Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
AMER. ZOOL., 25:853-856 (1985) Paternal Care in Canids1 JAMES R. MALCOLM Department of Biology, University of Redlands, Redlands, California 92374 SYNOPSIS. Paternal care has never been reported as absent in any canid species, and some form of care has been seen in 18 of the 36 species in the family. Food provisioning, active defense of the young, and protecting young by remaining at the den as the female forages appear to be the commonest forms of male care. In addition males may groom, retrieve, play and rest with young. Male canids are rarely involved in den selection or construction. The effect on the fitness of the young of indirect forms of male care such as provisioning the female and territory defense are hard to assess. Quantitative studies of male provisioning in seven species offer few generalizations. In two species (Canis aureus, C mesomelas) females provided more food to the young than males; in one species (Alopex lagopus) the pair contributed equally to feeding young, and in four species (Canis lupus, Vulpes vulpes, Chrysocyon brachyurus, and Lycaon pictus), males provided more food than females. Much more data are required, particularly from field studies, before patterns of variation can be interpreted. Indirect care involves those behaviors Some form of male parental care, usually in the context of biparental investment in which could benefit offspring even if there monogamous pairs, is probably universal is no direct interaction between the male in the family Canidae. Kleiman and Mal- and young. Den construction, giving alarm colm (1981) reviewed literature up to 1979, calls, and caring for females fall into this and found reports of male care in 17 of 35 category. In addition, the establishment species. In no species was male care and maintenance of a territory may be a reported to be completely absent. Paternal prerequisite for breeding but also benefits care has subsequently been reported in two young. more fox species {Vulpes bengalensis (John- In most canids, pairs or groups defend singh, 1978], and V. macrotis [McGrew, multi-purpose territories (Kleiman and 1979] now recognized as distinct from V. Eisenberg, 1973) and all adults help directly velox, in which male care was reported by in raising the young. The most commonly Seton [1909]). Male care has, therefore, recorded forms of care are food provisionbeen seen in exactly half (18/36) of the ing (by regurgitation in larger species, and species in the family. carrying food items to the den in smaller Kleiman and Malcolm (1981) distin- species), active defense of the young (often guished between direct and indirect pat- reported in captive situations where males, terns of care-giving behavior. Direct care with young, attack "keepers"), and care to comprises those interactions between males the female, usually in the form of food proand their offspring which can reasonably visioning. However, in the species with the be interpreted to increase offspring fitness. most extensive direct field observations, the In canids, direct care can be classified into African wild dog {Lycaon pictus), all cateeight categories: resting and huddling with gories of male care were observed, with the young, grooming and cleaning, carrying, exception of huddling with neonates, and retrieving, providing food, defending den construction. Most categories of male against predators, babysitting (remaining care probably occur in other species but with young while others forage), and play- have not been reported. Den selection and construction may be an exception, as it ing or socializing. appears to be a female perogative in canids. (Data from Kleiman and Malcolm, 1981, 1 From the Symposium on Paternal Behavior pre- Table 2.) sented at the Annual Meeting of the American SociSome recent quantitative studies of ety of Zoologists, 27-30 December 1983, at Philaparental behavior in canids (Table 1) prodelphia, Pennsylvania. 853 854 JAMES R. MALCOLM TABLE 1. Quantitative studies of parental care in canids. Species Location Behaviors Canis lupus Field Atten. Wolf Prov. Canis lupus Capt. Canis aureus Atten.; Prov. Field Golden jackal Canis mesomelas Atten.; Prov. Field Silver-back jackal Vulpes vulpes Prov.; Def. Capt. Red fox Aloxpex lagopus Atten.; Prov. Field Arctic fox Chrysocycn brachyurus Prov. Capt. Maned wolf Lycaon pictus Field Atten.; Prov.; African wild dog Def.; Hunt Key: Capt., captivity; Prov. provisioning young; Atten., attendance at hunting. vide some details on the distribution of care giving behavior between the sexes. Five of seven species (wolf, both jackals, the African wild dog and the red fox) on which quantitative data have been reported, may live in groups which are larger than the nuclear family. In these species paternal care can be directed to offspring from two years, and care-giving behavior can also be provided to younger offspring by males other than the father. Some inter-species comparisons can be made with respect to attendance at the den and food provisioning. Female canids usually spend more time at the den than males. This seems to be universally true in the first two weeks after parturition when females seldom leave the den. When young are more than a month old, females in wolves, silver (black)-backed jackals and Arctic foxes are reported to spend more time than males in the vicinity of pups (although the total time either adult is present decreases with the pups' age). Similarly, in African wild dogs, dominant males (the probable fathers) are significantly less likely than mothers to remain at the den when the pack hunts, and mothers actively chased more predators from the vicinity of the den than did dominant males (34 vs. 24). Conversely, dominant males played a greater role in the pack's Aulhor(s) Harrington and Mech (1982) Fentress and Ryon (1982) Moehlman(1983) Moehlman(1983) Macdonald(1979) Garrott (1980) Rasmussen and Tilson (1984) Malcolm and Marten (1982) den; Def., defense of young; Hunt, hunting than did mothers. (Data from Malcolm and Marten, 1982.) In only two cases have males been recorded spending more time at the den than females. In silver (black)-backed jackals, the mother was with her pups almost constantly for the first two weeks. However, in the third week her attendance dropped sharply (to «35% of the time). Simultaneously, the male's attendance rose steadily from the second to the fourth week, and in the third week the males spent almost twice as much time at the den as females. It appeared as if the female, after two weeks confinement, foraged extensively in the third week, and the male compensated by babysitting. A preponderance of male attendance has been recorded in bat-eared foxes (Otocyon megalotis) (Malcolm, personal observation; Nel, 1978). In this nocturnal insectivorous species, the young are unusually dependent on milk as it is not economical to carry insects back to the den. Females foraged through most of the night and the male at two dens spent significantly more time at the den than did the female. Contributions of males versus females to provisioning young are variable. In one captive wolf pair (Fentress and Ryon, 1982; litter of 1973), two captive pairs of maned wolves (Rasmussen and Tilson, 1984; Anon., 1983), and a family of red foxes in 855 PATERNAL CARE IN CANIDS a pen (Macdonald, 1979) males provided twice as much food to the pups as the mother. In four packs of African wild dogs, dominant males provided slightly more food to pups than did mothers, but this difference was mainly a result of mothers staying with young pups. In Fentress and Ryon's (1982) captive wolf pack (with multiple litters and uncertain paternity), males, on a per capita basis, provided more food than females in four offiveyears. The sexes contributed almost exactly equally to young of the year, but the males (and not the females) also responded to begging by yearlings which increased their total contribution. In Arctic foxes there was no difference in the food brought to the four dens by males and females (Garrott, 1980). However, in both golden and silver (black)backed jackals females were recorded regurgitating more frequently than males (Moehlman, 1983). The data on male care allow few definite conclusions on the influence of male parental investment on canid social organization. However, the results seem broadly in agreement with Trivers' (1972) predictions that the sex providing the most parental investment becomes a limiting commodity for the other sex. Although both sexes play an important role in raising the young in canids, females play a special role in choosing a den, lactating and remaining with young pups. In quantitative field studies females also appear to provide as much or more food directly to the young as males. It appears that, as predicted, most deviations from monogamy, at least in smaller species, are in the direction of polygyny (e.g., Reynolds, 1977 for Otocyon, von Schantz, 1981 for Vulpes females. In African wild dogs (Frame et al, 1979), this seems to have led to a sexreversed species in which females compete for access to groups of investing males. REFERENCES vulpes). Anon. 1983. Maned wolf breeding. Wildlife 25:389. Fentress, J. C. and J. Ryon. 1982. A long term study of distributed pup feeding in captive wolves. In F. H. Harrington and P. C. Paquet (eds.), Wolves of the world, pp. 238-261. Noyes Publications, Park Ridge, New Jersey. Frame, L. H., J. R. Malcolm, G. W. Frame, and H. van Lawick. 1979. Social organization of African wild dogs on the Serengeti Plains, 1967-1978. Z. Tierpsychol. 50:225-249. Garrott, R. A. 1980. Den characteristics, productivity, food habits and behavior of arctic foxes in Northern Alaska. Master's Thesis, Pennsylvania State University. Harrington, F. H. and L. D. Mech. 1982. Patterns of homesite attendance in two Minnesota wolf packs. In F. H. Harrington and P. C. Paquet (eds.), Wolves of the world, pp. 81-105. Noyes Publications, Park Ridge, New Jersey. Johnsingh, A.J.T. 1978. Some aspects of the ecology and behavior of the Indian fox (Vulpes bengalensis) (Shaw). J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc. 75:397-405. Kleiman, D. G. andj. F. Eisenberg. 1973. Comparisons of canid and felid social systems from an evolutionary perspective. Anim. Behav. 21:637659. Kleiman, D. G. and J. R. Malcolm. 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. In D. J. Gubernick and P. H. Klopfer (eds.), Parental care in mammals, pp. 347-387. Plenum, New York. Macdonald, D. W. 1979. Helpers in fox society. Nature 282:69-71. Malcolm, J. R. and K. Marten. 1982. Natural selection and the communal rearing of pups in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10:1-13. McGrew,J. C. 1979. Vulpes macrotis. Mamm. Species 123:1-6. Mech, L. D. 1970. The wolf. Natural History Press, Garden City, New York. Moehlman, P. D. 1983. Socioecology of silverbacked and golden jackals (Cams mesomelas and Cams aureus). In J. F. Eisenberg and D. G. Kleiman The situation is more complex in the large pack-hunting species, notably wolves, African wild dogs and Asiatic wild dogs (Cuon alpinus). In both the first two species, the sex ratio in adults usually favors males (Frame et al., 1979 for African wild dogs; Mech, 1970 for wolves). In these cases although the per capita investment by males may be less than that of females, the total investment by males may exceed that of pp. 423-453. Special Publication No. 7, the American Society of Mammalogists. Nel, J. A. J. 1978. Notes on the food and foraging of the bat-eared fox Otocyon megalotis Bull. Cam. Mus. Nat. Hist. 6:132-137. Rasmussen.J. L. and R. L. Tilson. 1984. Food provisioning by adult maned wolves. Z. Tierpsychol. (In press) Reynolds, J. F. 1977. Bat-eared fox cubs being suckled by two females. Bull. East African Nat. Hist. Soc. 1977(2):58. Schantz, T. von. 1981. Female cooperation, male (eds.), Advances in the study of mammalian behavior, 856 JAMES R. MALCOLM competition, and dispersal in the red fox Vulpes Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (ed.), Sexual selection and vulpes. Oikos 37:63-68. the descent ofman, pp. 136-179. Aldine Press, ChiSeton, E. T. 1909. Life histories of northern animals: An account of the mammals of Manitoba. Ch. Scribcago. ner's Sons, New York.