Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY SAN MARCOS PROJECT SIGNATURE PAGE PROJECT SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQIDRErv1ENTS FOR THE DEGREE MASTER OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE PROJECT TITLE: AUTHOR: The Effect ofMindfulness Meditation on Susceptibility lo Misinfom1ation Whitney C. Hawkins DATE OF SUCCESSFUL DEFENSE: May 8, 2017 THE PROJECT HAS BEEN ACCEPTED BY THE PROJECT COMMITTEE IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Dustin Calvillo. Ph.D. PROJECT COMMITTEE CHAIR Marie Thomas, Ph.D PROJECT COMMITTEE MEMBER Carrick Williams. Ph.D. PROJECT COMMITTEE MEIVIBER ~· SIGNATURE ~- ~ft/'1 SIGNATURE <li~ 15(<i(ll DATE s/e-/,'7 DATE S/&/17 DATE Running head: MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Susceptibility to Misinformation Whitney Hawkins California State University San Marcos 1 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 2 Abstract Eyewitnesses exposed to misleading post-event information often unknowingly incorporate false information into their testimony. The misinformation paradigm is used to study false memories by presenting participants with information about a crime, providing misleading post-event information about the crime, and finally having them complete a memory test. The misinformation effect occurs when participants remember misleading post-event information as having occurred in the original event. The goal of the current study was to examine the effects of a mindfulness induction at different points during a misinformation task on true memories and misinformation effects. Four groups of participants completed a misinformation task; three groups received a mindfulness induction, each at a different time during the task, and one group did not receive a mindfulness induction. It was predicted that misleading post-event information would be incorporated into memory most often when mindfulness was induced before the postevent information and least often when mindfulness was induced before the original information. It was also predicted that the endorsement of true items would be similar in pre-video and prePEI groups and would be higher than the pre-test and control groups. Results indicated that all groups showed a misinformation effect, but the size of the effect did not differ across conditions. There were also no significant differences in endorsing true information across conditions. These findings suggest that a brief mindfulness induction does not affect susceptibility to the misinformation effect. Limitations and future directions for research are discussed. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 3 Acknowledgments I would like to express my deepest appreciation to my committee chair, Dr. Dustin Calvillo. He has guided me for five years, always quickly responding to my ceaseless emails, and never hesitating to answer questions I should undoubtedly know the answers to. He has the makings of a genius and acknowledging him will surely make him uncomfortable, so I had to do it. This thesis would not be possible without him. I would also like to thank my committee members, Dr. Carrick Williams and Dr. Marie Thomas. Dr. Williams is always prepared with a whiteboard marker in hand to help me break down my questions and figure out the answers, even late at night. Dr. Thomas has provided the mindfulness expertise needed for this project and I thank her for sparking my interest in mindfulness meditation as an undergraduate. They have both been amazing committee members and I thank them for their unconditional support. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 4 Contents Acknowledgments........................................................................................................................... 3 Abstract ........................................................................................................................................... 4 The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Susceptibility to Misinformation ................................. 5 Susceptibility to Misinformation ............................................................................................. 6 Effects of Mindfulness on Cognitive Processing .................................................................... 9 Mindfulness and False Memories .......................................................................................... 13 Method ......................................................................................................................................... 17 Power Analysis and Participants ........................................................................................... 17 Materials .................................................................................................................................. 18 Procedure ................................................................................................................................. 20 Results .......................................................................................................................................... 22 Assumptions............................................................................................................................. 22 Mindfulness Manipulation Check (Hypothesis 1) ................................................................ 22 Misinformation Manipulation Check (Hypothesis 2) .......................................................... 23 Mindfulness on Magnitude of Misinformation Effect (Hypothesis 3)................................ 23 Mindfulness on Endorsement of True Items (Hypothesis 4) .............................................. 24 Sensitivity ................................................................................................................................. 24 Response Bias .......................................................................................................................... 24 References ..................................................................................................................................... 30 Table 1 .......................................................................................................................................... 37 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 5 The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Susceptibility to Misinformation A false memory is a type of memory distortion in which people remember detailed information about a past event that either never occurred or occurred differently from the way they remember (Loftus, 2003). In laboratory experiments, participants have been led to falsely remember a variety of events, including going on a hot air balloon ride (Wade, Garry, Read, & Lindsay, 2002) and having a negative experience with a Disneyland character (Berkowitz, Laney, Morris, Garry, & Loftus, 2008). In real-world eyewitness situations, false memories of crimes can have dire consequences. Eyewitnesses who are exposed to misleading post-event information (PEI) are likely to incorporate the incorrect information into their memory for the original event and thus include it into their testimony. This type of false memory is known as the misinformation effect (Loftus, 2005). In a typical misinformation experiment, participants view a crime being committed either in the form of pictures or a video. They are then given some post-event information that often has the same general occurrences as the original information, but a few details are changed in order to mislead participants. Participants are finally given a memory test to determine if they can remember details from the original information source. The misinformation effect occurs when participants remember some information as happening in the original event when, in fact, it occurred in the post-event information. It is important to understand what makes people more or less prone to these misinformation errors in order to know if the eyewitnesses are reliable. Research has found increased working memory capacity decreases misinformation effects (Calvillo, 2014; Jaschinski & Wentura, 2002; Zhu et al., 2010) while picture induced emotion (Porter, Spencer, & Birt, 2003; Van Damme & Seynaeve, 2013; Van Damme & Smets, 2014) and divided attention (Lane, 2006; Zaragoza & Lane, 1998) increase misinformation effects. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 6 One mechanism that has been found to influence cognitive processes is mindfulness. Mindfulness is defined as paying attention to the present moment, purposefully, and nonjudgmentally (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). Research on mindfulness using the public (as opposed to Buddhist Monks) was introduced around 1979 (Barker, 2014). This newly integrated mindfulness often starts by having participants sit in a comfortable position and are instructed to think about or on what to focus including, but not limited to, a candle flame (Lippelt, Hommel, & Colzato, 2014) or the sensation of breathing (Ainsworth, Eddershaw, Meron, Baldwin, & Garner, 2013). Among other topics, research has investigated the effects on mindfulness interventions on attention (Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008; Moore & Malinowski, 2009; Semple, 2010), working memory capacity (Chambers et al., 2008; Quach, Mano, & Alexander, 2016; Mrazek, Franklin, Phillips, Baird, & Schooler, 2013) and emotion regulation (Arch & Craske, 2006; Ortner & Kilner, 2007). Given the impact of mindfulness on these cognitive functions, it is surprising that research has yet to examine the effect of mindfulness inductions on the misinformation effect. In the current study, I tested whether a mindfulness intervention had an effect on true memories and misinformation effects in the misinformation paradigm. I will discuss the effects of attention, working memory capacity, and emotion on the misinformation effect and then discuss the effects of mindfulness training on attention, working memory capacity, and emotion regulation. Susceptibility to Misinformation Attention. Attention is an important factor in decreasing the misinformation effect. Past research has found that divided attention at either the original event or during the post-event information can lead to increased misinformation effects. Lane (2006) theorized that sourcespecifying information becomes lost when attention is divided during the original event. Once MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 7 the original event is complete and the contextual source details are not encoded, there are few characteristics to differentiate the original information from the post-event information leading to an increase in misinformation. Zaragoza and Lane (1998) had participants either identify music while reading post-event information or only read the post-event information. The purpose of having participants identify music while reading the post-event information was to induce divided attention. Results indicated that the divided attention group was significantly more likely to incorporate the misleading post-event information into their memory of the original event. Lane (2006) conducted a similar study and separated participants into either a divided attention task or a control misinformation task. Participants in the divided attention task listened to music while viewing slides (presentation of the original event); participants in the control condition did not listen to music during the slides. Results revealed that participants in the divided attention task had poorer memory for the true items and had increased misinformation effects compared to the control condition. These studies support the idea that divided attention can lead to an increased misinformation effect. English and Nielson (2010) conducted an experiment examining the effect of arousal on the misinformation effect and found increased arousal after presentation of the misinformation items led to decreased acceptance of misinformation one week after presentation. Kahneman (1973) found that arousal leads to increased use of attentional resources so we can interpret English and Nielson’s (2010) results as we would studies on attention and the misinformation effect. Working memory capacity. Working memory is a system of memory that holds available mental representations for processing (Oberauer, Farrell, Jarrold, & Lewandowsky, 2016). Research has shown there is a negative relationship between working memory capacity and susceptibility to the misinformation effect (Calvillo, 2014; Jaschinski & Wentura, 2002; Zhu MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 8 et al., 2010). The predominant explanation for this relationship is that participants with greater working memory capacity are better able to remember the details of the original event (Jaschinski & Wentura, 2002; Zhu et al., 2010), possibly due to being more attentive during the original event. This negative relationship between working memory capacity and susceptibility to the misinformation effect has been shown using both the n-back measure (Zhu et. al, 2010) and the operation span (OSPAN; Calvillo, 2014; Jaschinski & Wentura, 2002). A meta-analysis has also found a significant negative relationship between working memory capacity and the misinformation effect (Calvillo, Hawkins, Amoroso, & Waltrip, 2017). One theory used to explain the relationship between working memory capacity and the misinformation effect is the source-monitoring framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). Gerrie and Garry (2007) explain that people use qualitative characteristics of remembered information to determine if it is old or new. As it pertains to the misinformation paradigm, when someone is presented post-event information that is inconsistent with the original event, that person may recognize it as new information depending on the person’s ability to monitor the sources of information when they are presented. Gerrie and Garry (2007) also theorize that people with higher working memory capacity are better able to source monitor. Kane, Bleckley, Conway, and Engle (2001) theorize that individual differences in working memory capacity may be due to a difference in executive controlled attention. This executive controlled attention includes the ability to maintain information in an accessible and active state, even in the face of distractions (Kane et al., 2001). Integrating this theory, increased working memory capacity should lead to higher scores on attentional measures such as the OSPAN because of this increased executive control of attention. Taken together, the results of past MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 9 research and the source-monitoring framework indicate higher working memory capacity is predictive of lower misinformation effects. Emotion. Past research indicates increased emotion (as induced by looking at positive or negative valence pictures) has a significant impact on the misinformation effect. Porter, Brinkle, Riley, and Baker (2014) speculate a possible reason for this occurrence is due to emotion causing a narrowed field of view. They theorize that once a person is primed with either a positive or negative emotion, that person focuses on aspects of the scene that parallel his or her emotion and tends to ignore non-mood congruent aspects. Van Damme and Smets (2014) had participants look at positive valence-low arousal, positive valence-high arousal, negative valence-low arousal, or positive valence-high arousal pictures. The results indicated three of the four picture types were significantly more likely to endorse misleading post-event information compared to controls (positive valence-low arousal was not). Porter et al. (2003) found that participants who were presented with a negative scene (graphic fatal accident) before misleading post-event information were twice as likely to recall the misinformation than participants who were presented with either a positive or neutral scene before misinformation. Porter et al. (2014) found that both positive and negative emotional priming at encoding lead to an increased susceptibility to misinformation. This may be due to the narrowing of focus induced by the pictures not allowing participants to take in all of the presented information. Taken together, these results suggest emotion is significantly positively related to the misinformation effect; thus, people who are better able to regulate their emotions should be less susceptible to misinformation effects. Effects of Mindfulness on Cognitive Processing Attention. Mindfulness meditation has been found to significantly improve performance MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 10 on attentional tasks such as the Stroop task and the d2-concentration and endurance test. One of the reasons this enhanced attention occurs is because during mindfulness induction, participants are given instructions to pay attention to one object or sensation (most often breathing) and are instructed to ignore any distractions. This focused attention during the induction is thought to enhance the participant’s attentional control even after the mindfulness induction is completed (Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, David, & Goolkasian, 2010). Mindfulness has also been found to help decrease distractions by reducing mind wandering (Mrazek et al., 2013). Semple (2010) compared participants in a four-week mindfulness meditation group, progressive relaxation group, or a waitlist group on sustained selective attention using the Continuous Performance Test. Results indicated that the mindfulness meditation group had improved scores on the sustained selective attention tasks that were not seen in either the progressive relaxation group or the waitlist group. Moore and Malinowski (2009) similarly measured attentional performance of mindfulness meditators versus non-meditators using multiple cognitive tasks and found that the meditators had significantly higher scores on the d2-concentration and endurance test and the Stroop task compared to the control group. Chambers et al. (2008) examined the effect of a 10week mindfulness training on reaction time. Results indicated participants in the mindfulness group had a significant decrease in reaction time, a difference not seen in the control group. Wenk-Sormaz (2005) found that even a single 20-minute session of mindfulness significantly reduced the Stroop effect, which may be due to mindfulness increasing attention and decreasing habitual responding. Taken together, the results of Semple (2010), Moore and Malinowski (2009), Chambers et al. (2008), and Wenk-Sormaz (2005) indicate mindfulness training can lead to significant improvements in attentional control. Working memory capacity. There has been some research examining the effect of MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 11 mindfulness training on working memory capacity. Chambers et al. (2008) separated participants into either a mindfulness group or a control group and measured participants working memory capacity using the backward digit span. It is important to note that the backward digit span is primarily used to measure short-term memory; however, Engle (2002) found working memory and short-term memory constructs are highly correlated. Results indicated the mindfulness group had a significant increase in working memory capacity after a 10-day meditation course; the control group did not show a significant increase across conditions. This increased working memory capacity may stem from Kane et al.’s (2001) theory of increased executive attention. Quach et al. (2016) separated participants into an eight-week mindfulness meditation group, yoga group, or control group and completed the OSPAN as a measure of working memory capacity. Results indicated participants in the mindfulness meditation group had significantly increased working memory at time two compared to time one. There was no significant difference between time one and time two for either the yoga or waitlist group. Mrazek et al. (2013) had participants take either an eight-session mindfulness course or a control course and gave participants the OSPAN and a reading comprehension test before and after the class. Results indicated the participants in the mindfulness class had significant improvements in both working memory capacity and reading comprehension compared to the control course. Jha, Stanley, Kiyonaga, Wong, and Gelfand (2010) found higher mindfulness training practice in military cohorts improved working memory capacity compared to a military control group and a civilian control group. Similar results have also been found using an n-back task (Zeiden et al., 2010). Taken together, these results indicate mindfulness training can significantly increase working memory capacity. It has been theorized the positive relationship between mindfulness and working memory capacity is due to the increase in attention and focus MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 12 that is seen after mindfulness induction (Mrazek et al., 2013). Emotion. Past research has suggested mindfulness meditation can improve emotion regulation by encouraging participants to non-judgmentally bring their awareness to the present moment. This awareness helps to promote self-control and thus increases emotional control (Tang, Tang, & Posner, 2016). Arch and Craske (2006) had participants complete an affect scale while monitoring their heart rate, complete either a 15-minute mindfulness induction or a mindwandering induction, complete another affective scale, then watch negative affective slides while monitoring their heart rate. Results from this study indicated that the mindfulness group reported lower negative affect and maintained lower heart rates between time 1 and time 2. In the second study of Ortner and Kilner (2007), participants viewed neutral, pleasant, and unpleasant scenes and then they completed a reaction time measure. Participants then completed either a sevenweek mindfulness training, relaxation training, or nothing; after seven weeks participants completed the same reaction time task again. Results showed that the mindfulness group had significant decreased reaction time to the negative scenes between time 1 and 2. The relaxation induction group had a significant increase in reaction time between time 1 and time 2. Finally, the control group did not show any significant difference in reaction time between time 1 and time 2. These results suggest that the mindfulness group was better able to not dwell on the negative stimuli, which led to faster reaction times compared to the other two groups. Watier and Dubois (2016) also found participants with low state mindfulness who received a 10-minute mindfulness induction had less interference in the Emotional Stroop task compared to a control group. These studies demonstrate mindfulness meditation can improve emotion regulation. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 13 Mindfulness and False Memories Past research has examined misinformation effects and mindfulness separately, but only two studies have been conducted to examine the effect of mindfulness on false memories, both of which used the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). Because these studies are the closest past research has come to my study, I will explain them in more detail. In DRM experiments, participants are presented a list of words, each of which are related to one non-presented critical lure. For example, participants may be presented with sit, seat, desk, bench, couch, etc., that are all related to the non-presented critical lure chair. Participants often falsely remember the non-presented critical lure as having been presented in the word list (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). In two of three experiments conducted by Wilson, Mickes, Stolarz-Fantino, Evrard, and Fantino (2015) researchers examined the effects of mindfulness induction on susceptibility to false memories using the DRM paradigm. In Experiment 1, participants were given either a 15-minute mindfulness or mind-wandering induction followed by DRM word lists and a recall test. Experiment 2 was switched to a pretestposttest design which presented participants with DRM word lists, had them complete a recall test, complete either a mindfulness induction or a mind-wandering induction, followed by more DRM wordlists, and a final recall test. Results of both experiments showed that the mindfulness groups were significantly more likely to falsely remember the non-presented critical lure as having been present in the word lists. Rosenstreich (2015) also conducted two experiments to examine the effects of mindfulness induction on false memories using the DRM paradigm. In Experiment 1, all participants were presented with DRM wordlists then completed a recognition test. Participants were then divided into either a mindfulness training group or a waitlist group. The mindfulness MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 14 training consisted of five 30-minute weekly sessions of mindfulness training accompanied by daily home practice. After five weeks, all participants were presented with additional DRM wordlists and completed another recognition test. In Experiment 2, participants were assigned to either a 30-minute mindfulness induction or mind-wandering induction. After completion of the induction, participants were presented with DRM word lists then were given a recall test. Results from both experiments support the findings of Wilson et al. (2015); the mindfulness groups more often remembered the non-presented critical lure as having been presented compared to the control groups. Taken together, the findings from Wilson et al. (2015) and Rosenstreich (2015) indicate mindfulness increases false memories in the DRM paradigm. Although the results of these studies suggest mindfulness training may not be effective in decreasing false memories, it is important to note these studies were completed using the DRM paradigm, not the misinformation paradigm. Studies have shown that susceptibility to false memories in the DRM and the misinformation paradigms are unrelated (Calvillo & Parong, 2016; Ost et al., 2013). Calvillo and Parong (2016) suggested that DRM effects and misinformation effects are not related because of how the false memories are generated. DRM effects are the result of self-generated internal suggestion (lures) while misinformation effects are the result of external suggestions. In addition, Hardt, Einarsson, and Nader (2010) classified misinformation effects as a result from reconsolidation but did not include DRM effects, which may be due to encoding (Calvillo & Parong, 2016). This indicates that even though mindfulness significantly increases false memories in the DRM paradigm, it may have different effects as an intervention in the misinformation paradigm. The Present Study MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 15 The current literature suggests that mindfulness inductions increase working memory capacity (Chambers et al., 2008; Mrazek et al., 2013; Quach et al., 2016), attentional control (Chambers et. al, 2008; Moore & Malinowski, 2009; Semple, 2010), and emotion regulation (Arch & Craske, 2006; Ortner et al., 2007). Research has also demonstrated that increased working memory capacity (Calvillo, 2014; Jaschinski & Wentura, 2002; Zhu et. al, 2010) and attentional control (Lane, 2006; Zaragoza & Lane, 1998) decrease misinformation effects and increased emotion (Porter et. al, 2003; Van Damme & Seynaeve, 2013; Van Damme & Smets, 2014) increases misinformation effects. There has yet to be research examining the direct effects of a mindfulness induction on misinformation effects. The current study examined the proportion of true memories and misinformation effects reported by four groups of participants using a true/false recognition test. All participants watched a video of a crime. Three of the four groups of participants received a mindfulness induction and one group was a control group. Of the three mindfulness induction groups, the pre-video group received the induction before the original video, the pre-PEI group received the induction before the post-event information, and the pre-test group received the induction before the memory test. All three groups received the same mindfulness induction, and all participants completed the same true/false recognition test. The recognition test was comprised of three types of questions: 1) true items were consistent with the information in the original video and post-event information, 2) misinformation items were consistent with the post-event information but inconsistent with the original video, and 3) foils were inconsistent with both the original video and the post-event information. Hypotheses: 1) Mindfulness scores would be higher immediately after mindfulness induction compared to after the control task. This would indicate participants were more mindful after the MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 16 mindfulness induction than after the history of radio recording. 2) True items would be endorsed more frequently than misinformation items, and foil items will be endorsed the least. Greater endorsement of misinformation items than foils would indicate the misinformation effect. 3) The magnitude of the misinformation effect would be highest in the pre-PEI group, followed by the control, the pre-test, and the pre-video groups, respectively. Because mindfulness seems to improve attention and working memory capacity, I hypothesized that the largest misinformation effect would be generated when mindfulness is given immediately before the post-event information. This is due to the mindfulness helping the participant to learn the post-event information better than the other groups, which would then lead them to more likely endorse the items consistent with that post-event information. The control group would have the second highest misinformation effect because they would not receive any mindfulness induction. The pre-video would have the smallest misinformation effect because they would receive the mindfulness before the original video, which would increase memory for the true items on the memory test. 4) The endorsement of true items would be similar in pre-video and pre-PEI groups and would be higher than the pre-test and control groups. Mindfulness has been found to increase memory so the two groups that receive mindfulness before any information (the pre-video group and the pre-PEI group) should have better memory compared to the other two groups. The pre-test and the control groups should have similar numbers of true memories. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 17 Method Power Analysis and Participants My second and third hypotheses were expected to generate the most important findings, so I focused the power analyses on those. For my second hypothesis, a power analysis for an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was run using G*Power software version 3.0 (Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996). An alpha level of .05 (two-tailed) and a power of .80 were chosen. Based on the analyses conducted by Calvillo et al. (2016) for the misinformation stimuli, a large effect size was expected (d = 2.00). This effect size was calculated by subtracting the endorsement of foil items from the endorsement rate of the misinformation items. Based on these calculations, a total sample size of 76 was needed for the study. For my third hypothesis, a power analysis for an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was run using G*Power software version 3.0. An alpha level of .05 (two-tailed) and a power of .80 were chosen. Based on the on the analyses conducted by Wilson et al. (2015) examining the effects of mindfulness on the DRM paradigm, a small to medium effect size was expected (d = .33). This effect size is for the difference in memory of critical lures before mindfulness minus the memory for critical lures after mindfulness. Based on these calculations, a total sample size of 384 was needed for the study. Participants were recruited from the human participant pool at California State University San Marcos. This pool consists of psychology students who volunteer to participate in research projects conducted at the university. Students participated in exchange for credit toward a research requirement in a lower division psychology course. Three hundred fourteen participants were initially recruited; twelve participants were excluded due to computer malfunctions. Three hundred and two participants (81% women) aged 18 to 32 (M = 19.92, SD = 2.37) were included in the final analyses. This sample size with the small to medium expected effect size, yielded a MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 18 power of .69. Of the participants, 63% had never previously practiced mindfulness meditation, 26% complete a mindfulness meditation less than once per month, and 11% meditate more than once per month. Design I employed a 4 (induction condition) X 4 (statement type) mixed factorial design to test the effects of mindfulness induction on number of true memories and misinformation effects. The induction was between participants and the statement type was within participants. The four induction times were before the original video (pre-video), before the post-event information (pre-PEI), before the memory test (pre-test), and no induction (control). There were two types of true items, misinformation items, and foil items. One type of true statement corresponded to the both the original video and post-event information and the other type of true statement corresponded to the original video only. The misinformation statements corresponded only to the post-event information, and the foil statements did not correspond to either the original video or the post-event information. The dependent variable was the endorsement of items on the recognition test. Materials Mindfulness induction. The mindfulness induction was a 10-minute audio recording of a script taken from Erisman and Roemer (2010). It began by instructing participants to focus their attention on the feeling of their body in the chair and the feeling of the air touching their skin. The recording continued by instructing the participants to focus on their breathing and the feeling of the breath entering and exiting the body. Throughout the recording, participants were instructed to bring their attention back to their breath if their mind began to wander. This MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 19 mindfulness script was chosen because it was the duration needed and had been successfully used in a previous study (Erisman & Romer, 2010). Manipulation check. I used the Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS) to determine if the mindfulness induction was effective. The TMS was developed by Lau et al. (2006) and is a 13item questionnaire in which respondents rate on a 5-point Likert-type scale (0 = not at all; 4 = very much) the level of state awareness and attention that had occurred during the induction. An example item is “I was more concerned with being open to my experiences than controlling or changing them.” This scale has high internal consistency (α = .87; Lueke & Gibson, 2016). Filler audio. I used an audio recording of the history of radio (Modern marvels, 1997). This active control is common for mindfulness inductions (Lloyd, Szani, Rubenstein, Colgary, & Pereira-Pasarin, 2016). This recording began by explaining how the radio was invented and how it was first used for military communication. The recording continued by discussing the first time music was played and how that led to radios in homes. The recording discussed the struggle for control over broadcasts and patents and finished by explaining some radio tragedies such as the infamous War of the Worlds broadcasts. The recording was 40-minutes long and was been broken into four 10-minute segments. Video. The original event video for the misinformation task was taken from Takarangi et al. (2006). It is a 6.5-minute video depicting an electrician entering and working in an unoccupied house, rifling through the owners’ belongings, and stealing some items. The video was played without audio. Post-event information. Participants simultaneously read and listened to an automated narration describing the events in the video. Presenting the information in both written and audio format ensured all participants received the information at the same pace. The information in the MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 20 narrative was consistent with the original video, with the exception of four items: whether the bed was made, the color of the mug near the electrician, the magazine he looked through, and the picture hanging on the wall. The narrative was four minutes long and was shown on the computer screen, broken into one paragraph per slide, with the audio played simultaneously through speakers. Memory test. The memory test consisted of a 16-item true-or-false recognition test. It included four items consistent with both the video and the post-event information (true in both original video and post-event information), four items consistent in the original video and not reinforced in the post-event information (true in original video only), four items from the postevent information that contradicted the video (misinformation items), and four items that were inconsistent with both video and narrative (foils). An example true in both original video and post-event information item is, “The name on the van was ‘AJ’s electricians,’” which occurred in both the original video and the post-event information. An example true in original video only item is, “Eric was wearing a tool belt,” which occurred in the video but was not mentioned in the post-event information. An example misinformation item is, “The magazine Eric read was Time magazine”; he read Newsweek in the video but it was described as Time in the narrative. Finally, an example foil is, “Eric ate a banana”; he ate an apple in the video and it was described as an apple in the narrative. Procedure Participants signed up for a session online. Before arriving, participants were assigned to one of four groups: pre-video, pre-post-event narrative, pre-test, and control. Participants in three of the four groups received a mindfulness induction at different times during a misinformation task (and a control task at other times), whereas participants in the fourth group MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 21 only received control tasks. When participants arrived, they were greeted by a research assistant. The elements of informed consent were explained and they were given the consent form. Once they had signed the form, they received instructions for the study. They were told they would listen to an audio clip. After the recording, participants answered the Toronto Mindfulness Scale questionnaire. After the questionnaire, all participants watched a video of an electrician entering a house and stealing some items. Once the video was complete, participants listened to another audio clip then completed the same Toronto Mindfulness Scale. Participants were presented with a narration of the original event but with a few details changed. After the narration, participants listened to the last audio clip, completed another Toronto Mindfulness Scale, and then completed the recognition test. Participants were then thanked and debriefed. After researchers began the audio clips, they left the room and watched the participants through a one-way mirror. Once the audio clip ended, the researcher re-entered the room. The pre-video group received a 10-minute mindfulness induction as the first audio file; the pre-PEI group, pre-test group, and control group listened to a clip on the history of radio. For the second audio clip, the pre-PEI group listened to the mindfulness induction; the pre-video, pre-test, and control groups listened to the second part of the history of radio recording. The pre-test group listened to the mindfulness induction for the final audio clip while the pre-video, pre-PEI, and control groups listened to the third part of the history of radio audio clip. The pre-video and pretest groups did not receive one chunk of the audio (e.g. the pre-video group did not hear the first 10-minute section but heard the second and third chunks), but this method was chosen to maintain consistency between groups. The procedure is illustrated in Figure 1. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 22 Results Assumptions Homogeneity of variance was examined using Levene’s test. The endorsement of original video only, F(2, 298) = 0.99, p = .394, and endorsement of foils F(2, 298) = .36, p = .779, were not significant which indicates there was homogeneity of variances. However, endorsement of post-event information only, F(2, 298) = 2.79, p = .041, and endorsement of original video and post-event information, F(2, 298) = 2.82, p = .039, were significant which indicates they lacked homogeneity of variance. To examine normality, skewness and kurtosis were examined and revealed that endorsement of original video and post-event information was not normally distributed. Data were not transformed because the sample size was large and the group sizes were relatively equal therefore the ANOVA tests should still be robust in spite of the violations. However, because some of the assumptions of the ANOVA were not met, all analyses should be interpreted with caution. Mindfulness Manipulation Check (Hypothesis 1) Each participant completed three separate TMS questionnaires. To ensure mindfulness scores were higher immediately after mindfulness induction compared to after the control task, I used the TMS questionnaire that was completed immediately after the mindfulness induction and compared it to the control TMS scores from the same time. I then conducted three independent measures t-tests to determine whether TMS scores were higher immediately after the mindfulness induction compared to control. Results confirm that participants who received the mindfulness before the original video (the Pre-video condition) had significantly higher TMS scores (M = 3.34, SD = 0.57) than participants in the control group (M = 2.94, SD = 0.60), t(150) = 4.23, p < .001, d = 0.69. Participants who received the mindfulness before the post-event MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 23 information (the Pre-PEI condition) had significantly higher TMS scores (M = 3.37, SD = 0.78) than participants in the control group (M = 3.02, SD = 0.59), t(147) = 3.06, p = .003, d = 0.50. Participants who received the mindfulness before the test (the Pre-test condition) had marginally significantly higher TMS scores (M = 3.29, SD = 0.87) than participants in the control group (M = 3.04, SD = 0.70), t(148) = 1.90, p = .059, d = 0.31. The results indicated that participants’ mindfulness scores were higher after the mindfulness induction compared to control in two of the three comparisons so my first hypothesis was partially supported. Misinformation Manipulation Check (Hypothesis 2) To ensure my stimuli produced the standard misinformation effect, I examined the endorsement ratings of true items, misinformation items, and foil items. I conducted a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) examining endorsement rates across statement types. Results revealed that there was a significant difference in the endorsements of statement types F(3, 903) = 825.75, p < .001, η2 = .97. Pairwise LSD comparisons indicated that there are significant differences between all comparisons, all ps < .001. The mean endorsement ratings of each statement type are shown in Table 1. These results supported my second hypothesis. Mindfulness on Magnitude of Misinformation Effect (Hypothesis 3) To examine the magnitude of the misinformation effect, I subtracted foil endorsement rates from misinformation rates to yield a misinformation magnitude. I then compared the misinformation magnitude across conditions. A one-way ANOVA indicated there was not a significant difference between conditions, F(3, 298) = 1.24, p = .295, η2 = .01. Planned comparisons tests revealed there were no significant differences between conditions. The mean magnitude of the misinformation effect across conditions is shown in Figure 2. The data did not support my third hypothesis. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 24 Mindfulness on Endorsement of True Items (Hypothesis 4) To examine the endorsement of true items I compared statements true in original video only across conditions. I also compared statements true in both the original video and post-event information across conditions. A one-way ANOVA on endorsement rates of statements true in the original video only revealed no significant differences between conditions, F(3, 298) = 1.12, p = .341, η2 = .01. Another one-way ANOVA on endorsement rates of statements true in both the original video and post-event information indicated there was not a significant difference between conditions, F(3, 298) = 0.90, p = .443, η2 = .01. No planned comparisons were significant in either analysis. Mean endorsement ratings for item type across conditions are shown in Table 1. These results did not support my fourth hypothesis. Sensitivity Two exploratory signal detection analyses were conducted. The first signal detection measure was A’, which was based on the calculations of Snodgrass and Corwin (1988). A’ calculates the participant’s ability to discriminate between presented items (hits) and nonpresented misinformation items. The possible range of scores for A’ is 0 to 1, with scores closer to 1 indicating greater sensitivity (discrimination). Measuring sensitivity is important to understand how well participants can separate their memory for the original video and their memory for the post-event information. A one-way ANOVA indicated that A’ scores did not significantly differ across conditions, F(3, 298) = 2.14, p = .095, η2 = .02. Mean A’ scores for the four conditions are shown in Figure 3. Response Bias The second exploratory signal detection measure was B’’D which is a measure of response bias. B’’D was based on the calculations of Donaldson (1992). The possible range of MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 25 scores is -1 to 1 with negative scores indicating a liberal bias (responding true more often than one should) and positive scores indicating a conservative bias (responding true less often than one should). This measure is useful in determining if the participant had any liberal or conservative response patterns (responding “true” more or less likely than they should). A oneway ANOVA on B” D scores indicated there was no significant difference between conditions, F(3, 298) = 0.43 p = .734, η2 =.00. Mean B” D scores for the four conditions are shown in Figure 4. Discussion The current study examined the effects of mindfulness meditation on the endorsement of true and false statements in the misinformation paradigm. Four hypotheses were tested as well as two exploratory signal detection analyses. The first two hypotheses were manipulation checks for stimuli while the third and fourth hypotheses examined the effect of mindfulness on endorsement of true and false statements. The first hypothesis was a manipulation check for the mindfulness induction--I predicted that mindfulness scores would be higher immediately after mindfulness induction compared to after the control task. I used the mean TMS scores immediately after the mindfulness induction compared to mean TMS scores in the control condition at the same point in the study. The results of two TMS scores compared to control were statistically different and the third was marginally different compared to control. This indicated that participants were more mindful after the mindfulness induction than after the history of radio recording with the exception of participants who received the mindfulness induction prior to the memory test. This means the mindfulness manipulation was successful in two of three conditions and participants in those conditions were more mindful after receiving the mindfulness induction. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 26 Hypothesis 2 was the misinformation stimuli manipulation check--I predicted that true items would be endorsed more frequently than misinformation items and foil items will be endorsed the least. Endorsement ratings for each type of item followed this pattern. This indicated that participants endorsed true items the most and foil items the least, which suggests the misinformation stimuli produced the standard misinformation effect. The third hypothesis was that the magnitude of the misinformation effect would be highest in the pre-PEI group, followed by the control, the pre-test, and the pre-video groups, respectively. To analyze this, I calculated the magnitude of misinformation and compared it across conditions. The results were not significant which did support my hypothesis. This indicates that there was not an effect of mindfulness on the magnitude of misinformation. My final hypothesis was that the endorsement of true items would be similar in pre-video and pre-PEI groups and would be higher than the pre-test and control groups. To calculate this, I compared true statements in the original video only and true statements in both the original video and post-event information. The results indicated that there was no significant effect of mindfulness on endorsement of true items. Mindfulness not having an effect on memory is contradictory to past research, which has shown that mindfulness increases both true and false memories. One reason for this lack of significance may be due to the length of the mindfulness induction. Most studies that examined the effects of mindfulness on cognitive functioning utilized either long-time meditators (Lykins, Baer, & Gottlob, 2012; Xiong & Doraiswamy, 2009) or had participants complete a multisession mindfulness based course (Jha et al., 2010; Mrazek et al. 2013; Quach et al., 2016). The brief mindfulness induction used in the current study lasted only 10-minutes. Although the induction was long enough to effect the mindfulness groups immediately after the mindfulness MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 27 induction (compared to the control group), we do not know how long the effects of the mindfulness carried over. It is possible that the benefits of the mindfulness did not last through the original video, post-event information, or the memory test (depending on the condition). If participants had completed a multi-session mindfulness course, the effects of the mindfulness may have lasted longer and, therefore, may have had an effect on susceptibility to misinformation. Although this is the first study examining the effects of a mindfulness induction on susceptibility to misinformation, there have been a few studies examining the effects of mindfulness on susceptibility to other types of false memories (Rosenstreich, 2016; Wilson et al., 2015). Those studies found that mindfulness increased false memories, which is inconsistent with my findings. One possible reason for these conflicting results is that DRM effects and misinformation effects are unrelated and therefore susceptibility to them is unrelated. For example, Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) found that DRM errors were not related to imagined childhood events. Therefore, although DRM effects and misinformation effects both seem to stem from source monitoring errors, the sources that need to be monitored are different. In DRM tasks, participants need to monitor the sources of both externally presented stimuli and internally presented stimuli while in misinformation tasks, participants need to monitor two external sources of information. Specifically, the internal source monitoring error may be due to a spreading activation of semantic memory (Calvillo & Parong, 2016). The difference in processing of the false memories within these two paradigms may explain why the results of the current study are dissimilar to past mindfulness and false memory research. Many factors have been found to influence susceptibility to misinformation. Zhu et al., (2010) found that acceptance of misinformation was negatively related to intelligence measures MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 28 of the Wechsler Scale. Similarly, Howard and Hong (2002) found misinformation from interviews was negatively related to intelligence measures on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS). Working memory capacity is another factor that has been found to have a negative relationship with susceptibility to misinformation. Calvillo (2014), Jaschinski and Wentura (2002) and Zhu et al., (2010) all found that working memory was negatively related to susceptibility to misinformation. There has also been research on personality factors and their effects on susceptibility to misinformation. For example, Gudjonsson (1988) found a positive relationship between anxiety and misinformation effect, and Howard and Hong (2002) found a positive relationship between emotional coping strategies and misinformation effects. Past research suggests that there are many factors that influence susceptibility to misinformation, and it is possible that changing only one aspect (being more mindful) was not be enough to elicit a significant effect. There are some limitations to the current study. The first limitation is the short mindfulness induction. Future research may want to use multiple mindfulness sessions or mindfulness programs such as a mindfulness based stress reduction (MBSR). It has been shown that having participants complete more than one brief mindfulness induction leads to continuous changes in both state and trait mindfulness (Kiken, Garland, Bluth, Palsson & Gaylord, 2015). Therefore having participants complete multiple mindfulness inductions may lead to significant findings pertaining to the misinformation effect. Another limitation was that the data were nonnormally distributed. The way in which the memory test was executed could also be a limitation. The memory test was a 16-item true-or-false recognition test and did not include any follow-up questions. Although this type of memory test is often used in misinformation studies, source memory tests could also be beneficial. In source memory tests, participants are usually given MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 29 either a free recall or multiple choice recognition test then are asked to report where they remember seeing that information (the original video or post-event information). Including this source memory test could help to identify if the participant remembered seeing the information or if he or she was simply guessing. While including a source memory test would not change the results, it could provide information not obtained with a true-or-false recognition test. The findings from this study contribute to the misinformation literature by demonstrating that a mindfulness induction, which has been shown to affect other forms of false memories, does not influence the misinformation effect. Studying the misinformation paradigm is important to help understand what makes eyewitnesses more or less likely to include misleading post-event information into their memory of the original event. Understanding what makes people more or less prone to misinformation errors can help understand what makes eyewitnesses more reliable. Due to the important implications the misinformation effect can have, it is important to continue to expand research on the subject. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 30 References Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210, 1226-1231. Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1849-1858. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007 Barker, K. (2014). Mindfulness meditation: Do-it-yourself medicalization of every moment. Social Science & Medicine, 106, 168-176. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.024 Berkowitz, S. R., Laney, C., Morris, E. K., Garry, M., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Pluto behaving badly: False beliefs and their consequences. The American Journal of Psychology, 121, 643-660. doi: 10.2307/20445490 Calvillo, D. P. (2014). Individual differences in susceptibility to misinformation effects and hindsight bias. Journal of General Psychology, 141, 393-40. doi:10.1080/00221309.2014.954917 Calvillo, D. P., & Hawkins, W. C. (2017). Working memory capacity and the misinformation effect: A meta-analysis. Manuscript submitted for publication. Calvillo, D. P., & Parong, J. A. (2016). The misinformation effect is unrelated to the DRM effect with and without a DRM warning. Memory, 24, 324-333. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1005633 Chambers, R., Lo, B., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 303322. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 31 Donaldson, W. (1992). Measuring recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 275–277. doi:10.1037/00963445.121.3.275 Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19-23. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00160 English, S. M., & Nielson, K. A. (2010). Reduction of the misinformation effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition, 117, 237-242. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.014 Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 1-11. doi: 10.3758/BF03203630 Erisman, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2010). A preliminary investigation of the effects of experimentally induced mindfulness on emotional responding to film clips. Emotion, 10, 72–82. doi: 10.1037/a0017162 Gerrie, M. P., & Garry, M. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity affect false memories for missing aspects of events. Memory, 15, 561-571. doi: 10.1080/09658210701391634 Gudjonsson, G. H. (1988). Interrogative suggestibility: Its relationship with assertiveness, socialevaluative anxiety, state anxiety and method of coping. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 159–166 Hardt, O., Einarsson, E.Ö., & Nader, K. (2010). A bridge over troubled water: Reconsolidation as a link between cognitive and neuroscientific memory research traditions. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 141–167. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100455 Howard, R., & Hong, N. S. (2002). Effects of coping style on interrogative suggestibility. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 479–485. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 32 Jaschinski, U., & Wentura, D. (2002). Misleading post-event information and working memory capacity: An individual differences approach to eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 223-231. doi: 10.1002/acp.783 Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10, 54-64. doi:10.1037/a0018438 Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3-28. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are. New York: Hyperion. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall Inc. Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R., Bleckley, M. K., & Engle, R. W., A controlled-attention view of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169-183. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.169 Kiken, L. G., Garland, E. L., Bluth, K., Palsson, O. S., & Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a state to a trait: Trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personality and Individual differences, 81, 41-46. Lane, S. (2006). Dividing attention during a witnessed event increases eyewitness suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 199-212. doi; 10.1002/acp.1177 Lau, M., Bishop, S., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N., Carlson, L…Carmody, J. (2006). The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1445-1467. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20326 Lippelt, D. P., Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2014). Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: Effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity—A MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 33 review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. Retrieved from http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01083/full Loftus, E. F. (2003). Make-believe memories. American Psychologist, 58, 867-873. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.11.867 Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation into the human mind: A 30-year investigation into the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12, 361-366. doi: 10.1101/lm.94705 Lloyd, M., Szani, A., Rubenstein, K., Colgary, C., & Pereira-Pasarin, L. (2016). A brief mindfulness exercise before retrieval reduces recognition memory false alarms. Mindfulness, 7, 606-613. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0495-y Lueke, A., & Gibson, B. (2016). Brief mindfulness meditation reduces discrimination. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 3, 34-44. doi: 10.1037/cns0000081 Lykins, E. B., Baer, R. A., & Gottlob, L. R. (2012). Performance-based tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy And Research, 36, 103-114. doi:10.1007/s10608-0109318-y Modern Marvels. (1997). Radio: Out of thin air. Retrieved from http://www.youtube.com/modern-marvels/episode-guide/radio-radio-out-of-thin-air/ Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness, and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 176-186. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008 Mrazek, M., Franklin, M., Phillips, D., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24, 776-781. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459659 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 34 Oberauer, K., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Lewandowsky, S. (2016). What limits working memory capacity? Psychological Bulletin, 142, 758-799. doi: 10.1037/bul0000046 Ortner, C., & Kilner, S. (2007). Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 271-283. doi: 10.1007/s11031-007-9076-7 Ost, J., Blank, H., Davies, J., Jones, G., Lambert, K., & Salmon, K. (2013). False memory ≠ false memory: DRM errors are unrelated to the misinformation effect. PLoSOne, 8, e57939. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0057939.t003 Porter, S., Brinke, L. T., Riley, S. N., & Baker, A. (2014). Prime time news: The influence of primed positive and negative emotion on susceptibility to false memories. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1422-1434. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.887000 Porter, S., Spencer, L. & Birt, A. (2003). Blinded by emotion? Effect of the emotionality of a scene on susceptibility to false memories. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 35, 165-175. doi: 10.1037/h0087198 Quach, D., Mano, K. J., & Alexander, K. (2015). A randomized controlled trial examining the effect of mindfulness meditation on working memory capacity in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 58, 489-496. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.09.024 Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803-814. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803 Rosenstreich, E. (2015). Mindfulness and false-memories: The impact of mindfulness practice on the DRM paradigm. The Journal of Psychology, 150, 58-71. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2015.1004298 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 35 Semple, R. (2010). Does mindfulness meditation enhance attention? A randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness, 1, 121-130. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0017-2 Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34 –50. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34 Takarangi, M., Parker, S., & Garry, M. (2006). Modernizing the misinformation effect: The development of a new stimulus set. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 583-590. doi: 10.1002/acp.1209 Tang, Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2016). Mindfulness meditation improves emotion regulation and reduces drug abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 163, S13-S18. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.11.041 Van Damme, I., & Seynaeve, L. (2013). The effect of mood on confidence in false memories. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 309-318. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.769440 Van Damme, I. & Smets, K. (2014). The power of emotion versus the power of suggestion: Memory for emotional events in the misinformation paradigm. Emotion, 14, 310-320. doi: 10.1037/a0034629 Wade, K. A., Garry, M., Read, J. D., & Lindsay, S. (2002). A picture is worth a thousand lies: Using false photographs to create false childhood memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 597-603. doi:10.3758/BF03196318 Watier, N., & Dubois, M. (2016). The effects of a brief mindfulness exercise on executive attention and recognition memory. Mindfulness, 7, 745-753. doi: 10.1007/s12671-0160514-z MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 36 Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 11, 42-58. Wilkinson, C., & Hyman, I. E. (1998). Individual differences related to two types of memory errors: Word lists may not generalize to autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, S29– S46. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720 Wilson, B. M., Mickes, L., Stolarz-Fantino, S., Evrard, M., & Fantino, E. (2015). Increased false-memory susceptibility after mindfulness meditation. Psychological Science, 26, 1567-1573. doi: 10.1177/0956797615593705 Xiong, G. L., & Doraiswamy, P. M. (2009). Does meditation enhance cognition and brain plasticity? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172, 63-69. Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. M. (1994). Source misattributions and the suggestibility of eyewitness memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 934. Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. M. (1998). Processing resources and eyewitness suggestibility. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 305-320. doi: 10.1111/j.2a0448333.1998.tb00368.x Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 19, 597-605. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014 Zhu, B., Chen, C. L. H., Loftus, E. F., Ling, C., He, Q., Chen, C. L. H., ... Dong, Q. (2010). Individual differences in false memory from misinformation: Cognitive factors. Memory, 18, 543–555. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2010.487051 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 37 Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for Endorsement of Statement Type by Condition OV Only PEI Only OV & PEI Foil M 95% CI M 95% CI M 95% CI M 95% CI Control .66 [.61, .72] .35 [.30, .41] .94 [.92, .97] .14 [.10, .18] Pre-video .65 [.60, .70] .44 [.37, .50] .94 [.91, .96] .14 [.10, .18] Pre-PEI .64 [.59, .69] .41 [.35, .45] .91 [.88, .95] .19 [.15, .24] Pre-test .70 [.66, .75] .39 [.32, .46] .94 [.91, .97] .15 [.11, .19] Note. OV only = statements true in original video only; PEI only = statements true in post-event information only; OV & PEI = statements true in both original video and post-event information. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION Figure 1. Procedure of proposed study. The control group did not get the mindfulness induction. The pre-video group received the mindfulness induction before the original event. The pre-PEI group received the mindfulness induction before the post-event information. The pre-test group received the mindfulness induction before the memory test. 38 MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 39 Mean Magnatude of Misinformation 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Control Pre-video Pre-PEI Condition Pre-test Figure 2. Mean magnitude of misinformation effect across conditions. Error bars represent 95% CI. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 40 1 Mean A' Scores 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 Control Pre-video Pre-PEI Condition Pre-test Figure 3. Mean A’ scores across conditions. Error bars represent 95% CI. MINDFULNESS AND MISINFORMATION 41 Mean B’’D Scores 0.2 0.1 0 -0.1 -0.2 -0.3 Control Pre-video Pre-PEI Condition Pre-test Figure 4. Mean B’’D scores across conditions. Error bars represent 95% CI.