Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Adamopoulos 1 Robin Adamopoulos ENGL 539: Second Language Acquisition Dr. Vila Spring, 2004 Personality and Second Language Acquisition Introduction Language teachers have always had an intuitive sense that some language learners will achieve acquisition of the second language faster and easier than others. What accounts for these individual differences in language learning? Research has shown that several factors contribute to achievement in second language learning, including: language aptitude or the ability to learn languages; cognitive style and memory; and affective factors, such as attitude and motivation. This paper will examine some of the affective factors believed to contribute to learners’ success or failure in acquiring a second language, specifically, the effects of personality. Teachers can not do much to control the learner’s language aptitude; however, research by Gardner and Lambert (MacIntyre & Charos, 1996) begun in the 1950’s, has shown that the teacher can have a great deal of influence over the affective factors, attitude and motivation. The student’s attitude towards the teacher and attitude towards the language directly impact success in acquiring the target language, according to Gardner and Lambert (MacIntyre & Charos, 1996). Gardner and Lambert studied Canadian students in French immersion programs and found that the more successful, higher achieving students had a positive attitude towards the learning situation. Gardner and Lambert’s studies went on to examine the impact of motivation, or the drive to learn a second language, on the acquisition of the target language. These Adamopoulos 2 researchers are responsible for identifying two types of motivation in second language learning: integrative and instrumental. Integrative motivation is defined as the drive or desire to be a part of the ethnic group or culture of the target language. Instrumental motivation comes from external factors such as economics – to learn the language to succeed in a job, or academics– to gain college admission upon successful completion of a foreign language course, for example. At first, Gardner and Lambert (MacIntyre & Charos, 1996) posited that integrative motivation was a more powerful factor in second language achievement than instrumental motivation. However, recent research has found that the two exist on a continuum; the extent of the impact of each type of motivation on the learner’s acquisition of the target language depends upon context – where the learner is situated. Instrumental motivation can be just as influential for students considering emigration to the country where the language is spoken in order to seek better employment and economic conditions. Motivational factors as well as attitudes towards the learning situation have a direct correlation to success in second language acquisition which has been established through various research studies since Gardner and Lambert. But, there are many factors which contribute to an individual learner’s motivation or desire to learn and his or her attitude towards the learning situation. Personality must have an underlying part to play in one’s motivation and attitude in learning a second language. Several personality traits have been studied in relation to the impact of these traits on learning success in a second language classroom. Anxiety is one personality factor which has a strong correlation to achievement in L2 learning (MacIntyre & Charos, Adamopoulos 3 1996): “…high levels of motivation are likely to abate anxiety, and high levels of anxiety are likely to inhibit motivation” (p. 6). Highly anxious learners are less likely to communicate, according to a study done by Horwitz in 1986 (Gardner & MacIntyre, 1993). This “communicative apprehension” leads to fewer opportunities for interaction with native speakers and less language input. Learners will not acquire the language as easily, if they experience too much anxiety. The connection between anxiety and second language acquisition has been firmly established by further studies. Other studies of anxiety have confirmed that those individuals who are more inclined to take risks and who have high levels of selfconfidence are more likely to be successful in L2 acquisition (Gardner & Clément, 1990). However, the question remains as to how different personality types react to anxiety and the impact of global personality traits on second language learning. Two assumptions follow, based on the available research on attitude, motivation and anxiety: Outgoing, extroverted, risk-taking, impulsive individuals who possess an ability to identify with the needs of others and a tolerance for ambiguity will acquire a second language more easily in a natural communicative setting than those who do not possess these inherent personality traits. Reticent, thoughtful, serious and analytic individuals with a tendency towards self-involvement will succeed at L2 acquisition at higher rates in a formal classroom environment than those who do not possess these personality traits. Discussion A survey of the literature found three personality factors which have been identified for the study of the relationship between these variables and successful second Adamopoulos 4 language acquisition: extroversion-introversion; field independence-field dependence; and empathy. All three of these personality traits will be examined in light of the research. The first to be discussed is extroversion versus introversion and the impact of these traits on second language acquisition. Extroversion versus Introversion Extroverts are individuals who seek out social situations. Extroverted personality types get energy from other people and are usually the ones who never want to leave the party. Introverts, on the other hand, need time alone and prefer individual activities or gatherings with a few intimate friends. Introverted personality types are known to be more cautious, conservative and serious than extroverts, who are more impulsive and prefer to take more risks. From the research on attitude, motivation and anxiety, it would seem that extroverts, who are more sociable and outgoing, would be more successful at second language learning. However, studies have shown that introversion or extroversion do not have a significant impact on second language learning in the classroom. Naiman, Frohlich and Stern (Gardner & Clément, 1990) determined that both types of learners had equal opportunities for achievement, and that language teachers should address the needs of both personality types. However, Wakamoto (2000) studied 254 Japanese students learning English as a Foreign Language and found that extroversion did have a connection to the learning strategies employed by language learners. Wakamoto surveyed junior college students in an English language course for their learning preferences and matched these results with personality types. The study found that extroverts used more “functional strategies” and Adamopoulos 5 “social-affective strategies” in language learning than introverts (p. 73). Extroverts tended to focus on meaning rather than form – a strategy which has been confirmed as contributing to success in L2 acquisition (Brown, 2001). Extroverts asked more questions than their introverted counterparts. Wakamoto (2000) concluded that extroverts will ask for clarification more readily than introverts, thus improving their chances for input essential for developing an interlanguage. Furnham (1990) quotes a study by Thorne in 1987 which examined the interaction between introverts and extroverts. The study mixed and matched partners of extroverts and introverts in conversational situations (p. 77). When introverts were paired with introverts, the conversation they engaged in focused on “problem talk,” according to Thorne (p. 77). On the other hand, “extroverts with extroverts showed a wide range of topics and more claims of common ground,” (p. 77). Furnham concludes from this study and subsequent observations that extroverts talk more, are more impulsive and take more risks with speech than introverts. Introverts are more careful with speech and more focused on form: vocabulary, grammar and pronunciation. (See Table: Possible Speech Variations According to Personality Traits, Appendix A.) Recent studies have proposed a different construct which is directly related to extroversion versus introversion. MacIntyre (1996) proposes that there is a “Willingness to Communicate” factor in L2 acquisition which is connected to communication apprehension or anxiety in L2 contexts as suggested by Horwitz et. al. “The global trait of introversion contributes to both communication apprehension and the perception of communicative competence, and self-esteem was found to play a role in developing communication apprehension” (MacIntyre & Charos, 1996, p. 7). In other words, Adamopoulos 6 introverts are less likely to talk and have a lower self-confidence in their second language. (See Appendix B: MacIntyre’s Willingness to Communicate Model) Self-confidence has been linked to success in second language acquisition in natural settings. According to a study by Clément and Kruidenier (Gardner & Clément, 1990), self-confidence is one cause of motivation in acquiring a second language. Clément and his colleagues defined self-confidence as a “combination of low levels of language-specific anxiety, confidence in one’s language skills, and self-perceptions of high levels of proficiency,” (p. 503). From these studies, it would seem that an extrovert with an outgoing personality and more tolerance for risk would be a better language learner than the more introverted personality who is more conservative and more self-conscious. According to Dulay, Burt and Krashen (1982), “In nearly all the studies conducted to determine the personality characteristics associated with successful L2 learning, researchers have concluded that lower anxiety levels and a tendency to be outgoing were connected with successful L2 acquisition” (p. 75). However, as stated earlier, some researchers have not found a significant difference between extroverts and introverts in terms of achievement in second language acquisition in classroom environments. In fact, a more introverted personality may be better suited to classroom learning. MacIntyre and Charos (1996) write, “…for academic achievement in general, introversion is usually the more desirable end of the trait dimension. But for language learning, the desirable end may be either extroversion or introversion, depending on the learning context and instructional methods” (p. 9). Adamopoulos 7 Research has yet to be able to identify a direct relationship between introversion or extroversion and success in second language acquisition. What researchers do know is that introverts seem to prefer learning tasks which involve memorization of vocabulary and study of grammatical rules and structures, while the communicative approach with an emphasis on social interaction may be more beneficial for extroverts. Field Independence-Field Dependence The next personality factor, field independence versus field dependence has generated as much controversy as extroversion versus introversion in studies of second language achievement. Some would argue that field independence-field dependence should be considered a factor of cognitive style or learning preference more than a personality trait. However, for the purpose of this discussion, this factor has been studied as a personality attribute with an effect on personal preferences and learning styles. Field independent individuals have been identified as having more of an analytical personality. These individuals will see the “part before the whole” in terms of geo-spatial tasks, and this assumption has been applied to the individual’s approach to problem solving. Field independent persons will be more focused on discrete parts of language rather than chunks of conversation, for example. On the other end of the spectrum, field dependent individuals see the “whole before the parts.” These individuals will fix a picture frame to match the lines of the rest of a room. Field dependent persons are thought to be more creative, more inclined to participate in group activities and to take a global approach to problem solving. Early research into the relationship of field independence/field dependence to L2 acquisition concluded that field independence was a more desirable trait for classroom Adamopoulos 8 language learning. According to Dulay, Burt and Krashen (1982), “More analytical, field independent characteristics appear to be related to the acquisition of metalinguistic skills through conscious learning, while the field dependent person seems to be more apt to acquire communication skills through subconscious learning,” (p. 77). Krashen (1981) seems to agree that the more analytical personality trait of field independence contributes to the effective use of the Monitor. Field independent persons are able to “tune” their senses to the discrete parts of language: vocabulary, grammatical rules and phonetic awareness necessary for conscious language learning. Field dependent individuals would not rely as much on the Monitor, but use subconscious processes which are more desirable for language acquisition in communicative situations, according to Krashen. Lalonde and Gardner (1984) researched two personality constructs related to field independence: analytic orientation and seriousness. They posited that the more serious language learner would have “heightened levels of motivation and a better attitude toward language learning” (p. 226). Analytical personalities would be more motivated and more responsible according to their hypothesis. They refer to Krashen’s hypothesis that an individual with an analytic orientation would be “open to new ideas, flexible, [and] broad minded” (Lalonde & Gardner, 1984, p. 226). However, Gayle (1981) found that an orientation towards field dependence was strongly correlated with integrative motivation, thought to be more valuable to second language acquisition. Gayle suggests that according to Witkin’s theory, the field dependent person is more interested in fitting in with a certain group and possesses more of a tolerance for ambiguity (p. 58). The field dependent person will seek out “external Adamopoulos 9 sources” to “eliminate existing ambiguities” or would be more likely to interact with native language speakers to get a “feel” for the language, according to Gayle (p. 58). Gayle further hypothesized that the field dependent person would be more capable of empathy or identification with others outside of his/her own group. “In addition, the field dependent person has been described as warm, affectionate, tactful, accommodating, non-evaluative, and more socially outgoing and accepting of others than the extremely field independent person” (p. 58). This identification with others strongly correlates with integrative motivation as described by Gardner: the drive or desire to become a part of the target language speaker’s culture. The data gathered from Gayle’s study of 414 university students of French in Canada confirmed her hypothesis. The more field dependent students scored higher on a survey of integrative orientation. Gayle concluded from her study that field dependent individuals are more likely to seek out communicative situations than field independent learners, contributing to success in natural acquisition of a second language (p. 66). Field independence/field dependence, like extroversion and introversion are seemingly connected to motivation in acquiring a second language. Field independent persons are more likely to benefit from traditional, classroom instruction with a focus on the discrete parts of language, while field dependent learners would benefit from a communicative approach. As Naiman and others have hypothesized, both personality types are capable of language learning and teachers must adjust instruction to meet the needs of both. Adamopoulos 10 Empathy The last personality trait examined in this review of the literature is by far the most elusive and most controversial in terms of its relationship to second language acquisition. Researchers still have not confirmed a direct relationship between the quality of empathy, or the ability “to put oneself in someone else’s shoes” (Raymond, 1982, p. 47) and achievement in learning a second language. Krashen (1981) suggests that there is not an adequate measure of the quality of empathy to allow for proper research of this factor’s influence on the acquisition of an L2. Guiora (Raymond, 1982) led the studies of empathy’s relationship to L2 acquisition in terms of pronunciation in the early 1970’s. He hypothesized that more empathetic individuals would be better listeners and better attuned to the native features of speech; therefore, empathetic individuals would acquire a more native-like proficiency in pronunciation. He set out to study this phenomenon in 1972 by using a Micromomentary Expressions Test or MME to measure the quality of empathy. This test showed a film of the head and shoulders of a woman conversing. Subjects were asked to identify changes in the various facial expressions of the woman as she talked by pressing a button which would register their responses. Later, Guiora used skin responses and heart rate to determine levels of empathy. Schumann (Raymond, 1982) also studied empathy through hypnosis of individuals and concluded that empathy was more than a factor in pronunciation in an L2, and had a positive relationship to overall success in L2 acquisition. Guiora continued his studies of empathy and pronunciation including his now famous alcohol studies where subjects were given alcoholic beverages believed to lower inhibitions and lessen ego Adamopoulos 11 boundaries. Guiora was highly criticized for these studies and many of his results supporting alcohol’s relationship to improved pronunciation in an L2 have been discounted. Empathy remains an enigma in terms of how to measure this quality and accurately define its relationship to second language acquisition. Implications for Further Research Although no direct relationship can be confirmed between achievement in second language acquisition and extroversion-introversion; field independence-field dependence; or empathy, researchers have recently found that there is an indirect connection tied to motivation and attitude. Lalonde and Gardner (1984) have studied personality traits and motivation in order to determine if there is a causal relationship between these variables. As described in the discussion above, they isolated two personality traits, analytic orientation and seriousness, and studied these in relation to motivation to learn French among English-speaking students in Canada. Results of this study suggest that personality variables be examined “along with other variables relevant to the learning process,” such as attitudes and motivation (p.235). Lalonde and Gardner (1984) conclude, “Hypothesizing personality traits to be directly related to second language achievement may lead researchers to obtain less than encouraging results, and as a consequence the role of such variables may be underestimated” (p. 235). The study resulted in a Causal Model of personality and second language acquisition. Analytic orientation and seriousness were found to have an indirect relationship to success in second language acquisition by the impact of these variables on attitude and motivation. Adamopoulos 12 Later studies of personality and second language acquisition have investigated the causal model and better defined personality traits. A study conducted by MacIntyre and Charos (1996) combined Lalonde and Gardner’s (1984) Causal Model of Second Language Acquisition and Goldberg’s Five Personality Factors: Introversionextroversion; pleasantness-agreeableness; conscientiousness-dependability; emotional stability; and intellect or sophistication also labeled culture or openness to experience. MacIntyre and Charos (1996) found in a study of this hybrid model of Lalonde and Gardner and Goldberg’s “Big Five” that these traits indirectly affect L2 learning by “their influence on language-related attitudes, language anxiety, perceived L2 competence, motivation for language learning and willingness to communicate” (p. 20). Conclusion The research has found no direct relationship between personality traits such as extroversion-introversion; field independence/field dependence and empathy with higher achievement in second language acquisition. However, as Lalonde and Gardner (1984) and MacIntyre and Charos (1996) have concluded, personality does have an indirect effect on the success or failure of individual language learners based on the impact of personality on attitude and motivation. Attitude and motivation have been confirmed as factors in second language acquisition, and the relationship between personality and attitude and motivation needs further study to confirm the extent of personality’s influence on second language acquisition inside and outside of the classroom. Recent studies focusing on “willingness to communicate” and the skills and strategies needed for success outside of the classroom are yielding more implications for the role of personality in L2 acquisition. It seems that more extroverted, outgoing, Adamopoulos 13 sociable, field dependent and empathetic personality types are better communicators. They are better at listening and have a higher tolerance for ambiguity, seeking out interaction with others. They are more likely to take risks. All of these are the qualities of good language learners. As the research of Rubin and Thompson (Brown, 2001) has found, the “Good Language Learner” is creative, experimental and has a “feel for the language,” a characteristic of empathetic individuals (p. 209). Good language learners “learn to live with uncertainty” and “learn chunks of language as wholes…” similar to field dependent traits (p.209). They “make their own opportunities for practice inside and outside the classroom” as extroverts seek out situations for socialization and interaction with others (p. 209). As Naiman and others have concluded, teachers must account for personality differences in the classroom, but they must not prejudge students who exhibit personality traits that are not associated with communicative success in second language learning. In classroom settings, every learner should have a chance to use strategies for language learning according to his or her personality preferences. Personality traits are another individual difference closely tied to attitude and motivation that teachers must account for in planning lessons and activities which will tap into the learner’s innate language abilities and intrinsic motivation or desire to persevere in second language learning. Adamopoulos 14 References Brown, Douglas H. (2001). Teaching by Principles: An Interactive Approach to Language Pedagogy 2nd ed. White Plains, NY: Addison Wesley Longman, Inc. Dulay, Heidi, Marina Burt and Stephen Krashen. (1982). Language Two. New York: Oxford University Press. Furnham, Adrian. (1990) Language and Personality. In Howard Giles and W. Peter Robinson Eds. Handbook of Language and Social Psychology. (pp. 73-95). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Gardner, Robert C. and Richard Clément. (1990). Social Psychological Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition. In Howard Giles and W. Peter Robinson Eds. Handbook of Language and Social Psychology. (pp.495-517). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Gardner, R.C. and P.D. MacIntyre. (1993). A student’s contributions to secondlanguage learning. Part II: Affective variables. Language Teaching, 26, 1-11. Gayle, Grace M.H. (1981) Personality, Motivation, and Second Language Learning. Canadian Journal of Education, 6 (3), 55-67. Krashen, Stephen D. (1981). Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning. California: Pergamon Press, Inc. Internet edition published December, 2002. Avialable http://www.sdkrashen.com/SL_Acquisition_and _Learning/024.html. Lalonde, R.N. and R.C. Gardner. (1984). Investigating a Causal Model of Second Language Acquisition: Where does personality fit? Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 16 (3), 224-237. Adamopoulos 15 MacIntyre, Peter and Catherine Charos. (March, 1996). Personality, Attitudes, and Affect as Predictors of Second Language Communication. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 15 (1), 3-26. Raymond, Patricia. (1982). Attitude, Motivation and Personality in Second Language Acquisition: From Macro Approach to Micro Approach. TESL Talk, 13 (1), 44-54. Wakamoto, Natsumi. (2000). Language Learning Strategy and Personality Variables: Focusing on Extroversion and Introversion. IRAL: International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 38 (1), 71-81. Adamopoulos 16 Appendix A Note. From “Language and Personality,” by Adrian Furnham, 1990, Handbook of Language and Social Psychology, p. 76. Adamopoulos 17 Appendix B Note. From “Personality, Attitudes and Affect as Predictors of Second Language Communication,” by Peter MacIntyre and Catherine Charos, 1996, Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 15 (1), p. 8.