* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Convergent song preferences between female
Human male sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Female ejaculation wikipedia , lookup
Sexual coercion wikipedia , lookup
Human female sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Female promiscuity wikipedia , lookup
Sexual reproduction wikipedia , lookup
Sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup
Body odour and sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup
Age disparity in sexual relationships wikipedia , lookup
Behavioral Ecology Vol. 7 No. 3: 279-285 Convergent song preferences between female field crickets and acoustically orienting parasitoid flies William E. Wagner, Jr. Nebraska Behavioral Biology Group, School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA Traits that increase the attractiveness of males to females often make them more conspicuous to predators. In the field cricket (Gryilus lineaticeps), males are attacked by parasitoid tachinid flies (Ormia ochracea) that locate males through their calls. Female flies larviposit on crickets and the larvae burrow into and feed on the cricket, killing the cricket upon emergence. To determine whether traits preferred by females increase a male's risk of attracting a predator, I examined the effect of variation in male singing behavior on mate and predator attraction. Both female crickets and female flies preferred male calling songs with higher chirp rates, longer chirp durations, and higher chirp amplitudes. In addition, both female crickets and female flies preferred male calling songs with higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations, even when these songs were of lower amplitude. These results suggest that sexual selection by female choice will favor the evolution of higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations. However, call types that increase a male's attractiveness to females also appear to increase a male's risk of attracting parasitoids. Sexual and natural selection appear to have opposing effects on the evolution of male singing behavior in this species. Key words: female choice, field crickets, Gryilus lineaticeps, Ormia, parasitoid, predation, sexual selection, tachinid flies. [Behav Ecol 7:279-285 (1996)] I n many animals males possess conspicuous structures or produce elaborate displays that function, in part, to attract females for mating (reviewed by Darwin, 1871; Kirkpatrick, 1987b; Searcy and Andersson, 1986; Thornhill and Alcock, 1983). Females often select mates based on variation in male morphology or behavior, resulting in sexual selection on these traits (Endler, 1980; Price, 1984; Ryan, 1985; Sullivan, 1987). Male traits preferred by females also can make a male more conspicuous to predators (Endler, 1980; Ryan et al., 1982). Sexual selection by female choice and natural selection by predation may thus have opposing effects on the evolution of male traits. In general, the state of male traits preferred by females is thought to reflect a balance between natural and sexual selection (Endler, 1982; Fisher, 1958). In field crickets, males commonly sing from within or near burrows to attract females (Alexander, 1961), and females often select mates based on variation in male calling song (Hedrick, 1986; Popov and Shuvalov, 1977; Simmons, 1988; Walker, 1957). The calling song of some species also attracts parasitoid female tachinid flies of the genus Ormia (e.g., Cade, 1975; Walker, 1986). These flies possess a unique hearing organ that allows them to locate singing males (Robert et al., 1992), and female flies discriminate between species of field cricket based on male calling songs (Walker, 1986, 1993). Once a male cricket is located, a female fly deposits larvae on and around the cricket (Cade, 1975). The larvae burrow into the cricket, feed, and grow. Approximately 7 days after entering a cricket the larvae emerge and pupate, killing the host. Fly predation may favor both the evolution of noncalling, satellite mating tactics and reduced nightly singing duration in male field crickets (Cade, 1975, 1979; Cade and Wyatt, 1984). In addition, fly predation may produce evolutionary changes in male calling song structure (Zuk et al., 1993). It Received 27 February 1995; revised 11 July 1995; final revision 25 August 1995; accepted 25 August 1995. 1045-2249/96/S5.00 © 1996 International Society for Behavioral Ecology is not known, however, if, within a species, some types of calling songs are more likely to attract flies than others, although studies have shown that interspecific variation in song structure can affect the attractiveness of songs to flies (Walker, 1993). Flies might discriminate between cricket song types for at least two reasons. First, some types of song may be easier for flies to localize. Second, songs may contain information about host quality. Other parasitoids are known to discriminate between hosts based on host quality (reviewed by Godfray, 1994). In this article I test the hypothesis that parasitoid flies are preferentially attracted to the same types of calling songs as females in the field cricket Gryilus lineaticeps. Male singing behavior can vary in a number of ways, including the rate at which chirps are produced, the duration of chirps, and the rate at which pulses are produced within chirps (Wagner WE, unpublished data; Weissman et al., 1980; Figure 1). While G. lineaticeps has not previously been reported to be a host for Ormia, larvae of O. ochracea have emerged from male crickets that I collected in the field. First, I examined the influence of male chirp rate and chirp duration on mate and fly attraction. Second, I examined the influence of chirp amplitude on mate and fly attraction. Although chirp amplitude can potentially vary among males, the amplitude of a sound for the receiver primarily depends on the distance of the receiver from the animal producing the signal (e.g., Michelsen, 1985; Piercy et al., 1977). Consequently, chirp amplitude likely provides information primarily about a male's distance. Third, I examined the relative importance of chirp rate and chirp duration in mate and fly attraction. Fourth, I examined the amplitude dependence of mate and fly preferences based on chirp rate and chirp duration. In some animals mating preferences can be eliminated by reducing the amplitude of the preferred signal (e.g., Gerhardt, 1991), suggesting that these animals either prefer signals that are more conspicuous or are unwilling to travel longer distances to mate with a male producing a preferred signal type. Behavioral Ecology Vol. 7 No. 3 280 Table 1 Stimuli used to test female cricket and By song preferences Test Stimulus 1 2 3 300 4 Figure 1 Waveform of the calling song of a male field cricket, G. limaticeps. METHODS Preferences of female field crickets Female choice based on variation in male calling song was tested in the laboratory from 1 July to 11 July 1994. Females used in the tests were offspring of field-caught crickets from Tucker's Grove County Park, Santa Barbara, California. Crickets were raised in a common culture (a 45 1 plastic trash can) provisioned with water, cat food, oviposition dishes containing moist vermiculite, and cardboard egg cartons. Prior to their final molt, females were removed from the culture and isolated in individual containers to ensure that all females tested were virgins. Females were tested between 10 and 21 days after their final molt Females are both sexually mature and receptive to mating at this time (Wagner WE, personal observation). Female preferences were tested using six pairs of stimuli (Table 1). The chirp rates and chirp durations used fall within the natural range of variation for male calling song; chirp rates vary between 0.83 and 4.65 chirps/s, while chirp durations vary between 0.086 and 0.211 s (Wagner WE, unpublished data). The stimuli were constructed by digitizing a male chirp using a Macintosh Ilci computer, a MacRecorder sound digitizer, and SoundEdit Pro software. A single pulse was selected from this natural chirp and used to construct artificial chirps (pulse duration = 14 ms, pulse rate of artificial chirps = 0.038 pulses/ms). First, female preferences based on chirp rate were tested by giving females a choice between male songs that varied only in chirp rate. Second, female preferences based on chirp duration were tested by giving females a choice between male songs that varied only in chirp duration. Third, female preferences based on chirp amplitude were tested by giving females a choice between male songs that varied only in amplitude. The amplitude of the high amplitude stimulus was twice that of the low amplitude stimulus. Fourth, the relative importance of chirp rate and chirp duration was examined. Females were given a choice between a song of high chirp rate and short chirp duration and a song of low chirp rate and long chirp duration. The overall acoustic energy of these stimuli was equal. A preference for the former stimulus would suggest that chirp rate is more important to females, while a preference for the latter stimulus would suggest that chirp duration is more important to females. Fifth, the amplitude dependence of female preferences based on chirp rate was examined. Females were given a choice between a song of high chirp rate and low chirp amplitude and a song of low chirp rate and high chirp amplitude. Sixth, the amplimde dependence of female preferences based on chirp duration was examined. Females were given a choice between 5 6 High chirp rate Low chirp rate Long chirp duration Short chirp duration High chirp amplitude Low chirp amplitude High rate short duration Low rate long duration High rate low amplitude Low rate high amplitude Long duration low amplitude Short duration high amplitude Chirp Chirp rate duration (s) (/s) Chirp amplitude (dB SPL)' 2 1 2 2 2 2 2 75 75 75 75 75 69 75 75 69 75 69 75 1 2 1 2 2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.1 0.2 0.2 0.1 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.1 (100) (100) (100) (100) (100) (94) (100) (100) (94) (100) (94) (100) ' Chirp amplitude differed between female cricket and female fly preference tests. Chirp amplitudes for fly preference tests are given in parentheses. Amplitude in decibels is re 20 jJ-Pa, measured at a distance of 1 m from the speaker. a song of long chirp duration and low chirp amplitude and a song of short chirp duration and high chirp amplitude. Female preferences were tested in a 1.22 X 0.61 X 0.31 m (1 X w X h) wooden arena. The sides were covered with foam to improve the localizability of broadcast sound. A Realistic 40-1259A amplified speaker was placed at each end of the arena. A 40 watt red light allowed observation of females within the arena. Prior to a test, a female was placed under an opaque plastic dish equidistant between the speakers and allowed to acclimate for 10 min. A small piece of cardboard egg carton provided shelter for the female. A pair of stimuli were then broadcast from a Toshiba KT-4529 personal cassette player through the speakers; the stimuli were recorded on different tracks, and each stimulus was broadcast through a different speaker. Stimulus amplitudes were adjusted prior to the beginning of each test using a Realistic Sound Level Meter. The dish was removed and the female was observed until she contacted one of the speakers, at which time a choice was considered to have occurred. Some females failed to respond after 20 min. These females were removed from the arena and were retested on a subsequent night Between the tests, the stimuli were switched between speakers to control for potential side biases. To determine whether females preferred one alternative stimulus over another, the number of females selecting each stimulus was compared with a two-tailed binomial test (Sokal and Rohlf, 1981). Females that continued to respond were occasionally tested with more than one pair of stimuli within a night. Most females were tested on more than one night, although each female was tested only once with a given pair of stimuli. All tests were conducted between 18.3°C and 19.8°C (x = 19.1°C). The chirp rate and chirp duration stimuli fall within the natural range of variation for males calling at this temperature (Wagner WE, unpublished data). Preferences of parasitoid flies The influence of variation in male singing behavior on fly attraction was tested in the field from 18 September to 7 October 1992 at Tucker's Grove County Park. Two Realistic 401259A amplified speakers were placed 1 m apart at the edge of a dirt and grass clearing from which male G. lineaticeps had previously called. During the experiments no calling males were within 40 m of the speakers (any males present were relocated). Alternative stimuli were then broadcast through Wagner • Convergent song preferences Test 281 Stimuli Number of female crickets Number of female flies (a) High chirp rate 13** 23** 12* 19* 12* 20** Low chirp rate (b) Long chirp duration Short chirp duration (c) High chirp amplitude Low chirp amplitude * P < 0.05, • • P < 0.01, *•• P < 0.001 the speakers from a Toshiba KT-4529 personal cassette recorder. The stimuli used were the same as those used to examine female cricket preferences (see above), except that the stimuli were broadcast at a higher amplitude (Table 1). The higher amplitudes were used to maximize the area over which the songs were detectable by flies to increase the number of flies attracted to the playbacks. Playbacks were started approximately 15 min prior to sunset, and continued until approximately 45 min after sunset. O. ochracea approach broadcast song primarily in the period shortly before and after sunset. All tests were conducted between 17.3°C and 21.8°C (x = 18.5°C). In response to the song playbacks, female flies either landed directly on one of the speakers or landed within 40 cm of a speaker and walked toward it. Once in contact with a speaker, flies generally moved over the surface of the speaker, apparently searching for the calling male cricket. Flies that contacted a speaker were collected and counted. To determine whether flies preferred one type of song to another, the number of flies landing on each speaker was compared with a twotailed binomial test. Most of the experiments were conducted over a number of nights (x = 2.0 nights, SE = 0.26). Stimuli were switched between speakers between nights to help con- Figure 2 The preferences of female field crickets (G. lineaticeps) and female tachinid flies (O. ockracea) based on variation in (a) chirp rate, (b) chirp duration, and (c) chirp amplitude of male calling song. The number of female field crickets and female tachinid flies attracted to each stimulus is compared with a two-tailed binomial test trol for potential position effects. In addition, collected flies were preserved to prevent individual flies from responding in the same test both within and between nights. RESULTS Single song characters Chirp rate, duration, and amplitude all influenced female mate choice and the probability of fly attraction. When presented with songs that varied only in chirp rate, significantly more female crickets and female flies were attracted to the higher chirp rate song (Figure 2a). When presented with songs that varied only in chirp duration, significantly more female crickets and female flies were attracted to the longer chirp duration song (Figure 2b). And when presented with songs that varied only in chirp amplitude, significantly more female crickets and female flies were attracted to the higher chirp amplitude song (Figure 2c). These results suggest that female C. lineaticeps prefer higher chirp rates, longer chirp durations, and higher chirp amplitudes. These results also suggest that males with these call characters have a higher risk of attracting parasitoid flies. Behavioral Ecology Vol. 7 No. 3 282 Test Stimuli Number of female crickets Number of female flies (a) Long chirp duration Low chirp rate 22 Short chirp duration High chirp rate 19 (b) High chirp rate Low chirp amplitude -HttHH 13** Low chirp rate High chirp amplitude Figure 3 The preferences of female field crickets (G. lineaticeps) and female tachinid flies (O. ochracea) based on (a) simultaneous variation in chirp rate and chirp duration, (b) simultaneous variation in chirp rate and chirp amplitude, and (c) simultaneous variation in chirp duration and chirp amplitude in male calling song. The number of female field crickets and female tachinid flics attracted to each stimulus is compared with a two-tailed binomial test. 38* 21 (c) Long chirp duration Low chirp amplitude 15* 26** Short chirp duration High chirp amplitude * P < 0.05, •* P < 0.01, *** P < 0.001 Multiple song characters Chirp rate and chirp duration did not appear to differ in their relative importance to female crickets and female flies. Two songs were presented that simultaneously varied in chirp rate and chirp duration: one song contained chirps 100 ms in duration at a rate of 2 chirps/s while the other song contained chirps 200 ms in duration at a rate of 1 chirp/s. The acoustic energy of the two songs was thus equal. The number of female crickets and the number of female flies attracted to each song type did not significantly differ (Figure 3a). The influence of variation in chirp rate and chirp duration on both female mate choice and the probability of fly attraction were at least partially independent of chirp amplitude. When presented with songs of 2 chirps/s and 1 chirp/s, where the amplitude of the former was half that of the latter, significantly more female crickets and female flies were attracted to the high chirp rate/low chirp amplitude song (Figure 3b). The number of female crickets and female flies attracted to the high versus low chirp rate song did not significantly differ from the experiments where chirp amplitude was constant (female crickets: G = 0.00, df = 1, p = 1.00; female flies: G = 2.12, df = 1, p > .20). Similarly, when presented with songs with chirps of 200 ms and 100 ms, where the amplitude of the former was half that of the latter, significantly more female crickets and female flies were attracted to the long chirp duration/low chirp amplitude song (Figure 3c). The number of female crickets and the number of female flies attracted to the long versus short chirp duration song did not significantly differ from the experiments where chirp amplitude was constant (female crickets: G = 0.12, df = 1, p > .90; female flies: G = 1.61, df = 1, p > .40). These results suggest that variation in chirp amplitude does not have a major effect on the preferences of female crickets and female flies based on other chirp characteristics. DISCUSSION Sexual and natural selection on male calling song G. lineaticeps calling songs with higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations were more likely to attract both conspecific females and parasitoid flies (O. ochracea). The song preferences of female G. lineaticeps are similar to those of other crickets where females prefer longer calls or calls produced at a more rapid rate (e.g., Hedrick 1986; Pollack and Hoy, 1981; Popov and Shuvalov, 1977; Stout and McGhee, 1988; Wagner et al., 1995). While female flies are known to discrim- Wagner • Convergent song preferences 283 and Ryan, 1981). Similarly, in G. lineaticeps, sexual selection inate between the calling songs of different species of cricket by female choice and natural selection by fly predation should (Walker, 1986, 1993), little is known about the preferences of have opposing effects on the evolution of male singing befemale flies based on intraspecific variation in song structure. havior. Both female crickets and female flies preferred higher chirp amplitudes. Because the amplitude of a chirp at any given location will depend primarily on the distance of the The evolution of female mating preferences male producing the chirp, these results suggest that female crickets and female flies prefer closer males. Mating preferFemale mating preferences can evolve because of Fisherian ences based on amplitude are common in species where feprocesses (Kirkpatrick, 1982; Lande, 1981), because of assomales select mates based on acoustic signals (Gerhardt, 1991; ciations between female preferences and female fitness (reRyan and Keddy-Hector, 1992; Walker and Forrest, 1989), but viewed by Kirkpatrick and Ryan, 1991; Thorn hill and Alcock, little is known about the amplitude preferences of acoustically 1983), because of differences between males in heritable comorienting predators. These results are not surprising, however, ponents of fitness (Grafen, 1990; Kirkpatrick, 1987a; Pomiangiven that higher chirp amplitudes often mean shorter travel kowski, 1987; Zahavi, 1975), or because of evolutionary prodistances, and given that search costs are expected to influcesses independent of sexual selection on the trait in question ence both female preferences (Kirkpatrick, 1987a; Pomian(Basolo, 1990; Endler and McClellan, 1988; Kirkpatrick, kowski, 1987; Real, 1990) and foraging behavior (e.g., God1987b; Ryan, 1990). In contrast, search costs associated with fray, 1994; Stephens and Krebs, 1986). Despite these preferfemale preferences are expected to favor less discriminating ences for higher amplitude chirps, when presented with songs females (Kirkpatrick, 1987a; Maynard Smith, 1991; Pomianof high and low chirp rate, where the high chirp song rate kowski, 1987). was of lower amplitude, both female crickets and female flies The reason female G. lineaticeps prefer higher chirp rates preferred the higher chirp rate song. Similarly, when preand longer chirp durations is not known. In some crickets sented with songs of long and short duration, where the long song characters preferred by females may be indicative of largchirp duration song was of lower amplitude, both female er male size (e.g., Simmons, 1988), and larger male size may crickets and female flies preferred the longer chirp duration be indicative of greater material benefits or higher fitness. In song. These results suggest that female crickets and female G. lineaticeps, however, neither chirp rate nor chirp duration flies will often bypass closer males producing less preferred is correlated with male size (Wagner WE, unpublished data). songs in favor of more distant males producing more preFemale choice based on these characters thus will not result ferred songs. in matings with larger males. It is not known whether chirp amplitude is correlated with male size. However, because variSome male traits may be more important than others both ation in chirp amplitude will primarily result from variation to conspecific females and to eavesdropping predators (e.g., in male distance and variation in male position relative to a Gerhardt, 1991; Wells and Taigen, 1986). However, neither female (i.e., inside or outside a burrow), it seems unlikely that female crickets nor female flies discriminated between high female choice based on this character will generally result in chirp rate/short chirp duration song and low chirp rate/long matings with larger males (but see Arak, 1988). chirp duration song. There is thus no evidence that these traits differ in their importance to either female crickets and In many animals, females prefer more energetic displays female flies, although differences still might exist in the rel(Ryan and Keddy-Hector, 1992). Because males with higher ative importance of chirp rate and chirp duration to individchirp rates and longer chirp durations produce songs conuals of both groups. taining greater acoustic energy, it is possible that female G. lineaticeps simply prefer more conspicuous song, either beFemale crickets often have the opportunity to exercise mate cause such song results in greater neural stimulation or bechoice under natural conditions (Loherand Dambach, 1989). cause such song is easier to localize. However, in tests where Because females preferred higher chirp rates and longer the acoustic energy contained in male calls was controlled, chirp durations, directional sexual selection should favor higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations in G. lineaticeps. females preferred higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations despite these song types being of lower amplitude. These It is possible that measurable preferences may not result in results suggest that females do not simply prefer songs of sexual selection on the preferred male trait, even when fegreater energy, but instead prefer higher chirp rates and lonmales have the opportunity to exercise mate choice (e.g., ger chirp durations per se; because a song that is half the Wagner and Sullivan, 1995). However, female preferences ofamplitude of another song is generally twice as far away, feten result in sexual selection if females have the opportunity males appear willing to travel at least twice as far to mate with to exercise mate choice (reviewed by Andersson, 1994; Kirka male producing a preferred song type. The reason that fepatrick, 1987b; Searcy and Andersson, 1986). males prefer males with higher chirp rates and longer chirp Previous work on field crickets has suggested that fly predurations remains to be determined. Possibilities include redation has influenced the evolution of noncalling, satellite duced search costs due to ease of localization, male genetic behavior in male field crickets (Cade, 1975, 1979), the evoquality (Grafen, 1990; Heywood, 1989; Pomiankowski, 1988; lution of a reduced nightly duration of male calling (Cade Zahavi, 1975), spermatophore quantity or quality (Zuk, 1987), and Wyatt, 1984; Zuk et al., 1993), and evolutionary changes and avoidance of transmissible parasites (Borgia and Collis, in calling song structure (Zuk et al., 1993). The expression of 1990). song preferences by female flies likewise suggests that fly predation will have an important consequence for male singing behavior; fly predation should result in selection against higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations in G. lineaticeps call- The evolution of fly preferences ing song. In other species predation is known to counter sexFemale O. ochracea show extensive geographic variation in ual selection by female choice. In guppies (Poedlia reticulata), their hosts. In central Texas they utilize G. integer (Cade, females prefer males with red coloration, a trait that increases 1975), in Florida they utilize G. rubensand G. firmus (Walker, a male's probability of being eaten by a predator (Endler, 1986; Walker and Wineriter, 1991), m California they utilize 1980, 1982). In a frog (Physalaemus puslulosus), females pre- G. lineaticeps (this article), and in Hawaii, where the fly apfer males that produce chucks, a behavior that increases a pears to have been introduced, they utilize an introduced male's risk of being eaten by acoustically orienting bats (Tuttle field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus (Zuk et al., 1993). A num- 284 ber of observations suggest that there are evolved differences between populations in the responses of O. ochracea to cricket calling song. In central Texas, flies respond readily to the trilled calling song of G. integer (Cade, 1975), but in Florida, where C. integer does not occur, they rarely respond to G. integer song (Walker, 1993). In Florida, flies respond readily to the trilled calling song of G. rubens but rarely respond to the chirped calling song of G. firmus (Walker, 1993). In contrast, flies readily respond to the chirped calling song of G. lineaticeps in California (this article) and of T. oceanicus in Hawaii (Zuk et al., 1993). As the results presented here illustrate, female O. ochracea not only discriminate between the songs of potential host species, but they also discriminate between song types within a given host species. The reason that female O. ochracea prefer G. lineaticeps song of higher chirp rate and longer chirp duration is currently unclear. In general, the factors influencing the evolution of host preferences in O. ochracea should be similar to those that influence foraging behavior in other animals, including other parasitoids, such as search costs and food or host profitability (reviewed by Godfray, 1994; Stephens and Krebs, 1986). First, higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations may be easier for flies to localize, even when such songs are of much lower amplitude. Female flies may thus minimize their search costs by orienting to such songs. Second, male crickets producing higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations may be better hosts for a female fly's larvae. For example, males producing higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations may be in better physiological condition, and male crickets in better physiological condition may be more likely to survive until larval emergence, may be capable of supporting more larvae, and may provide more resources for each larva, resulting in larger or more viable larvae. Third, the presence of larvae in a male may affect male singing behavior, as it is known to affect other aspects of male behavior (Adamo et al., 1995a; Cade and Wyatt, 1984). If larvae deposited on a previously infested male have a reduced probability of successful emergence or reduced pupal size (e.g., Adamo et al., 1995b), the preferences of female flies may enhance offspring survivorship or fitness. Fourth, males with higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations may be more likely to have conspecific females within or near their burrows. Female flies may prefer such males because there is a higher probability that two or more hosts will be present rather than one (e.g., Walker and Wineriter, 1991). Host preferences based on host quality are common in animals (see Godfray, 1994), and female flies may prefer some song types because they provide information about a male cricket's suitability as a host or information about the number of hosts that may be present. I thank the Santa Barbara Parks and Recreation Department for permission to conduct research in Tucker's Grove County Park, and J. A. Endler for use of his laboratory. I also thank A. L. Basolo, W. H. Cade, D. W. Leger, D. J. Mountjoy, A.-M. Murray, L. W. Simmons, T. J. Walker, and an anonymous reviewer for their criticism of the manuscript. The research was supported by North Atlantic Treaty Organization and National Science Foundation postdoctoral fellowships. Preparation of the manuscript was supported by the National Science Foundation's Experimental Program to Stimulate Competitive Research, through funding of the Nebraska Behavioral Biology Group. REFERENCES Adamo SA, Robert D, Hoy RR, 1995a. Effects of a tachmid parasitoid, Ormia ochracea, on the behaviour and reproduction of its male and female field cricket hosts (GryUus spp). J Insect Physiol 41:269-277. Adamo SA, Robert D, Perez J, Hoy RR, 1995b. The response of an insect parasitoid, Ormia ochracea (Tachinidae), to the uncertainty Behavioral Ecology Vol. 7 No. 3 of larval success during infestation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36:111118. Alexander RD, 1961. Aggressiveness, territoriality, and sexual behavior in field crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Behaviour 17:130-223. Andersson M, 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Arak A, 1988. Female mate selection in the natterjack toad: active choice or passive attraction? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:317-327. Basolo AL, 1990. Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish. Science 250:808-810. Borgia G, Collis K, 1990. Parasites and bright male plumage in the satin bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus). Am Zool 30:279—285. Cade WH, 1975. Acousucally orienting parasitoids: fly phonotaxis to cricket song. Science 190:1312-1313. Cade WH, 1979. The evolution of alternative male reproductive strategies in field crickets. In: Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects (Blum MS, Blum NA, eds). New York: Academic Press; 343-379. Cade WH, Wyatt DR, 1984. Factors affecting calling behaviour in field crickets, TeUogryUus and GryUus (age, weight, density, and parasites). Behaviour 88:61-75. Darwin C, 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: Murray. Endler JA, McLellan T, 1988. The processes of evolution: towards a newer synthesis. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 19:395—421. Endler JA, 1980. Natural selection on color patterns in Poectlia retxculata. Evolution 31:78-91. Endler JA, 1982. Convergent and divergent effects of natural selection on color patterns in twofishfaunas. Evolution 36:178-188. Fisher RA, 1958. The genetical theory of natural selection, 2nd rev. ed. New York: Dover Press. Gerhardt HC, 1991. Female mate choice in treefrogs: static and dynamic acoustic criteria. Anim Behav 42:615-635. Godfray HCJ, 1994. Parasitoids: behavioral and evolutionary ecology. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Grafen A, 1990. Sexual selection unhandicapped by the Fisher process. J Theor Biol 144:473-516. Hedrick AV, 1986. Female preferences for male calling bout duration in a field cricket. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:73-77. HeywoodJS, 1989. Sexual selection by the handicap mechanism. Evolution 43:1387-1397. Kirkpatrick M, 1982. Sexual selection and the evoluuon of female choice. Evolution 36:1-12. Kirkpatrick M, 1987a. The evolutionary forces acting on female mating preferences in polygynous animals. In: Sexual selection: testing the alternatives (Bradbury JB, Andersson MB, eds). Chichesten John Wiley & Sons; 67-82. Kirkpatrick M, 1987b. Sexual selection by female choice in polygynous animals. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 18:43-70. Kirkpatrick M, Ryan MJ, 1991. The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek. Nature 127:403-409. Lande R, 1981. Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc Nat! Acad Sci USA 78:3721-3725. Loher W, Dambach M, 1989. Reproductive behavior. In: Cricket behavior and neurobiology (Huber F, Moore TE, Loher W, eds). Ithaca; Cornell University Press; 83-113. Maynard Smith J, 1991. Theories of sexual selection. Trends Ecol Evol 6:146-151. Michelsen A, 1985. Environmental aspect of sound communication in insects. In: Acoustic and vibrational communication in insects (Kalmring K, Eisner N, eds). Berlin: Paul Parey, 1-9. PiercyJE, Embleton TFW, Sutherland LC, 1977. Review of noise propagation in the atmosphere. J Acoust Soc Am 61:1403-1418. Pollack GS, Hoy R, 1981. Phonotaxis to individual rhythmic components of a complex cricket calling song. J Comp Physiol 144:367373. Pomiankowski A, 1987. The 'handicap principle' does work—sometimes. Proc R Soc Lond B 127:123-145. Pomiankowski A, 1988. The evolution of female mate preferences for male genetic quality. Oxford Survey Evol Biol 5:136-184. Popov AV, Shuvalov VF, 1977. Phonotactic behavior of crickets. J Comp Physiol 119:111-126. Price TD, 1984. Sexual selection on body size, territory and plumage variables in a population of Darwin's finches. Evolution 38:327-341. Wagner • Convergent song preferences Real L, 1990. Search theory and mate choice. I. Models of single-sex discrimination. Am Nat 136:376-405. Robert D, Amoroso J, Hoy RR, 1992. The evolutionary convergence of hearing in a parasitoid fly and its cricket host Science 258:11351137. Ryan MJ, 1985. The Tiingara frog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Ryan MJ, 1990. Sexual selection, sensory systems, and sensory exploitation. Oxford Survey Evol Biol 7:157-195. Ryan MJ, Keddy-Hector A, 1992. Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensor)' biases. Am Nat 139:S4-S35. Ryan MJ, Tuttle MD, Rand AS, 1982. Bat predation and sexual advertisement in a neotropical frog. Am Nat 119:136-139. Searcy WA, Andersson M, 1986. Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 17:507-534. Simmons LW, 1988. The calling song of the field cricket, Gryllus btmaculatus (De Geer): constraints on transmission and its role in intermale competition and female choice. Anim Behav 36:380-394. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ, 1981. Biometry, 2nd ed. New York: W. H. Freeman. Stephens DW, KrebsJR, 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Stout JF, McGhee R, 1988. Attractiveness of the male Acheta domatica calling song to females. II. The relative importance of syllable period, intensity, and chirp rate. J Comp Physiol 164:277-287. Sullivan BK, 1987. Sexual selection in Woodhouse's toad (Bufo woodhousei). III. Seasonal variation in male mating success. Anim Behav 35:912-919. Thornhill R, Alcock J, 1983. The evolution of insect mating systems. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Tuttle MD, Ryan MJ, 1981. Bat predation and the evolution of frog vocalizations in the neotropics. Science 214:677-678. Wagner WEJr, Sullivan BK, 1995. Sexual selection in the Gulf Coast 285 toad (Bufo valliceps): female choice based on variable characters. Anim Behav 49:305-319. Wagner WEJr, Murray A-M, Cade WH, 1995. Phenotypic variation in the mating preferences of female field crickets, Gryllus integer. Anim Behav 49:1269-1281. Walker TJ, 1957. Specificity in the response of female tree crickets (Orthoptera: Grylliclae: Oecanthinae) to calling songs of the males. Ann Entomol Soc Am 50:626-636. WalkerTJ, 1986. Monitoring the flights of field crickets (Gryllusspp.) and a tachinid fly (Euphasiopteryx ochracea) in North Florida. Florida Entomol 69:678-685. Walker TJ, 1993. Phonotaxis in female Ormia ochracea (Diptera, Tachinidae), a parasitoid of field crickets. J Insect Behav 6:389—410. Walker TJ, Forrest TG, 1989. Mole cricket phonotaxis—effects of intensity of synthetic calling song (Orthoptera, Gryllotalpidae, Scapteriscus acletus). Florida Entomol 72:655-659. Walker TJ, Wineriter SA, 1991. Hosts of a phonotactic parasitoid and levels of parasitism (Diptera: Tachinidae: Ormia ochracea). Florida Entomol 74:554-559. Weissman DB, Rentz DCF, Alexander RD, Loher W, 1980. Field crickets (Gryllus and Acheta) of California and Baja California, Mexico (Orthoptera: Gryllidae: Gryllinae). Trans Am Entomol Soc 106: 327-356. Wells KD, Taigen TL, 1986. The effect of social interactions on calling energetics in the gray treefrog (Hyla versicolor). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:9-18. Zahavi A, 1975. Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. J Theor Biol 39:66-82. Zuk M, 1987. The effects of gregarine parasites, body size, and time of day on spermatophore production and sexual selection in field crickets. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 21:65-72. Zuk M, Simmons LW, Cupp L, 1993. Calling characteristics of parasitized and unparasidzed populations of the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 33:339-343.