Gibson JR, Rice WR. 2001 Negative genetic correlation for adult fitness between sexes reveals ontogenetic conflict in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 1671–1675. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 98.4.1671) 11 Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20120047 in the Drosophila brain. Nature 438, 229–233. (doi:10.1038/nature04229) 127. Williams TM, Carroll SB. 2009 Genetic and molecular insights into the development and evolution of sexual dimorphism. Nat. Rev. Genet. 10, 797– 804. (doi:10.1038/nrg2687) 128. Berger FG, Watson G. 1989 Androgenregulated gene expression. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 51, 51 –65. (doi:10.1146/annurev.ph.51. 030189.000411) 129. Carroll JS et al. 2006 Genome-wide analysis of estrogen receptor binding sites. Nat. Genet. 38, 1289 –1297. (doi:10.1038/ng1901) rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org 123. Rice WR. 1984 Sex chromosomes and the evolution of sexual dimorphism. Evolution 38, 735– 742. (doi:10.2307/2408385) 124. Meisel RP, Malone JH, Clark AG. 2012 Disentangling the relationship between sex-biased gene expression and X linkage. Genome Res. 22, 1255–1265. (doi:10.1101/gr. 132100.111) 125. Stewart AD, Pischedda A, Rice WR. 2010 Resolving intralocus sexual conflict: genetic mechanisms and time frame. J. Hered. 101, S94–S99. (doi:10.1093/ jhered/esq011) 126. Kimura KI, Ote M, Tazawa T, Yamamoto D. 2005 Fruitless specifies sexually dimorphic neural circuitry