Download Additive Particles

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Relativistic quantum mechanics wikipedia , lookup

Theoretical and experimental justification for the Schrödinger equation wikipedia , lookup

Grand Unified Theory wikipedia , lookup

Weakly-interacting massive particles wikipedia , lookup

ALICE experiment wikipedia , lookup

Double-slit experiment wikipedia , lookup

Electron scattering wikipedia , lookup

Standard Model wikipedia , lookup

Compact Muon Solenoid wikipedia , lookup

ATLAS experiment wikipedia , lookup

Identical particles wikipedia , lookup

Elementary particle wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Christine Dimroth: Additive particles & contrastive topics
While German restrictive particles like nur (only) are nearly always unstressed and precede their
(focused) domain of application, the additive scope particle auch (also) is frequently marked by a
pitch accent and follows its domain of application as in (1).
(1) A: Du warst also auf der Party. Und Karl?
'So you went to that party. And what about Karl?'
B: [Karl] war auch da.
'Karl was also there.'
It has been assumed that auch then applies its additive meaning to the (contrastive) topic of the
utterance (cf. Dimroth, 1998 and Krifka, 1999). Contrastive topics refer to something about
which information is required and they are contrasted to some alternative topic in context. Their
relation to an alternative makes contrastive topics a suitable domain of application for additive
particles. While earlier work focused on the local conditions of topic contrast in isolated utterance pairs, the present paper investigates the impact of information structure and referential
movement on the integration of stressed additive particles in coherent discourse. It is based on
the results of an empirical study of stressed auch and other additive particles/adverbs that commonly interact with contrastive topics. The general idea is thereby that auch can be applied to all
kinds of topical information (e.g. topics that refer to persons, objects, places, times etc.) whereas
other particles like (immer)noch (still/another) or wieder (again) can only apply to (contrastive)
TopicTimes in the sense of Klein (1994), as illustrated by example (2):
(2) A: Als du kamst, hat Karl also ein Bier getrunken. Und dann?
'When you arrived, Karl was drinking a beer. And what happened then?'
B: Dann ist er spazierengegangen. Und [spaeter] hat er noch/wieder ein Bier
getrunken.
'Then he went for a walk. And later he had another beer/again a beer.'
The data stem from a discourse elicitation task which provided contexts for the use of additive
particles and contrastive topics of different types and which also allowed to control the discourse
dependent information structure as well as the local topic conditions of the relevant utterances.
The study compares the encoding of the relevant informational settings by native speakers to that
of untutored learners of L2 German at different stages of acquisition. Given their limited command of the language, L2 learners need to reduce complexity. A look at learner languages therefore offers some interresting insights in the interrelation between referential movement, information structure and the domain of application of additive particles which are equally valid but
sometimes less visible in native German.
References:
Dimroth, C. (1998). Fokuspartikeln und Informationsgliederung im Diskurs. Berlin, Dissertation
Freie Universität Berlin.
2
Klein, W. (1994). Time in Language. London etc.: Routledge.
Krifka, M. (1999). Additive Particles under Stress. SALT 8.