* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Changes I have in mind - McGraw Hill Higher Education
Reproductive health wikipedia , lookup
Sexual ethics wikipedia , lookup
Female promiscuity wikipedia , lookup
Hookup culture wikipedia , lookup
History of human sexuality wikipedia , lookup
Age of consent wikipedia , lookup
Sexual attraction wikipedia , lookup
Rochdale child sex abuse ring wikipedia , lookup
Slut-shaming wikipedia , lookup
Chapter 13 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence Learning Objectives When students have studied the material in this chapter, they will be able to answer the following: • Introduction 1. When did adolescence first emerge as a distinct period of development and how has it changed since then? 2. How are physical, cognitive, and social development interrelated during adolescence? 3. How well does the education system meet the developmental needs of adolescents? • Biological Changes During Adolescence 4. What factors influence the timing of puberty? 5. Explain the operation of the hormonal systems that control pubertal change. 6. Summarize the changes in appearance associated with puberty. 7. What neurological changes are associated with puberty? 8. Summarize the impacts of pubertal change on body image, social relationships, and problem behaviors for both early- and late-maturing boys and girls. • Changes In Thinking During Adolescence 9. What are the major cognitive advances of adolescence? 10. How did Piaget explain adolescent cognitive development? 11. What improvements in information-processing abilities occur during adolescence? 12. What impact does the social environment have on cognitive development in adolescence? • Social Cognitive Changes Of Adolescence 13. How do cognitive advances affect adolescents’ reasoning in the social domain? 14. What does research on adolescents' moral reasoning reveal? • An Overview Of Adolescent Physical And Cognitive Development 15. What cognitive limitations remain at the end of adolescence? 204 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence Chapter Summary Outline Note: Terms in bold print are chapter vocabulary words. Introduction • • • • • Adolescence, the period from roughly age 12 through the late teens, is a time of great change, with close connections between physical and psychological development. New cognitive skills emerge too, such as the ability to reason abstractly. Until the early twentieth century adolescence was not regarded as a distinct developmental period. Before then, reproductive maturity marked entry into adulthood, not into a transitional phase. G. Stanley Hall inspired much change in the way we perceive adolescence. Storm and stress was his theme. Adolescence is a longer time period than in Hall’s day, partly because of earlier puberty and partly because of the number of years required for education. Substages of adolescence: early adolescence (puberty to age 13), middle adolescence (ages 14 – 16), late adolescence (17 to early adulthood). Biological Changes During Adolescence • Puberty: Norms and Individual Differences 1. Puberty is the period during which a young person becomes capable of reproduction. The clearest markers of this change are menarche for girls, although girls do not reach reproductive maturity until they ovulate, and spermarche for boys, first ejaculation of mobile sperm. 2. Rather than a single event, puberty is best thought of as an extended period that begins during early adolescence (note: first hormonal changes can begin as early as age 7 or 8), and the latest phases can continue into mid-adolescence, when sexual organs and other sexual characteristics are developing rapidly. 3. There are sex differences in the timing of puberty. Onset is influenced by heredity, nutrition, stress, and exercise. 4. There are individual differences in the timing of puberty. Normal (= 95% of the population) menstruation begins between ages 9 and 16, and normal sperm production between 10 and 19. There has been a trend toward earlier puberty for at least the last 100 years in industrialized countries. African-American girls are more likely to show signs of early pubertal development before age 8. 5. Improved nutrition and health have influenced the changes in the timing of puberty. • Hormonal Control of Puberty 1. Puberty is the final stage in a much longer process of sexual development, beginning soon after conception when the amount of androgens determines development of male or female sex organs. At puberty, hormones again govern sexual changes. 2. The changes of puberty are governed by hormones operating in a feedback system involving the pituitary gland, hypothalamus, gonadotropins, and gonads. At the end of middle childhood the brain directs the gonads to step up production of sex hormones. 3. It was once thought that the hormonal changes of puberty were triggered by body weight (critical weight hypothesis), but the critical factor is now thought to be the proportion of fat to body mass or some other related factor. • Changes in Appearance at Puberty 205 Chapter 13 1. In addition to the development of reproductive capacities, the physical changes of puberty also include the emergence of secondary sex characteristics. There are five stages of pubertal development involving these sex characteristics. 2. Acne may develop and new body odors are noted. 3. A major growth spurt occurs, 2 years earlier for females. Growth occurs near the epiphyseal growth plates. See increases in weight, strength, and endurance. • • Neurological Changes at Puberty 1. Between childhood and adolescence the brain shows 1) decline in plasticity and 2) increase in efficiency of brain functioning. Hemispheric specialization may contribute to a loss of plasticity. Decrease in the number of synapses is likely a contributor. Between ages 10 and 14, there is a 50 % drop in the energy being used in certain parts of the cortex. Decrease in synapses allows for more efficient functioning of the remaining connections. Sleep patterns also support the idea of brain changes at puberty; deep sleep levels reach that of adult patterns by ages 11 to 14. Impacts of Pubertal Change 1. Some behavioral changes of adolescence are more clearly associated with pubertal status, the timing of maturation, than with chronological age or cognitive development. 2. Puberty and Body Image Body image is strongly affected by puberty and its timing. In early adolescence, boys who are more physically mature are more likely to have a positive body image and to perceive themselves as generally more attractive than are boys who are less physically mature. For girls early puberty tends to be associated with a poorer body image, emphasis on being too heavy, though they are happier with breast development. Among early adolescent girls, those who are about average in physical development generally have the most positive body image and the greatest feelings of attractiveness. 3. Puberty and Social Relationships Puberty is associated with increased interest in the opposite sex and an increased likelihood of dating and sexual activity. Pubertal change and the timing of puberty also affect parent-child relationships, particularly in the area of autonomy. Parent-child conflicts are greatest with early maturers. 4. Puberty and Problem Behaviors A number of studies have documented an increase in various problem behaviors at puberty, especially in early-maturing girls. They are more likely to have children earlier and to complete fewer years of education. Studies also have shown that early maturing boys are more likely than late maturing boys to smoke and drink regularly, and to experience truancy and early sexual activity. 5. Timing of Puberty and Overall Adjustment The timing of puberty has different effects for overall adjustment of boys and girls. Early-maturing girls have lower self-esteem and are more at risk for emotional problems. Late-maturing boys are less popular and less self-confident. Early-maturing females can reach puberty at age 9 and late-maturing males can reach puberty as late as age 19. 6. Direct and Indirect Effects of Puberty 206 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence Puberty produces both unseen internal and visible external physical changes in adolescents. The internal changes may directly affect adolescents' feelings and behaviors through the influences of hormones. The external changes may affect feelings and actions too, both through the impact they have on an adolescent's own body image and through the reactions they trigger in others. Changes in Thinking During Adolescence • Three major cognitive advances during adolescence are: 1. An increased ability to apply logical thinking to the possible, not just to the real (deal with necessary not just contingent truth). 2. The ability to think about relationships among mentally constructed concepts (abstract). 3. More logical and systematic thinking than in childhood. Adolescents engage in hypothetico-deductive reasoning. Can think of hypothetical solutions to a problem. Developmentalists are still trying to determine what underlies these cognitive changes. • Piaget's Theory of Formal Operations 1. Piaget explained adolescents' new thinking skills as a product of their ability to use formal operations. These cognitive structures are qualitatively different from those acquired earlier in childhood, but they result from the same developmental processes (equilibration, etc.). 2. Believed that formal operations could not be taught. • Piaget's Experiments 1. Piaget's theory of formal operations is based on studies of adolescents' reasoning on tasks related to various scientific principles. These tasks used in Piaget's experiments demonstrated adolescents' use of one or more general cognitive abilities. 2. The Law-of-Floating-Bodies Study Adolescents become able to see how two or more mentally constructed concepts can be related to produce a third even more abstract concept. Thus, in the floating-bodies task, they grasp that the ratio of weight to volume yields the concept of density. 3. The Pendulum Study The pendulum task measures the ability to investigate the effects of a single variable while holding all other factors constant. Adolescents systematically test each of four relevant factors while holding the others constant and are thus able to discover the right answer. 4. The All-Possible-Combinations Study By mid-adolescence, children adopt a systematic approach and try all possible combinations in order to discover which combination of five different colorless liquids causes a yellow color to appear. • Is Piaget's View Correct? 1. Piaget's description of adolescent thinking abilities has proven to be quite accurate, but his explanation of these abilities has been criticized. 2. How Pervasive are Formal Operations? Emergent formal operations are typical in early adolescence while consolidated formal operations are more often used in middle to late adolescence. Many adolescents and adults do not use formal operations in routine problem solving. Their everyday cognitive performance does not seem to match their cognitive competence. Formal operations are more culture-bound than earlier cognitive abilities in his theory. 207 Chapter 13 Defenders of the theory say that the lack of pervasiveness of formal operations reflects the distinction between cognitive competence (optimal ability) and congitive performance (actual behavior in a particular situation). 3. Can Formal Operations Be Taught? Contrary to Piaget's predictions, some researchers have successfully taught the use of formal operations to older elementary school children and to adolescents who were not already using them. 4. Are Formal Operations Related to Academic Performance? Use of formal operations is positively related to general intelligence and to success in school, especially in math and science. • Other Approaches to Adolescent Cognition 1. Information-Processing Explanations Research on adolescents' information-processing abilities consistently indicates continuing improvement in attention skills. Adolescents are better than younger children at both selective attention and divided attention. Adolescents also surpass younger children in the areas of short- and long-term memory. Improvements may be due to increases in memory capacity (6 items by age 9). Development of metamemory and increased sophistication in using mnemonic strategies account for much of the improvement in memory processes. Adolescents use more complex strategies, spontaneously, and become more planful and flexible in their memory strategy usage. They do show some metamemory limitations such as recognizing the most appropriate strategy. Increase in available cognitive processing capacity called automatization may also play a role. With more experience or practice, processes become less effortful. Another factor: expanding knowledge base. 2. Cognitive Socialization Cognitive socialization refers to the influence of social environment on the development of cognitive skills. The reason for much of the impact of social interaction on cognitive development is that it provides a setting for trying out ideas, responding to opposing points of view, and learning to evaluate the soundness of arguments and evidence. Adolescents do not simply invent principles of logical reasoning and effective approaches to problem solving on their own. Instead, especially during early adolescence, discussion with others, individually or in small groups, seems to foster the emergence of higher-order thinking skills. Schools play the most obvious role in adolescents’ cognitive socialization. Emphasis on test score outcomes has detracted from the development of higher-order cognitive abilities such as critical thinking. In a variety of subject areas, including science, math, and social studies, adolescents in the United States show low levels of critical thinking. It is clear that critical thinking, like formal operations, does not automatically emerge in adolescence. Adolescents may fail to think critically for a number of reasons, both cognitive and social. Developmental psychologist Daniel Keating believes that critical thinking may be fostered by parents and teachers who provide specific experiences and learning contexts for adolescents. Television, especially commercials, has discouraged critical thinking and reasoned decision-making. Social Cognitive Changes of Adolescence 208 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence • Adolescent Egocentrism 1. Adolescents' new abilities for abstract thought give rise to a new form of egocentrism that includes concern about an imaginary audience, an unjustified concern that one is the focus of others’ attention. They can think about thinking and thus can think about what others may be thinking about them. 2. Another aspect of adolescent egocentrism is the personal fable, the belief that they are unique and no one has ever had the same thoughts or feelings they are having. It is also related to feelings of invincibility to risks and physical dangers. 3. Much of this is due to their more abstract thinking about topics with which they have little experience (romantic love, sex). 4. The emergence of adolescent egocentrism may be associated with processes of identity development as well as with cognitive changes. • Moral Reasoning 1. Moral reasoning – the process of thinking and making judgments about the right course of action in a given situation. 2. Piaget's Model Piaget discussed the development of moral reasoning within his broad theory of cognitive development. His model included an amoral stage (through age 7), a stage of moral realism (middle childhood), and a stage of autonomous morality (by early adolescence). Piaget believed that moral development is a direct consequence of both cognitive development and increased social experience. 3. Kohlberg's Model Kohlberg expanded on Piaget's approach to include six stages of moral development within three broader periods: preconventional morality, conventional morality, and postconventional or principled morality. Most adolescents and adults operate at the level of conventional morality where moral judgments are based on internalized standards arising from concrete experience with the world—focuses on opinions of others or on formal laws. 4. Criticisms of Kohlberg's Model Piaget's and Kohlberg's models have both been criticized because of the weak relationship between moral reasoning and moral behavior. Kohlberg's theory has also been criticized for separating the form and content of moral reasoning, for the reliability of his measures, for bias against women, and for being too culture-specific. An Overview of Adolescent Physical and Cognitive Development • • Physical changes that occur during adolescence are a good example of qualitative change; puberty produces a transformation in a young person’s physical appearance and a reorganization of his or her functioning. These physical changes have far-reaching implications for other areas of an adolescent’s life. Cognitive abilities also undergo qualitative changes by the end of adolescence. Lecture Topics The following are lecture topic suggestions to complement Chapter 13 reading material. Topic 1: Recent Trends in Adolescent Sexual and Contraceptive Behavior 209 Chapter 13 • • • Research Questions: How have attitudes toward sexual behavior in general and adolescent sexuality in particular changed over the last 70 years? How has adolescents' sexual and contraceptive behavior changed during this time period? Major changes in adolescent sexual and contraceptive behavior occurred between 1965 and 1995, along with a major increase in both pregnancies and abortions among unmarried teenage girls. Some of those trends are discussed in Chapter 14, but the topic could also be discussed in connection with the material on physical development in Chapter 13. Chilman (1986) has summarized the results of a large number of surveys and statistical studies related to teenage sexuality and fertility. Research Literature: 1. Brooks-Gunn, J., & Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (1989). Adolescent sexual behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 249-257. 2. Chilman, C. S. (1986). Some psychosocial aspects of adolescent sexual and contraceptive behaviors in a changing American society. In J. B. Lancaster & B. A. Hamburg (Eds.), School-age pregnancy and parenthood: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 191-217). New York: Aldine De Gruyter. 3. Crockett, L. J., & Chopak, J. S. (1993). Pregnancy prevention in early adolescence: A developmental perspective. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Early adolescence: Perspectives on research, policy, and intervention (pp. 315-333). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 4. D'Augelli, A. R., & Bingham, C. R. (1993). Interventions to prevent HIV infections in young adolescents. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Early adolescence: Perspectives on research, policy, and intervention (pp. 353-368). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 5. Fennelly, K. (1993). Sexual activity and childbearing among Hispanic adolescents in the United States. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Early adolescence: Perspectives on research, policy, and intervention (pp. 335-352). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 6. Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Chase-Lansdale, L. (1989). Teenaged pregnancy and childbearing. American Psychologist, 44, 313-320. 7. Holmbeck, G. N., Crossman, R. E., Wandrei, M. L., & Gasiewski, E. (1994). Cognitive development, egocentrism, self-esteem, and adolescent contraceptive knowledge, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 23, 169-193. 8. Holden, G. W., Nelson, P. B., Velasquez, J., & Ritchie, K. L. (1993). Cognitive, psychosocial, and reported sexual-behavior differences between pregnant and nonpregnant adolescents. Adolescence, 28, 557-570. 9. Hurrelman, K. (Ed.). (1994). International handbook of adolescence. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. 10. Koch, P. B. (1993). Promoting healthy sexual development during early adolescence. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Early adolescence: Perspectives on research, policy, and intervention (pp. 293307). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 11. Moore, S. (1993). Sexuality in adolescence. New York: Routledge. 12. Nguyet, N. T. M., Maheux, B., Beland, F., & Pica, L. A. (1994). Sexual behaviors and condom use: A study of suburban male adolescents. Adolescence, 29, 37-48. • Methods and Results: 1. Attitudes toward sexual behavior. Attitudes toward sexual behavior in general and adolescent sexuality in particular have changed gradually over the last 70 years. Surveys show sharp changes in attitudes during the 1920's and again in the late 1960's, in both cases mainly in the direction of increasingly liberal attitudes toward women's sexual behavior. Double-standard attitudes toward nonmarital sexual behavior were commonly found through the early 1960's, with young women believing that petting and intercourse were unacceptable outside of a love relationship and young men taking a more permissive viewpoint. The differences between young women's and young men's attitudes began to decline in the mid-1960's and continued to do so 210 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence throughout the 1970's. In general, high school and college students' attitudes toward nonmarital petting and intercourse became more liberal through at least 1979, with the change greater for women than for men. There is some evidence that sexual permissiveness among youth may now be on the decline, however. 2. Changes in adolescent sexual behavior. One recent change in adolescent sexual behavior is a more rapid movement to intercourse, with less participation in such sexual activities as petting. Whereas petting often substituted for intercourse in the 1950's and early 1960's, at least for white teenagers, it is now less common for adolescents to have experienced petting but not intercourse. The pattern for black teenagers has been different, with less use of petting as a substitute for intercourse in the past or as an intermediate step toward intercourse in the present. In one 1983 study of junior high school students, blacks reported no kissing and petting experience prior to first intercourse—a sharply different pattern than that reported by whites. Until recently, surveys from which information about adolescent sexual behavior has been obtained have focused on black and white youth. As a consequence, information about sexual behavior among Hispanic youth has been available only since the late 1980's (Fennelly, 1993). Actual rates of intercourse among adolescents appear to have risen sharply since about 1967, particularly for white females. Between 1925 and 1965, studies found an average of 10% of white high school senior girls and 25% of white high school senior boys had experienced intercourse; by 1979, the figures had risen to 44 % for girls and 53 % for boys. Among Hispanic adolescents, 80% of boys and nearly 60% of girls report having had intercourse by age 19. Comparable figures for black teenagers are higher, with 73% of senior girls and over 90% of senior boys indicating sexual experience. The 1983 study of junior high students already mentioned yielded even higher rates of reported sexual activity, with 51% of white male ninth-graders and 91% of black male ninth-graders claiming they had had intercourse. Such extremely high rates must be regarded with caution, however, since exaggeration probably plays a prominent role in surveys on adolescent sexual activity, particularly among younger adolescents. 3. Factors associated with adolescent sexual activity. Factors that increase the likelihood of sexual activity for adolescents include sexually active friends; a permissive, risk-taking attitude toward sex; positive orientations toward deviance and independence; low grades; low expectations of achievement; low acceptance of parental controls; high social alienation; low religiosity; drug and alcohol use; poor communication with parents; coming from a single-parent family; and, for girls, a traditional sex-role orientation. Factors that decrease the likelihood of sexual activity include positive attitudes toward education, high levels of educational achievement, clear educational goals, high SES, and the reverse of most of the factors associated with sexual activity. Adolescents who experience intercourse at a very early age (12 or 13) are more likely to have serious problems of various sorts than older sexually active adolescents are. 4. Adolescent fertility rates. Although the overall rate of births to teenagers has declined in the United States over the last 20 years, three teenage reproductive rates have greatly increased during that time: (1) the pregnancy rate, (2) the abortion rate, and (3) the percentage of births occurring outside of marriage. In 1979, the fertility rate for white American women aged 14 to 19 was 221 per 1000; for blacks, it was 515 per 1000. Those rates were higher than in most other Western countries, and the rate for blacks was the highest rate for any group in 30 countries surveyed. In contrast, the rates in Scandinavian countries were under 100 per 1000; in Japan, it was only 17 per 1000. In the United States about half of those pregnancies ended in abortion. Of the adolescent pregnancies that were carried to term, 31 percent of the white births and 83 percent of the black births were to unmarried mothers. 5. Adolescent contraceptive use. One reason for the high pregnancy rate among American teenagers is their correspondingly low rate of contraceptive use. In 1979, about 45 percent of sexually active adolescent girls in one study said they had used contraception at their first intercourse, about one-third said they always used contraception when they had intercourse, and about one-third said they had never used contraception. Although the percentage of contraceptive users had increased between 1976 and 1979, the use of effective methods had declined, with a shift away from birth control pills and toward such methods as withdrawal. About 211 Chapter 13 40 percent of the girls in the study reported that they depended on male methods of contraception, but the boys in the study expressed little concern about contraception. Because younger adolescents are typically not consistent or effective contraceptive users, they may be at an especially high risk for unwanted pregnancy. Indeed, whereas pregnancy rates have declined for most age groups in recent years, the pregnancy rate among girls under age 15 has shown a 23 percent increase (Crockett & Chopak, 1993). 6. Factors associated with adolescent contraceptive use. Factors associated with failure to use effective contraception include: youth, being single, low SES, minority group membership, fewer years of education, not being in a steady relationship, not having experienced a pregnancy, having intercourse sporadically, not having contraceptives when they were needed, not having access to free and confidential family planning services, lack of communication with parents about contraception, desire for pregnancy, ignorance of pregnancy risks, attitudes of fatalism and powerlessness, traditional female role attitudes, low self-esteem, lack of acceptance of one's own sexual behavior, disturbed family situation, risk-taking, pleasure-oriented attitudes, fear of side effects, incorrect assumptions about fertility, immature levels of cognitive development, poor problem-solving skills, lack of planfulness, and presence of siblings and friends who have babies. • Discussion Questions: How well do the statistics presented here match your sense of what went on in your own high school? What do you see as the major problem—the level of teenage sexual activity or the level of teenage pregnancy? What could be done to deal with that problem? Topic 2: Alternative Explanations for Adolescent Egocentrism • • • Research Questions: What is the phenomenon of adolescent egocentrism? How prevalent is it and what causes it? When Elkind (1967) originated the concept of adolescent egocentrism, he tied its appearance to the emergence of formal operations. In Elkind's formulation, the emergence of formal operational thought allowed adolescents to conceptualize both their own thought and the thought of others, resulting in the phenomena Elkind labeled the imaginary audience and the personal fable. However, there are problems with Elkind's characterization of adolescent egocentrism, and recent researchers have begun to look more closely at both the incidence of these phenomena and the developmental factors associated with them. Research Literature: 1. Blasi, A., & Hoeffel, E. C. (1974). Adolescence and formal operations. Human Development, 17, 344-363. 2. Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 1025-1034. 3. Lapsley, D. K. (1990). Continuity and discontinuity in adolescent social cognitive development. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), From childhood to adolescence: A transitional period? Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 4. O'Connor, B. P., & Nikolic, J. (1990). Identity development and formal operations as sources of adolescent egocentrism. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 149-158. • Methods and Results: One problem with Elkind's account of adolescent egocentrism is that, as discussed in the textbook, most adolescents do not use formal operational reasoning to any great extent. Thus, if this type of egocentrism is indeed typical of early adolescence, it must be based on factors other than formal operational thought. In fact, some researchers (e.g., Blasi & Hoeffel, 1974) have argued that formal operations are not well suited to the kinds of reasoning about self, others, and relationships that seem typical of adolescent social cognition. Blasi and Hoeffel, among others, suggest that adolescent social reasoning may be explained strictly on the basis of concrete operational thought. 1. Researchers who have tested subjects on both formal operational reasoning and adolescent egocentrism have not typically found a connection between the two (Lapsley, 1990). Some have 212 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence found that scores on imaginary audience and personal fable measures are the highest for concrete operational subjects and that the appearance of formal operational thought actually diminishes concern with the imaginary audience. Others have found no correlation at all between measures of formal operations and of adolescent egocentrism. 2. The findings on age trends in adolescent egocentrism have also been inconsistent (Lapsley, 1990), though they must be interpreted with caution since there have been no longitudinal studies. Some researchers have reported peaks in adolescent egocentrism in eighth or ninth grade, as might be expected from Elkind's original proposal, while others have reported increases through age 18, decreases from fifth or sixth grade through college, or a complete absence of age effects. Overall, concerns with the imaginary audience and personal fable seem to be present during adolescence more often than not, but the developmental course of these concerns is far from clear. 3. Currently, researchers are examining links between adolescent egocentrism and various other aspects of social cognitive development. As mentioned in the textbook, O'Connor and Nikolic (1990) have found a connection between processes of identity formation and egocentrism in high school and college students. Other researchers have argued for connections to Selman's stages of interpersonal understanding and to the separation-individuation process of adolescent ego development (Lapsley, 1990). • Discussion Questions: Would cross-cultural research shed light on the origins of adolescent egocentrism and possible links to cognitive development, especially formal operations? Suggest a study that would investigate these issues cross-culturally and explain what information it might yield. Topic 3: Antecedents of Menarcheal Age • • • Research Question: How do environmental and psychological stressors interact with heredity to influence the development of the reproductive system, specifically the timing of menarche? Studies of menarche have shown that the timing of menarche has psychological significance for girls, their families, and friends (e.g., Petersen, Sarigiani, & Kennedy, 1991). The timing of menarche is influenced to a great extent by heredity, but studies of girls engaged in intensive physical training (e.g., ballet, gymnastics) have shown that environmental factors can override genetic links (e.g., Brooks-Gunn & Warren, 1988). Recent research has investigated the influence on the timing of menarche of a broad range of environmental stressors, including the family environment. Research Literature: 1. Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647670. 2. Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1988). Mother-daughter differences in menarcheal age in adolescent dancers and nondancers. Annals of Human Biology, 15, 35-43. 3. Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1995). The antecedents of menarcheal age: Heredity, family environment, and stressful life events. Child Development, 66, 346-359. 4. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Belsky, J., & Silva, P. A. (1992). Childhood experience and the onset of menarche: A test of a sociobiological model. Child Development, 63, 47-58. 5. Petersen, A. C., Sarigiani, P. A., & Kennedy, R. E. (1991). Adolescent depression: Why more girls? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 247-271. 6. Steinberg, L. (1988). Reciprocal relation between parent-child distance and pubertal maturation. Developmental Psychology, 24, 122-128. 213 Chapter 13 7. Steinberg, L. (1989). Pubertal maturation and parent-adolescent distance: An evolutionary perspective. In G. R. Adams, R. Montemayor, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Biology of adolescent behavior and development (pp. 71-97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 8. Surbey, M. K. (1990). Family composition, stress, and the timing of human menarche. In T. E. Zigler & F. B. Bercovitch (Eds.), Socioendocrinology of primate reproduction (pp. 11-32). NY: Wiley. • Methods and Results: 1. Surbey (1990) investigated the effects of family structure and life stress on the timing of menarche. Using retrospective reports, she found that young women who experienced father absence prepubertally reported earlier ages of menarche than those women from intact homes. She also found that women who reported more stressful life events had experienced menarche at an earlier age than women who had less stressful lives. 2. Steinberg (1989) speculated that individuals growing up in homes characterized by conflict and instability are more likely to experience the onset of pubertal development earlier than individuals who live in more optimal family environments. Indeed, Steinberg (1988) reported accelerating effects of mother-daughter distance on the rate of pubertal development. No accelerating effects were found for boys or for father-daughter distance. 3. Graber, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren (1995) investigated a number of possible antecedents of menarche in girls between the ages of 10 and 14 years, including: hereditary transmission, weight and weight for height, stressful life events, family relations, absence or presence of an adult male in the household, and psychological adjustment. • Girls in this study were part of a larger longitudinal study and were premenarcheal at the first time of testing. At the outset of the study and annually thereafter the girls completed questionnaires about their psychosocial functioning and development; yearly physical examinations measured the girls' pubertal and physical development. The questionnaires the girls completed each year measured stressful life events, family environment and relations, and depressive affect. In their analysis, Graber et al. explored the relation between psychosocial functioning at the outset of the study—when all of the girls were premenarcheal—and age at menarche. Results showed that age at menarche was predicted by breast development and family relations but not by weight for height, presence of an adult male in the household, or stressful events. A trend for maternal age at menarche to predict adolescent's age at menarche was found. Depressive affect was moderately predictive of the age at menarche. Additional analysis revealed that girls with positive family relations and low depressive affect had the latest age of menarche (M = 13.72), whereas girls with poor family relations and high depressive affect had the earliest ages of menarche (M = 12.66). Intermediate ages at menarche were found for girls with positive family relations and high depressive affect (M = 13.56) and for girls with poor family relations and low depressive affect (M = 13.36). These findings are consistent with previous studies in suggesting that psychosocial factors, particularly family relations, are associated with subsequent age at menarche. Although the mechanisms that link family relations to pubertal development have yet to be identified, Graber et al. speculate that "multiple hormonal pathways may be influenced by internal and external environments" (p. 357). Discussion Questions: Belsky, Steinberg, and Draper (1991) have proposed an evolutionary account of links between psychosocial stress and pubertal development. How might such an account explain Graber et al.'s findings? Are there other theoretical perspectives that seem to be useful in understanding the interaction of heredity and the environment in pubertal development? 214 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence Classroom Discussion Topics and Activities 1. Adolescent Writing Samples: In order to fully appreciate changes from concrete operations to formal operations, it is nice for students to visit http://www.teenink.com to see poems, fiction, and nonfiction written by adolescents, ages 13 and up. Have students comment on themes and sophistication of content, and have them analyze the samples from a Piagetian perspective. Then, have students compare the poems read on Teen Ink with those submitted by children (mostly ages 7 – 12) to the Poetry Zone, http://www.poetryzone.ndirect.co.uk/poems.htm. Compare and contrast the entries from a Piagetian perspective. It would be fun to bring some samples to class to read aloud, from early middle childhood, late middle childhood, early adolescence through to late adolescence. Do students have their own poems saved from childhood and adolescence to share? 2. Motivating Adolescents: A practical concern of many parents and teachers is how to motivate adolescents. This activity involves students suggesting ways to motivate adolescents based on their own experiences and the course material on adolescent development. Students should list two ways to motivate adolescents. Then students can be divided into groups and asked to discuss their ways to motivate adolescents with one another. It is important to speak about external versus internal motivational issues. Also, points about what parents can do to motivate adolescents should be addressed. The same should be discussed about what roles educators can play in motivating adolescents. There should be suggestions specific to homework, housework, personal hygiene, and athletics. Have students talk about what is likely to not work well (e.g., monetary reinforcements) and why. What worked best for them? Why? 3. Adolescents and Money: Students can form small groups and discuss adolescents and money management. The class can then reconvene as a whole and develop a list of suggestions for helping adolescents deal with the challenges of money management. Course material on independent thinking and hypothetical-deductive reasoning can be integrated into the discussion. Topics of interest are: developing a budget, saving money to meet goals, saving and investing, earning money, taxes and inflation (examine pay stubs), use/abuse of credit, buying a car, maintaining a car, and college. 4. Stereotype Activity: Start off by asking your students how many would like to return to adolescence— without what they know now. Very few usually raise their hands. Make note of that. Then, have your students free associate to the word adolescence or teenager. Write down their responses on the board or overhead projector. Point out the themes that are apparent—many students will focus on the difficulties that we perceive to be inherent to the age period (the storm and stress issues of attitude, hormonal influences, drugs, peer pressure). Some positive factors will be listed (getting to drive, having spending money from a part-time job, figuring out a place to be in the world), but these will not be the major emphasis of their free associations. Very little will be mentioned regarding changes in cognitive functioning, if anything, which is good to point out as it pertains to Chapter 13. Have them consider the influences of negative stereotypes on how they perceive adolescents. Ask them whether they avoid being around “them” or roll their eyes when they see “them.” Ask whether there are students in the class who really like adolescents. Have them comment on why. Reinforce the positive points they bring up. One of the tasks of a course on development is to give students a sense of the tasks for each age period. The better we understand what is really supposed to be going on and how the behaviors we see fit these changes, the more tolerant we are of individuals in that age period. Explain that a goal of exploring the next two chapters is to better appreciate the interesting and wonderful changes going on during adolescence. 5. Body Image— Magazines and TV Shows for Adolescents: • • Have your students look through magazines (or e-zines for adolescents) that are targeted for an adolescent market. What is the sense they get about the messages being sent to adolescents? Have them explore the images within the pages for a discussion of body image in America. Have students comment on television shows that are directed to adolescents. What are the messages they see being directed to the viewers? What about body image—how realistic are the 215 Chapter 13 bodies one sees on the shows that we are “all supposed to look like to be happy.” There are some facts and figures on body-image-related topics at http://www.focusas.com/BodyImage.html. • Have students discuss whether the “new, female athlete” is a more realistic body type than the runway model look that has been so popular in Hollywood. Do they foresee any changes on the horizon in terms of what adolescents and young adults will accept as “the” body type to have? Are strong, fit bodies the wave of the future? 6. Memory Lane: • • • Have students consider examples from everyday life during adolescence of the capacity for hypothetical-deductive reasoning. It is more than being good at Twenty Questions and figuring out scientifically-based problems. It is the ability to persuade parents to let you have your own room, new wardrobe, etc. It also is the ability to figure out how to get around “the system.” How many used their burgeoning formal operational skills to attend a concert or a party after the parents said “no”? How did they try to avoid getting caught? It is an intense game of planning and hypothetically imagining roadblocks to overcome. Have students consider the moral-ethical issues that were important to them (homelessness, rainforest destruction, war crimes) during adolescence. How does moral thinking relate to formal operational thought processes? While they are thinking back, have them generate examples of their own (or perhaps some adolescent siblings in their homes) adolescent egocentrism. Do they remember the imaginary audience? How about the personal fable—“it can’t happen to me” issues? 7. Class Discussion—The body project: Discuss issues addressed in J. Brumberg’s (1997) book The Body Project, an historical look at society’s changing conceptualizations of femininity. Engage the class in a discussion of the change in ways we have treated the domain of women’s and girls’ bodies. The author states that the more women have been allowed to show parts of their bodies publicly, the less liberating the experience has actually been for women. Worrying about showing faces, necks, and hands is different from the worry about a body’s attributes in a bikini. The more we can show, the more we have to control the appearance of finer and finer details of our bodies. How has this more public body orientation impacted the focus of adolescent females on their bodies? 8. Observational Task: Have your students make informal observations of adolescents at their place of employment, at the mall, at a restaurant, etc. Have them casually note the estimated ages of these adolescents on the basis of their physical development (e.g., height, weight, voice, evidence of secondary sex characteristics). Can they note the variability in pubertal timing from these casual observations? Are there any obvious outcomes from differences in pubertal timing—for instance, on a playing field at school, are the early maturing males the ones in the team leadership roles? At the mall, are the early maturing females more likely to be with a date or to be concerned with how males attend to them? Have them make informal comments to class about their observations. 9. Four Adolescents: From the vignettes about Malcolm, Mikey, Maggie, and Meryl at the beginning of the adolescence section of the text, have your students note evidence of formal operational thinking. Have them write down the examples before coming to class for the lecture material on cognition in adolescence. Also, look for examples of adolescent egocentrism. Are there examples of other cognitive functioning demonstrated in the vignettes? Discuss these. 10. Preparation for Puberty: Have students comment on how prepared they were for the more notable announcements of the onset of puberty—menarche and first nocturnal emission. Did preparation for or lack of preparation for puberty have an impact on their experiences of puberty and/or on their relationships with their family? Were there any celebrations of your transition into adolescence? Do they remember becoming more preoccupied with their bodies? Did they feel more private about their bodies? Was this also about the time that they kept a journal (diary)—if indeed they ever did so? 11. Memory Lane – Pubertal Timing: Have students rate (for themselves) their own passage through puberty as early, average, or late compared to their own peer group. Then discuss as a class the 216 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence relative advantages and disadvantages of early, average, and late maturation for each sex. Encourage students to come up with as many specific examples as possible from their own experience or the experiences of people they knew in adolescence. Along with this discussion, have your students reflect on the popular males in 7th, 8th, or 9th grade class. What attributes did these boys have? What about the unpopular boys (geeks)? Likely, the popular boys were early maturers while the unpopular were late maturers. What social advantages are afforded early maturing males? Does the same hold for early maturing females? Have students comment on their beliefs about the impact of maturation schedules on adolescent experience. Do these effects carry over into adulthood at all? 12. Writing Activity: One way to help students check their understanding of the physical changes that occur at puberty is to assign them to write letters explaining these changes to imaginary children about to enter adolescence. Divide the class into mixed-sex groups and ask each group to produce one letter to a boy and one to a girl, focusing on the changes members of each sex can expect in their own bodies and in the feelings that accompany the physical changes. [Adapted from J. R. Charlesworth, Jr., & J. R. Slate (1986). Teaching about puberty: Learning to talk about sensitive topics. Teaching of Psychology, 13, 215-217.] 13. Formal Reasoning in High School: Does the current type of education offered in American high schools foster the development of formal reasoning skills in students? What changes could be made in American high school education to make it more likely that students will develop such skills? Films, Videos, and Internet Resources Films and Videos: • • • • • • Adolescent Cognition: Thinking In A New Key (33 min., Davidson Films). With David Elkind. Exploration of the many changes in cognition in adolescence. Adolescent Development (1990, 30 min., Insight Media). Covers diverse aspects of physical, social, and psychological development. Discusses developmental tasks, puberty, self-image, Elkind on social cognition, formal operations, and Kohlberg’s theory of moral development. American Adolescence (1999, 30 minutes, Films for the Humanities and Sciences). A rising divorce rate, frequent relocation, and a fascination with TV, video games, and computers are leaving more and more teenagers under-supervised, rootless, and isolated. Increasingly detached from traditional values and lifestyles, these young adults face daunting obstacles without the support base enjoyed by previous generations. This program investigates the hurdles faced by today’s teens and discusses how their hopes, fears, and expectations will shape American society. Experts include top officers of the Families and Work Institute, National Teen Pregnancy Prevention Research Center, and National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse Control. High School Stories: One Day in America's Schools (1993, 60 min., Films for the Humanities and Sciences). In this documentary, produced by Wisconsin Public Television, public television stations in seven different locations across America document a day in the life of high school seniors. Explores how classroom experiences help young people form their sense of who they are and what they want to become. The House of Tomorrow (1991, 57 min., Ambrose Video). Childhood Series (video 7). Features the ways various cultures celebrate the passage from childhood to adolescence. Includes boys from Baka and a young girl in Brazil. The Independent Thinker (2002, 47 min., Films for the Humanities and Sciences). It takes over 12 years for the average child to acquire all the skills needed to be an independent adult—from solving algebra equations to behaving properly in society. This program presents the story of how we learn to think for ourselves, beginning with the development of recall and memory, then the acquisition of language, and finally the mastering of social skills—learning to be an adult by playing at it. In various 217 Chapter 13 cognitive experiments with an array of children, milestones such as abstract reasoning and spatial memory are charmingly highlighted. A Discovery Channel Production. • Misunderstood Minds (2002, 1 hr. 30 min., PBS). Follow the compelling personal stories of five children struggling with learning differences and their families in this remarkable 90-minute documentary. As parents, educators, clinicians, and other professionals help the kids better understand their learning profiles, they gain strategies and solutions that lead toward success both in and out of the classroom. • • • • Moral Development (1997, 28 min., Insight Media). This video explains the concept of morality and defines such terms as moral code, moral judgment, and moral intelligence. It presents key values that are fundamental to most moral codes and examines the principal theories of moral development, including psychoanalytic, sociobiological, social learning, and cognitive learning. The Secret Life of the Brain, The Teenage Brain (2002, 30 min., PBS). This NOVA program explores the latest knowledge we have about the underlying impacts of brain development and changes during the adolescent years. Shortchanging Girls, Shortchanging America (1992, 19 min., Insight Media). This program interviews educators and business leaders to illuminate the devastating effects of gender bias in schools. It calls upon educators to encourage girls to develop math and science skills. Student-Directed Learning in the Alpha Program: A Middle School Approach That Works (1993, 44 min.). Documents the Alpha Program in a multiage junior high school classroom in Shelburne, Vermont, based on collaboration and peer mentoring. Students determine their own learning goals, either as individuals, small groups, or in whole class meetings, while teachers act as facilitators. Teenage Mind and Body (Program 22) (1992, 30 min., Insight Media). Time to Grow Series - Focuses on psychological and cognitive development in adolescence. Experts and teens themselves discuss differences between teens' abilities and interests and parents' expectations, as well as moral development and psychological changes which occur in the teenage years. A-V Resource List Information: • List of providers for most of the videos listed above: 1. Ambrose Video at www.ambrosevideo.com or 800-526-4663 2. Davidson Films at www.davidsonfilms.com or 888-437-4200. 3. Films for the Humanities and Sciences at www.films.com or 800-257-5126. 4. Insight Media at www.insight-media.com or 212-721-6316. 5. Public Broadcasting Service at 1-800-949-8670 or www.shop.pbs.org 6. Yale University Films at 203-432-0148. Additional Internet Resource Options for Chapter 13: http://www.mhhe.com/dehart5 A variety of teaching tools for this textbook are available from the Web site for McGraw-Hill. http://www.pbs.org/wnet/brain/episode3/index.html Information about the adolescent brain, corresponding with the NOVA program on “The Secret Life of the Brain” (order from PBS.org). http://www.getty.edu/artsednet/Exhibitions/Eisner/ Cognition and Creation: An Educationally Interpretive Exhibition of Student Art. Take a stroll through this virtual gallery of student art and discover the links between student art and the stages of cognitive 218 Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence development, the value of a student exhibition as an advocacy tool to strengthen the case for arts education, innovative ways to organize and display your students' artworks. http://www.apa.org/pubinfo/school/page6.html Adolescents’ choices related to schooling and work occur within the context of their cognitive, social, and vocational development. As part of our examination of the school-to-work transition or any school-based preparation for that transition, we included consideration of the developmental changes in basic processes during adolescence. These include cognitive development, social cognition, and decision making. We also have included a discussion of theoretical frameworks of adolescent vocational development. http://www.findarticles.com/cf_0/m2294/1999_Oct/59426460/p1/article.jhtml?term=%2BMass+%2B media+%2Band+%2Bchildren+%2BResearch Gender stereotyping in televised media sport coverage. From Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, October, 1999. http://www.findarticles.com/cf_0/m2294/2001_August/82782447/p1/article.jhtml?term=%2BMass+% 2Bmedia+%2Band+%2Bchildren+%2BResearch He's a Laker; She's a "Looker": The consequences of gender-stereotypical portrayals of male and female athletes by the print media. http://www.findarticles.com/cf_0/m2250/4_40/73278366/p1/article.jhtml The primary effects of media exposure are increased violent and aggressive behavior, increased high-risk behaviors, including alcohol and tobacco use, and accelerated onset of sexual activity. The newer forms of media have not been adequately studied, but concern is warranted through the logical extension of earlier research on other media forms and the amount of time the average child spends with increasingly sophisticated media. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 2001, 40(4): 392-401. http://www.myfuture.com/ Information about college, military, and other life opportunities for adolescents and young adults. http://journals.cec.sped.org/EC/Articles/Article%203.pdf Critical Social Skills for Adolescents With High Incidence Disabilities: Parental Perspectives. This qualitative research explored parental views about critical social skills for adolescents with high-incidence disabilities. Parents in this study shared their beliefs that emotional intelligence and character play critical roles in the social and emotional development of their children. Findings indicate that although parents agree that academic performance is important, they want their children to develop skills in two major areas: (a) interpersonal and intrapersonal skills, which include skills such as communicating, listening, interpreting, and discerning; and (b) moral development, which includes areas of character, empathy, and perseverance/motivation. This article is only available only to members of the Council for Exceptional Children. http://www.anred.com/toc.html This site gives statistics, definitions, and descriptions of eating disorders, including obesity. Links to additional sources of information are also available. http://www.parentsplace.com/fertility/adoptioncentral/articles/0,,166271_253105,00.html What Children Understand About Adoption at Different Ages 219