* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download self-disclosure in long-distance friendships
Social perception wikipedia , lookup
Interpersonal attraction wikipedia , lookup
Relationship counseling wikipedia , lookup
Relational transgression wikipedia , lookup
Impression management wikipedia , lookup
Internet relationship wikipedia , lookup
Interpersonal relationship wikipedia , lookup
James M. Honeycutt wikipedia , lookup
Intimate relationship wikipedia , lookup
SELF-DISCLOSURE IN LONG-DISTANCE FRIENDSHIPS: A COMPARISON BETWEEN FACE-TO-FACE AND COMPUTER-MEDIATED COMMUNICATION _______________ A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of San Diego State University _______________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Communication _______________ by Hua Wang Spring 2004 THE UNDERSIGNED FACULTY COMMITTEE APPROVES THE THESIS OF HUA WANG: ___________________________________________ Peter A. Andersen, Chair Department of Communication ___________________________________________ Brian H. Spitzberg Department of Communication ___________________________________________ Minjuan Wang Department of Educational Technology SAN DIEGO STATE UNIVERSITY Spring 2004 ____________ Date iii © 2004 by Hua Wang iv DEDICATION This thesis is dedicated To my parents and brother for encouraging me to pursue my dreams; To my good friends for embracing me with warmth and support; and To my caring husband for walking me through all the ups and downs, joys and tears…… v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I want to take this opportunity to acknowledge my gratitude to the people who have directly contributed to the completion of this thesis and made this happen. Let me start by thanking the members of my committee. Dr. Peter Andersen, I certainly could not have accomplished this much without your constant encouragement, advice, and inspirations! You are one of the best professors I have ever met, and I do mean it! I have learned so much from you through this process, from academic professionalism, balanced lifestyle, to ways of communicating with other people and strategies for dealing with different issues. You are truly a great mentor and role model. I’m really grateful to have you as my committee Chair, and it’s been a great pleasure working with you!!! I’m also indebted to Dr. Brain Spitzberg. Thank you for guiding me through the first stages of writing my thesis proposal and giving me both general directions and detailed feedback. More importantly, I want to thank you for allowing me to use your CMC competence measures in my study and sharing with me your latest research results. You have been an indispensable part of this project! Finally, Dr. Minjuan Wang, I’m so glad that you agreed to be on my committee. Your straight-forwardness, kind understanding, and confidence in me are greatly appreciated! In addition, I would also like to thank Dr. Mei Zhong and Dr. David Dozier in our department for their comments and assistance to an earlier version of this project. Last but not least, many thanks to my husband Richard Huimin Ni. I could not have gone this far without your love and support. Thanks for taking care of me and helping me out all the time. Remember, I could not have produced “THIS baby” without you either!!! ☺ 6 TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.......................................................................................................v LIST OF TABLES.....................................................................................................................x CHAPTER I. RATIONALE.................................................................................................................1 Long-Distance Friendship........................................................................................2 Literature Review.....................................................................................................3 Self-Disclosure in Friendship in General and LDF in Particular.......................5 Computer-Mediated Communication and LDF .................................................7 Email Usage .................................................................................................8 Online Instant Messenger Usage .................................................................9 CMC Disclosure in LDF..................................................................................10 Gender, Friendship, and Self-Disclosure .........................................................15 Self-Disclosure and Cultural Values................................................................17 Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality........................................................18 Research Questions and Hypotheses ...............................................................19 II. METHODOLOGY ......................................................................................................22 Participants.............................................................................................................22 Procedures..............................................................................................................23 Measures ................................................................................................................24 Self-Disclosure Measures ................................................................................24 CMC Competence Measures ...........................................................................25 7 CHAPTER PAGE II. (continued) Relationship Quality Measures ........................................................................25 Geographic Distance Measurement .................................................................26 Cultural Dimension Measures..........................................................................27 Statistical Analyses ................................................................................................27 III. RESULTS ....................................................................................................................29 Descriptive Analyses of LDFs ...............................................................................29 Research Question 1: Self-Disclosure in FtF versus CMC....................................30 Hypothesis 1: Correlation between FtF Self-Disclosure and CMC Disclosure ..............................................................................................................30 Research Question 2: Distance and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ..................30 Research Question 3: CMC Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and Distance .......30 Research Question 4: Relationship Initiation and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC.......................................................................................................................31 Hypothesis 2: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relational State in FtF and CMC .........................................................................................................31 Hypothesis 3: Gender and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC.................................32 Hypothesis 4: High- versus Low-Context Cultures and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC .........................................................................................................32 Hypothesis 5: Individualism versus Collectivism and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC................................................................................................................32 Hypothesis 6: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality in FtF and CMC ........................................................................................33 Research Question 5: Contextual Influence on Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality ..............................................................................................33 Hypothesis 7: Correlation between CMC Competence and CMC Disclosure ......33 Research Question 6: Influence of CMC Competence on Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality........................................................................................33 8 CHAPTER IV. PAGE DISCUSSION ..............................................................................................................35 Executive Summary ...............................................................................................35 Discussions on Research Questions and Hypotheses ............................................37 Research Question 1: Self-Disclosure in FtF versus CMC..............................37 Hypothesis 1: Correlation between FtF Self-Disclosure and CMC Disclosure ........................................................................................................38 Research Question 2: Distance and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ............38 Research Question 3: CMC Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and Distance............................................................................................................39 Research Question 4: Relationship Initiation and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC..........................................................................................................40 Hypothesis 2: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relational State in FtF and CMC ...............................................................................................40 Hypothesis 3: Gender and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC...........................41 Hypothesis 4: High- versus Low-Context Cultures and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ...............................................................................................41 Hypothesis 5: Individualism versus Collectivism and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ...................................................................................................42 Hypothesis 6: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality in FtF and CMC ..................................................................................42 Research Question 5: Contextual Influence on Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality ........................................................................................43 Hypothesis 7: Correlation between CMC Competence and CMC Disclosure ........................................................................................................43 Research Question 6: Influence of CMC Competence on SelfDisclosure and Relationship Quality ...............................................................43 Theoretical Implications .......................................................................................44 Practical Implications ............................................................................................44 Limitations ............................................................................................................46 9 CHAPTER PAGE IV. (continued) Suggestions for Future Studies .............................................................................47 REFERENCES ........................................................................................................................49 APPENDICES A. RESEARCH ANNOUNCEMENT SENT TO COMMUNICATION STUDENTS .................................................................................................................61 B. RESEARCH ANNOUNCEMENT SENT THROUGH ISC WEEKLY NEWSLETTER ...........................................................................................................63 C. EMAIL MESSAGE WITH LINK TO WEB SURVEY ..............................................65 D. HELEN’S WEB SURVEY ..........................................................................................67 E. TABLES ......................................................................................................................93 ABSTRACT...........................................................................................................................105 10 LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE 1. One-Factor Solution for FtF Self-Disclosure..................................................................94 2. One-Factor Solution for CMC Disclosure ......................................................................95 3. One-Factor Solution for CMC Competence ...................................................................96 4. One-Factor Solution for Relationship Quality................................................................97 5. Analysis of Variance for Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC............................................98 6. Correlation between FtF Self-Disclosure and CMC Disclosure.....................................98 7. Interrelations among FtF Self-Disclosure, CMC Disclosure, and Distance ...................98 8. Partial Correlation Coefficient for CMC Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and Distance ..........................................................................................................................99 9. Analysis of Variance for Relationship Initiation and FtF Self-Disclosure .....................99 10. Analysis of Variance for Relationship Initiation and CMC Disclosure .......................100 11. Correlation between Relational Stage and Self-Disclosure ..........................................100 12. Analysis of Variance for Gender and FtF Self-Disclosure ...........................................101 13. Analysis of Variance for Gender and CMC Disclosure................................................101 14. Analysis of Variance for High- versus Low-Context Culture and FtF Self-Disclosure..............................................................................................................102 15. Analysis of Variance for High- versus Low-Context Culture and CMC Disclosure .....................................................................................................................102 16. Correlation between Individualism versus Collectivism and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ................................................................................................................103 17. Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality in FtF and CMC........103 18. Summary of Regression Analysis for Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Predicting Relationship Quality .....................................................................................................103 19. Correlation between CMC Competence and CMC Disclosure ....................................104 20. Partial Correlation Coefficient for Self-Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and CMC Competence ........................................................................................................104 1 CHAPTER I RATIONALE Friendship is an important element of people’s personal and social life. Friendship is such a basic process that children begin to engage in complex social behaviors in the early part of the second year of their life (Brownell & Carriger, 1990; Ross & Lollis, 1987) and can distinguish friends from unfamiliar peers by the age of four (Hartup, 1983). Throughout life, conceptions of friendship and its specific functions change depending on a variety of factors such as age, gender, marital status, and work, but “what remains constant across the life span is the significance of friendship to one’s physical and emotional well-being” (Samter, 2003, p. 637). Friendship holds a unique position among many other interpersonal relationships including kin with certifiable blood ties, marriage under legal and /or religious warrants, casual romantic relationships that are sexual in nature, and work relationships associated with economic contracts (Rawlins, 1992). Although researchers have provided definitions and characteristics of friendship in their own specifics, most would agree on at least three core features: friendships are voluntary, equal, and reciprocal (e.g., Hays, 1988; Rawlins, 1992; Samter, 2003; Wright, 1984). Some scholars refer to the person qua person perspective of friendship in which friends perceive and treat each other as a unique individual (Suttles, 1970; Wright, 1974). Friendship can also be viewed as a type of relationship, “intended to facilitate social-emotional goals of the participants, and may involve varying types and degrees of companionship, intimacy, affection, and mutual assistance” (Hays, 1988, p. 395). 2 Friendship serves as a crucial factor in people’s personal development and social life. Studies show that a lack of friends may cause numerous problems including academic failure, drug and alcohol abuse, depression, anxiety, and fatigue (Fehr, 1996; Ladd, 1999). Current literature has investigated different aspects of friendship, such as the conceptualization or definition of friendship (e.g., Hays, 1988; Rawlins, 1992; Robin, 1985; Wright, 1974), activities and intimacy related behaviors (e.g., Dindia & Allen, 1992; Reis, 1998), gender differences in friendship (e.g., Fehr, 1996; Wright, 1988, 1998), and various friendship phases in one’s lifetime (e.g., Fehr, 1996; Rawlins, 1992). One domain that still remains relatively unexplored is the long-distance friendship. Long-Distance Friendship In her seminal research, Rohlfing (1995) claimed that long-distance friendship (LDF) as a type of long-distance relationship (LDR) is still under-studied, despite the fact that this phenomenon has become prevalent with more and more people pursuing higher education or professional careers, and immigrating to another country (Stafford, 1988). Existing studies suggest LDFs are common; close to 90% of the subjects in Rohfing’s research reported having at least one close, long-distance friend (Rohlfing, 1990; Rohlfing & Healey, 1991). However, being separated by time and space is a substantial challenge to those who were at one time proximal friends and who wish to maintain their relationships. Earlier studies found that geographic separation was the most frequent reason people cited for friendship dissolution (Parlee, 1979; Rose, 1984). Other studies investigating the consequences of mobility on interpersonal communication showed that highly mobile individuals have fewer friendships and experience greater level of anomie than proximal friendships (Hunt & Butler, 1972; Parks, 1977). 3 So what are the effects of geographic separation on friendship in today’s society? Will new communication technologies make a difference in sustaining LDFs? How similar or different are men and women when communicating friendship through both face-to-face (FtF) and computer-mediated communication (CMC)? How much does culture influence this phenomenon? This study attempts to understand how people communicate with their longdistance friends in different contexts through the lens of self-disclosure. Literature Review Self-disclosure has been commonly defined as the verbal messages that people reveal about themselves including personal information, experiences, thoughts, and feelings (e.g., Archer, 1980; Cozby, 1973; Derlega, Metts, Petronio, & Margulis, 1993). Some scholars insist that the revealed information be of an intimate or private nature, which is normally hard to obtain unless disclosed by oneself (e.g., Dindia, 2000a; Goodstein & Reinecher, 1974; Pearce & Sharp, 1973). As Dindia (2000a) maintains, While some information about one’s self is rather public … there is other information about one’s self that is rather private or intimate and is disclosed under special circumstances. This private, intimate information about the self ought to be the focus of both research and theorizing about self-disclosure. If this is not done, the term self-disclosure becomes vague and general … losing any special meaning (p.148). Based on these discussions, FtF self-disclosure is specifically defined in the current study as the encoding of verbal messages that reveal to other(s) private, intimate, and/or risky personal information, experiences, thoughts, feelings, and emotions without the use of mediated channels. In comparison, CMC disclosure is defined as the text messages and emoticons sent to other(s) through the Internet, conveying private, intimate, and/or risky personal information, experiences, thoughts, feelings, and emotions. There are also 4 categories of other potentially important disclosure such as self-disclosure via telephone or mail that are not concerned with in this study. One of the first theoretical explanations of self-disclosure was developed from social penetration theory, which predicted that self-disclosure increases gradually as people develop their relationships (Altman & Taylor, 1973). Three basic dimensions of selfdisclosure were proposed: (1) breadth indicates the number of topics a person feels free to discuss; (2) depth shows the intimacy of information disclosed; and (3) frequency means how often self-disclosure occurs (Altman & Taylor, 1973). In addition, scholars also uncovered other dimensions relevant to self-disclosure: valence refers to the extent to which a message has good/bad implications for the sender, duration reflects how long two people spend disclosing in a single conversation, and reciprocity implies whether communication matches each others' levels of disclosure (e.g., Dindia, 2000b; Gilbert, 1976; Tolstedt & Stokes, 1984; Wheeless & Grotz, 1976). A few researchers have pointed out that certain measures related to time and proximity may not be relevant to examining self-disclosure in long-distance relationships (e.g., Johnson, Becker, Wigley, Wittenberg, & Haigh, 2003). Therefore, the dimensions that are comparatively more applicable to both FtF self-disclosure and CMC disclosure would be: breadth, depth, valence, and reciprocity. In her comprehensive reviews, Dindia (1997, 2000a) summarized four research perspectives toward self-disclosure in personal relationships: (1) self-disclosure as a personality trait and a stable characteristic of relationships, which attempts to identify individual differences in self-disclosure and correlate with demographic variables (e.g., Archer, 1979; Jourard, 1971); (2) self-disclosure as a one-time action or event, which focuses on individual communication messages or behaviors (e.g., Cline, 1983); (3) self- 5 disclosure as a transactional process, which views self-disclosure as a dynamic, continuous, and circular process (e.g., Pearce & Sharp, 1973); and (4) self-disclosure as a dialectical process, which adds contradictory forces such as openness versus closedness, expressiveness versus protectiveness to the transactional perspective (e.g. Baxter & Montgomery, 1996, 2000; Petronio, 2000; Rawlins, 1992; Rosenfeld, 2000). This study adopted the perspective that views self-disclosure as a dialectical process when proposing research questions. Self-Disclosure in Friendship in General and LDF in Particular Self-disclosure has constantly appeared on the listed core conceptions and values of intimate relationships in general (Laurenceau, Barrett, & Pietromonaco, 1998; Roscoe, Kennedy, & Pope, 1987; Waring, Tillman, Frelick, Russell, & Weisz, 1980), and friendship in particular (Bradac, 1983; Bruess & Pearson, 1997; Davis & Todd, 1985; Helgeson, Shaver, & Dyer, 1987). Monsour (1992) found that both women and men agreed on the ordering of intimacy indicators in friendship, ranking self-disclosure and emotional expressiveness highest on the list (cf., Helgeson et al., 1987). Self-disclosure also plays a crucial role in relationship development and maintenance (Derlega et al., 1993). Several studies suggested that individuals who reveal personal information tend to initiate and form friendships because such disclosure signals a desire for closeness (Collins & Miller, 1994; Fehr, 1996; Hays, 1984, 1985). Some researchers even claimed that it is “hard to imagine how a relationship might get started without such selfdisclosure” (Derlega et al., 1993, p. 2). Considerable evidence shows that openness including both routine talk and intimate conversation, and in many cases intimate self-disclosure alone, is the most important maintenance behavior and predictor of relational closeness in 6 friendships (Afifi, Guerrero, & Egland, 1994; Fehr, 1996; Johnson, 2001; Rose, 1985; Rosenfeld & Kendrick, 1984). In the study of women’s LDFs, Rose (1984) found participants sustained their relationships through “intimate self-disclosure about daily events” in their routine telephone conversations. More recently and specifically, Johnson and associates (2001; Johnson et al., 2003) discovered that self-disclosure is one of the most commonly reported perceptions of relational closeness and relational maintenance behavior between college student subjects and their long-distance friends. In other words, people intend to engage self-disclosure in order to make themselves feel close to their friends. In LDRs, communication is often focused on quality rather than quantity (Aron & Fraley, 1999). However, some scholars view self-disclosure as a privacy regulation mechanism and argue that revealing private information is risky because people are put into a potentially vulnerable situation that would result in need of self-protection (Altman, 1975; Bochner, 1982; Derlega & Chaikin, 1977; Petronio, 1988, 1991). Rosenfeld (1979) and Hatfield (1984) suggested people might keep away from intimate self-disclosure for several reasons such as fear of exposure, abandonment, angry attacks, loss of control, destructive impulses, and/or losing individuality. Studies also showed that people sometimes want to keep the intimacy level of self-disclosure to a certain degree so that they could protect themselves from emotional overload, increased conflict, and excessive dependence (Afifi & Guerrero, 1998; Guerrero & Afifi, 1995). Van Horn el al. (1997) found in their LDR study that people tend to disclose less and avoid bringing up sensitive or problematic issues during long-distance communication than FtF interactions. 7 Computer-Mediated Communication and LDF With the advances in communication technologies, people today have a wide variety of ways to connect with each other. Some scholars claimed that given a greater number of communication channels, the impact of geographic distance is decreasing, allowing people to develop and maintain relationships with those who live too far away to meet face to face frequently (Blieszner & Adams, 1992; Wood, 1995). People in LDRs mostly rely on letters, telephone, and more recently computer-mediated communication. According to Ferris (1997), computer-mediated communication is generally understood as both task- and relationship-oriented communication conducted by computers, which include asynchronous communication by the use of email, newsgroups, or an electronic message board system, synchronous communication via online chat rooms, MUDs and MOOs (Multi-user ‘places’) or instant messengers, and information retrieval, storage and manipulation through computers and electronic databases. In a real break from traditional long-distance communication media, CMC dramatically helps to overcome challenges and difficulties associated with communication across time and distance (Baron, 1998; Pew, 2000; Sproull & Faraj, 1997). This partially explains why many people report that they make fewer long-distance phone calls with friends and family after going online (Dimmick, Kline, & Stafford, 2000; Pew, 2000). Statistics from recent studies showed empirical evidence of the powerful impact of CMC on relationship development (for a review, see Spitzberg, 2004, Table 1). Rumbough (2001) reported that 10.6% of research subjects established a LDR and 43.6% have maintained a LDF because of the Internet. In another study, 40% or more college students sampled stated that their goal in meeting people online was friendship and 60% admitted that they had met someone via the Internet, of which 26% became friendships (Knox, Daniels, 8 Sturdivant, & Zusman, 2001). McKenna, Green, and Gleason (2002) found that 54% of respondents had met with an Internet friend face to face. And according to the Pew Internet and American Life Project (2002), 72% of the college participants said most of their communication on the Internet was with friends. In addition, of a wide variety of online communication tools, email and instant messaging are suggested to be especially interpersonal relating oriented given their user specific features (Baym, 2002). Email usage. Since the creation of the World Wide Web, the Internet has been widely diffusing all around the world. According to Nielsen/NetRatings (2003), the number of global Internet users increased from 563 million people in Quarter 3, 2002 to 580 million people in Quarter 4, 2002. The Internet, especially email, has been playing a key role in many aspects of people’s life, such as information exchange and social interactions. By the year of 2001, 55% of American adults accessed Internet and pursued a wide range of online activities, and 48 million Americans were using e-mail as Internet’s prime communication feature during an average day (Howard, Rainie, & Jones, 2001). More specifically, 92% of teenagers aged 12 through 17 had been using the Internet sending or reading emails in a recent study conducted among 754 youth (Pew, 2001). College students are frequently looking for email, with 72% checking email at least once a day and two-thirds use at least two email addresses (Pew, 2002). Furthermore, people are using email to enrich their important relationships and enlarge their networks. A recent showed that 55% of Internet users said their email exchanges had improved their connections to family members; 66% said email had enhanced their communications with significant friends; and women tend to make these statements more than men (Pew, 2000). 9 Online instant messenger usage. Instant messaging (IM) is one of the newest and most popular forms of CMC. IM is commonly used on the Internet via online instant messenger (OIM), which is a form of interactive text-related online communication software. People of all walks of life have come to utilize OIM for communication. Although email has been a predominant mode of CMC among those who are involved in LDRs, more and more people, especially college students have begun to use OIMs to communicate with their friends and romantic partners in another city or a country. Besides the email characteristics of being economical, casual, time-flexible, and user friendly, OIM additionally provides services enabling people to see who is online at the moment, exchange instant messages in different languages, conduct a voice or video conversation instead of typing, share pictures or any other files, allow a third member to join the communication process, and visualize personal emotions (e.g., Cummings, 2002; Nardi, Whittaker & Bradner, 2000). One of the main functions of OIM is to maintain a sense of connection with friends and family. Another Pew study (2001) discovered that almost threequarters of online teens in the United States, roughly 13 million youth, had used an instant messaging program; fully 90% of instant messengers said they used this Internet tool to stay in touch with friends and relatives who do not live nearby; and 37% of online teens used IM to write something they would not have said in person. In their ethnographic study, Chao and Yang (2001) rather elaborated their online interactions, emotions, and changes in familial relationships between sojourners and distance families via OIM. As we can see, CMC has instigated a technological revolution and changed our human society profoundly. Mostly conducted in organizational environment, early studies on CMC tend to focus on its task-oriented functions (e.g., Culnan & Markus, 1987). However, 10 when one examines the developing civilization of virtual worlds, it becomes apparent that interpersonal relationships have experienced a transformation from primarily through FtF interactions to CMC (Merkle & Richardson, 2000). More and more scholars have realized the importance of understanding the social impact of CMC on relational communication (e.g., Baym, 2002; Chenault, 1998; Clark, 1998; Lea & Spears, 1995; Parks & Floyd, 1996; Utz, 2000). Although CMC was not invented with interpersonal interaction in mind, the rise of the Internet has clarified that this technology is fundamentally social (Parks & Roberts, 1998). The exponential growth of computer and Internet use has ushered in a new information age that promises to redefine both our communication and our relationships (Waugh, 1995). CMC has automatically become an alternative solution to many problems or challenges to LDRs and commonly used in LDFs (Dimmick, Kline, & Stafford, 2000; Pew, 2000; Stafford, Kline, & Dimmick, 1999). This study examines (1) LDFs initiated in FtF context, which means people have got to know each other well face-to-face and they are simply continuing their interpersonal relationships through CMC, (2) LDFs initiated in CMC context, or mixed-mode relationships (MMRs), which means people establish relationships on the Internet and maintain or even develop their relationships via both FtF interactions and CMC (Walther & Parks, 2002), and (3) LDFs initiated through other possible communication channels. CMC Disclosure in LDF Although long-distance relational communication has been mentioned in CMC studies, how CMC affects LDFs is not yet well understood, especially with respect to the role of self-disclosure. To date, only a few studies have covered CMC disclosure and a majority 11 of them focused on self-disclosure behaviors between strangers (e.g., Joinson, 2001; McKenna et al., 2002; Rumbough, 2001; Sandlund & Geist-Martin, 2001; Tidwell & Walther, 2002; Whitty, 2002). In the study on self-disclosure in CMC, Joinson (2001) found significantly higher level of spontaneous self-disclosure in CMC compared to FtF overall, significantly higher CMC disclosure among visually anonymous participants than nonvisually anonymous participants, and significantly higher levels of CMC disclosure when heightened private self-awareness is combined with reduced public self-awareness. McKenna et al. (2002) hypothesize that CMC increases self-disclosure and expression of one’s “true” self because of its three important features: anonymity, lack of “gating features”, and facilitation of locating those with shared interests. Results of their research showed evidence that people who could better disclose their inner self to others online than in FtF context were more likely to form close relationships on the Internet as well as moved their virtual relationships into real life.. Tidwell and Walther (2002) suggested that participants interacted in CMC context tend to use a greater proportion of more direct and intimate uncertainty reduction behaviors than unmediated participants in the initial stage of relational communication. Compared to FtF interactions, communication through CMC context appeared to display greater self-disclosure and more depth and breadth of questions (Whitty, 2002). However, in a descriptive study, 25.7% of the survey respondents indicated that they disclose personal information to strangers who are physically present, while only 12.4% disclose personal information to strangers in a chat room (Rumbough, 2001). Sandlund and Geist-Martin (2001) did not focus their study on self-disclosure, but their ethnographic stories about reconnecting with their former lovers via email certainly showed the power of 12 CMC in terms of disclosing private, intimate, and even risky information and strong emotions through the Internet. Obviously, with the advent of CMC, people are given possibilities to exchange information more quickly and instantly, express personal emotions and feelings more freely, and even maintain or develop interpersonal relationships more efficiently. In addition to the economic and convenient features, another characteristic of CMC more related to selfdisclosure is that computer-mediated messages can be stored in memory, replicated, retrieved at later dates, and edited prior to sending, which would lead to interpersonal consequences such as better organized and thought out statements than FtF communication (e.g., Walther, 1996). Some studies suggested that people are more likely to disclose via CMC than FtF (Archer, 1980; Joinson, 1998, 2001; Rheingold, 1993). A growing body of literature (e.g., Baym, 2002) argues that people increasingly prefer to communicate and develop interpersonal relationships through CMC in many occasions. Some researchers even emphasize that the system we presently refer to as information technologies should be more aptly named as relationship technologies, because the new machines of today are between human beings rather than between man and nature, and not only material products but feelings, emotions, and relations are being processed in these machines (Wood & Smith, 2001). However, even though people are provided great opportunities to keep in touch with their long-distance friends in a possibly more convenient way compared to letter and telephone, this does not mean that CMC would necessarily become a safe and effective relational communication environment for self-disclosure in LDFs and therefore contribute to high relationship quality. In fact, whether or not CMC can be an effective context for 13 relational communication has been an issue of contention among scholars since the formation of networked interaction began. Early research concluded computer usage drove people to focus more on task-oriented messages and precluded the development of social relationships among users (e.g., Short, Williams, & Christie, 1976). Many people think of CMC as a substitute for or a temporary solution rather than the essence of the relationship. Research on relational maintenance also provided evidence that people sometimes are using mediated communication as a tool until they can be together again (e.g., Aylor, 2003; Rabby & Walther, 2003). In a more recent study on cyberfeelings, Hack (1999) suggested that although emotions exist in CMC and can be disclosed with varying intensity, it is apparent that emotions are much more prevalent in FtF communication than in CMC, or in other words, “CMC is not unemotional, but is clearly less emotional” (p. 102). Other studies suggest that deception can distort the nature of disclosures, such as when people claim to be younger, lighter, or somewhere they are not (see Cornwell & Lundgren, 2001; Knox et al., 2001; Pew, 2001; Rumbough, 2001; Whitty, 2002). At this point of time, theories developed in traditional communication contexts have begun to be tested in CMC environment and the results vary depending on specific topics and research design. Walther and Parks (2002) summarized some general theories that apply to both interpersonal dynamics and CMC in terms of five approaches: (1) cues filtered out, which emphasizes the impersonal nature of CMC because the lack of nonverbal cues (e.g., Culnan & Markus, 1987; Short, Williams, & Christie, 1976; Sproull & Kiesler, 1986), and this may infer that people are not likely to disclose in CMC than FtF; (2) cues to choose by, which asserts that some types of messages might be conveyed more efficiently in one medium than in another and people choose the Internet and FtF communication for 14 particular needs (e.g., Daft & Lengel, 1984; Daft, Lengel, & Refvino, 1987, Flaherty, Pearce, & Rubin, 1998), and this may lead to an assumption that level of self-disclosure may be related to how people look at different media and how the media can satisfy their particular needs; (3) cues filtered in, which assumes that communicators are just as motivated to reduce interpersonal uncertainty, form impressions, and develop affinity in CMC settings as they are in other settings (e.g., Walther, 1992; Utz, 2000), emoticons are particularly studied as widely accepted graphic representations of nonverbal cues on the Internet (e.g., Castellá, Abad, Alonso, & Silla, 2000; Reid, 1991; Thomrson & Foulger, 1996; Walther & D’Addario, 2001), and this implies computer mediation may not influence people’s self-disclosure behaviors; (4) cues about us, not you or me, which suggests that people online are likely to set aside personal identity and adopt the appropriate social identity in order to find acceptance among others (e.g., Lea & Spears, 1995; Postumes, Spears, & Lea, 1998); and (5) cues bent and twisted, which suggests a hyperpersonal perspective of CMC (e.g., Walther, 1996; Wood & Smith, 2001). More recently, Ramirez, Walther, Burgoon, and Sunnafrank (2002) proposed a conceptual model for examining social information seeking in CMC and new media. They argue that CMC may eliminate or reduce nonverbal and contextual information; such communication environment offers new and unique alternative mechanisms and advantages otherwise unavailable in FtF context for acquiring social information about other people. Spitzberg (2004) addresses the important role of new media in the formation and development of personal relationships and proposes a model and measure of CMC competence. These approaches present a diverse but also complicated context where CMC disclosure occurs. 15 Gender, Friendship, and Self-Disclosure Several scholars suggested that women’s friendships engage more intimacy related behaviors including self-disclosure, emotional expressiveness, and mere talk, whereas men’s friendships tends to be inexpressive and activity-centered (e.g., Brehm, 1992; Fehr, 1996). Some used similar descriptions of the dichotomy, such as talking versus doing (Caldwell & Peplau, 1982), face-to-face versus side-by-side (Wright, 1988), and communal versus agentic (Duck & Wright, 1993). However, as mentioned earlier, Monsour (1992) found a difference in degree between men’s and women’s intimacy behavior, but really more similarity overall. Also, in a more recent selective but representative review on gender differences in friendship, Wright (1998) posited that differences in male and female’s friendships emerge in a context of more fundamental similarities, and even when differences were found, they were often small despite statistically significant comparisons. In other words, women’s and men’s friendships are similar in more ways or to a greater degree than they are different. Studies showed that as friendship grow closer, gender differences in communal characteristics like intimate communication and companionship are even smaller (e.g., Candy-Gibbs, 1882; Hays, 1985; Wright, 1982). Very often exemplified as a typical intimacy related behavior, self-disclosure is a central factor in research about gender differences in friendship. As Hill and Stull (1987) stated, the relationship between self-disclosure and gender has been a topic of research for which some of the clearest predictions have been made, yet some of the most confusing results have been obtained. The issue of gender differences in self-disclosure was first proposed in research based upon Jourard’s Self-Disclosure Questionnaire (JSDQ). Jourard pointed out that there is gender difference in self-disclosure, and basically women tend to disclose more private 16 information to others than men (Jourard, 1961; Jourard & Lasakow, 1958; Jourard & Richman, 1963). Jourard attributed this difference to gender role, particularly because the male gender role inhibits men’s self-disclosure (Jourard, 1971). However, narrative reviewers who continued the investigation came to inconclusive results and suggested several possible intervening factors: (a) situational factors including topic of disclosure, sex of target (disclosure recipient), and relationship to target, (b) sex role attitudes, (c) sex role identity, (d) sex role norms, and (e) measure of self-disclosure (Cozy, 1973; Hill & Stull, 1987; Rosenfeld, Civikly & Herron, 1979). Unfortunately, no clear consensus has yet emerged: more studies found greater self-disclosure by women, fewer others revealed greater selfdisclosure by men, and still some obtained no gender difference (Reis, 1998). Despite all these controversial research findings, scholars claim that there are gender differences in self-disclosure and generally females tend to self-disclose somewhat than males do (e.g., Dindia, 2000b; Dindia & Allen, 1992; Reis, 1998). In 1990s, Dindia and Allen (1992) conducted a meta-analysis of gender differences in self-disclosure. They located 205 studies involving 23,702 participants that tested the relationship between gender and self-disclosure. Evidence showed that females self-disclose more than males, though the difference is not as large as self-disclosure theorists and researchers have suggested. Furthermore, Reis (1998) provided a meta-analytic summary of gender differences obtained in eight studies with somewhat diverse sample subjects in varying locations and contexts: Men’s same-sex interaction is substantially less intimate than is women’s same-sex interaction, whereas opposite-sex interaction yields no consistent trend. Although selfdisclosure was not specifically tested in these eight studies, women were found more often the targets of self-disclosure than men. 17 Self-Disclosure and Cultural Values The meaning of self-disclosure and its functions in interpersonal communication may vary in cultures possessing different value orientations (Nakanishi & Johnson, 1993). With the tendency of globalization, studies today should take a worldwide view. A vast majority of communication studies have been conducted in North American culture using Native American populations. It is important to test the generalizability of existing theories and principles in different cultural environments. Hall (1976) posed two important concepts about context and meaning – in low context culture (LCC), communication messages are conveyed mainly through explicit code such as language, whereas people in high context culture (HCC) utilize mostly physical context or nonverbal cues to imply their messages. This directly relates to self-disclosure behavior in different cultures. Thus, people in LCC would rely on verbal expression in their relational communication and are likely to disclose more than those in HCC. Hofstede (1983, 1986; 2001) proposed some other fundamental cultural dimensions, including individualism and collectivism. He proposed that individualistic cultures are more self-oriented whereas collectivistic cultures are more group-oriented. The implications of these cultural norms on self-disclosure suggest that people in individualistic cultures value embody open expressiveness and interpersonal assertiveness while people in collectivistic cultures tend to be more inexpressive, inscrutable, and even impassive, balancing between expressing true emotion and desire by the individual and regulating that expression by propriety (Nakanishi & Johnson, 1993; Rubin, Yang, & Porte, 2000; Yum, 1991). Although LCC versus HCC and individualism versus collectivism are separate different sets of concepts, they are also interrelated. People from LCC and individualistic cultures have many similarities and people from HCC and collectivistic cultures also share 18 things in common. Typically, U.S. American and many other Western cultures (French, German, etc.) are LCC and individualistic cultures while Asian (Chinese, Japanese, etc.) and Middle Eastern cultures are HCC and collectivistic cultures (Hofstede, 1986; Triandis, 1995). A number of cross-cultural studies have attempted to verify contrasting norms and practices for self-disclosure. In the studies of self-disclosure in relationship maintenance, Ting-Toomey (1991) found that Japanese exhibited a much lower level of self-disclosure in both quality and quantity than North Americans and French. Similarly, self-reported disclosure among Taiwanese was found lower than North Americans (Chen, 1995; Wolfson & Pearce, 1983). In a meta-analysis of gender differences and intimacy behaviors cross five cultures, Reis (1998) concluded that gender differences in intimacy are influenced by cultural norms. More specifically, males from the United States, Germany, and the Netherlands reported significantly higher intimacy with male than with female partners, whereas in Jordan and Hong Kong, on average, men showed no differences as a function of partner sex (Reis, 1998). Although a considerable body of intercultural theory and research suggested the expectations that people in HCC/collectivistic cultures report substantially less selfdisclosure than those in LCC/individualistic cultures, other research yielded conflicting results. In a comparison study of self-disclosure among Chinese and North Americans, Rubin, Yang, and Porte (2000) discovered that their data did not reveal any main effect for cultural values in self-disclosure. Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality Relationship quality has been extensively investigated in long-distance romantic relationship (e.g., Guldner & Swensen, 1995) and friendship (e.g., Parker & Asher, 1993; 19 Rose & Roades, 1987; Nicotera, 1993), but not particularly in the context of LDF or directly tested in relation to self-disclosure in FtF and CMC environment. In order to find out whether the new information and communication technologies really make a difference in selfdisclosure between long-distance friends, it is very important to evaluate the overall relationship quality perceived by people who are involved in both FtF and CMC contexts. Research Questions and Hypotheses Self-disclosure is a dynamic and complicated process. The review of literature has suggested that self-disclosure behaviors vary across different contexts. This research study attempts to examine self-disclosure specifically in long-distance friendships, and compare and contrast self-disclosure behaviors in FtF and CMC interactions for the first time. Looking at this issue from a dialectical perspective, we should not ignore evidence from previous research on both advantages and challenges to self-disclosure via CMC compared to FtF communication. This discussion leads to my first research question: RQ1: Among long-distance friends, are there differences in self-disclosure between FtF and CMC contexts? Although an individual’s self-disclosure behavior may or may not vary across communication contexts, the personal tendency to self-disclose should be comparatively stable, which leads to my first hypothesis: H1: An individual’s level of FtF self-disclosure is positively correlated with CMC disclosure in LDFs. This study does not simply test the amount of self-disclosure in FtF and CMC environments. More importantly, the study attempts to investigate the role and impact of new information technologies in people’s lives, in terms of their self-disclosure behaviors. This 20 interest leads to the following two research questions in regard to the influence of geographic distance on self-disclosure in LDFs: RQ2: In both FtF and CMC contexts, what is the relationship between geographic distance and self-disclosure in LDFs? RQ3: Is the amount of CMC disclosure more important to relational quality in LDFs of greater geographic distance? Self-disclosure very often interacts with many situational variables, such as the context of relationship initiation, relational stage, gender, and cultural background, etc. Although prior studies yielded contradicting results, based on the overall conclusion drawn from the literature, a research question and four hypotheses can be stated as below: RQ4: Does the context of friendship initiation influence self-disclosure in FtF and CMC? H2: In both FtF and CMC contexts, self-disclosure is positively correlated with relational stage. H3: In both FtF and CMC contexts, females are more likely to disclose than males. H4: In both FtF and CMC contexts, self-disclosure of people from low context cultures is greater than those from high context cultures. H5: In both FtF and CMC contexts, self-disclosure of people from individualistic cultures is greater than those from collectivistic cultures. Self-disclosure is not an independent relational behavior. In fact, prior research shows that self-disclosure is closely related to relationship quality. In order to re-test the correlation between these two variables in FtF context, and extend the examination to CMC environment, the following two research questions and two hypotheses are proposed: 21 H6: In both FtF and CMC contexts, self-disclosure is positively correlated with relationship quality. RQ5: What combination of FtF and CMC contexts mediates the relationship between self-disclosure and relationship quality in LDF? H7: An individual’s CMC competence is positively correlated with CMC disclosure. RQ6: How does a person’s CMC competence mediate the relationship between selfdisclosure and relationship quality? 22 CHAPTER II METHODOLOGY A total of 388 qualified respondents completed the present study within the time period of December 2, 2003 to February 2, 2004. Research subjects were assured in advance that (1) their participation be voluntary, (2) their answers to the questions be completely anonymous, and (3) all the data collected be used only for the current study. They were also notified that the Institutional Review Board at SDSU approved this study. Contact information was given for further questions and concerns about the study. Participants Participants for this study were recruited from two sources. Close to 70% of the research subjects were students who were enrolled in a basic communication course at SDSU (either in Fall 2003 or Spring 2004 semester) with an extra credit incentive. Approximately 30% of the subjects were international students recruited through weekly electronic newsletter from the International Student Center at SDSU. In order to qualify for the study, all respondents had to be (1) 18 years of age or older, (2) enrolled at SDSU at the time of the study, (3) have at least one friend living in another city, state, or country, (4) have physically met their long-distance friends, and (5) have communicated with their long-distance friends through the Internet. A total of 418 students accessed the web survey for the study. However, 24 respondents were not qualified for the study, and 6 respondents did not complete a majority of the questions on the survey, which reduced the actual sample size to 388 participants. 23 Among all the respondents who answered the demographic questions, close to 70% were females and over 30% were males. The respondents aged from 18 to 42 (M = 20.40), although 90% were under the age of 26. Participants for the study were from diverse cultural backgrounds as they were raised in 43 countries and regions. Procedures Research announcements (see Appendix A & B) were sent to the target population with basic information about general study topic, research purpose, significance, and security assurance as well as a contact name and an email address. Students interested in participating were encouraged to send a message to a designated email address for further instructions. Thus, a list of email address of all potential subjects was obtained and a separate email message (see Appendix C) with the link to a web survey was sent out to each person. To measure self-disclosure between long-distance friends in both FtF and CMC contexts, a web survey (see Appendix D) was developed through the SurveyMonkey.com online research tool. The questionnaire contained 24 questions divided in eight sections (introduction, qualification test, CMC competence, long-distance friend, self-disclosure in FtF and CMC contexts, relationship quality, demographics, and extra credit info). All the listed options or statements in a matrix question were randomly rotated to avoid response order bias. Interested students could easily access the web survey by clicking on the link sent via email and simply follow the instructions on the computer screen and click the button of their choice. With the function of hyperlink on the Internet, the information system for this web survey could automatically skip to the designated question according to the respondent’s answer to previous question. In order to reduce response error, all respondents were notified in advance that they would not be able to backward and change their answers once they 24 passed a screen. However, they would retain their right to exit the survey at any point and not to complete the survey. Respondents taking the web survey for class extra credit were instructed to send a message to a designated email address with their full names, class instructor’s name, and/or class section number after they completed the study online. All this extra credit information was kept separately from the web survey, so the study stayed anonymous as the name lists were destroyed after being delivered to corresponding class instructors. Once respondents finished the survey, they could choose “done” to contribute their answers to the research database. Measures Self-Disclosure Measures Three statements were developed for each one of the four self-disclosure dimensions (breadth, depth, valence, and reciprocity) suggested by the literature. Wheeless and Grotz (1976) proposed a 30-item questionnaire as multidimensional measurement of selfdisclosure. In a follow-up study, Wheeless (1978) revised the self-disclosure scales to include 40 items, which are grouped into five categories: (1) intended disclosure factor; (2) amount factor; (3) positiveness-negativeness factor; (4) control of depth factor; and (5) honestyaccuracy factor. Among those five categories, positiveness-negativeness factor and control of depth factor match the two dimensions of self-disclosure in the present study: valance and depth. Therefore, several questions from Wheeless original questionnaire were selected and modified to the purpose of this study. Three new questions are created to measure the breadth and reciprocity of self-disclosure in this study. Data were collected from subjects on the four dimensions of self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts. However, factor analyses of FtF self-disclosure (see Table 1; all 25 tables shown in Appendix E) and CMC disclosure (see Table 2) both suggested that a unidimensional scale would accomplish a much higher reliability of measurement. More specifically, The Scree Plot tests indicated a one-dimension scale for both FtF self-disclosure and CMC disclosure. No viable two, three, or four- dimensional structure merged from the factor analyses. A 7-item scale of FtF self-disclosure (α = .82) merged as a one-factor solution. Reliability for this single factor scale was the highest. A unidimensional same 7item scale of CMC disclosure (α = .81) was created and used for data analyses to make the self-disclosure measures comparable between FtF and CMC contexts. CMC Competence Measures One of the primary goals of this research study is to examine the role of CMC in relationship maintenance and disclosure. Hence, a set of measures that operationalize various aspects of the CMC dimension of research subjects’ relationships and communication activity were adopted from Spitzberg’s (2004) CMC competence measurement including motivation, knowledge, skills, and outcomes. Two statements were selected from the items testing each of the following dimensions or sub-dimensions: motivation, knowledge, coordination, attentiveness, expressiveness, composure, appropriateness, and effectiveness. The Scree Plot indicated a one-dimension scale. No viable multi-factor solutions emerged from the facture analysis. Reliability for a single-factor unidimensional 15-item scale for CMC competence measure (see Table 3) was the highest (α = .87) when one item was deleted. Relationship Quality Measures Measurement of perceived relationship quality components, especially friendship quality, was also incorporated into this research study. Nicotera (1993) suggested that 26 increasing intimacy, personal growth, and communication effectiveness are three major characteristics of friendship quality. Specific measures may include tolerance, honesty, caring, trustworthiness, humor, intelligence, responsibility, religion, self-discipline, and resourcefulness (Moeneka & Nicotera, 1993). In a more recent study, Fletcher, Simpson, and Thomas (2000) developed an inventory (PRQC) to evaluate different elements of relationship quality. The 18-item inventory consists of six categories: (1) relationship satisfaction; (2) commitment; (3) intimacy; (4) trust; (5) passion; and (6) love. Some of the questions were modified to detect the relationship between online self-disclosure and quality of LDFs in this study. Based on the three categories Nicotera (1993) suggested, three questions were selected from PRQC and revised to measure each of the following: increasing intimacy, and communication effectiveness. Three questions were created to examine the dimension of personal growth. In an orthoginal factor analysis, the Scree Plot test indicated a onedimension scale. No viable multi-factor solutions emerged from the factor analysis. And a single-factor unidimensional 9-item scale (see Table 4) was the most reliable measure for relationship quality (α = .78). Geographic Distance Measurement Another important part of this study is to test the impact of geographic distance on self-disclosure in LDFs in both FtF and CMC contexts. Research subjects were asked about the location of their long-distance friends in the study. An online GIS system called “how far it is” (http://www.indo.com/distance/) was used to measure the geographic distance between these long-distance friends. This system uses the data from the US Census and a supplementary list of cities around the world and the calculations are based on the latitude and longitude of two places. If the respondent provided only the name of the state in the 27 United States, a city close to the center of the state was used for recoding purpose. And if only given the name of the country or if the city in a country other than the United States was not listed in the database, the capital city of the country was used for recoding purposes. Cultural Dimension Measures To explore cultural influence on self-disclosure behaviors in both FtF and CMC environments, this study also attempted to test the relationships between respondent’s cultural background and self-disclosure in LDFs. In the study, research subjects were asked about country in which they were raised. A number for cultural background was assigned to each respondent according to the Individualism score of the country in Hofstede’s (2001) summary of country index scores. A different score in terms of high- versus low-context culture was given to each respondent according to relevant literature (e.g., Andersen, 2000; Hall, 1976). Statistical Analyses The Statistical Package for the Social Science (SPSS) was utilized for data analyses. Twenty-four unqualified respondents and six incomplete records were deleted from the final data file. Research questions and hypotheses were tested using Pearson Correlation Coefficient, Partial Correlations, Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), and Regression Analysis. Differences and relationships among variables were considered statistically significant at .05 level of alpha error. Power coefficients were computed for all non-significant findings. Cohen (1988) outlines a methodological approach for determining power of statistical analyses in 28 behavioral sciences. These analyses report the percentage chance of committing a beta error. The results of these findings are discussed in Chapter Three. 29 CHAPTER III RESULTS In this chapter, each research question and hypothesis is examined in sequential order. Results of statistical analyses are reported accordingly. Descriptive Analyses of LDFs Although no specific research questions or hypotheses were asked about longdistance friends in this study, the web survey yielded some important descriptive data about these target people of self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts. Among 383 respondents in the sample, the number of close, long-distance friends ranged from 1 to 200. The average number was 10.9 and the mode was 5. A majority of LDFs started from face-to-face interactions although the two friends may be apart from each other now as far as thousands of miles. Telephone, email, and IM are the three primary communication channels in LDFs. Surprisingly, one third of the respondents admitted that they always used IM to communicate with their long-distance friends. Among 377 respondents in the sample, 66.3% of their longdistance friends were females, and 33.7% were males. They ranged from 15 to 63, with a mean of 21.3 and a mode of 18, and they were located in 35 countries and regions around the world. 30 Research Question 1: Self-Disclosure in FtF versus CMC Context The first research question asked if there are differences in self-disclosure between FtF and CMC contexts. Results of one-way ANOVA (see Table 5) revealed that people tend to disclose more in face-to-face (M = 29.76) than in a computer-mediated (M = 28.17) context. [F (1/759) = 25.1; p < .001; eta² = .03]. Hypothesis 1: Correlation between FtF Self-Disclosure and CMC Disclosure Correlation coefficients (see Table 6) confirmed H1 that an individual’s level of FtF self-disclosure is positively correlated with CMC disclosure in LDFs. The test suggested a strong positive relationship between disclosure in these two contexts (r = .58, n = 378, p < .001, r² = .34). Research Question 2: Distance and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Correlation coefficients (see Table 7) suggested that no relationship between geographic distance and self-disclosure was detected in either FtF (r = -.04, n = 380, p= .41) or CMC context (r = -.02, n = 376, p = .75). There was only about 60% chance of detecting a small effect. However, the chance of detecting a medium or a large effect exceeded 99%. Research Question 3: CMC Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and Distance Partial correlations were computed to test the influence of geographic distance on the relationship between CMC disclosure and relationship quality (see Table 8). The relationship 31 between these two variables before controlling for the third variable “distance” (r = .522, n = 376, p < .001) was virtually identical after controlling for “distance” (r = .524, n = 373, p < .001). To further examine the relationship, research subjects were divided into three groups in terms of how far apart they are from their long-distance friends. Correlation coefficients for each group showed that the closer long-distance friends are the stronger the relationship between CMC disclosure and relational quality. But the differences between groups were not discernible (closer distance group: r = .59, n = 127, p < .001; middle distance group: r = .47, n = 123, p < .001; farther distance group: r = .46, n = 126, p < .001). Research Question 4: Relationship Initiation and SelfDisclosure in FtF and CMC Results of a one-way ANOVA indicated that the context of friendship initiation does not influence self-disclosure behavior in either FtF or CMC environment. The results (see Table 9) suggested that differences in FtF self-disclosure between LDFs initiated face to face (M = 29.82), through CMC (M = 28.33), and other types of friendship initiation (M = 29.81) were not statistically significant [F (2/381) = .92, p = .401]. Similar results (see Table 10) emerged in CMC disclosure that differences between LDFs initiated face to face (M = 28.09), through CMC (M = 29.53), and other types of friendship initiation (M = 28.56) were not statistically significant [F (2/375) = .78; p = .461]. Hypothesis 2: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relational Stage in FtF and CMC Correlation coefficients confirmed H2 that in both FtF (r = .54, n = 377, p< .001, r² = .29) and CMC (r = .37, n = 377, p < .001, r² = .14) contexts, indicating that selfdisclosure is positively correlated with relational stage (see Table 11). 32 Hypothesis 3: Gender and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Results of a one-way ANOVA confirmed H3, that women (M = 30.51) tend to disclose more than men (M = 28.13) do face-to-face [F (1/375) = 27.96; p < .001; eta² = .07], and women (M = 28.52) also tend to disclose greater than men (M = 27.37) do in CMC context [F (1/375) = 5.16; p = .024; eta² = .01] (see Table 12 and 13). Hypothesis 4: High- versus Low-Context Cultures and SelfDisclosure in FtF and CMC According to the results of a one-way ANOVA (see Table 14 and 15), no difference in self-disclosure was found between people from LCCs (M = 29.82) and those from HCCs (M = 29.84) in FtF context [F (1/363) = .002; p = .966]. Nor did it indicate any difference in self-disclosure between people from LCCs (M = 28.27) and HCCs (M = 28.17) in CMC context [F (1/363) = .024; p = .876]. Therefore, H4 was not confirmed in this study. There was only about 80% chance of detecting a small effect. However, the chance of detecting a medium or a large effect exceeded 99%. Hypothesis 5: Individualism versus Collectivism and SelfDisclosure in FtF and CMC Correlations failed to confirm H5 (see Table 16). No relationship was found between self-disclosure and people’s cultural background based on Hofstede’s (1983, 1986, 2001) conception of individualism versus collectivism in either FtF (r = .001, n = 373, p = .492) or CMC context (r = .048, n = 373, p = .179). There was only about 60% chance of detecting a small effect. However, the chance of detecting a medium or a large effect exceeded 99%. 33 Hypothesis 6: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality in FtF and CMC Correlation coeffitients suggested there is a positive relationship between FtF selfdisclosure and relationship quality (r = .56, n = 378, p < .001, r² = .31) and between CMC disclosure and relationship quality (r = .52, n = 378, p < .001, r² = .27). Therefore, H6 was confirmed in the present study (see Table 17). Research Question 5: Contextual Influence on SelfDisclosure and Relationship Quality According to a regression analysis, self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts is important to relationship quality in LDFs (see Table 18). More specifically, FtF selfdisclosure accounts for 31.4% of the variance in relational quality. Beyond that, there is additional unique variance associated with CMC disclosure. A combination of both FtF selfdisclosure and CMC disclosure accounts for 37.1% of the variance in relationship quality. Hypothesis 7: Correlation between CMC Competence and CMC Disclosure Correlation coefficients (see Table 19) confirmed H7, that there is a positive relationship between an individual’s CMC competence and CMC disclosure (r = .34, n= 377, p < .001, r² = .12). Research Question 6: Influence of CMC Competence on SelfDisclosure and Relationship Quality Results of a partial correlation coefficient indicated that a person’s CMC competence does not influence the relationship between CMC disclosure and relationship quality (see Table 20). The relationship between these two variables before controlling for CMC 34 competence (r = .52, n = 376, p < .001) was not substantially different from after controlling for CMC competence (rp = .47, n = 374, p < .001). 35 CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION In this chapter, a summary of research findings is reported, followed with discussions of each research question and hypothesis in detail. Then, both theoretical and practical implications of this study are addressed. Lastly, limitations of the current study and suggestions for future studies are provided. Executive Summary Long-distance friendship is a prevalent and increasing phenomenon. In today’s world, virtually everybody has friends living in a different city, state, or country. This study provides the following conclusions: Although a considerable amount of CMC studies have suggested that people are more likely to disclose on the Internet than face to face, especially between strangers, this study revealed contradictory results in LDFs. Among long-distance friends, there is slight difference in self-disclosure between FtF and CMC contexts. Generally speaking, people report that they tend to engage in somewhat more self-disclosure when they communicate face to face than through the Internet. And women are slightly more selfdisclosive than men are in both contexts though there is little gender difference in the FtF context. However, people’s self-disclosure tendency is relatively consistent across these communication environments. So highly FtF disclosive people would still reveal considerable personal or private information to their long-distance friends via the Internet. Furthermore, self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts are found crucial to relationship quality in LDFs. FtF self-disclosure is the primary predictor of high relational 36 quality and CMC disclosure holds an additional unique value to relationship quality as well. Therefore, if people want to achieve a maximally satisfactory relationship with their longdistance friends, they should engage in a certain degree of self-disclosure both face to face and via the Internet. The current study also presents a few interesting null findings. First, geographic distance has no significant influence on self-disclosure behaviors or relationship quality in LDFs. As Cairncross (2001) has suggested, once two people are separate, it really doesn’t matter how far apart they are from each other. The death of distance shapes our future and penetrates to almost every aspect of our life. Jackson (2002) discussed about blurred boundaries and imagined interactions between human and computers. An imagined image of one’s long-distance friend may have played a much more active role than physical distance in the online communication of LDFs. This might help to explain the reason why CMC disclosure was not found more important to relationship quality in LDFs of greater geographic distance in the current study. Second, a person’s cultural background and values do not have much influence on self-disclosure behaviors in either FtF or CMC context. Contradicting to the findings of many cross-cultural studies on self-disclosure of different cultural groups, self-disclosure trumps culture in the present study, which suggests that the intimate nature of friendship may be trans-cultural. In addition, participants in the study started their LDFs through various communication channels, but the context of relationship initiation has no impact on selfdisclosure in either FtF or CMC context. People who consider each other as more intimate friends tend to disclose more than those who do not. Individuals with higher level of CMC 37 competence are more likely to disclose through the Internet, but CMC competence doesn’t mediate the relationship between self-disclosure and relationship quality in this study. Discussions on Research Questions and Hypotheses Research Question 1: Self-Disclosure in FtF versus CMC Literature has suggested CMC disclosure is generally greater than FtF self-disclosure between strangers. Results of this study showed the opposite among people who know each other well: Among long-distance friends, people are likely to disclose more in FtF settings than CMC. However, the medium produces a small effect accounting for only a bit over 3% of the variation in disclosure. Therefore, people in LDFs do report disclosing more in FtF communication, but only slightly more. The present study yielded different results from many other research studies related to CMC disclosure may be explained with following possible reasons: most CMC studies were conducted among people who are not well acquainted. Thus, greater self-disclosure on the Internet mainly resulted from the advantages of anonymity, lack of gating features, and other similar factors. However, the influence of these factors would be excluded from the interactions between long-distance friends who have already physically met each other and just use computers to keep in touch and maintain their relationships. It’s actually reasonable that people would be more disclosive when talking to their long-distance friends on less frequent face-to-face meetings than via the Internet. 38 Hypothesis 1: Correlation between FtF Self-Disclosure and CMC Disclosure Results of the study indicated that there is a strong positive relationship between an individual’s level of FtF self-disclosure and CMC disclosure in LDFs. This suggests that selfdisclosure is a relatively consistent relational behavior across individuals. The type of communication medium or context does not have much influence on one’s self-disclosure decisions in LDFs. This also showed the significant difference in self-disclosure behaviors between strangers and acquaintances. An individual may be very shy or even have communication anxiety when talking to people face-to-face but the same person may be perceived as outgoing, humorous, and talkative when interacting with others online. Different personalities can be revealed between strangers depending on the type of communication medium. However, this may not be the same case to those who already know each other well and take their relationships seriously. To long-distance friends, the variety of media is more of an option for communication channels in the existing relationship, rather than ways of attracting other people, although there is still a possibility that long-distance friends discover something they would not be able to discover through another medium. Research Question 2: Distance and SelfDisclosure in FtF and CMC The current study showed no relationship between geographic distance and selfdisclosure in either FtF or CMC context. Some studies may have suggested the important role of geographic distance in LDRs, which implies that greater distance may lead to more involvement in self-disclosure. However, results of this study do not support this position. Once people are involved in LDFs, how far they are apart from their long-distance friends 39 does not have much influence on their self-disclosure behaviors when they get a chance to meet face to face or communicate through the Internet. As Cairncross (2001) has suggested in her book, in today’s society the death of distance has a profound impact on our lives. Especially in the case of LDFs, people have a variety of channels to keep in touch with their friends, maintain or even develop their relationships despite the challenges caused by geographic separation. Given the results of this study, it really wouldn’t matter if an individual’s long-distance friend in located in Los Angeles, in Tokyo, or just in the next door, people would still disclose to their friends through these various communication channels in order to maintain the existing relationships. Research Question 3: CMC Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and Distance Research question three further explored the influence of geographic distance on the relationship between CMC disclosure and relationship quality. The original assumption was that CMC disclosure might be more important to relationship quality in LDFs of greater geographic distance. It is sound logic that when people are separated at greater distances, the possibility of using CMC to maintain or develop their LDFs increases, and as people engage more CMC disclosure, they might be more satisfied with their relationship. However, this study suggests that distance does not influence the relationship between CMC disclosure and people’s perceptions of their relational quality although examination among groups of different geographic distances showed that LDFs at lesser distances people engage in slightly more self-disclosure than those at greater distances. Jackson (2002) examined the human-technology interface and some of the ways people relate to computers. He discussed the dissolution of the boundaries between nature and culture, humans and machines. As new technologies penetrate to every corner of our 40 daily lives, it not only changes our ways of communication, but influence our perceptions of others during interactions. It is suggested that people tend to create an image of their communication partner when they sit in front of a computer and start “talking” to a friend. In fact, you may be chatting with a person sitting in the next door. This concept of imagined interactions perhaps helps to explain the non-significant effect of physical distance. Research Question 4: Relationship Initiation and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Research question four attempts to test the relationship between context of friendship initiation and self-disclosure. The review of the literature mentioned that friendships are still commonly initiated in FtF settings, but increasing number of people begin their relationships through CMC and engage in MMRs. Would LDFs started from FtF context have greater FtF self-disclosure? Or would LDFs initiated via the Internet have more CMC disclosure? Results of this study did not show any relationship between the context of relationship initiation and self-disclosure in either FtF or CMC context. This indicates the type of media where two people start a LDF does not influence their later on self-disclosure behaviors. Hypothesis 2: Correlation between SelfDisclosure and Relational Stage in FtF and CMC Prior studies suggest that the more intimate two people perceive their relationship to be, the more likely they engage greater self-disclosure. Results of this study supported this relationship and extended it to LDFs and CMC environments. More specifically, the respondents were asked in the study about how intimate they would consider their friendship to be. They were offered five options: causal acquaintance, just friends, good friend, close friend, or best friend. And the results suggest that among long-distance friends, the closer the friendship is, the greater self-disclosure they would engage in both FtF and CMC contexts. 41 This is a moderately large effect as relational stage accounts for 14% of the variance in CMC disclosure and 29% in FtF self-disclosure. Hypothesis 3: Gender and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Although scholars have been debating whether or not there is gender difference in self-disclosure, this study hypothesized that females tend to disclose more than males do in both FtF and CMC contexts. Results of the study confirmed the hypothesis; however, statistics indicate that this is a relatively small effect, as a difference accounting for 7% of the variation in FtF self-disclosure and only 1% of the variation in CMC disclosure. Therefore, women do disclose more than men do in both contexts, but only slightly more. These results are also consistent with other research which has suggested that females are less comfortable when communicating with invisible partner whereas they are reported more interactive in FtF interactions. In the present study, women are more likely to disclose then men are in both FtF and CMC settings. However, the gender difference is much larger in FtF self-disclosure than in CMC disclosure. Hypothesis 4: High- versus Low-Context Cultures and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Literature suggested that people from LCCs tend to disclosure more than those from HCCs. However, results of this study did not report any difference between these two variables in either FtF or CMC context. One possible reason would be previous cross-cultural studies mainly focused on comparison and contrast of general self-disclosure behaviors among people of different cultural backgrounds, whereas this study specifically examine selfdisclosure in LDFs and test the relationship between cultural influence and self-disclosure in FtF and CMC contexts. Some cultural values may influence people’s self-disclosure in 42 general, but this may not be the case in LDFs. Is culture breaking down? Or is technology an equalizer? The current study indicates that the intimate nature of friendship subsumes over cultural differences when it comes to self-disclosure behaviors in LDFs. People from HCCs can also be very explicit and expressive as those from LCCs when communicating with their long-distance friends. Hypothesis 5: Individualism versus Collectivism and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Similar to hypothesis four, cross-cultural studies suggested that people from Individualistic cultures might disclose more than those from Collectivistic cultures do. However, results of this study did not support the hypothesis either. Once again, cultural identities and behaviors guided by these values and perceptions did not have much influence on self-disclosure in LDFs in either FtF or CMC setting. This cultural dimension is even more subtle than high- versus low-context culture, which is more relative to the dependence on verbal expressions in communication and self-disclosure behaviors. All the respondents in the present study are young college students, studying in the United States. Considering their age and educational background, it is reasonable that many traditional cultural values did not show much influence among the group. In addition, the nature of these existing relationships may also be more influential than culture. Hypothesis 6: Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality in FtF and CMC Results of the study indicated that there is a strong positive relationship between selfdisclosure and relationship quality in both FtF and CMC contexts. This confirms what literature has suggested that self-disclosure is one of the most important factors that 43 contribute to relationship quality (e.g., Parker & Asher, 1993; Rose & Roades, 1987; Nicotera, 1993) and expanded the results to specifically contexts of LDFs and CMC. The greater people disclose to their long-distance friends, no matter face to face or via the Internet, the more satisfied they are with the relationship. Research Question 5: Contextual Influence on Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality Results of this study suggest that self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts is important to relationship quality in LDFs. FtF self-disclosure is the primary factor, which accounts for 31.4% of the variance in relational quality. And beyond that, there is an additional unique variance associated with CMC disclosure. A combination of FtF selfdisclosure and CMC disclosure would account for 37.1% of the variance in relationship quality. This suggests that if people want to keep their LDFs on a satisfactory level, they should engage in both FtF self-disclosure and CMC disclosure. Hypothesis 7: Correlation between CMC Competence and CMC Disclosure CMC literature suggested that if one person is competent in communicating with others through the Internet, he or she may feel more comfortable to disclose more to others in CMC environment. Results of the study confirmed that there is a moderate positive relationship between CMC competence and CMC disclosure. Research Question 6: Influence of CMC Competence on Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality Research question six attempted to test how a person’s CMC competence mediates the relationship between CMC disclosure and relationship quality. Results of the study did 44 not show any influence of CMC competence on the relationship between these two variables. In other words, whether or not a person is competent in mediated communication via the Internet, if he or she is likely to disclose more to others in CMC environment, a more satisfactory relationship can be achieved in LDFs. However, further examination indicated that in this population there is a ceiling effect associated with CMC competence. Although the hypothetical mean value of CMC competence should be 45, people who participated in the study showed much higher CMC competence level (M = 61.37, SD = 7.068). In other words, virtually every respondent in the study was competent in using CMC for communication in LDFs. This helps explain why CMC competence did not have much influence on the relationship between CMC disclosure and relationship quality. Theoretical Implications Theoretically the present study adds to a rich body of research on relational communication and long-distance relationships, especially self-disclosure studies, in traditional face-to-face context and extends it to the computer-mediated communication environment. Literature shows that self-disclosure is a very important relational maintenance strategy and is closed correlated with relational quality. The current study also proved that self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts are crucial to the quality of LDFs. In addition to testing traditional communication theories in a new context, this study also attempts to answer several controversial research questions. For instance, there have been debates about gender differences in FtF self-disclosure. The present study not only confirmed that there is gender difference in self-disclosure, but females tend to disclose slightly more than males do in both FtF and CMC contexts. 45 Additionally, the impact of geographic distance in today’s society, particularly on long-distance relationships was examined. Although the overall trend in the world is that the influence of geographic distance is diminishing, scholars hold different opinions with regard to the role of distance in long-distance relational communication. The results of this study suggest that geographic distance is no longer a factor in the degree self-disclosure in LDFs. Once people are separate, or in other words are involved in long-distance relational status, it does not matter how far they are apart from each other. As the review of CMC literature shows, relational communication behaviors have not yet been well-studied. Research on self-disclosure in CMC context is limited to people who do not know each other or romantic relationships on the Internet. The present study helps to better understand the prevalent relational phenomenon of LDFs and CMC disclosure behaviors between people who know each other well and significantly value their relationships. Looking at LDFs from an intercultural communication perspective, the current study took an important step and tested cultural influence on self-disclosure behaviors in both FtF and CMC contexts. And the research findings suggest that we take self-disclosure as a human factor rather than a culture-specific factor. Practical Implications The research findings of this study also provide practical information for the people in LDFs. Although the present study was not designed to examine the prevalence of this phenomenon, the descriptive data reported in the early session of research results would help give a general picture of LDFs in today’s society. 46 More importantly, these findings reinforce the importance of self-disclosure in FtF communication as well as in CMC context and how such a combination would lead to relationship quality. People should not assume there would be difficulties or challenges when it comes to LDRs. Self-disclosure can help maintain LDFs despite geographic distance. And different cultural backgrounds should not inhibit people from enhancing their friendship through self-disclosure either. Limitations The present study has a number of limitations. First, the structure of research participants is somewhat unbalanced. Close to 70% of the respondents were U.S. American undergraduate students and a majority of those were from European descent. Although today more females attend college than males do, women were still slightly over-represented in the sample of this study. A second limitation of the study lies in the web survey. A number of potential respondents were excluded from the sample for not completing a major part of the questionnaire. There are certain factors in conducting a research study online that are beyond the researcher’s control. Computer breakdown, Internet browser error, personal security system may all eliminate people from completing the survey. Also, having matrix questions with many statements may reduce the respondent’s interest in answering the questions and cause more response error. In addition, the study used self-reported survey as research methodology. Speaking from psychological perspective, all self-disclosure reported in the study is measured as perceived behaviors rather than actual behaviors. And many studies (e.g. Floyd, 1998; Hook, Gerstein, Detterich, & Gridley, 2003) have shown that people tend to associate self- 47 disclosure with intimacy, which means they would naturally think self-disclosure as part of intimacy which leads to intimate relationship – LDF in this case. Finally, the role of target person is an important variable in testing self-disclosure. And given the information about the country a respondent was raised in, more cultural dimension scores could be entered to the data file for further analyses. However, given constraint timeframe, this study did not touch on those topics. Suggestions for Future Studies Future studies on this topic may further explore the differences in self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts in terms of how the behaviors vary in different dimensions of self-disclosure with a better multi-factor measurement. In stead of a self-reported survey questionnaire, other type of quantitative research methodology can be used to code actual self-disclosure behaviors in order to reduce respondents’ perceptional bias. In addition, the target person has been suggested as an important factor to selfdisclosure. Given time constraint, research questions or hypotheses were not included in relation to the target person, and many other possible dynamics between target influence and associated variables. Future studies should provide descriptive analyses about the longdistance friends chosen as the target for self-disclosure and further research questions and hypotheses should be proposed and tested. For example, are gender differences consistent across dyads with different gender combinations? Would the cultural background of the target person impact self-disclosure? Furthermore, the few null findings provided by the current study are worth further investigations. Same or similar research questions about the influence of geographic distance, cultural values, and CMC competence on self-disclosure and its relationship with friendship 48 quality should be replicated among different groups and even a larger number of people. Hofstede’s country scores for cultural dimensions other than individualism versus collectivism can also be added to the research data to detect the influence of other factors on self-disclosure in both FtF and CMC contexts. The current study only aimed exploring the differences in self-disclosure between FtF and CMC settings in LDFs. However, other types of mediated communication, such as telephone, can also be added to future studies in order to provide a better and complete picture to help explain where CMC stands on the relational maintenance in LDRs and how it functions differently from other channels. 49 REFERENCES Afifi, W. A., & Guerrero, L. K. (1998). Some things are better left unsaid II: Topic avoidance in friendships. Communication Quarterly, 46, 231-249. Afifi, W. A., Guerrero, L. K., & Egland, K. L. (1994, June). Maintenance behaviors in sameand opposite-sex friendships: Connections to gender, relational closeness, and equity issues. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Network on Personal Relationships, Iowa City, IA. Altman, I. (1975). The environment and social behavior: Privacy, personal space, territory, and crowding. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Andersen, P. A. (2000). Cues of culture: The basis of intercultural differences in nonverbal communication. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (pp. 258-270). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Archer, R. L. (1979). Anatomical and psychological sex differences. In G. J. Chelune and Associates (Eds.), Self-disclosure: Origins, patterns, and implications of openness in interpersonal relationships (pp. 80-109), Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA. Archer, R. L. (1980). Self-disclosure. In D. M. Wegner & R. R. Vallacher (Eds.), The self in social psychology (pp. 183-204). London: Oxford University Press. Aron, A., & Fraley, B. (1999). Relationship closeness as including other in the self: Cognitive underpinnings and measures. Social Cognition, 17, 140-160. Aylor, B. (2003). Maintaining long-distance friendships. In D. J. Canary & M. Dainton (Eds.), Maintaining relationships through communication: Relational, contextual, and cultural variations (pp.127-140). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Baron, N. S. (1998). Letters by phone or speech by other means: The linguistics of e-mail. Language and Communication, 18, 133-170. Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogs and dialectics. New York: Guilford. Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (2000). Rethinking communication in personal relationships from a dialectical perspective. In K. Dindia & S. Duck (Eds.), 50 Communication and personal relationships (pp. 31-53). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Baym, N. K. (2002). Interpersonal life online. In L. A. Lievrouw & S. Livingstone (Eds.), Handbook of new media (pp. 62-76). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Blieszner, R., & Adams, R. G. (1992). Adult friendship. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bradac, J. J. (1983). The language of lovers, flowers, and friends: Communicating in social and personal relationships. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 2, 141-162. Brehm, S. S. (1992). Intimate relationships (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Brownell, C., & Carriger, M. (1990). Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. Child Development, 61, 1164-1174. Bruess, C. J. S., & Pearson, J. C. (1997). Interpersonal rituals in marriage and adult friendship. Communication Monographs, 64, 25-46. Cairncross, F. (2001). The death of distance: How the communications revolution is changing our lives. Boston: Harvard Business School Press. Caldwell, M. A., & Peplau, L. A. (1982). Sex differences in same-sex friendships. Sex Roles, 8, 721-732. Candy-Gibbs, S. E. (1982, November). The alleged inferiority of men’s lose relationships: An examination of sex differences in the elderly widowed. Paper presented at the annual scientific meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, Boston. Castellá, V. O., Abad, A. M. Z., Alonso, F. P., & Silla, J. M. P. (2000). The influence of familiarity among group members, group atmosphere and assertiveness on uninhibited behavior through three different communication media. Computers in Human Behavior, 16, 141-159. Chen, G. M. (1995). Differences in self-disclosure patterns among Americans versus Chinese: A comparative study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 84-91. Clark, L. S. (1998). Dating on the net: Teens and the rise of “pure” relationships. In S. G. Jones (Ed.), Cybersociety 2.0: Revisiting computer-mediated communication and community (pp.159-183). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cline, R. J. (1983, November). Promising new directions for teaching and research: Self disclosure. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the Speech Communication Association, Washington, D.C. 51 Chao, I. L. & Yang, N. H. (2001, November). 10 o’ clock date: Let’s go Yahoo! Revealing and managing on-line emotions in a Taiwanese distance family. Paper presented to the Ethnography Division of the National Communication Association, Atlanta, GA. Chenault, B. G. (1998, May). Developing personal and emotional relationships via computermediated communication. CMC Magazine. Retrieved September 7, 2000 from http://www.december.com/cmc/mag/1998/may/chenault.html Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cozby, P. C. (1973). Self-disclosure: A literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 73-91. Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. L. (1987). Information technologies. In F. M. Jablin, L. L. Putnam, K. H. Roberts, & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizational communication: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 420-443). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Cummings, J. N. (2002). Moving beyond media: A comparison of face-to-face, phone, email, and instant messaging content. Unpublished manuscript, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA. Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1984). Information richness: A new approach to managerial behavior and organization design. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (vol. 6, pp. 191-233). Greewich, CT: JAI. Daft, R. L., Lengel, R. H., & Trevino, L. K. (1987). Message equivocality, media selection, and manager performance: Implications for information systems. MIS Quarterly, 11, 355-368. Davis, K. E. & Todd, M. (1985). Assessing friendship: Prototypes, paradigm cases, and relationship description. In S. W. Duck & D. Perlman (Eds.), Understanding Personal Relationships (pp.17-38). London: Sage. Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Margulis, S. T. (1993). Self-disclosure. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Derlega, V. J., & Chaikin, A. (1977). Privacy and self-disclosure in social relationships. Journal of Social Issues, 33, 102-115. Dimmick, J., Kline, S. L., & Stafford, L. (2000). The gratification niches of personal e-mail and the telephone: competition, displacement, and complementarity. Communication Research, 27 (2), 227-248. 52 Dindia, K. (1997). Self-disclosure, identity, and relationship development: A transactional/dialectical perspective. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 411-426). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Dindia, K. (2000a). Self-disclosure, identity, and relationship development: A dialectical perspective. In K. Dindia & S. Duck (Eds.), Communication and personal relationships (pp. 147-162). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Dindia, K. (2000b). Sex differences in self-disclosure, reciprocity of self-disclosure, and selfdisclosure and liking: Three meta-analyses reviewed. In S. Petronio (Ed.), Balancing the secrets of private disclosures (pp. 21-37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Dindia, K., & Allen, M. (1992). Sex differences in self-disclosure: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 106-124. Duck, S., Rutt, D. J., Hurst, M. H., & Strejc, H. (1991). Some evident truths about conversations in everyday relationships: All communications are not created equal. Human Communication Research, 18, 228-267. They found that "Self-disclosure is much less frequent in everyday life than assumed on the basis of laboratory work". Duck, S., & Wright, P. H. (1993). Reexamining gender differences in friendship: A close look at two kinds of data. Sex Roles, 28, 709-727. Fehr, B. (1996). Friendship processes. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ferris, P. (1997). What is CMC? An overview of scholarly definitions. Computer-Mediated Communication Magazine. Retrieved March 15, 2002 from http://www.december.com/cmc/mag/1997/jan/ferris.html Flaherty, L. M., Pearce, K. J., & Rubin, R. R. (1998). Internet and face-to-face communication: Not functional alternatives. Communication Quarterly, 46, 250-268. Floyd, K. (1998). Intimacy as a research construct: A content-analytic review. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 22, 28-32. Gilbert, S. J. (1976). Empirical and theoretical extensions of self-disclosure. In G. R. Miller (ed.), Explorations in interpersonal communication, (pp. 197-215). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Goodstein, L. & Reinecker, V. (1974). Factors affecting self-disclosure: A review of the literature. Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 7, 49-77. Guerrero, L. K., & Afifi, W. A. (1995). Some things are better left unsaid: Topic avoidance in family relationships. Communication Quarterly, 43, 276-296. 53 Guldner, G. T., & Swensen, C. H. (1995). Time spent together and relationship quality: Long-distance relationships as a test case. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12, 313-320. Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. New York: Anchor Press. Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relations. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol 4: Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 103-196). New York: Wiley. Hays, R. B. (1985). A longitudinal study of friendship development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 909-924. Hays, R. B. (1988). Friendship. In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 391-408). London: John Wiley & Sons. Helgeson, V. S., Shaver, P., & Dyer, M. (1987). Prototypes of intimacy and distance in samesex and opposite-sex relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 4, 195-233. Hill, C. T., & Stull, D. E. (1987). Gender and self-disclosure: Strategies for exploring the issues. In V. J. Derlega & J. H. Berg (Eds.), Self-disclosure: Theory, research and therapy (pp. 81-100). New York: Plenum Press. Hofstede, G. (1983). Dimensions of national cultures in fifty countries and three regions. In J. B. Deregowski, S. Dziurawiec, & R. C. Annis (Eds.), Expectations in crosscultural psychology (pp. 335-355). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger. Hofstede, G. (1986). Culture’s consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hook, M. K., Gerstein, L. H., Detterich, L., & Gridley, B. (2003). How close are we? Measuring intimacy and examining gender differences. Journal of Counseling & Development, 81, 462-472. Howard, P. E. N., Rainie, L., & Jones, S. (2001). Days and nights on the Internet: The impact of a diffusing technology. American Behavioral Scientist, 45, 383-404. Hunt, G. J., & Butler, E. W. (1972). Migration, participation, and alienation. Sociology and Social Research, 56, 440-452. Jackson, M. (2002). Familiar and foreign bodies: A phenomenological exploration of the human-technology interface. Royal Anthropological Institute, 8, 333-346. Johnson, A. J. (2001). Examining the maintenance of friendships: Are there differences between geographically close and long-distance friends? Communication Quarterly, 49, 425-436. 54 Johnson, A. J., Becker, J., Wigley, S., Wittenberg, E., & Haigh, M. (2003, May). What geographic distance can illustrate about relational closeness: Close long-distance friendships. Paper presented at the annual conference of the International Communication Association, San Diego. Joinson, A. N. (1998). Causes and implications of disinhibited behavior on the Internet. In J. Gackenbach (Ed.), Psychology and the Internet: Intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal implications (pp. 43-60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Joinson, A. N. (2001). Self-disclosure in computer-mediated communication: The role of self-awareness and visual anonymity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 177-192. Jourard, S. M. (1961). Age trends in self-disclosure. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 7, 191-197. Jourard, S. M. (1971). Self-disclosure: An experimental analysis of the transparent self. New York: Wiley. Jourard, S. M., & Lasakow, P. (1958). Some factors in self-disclosure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 91-98. Jourard, S. M., & Richman, P. (1963). Disclosure output and input in college students. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 9, 141-148. Knox, D., Daniels, V., Sturdivant, L., & Zusman, M. E. (2001). College student use of the internet for mate selection. College Student Journal, 35, 158-160. Ladd, G. W. (1999). Peer relationships and social competence during early and middle childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 333-359. Laurenceau, J., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1238-1251. Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1995). Love at first byte? Building personal relationships over computer networks. In J. Y. Wood & S. Duck (Eds.), Understanding relationships: Off the beaten track (pp. 197-233). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. McKenna, K. Y.A., Green, A. S., & Gleason, M. E. J. (2002). Relationship formation on the Internet: What’s the big attraction. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 9-32. 55 Merkle, E. R., & Richardson, R. A. (2000). Digital dating and virtual relating: Conceptualizing computer mediated romantic relationships. Family Relations, 49, 187-192. Moemeka, A. A. & Nicotera, A. M. (1993). The friendship formation process in Nigeria: A preliminary study of cultural impact, communication pattern, and relationship variables. In A. M. Nicotera (ed.), Interpersonal communication in friend and mate relationships (p. 107-124). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Monsour, M. (1992). Meanings of intimacy in cross- and same-sex friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 9, 277-295. Nakanishi, M., & Johnson, K. M. (1993). Implications of self-disclosure on conversational logics, perceived communication competence, and social attraction. In R. L. Wiseman & J. Koester (Eds.), Intercultural communication competence (pp. 204-221). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Nardi, B. A., Whittaker, S., & Bradner, E. (2000). Interaction and outeraction: Instant messaging in action. Retrieved March, 12, 2002, from Proceedings of the Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW 2000) http://www.research.att.com/~stevew/outeraction_cscw2000.pdf Nicotera, A. M. (1993). Summary of studies on the theory of friendship and consideration of implication. In A. M. Nicotera (ed.), Interpersonal communication in friend and mate relationships (pp. 125-138). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Nielsen/NetRatings (2003, February 20). Global Internet population grows an average of four percent year-after-year. Retrieved May 9, 2003 from http://www.nielsennetratings.com/pr/pr_030220.hk.pdf Parker, J. G. & Asher, S. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 611-621. Parks, M. R. (1977). Anomie and close friendship communication networks. Human Communication Research, 4, 48-57. Parks, M. R. & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communication, 46, 80-97. Parks, R. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). ‘Making MOOsic’: The development of personal relationships on line and a comparison to their off-line counterparts. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 517-537. Parlee, M. B. (1979). The friendship bond. Psychology Today, 113, 43-54. 56 Pearce, W. B., & Sharp, S. M. (1973). Self-disclosing communication. Journal of Communication, 23, 409-425. Petronio, S. (1988, November). The dissemination of private information: The use of a boundary control system as an alternative perspective to the study of disclosures. Paper presented at the Speech Communication Association Convention, New Orleans, LA. Petronio, S. (1991). Communication boundary management: A theoretical model of managing disclosure of private information between marital couples. Communication Theory, 1, 311-335. Petronio, S. (2000). The boundaries of privacy: Praxis of everyday life. In S. Petronio (Ed.), Balancing the secrets of private disclosures (pp. 37-50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Pew Internet & American Life Project. (2000, May 10). Tracking online life: How women use the Internet to cultivate relationships with family and friends. Washington, DC: Pew Internet and American Life Project. Retrieved January 10, 2003 from http://www.pewinternet.org Pew Internet & American Life Project. (2001, June 20). Teenage life online: The rise of the instant-message generation and the Internet’s impact on friendships and family relationships. Washington, DC: Pew Internet and American Life Project. Retrieved December 8, 2002, from http://www.pewinternet.org Pew Internet & American Life Project. (2002, September 15). The Internet goes to college: How students are living in the future with today's technology. Washington, DC: Pew Internet and American Life Project. Retrieved December 8, 2002, from http://www.pewinternet.org Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Lea, M. (1998). Breaching or building social boundaries? SIDEeffects of computer-mediated communication. Communication Research, 25, 689715. Rabby, M. & Walther, J. (2003). Computer-mediated communication effects on relationship formation and maintenance. In D. J. Canary & M. Dainton (Eds.), Maintaining relationships through communication: Relational, contextual, and cultural variations (pp.141-162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Ramirez, Jr. A., Walther, J. B., Burgoon, J. K., & Sunnafrank, M. (2002). Informationseeking strategies, uncertainty, and computer-mediated communication: Toward a conceptual model. Human Communication Research, 28, 213-228. Rawlins, W. K. (1992). Friendship matters: Communication, dialectics, and the life course. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine De Gruyter. 57 Reid, E. (1991). Electropolis: Communication and community on Internet Relay Chat. Unpublished honors thesis, Department of History, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia. Reis, H. T. (1998). Gender difference in intimacy and related behaviors: Context and process. In D. J. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication (pp. 203-231). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rheingold, H. (1993). The virtual community: Homesteading on the electronic frontier. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Robin, L. (1985). Just friends: The role of friendship in our lives. New York: HarperCollins. Rohlfing, M. E. (1990, November). Communicating long-distance friendship. Paper presented at the annual convention of the Speech Communication Association, Chicago. Rohlfing, M. E. (1995). “Doesn’t anybody stay in one place anymore?” An exploration of the under-studied phenomenon of long-distance relationships. In J. T. Wood & S. Duck (Eds.), Under-studied relationships: Off the beaten track (Vol. 6) (pp. 173-196). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rohlfing, M. E., & Healey, A. C. (1991, May). Keeping the tie that binds: The communication patterns of geographically separated female friends. Paper presented at the annual convention of the International Communication Association, Miami. Roscoe, B., Kennedy, D., & Pope, T. (1987). Adolescents' views of intimacy: Distinguishing intimate from nonintimate relationships. Adolescence, 22, 511-516. Rose, S. & Roades, L. (1987). Feminism and women’s friendships. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 243-254. Rose, S. M. (1984). How friendships end: Patterns among young adults. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1, 267-277. Rose, S. M. (1985). Same- and cross-sex friendships and the psychology of homosociality. Sex Roles, 12, 63-74. Rosenfeld, L. B. (2000). Overview of the ways privacy, secrecy, and disclosure are balanced in today’s society. In S. Petronio (Ed.), Balancing the secrets of private disclosures (pp. 3-17). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rosenfeld, L. B., Civikly, J. M., & Herron, J. R. (1979). Anatomical and psychological sex differences. In G. J. Chelune (Ed.), Self-disclosure: Origins, patterns, and implications of openness in interpersonal relationships (pp. 80-109). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 58 Rosenfeld, L. B., & Kendrick, W. L. (1984). Choosing to be open: An empirical investigation of subjective reasons for self-disclosing. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 48, 326-343. Ross, H., & Lollis, S. (1987). Communication within infant social games. Developmental Psychology, 23, 241-248. Rubin, D. L., Yang, H., & Porte, M. (2000). A comparison of self-reported self-disclosure among Chinese and North Americans. In S. Petronio (Ed.), Balancing the secrets of private disclosures (pp. 215-234). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rumbough, T. (2001). The development and maintenance of interpersonal relationships through computer-mediated communication. Communication Research Reports, 18, 223-229. Samter, W. (2003). Friendship interaction skills across the life span. In J. O. Greene & B. R. Burleson (Eds.), Handbook of communication and social interaction skills (pp. 637684). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Sandlund, E. & Geist-Martin, P. (2001). A blast from the past: Emotions in e-motion in the email reconstruction of past love. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Atlanta, GA. Short, J., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976). The social psychology of telecommunications. London: John Wiley. Spitzberg, B. H. (2004, February). Preliminary development of a model and measure of computer-mediated communication (CMC) competence. Paper presented to the Communication Theory Interest Group, Western States Communication Association Conference, Albuquerque, NM. Sproull, L., & Faraj, S. (1997). Atheism, sex, and databases: The net as a social technology. In S. Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the Internet (pp. 35-52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Sproull, L. & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Science, 32, 1492-1512. Stafford, L, (1988, October). Communication in long-distance relationships. Paper presented at the annual convention of the Speech Communication Association, Chicago, IL. Stafford, L., Kline, S. L., & Dimmick, J. (1999). Home e-mail: Relational maintenance and gratification opportunities. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 43, 659669. 59 Suttles, G. D. (1970). Friendship as a social institution. In G. J. McCall, M. McCall, N. K. Denzin, G. D. Suttles, & S. Kurth (Eds.), Social relationships (pp. 95-135). Chicago: Aldine. Thompson, P. A., & Foulger, D. A. (1996). Effects of pictographs and quoting on flaming in electronic mail. Computers in Human Behavior, 12, 225-243. Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). Computer-mediated communication effects on disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one another a bit at a time. Human Communication Research, 28, 317-348. Ting-Toomey, S. (1991). Intimacy expressions in three cultures: France, Japan, and the United States. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 15, 29-46. Tolstedt, B. E., & Stokes, J. P. (1984). Self-disclosure, intimacy, and the depenetration process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 84-90. Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview. Utz, S. (2000). Social information processing in MUDs: The development of friendships in virtual worlds. Journal of Online Behavior, 1 (1). Retrieved April 29, 2003 from http:// www.behavior.net/JOB/v1n1/utz.html Van Horn, K. R., Arnone, A., Nesbitt, K., Desilets, L., Sears, T., Giffin, M., & Brudi, R. (1997). Physical distance and interpersonal characteristics in college students’ romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 4, 15-24. Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interactions: A relational perspective. Communication Research, 19, 52-90. Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersoanl interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3-43. Walther, J. B., & D’Addario, K. P. (2001). The impacts of emoticons on message interpretation in computer-mediated communication. Social Science Computer Review, 19, 323-345. Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues filtered out, cues filtered in: Computer-mediated communication and relationships. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of Interpersonal Communication (pp. 529-563). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Waring, E. M., Tillman, M. P., Frelick, L., Russell, L., & Weisz, G. (1980). Concepts of intimacy in the general population. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 168, 471-474. 60 Waugh, A. L. (1995). Relational computer mediated communication: Impression forming, uncertainty reduction, competence, satisfaction and attraction. Unpublished master’s thesis, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA. Wheeless, L. R., & Grotz, J. (1976). Conceptualization and measurement of reported selfdisclosure. Human Communication Research, 3, 338-346. Wheeless, L. R. (1978). A follow-up study of the relationships among trust, disclosure, and interpersonal solidarity. Human Communication Research, 4, 143-157. Whitty, M. T. (2002). Liar, liar! An examination of how open, supportive and honest people are in chat rooms. Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 343-352. Wolfson, K., & Pearce, W. B. (1983). A cross-cultural comparison of the implications of self-disclosure on conversational logics. Communication Quarterly, 31, 249-256. Wood, J. T. (1995). Relational communication: Continuity and change in personal relationships. Detroit: Wadsworth. Wood, A., & Smith, M. (2001). Online communication: Linking technology, identity, and culture. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Wright, P. H. (1974). The delineation and measurement of some variables in the study of friendship. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 5, 93-96. Wright, P. H. (1982, February). The development and selected applications of a conceptual and measurement model of friendship. Paper presented at the Texas Tech University Conference on Families and Close Relationships, Lubbock, TX. Wright, P. H. (1984). Self referent motivation and the intrinsic quality of friendship. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1, 114-130. Wright, P. H. (1988). Interpreting research on gender differences in friendship: A case for moderation and a plea for caution. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 367-373. Wright, P. H. (1998). Toward an expanded orientation to the study of sex differences in friendship. In D. J. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication: Critical essays and empirical investigations of sex and gender in interaction (pp. 41-63). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Yum, J. O. (1991). The impact of Confucianism on interpersonal relationships and communication patterns in Eat Asia. In L. Samovar & R. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (5th ed., pp. 66-77). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. 61 APPENDIX A RESEARCH ANNOUNCEMENT SENT TO COMMUNICATION STUDENTS 62 Research Announcement Hello, My name is Helen. I'm a graduate student in the School of Communication at SDSU. I’m currently working on my Master’s thesis about long-distance friendships. This research study attempts to understand how people communicate with their long-distance friends in different contexts. Today, in order to pursue higher education, develop professional careers, and immigrate to different countries, more and more people are separated from their friends and involved in long-distance relationships. However, this prevalent phenomenon of long-distance friendship is still under-studied. So do YOU have at least one friend who lives in another city, state, or country? Have you both met your long-distance friend in person and communicated through the Internet? And are you currently enrolled in SDSU? If ALL your answers to the questions above are yes, then I would really appreciate it if you could spare 10 minutes of your time to help me out. Your participation in this study is completely voluntary. All your responses will remain anonymous. And all the information collected will be used only for this study. This study has been reviewed by the Institutional Review Board. If you have any questions or concerns, please feel free to contact them at [email protected] or 619-594-6622. If you are interested in participating in this study, please send an email to [email protected] Thank you very much! Sincerely, Helen 63 APPENDIX B RESEARCH ANNOUNCEMENT SENT THROUGH ISC WEEKLY NEWSLETTER 64 Your help needed My name is Helen. I'm a graduate student in the School of Communication at SDSU. I’m currently working on my Master’s thesis about long-distance friendships. This research study attempts to understand how people communicate with their long-distance friends in different contexts. Today, in order to pursue higher education, develop professional careers, and immigrate to different countries, more and more people are separated from their friends and involved in long-distance relationships. However, this prevalent phenomenon of long-distance friendship is still under-studied. So do YOU have at least one friend who lives in another city, state, or country? Have you both met your long-distance friend in person and communicated through the Internet? And are you currently enrolled in SDSU? If ALL your answers to the questions above are yes, then I would really appreciate it if you could spare 10 minutes of your time to help me out. Your participation in this study is completely voluntary. All your responses will remain anonymous. And all the information collected will be used only for this study. This study has been reviewed by the Institutional Review Board. If you have any questions or concerns, please feel free to contact them at [email protected] or 619-594-6622. If you are interested in participating in this study, please send an email to [email protected] Thank you very much! Sincerely, Helen 65 APPENDIX C EMAIL MESSAGE WITH LINK TO WEB SURVEY 66 Helen's Web Survey on Long-Distance Friendships Hello, I'm conducting this web survey for my Master's thesis on long-distance friendships, and your response would be appreciated. Here is a link to the survey: [SurveyLink] Please be noted that once you start the survey online, you can NOT go back and change your answers. If you choose to exit the survey in the middle of the questionnaire, you won't be able to complete the study. If you encounter any problem with the web survey, please feel free to email me. Thanks for taking time to help me out and good luck with everything! Sincerely, Helen 67 APPENDIX D HELEN’S WEB SURVEY 68 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Introduction Welcome! My name is Helen. I'm a graduate student in the School of Communication at San Diego State University. This research study attempts to understand how people communicate with their long-distance friends in different contexts. Let (1) (2) (3) me assure you that your participation is completely voluntary; all your responses will remain anonymous; and all the information collected will be used only for this study. The survey takes about 10 minutes. I would really appreciate your time and input! Also, please be noted that this study has been reviewed by the Institutional Review Board (IRB). If you have any questions or concerns, please feel free to contact them ([email protected] and 619-594-6622). Next >> 69 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Qualification Test To start off with, I need to find out if you are eligible for this study. Please test your qualification by answering the following questions: 1. Are you 18 years of age or older? Yes No 2. Are you currently enrolled at SDSU? Yes No 3. Do you have at least one friend who lives in another city, state, or country? Yes No 4. Have you physically met your long-distance friend? Yes No 5. Have you ever communicated with your long-distance friend through the Internet? Yes No Next >> 70 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section A-Intro This first section asks about your use of CMC technologies. CMC stands for computer-mediated communication. Examples of CMC include interactions through the Internet or World Wide Web, email, chat room, BBS, online instant messenger, etc. Next >> 71 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements regarding your use of computer-mediated communication (CMC) technologies. Please use the 5-point scale below: 1 = not at all true of me 2 = mostly not true of me 3 = neither true nor untrue of me, undecided 4 = mostly true of me 5 = very true of me not at all mostly not I manage the give and take of computer-mediated interactions skillfully I am very articulate and vivid in my CMC messages I show concern for and interest in the person I'm conversing with in CMC I use an assertive style in my CMC writing I'm very familiar with email and communication networks I generally get what I want out of my CMC interactions I am very motivated to use computers to communicate with others I know when and how to close down a topic of conversation in CMC dialogues neither / mostly undecided very 72 I am expressive in my CMC conversations My CMC messages are written in a confident style My CMC messages are appropriate to the situation I avoid saying things through CMC that might offend someone I am very knowledgeable about how to communicate through computers I ask questions of the other person in my CMC I am effective when using CMC technologies I enjoy communicating using computer media Next >> 73 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section A-2 Approximately, how many close, long-distance friends do you have right now? (Please use your best estimate!) Next >> 74 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section B-Intro Now, please pick a friend with ALL of the following characteristics: * who lives in another city, state, or country * with whom you have both face-to-face and computer-mediated communication experience * who is not your primary romantic partner or "a friend with benefits" Please write down the first name, nickname, or a pseudonym of your long-distance friend below, and keep this person in mind when you answer the rest part of this questionnaire! The long-distance friend I choose to complete this study is called: Next >> 75 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section B-1 How did you get to know this long-distance friend? through face-to-face interactions through communication via Internet through other channels Where is your long-distance friend located now? City: State/Province: Country: Zip Code:(only for the areas in the U.S.) Next >> 76 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section B-2 How often do you communicate with your long-distance friend through the following channels? never seldom sometimes usually online instant messenger letters blog email telephone chat room pager face to face Next >> always 77 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section C-1a Now, please think of a typical situation when you reveal to your long-distance friend FACE TO FACE about private, intimate, and/or risky personal information, experiences, thoughts, feelings, and emotions. Remember these questions are about FACE TO FACE communication! Next >> 78 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements based on the face-to-face interactions between you and your long-distance friend? strongly somewhat disagree disagree My long-distance friend always lets me know about his/her feelings I tend to reveal "good" feelings about myself I tell my longdistance friend personal opinions about sensitive issues Keeping our conversations on a superficial level is all right There are certain topics I do not discuss with this long-distance friend I talk about all kinds of issues with my long-distance friend I share feelings with my long-distance friend when I'm happy, but not when I'm sad I know my intimate disclosure to this long-distance friend will be rewarded neutral / undecided somewhat agree strongly agree 79 I tell this longdistance friend about things I normally would not tell others Our conversations usually cover a variety of topics I do not say anything that might ruin our longdistance friendship My long-distance friend discloses about the same amount as I do Next >> 80 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section C-2a Now, please think of a typical situation when you reveal to your long-distance friend through communication on the INTERNET about private, intimate, and/or risky personal information, experiences, thoughts, feelings, and emotions. Remember these next questions are about communication via the INTERNET! Next >> 81 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements based on the online communication between you and your long-distance friend? strongly somewhat neutral / somewhat strongly disagree disagree undecided agree agree My long-distance friend always lets me know about his/her feelings I tend to reveal "good" feelings about myself I tell my longdistance friend personal opinions about sensitive issues Keeping our conversations on a superficial level is all right There are certain topics I do not discuss with this long-distance friend I talk about all kinds of issues with my long-distance friend I share feelings with my long-distance friend when I'm happy, but not when I'm sad I know my intimate disclosure to this long-distance friend will be rewarded I tell this longdistance friend about things I normally would not tell others Our conversations 82 usually cover a variety of topics I do not say anything that might ruin our longdistance friendship My long-distance friend discloses about the same amount as I do Next >> 83 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section D-Intro This next section asks about the FRIENDSHIP between you and the long-distance friend you chose for this study. Next >> 84 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements? strongly somewhat neutral / somewhat strongly disagree disagree undecided agree agree I feel close to my longdistance friend I feel emotionally distant from my long-distance friend Overall, the communication is effective between my long-distance friend and I I have developed into a better person because of this long-distance friend I am unhappy with my longdistance friendship This longdistance friend takes more than he/she gives I have learned a lot from my long-distance friend I am satisfied with my longdistance friendship 85 I have warm feelings toward my longdistance friend Next >> 86 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Section E-1 This last section asks about some information about YOU and YOUR LONG-DISTANCE FRIEND. What is your sex? female male What is the sex of your long-distance friend? female male What is your age? What is the age of your long-distance friend? Next >> 87 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> What country were you raised in? the United States Other (please specify) What country was your long-distance friend raised in? the United States Other (please specify) Which of the following is your primary cultural background? Which of the following is your long-distance friend's primary cultural background? What is your current relational status with your long-distance friend? casual acquaintance just friend good friend close friend best friend Next >> Helen's Web Survey Section F Exit this survey >> 88 Are you taking COMM103 (Oral Communication) class this semester? Yes No Next >> 89 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Extra Credit If you are taking this web survey for extra credit in COMM103 class, please send an email to Helen at [email protected] RIGHT NOW! In the subject field, please type in: Subject: Helen's Web Survey In the text, please provide the following information: 1) your first name AND last name, 2) your COMM103 instructor's name, 3) your COMM103 section number (if known for sure) Your name will be sent to your instructor to confirm that you completed this web survey. Please be noted that you will NOT be able to receive the extra credit if the information you provided in the email to Helen is INCOMPLETE or INCORRECT. Next >> 90 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Information Confirmation When you come to this screen, you should have already sent the confirmation email to Helen. If NOT, please go back and send the email following the instructions. Are you sure the information you provided in the email to Helen is both COMPLETE and ACCURATE? Yes No Next >> 91 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Congratulations! Congratulations! You have just completed the web survey! Once you click the "Next" link below, your answers will be automatically sent to Helen. Next >> 92 Helen's Web Survey Exit this survey >> Thank you so much for your time! Have a nice day! Done 93 APPENDIX E TABLES 94 Table 1 One-Factor Solution for FtF Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Item Original Statement FTFSDD1 I tell my long-distance friend personal opinions about sensitive issues .764 My long-distance friend always lets me know about his/her feelings .757 I talk about all kinds of issues with my longdistance friend .750 Our conversations usually cover a variety of topics .679 I tell this long-distance friend about things I normally would not tell others .654 I know my intimate disclosure to this longdistance friend will be rewarded .580 My long-distance friend discloses about the same amount as I do .564 There are certain topics I do not discuss with this long-distance friend .492 I share feelings with my long-distance friend when I'm happy, but not when I'm sad -.451 Keeping our conversations on a superficial level is all right .413 I tend to reveal "good" feelings about myself .262 I do not say anything that might ruin our longdistance friendship ---- (FtFSD depth1) FTFSDR3 (FtFSD reciprocity3) FTFSDB1 (FtFSD breadth1) FTFSDB3 (FtFSD breadth3) FTFSDD3 (FtFSD depth3) FTFSDR2 (FtFSD reciprocity2) FTFSDR1 (FtFSD reciprocity1) FTFSDB2R (FtFSD breadth2 reverse) FTFSDV3 (FtFSD valence3) FTFSDD2R (FtFSD depth2 reverse) FTFSDV1 Component Loading (FtFSD valence1) FTFSDV2 (FtFSD valence2) ________________________________________________________________________ 95 Table 2 One-Factor Solution for CMC Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Item Original Statement CMCSDR3 My long-distance friend always lets me know about his/her feelings .774 I talk about all kinds of issues with my longdistance friend .771 I tell my long-distance friend personal opinions about sensitive issues .762 Our conversations usually cover a variety of topics .740 I tell this long-distance friend about things I normally would not tell others .647 There are certain topics I do not discuss with this long-distance friend .549 I share feelings with my long-distance friend when I'm happy, but not when I'm sad -.499 My long-distance friend discloses about the same amount as I do .462 I know my intimate disclosure to this longdistance friend will be rewarded .459 Keeping our conversations on a superficial level is all right .456 I tend to reveal "good" feelings about myself .243 I do not say anything that might ruin our longdistance friendship -.063 (CMCSD reciprocity3) CMCSDB1 (CMCSD breadth1) CMCSDD1 (CMCSD depth1) CMCSDB3 (CMCSD breadth3) CMCSDD3 (CMCSD depth3) CMCSDB2R (CMCSD breadth2 reverse) CMCSDV3 (CMCSD valence3) CMCSDR1 (CMCSD reciprocity1) CMCSDR2 (CMCSD reciprocity2) CMCSDD2R (CMCSD depth2 reverse) CMCSDV1 Component Loading (CMCSD valence1) CMCSDV2 (CMCSD valence2) ________________________________________________________________________ 96 Table 3 One-Factor Solution for CMC Competence ________________________________________________________________________ Item Original Statement CMCOEF2 I am effective when using CMC technologies .730 I manage the give and take of computer-mediated interactions .677 I am very motivated to use computers to communicate with others .662 I am expressive in my CMC conversations. .661 I know when and how to close down a topic of conversation in CMC dialogues .656 I am very knowledgeable about how to communicate through computers .654 I enjoy communicating using computer media .624 I am very articulate and vivid in my CMC messages .618 I am very familiar with e-mail and communication networks .598 My CMC messages are appropriate to the situation .553 I ask questions of the other person in my CMC .545 I generally get what I want out of my CMC interactions .541 I show concern for and interest in the person I'm conversing with in CMC .528 I use an assertive style in my CMC writing .495 My CMC messages are written in a confident style .465 I avoid saying things through CMC that might offend someone .191 (CMC outcome-effectiveness2) CMCSCD2 (CMC skill-coordination2) CMCM2 (CMC motivation2) CMCSEX2 (CMC skill-expressiveness2) CMCSCD1 (CMC skill-coordination1) CMCK1 (CMC knowledge1) CMCM1 (CMC motivation1) CMCSEX1 (CMC skill-expressiveness1) CMCK2 (CMC knowledge2) CMCOAP2 (CMC outcome-appropriateness2) CMCSAT1 (CMC skill-attentiveness1) CMCOEF1 (CMC outcome-effectiveness1) CMCSAT2 (CMC skill-attentiveness2) CMCSCP1 (CMC skill-composure1) CMCSCP2 (CMC skill-composure2) CMCOAP1 (CMC outcome-appropriateness1) Component Loading ________________________________________________________________________ 97 Table 4 One-Factor Solution for Relationship Quality ________________________________________________________________________ Item Original Statement Component Loading RQIT2 I feel close to my long-distance friend .723 Overall, the communication is effective between my long-distance friend and I .712 I have learned a lot from my long-distance friend .683 I am satisfied with my long-distance friendship .673 I have developed into a better person because of this long-distance friend .643 I am unhappy with my long-distance friendship .643 I feel emotionally distant from my long-distance friend .513 This long-distance friend takes more than he/she gives .512 I have warm feelings toward my long-distance friend .493 (RQ intimacy2) RQCE1 (RQ communication effectiveness1) RQPG3 (RQ personal growth3) RQCE3 (RQ communication effectiveness3) RQPG2 (RQ personal growth2) RQCE2R (RQ communication effectiveness2 reverse) RQIT3R (RQ intimacy3 reverse) RQPG1R (RQ personal growth1 reverse) RQIT1 (RQ intimacy1) ________________________________________________________________________ 98 Table 5 Analysis of Variance for Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ________________________________________________________________________ Channel of Self-Disclosure (CHANNEL) M SD N ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________ FtF 29.76 4.164 382 CMC 28.17 4.572 378 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ CHANNEL 480.235 1 14965.242 759 652734.000 760 25.131 <.001 .032 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Table 6 Correlation between FtF Self-Disclosure and CMC Disclosure __________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 ___________________________________________________________________________ n = 378 1. FtF Self-Disclosure ---- .578** 2. CMC Disclosure ---- ___________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 Table 7 Intercorrelations among FtF Self-Disclosure, CMC Disclosure, and Distance ______________________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 3 _____________________________________________________________________________________________ 1. FtF Self-Disclosure 2. CMC Disclosure 3. Distance ---- .578** -.042 ---- -.016 ---- _____________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 99 Table 8 Partial Correlation Coefficient for CMC Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and Distance ______________________________________________________ Zero Order Partials _____________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 3 _______________________________________________________________________________ 1. CMC Disclosure ---- 2. Relationship Quality .522** -.017 ---- -.013* 3. Distance ---- _______________________________________________________________________________ Controlling for DISTANCE _____________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 _______________________________________________________________________________ 1. CMC Disclosure ---- .524** 2. Relationship Quality ---- _______________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001, * p < .05 Table 9 Analysis of Variance for Relationship Initiation and FtF Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Context of Friendship Initiation (KNOW) M SD N ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ FtF 29.82 4.114 351 CMC 28.33 5.420 15 Other 29.81 4.004 16 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ KNOW 31.742 2 6605.843 381 344908.000 382 .915 .401 .005 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ 100 Table 10 Analysis of Variance for Relationship Initiation and CMC Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Context of Friendship Initiation (KNOW) M SD N ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ FtF 28.09 4.633 347 CMC 29.53 3.852 15 Other 28.56 3.759 16 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ KNOW 32.444 2 7879.164 377 307826.000 378 .775 .461 .004 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Table 11 Correlation between Relational Stage and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC _____________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 3 _______________________________________________________________________________ 1. FtF Self-Disclosure 2. CMC Disclosure 3. Relational Stage ---- .578** .536** ---- .371** ---- _______________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 101 Table 12 Analysis of Variance for Gender and FtF Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Respondent’s Sex (RESPSEX) M SD N ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ female 30.51 3.857 261 male 28.13 4.428 116 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ RESPSEX 456.463 1 6578.727 376 340917.000 377 27.959 <.001 .069 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Table 13 Analysis of Variance for Gender and CMC Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Respondent’s Sex (RESPSEX) M SD N ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ female 28.52 4.742 261 male 27.37 4.091 116 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ RESPSEX 106.987 1 7879.135 376 307042.000 377 5.162 .024 .014 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ 102 Table 14 Analysis of Variance for High- versus Low-Context Culture and FtF Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Respondent’s Culture (CONTEXTR) M SD N ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ high-context culture 29.84 3.174 69 low-context culture 29.82 4.224 296 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ CONTEXTR .030 1 5949.425 364 330561.000 365 .002 .966 .000 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Table 15 Analysis of Variance for High- versus Low-Context Culture and CMC Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________________ Respondent’s Culture (CONTEXTR) M SD N ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ high-context culture 28.17 3.850 69 low-context culture 28.27 4.570 296 ________________________________________________________________________ Source Sum of Sq. df F p Eta Sq. ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ CONTEXTR .484 1 7168.312 364 298447.000 365 .024 .876 .000 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ 103 Table 16 Correlation between Individualism versus Collectivism and Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC ________________________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 3 _______________________________________________________________________________________________ 1. FtF Self-Disclosure ---- 2. CMC Disclosure .578** .001 ---- .048 3. High- vs. Low-Context Culture ---- _______________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 Table 17 Correlation between Self-Disclosure and Relationship Quality in FtF and CMC ________________________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 3 _______________________________________________________________________________________________ 1. FtF Self-Disclosure ---- 2. CMC Disclosure .578** .562** ---- .522** 3. Relationship Quality ---- _______________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 Table 18 Summary of Regression Analysis for Self-Disclosure in FtF and CMC Predicting Relationship Quality _______________________________________________________________________ Adjusted Variable R R Sq. R Sq. F Change df1 df2 p __________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Step 1 FtF Self-Disclosure .562 .315 .314 173.212 1 376 <.001 .612 .374 .371 35.113 1 375 <.001 Step 2 FtF Self-Disclosure CMC Disclosure ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________ 104 Table 19 Correlation between CMC Competence and CMC Disclosure ____________________________________________ Variables 1 2 _________________________________________________________________ n = 378 1. CMC Disclosure ---- .343** 2. CMC Competence ---- _________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 Table 20 Partial Correlation Coefficient for Self-Disclosure, Relationship Quality, and CMC Competence ____________________________________________________________ Zero Order Partials ____________________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 3 _________________________________________________________________________________________ 1. CMC Disclosure ---- 2. Relationship Quality .522** .343** ---- .295** 3. CMC Competence ---- __________________________________________________________________________________________ Controlling for CMCCOMP ____________________________________________________________ Variables 1 2 __________________________________________________________________________________________ 1. CMC Disclosure 2. Relationship Quality ---- .469** ---- __________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: ** p < .001 105 ABSTRACT 106 ABSTRACT Long-distance friendship (LDF) is still under-studied, despite the fact that this phenomenon has become prevalent in today’s society. This research study attempts to examine self-disclosure behaviors in LDF through both face-to-face (FtF) interactions and computer-mediated communication (CMC) channels and understand social impact of new information technologies on relational communication between long-distance friends. Specifically, relationships between self-disclosure and geographic distance, relationship stage, gender, cultural values, and friendship quality are tested in both FtF and CMC contexts. Research subjects were recruited from the students at SDSU and a web survey was used for data collection. Results of the study suggest that people tend to disclose slightly greater in FtF settings than in CMC environment and women are likely to reveal personal or private information to others slightly more than men do. Geographic distance does not influence selfdisclosure behaviors in either FtF or CMC context in LDFs. Once people are separate, it does not matter how far apart they are from each other. However, both FtF self-disclosure and CMC disclosure are important to friendship quality at all distances. If people engage in greater self-disclosure, they are likely to achieve higher relationship quality. An individual’s cultural background and values do not have much impact on self-disclosure behaviors among long-distance friends in either FtF or CMC context. A person’s CMC competence level is correlated with CMC disclosure, but it has not much influence on the relationship between CMC disclosure and relationship quality.