* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Bystander Behavior Intervention in Risky Sexual Assault Situations
Age disparity in sexual relationships wikipedia , lookup
Penile plethysmograph wikipedia , lookup
Sexual violence wikipedia , lookup
Sexual abstinence wikipedia , lookup
Homosexualities: A Study of Diversity Among Men and Women wikipedia , lookup
Sexual stimulation wikipedia , lookup
Ego-dystonic sexual orientation wikipedia , lookup
Sexological testing wikipedia , lookup
Age of consent wikipedia , lookup
2012 Delhi gang rape wikipedia , lookup
Sexual racism wikipedia , lookup
Erotic plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Hookup culture wikipedia , lookup
Human sexual response cycle wikipedia , lookup
Incest taboo wikipedia , lookup
Sex in advertising wikipedia , lookup
Ages of consent in South America wikipedia , lookup
Sex and sexuality in speculative fiction wikipedia , lookup
Human mating strategies wikipedia , lookup
Corrective rape wikipedia , lookup
Lesbian sexual practices wikipedia , lookup
Human female sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Sexual assault wikipedia , lookup
Human male sexuality wikipedia , lookup
History of human sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Sexual ethics wikipedia , lookup
Female promiscuity wikipedia , lookup
Sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup
Bystander Behavior Intervention in Risky Sexual Assault Situations: An Examination of Social Norms and Situational Factors A thesis presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Master of Science Joel D. Wyatt December 2016 © 2016 Joel D. Wyatt. All Rights Reserved. 2 This thesis titled Bystander Behavior Intervention in Risky Sexual Assault Situations: An Examination of Social Norms and Situational Factors by JOEL D. WYATT has been approved for the Department of Psychology and the College of Arts and Sciences by Christine A. Gidycz Professor of Psychology Robert Frank Dean, College of Arts and Sciences 3 ABSTRACT Wyatt, Joel D., M.S., December 2016, Psychology Bystander Behavior Intervention in Risky Sexual Assault Situations: An Examination of Social Norms and Situational Factors Director of Thesis: Christine A. Gidycz Sexual assault is a well-documented problem on college campuses. One way to prevent sexual assault is for bystanders to intervene, and social norms theory posits that an individual’s decision to intervene in a risky situation is influenced by perceived peer norms and situational factors (e.g., alcohol consumption, riskiness for the victim, knowing the potential victim). Previous research has provided evidence that perceived peer norms are associated with whether or not bystanders decide to intervene in risky situations, but these studies have been limited by typically using measures to assess bystander behavior that do not account for whether an individual has an opportunity to intervene. Further, many of the situational variables that have been examined have been limited in that they have been dichotomized when the construct that is being measured is inherently continuous (e.g., relationship with a potential victim). The purpose of this study is to predict bystander intervention using a set of social norm variables (perception of peers’ rape myth acceptance, prosocial tendencies and hypergender ideology) and individual-level variables in a sample of college men and women, as well as examine situational-specific variables at the time of an event. Perceptions of peer norms were unrelated to bystander behavior; however, perceived impact of alcohol use, perceived risk for a bystander, perceived closeness to a potential victim and previous intervention were related to bystander behavior. Implications of the findings are discussed. 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express appreciation and gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Christine Gidycz, for her support and guidance through this process. I would also like to thank my other committee members, Dr. Ryan Shorey and Dr. Brian Wymbs for their consideration and insight. 5 TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Abstract……………………………………………………………………………………3 Acknowledgements………………………………………………………………………..4 List of Tables…………………………………………………...……….………………...7 List of Figures……………………………………………………………………………..8 Chapter 1: Introduction……………………………………………………………………9 Chapter 2: Methods………………………………………………………………………20 Participants…………………………………………………………………….…20 Procedure……...……………………………………………………………........20 Measures……………………………………………………………………........21 Chapter 3: Results………………………………………………..………………………31 Descriptive Statistics……………………………………………………………..31 Specific Hypothesis and Corresponding Analyses………………………………32 Exploratory Analyses…………………………………………………………….44 Chapter 4: Discussion……………………………………………………………...…….47 References………………………………………………………………………………..57 Appendix A: Demographics Questionnaire ……….……………………………….……64 Appendix B: Sexual Experiences Survey – Victimization…………………….………...67 Appendix C: Sexual Experiences Survey – Perpetration……………………….…….….70 Appendix D: Revised Conflict Tactics Scale …………...……………………………….74 Appendix E: Rape Myth Acceptance Scale – Self………………………………………75 Appendix F: Rape Myth Acceptance Scale – Peers……………………………………..77 Appendix G: Hypergender Ideology Scale – Self……………………………………….79 6 Appendix H: Hypergender Ideology Scale – Peers……………….………………….....81 Appendix I: Prosocial Tendencies Measure – Self…………...…………………………83 Appendix J: Prosocial Tendencies Measure – Self……………..……………………….85 Appendix K: Bystander Opportunity Scale……………………………………………..87 Appendix L: Bystander Behavior Scale………….………………………………………91 Appendix M: Situational Follow-up Questionnaire…………………………………...…95 7 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Demographic Characteristics of the Sample…………………………………………..21 2. Frequency of Endorsement of Each Risky Situation in the Situational Analyses…….29 3. Descriptive Statistics for Social Norms Analyses…………………………………….31 4. Correlations Matrix of Social Norms Analyses Variables……………………………32 5. Descriptive Statistics for Continuous Variables in the Situational Analyses…………33 6. Summary of Support for Each Hypothesis……………………………………………34 7. Summary of Hierarchical Linear Regression Predicting Bystander Behavior Frequency from Gender, Individual Level, and Social Norms Level Factors……………………….36 8. Exploratory Logistic Regression Coefficients…………………………….………......45 8 LIST OF FIGURES Figures Page 1. Model containing influences of bystander behavior, including individual, social norms level and situational factors…………………………...…………………………………10 2. Predicted bystander frequencies by individual rape myth acceptance for men and women at 1 SD below the mean and 1 SD above the mean……………………………..37 3. Predicted bystander frequencies by individual hypergender ideology for men and women at 1 SD below the mean and 1 SD above the mean……………………………..38 9 CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION Sexual assault, defined as any unwanted sexual act (e.g., touching someone in a sexual way without consent, coercing someone either verbally or by force or threats of force to engage in sexual acts they do not want to engage in), is a well-documented problem on college campuses (Fisher, Daigle, Cullen, & Turner, 2003; Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski, 1987). Research has shown that approximately 25% of college women and 6% of college men will be victims of some form of sexual assault during their college career (Gross, Winslett, Roberts, & Gohm, 2006; Krebs, Lindquist, Warner, Fisher, & Martin, 2007; Koss et al., 1987). Further, the consequences of sexual assault can be devastating for victims, affecting both mental and physical health (e.g., Campbell, Dworkin, & Cabral, 2009; Tansill, Edwards, Kerns, Gidycz, & Calhoun, 2012). As a result of these high rates of sexual assault victimization and negative physical and mental health consequences associated with victimization, colleges and universities have implemented programming to prevent sexual assault. Although there are many approaches to reducing and preventing sexual assault, one way is for bystanders to intervene (Banyard, 2011). Bystanders are individuals who are not directly involved in a situation, but have the ability to impact it. The White House Task Force to Protect Students from Sexual Assault has identified bystander intervention as a “promising practice” for sexual assault prevention (2014). Although development of current bystander programs is ongoing and informed by research, much is still unknown regarding predictors of bystander behavior in risky sexual assault situations and there is only limited evidence to date that programs that focus on bystander intervention are 10 effective (Banyard, Moynihan, & Plante, 2007; Gidycz, Orchowski, & Berkowitz, 2011; McMahon, 2015). Thus, the purpose of this thesis is to examine predictors of bystander behavior in situations that are potentially risky for sexual assault in order to inform bystander intervention programs. The current investigator will examine variables related to broad social norms, as well as situational factors that influence bystander intervention (see Figure 1). Figure 1. Model containing influences of bystander behavior, including individual, social norms level and situational level factors. 11 One theoretical approach used to create bystander intervention education programs is social norms theory (Berkowitz, 2002; 2010). According to Berkowitz (2002), social norms theory posits that bystander intentions and behaviors are dependent upon perceived peer bystander attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Berkowitz posits that actions are often based on misinformation about or misperceptions of others’ attitudes and/or beliefs. Put simply, people behave because they think that others believe that they should behave that way, and what they think others believe is often incorrect. For example, Dardis, Murphy, Bill, & Gidycz (2015) surveyed dyads of men and found that perpetrators of sexual assault often overestimated a close friend’s sexual assault perpetration and rape supportive beliefs and attitudes. Research in the bystander literature also has shown men are less likely to report intent to intervene if they perceive that their peers are supportive of sexually aggressive acts (Brown & Messman-Moore, 2010), or if they perceive peers to have low intentions of intervening (Brown, Banyard, & Moynihan, 2014), regardless of the beliefs or attitudes about sexual aggression and intervening that they may have. Thus, these misperceptions of social norms may impact whether or not an individual intervenes. Furthermore, Berkowitz (2010) states that these misperceptions are passively accepted, and individuals do not actively intervene to change then. Thus, if no one intervenes or speaks against those who actively voice these misperceptions, the misperception and the related impact will be reinforced; thus creating a self-perpetuating cycle. Fabiano, Perkins, Berkowitz, Linkenbach, and Stark (2003) found that those who believe that sexual aggression is a normal part of society are less likely to intervene, 12 especially when these beliefs are generated from interactions with peers. Fortunately, Berkowitz (2010) argues that when individuals are given the correct information, it may break this self-perpetuating cycle and encourage change. Gidycz et al. (2011) found that participants in their bystander education program, which utilized a social norms exercise addressing misperceptions, found sexual assault behavior less reinforcing, associated with sexually aggressive peers less, believed friends would be more likely to intervene, and engaged in less sexually aggressive behavior. However, many variables found to predict bystander intentions and behaviors have not been examined from a social norms theory approach (i.e., taking into account individual’s perceptions of peers’ beliefs), such as rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies. Given the limited empirical support behind social norms theory and how it relates to bystander behavior, as well as the fact that it is the foundation for bystander interventions, it is important to examine predictors and correlates of bystander behavior from a social norms theory approach (McMahon, 2015). Individual’s endorsements of rape myth acceptance (i.e., attitudes or beliefs that are held by most, despite being generally false, which help justify sexual aggression towards women; Lonsway & Fitzgerald, 1994) and hypergender ideology (i.e., the extent to which one is socialized adherent to traditional gender norms, such as masculinity and femininity) have been found to be positively related to sexual assault perpetration (Murnen et al., 2002; Swartout, 2013) and negatively related to bystander intentions and behavior (Austin, Dardis, Wilson, Gidycz, & Berkowitz, 2015; Banyard & Moynihan, 2011; Brown & Messman-Moore, 2010; Carlson, 2008; McMahon, 2010). However, 13 research is yet to examine how perceptions of peers’ endorsement of these variables impact bystander behavior. Dardis et al., (2015) found that men who commit sexual assault believe that their peers endorse higher levels of rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology, suggesting that perceptions of these peer beliefs may impact behavior related to risky sexual assault situations. Theoretically, if one believes that their peers endorse high levels of rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology, they may be less likely to intervene in risky sexual assault situations, due to fear of negative evaluation from peers, such as looking “weak” in front of peers (Carlson, 2008). The majority of research assessing bystander behaviors have examined barriers or beliefs that are negatively associated with bystander intervention (e.g., Burn, 2009). However, variables associated with helping are equally as important, given they may have an impact on whether or not an individual intervenes. One potential facilitator of bystander intervention is an individual’s general tendency to engage in prosocial behaviors. Bennett, Banyard, and Garnhart (2014) found prosocial tendencies were significantly related to helping strangers, suggesting that those who are willing to help more generally (i.e., have prosocial tendencies) may be more likely to intervene. However, perceptions of peers’ prosocial tendencies have yet to be examined in the bystander literature. If people perceive peers to be prosocial, it may make them want to fit in as a failure to intervene may lead to negative evaluation from peers. In addition to factors related to social norms, history of sexual assault is another important factor when considering bystander behavior among college students, as history of sexual assault victimization is thought to be positively related to bystander behavior 14 (Banyard & Moynihan, 2011; Banyard, Plante, & Moynihan, 2004) and history of sexual assault perpetration is thought to be negatively related to bystander behavior. However, few studies have actually empirically examined the relationship between sexual assault victimization or perpetration and bystander behavior. Murphy (2012) found that female victims of sexual assault reported engaging in a higher percentage of bystander behaviors over an eight-week period than non-victims but failed to find any difference among perpetrators, which may have been due to the small percentage (i.e., 13%) of participants who endorsed perpetration (Murphy, 2012). However, there are several limitations that are important to note related to studies that have examined social norms and previous sexual assault history, and their relationship to bystander behavior. A major limitation in the current literature is the lack of research examining both men and women in studies measuring bystander behavior. Berkowitz (2002) argues that it is the responsibility of men to prevent sexual violence of other men, which may be a key factor contributing to the exclusion of women in studies examining bystander intervention of sexual violence. However, college women are commonly in situations where they can also be active bystanders and intervene in risky sexual assault situations. More importantly, research has shown that women report that they are more likely to intervene than men (Banyard, 2008). Yet, it is also the case that women do not intervene 100% of the time, thus examining factors that encourage or discourage women from intervening is also important. Furthermore, men and women often endorse differing levels of beliefs about sexual assault (e.g., men endorse higher 15 levels of rape myth acceptance than women; McMahon & Farmer, 2011), and it may be fruitful to examine how these differences impact bystander behavior. In addition to the lack of women in studies of bystander behavior, there have been difficulties in measuring bystander behavior. Until recently studies examining bystander behavior have not taken into account opportunity to intervene (e.g., Banyard, Palmer, Murphy, & Gidycz, in press; Murphy, 2012). In previous measures of bystander behavior (e.g., Banyard, 2008), questions were asked about specific behaviors (e.g. “I interfered with another guy’s “action” because I thought it might stop them from possibly committing a sexual assault”), but not about opportunities, in “yes/no” format. Participants, then, may select no for one of two reasons: either they were in the situation where they could have intervened and choose not to intervene, or were not in the situation at all. If participants are selecting “no” because they just did not have the opportunity and they are still included in analyses, this may artificially skewed the results in a way that does not accurately reflect how much people are actually intervening such that it lowers estimates of bystanders who intervene. Thus, the opportunity to intervene must be taken into account in order to accurately reflect the extent of intervention behavior. Murphy (2012) recently developed a new measure that sufficiently takes into account opportunities for bystander behavior in light of the current gaps in the field’s ability to measure bystander behavior. Furthermore, there are several situational-specific variables that may impact intervention. One challenge that has faced researchers studying bystander behavior is that each risky sexual assault situation is often unique in nature, making it difficult to fully 16 capture the barriers (i.e. what makes it more difficult to intervene) and facilitators (i.e., what makes it easier to intervene) in each unique situation. Previous research has shown that individuals are more likely to intervene if there is a close relationship with the victim, but not the perpetrator (Bennett & Banyard, 2014), if there is not a relationship between the potential victim and perpetrator (Murphy, 2012), if there are fewer people present at the time of the event (Katz, 2015; Katz et al., 2015), and if the situation is high risk for the victim (McMahon, Banyard, & McMahon, 2015) or low-risk for the potential bystander (Bennett et al., 2014). Moreover, alcohol consumption at the time of the event has been examined (e.g., number of drinks consumed prior to the event), but not found to be a significant predictor in the presence of other variables (Austin et al., 2015). However, limited evidence suggests that alcohol use may increase helping behavior (Steele, Critchlow, & Liu, 1985), as the disinhibiting effects of alcohol consumption may help those overcome the consequences associated with helping (e.g., audience inhibition). Furthermore, it may be the subjective experience of the effects of alcohol use (e.g., perceived intoxication) that impact whether or not an individual intervenes, rather than the actual number of drinks. However, many of the findings of studies that examine situational factors are difficult to generalize, as the methodology used to draw conclusions has relied primarily on having participants imagine themselves in hypothetical situations, rather than actual retrospective experiences of potentially risky sexual assault situations, or has failed to take into account important factors that may influence whether or not a potential bystander intervenes when faced with a risky sexual assault situation. Gender differences 17 in these situational factors may be important, as previous research has shown different barriers to bystander intervention for women and men, such that women report lacking the skills to intervene and men do not (Burn, 2009). Moreover, many of the variables assessed (e.g., risk, perceived relationships) have been dichotomized, rather than examined continuously, which limits conclusions drawn from the results (e.g., Bennet & Banyard, 2014; Katz, 2015; McMahon et al., 2015; Murphy, 2012). Furthermore, the majority of variables have only been examined in linear relationships with helping, which may not be appropriate. In particular, with alcohol use, it may be that a curvilinear relationship between alcohol use and bystander behavior intervention exists (Steele et al., 1985). The purpose of this study is to improve upon the previous literature by investigating predictors of bystander behavior from a social norms approach utilizing a measure of bystander behavior that takes into consideration the opportunity to intervene. Specifically, I will explore how peer perceived rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies correlate with bystander behavior. I also hope to further assess how previous victimization and perpetration history relate to bystander behavior, as previous research has insinuated such a relationship exists, but few studies have examined this assumption empirically (Murphy, 2012). Furthermore, the current study will explore the relationship between group size and bystander behavior by considering the amount of “close” friends who were nearby, rather than just the total number of people at the time of an event, such as in previous research (Austin et al., 2015). The relationship between bystander behavior and a 18 bystander’s relationship to a victim and their relationship to a potential perpetrator by assessing perceived closeness of the relationship will also be explored, as previous research has just focused on whether the potential bystander knew the victim or not (Banyard, 2008; Bennett & Banyard, 2014). I will also assess bystander behavior when the potential bystander perceives a relationship between a potential victim and a potential perpetrator by assessing how close they perceive that relationship to be. Moreover, I hope to further expand our understanding of the relationship between perceived risk and bystander behavior by assessing perceived risk at the time of the event for both the potential victim and the potential bystander using a continuous measure or risk, rather than dichotomizing situations as “high risk” or “low risk” as has been done in previous research (Katz, 2015; Katz et al., 2015; McMahon et al., 2015). Furthermore, the relationship between alcohol consumption and bystander behavior will be examined by assessing perceived impairment and intoxication at the time of an event, rather than just the total number of drinks (Austin et al., 2015; Murphy, 2012). In light of previous research, the following are the hypotheses for the whole sample. I predict that (1) perceptions of peers’ rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology and prosocial behavior tendencies variables will significantly predict bystander behavior percentage, controlling for individual levels of these variables. This is, perceptions of peers’ rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology will predict a decrease in bystander behavior percentage, and perceptions of peers’ prosocial tendencies will positively predict bystander behavior percentage. Furthermore, I predict that (2) victims of sexual assault will perceive more opportunities to intervene and intervene 19 more than non-victims, and that (3) perpetrators of sexual assault will perceive less opportunities to intervene and intervene less than non-perpetrators. The following hypotheses refer to the most recent situation participants have observed (for possible situations, see Appendix M), where each participant indicated that they had the opportunity to intervene. I predict that individuals who intervene will (4) report having a lower percentage of close friends with them at the time of an incident, (5) perceive having a stronger relationship with the potential victim, (6) perceive having a stronger relationship with the potential perpetrator, (7) perceive a weaker relationship between the potential perpetrator and potential victim, (8) perceive a situation as more risky for a victim, and (9) perceive the situation as less risky for themselves than those who do not intervene. I also predict that (10) individuals will be more likely to intervene if someone else has already intervened and that (11) whether or not an individual intervenes will have a curvilinear relationship with perceived impairment due to alcohol at the time of the event, such that at low (e.g., sober) and high levels (e.g., drunk or very drunk) of perceived intoxication bystanders will be less likely to intervene, and at medium levels (e.g., buzzed; tipsy) of intoxications bystander will more likely to intervene. Furthermore, the researcher will explore gender as a moderator of the relationship between the individual level, social norm level, and situational variables and bystander behavior, as well as examine which situational factors that are bivariately associated with bystander behavior are predictive of bystander behavior in the presence of all other factors. 20 CHAPTER 2: METHODS Procedure All procedures were approved by the university institutional review board (IRB). Participants were recruited through the university psychology subject pool. Each participant completed an online consent form prior to beginning the survey. After completing several questionnaires, participants were debriefed and provided with mental health resources on campus. Each participant was granted course credit for their participation. Participants Data were collected from 347 undergraduate students from a psychology subject pool at a medium-sized Midwestern university. Seventeen participants were omitted from the analyses because they either did not complete the necessary measures (N = 16) or they did not endorse any of the 35 opportunities for intervention (N = 1). Thus 330 participants were included in the analyses. The mean age of the sample was 19.23 years old and the majority of participants were 1st year college students (65.2%; N = 215). Consistent with the university demographics, 85.2% identified as white (N =281), 93.3% identified as heterosexual (N = 308), and 76.7% identified as female (N = 253). Furthermore, 23% (N = 76) of participants reported being involved in fraternity and sorority life, and 41.2% (N = 136) of participants reported previous participation in a sexual assault prevention or risk reduction program. See Table 1 for demographic factors. 21 Measures Demographics Participants completed a demographics questionnaire (Appendix A) to assess variables such as age, sex, race/ethnicity, year in college, and history of participation of sexual assault risk reduction/prevention programing. Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of the Sample (N = 330) Demographic Factor Gender Male Female Race Caucasian, Non-Hispanic African American Latino or Hispanic Asian or Pacific Islander Two or more races Other Sexual Orientation Heterosexual Bisexual Homosexual Asexual Year in College 1st Year 2nd Year 3rd Year 4th Year 5th Year + Fraternity/Sorority Life Yes No Previous Program Participation Yes No Frequency (N) Percentage 77 253 23.3 76.7 281 13 8 15 9 4 85.2 3.9 2.4 4.5 2.7 1.2 308 15 4 3 93.3 4.6 1.2 .9 215 71 27 13 4 65.2 21.5 8.2 3.9 1.4 76 254 23.0 77.0 136 194 41.2 58.8 22 Sexual Assault Victimization The Sexual Experiences Survey – Short Form Victimization (SES-SFV; Koss et al., 2007) was used to assess history of participants’ sexual assault victimization experience in adolescence and early adulthood since age 14 (see Appendix B). The SES was revised from the original Sexual Experience Survey (Koss & Oros, 1982), and consists of 7-behaviorally specific experiences related to sexual assault victimization (e.g., A man put his penis into my vagina, or someone inserted fingers or objects without my consent). Each experience has five possible tactics that could have been used to coerce the victim (e.g., Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon), which creates 35 combinations of behaviors and tactics. Participants were asked to indicate whether or not each of these experiences has happened to them since age 14. A range of sexually aggressive experiences (i.e., unwanted sexual contact, sexual coercion, attempted rape, and rape) were assessed using this measure. Internal consistency in the current study was good (Cronbach’s alpha = .95). Furthermore, several researchers have found support for the measure’s convergent and discriminant validity (Cecil & Matson, 2006; Johnson et al., in press). The final variable was dichotomized because we were interested comparing any victimization to non-victimization. Participants who endorsed any of the items of the SES-SFV received a score of 1, and those who did not endorse any items on the SES-SFV received a score of 0. 23 Sexual Assault Perpetration The Sexual Experience Survey – Short Form Perpetration (SES-SFP; Koss et al., 2007), a revised version of the Sexual Experiences Survey (Koss & Oros, 1992), was used to assess men’s history of sexual assault perpetration since age 14 (see Appendix C). A range of sexually aggressive experiences (i.e., unwanted sexual contact, sexual coercion, attempted rape, and rape) were assessed using this measure. The survey consists of 7 behaviorally-specific experiences each with five possible tactics for a combination of 35 combinations of behaviors and tactics. Participants were asked to indicate whether they have engaged in these behaviors since age 14 (yes/no). In the current study, the SES-SFP had strong internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .98). Furthermore, evidence supports convergent and predictive validity of the SES-SFP (Johnson et al., in press). Scores were dichotomized to compare perpetrators to nonperpetrators. Participants who endorsed any of the items of the SES-SFP received a score of 1, and those who did not endorse any items on the SES-SFP will received a score of 0. A modified version of 7-items of the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales - Sexual Violence Subscale (CTS2; Straus, Hamby, McCoy & Sugarman, 1996) pertaining to sexual violence was used as an additional measure of sexual perpetration (Appendix D). Participants reported the number of times they have engaged in sexual violence perpetration (e.g., I used threats to force someone to have sex) from 0 times to 20 or more times since age 14. Internal consistency for the current study was good (Cronbach’s alpha = .94). Furthermore, Straus et al. (1996) have found evidence for the convergent validity of the measure. Participants who endorsed any item on the CTS2 received a score of 1, 24 and those who did not endorse any item received a score of 0. The CTS2 was combined with the SES-SFP to form a total sexual assault perpetration history variable for analyses. Rape Myth Acceptance The Updated Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale (IRMA; Payne, Lonsway & Fitzgerald, 1999; McMahon & Farmer, 2011) was used to assess participants’ individual rape myth acceptance as well as participants’ perceptions of peers’ rape myth acceptance (Appendix E & F). The scale was originally created by Payne et al. (1999) and McMahon and Farmer (2011) updated the scale recently to measure modern vernacular among young people (e.g. when girls go to parties wearing slutty clothes, they are asking for trouble). Participants answered the 22-item questionnaire two times; once for how much they endorsed the rape myths and once for how they believed their peers endorsed the rape myths. Participants rated each item on a 5-point rating scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). In the current study, internal consistencies for self (Cronbach’s alpha = .93) and peer (Cronbach’s alpha = .96) versions of the questionnaire were high. Furthermore, McMahon and Farmer (2011) found the updated measure to have sufficient criterion and construct validity. Scores were summed, with higher scores indicating higher levels of rape myth acceptance Hypergender Ideology The Hypergender Ideology Scale – Short Form (HGIS-SF; Hamburger, Hogben, McGown, & Dawson, 1996) was used to assess participants’ endorsement of stereotypical gender roles and participants’ perception of peers’ endorsement of stereotypical gender roles (Appendix G & H). Participants were asked to answer the 18- 25 item questionnaire two times; once in respect to their own beliefs and once in respect to how much they believe their peers endorse these beliefs. Each item was rated on a 6-point scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The HGIS is a unidimensional scale for both men and women, with questions asking about stereotypes each gender would typically endorse (e.g., A true man knows how to command others). Hamburger et al. (1996) found that the HGIS is highly correlated with scales that measure hypermasculinity (r = .61) and hyperfemininity (r = .60), implying support for convergent validity of the measure. In the current study, internal consistencies were strong for the self (Cronbach’s alpha = .92) and peer (Cronbach’s alpha = .94) versions of the scale. Scores were summed, with higher scores indicating higher levels of hypergender ideology. Prosocial Tendencies The Prosocial Tendencies Measure – Revised (PTM-R; Carlo, Hausmann, Christiansen, & Randall, 2003) was used to assess participants’ endorsement of engagement in prosocial behaviors and participants’ perception of peers’ involvement in prosocial behaviors (Appendix I & J). Participants were asked to answer the 25-item questionnaire two times; once in respect to their own behaviors and once in respect to how much they believe their peers engage in these behaviors. Each item was rated on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree) scale with a sample item being “I often help even if I don’t think I will get anything out of helping.” In the current study, internal consistencies were acceptable for the self (Cronbach’s alpha = .75) and peer (Cronbach’s alpha = .76) versions of the scale. Furthermore, in a previous study, the PTM was 26 correlated with the global prosocial behavior scale (Rushton, Chrisjohn, & Fekken, 1981) providing evidence for convergent validity of the measure. Scores were summed, with higher scores indicating higher levels of prosocial tendencies. Bystander Opportunities The Bystander Opportunity Scale (BOS; Murphy, 2012) was used to assess whether or not participants witnessed situations in which bystander behavior was warranted (i.e., situations in which sexual assault risk markers are present) in 35 situations (e.g., I heard someone plan to give someone alcohol to get sex) over the last four months (Appendix K). Four months was chosen because it was believed to be an adequate amount of time for college students to have witnessed a situation. Recognizing that many measures of bystander behavior lacked opportunity, Murphy (2012) created the BOS to address this gap in the literature. Each item on the BOS corresponds with an item on the Bystander Behavior Scale – Revised (BBS-R), which was adapted from items from Burn’s (2009) Bystander Intervention Behavior Scale and Banyard et al.’s (2005) Bystander Behavior Scale. Participants were asked to indicate how many times they witnessed each item/incident. Items were summed in order to create a total score with higher scores indicating a greater number of sexual assault risk markers witnessed over the last four months. Internal consistency of the BOS was adequate in the current study (Cronbach’s alpha = .72), consistent with Murphy (2012) (Cronbach’s alpha = .83). As recommended by Murphy (2012), the BOS was used in conjunction with the (BBS-R) to compute the dependent variable, percentage of bystander behavior. 27 Bystander Behavior The Bystander Behavior Scale – Revised (BBS-R; Murphy, 2012) was used to measure 35 behaviors (one for each situation in the BOS) individuals could have used to respond to emergency situations endorsed on the BOS (e.g., I confronted someone who planned to give someone alcohol to get sex) over the last four months (Appendix L). Items were stated in the past tense and students were instructed to read each item and indicate how many times they had engaged in that behavior over the last four months. The items were summed to create a total score, with higher scores indicating higher number of bystander intervention behaviors. The internal consistency of the items in the Bystander Behavior Scale – Revised has been found to be good in previous research (Cronbach’s alpha = .87) and in the present study (Cronbach’s alpha = .86). Situational Variables The Specific Sexual Assault Situations with Situational Follow-Up Items Questionnaire (SFQ) was created for the purposes of this study and adapted from Murphy’s (2012) measure (Appendix M). Participants were asked if they had visually witnessed at least one of six potentially risky sexual assault situations over the last four months. The situations were selected from the BOS (Murphy, 2012; e.g., I saw a man talking to a woman at a bar. He was sitting very close to her and by the look on her face I could see she was uncomfortable; I saw someone’s drink get spiked with a drug; I saw someone trying to take advantage of someone’s intoxicated state to have sex with them). Only participants who reported observing one of the six potential risky sexual assault situations were then asked to respond to follow-up questions about the situation they 28 most recently observed. If participants witnessed more than one situation over the last four months or the same situation multiple times over the last four months, they were asked to answer the follow-up questions regarding the situation in which they most recently observed. The six situations were grouped in the analyses. See table 2 for the percentage of people who endorsed each situation. The follow-up items assessed the following information: (1) whether or not an individual intervened (yes, no), (2) participants’ alcohol consumption and subjective intoxication at the time of the event, (3) group size, including number of close friends present, (4) the relationship between the participant and the potential victim, including their perception of the closeness of that relationship, (5) the relationship between the participant and the potential perpetrator, including their perception of the closeness of that relationship, (6) the relationship between the potential victim and the potential perpetrator, including their perception of the closeness of that relationship, (7) perceived risk for the potential victim at the time of the event, (8) perceived physical, social, legal and emotional risk associated with intervention for the participant at the time of the event, and (9) whether or not someone else had already intervened. 29 Table 2 Frequency of Endorsement of Each Risky Situation in the Situational Analyses (N = 153) Situation 1. I saw a man talking to a women at a bar. He was sitting very close to her and by the look on her face, I could see she was uncomfortable. 1. I saw someone’s drink get spiked with a drug. 2. I saw someone who looked drunk go to a room with someone else at a party 3. I saw someone trying to take advantage of someone’s intoxicated state to have sex with them. 4. I saw/heard about someone who was hooking up with someone who was passed out. 5. I saw someone doing things that might meet the definition of sexual assault. Frequency (N) 84 Percentage 3 40 2.0% 26.1% 14 9.2% 5 3.3% 7 4.6% 54.9% Specific to alcohol consumption, participants were asked how many drinks they had consumed up until the event over how much time. Then, participants were asked on a scale from 1 (sober) to 5 (very drunk) how intoxicated they were at the time of the event. Participants were also asked on a scale from 1 (not impaired) to 7 (very impaired) how impaired from alcohol use they were at the time of the event. Specific to group size, participants were asked to estimate how many people were present at the time of the event, and asked to identify how many of those people they would consider their “close friend.” The number of close friends were divided by the number of people present to create a percentage of close friends present during the time of the event. Specific to the perceived relationship between the participant and the victim or perpetrator, or the perceived relationship between the victim and perpetrator, participants were asked to rate how close the perceived relationship was on a scale of 1 (not close at all) to 7 (very 30 close). Specific to perceived risk for the potential victim, individuals were asked how risky the situation was on a scale of 1 (not at all risky) to 7 (very risky). Specific to perceived risk for the participant (bystander), participants were asked four questions regarding perceived social, physical, emotional and legal risk to intervention on a scale from 1 (not at all risky) to 7 (very risky). Scores were summed, with higher scores indicative of greater perceived risk of intervention for the bystander. The perceived risk for the bystander scale was found to be reliable in the present study (Cronbach’s alpha = .87) 31 CHAPTER 3: RESULTS Descriptive Statistics There was a total of 323 individuals who completed all of the measures to be included in the social norm analyses. On average, participants reported having observed 54.63 potential bystander situations over a four-month period and reported intervening an average of 25.67 times. Descriptive statistics and t-tests for gender differences for the social norms and individual level variables can be found in Table 3. Women intervened more frequently than men, as well as endorsed higher levels of individual and perceived peer prosocial tendencies. Men endorsed higher levels of individual rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology, as well as higher levels of perceived peer hypergender ideology than women. A correlation matrix of the social norm and individual level variables can be found in Table 4. Table 3 Descriptive Statistics for Social Norms Analyses (N = 323) Total Men Women Variable Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean(SD) t (321) Bystander Frequency .47 (.32) .37 (.30) .50 (.32) -3.12** RMA – Self 43.01 (14.30) 52.86 (13.60) 40.14 (13.12) 7.20** RMA – Peers 54.73 (18.61) 56.89 (16.19) 54.08 (19.28) 1.30 HGI – Self 34.89 (14.51) 46.43 (15.88) 31.56 (12.18) 8.49** HGI – Peers 50.06 (18.94) 54.12 (19.58) 48.84 (18.71) 2.09* PT – Self 74.70 (10.25) 71.98 (9.54) 75.72 (10.14) -2.81** PT– Peers 70.91 (10.02) 67.76 (9.25) 72.08 (9.82) -3.33* Note. *p < .05, **p < .01; RMA = Rape Myth Acceptance Scale; HGIS = Hypergender Ideology; PT = Prosocial Tendencies 32 Table 4 Correlations Matrix of Social Norms Analyses Variables (N = 323) Measure 1 2 3 4 5 1. Bystander -.28* -.16 -.33** -.11 Frequency 2. RMA – Self .06 .51** .72** .21 3. RMA – Peers -.03 .39** .32** .67** 4. HGI – Self .08 .67* .15* .46** 5. HGI – Peers -.00 .16* .60** .28** 6. PTM - Self .15* -.17* -.10 -.25** -.06 7. PTM – Peers .13* -.04 -.18** -.09 -.18** Note. *p < .05, **p < .01; Males are above and Females are below 6 .26* 7 .07 -.09 .10 -.35** -.06 .37* .09 -.06 -.09 -.05 .43* - Of those who completed the sexual assault victimization (N = 324) and perpetration (N = 327) measures, 27.8% reported some form of sexual victimization and 26.6% reported some form of sexual assault perpetration. Specifically for sexual assault perpetration, 3.1% of participants endorsed sexual assault perpetration on the SES-SFV only, 17.1% of participants endorsed sexual assault perpetration on the CTS-2 only, and 6.4% of participants endorsed sexual assault perpetration on both. Women (31.1%) were more likely than men (15.8%) to report sexual assault victimization, χ2(1, N = 323) = 6.39, p < .05, OR = 2.34, and men (41.6%) were more likely than women (21.7%) to report sexual assault perpetration, χ2(1, N = 327) = 12.86, p < .05, OR = 2.34. There were a total of 153 individuals (22.9% male, 77.1% female) who endorsed observing one of the six situations required to be included in the situational analyses. Bystander intervention was coded dichotomously (yes, no), and 37.3% of the sample intervened in their most recent situation. In 17% of the situations, someone else had already intervened, and 89% of individuals reported that they had consumed alcohol at 33 the time of the event. Descriptive statistics of the continuous variables included in the situational analyses can be found in Table 5. For perceived risk for oneself, men (M = 8.89, SD = 4.89) perceived higher risk than women (M = 6.76, SD = 4.20), t(150) = 2.29, p < .05. There were no other gender differences for any of the other situational variables. Table 5 Descriptive Statistics for Continuous Variables in the Situational Analyses Total Men Variable Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Percentage of Close Friends .25 (.13) .23 (.30) Present Perception of Alcohol Impact 5.16 (2.66) 5.33 (2.84) Perceived Closeness with Victim 2.03 (1.68) 2.14 (1.59) Perceived Closeness with 1.51 (1.19) 1.65 (1.23) Perpetrator Perceived Closeness between 2.24 (1.49) 2.11 (1.49) Victim and Perpetrator Perceived Risk for the Victim 2.16 (1.33) 2.31 (1.60) Perceived Risk for Bystander 7.20 (4.43) 8.67 (4.89) Note. *p < .05, **p < .01; N’s ranged from 140-153 Women Mean (SD) .26 (.33) T -.57 5.11 (2.63) 1.99 (1.70) 1.47 (1.17) .42 .50 .84 2.27 (1.48) -.54 2.12 (1.24) 6.76 (4.20) .76 2.30* Specific Hypotheses and Corresponding Analyses A summary of the findings can be found in Table 6. 34 Table 6 Summary of Support for Each Hypothesis Hypothesis Support 1. Perceptions of peers’ rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology and No prosocial behavior tendencies variables will significantly predict bystander behavior percentage, controlling for individual levels of these variables and gender 2. Victims will perceive more opportunities to intervene than non-victims Yes Victims will intervene more than non-victims Yes 3. Perpetrators will perceive less opportunities than non-perpetrators No Perpetrators will intervene less than non-perpetrators No 4. Individuals who intervene will report having a lower percentage of close No friends present 5. Individuals who intervene will perceive a stronger relationship between Yes themselves and the potential victim 6. Individuals who intervene will perceive a stronger relationship between No themselves and the potential perpetrator 7. Individuals who intervene will perceive a weaker relationship between No the potential victim and potential perpetrator 8. Individuals who intervene will perceive a situation as more risky for the No victim 9. Individuals who intervene will perceive a situation as less risky for Yes themselves 10. Individuals will be more likely to intervene if someone else has already intervened 11. There will be a curvilinear relationship between perceived impairment No due to alcohol and bystander intervention Perceptions of peers’ rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology and prosocial behavior tendencies variables will significantly predict bystander behavior percentage, controlling for individual levels of these variables and gender. It was hypothesized that perceptions of peers’ rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology would predict decrease in bystander behavior percentage, and perceptions of peers’ prosocial tendencies would positively predict bystander behavior percentage. A hierarchical liner regression was conducted to test hypothesis 1. Gender was entered into the first block, individual 35 levels of rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies were entered in the second block, and social norms levels of rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies were entered in the third block (see Table 7). When regressed on bystander behavior frequency, block 1 was significant, F(1, 321) = 9.70, p < .01, Adj. R2 = .03. There was a significant F change from block 1 to block 2, F∆(3, 318) = 3.08, p < .05 R2∆ = .03. However, contrary to hypothesis 1, there was no significant F change from block 2 to block 3, p = .61. In the final model, the main effect of gender was significantly predictive of bystander frequency, β = .15, t(321) = 2.42, p < .05, such that women intervened more than men, and a main effect of individual level prosocial tendencies also significantly predicted increases in bystander frequency, β = .15, t(321) = 2.37, p < .05. All other predictors were not significant, p > .05. Follow-up analyses positing gender as a moderator of the relationship between all predictors in the final model and bystander frequencies were explored. Six total hierarchal linear regression analyses were conducted. Each of the six regressions were identical for the first three blocks, where gender was entered into the first block, individual levels of rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies were entered in the second block, and social norms levels of rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies were entered in the third block. The interaction term between gender and each predictor was unique to each analysis, and was the fourth and final block entered into the model. 36 Table 7 Summary of Hierarchical Linear Regression Predicting Bystander Behavior Frequency from Gender, Individual Level, and Social Norms Level Factors (N = 323) Predictor Variable Block 1 Gender Block 2 Gender Rape Myth Acceptance – Self Hypergender Ideology – Self Prosocial Tendencies – Self Block 3 Gender Rape Myth Acceptance – Self Hypergender Ideology – Self Prosocial Tendencies – Self Rape Myth Acceptance – Other Hypergender Ideology – Other Prosocial Tendencies – Other Note. *p < .05, **p < .01 B SE B β .13 .04 .17** .12 .00 .00 .01 .05 .00 .00 .00 .15* .00 .02 .17** .11 .00 .00 .01 .00 .00 .02 .05 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .02 .15* .03 .00 .15* -.06 .02 .06 ΔF 9.71** ΔR2 .03 3.08** .03 .60 .01 Of the six separate analyses that were conducted, only two had significant interaction terms and will be reported here. In the analyses containing the interaction between individual rape myth acceptance and gender, the final block was significant, F∆(1, 312) = 6.70, p = .01 R2∆ = .02. Controlling for all other variables in the model, the interaction between individual rape myth acceptance and gender was significant, β = .17, t(312) = 2.58, p < .05, such that there was no relationship between individual rape myth acceptance and bystander frequency for women, and as individual rape myth acceptance increased for men, bystander frequency marginally decreased (β = -.26, p = .07). A plot of the estimated marginal means can be found in Figure 2. 37 Figure 2. Predicted bystander frequencies by individual rape myth acceptance for men and women at 1 SD below the mean and 1 SD above the mean. In the analyses containing the interaction between individual hypergender ideology and gender, the final block was significant, F∆(1, 312) = 7.84, p = .005 R2∆ = .02. Controlling for all other variables in the model, the interaction between individual hypergender ideology and gender was significant, β = .17, t(312) = 2.80, p < .01, such that there was no relationship between individual hypergender ideology and bystander frequency for women, and as hypergender ideology increased for men, bystander frequency marginally decreased (β = -.24, p = .06). A plot of the estimated marginal means can be found in Figure 3. 38 Figure 3. Predicted bystander frequencies by individual hypergender ideology for men and women at 1 SD below the mean and 1 SD above the mean. Victims of sexual assault will perceive more opportunities to intervene and intervene at a higher percentage than non-victims. Two separate 2 X 2 completely between-subjects factorial analyses of variances (ANOVAs) with bystander opportunities and bystander behavior frequency as the dependent variables were conducted. Previous victimization (victim, non-victim) and gender (male, female) were the between-subjects variables. The interaction between gender and previous victimization on opportunities 39 was not significant, F(1, 320) = .25, p = .62. However, a main effect of previous victimization was significant, F(1, 320) = 4.913, p < .05, partial η2 = .02, such that victims (M = 68.19, SD = 46.99) perceived more opportunities than non-victims (M = 49.68, SD =41.49). The main effect of gender was not significant, p = .86. In the analyses where bystander behavior frequency was the outcome variable, the interaction between gender and previous victimization was also not significant, F(1, 320) = 3.11, p = .08. However, a main effect of previous victimization was significant, F(1, 320) = 5.34, p < .05, partial η2 = .02, such that victims (M = .53, SD = .29) intervened at a higher percentage than non-victims (M = .45, SD = .33) The main effect of gender was also not significant, p = .21. Perpetrators of sexual assault will perceive fewer opportunities to intervene and intervene at a lower percentage than non-perpetrators. Two separate 2 X 2 completely between-subjects factorial analyses of variances (ANOVAs) with bystander opportunities and bystander behavior frequency as the dependent variables were conducted to test hypothesis 3. Previous perpetration (perpetrator, non-perpetrator) and gender (male, female) were the between-subjects variables. The interaction between gender and previous perpetration on bystander opportunities was not significant, F(1, 323) = .42, p = .52. Contrary to hypothesis 3, there was a main effect of previous perpetration, F(1, 323) = 4.09, p < .05, partial η2 = .01, such that perpetrators (M = 64.52, SD = 53.25) perceived more opportunities to intervene than non-perpetrators (M = 51.17., SD = 39.33). The main effect of gender was not significant, p = .53. 40 In the next analyses, the interaction between gender and previous perpetration on bystander frequency was not significant, F(1, 323) = .43, p = .51. Also contrary to hypothesis 3, the main effect of previous perpetration on bystander behavior frequency was also not significant, F(1, 323) = .65, p = .42. There was a main effect for gender, F(1, 323) = 13.04, p < .01, partial η2 = .04, such that females (M = .50, SD = .32) intervened at a higher percentage than men (M = .36, SD = .29). Individuals who intervene will report having a lower percentage of close friends with them at the time of an incident than those who do not intervene. An independent samples t-test was conducted to test hypothesis 4. There was no difference in percentage of close friends present at the time of the event for those who did intervene (M = .28, SD = .36) and those who did not intervene (M = .22, SD = .25), p = .34, contrary to hypothesis 4. A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the relationship between percentage of close friends present at the time of the event and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, percentage of friends one considers close present at the time of the event, and the interaction between gender and percentage of friends one considers close. The model was not significant, χ2(3, N =140) = .91, p = .82, therefore percentage of close friends at the time of the incident was unrelated to whether or not a bystander intervened. Individuals who intervene will report having a stronger perceived relationship with the potential victim at the time of an incident than those who do not intervene. An independent samples t-test was conducted to test hypothesis 5. Consistent with hypothesis 5, those who intervened perceived a closer relationship with the victim (M = 2.57, SD = 41 2.04) than those who did not intervene (M = 1.71, SD = 1.33), t(150) = 3.15, p < .05, Cohen’s d = .51. A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the relationship between perceived closeness with the potential victim and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, perceived closeness to the perceived victim at the time of the event, and the interaction between gender and perceived closeness to the perceived victim. The model was significant, χ2(3, N =150) = 9.68, p < .05, Nagelkerke’s R2 = .08, but the interaction between perceived closeness to the potential victim and gender was not significant, p = .63, therefore gender does not moderate the relationship between perceived closeness of the bystander to the potential victim and bystander intervention. Individuals who intervene will report having a stronger perceived relationship with the potential perpetrator at the time of an incident than those who do not intervene. An independent samples t-test was conducted to test hypothesis 6. There was no difference in perceived closeness to perpetrator between those who intervened (M = 1.40, SD = 1.05) and those who did not intervene (M = 1.57, SD = 1.26), p = .40, contrary to hypothesis 6. A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the relationship between perceived closeness with the potential perpetrator and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, perceived closeness to the perceived perpetrator at the time of the event, and the interaction between gender and perceived closeness to the perceived perpetrator. The model was not significant, χ2(1, N =152) = 1.71, p = .64, thus gender did not moderate the relationship 42 between perceived closeness to the potential perpetrator and bystander intervention at the time of an event. Individuals will be more likely to intervene if they perceive a weak relationship between the potential victim and the potential perpetrator than if they perceived a strong relationship. An independent samples t-test was conducted to test hypothesis 7. There was no difference in perception of the relationship between the victim and the perpetrator for those who intervened (M = 2.14, SD = 1.53) and those who did not intervene (M = 2.29, SD = 1.47). A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the relationship between the perceived relationship between the potential victim and potential perpetrator, and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, perceived closeness of the relationship between the perceived perpetrator and perceived victim at the time of the event, and the interaction between gender and perceived closeness of the relationship between the perceived perpetrator and perceived victim. The model was not significant, χ2(3, N =153) = 2.86, p = .41, therefore perceived closeness of the relationship between the perceived perpetrator and perceived victim was unrelated to whether or not a bystander intervened. Individuals who intervene will perceive a situation as more risky for the potential victim than individuals who do not intervene. An independent samples t-test was conducted to test hypothesis 8. There was no difference in perceived riskiness for the potential victim for those who intervened (M = 2.26, SD = 1.23) and those who did not intervene (M = 2.12, SD = 1.39). A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the relationship between perceived riskiness for the 43 potential victim and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, perceived risk for the perceived victim at the time of the event, and the interaction between gender and perceived risk for the perceived victim. The model was not significant, χ2(3, N =153) = 1.233, p = .75, therefore perceived risk was unrelated to whether or not a bystander intervened. Individuals who intervene will perceive a situation as less risky for themselves than individuals who do not intervene. An independent samples t-test was conducted to test hypothesis 9. Those who intervened perceived a situation as less risky for themselves (M = 6.29, SD = 3.65) than those who did not intervene (M = 7.78, SD = 4.78), t(149) = 2.02, p < .05, Cohen’s d = .38. A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the relationship between perceived risk for self and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, perceived risk for the bystander at the time of the event, and the interaction between gender and perceived risk for the bystander. The model was marginally significant, χ2(3, N =152) = 6.9, p = .08, Nagelkerke’s R2 = .06, but the interaction between riskiness for self and gender was not significant, p = .13 Individuals will be more likely to intervene in situations where someone else has already intervened than in situations where others have not intervened. A chi-square test of association was conducted to test hypothesis 10. Contrary to hypothesis 10, individuals were more likely to intervene in situations where there was no previous intervention than in situations when someone had intervened, χ2 (1, N = 153) = 8.86, p < .01, OR = 5.68. A logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine gender as a moderator of the 44 relationship between previous intervention at the time of an event and bystander intervention. Bystander intervention was regressed on gender, previous intervention at the time of the event was entered into the first block, and the interaction between previous intervention and gender was entered into the second block. The block with the interaction term was not significant, χ2(1, N =153) = .37, p = .55 Whether or not an individual intervenes will have a curvilinear relationship with perceived impairment due to alcohol at the time of the event, such that at low (e.g., sober) and high levels (e.g., drunk or very drunk) of perceived intoxication bystanders will be less likely to intervene, and at medium levels (e.g., buzzed; tipsy) of intoxications bystander will more likely to intervene. A logistic regression analyses was conducted to test hypothesis 11. In step 1, bystander intervention was regressed on gender and perceived impact of alcohol at the time of the event. In step 2, perceived impact of alcohol at the time of the event squared was added to the model. The step 1 model was significant, χ2(2, N =153) = 8.71, p < .05, Nagelkerke’s R2 = .08. There was a main effect of perceived impact of alcohol, such that perceived impact of alcohol was negatively associated with bystander intervention, β = -.19, p < .01, Exp(B) = .82. The step 2 model was not significant, χ2(1, N =153) = 2.35, p = .13, therefore perceived impact of alcohol does not have a curvilinear relationship with bystander intervention, contrary to hypothesis 11. Exploratory Analyses To explore which situational factors are significant in the presence of others, a logistic regression analyses was conducted. Bystander intervention was regressed on, 45 perceived closeness to the perceived victim, perceived risk for the bystander, previous intervention, and perceived impact of alcohol (see Table 8). The model was significant, χ2(4, N =148) = 32.00 p < .001, Nagelkerke’s R2 = .27. Perceived closeness to the perceived victim was positively related to bystander intervention (Β = .38, p < .05), and perceived impact of alcohol was negatively related to bystander intervention (Β = -.22, p < .05; perceived risk for the potential bystander was no longer significant (p > .05). Furthermore, individuals were more likely to intervene if no one else had previously intervened than if someone else had already intervened (Β = 1.90, p < .05). Table 8 Summary of Exploratory Logistic Regression Coefficients for N = 148 Perception of Alcohol Impact Perceived Closeness with Victim Perceived Risk for the Bystander Previous Intervention Note. *p < .05, **p < .01. B -.22 .38 -.07 1.90 S.E. .08 .13 .05 .70 Exp(B) .80** 1.47** .93 6.70** Because of the null findings with the social norms variables, possible indirect relationships between the social norms variables and bystander intervention were explored. Given that only some situations factors (e.g., perceived impairment from alcohol use, perceived riskiness for the bystander, previous intervention occurring, perceived closeness with the victim) were significantly related with bystander intervention, only these variables were considered. Intuitively, perceived closeness with the victim, previous intervention, and alcohol use likely would not be influenced by 46 social norms. However, the perceived riskiness for the bystander could be influenced by social norms, as perceived risk for the bystander includes risk of emotional and social consequences. Thus, the researcher explored the relationships between social norms, perceived risk for the bystander and whether an individual intervened in their situation. Perceived risk for the bystander was negatively related to bystander intervention, r = -.16, p < .05. Furthermore, upon a closer examination of the perceived risk for the bystander variable, perceived emotional risk for the bystander (i.e., made fun of for intervening) was negatively related to bystander behavior, r = -.22, p < .05, but social, physical, and legal risk were unrelated to bystander behavior p > .05. Furthermore, perceived emotional risk for the potential bystander was positively related to peer perceived rape myth acceptance (r = .26, p < .01) and peer perceived hypergender ideology (r = .20, p < .05), as well as negatively related to peer perceived prosocial tendencies (r = -.20, p < .05). Perceived social risk for the potential bystander was positively related to peer perceived rape myth acceptance (r = .20, p < .05) and peer perceived hypergender ideology (r = .20, p < .05), as well as negatively related to peer perceived prosocial tendencies (r = -.17, p < .05). 47 CHAPTER 4: DISCUSSION The present study assessed individual and social norms levels of rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies, as well as previous sexual assault history and situational factors as predictors of bystander intervention in risky sexual assault situations in college men and women. Furthermore, the researcher explored the impact of gender on the relationships between these factors and bystander intervention, as well as examined which situational variables predict bystander behavior in the presence of other similar factors. The current study is one of the first to use a measure of bystander behavior which controlled for opportunity to intervene (Murphy, 2012), and the first to examine the social norms levels of rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology and prosocial tendencies. Furthermore, the present study is the first to examine situational variables continuously (e.g., perceived relationships) and the perceived impact of alcohol, and bystander behavior intervention. Results indicated that, on average, individuals report intervening in about 47% of perceived risky sexual assault situations, which is a larger percentage than previous research has found (23%; Murphy, 2012). Women intervened at a higher percentage than men, which is consistent with previous literature (Banyard, 2008). Men endorsed higher levels of individual rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology, as well as lower levels of prosocial tendencies, which is also consistent with previous research (Bennet et al., 2014; McMahon, 2010). For men, the individual level and social norms level variables were moderately correlated, which is consistent with previous research (Dardis et al., 2015), whereas for women they were weakly to moderately correlated. 48 The first aim of our study was to examine the relationship between individual and social norm level variables and bystander behavior by using a measure of bystander behavior that takes opportunity to intervene into account. Inconsistent with previous research (e.g., Brown & Messman-Moore, 2010; Murphy, 2012) and social norms theory (Berkowitz, 2010), social norms level variables did not significantly predict frequency of bystander behavior in the presence of gender and individual level variables. In fact, there were no significant correlations between any of the social norm level variables and bystander behavior frequency for either gender, with the exception of perception of peers’ prosocial tendencies having a weak correlation with bystander behavior frequency for females. However, the relationship between peer perceived prosocial tendencies and bystander frequency did not remain significant when controlling for other variables in the model. Although these social norm beliefs may contribute and perpetuate a rape-prone culture (Berkowitz, 2010), they may not be impactful when facing a risky sexual assault situation. It may be that proximal factors, such as those influencing each unique risky situation, are more important to consider, and social norms may influence these proximal factors. For example, results showed significant associations between our social norms variables and perceived risk for the potential bystander, which is a proximal risk factor that may be more influential than social norms when in a potentially risky situation. Moreover, the failure to find a relationship between the social norm level variables and bystander behavior could be explained by the type of norms (i.e., descriptive norms) and who were the norms were reference too (i.e., peers). First, the researcher only examined perceptions of descriptive norms (i.e., what people do or 49 believe) rather than perceptions of injunctive norms (i.e., what peers approve of). Limited research has found support for the relationship between injunctive norms (e.g., peer approval for sexual assault, peer comfort with sexism) and bystander intervention (Brown & Messman-Moore, 2010; Murphy, 2012), but these variables were not examined in this study. A second explanation for the null findings may be that what is normative in any given situation may depend on the peers that are present (e.g., close friends versus acquaintances), which could vary in any given situation and is difficult to capture using a retrospective, self-report measure. Previous research that has examined descriptive norms and sexual assault examined descriptive norms by utilizing differing levels of relationships in which the norms were based on (e.g., close friend, average student at that university; Dardis et al., 2015). It may be that examining perceptions of close friend’s beliefs rather than “peers” would be more fruitful. The results found in the current study indicate that addressing descriptive social norms with regards to their “peers” about rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology and prosocial tendencies may not be a fruitful target in bystander programming, but addressing injunctive norms or proximal factors (e.g., perceived impairment due to alcohol) should receive more attention in programs aimed at both men and women. Moreover, future research aimed at better understanding how injunctive norms, specifically norms within close peer groups, regarding rape myth acceptance, hypergender ideology, and prosocial tendencies may find results. Interestingly, only individual level prosocial tendencies and gender predicted bystander behavior in the presence of all other social norms and individual level predictors in the model. One explanation for the null findings of individual levels of rape 50 myth acceptance and hypergender ideology may be that these variables are less relevant for women. Given women exhibited significantly lower levels of individual hypergender ideology and rape myth acceptance, it may be that these variables do not apply to this population. Furthermore, gender was a significant moderator of the relationships between individual beliefs and bystander behavior frequency, which suggests that rape myth acceptance and hypergender ideology have a negative influence on men’s bystander behavior and do not seem to influence women’s bystander behavior. These results suggest that programs for men and women may need to have different aims, goals, and content, as some factors that are relevant for men may not be relevant for women. The present study found mixed results in regards to the relationship between previous sexual assault history and bystander behavior. Consistent with previous research (Murphy, 2012), victims of sexual assault perceived more opportunities to intervene as well as intervened at a higher percentage than non-victims. It is plausible that victims of sexual assault perceived more opportunities because they are more vigilant as a result of their experience, and that they intervene more frequently because they have added empathy for other victims as a result of their own victimization experiences (Vonderhaar & Cormody, 2015). Contrary to what was hypothesized but consistent with previous research (Murphy, 2012), perpetrators perceived more opportunities to intervene than did non-perpetrators, but perpetrators and non-perpetrators did not differ in bystander intervention frequency. Perpetrators may be more likely to be in places where they are more likely to witness risky situations, such as bars or other parties. Given the behavioral way in which we asked the questions related to bystander opportunity, it is possible that 51 they may have endorsed being in situations where there was opportunity to intervene, but may not have actually identified several of the situations as problematic or worthy of intervention. According to the situational model of bystander intervention, one must identify a situation as problematic in order to intervene (Latane & Darley, 1970), but if perpetrators do not identify these situations as problematic it may prevent them from engaging in intervention. These results suggest that future research is needed to examine why perpetrators identify a greater number of risky situations, and ask perpetrators if they actually identify these situations as problematic, as well as identify other reasons why they are not intervening more (e.g., peer influences, personal beliefs). The second aim of this study was to examine situational factors as they related to actual experiences, rather than using hypothetical situations or vignettes. The current study failed to find a significant relationship between percentage of close friends present and bystander behavior, which suggests that total number of close friends may not be related, but rather the qualities of those who are there may matter more. It is possible that individuals who are surrounded by prosocial peers in one situation may be more likely to act in that situation than they would be if they were around peers who were less prosocial or held more positive attitudes or beliefs towards sexual assault (Gidycz, Wyatt, Galbreath, & Axelrad, 2016). Consistent with previous research, individuals who intervened perceived themselves as closer to the potential victim (Bennett & Banyard, 2014), which could imply that individuals are more likely to protect those who they care about. Furthermore, perceived closeness to the perceived perpetrator and perceived closeness of the relationship between the individuals involved in the incident were both 52 unrelated to whether an individual intervened. Taken together, these findings suggest that individuals help when they see their friends in a risky situation, may not perceive their close friends as being potential perpetrators, and intervene at the same rate regardless of how close they believe the perceived perpetrator and victim are. Thus, future programming efforts should aim to help individuals overcome barriers associated with intervening in situations in which they do not know the potential victim. Based on our results, bystander intervention programs may also need to focus on fostering discussions that help participants identify the situations that they commonly see as problematic. Furthermore, there was no difference in perceived riskiness for the potential victim between those who intervened and did not intervene, which is contrary to previous research (McMahon et al., 2015). One potential explanation for this finding was the overall low mean and variability reported in riskiness, as well as how the question was asked. Participants were asked about imminent danger, rather than overall risk for a sexual assault to eventually occur, which could have skewed the results. For example, the situation that the most participants identified as their most recent situation was “I saw a man talking to a woman at a bar. He was sitting very close to her and by the look on her face I could see she was uncomfortable.” Although this may be more dangerous if further actions were taken, it is unlikely that someone would commit an active act of sexual assault in a crowded bar. Although perceived riskiness for the victim was not significant, perceived riskiness to intervene for the bystander did impact bystander intervention, such that those who intervened felt safer than those who did not. Interestingly, men perceived more risk than females did, which is surprising given previous research has found that 53 skills deficits, including fearing for one’s own safety, is negatively related to bystander intervention for women, but not men (Burn, 2009). However, the measure we used encompassed individual risk beyond physical risk, including social, emotional, and legal risk. Our findings suggest that perceived risk may vary by gender, and certain types of perceived individual risk may matter more (e.g., emotional risk). Furthermore, it is possible that perceived risk may vary by whom one is with, such that emotional and social risk may matter more when individuals are with individuals they know, as the consequences associated with those peers are greater when they know them. Current programs could emphasize that the risk associated with intervention is minimal, given that nearly 80% of those who intervened in one sample have reported positive outcomes related to their intervention (Murphy, 2012). Contrary to hypothesized, we found that individuals were more likely to intervene if no one else had intervened than if someone else had already intervened. It makes sense that one may not intervene if intervention is already taking place; the perceived need for them to intervene at that point may be quite low. Furthermore, one may be more inclined to intervene if no one is addressing the situation, which is one aim of bystander intervention programs (e.g., Gidycz et al., 2011). It may be worth examining other dynamics in future research, such as how much support one gives to a friend if a friend or close peer chooses to intervene, or assess necessity for further intervention once someone else has intervened. In terms of programming applications, this finding elucidates the importance of educating individuals to intervene if intervention is already taking place, even if it as simple as asking, “Is everything okay?” 54 Although results did not support a curvilinear relationship between perceived alcohol intoxication and bystander behavior, we found a linear relationship; something previous research has failed to do (Murphy, 2012). As alcohol consumption increases, participants’ bystander intervention decreased, even when controlling for other situational factors. One potential explanation that we failed to find a curvilinear relationship is the scale we used and our sample size; two questions on Likert scales may not produce enough variability to find this relationship in a sample of 153. Furthermore, it may be difficult to gauge subjective impairment and intoxication retrospectively, and future research in laboratory settings that measure actual blood alcohol level may yield more fruitful results. Nonetheless, our findings imply that it is imperative to integrate alcohol education into bystander education programs and other sexual assault prevention programming (e.g., Gilmore, Lewis, & George, 2015). Although our findings may have implications for bystander intervention programs, there are several limitations that must be taken into consideration. Although the demographic characteristics are consistent with the population of the location where the data were collected, a primarily Caucasian, female, heterosexual sample limits generalizability of these findings. In addition to the methodological considerations noted above, in many instances one question was used to assess what may be complex constructs (e.g., risk for the victim), which makes it difficult to reliably make claims related to our results. Moreover, we used retrospective self-report measures, which may not always be accurate, especially when an individual may be trying to remember an event that occurred two, or even three months prior. In addition, the measure of bystander 55 behavior contained flaws worth noting. Each opportunity was paired with one specific intervention strategy, and it may be that participants intervened in some other way, which would not be captured by our measure. The bystander opportunity scale had some questions that counted as opportunities to intervene that may not have indicated any real risk for sexual assault, such as “I went to a party.” Despite these limitations, there are notable strengths to the study. Our study was one of the first to examine bystander behavior while accounting for opportunity to intervene and we used a measure that assessed actual bystander behavior rather than attitudes towards intervention or intent to intervene as a proxy for actual behavior. The finding that participants intervened in 47% of opportunities is nearly double what was found in previous research utilizing the same instrument to assess intervention behavior (Murphy, 2012), which may be a promising finding for bystander intervention educators. Furthermore, we asked participants about actual events, rather than using vignettes to create situations that they may or may not actually be in. To our knowledge, we are the first to examine the perceived risk for the potential bystander, previous intervention in any given situation, subjective intoxication and impairment related to alcohol use, and their relationships to whether or not an individual intervened in a variety of situations. Furthermore, the majority of our findings can be used to inform bystander educational programming, which could potentially have far-reaching implications. Future research should aim to address more diverse samples, given that race and sexual orientation have been found to contribute to bystander intervention (Brown et al., 2014; Katz, 2015). Moreover, future research should continue to evaluate measures of 56 bystander behavior, and explore different ways in which we can account for opportunity while accurately measuring intervention frequency (McMahon et al., in press). Future research should also further explore several of the situational variables in more depth, such as riskiness for the victim (e.g., what determines what is “risky” for a victim) or perceived impairment from alcohol (e.g., alcohol use in laboratory settings). Despite our null findings, research should continue to examine social norms variables, specifically the impact of injunctive social norms regarding sexual violence and related constructs (e.g., rape myth acceptance) and evaluate the use of a social norms approach to understanding bystander behavior (McMahon, 2015). 57 REFERENCES Austin, M. J., Dardis, C., Wilson, S., Gidycz, C. A., & Berkowitz, A. D. (2015). Predictors of sexual assault-specific prosocial bystander behavior and intentions: A prospective analysis. Violence Against Women. Banyard, V. L. (2008). Measurement and correlates of pro-social bystander behavior: The case of interpersonal violence. Violence and Victims, 23, 85–99. doi:10.1891/0886670823183 Banyard, V. L. (2011). Who will help prevent sexual violence: Creating an ecological model of bystander intervention. Psychology of Violence, 1, 216–229. doi:10.1037/a0023739 Banyard, V. L. & Moynihan, M. M. (2011). Variation in bystander behavior related to sexual and intimate partner violence prevention: Correlates in a sample of college students. Psychology of Violence, 4, 101-115. Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., & Plante, E. G. (2007). Sexual violence prevention through bystander education: An experimental evaluation. Journal of Community Psychology, 35, 463–481. doi:10.1002/jcop.20159 Banyard, V. L., Plante, E., & Moynihan, M. M. (2004). Bystander education: Bringing a broader community perspective to sexual violence prevention. Journal of Community Psychology, 32, 61-79. doi:10.1002/jcop.10078 Banyard, V.L., Plante, E., Ward, S., Cohn, E.S., Moorhead, C., & Walsh, W. (2005). Revisiting unwanted sexual experiences on campus: A 12 year follow- up. Violence Against Women, 11, 426 – 446. doi: 10.1177/1077801204274388 58 Bennett, S., & Banyard, V. L. (2014). Do friends really help friends? The effect of relational factors and perceived severity on bystander perception of sexual violence. Psychology of Violence. Advance online publication. Bennett, S., Banyard, V. L., & Garnhart, L. (2014). To act or not to act, that is the question? Barriers and facilitators of bystander intervention. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29, 476-496. doi: 10.1177/0886260513505210 Berkowitz, A. D. (2002). Fostering men’s responsibility for preventing sexual assault. In P. A. Schewe (Ed.), Preventing violence in relationships (pp. 163– 196).Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Berkowitz, A. D. (2010). Fostering healthy norms to prevent violence and abuse: The social norms approach. In K. Kaufman (Ed.), Preventing Sexual Violence and Exploitation: A Sourcebook (pp. 415-447). Holyoke, MA: Neari Press. Brown, A. L., Banyard, V. L., & Moynihan, M. M. (2014). College students as helpful bystanders against sexual violence: Gender, race, and year in college moderate the impact of perceived peer norms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 38, 350-362. doi: 10.1177/0361684314526855 Brown, A. L., & Messman-Moore, T. L. (2010). Personal and perceived peer attitudes supporting sexual aggression as predictors of male college students’ willingness to intervene against sexual aggression. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25, 503–518. doi: 10.1177/0886260509334400 Burn, S. (2009). A situational model of sexual assault prevention through bystander intervention. Sex Roles, 60, 779–792. doi:10.1007/s11199-008–9581-5 59 Campbell, R., Dworkin, E., & Cabral, G. (2009). An ecological model of the impact of sexual assault on women’s mental health. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 10, 225246. doi:10.1177/1524838009334456 Carlo, G., Hausmann, A., Christiansen, S., & Randall, B. A. (2003). Sociocognitive and behavioral correlates of a measure of prosocial tendencies for adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 107–134. Carlson, M. (2008). I’d rather go along and be considered a man: Masculinity and bystander intervention. Journal of Men’s Studies, 16, 3-17. doi: 10.3149/jms.1601.3 Cecil, H., & Matson, S. C. (2006). Sexual victimization among African American adolescent females: Examination of the reliability and validity of the Sexual Experiences Survey. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 89-104. doi:10.1177/0886260505281606 Dardis, T. M., Murphy, M. J., Gidycz, C. A. & Bill, A. (2015). An investigation of the tenets of social norms theory as they relate to sexually aggressive attitudes and sexual assault perpetration: A comparison of men and their friends. Psychology of Violence. doi: 10.1037/a0039443 Fabiano, P. M., Perkins, H. W., Berkowitz, A., Linkenbach, J., & Stark, C. (2003). Engaging men as social justice allies in ending violence against women: Evidence for a social norms approach. Journal of American College Health, 52, 105–112. 60 Fisher, B. S., Daigle, L. E., Cullen, F. T., & Turner, M. G. (2003). Reporting sexual victimization to the police and others: Results from a national-level study of college women. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 6-38. Gidycz, C. A., Orchowski, L. M., & Berkowitz, A. D. (2011). Preventing sexual aggression among college men: An evaluation of a social norms and bystander intervention program. Violence Against Women, 17, 720–742. doi:10.1177/1077801211409727 Gilmore, A. K., Lewis, M. A., & George, W. H. (2015). A randomized control trial targeting alcohol use and sexual assault risk among college women at high risk for victimization. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 74, 38-49. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.08.007 Gross, A. M., Winslett, A., Miguel, R., & Gohm, C. L. (2006). An examination of sexual violence against college women. Violence Against Women, 12, 288-300. doi: 10.1177/1077801205277358 Hamburger, M. E., Hogben, M., McGowan, S., & Dawson, L. J. (1996). Assessing hypergender ideologies: Development and initial validation of a gender-neutral measure of adherence to extreme gender-roles. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 157-178. Katz, J. (2015). Effects of group status and victim sex on male bystanders’ responses to a potential party rape. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment, & Trauma, 24, 588-602. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2015.1029184 Katz, J., Colbert, S., & Colangelo, L. (2015). Effects of group status and victim sex 61 on female bystanders’ responses to a potential party rape. Violence and Victims, 30, 265-. doi:10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-13-00099 Koss, M.P., Abbey, A., Campbell, R., Cook, S., Norris, J., Testa, M., Ullman, S., West, C., & White, J. (2007). Revising the SES: A collaborative process to improve assessment of sexual aggression and victimization. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 357-370. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2007.00385.x Koss, M. P., Gidycz, C. A., & Wisniewski, N. (1987). The scope of rape: Incidence and prevalence of sexual aggression and victimization in a national sample of higher education students. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 55, 162-170. Koss, M. P. & Oros, C. J. (1982). Sexual Experiences Survey: A research instrument investigating sexual aggression and victimization. Journal of Consulting and Counseling Psychology, 50, 455-457. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.50.3.455 Krebs, C. P., Lindquist, C. H., Warner, T. D., Fisher, B. S., & Martin, S. L. (2007). The campus sexual assault (CSA) study. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, US Department of Justice. Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn’t he help? New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1994). Rape myths: In review. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 133–164. McMahon, S. (2010). Rape myth beliefs and bystander attitudes among incoming college students. Journal of American College Health, 59, 3–11. doi:10.1080/ 07448481.2010.483715 62 McMahon, S. (2015). Call for research on bystander intervention to prevent sexual violence: The role of campus environments. American Journal of Community Psychology, 55, 472-489. doi:10.1007/s10464-015-9724-0 McMahon, S., Banyard, V. L., & McMahon, S. M. (2015). Incoming college students’ bystander behaviors to prevent sexual violence. Journal of College Student Development, 56, 488-493. McMahon, S., Banyard, V. L., Palmer, J. E., Murphy, M., & Gidycz, C. A. (in press). Chances to reduce violence: Measuring bystander behavior in the context of opportunity to help. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. McMahon, S., & Farmer, G. L. (2011). An updated measure for assessing subtle rape myths. Social Work Research, 35, 71-81. doi: 10.1093/swr/35.2.71 Murnen, S. K., Wright, C., & Kaluzny, G. (2002). If “boys will be boys” then girls will be victims? A met-analytic review of the research that relates to masculine ideology to sexual aggression. Sex Roles, 46, 359-375. Murphy, M. J. (2012). Sexual assault-specific bystander behavior: Accounting for opportunity in a prospective analysis of the effects of individual, social norms, and situational variables. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Ohio University, Athens. Obama, B. (2014, January 22) Memorandum -- Establishing a white house task force to protect students from sexual assault. Retrieved from https://www.whitehouse.gov/the-press-office/2014/01/22/memorandumestablishing-white-house-task-force-protect-students-sexual-a 63 Payne, D. L., Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1999). Rape myth acceptance: Exploration of its structure and its measurement using the Illinois rape myth acceptance scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 27-68. Rushton, J. P., Chrisjohn, R. D., & Fekken, G. C. (1981). The altruistic personality and the self-report altruism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 1-11. Swartout, K. M. (2013). The company they keep: How peer networks influence male sexual aggression. Psychology of Violence, 3, 157-171. doi: 10.1037/a0029997 Steele, C. M., Critchlow, B. C., & Liu, T. J. (1985). Alcohol and social behavior II: The helpful drunkard. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 35-46. Tansill, E. C., Edwards, K. M., Kearns, M. C., Gidycz, C. A., & Calhoun, K. S. (2012). The mediating role between trauma-related symptoms in the relationship between sexual victimization and physical health symptomatology in undergraduate women. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 25, 79-85. doi: 10.1002/jts.21666 Vonderhaar, R. L., & Carmody, D. C. (2015). There are no “innocent victims”: The influence of just world beliefs and prior victimization on rape myth acceptance. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30, 1615-1632. doi:10.1177/0886260514549196 64 APPENDIX A: DEMOGRAPHICS Demographic Questionnaire DIRECTIONS: Please choose the best response for each question. 1. What is your age? A. 18 B. 19 C. 20 D. 21 E. 22 F. 23 G. 24 H. 25 I. Other _________ 2. What is your current year in college? A. First B. Second C. Third D. Fourth E. Fifth or above F. Graduate student G. Other ___________ 3. Where do you currently live? A. College dormitory or residence hall B. Sorority house C. Other University/college housing D. Off-campus house or apartment E. Parent/Guardian’s home F. Other 4. What is your race/ethnicity? A. Caucasian, Non-Hispanic B. African American C. Latino or Hispanic D. Asian or Pacific Islander E. American Indian or Alaska Native F. Two or more races G. Other 5. What is your religion? 65 A. B. C. D. E. F. G. Catholic (Christian) Protestant (Christian) Jewish Muslim Nondenominational Other None 6. Approximately what is your parents’ yearly income? A. Unemployed or disabled B. $10,000 – $20,000 C. $21,000 - $30,000 D. $31,000 - $40,000 E. $41,000 - $50,000 F. $51,000 - $75,000 G. $76,000 - $100,000 H. $100,000 - $150,000 I. $151,000 or more 7. What is your sexual orientation? A. Exclusively heterosexual B. Predominantly heterosexual, only incidentally homosexual C. Predominantly heterosexual, but more than incidentally homosexual D. Equally heterosexual and homosexual. E. Predominantly homosexual, but more than incidentally heterosexual F. Predominantly homosexual, only incidentally heterosexual G. Exclusively homosexual H. Asexual, or Non-Sexual 8. What is your current marital status? A. Never married B. Cohabitating (living together) C. Married D. Divorced E. Widowed 9. What is your current dating status? A. I do not date B. I date casually C. I date seriously D. I am involved in a long-term monogamous relationship (more than 6-months) E. I live with my partner F. I am engaged G. I am married 10. Have you ever participated in a sexual assault prevention or risk reduction program? 66 A. Yes B. No 11. If yes, how long ago? _________ 12. Are you in a sorority/fraternity? A. Yes B. No 13. Are you on a sports team? A. Yes, collegiate/varsity B. Yes, club C. Yes, intramural D. No 14. Height: __________ 15. Current Weight: ____________ 16. How do you identify? A. Male B. Female C. FTM (female-to-male) D. MTF (male-to-female) E. Intersex F. Genderqueer/Androgynous G. Other (please fill in): ____________________ 67 APPENDIX B: SEXUAL EXPERIENCES SURVEY - VICTMIZATION Sexual Experiences Survey – Short Form Victimization The following questions concern sexual experiences that you may have had that were unwanted. We know that these are personal questions, so we do not ask your name or other identifying information. Your information is completely confidential. We hope that this helps you to feel comfortable answering each question honestly. Place a check mark in the box (□) showing that this experience has happened to you. If several experiences occurred on the same occasion—for example, if one night someone told you some lies and had sex with you when you were drunk, you would check both boxes a and c. Answer the questions for the time period "Since age 14 up until today” Sexual Experiences 1. Someone fondled, kissed, or rubbed up against the private areas of my body (lips, breast/chest, crotch or butt) or removed some of my clothes without my consent (but did not attempt sexual penetration) by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. 2. Someone had oral sex with me or made me have oral sex with them without my consent by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ 68 3. A man put his penis into my vagina, or someone inserted fingers or objects without my consent by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. 4. A man put his penis into my butt, or someone inserted fingers or objects without my consent by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. 5. Even though it did not happen, someone TRIED to have oral sex with me, or make me have oral sex with them without my consent by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ 69 6. 7. Even though it did not happen, a man TRIED to put his penis into my vagina, or someone tried to stick in fingers or objects without my consent by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. Even though it did not happen, a man TRIED to put his penis into my butt, or someone tried to stick in objects or fingers without my consent by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about me, making promises I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring me after I said I didn't want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing my sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force, after I said I didn't want to. c. Taking advantage of me when I was too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm me or someone close to me. e. Using force, for example holding me down with their body weight, pinning my arms, or having a weapon. This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ This has happened to me since age 14 □ □ □ □ □ 8. Did any of the experiences described in this survey happen to you one or more times? Yes No 9. What was the sex of the person or persons who did them to you? Female only Male only Both females and males I reported no experiences 10. Have you ever been raped? Yes No 70 APPENDIX C: SEXUAL EXPERIENCES SURVEY – PERPETRATION Sexual Experiences Survey – Short Form Perpetration The following questions concern sexual experiences. We know these are personal questions, so we do not ask your name or other identifying information. Your information is completely confidential. We hope this helps you to feel comfortable answering each question honestly. Place a check mark in the box for each of the experiences below that has happened. If several experiences occurred on the same occasion--for example, if one night you told some lies and had sex with someone who was drunk, you would check both boxes a and c. Since age 14 refers to your life starting on your 14th birthday and stopping one year ago from today. Answer the questions for the time period "Since age 14 up until today” Sexual Experiences 1. I fondled, kissed, or rubbed up against the private areas of someone’s body (lips, breast/chest, crotch or butt) or removed some of their clothes without their consent (but did not attempt sexual penetration) by: a. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. b. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. c. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. e. 2 Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. I had oral sex with someone or had someone perform oral sex on me without their consent by: a. This has happened to me since age 14 Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. This has happened to me since age 14 71 b. c. 3. d. Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. e. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. I put my penis (men only) or I put my fingers or objects (all respondents) into a woman’s vagina without her consent by: a. b. c. 4. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. e. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. I put in my penis (men only) or I put my fingers or objects (all respondents) into someone’s butt without their consent by: a. b. c. d. This has happened to me since age 14 Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. This has happened to me since age 14 72 e. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. This has happened to me since age 14 5. Even though it did not happen, I TRIED to have oral sex with someone or make them have oral sex with me without their consent by: a. b. c. d. e. 6. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. Even though it did not happen, I TRIED put in my penis (men only) or I tried to put my fingers or objects (all respondents) into a woman’s vagina without their consent by: a. b. c. Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. d. Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. e. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. This has happened to me since age 14 73 7. Even though it did not happen, I TRIED to put in my penis (men only) or I tried to put my fingers or objects (all respondents) into someone’s butt without their consent by: a. b. c. d. e. This has happened to me since age 14 Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship, threatening to spread rumors about them, making promises about the future I knew were untrue, or continually verbally pressuring them after they said they didn’t want to. Showing displeasure, criticizing their sexuality or attractiveness, getting angry but not using physical force after they said they didn’t want to. Taking advantage when they were too drunk or out of it to stop what was happening. Threatening to physically harm them or someone close to them. Using force, for example holding them down with my body weight, pinning their arms, or having a weapon. 8. Did you do any of the acts described in this survey 1 or more times? Yes 9. If yes, what was the sex of the person or persons to whom you did them? Female only Male only Both females and males I reported no experiences 10. Do you think you may have you ever raped someone? Yes No No 74 APPENDIX D: REVISED CONFLICT TACTICS SCALE CTS2 Directions: Please select below how many times you did each of these things in since age 14. 1 = Once since age 14 5 = 11-20 times since age 14 2 = Twice since age 14 6 = More than 20 times since age 14 3 = 3-5 times since age 14 before 7 = Not since age 14 but it did happen 4 = 6-10 times since age 14 0 = This has never happened 1. I made someone have sex without a condom. 2. I used force (like hitting, holding down, or using a weapon) to make someone have oral or anal sex. 3. I used force (like hitting, holding down, or using a weapon) to make someone have sex. 4. I insisted on sex when the other person did not want to (but did not use physical force). 5. I used threats to make someone have oral or anal sex. 6. I insisted someone have oral or anal sex (but did not use physical force). 7. I used threats to make my someone have sex. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 75 APPENDIX E: RAPE MYTH ACCEPTANCE – SELF Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale – Self Directions: For the following questions, please rate how much YOU agree with the following statements: 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree 1. If a girl is raped while she is drunk, she is at least somewhat responsible for letting things get out of control. 1 2 3 4 5 2. When girls go to parties wearing slutty clothes, they are asking for trouble. help her. 1 2 3 4 5 3. If a girl goes to a room alone with a guy at a party, it is her own fault is she is raped 1 2 3 4 5 4. If a girl acts like a slut, eventually she is going to get into trouble. 1 2 3 4 5 5. When girls are raped, it’s often because the way they said “no” is often unclear. 1 2 3 4 5 6. If a girl initiates the kissing or hooking up, she should not be surprised if a a guy assumes she wants to have sex. 1 2 3 4 5 7. When guys rape, it is usually because of their strong desire for sex. 1 2 3 4 5 8. Guys don’t usually intend to force sex on a girl, but sometimes they get too sexually carried away. 1 2 3 4 5 9. Rape happens when a guy’s sex drive gets out of control. 1 2 3 4 5 10. If a guy is drunk, he might rape someone unintentionally. 1 2 3 4 5 11. It shouldn’t be considered rape if a guy is drunk and didn’t realize what he was doing 1 2 3 4 5 12. If both people are drunk, it can’t be rape. 1 2 3 4 5 13 If a girl doesn’t physically resist sex – even if protesting verbally – it can’t be considered rape 1 2 3 4 5 14. If a girl doesn’t physically fight back, you can’t really say it was rape. 1 2 3 4 5 15. A rape probably didn’t happen if the girl has no bruises or marks. 1 2 3 4 5 76 16. If the accused “rapist” doesn’t have a weapon, you can’t really call it rape. 1 2 3 4 5 17. If a girl doesn’t say “no” she can’t claim rape. 1 2 3 4 5 18. A lot of times, girls who say they were raped agreed to have sex and then regret it. 1 2 3 4 5 19. Rape accusations are often used as a way of getting back at guys. 1 2 3 4 5 20. A lot of times, girls who say they were raped often led the guy on and then had regrets. 1 2 3 4 5 21. A lot of times, girls who claim they were raped just have emotional problems. 1 2 3 4 5 22. Girls who are caught cheating on their boyfriends sometimes claim that it was rape. 1 2 3 4 5 77 APPENDIX F: RAPE MYTH ACCEPTANCE – PEERS Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale – Peers Directions: For the following questions, please rate how much YOU BELIEVE YOUR PEERS AT OHIO UNIVERSITY agree with the following statements: 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree 1. If a girl is raped while she is drunk, she is at least somewhat responsible for letting things get out of control. 1 2 3 4 5 2. When girls go to parties wearing slutty clothes, they are asking for trouble. help her. 1 2 3 4 5 3. If a girl goes to a room alone with a guy at a party, it is her own fault is she is raped 1 2 3 4 5 4. If a girl acts like a slut, eventually she is going to get into trouble. 1 2 3 4 5 5. When girls are raped, it’s often because the way they said “no” is often unclear. 1 2 3 4 5 6. If a girl initiates the kissing or hooking up, she should not be surprised if a a guy assumes she wants to have sex. 1 2 3 4 5 7. When guys rape, it is usually because of their strong desire for sex. 1 2 3 4 5 8. Guys don’t usually intend to force sex on a girl, but sometimes they get too sexually carried away. 1 2 3 4 5 9. Rape happens when a guy’s sex drive gets out of control. 1 2 3 4 5 10. If a guy is drunk, he might rape someone unintentionally. 1 2 3 4 5 11. It shouldn’t be considered rape if a guy is drunk and didn’t realize what he was doing 1 2 3 4 5 12. If both people are drunk, it can’t be rape. 1 2 3 4 5 13 If a girl doesn’t physically resist sex – even if protesting verbally – it can’t be considered rape 1 2 3 4 5 14. If a girl doesn’t physically fight back, you can’t really say it was rape. 1 2 3 4 5 15. A rape probably didn’t happen if the girl has no bruises or marks. 1 2 3 4 5 78 16. If the accused “rapist” doesn’t have a weapon, you can’t really call it rape. 1 2 3 4 5 17. If a girl doesn’t say “no” she can’t claim rape. 1 2 3 4 5 18. A lot of times, girls who say they were raped agreed to have sex and then regret it. 1 2 3 4 5 19. Rape accusations are often used as a way of getting back at guys. 1 2 3 4 5 20. A lot of times, girls who say they were raped often led the guy on and then had regrets. 1 2 3 4 5 21. A lot of times, girls who claim they were raped just have emotional problems. 1 2 3 4 5 22. Girls who are caught cheating on their boyfriends sometimes claim that it was rape. 1 2 3 4 5 79 APPENDIX G: HYPERGENDER IDEOLOGY SCALE - SELF Hypergender Ideology Scale – Self Directions: For the following questions, please rate how much YOU agree with the following statements: 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Agree Slightly Agree Strongly Agree 1. A true man knows how to command others. 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. The only thing a lesbian needs is a good stiff cock. 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. Men should be ready to take any risk, if the payoff is large enough. 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. No wife is obliged to provide sex for anybody, even her husband. 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. Women should break dates with female friends when guys ask them out. 1 2 3 4 5 6 6. Men have to expect that most women will be something of a prick-tease. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7. A real man can get any women to have sex with him. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. Women instinctively try to manipulate men. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9. Get a woman drunk, high, or hot and she’ll let you do whatever you want. 1 2 3 4 5 6 10. Men should be in charge during sex. 1 2 3 4 5 6 11. It’s okay for a man to be a little forceful during sex. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. Women don’t mind a little force in sex sometimes because they know it means they must be attractive. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. Homosexuals can be just as good at parenting as heterosexuals. 1 2 3 4 5 6 13. Gays and lesbians are just like everybody else. 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. Pickups should expect to put out. 1 2 3 4 5 6 15. If men pay for a date, they deserve something in return. 1 2 3 4 5 6 16. Effeminate men deserved to be ridiculed. 1 2 3 4 5 6 17. Any man who is a man needs to have sex regularly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 80 18. I believe some women lead happy lives without having male partners. 1 2 3 4 5 6 81 APPENDIX H: HYPERGENDER IDEOLOGY SCALE - PEERS Hypergender Ideology Scale - Peers Directions: For the following questions, please rate how much YOU BELIEVE YOUR PEERS AT OHIO UNIVERSITY agree with the following statements: 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Agree Slightly Agree Strongly Agree 1. A true man knows how to command others. 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. The only thing a lesbian needs is a good stiff cock. 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. Men should be ready to take any risk, if the payoff is large enough. 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. No wife is obliged to provide sex for anybody, even her husband. 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. Women should break dates with female friends when guys ask them out. 1 2 3 4 5 6 6. Men have to expect that most women will be something of a prick-tease. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7. A real man can get any women to have sex with him. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. Women instinctively try to manipulate men. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9. Get a woman drunk, high, or hot and she’ll let you do whatever you want. 1 2 3 4 5 6 10. Men should be in charge during sex. 1 2 3 4 5 6 11. It’s okay for a man to be a little forceful during sex. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. Women don’t mind a little force in sex sometimes because they know it means they must be attractive. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. Homosexuals can be just as good at parenting as heterosexuals. 1 2 3 4 5 6 13. Gays and lesbians are just like everybody else. 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. Pickups should expect to put out. 1 2 3 4 5 6 15. If men pay for a date, they deserve something in return. 1 2 3 4 5 6 16. Effeminate men deserved to be ridiculed. 1 2 3 4 5 6 17. Any man who is a man needs to have sex regularly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 82 18. I believe some women lead happy lives without having male partners. 1 2 3 4 5 6 83 APPENDIX I: PROSOCIAL TENDENCIES MEASURE – SELF Prosocial Tendencies Measure – Self Directions: Below are a number of statements which may or may not describe you. Please indicate HOW MUCH EACH STATEMENT DESCRIBES YOU by using the scale below: 1 2 3 4 5 Does Not Describes Me Greatly Describes Me A Little Somewhat Describes Me Describes Me Well Describes Me Greatly 1. I can help others best when people are watching me. 1 2 3 4 5 2. It is most fulfilling to me when I can comfort someone who is very distressed. 1 2 3 4 5 3. When other people are around, it is easier for me to help needy others. 1 2 3 4 5 4. I think that one of the best things about helping others is that it makes me look 1 good. 2 3 4 5 5. I get the most out of helping others when it is done in front of others. 1 2 3 4 5 6. I tend to help people who are in a real crisis or need. 1 2 3 4 5 7. When people ask me to help them, I don't hesitate. 1 2 3 4 5 8. I prefer to donate money anonymously. 1 2 3 4 5 9. I tend to help people who hurt themselves badly. 1 2 3 4 5 10. I believe that donating goods or money works best when it is tax-deductible. 1 2 3 4 5 11. I tend to help needy others most when they do not know who helped them. 1 2 3 4 5 12. I tend to help others particularly when they are emotionally distressed. 1 2 3 4 5 13. Helping others when I am in the spotlight is when I work best. 1 2 3 4 5 14. It is easy for me to help others when they are in a dire situation. 1 2 3 4 5 15. Most of the time, I help others when they do not know who helped them. 1 2 3 4 5 16. I think there should be more recognition for the time and energy people spend on charity work. 1 2 4 5 3 84 17. I respond to helping others best when the situation is highly emotional. 1 2 3 4 5 18. I never hesitate to help others when they ask for it. 1 2 3 4 5 19. I think that helping others without them knowing is the best type of situation. 1 2 3 4 5 20. One of the best things about doing charity work is that it looks good on my resume 1 2 3 4 5 21. Emotional situations make me want to help needy others. 1 2 3 4 5 22. I often make anonymous donations because they make me feel good. 1 2 3 4 5 23. I feel that if I help someone, they should help me in the future. 1 2 3 4 5 85 APPENDIX J: PROSOCIAL TENDENCIES MEASURE – PEERS Prosocial Tendencies Measure – Peers Directions: Below are a number of statements which may or may not describe your peers. Please indicate HOW MUCH EACH STATEMENT DESCRIBES YOUR PEERS by using the scale below: 1 2 3 4 5 Does Not Describes Me Greatly Describes Me A Little Somewhat Describes Me Describes Me Well Describes Me Greatly 1. I can help others best when people are watching me. 1 2 3 4 5 2. It is most fulfilling to me when I can comfort someone who is very distressed. 1 2 3 4 5 3. When other people are around, it is easier for me to help needy others. 1 2 3 4 5 4. I think that one of the best things about helping others is that it makes me look 1 good. 2 3 4 5 5. I get the most out of helping others when it is done in front of others. 1 2 3 4 5 6. I tend to help people who are in a real crisis or need. 1 2 3 4 5 7. When people ask me to help them, I don't hesitate. 1 2 3 4 5 8. I prefer to donate money anonymously. 1 2 3 4 5 9. I tend to help people who hurt themselves badly. 1 2 3 4 5 10. I believe that donating goods or money works best when it is tax-deductible. 1 2 3 4 5 11. I tend to help needy others most when they do not know who helped them. 1 2 3 4 5 12. I tend to help others particularly when they are emotionally distressed. 1 2 3 4 5 13. Helping others when I am in the spotlight is when I work best. 1 2 3 4 5 14. It is easy for me to help others when they are in a dire situation. 1 2 3 4 5 15. Most of the time, I help others when they do not know who helped them. 1 2 3 4 5 16. I think there should be more recognition for the time and energy people spend on charity work. 1 2 3 4 5 17. I respond to helping others best when the situation is highly emotional. 1 2 3 4 5 86 18. I never hesitate to help others when they ask for it. 1 2 3 4 5 19. I think that helping others without them knowing is the best type of situation. 1 2 3 4 5 20. One of the best things about doing charity work is that it looks good on my resume 1 2 3 4 5 21. Emotional situations make me want to help needy others. 1 2 3 4 5 22. I often make anonymous donations because they make me feel good. 1 2 3 4 5 23. I feel that if I help someone, they should help me in the future. 1 2 3 4 5 87 APPENDIX K: BYSTANDER OPPORTUNITY SCALE Bystander Opportunity Scale Directions: Please read the list of situations below and estimate the number of times that you witnessed each situation over the past 4 months. If you did not see, hear, or suspect the item, please circle 0. If you witnessed or had this experience more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 1. I heard someone yelling and fighting. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 2. I saw a drunk person get left behind by their friends at a party. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 3. I saw someone at a party who had had too much to drink. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 4. Someone said that they had an unwanted sexual experience. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 5. I saw a woman being shoved or yelled at by a man. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 6. I heard what sounded like yelling and fighting through my dorm walls. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 7. I saw someone grabbing, pushing, or insulting their partner. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 8. I heard someone talking about forcing someone else to have sex with them. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 9. I saw someone’s drink get spiked with a drug. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 10. Someone told me they were sexually assaulted. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ Please read the list of situations below and estimate the number of times that you witnessed each situation over the past 4 months. If you did not see, hear, or suspect the item, please circle 0. If you witnessed or had this experience more than 88 5 times, please enter the number of times. 11. I suspected someone was in an abusive relationship. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 12. I suspected someone had been sexually assaulted. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 13. I suspected someone of engaging in behaviors that are abusive to others. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 14. I heard sexist jokes. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 15. I saw commercials that depict violence against women. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 16. I heard someone explain that women like to be raped. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 17. I heard someone say “she deserved to be raped.” 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 18. I had a drink with friends at a party. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 19. I went to a party with friends. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ Please read the list of situations below and estimate the number of times that you witnessed each situation over the past 4 months. If you did not see, hear, or suspect the item, please circle 0. If you witnessed or had this experience more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 20. I heard a sexist comment. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 21. I saw a man and his girlfriend get in a heated argument. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 89 22. I saw a man talking to a woman at a bar. He was sitting very close to her and by the look on her face I could see she was uncomfortable. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 23. Someone told me that they had had an unwanted sexual experience. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 24. I knew of information about an incident of sexual violence. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 25. I saw someone who looked drunk go to a room with someone else at a party. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 26. I heard someone use the words “ho,” “bitch,” or “slut” to describe girls. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 27. I heard someone plan to give someone alcohol to get sex. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ Please read the list of situations below and estimate the number of times that you witnessed each situation over the past 4 months. If you did not see, hear, or suspect the item, please circle 0. If you witnessed or had this experience more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 28. I witnessed an activity in which girls’ appearances were ranked/rated. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 29. I saw/heard about someone who was hooking up with someone who was passed out. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 30. I heard rumors that someone forced sex on someone else. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 31. I heard that someone had committed a rape. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 32. I saw someone doing things that might meet the definition of sexual assault. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 33. I saw someone trying to take advantage of someone’s intoxicated state to have with them. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 34. I heard someone talking about women in sexually 0 1 2 3 4 5 90 degrading ways. 35. I saw someone possibly committing a sexual assault. ____ 0 1 2 3 4 5 ____ 91 APPENDIX L: BYSTANDER BEHAVIOR SCALE Bystander Behavior Scale – Revised Please read the list below and indicate the number of times you have actually engaged in each of the BEHAVIORS below over the past 4 months. Please select 0 if you did not engage in this behavior over the past 8 weeks. If you engaged in this behavior more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 1. I Called 911 when I heard someone yelling and fighting. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 2. I talked to the friends of a drunk person to make sure they didn’t leave their drunk friend behind at the party. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 3. I asked someone I saw at a party who had had too much to drink if they needed to be walked home so they could go to sleep. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 4. When someone said that they had an unwanted sexual experience but they didn’t call it “rape” I questioned them further. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 5. I asked a woman who was being shoved or yelled at by a man if she needed help. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 6. I knocked on the door to see if everything was ok or talked with a resident counselor or someone who could help when I heard what sounded like yelling and fighting through my dorm walls. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 7. When I saw someone grabbing, pushing, or insulting their partner I confronted them or got help from other friends or university staff. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 8. I heard someone talking about forcing someone to have sex with them, and I spoke up against it and/or expressed concern for the person who was forced. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 9. I grabbed someone else’s cup and poured their drink out after I saw that someone slip something into it or I said something to the person whose drink was spiked even though I didn’t know them. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 92 10. I called a rape crisis center or talked to a resident counselor for help after someone told me they were sexually assaulted. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ Please read the list below and indicate the number of times you have actually engaged in each of the BEHAVIORS below over the past 4 months. Please select 0 if you did not engage in this behavior over the past 8 weeks. If you engaged in this behavior more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 11. I approached someone when I thought they were in an abusive relationship and let them know that I’m here to help. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 12. I let someone I suspected had been sexually assaulted know that I was available for help and support or shared information with them about sexual assault and violence. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 13. I confronted someone who made excuses for abusive behavior by others. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 14. I spoke up against sexist jokes. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 15. I spoke up against commercials that depicted violence against women. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 16. I spoke up when someone explained that women like to be raped. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 17. I spoke up when I heard someone say “she deserved to be raped.” 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 18. I watched my drinks and my friends’ drinks at parties. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 19. I made sure I left the party with the same people I came with. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 20. I heard a sexist comment and indicated my displeasure. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ Please read the list below and indicate the number of times you have actually engaged in each of the BEHAVIORS below over the past 4 months. Please select 0 if you did not engage in this behavior over the past 8 weeks. If you engaged in this 93 behavior more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 21. I saw a man and his girlfriend in a heated argument. The man’s fist was clenched and his partner looked upset. I asked if everything was ok. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 22. I saw a man talking to a woman at a bar. He was sitting very close to her and by the look on her face I could see she was uncomfortable. I asked her if she was ok. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 23. I encouraged people who said they had had unwanted sexual experiences to keep quiet so they didn’t get others in trouble. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 24. I knew information about an incident of sexual violence, and I told authorities what I knew in case it is helpful. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 25. I checked in with someone who looked drunk when s/he went to a room with someone else at a party. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 26. I challenged someone who used “ho,” “bitch,” or “slut” to describe girls. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 27. I confronted someone who planned to give someone alcohol to get sex. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 28. I refused to participate in activities where girls’ appearances are ranked/rated. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ Please read the list below and indicate the number of times you have actually engaged in each of the BEHAVIORS below over the past 4 months. Please select 0 if you did not engage in this behavior over the past 8 weeks. If you engaged in this behavior more than 5 times, please enter the number of times. 29. I confronted someone who was hooking up with someone who was passed out. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 30. I confronted someone when I heard rumors that s/he forced sex on someone. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 31. I reported someone who had committed a rape. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 94 32. To keep someone out of trouble, I stopped them from doing things that might meet the definition of sexual assault. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 33. I intervened when I saw someone trying to take advantage of someone else’s intoxicated state to have with them. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 34. I discouraged someone from talking about women in sexually degrading ways. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 35. I interfered with another guy’s “action” because I thought it might stop them from possibly committing a sexual assault. 0 1 2 3 4 5 ___ 95 APPENDIX M: SITUATIONAL FOLLOW-UP QUESTIONNAIRE Situational Follow-Up Questionnaire Directions: Please answer the following questions to the best of your ability. 1. Please indicate which of the following situations you were most recently in over the last 4 months: 1. I saw a man talking to a woman at a bar. He was sitting very close to her and by the look on her face I could see she was uncomfortable. 2. I saw someone’s drink get spiked with a drug. 3. I saw someone who looked drunk go to a room with someone else at a party. 4. I saw someone trying to take advantage of someone’s intoxicated state to have sex with them. 5. I saw/heard about someone who was hooking up with someone who was passed out. 6. I saw someone doing things that might meet the definition of sexual assault. 7. I was not in any of these situations in the past 4 months 2. Did you intervene or do anything to disrupt or help the situation? A. Yes B. No 3. If yes: How did you intervene (if you did not intervene, leave blank)? __________________________________________________________ 4. About how long ago did this happen in weeks? ________ 5. Where were you when you saw this happen? a) Dorm room b) Bar c) Fraternity/Sorority House d) House party e) Other _____________________________ 6. Approximately how many drinks had you had when you witnessed this event? ___________ 7. Approximately how long had you been drinking when this event occurred? ________ 8. How impaired were you from alcohol use when this event occurred? Not impaired A little Somewhat impaired impaired Moderately impaired Impaired Very impaired Extremel y 96 1 2 3 4 5 impaired 7 6 9. How drunk were you from alcohol use? Sober Buzzed Tipsy Drunk 1 2 3 4 Very Drunk 5 10. Estimate how many other people were present when this occurred. ______________ 11. Of the people who were present, how many did you consider your “close friend” were present when this event occurred? _______ 12. What was your relationship to the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? A. B. C. D. E. F. G. H. Stranger Acquaintance Friend Teammate Sorority Sister/Fraternity Brother Boyfriend/girlfriend Casual date Other ______________________ 13. How close were you with the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? Not close at all 1 Moderately Close 2 3 4 Extremely Close 5 6 7 14. What was the sex of the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? i) Male ii) Female 15. Was (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) under the influence of alcohol or drugs at the time of the event? A. Yes B. No 97 C. I don’t know 16. What was your relationship to the person you (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? A. Stranger B. Acquaintance C. Friend D. Teammate E. Sorority Sister/Fraternity Brother F. Boyfriend/girlfriend G. Casual date H. Other ___________________ 17. How close were you with the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? Not very close 1 Moderately close 2 3 4 Very close 5 6 7 18. What was the sex of the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? A. Male B. Female 19. Was the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) under the influence at the time of the event? A. Yes B. No C. I don’t know 20. What your perception of the relationship between the person who (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) and the person who (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) at the time of the event? A. Strangers B. Acquaintances C. Friends D. People on a first date E. Boyfriend and girlfriend 21. How close do you believe that these two people were? Not close Moderately close Very close 98 at all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 22. On a scale from 1 to 7 where 1 = “No imminent danger posed” and 7 = “Most imminent danger posed” how dangerous did you perceive the situation to be for (INSERT RESPONDS HERE) No imminent danger posed 1 2 3 Moderate imminent danger posed 4 5 6 Most imminent danger posed 7 23. On a scale from 1 to 7 where 1 = “Not at all risky” and 7 = “Very risky” how much physical risk (e.g., you would physically get hurt) did you perceive to be for yourself? Not at all risky 1 Moderately risky 2 3 4 Very risky 5 6 7 24. On a scale from 1 to 7 where 1 = “Not at all risky” and 7 = “Very risky” how much social risk (e.g., you would lose friends) did you perceive to be for yourself? Not at all risky 1 Moderately risky 2 3 4 Very risky 5 6 7 25. On a scale from 1 to 7 where 1 = “Not at all risky” and 7 = “Very risky” how much legal risk (e.g., get arrested) did you perceive there to be for yourself? Not at all risky 1 Moderately risky 2 3 4 Very risky 5 6 7 99 26. On a scale from 1 to 7 where 1 = “Not at all risky” and 7 = “Very risky” how much emotional risk (e.g., you would be insulted, made fun of) did you perceive there to be for yourself? Not at Moderately all risky risky 1 2 3 4 5 6 27. Had someone already intervene in the situation you noticed? A. Yes B. No Very risky 7 28. How did the (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) react to your intervention, if you intervened? (If you did not intervene, please leave blank) ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ __________________ 29. How did (INSERT RESPONSE HERE) react to your intervention? (If you did not intervene, please leave blank ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ __________________ ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! Thesis and Dissertation Services !