Download Eye gaze and verb agreement in ASL

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ojibwe grammar wikipedia , lookup

Chinese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old Norse morphology wikipedia , lookup

Navajo grammar wikipedia , lookup

Inflection wikipedia , lookup

Proto-Indo-European verbs wikipedia , lookup

Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Causative wikipedia , lookup

Ukrainian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Japanese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup

Germanic strong verb wikipedia , lookup

Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup

Russian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Germanic weak verb wikipedia , lookup

Icelandic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup

Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Georgian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup

American Sign Language grammar wikipedia , lookup

Sotho verbs wikipedia , lookup

Hungarian verbs wikipedia , lookup

Italian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Kagoshima verb conjugations wikipedia , lookup

Pipil grammar wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Eye gaze and verb agreement in ASL
Robin Thompson
University of California, San Diego. The Salk Institute for Biological Studies
Karen Emmorey
The Salk Institute for Biological Studies
A unique aspect of sign language structure that is shaped by the visual modality is the use of eye
gaze to express linguistic contrasts. For example, Neidle, Kegl, MacLaughlin, Bahan, and Lee
(NKMBL, 2000) propose that eye gaze in American Sign Language (ASL) functions
independently as a feature-checking mechanism for verb agreement. On this view, eye gaze is
seen as marking agreement features of a noun in much the same way that inflectional
morphology does in traditional syntax. NKMBL claim that all verb types in ASL (agreeing,
spatial, and plain) must mark agreement for subject and object. Verbs can be marked with either
manual agreement (verb directed toward locations associated with the subject/object), nonmanual agreement (eye gaze toward the object/head-tilt toward the subject), or through the use of
an overt pronoun/nominal. We conducted a language production experiment using head-mounted
eye-tracking to a) investigate NKMBL’s proposed analysis, b) to clarify the grammatical
functions of eye gaze in ASL, and c) to determine the extent to which eye gaze behavior in ASL
corresponds to agreement patterns found cross-linguistically. Eye-tracking technology allows us
to examine with high accuracy and precision exactly where signers are looking, and thus we can
pinpoint where and how eye gaze is directed during verb production.
Wearing an iView eye tracking system mounted on a bicycle helmet, 10 native signers told a
story designed to elicit all three verb types to another native signer. Participants were also given
a list of verbs (12 plain, 7 agreeing, and 7 spatial verbs) and were asked to make up sentences
forming a story about two characters (Jack and Jill) using each verb in turn. The results were
inconsistent with NKMBL’s claims. While eye gaze accompanying (morphologically) agreeing
verbs was most frequently directed toward the location of the syntactic object, eye gaze
accompanying plain verbs was seldom directed toward the location of the syntactic object.
Further, eye gaze accompanying spatial verbs was toward the locative argument, rather than the
object of transitive verbs/subject of intransitive verbs as predicted by NKMBL. Plain verbs were
never produced with null object pronouns, the only environment where eye gaze would function
independently from morphological agreement.
To further investigate the nature of eye gaze in ASL, we asked the following questions: a) Do
speakers alter their gaze when producing English verbs? b) Is directing eye gaze toward verb
argument locations so “natural” it is observed even for novice ASL signers? c) Do fluent latelearners of ASL acquire the correct eye gaze behavior, even though such behavior has never been
explicitly taught? Using the same methodology, we studied fluent hearing non-native signers (10
or more years of sign exposure beginning after age 15), novice hearing non-native signers (2 or
less years of sign exposure), and hearing non-signers. English translations of the ASL verbs
were used for the non-signers. The results showed that hearing speakers rarely varied eye gaze
when producing English verbs, directing their gaze toward the addressee over 90% of the time.
Novice signers did not direct their gaze appropriately when producing ASL verbs. For all verb
types, novice signers varied their eye gaze across spatial locations and the addressee. The results
from the speakers and novice signers indicate that the linguistic use of eye gaze does not arise
‘naturally’ and must be learned. The fluent late-learners of ASL followed the eye gaze patterns
we observed for native signers with one exception: they overgeneralized eye gaze agreement to
plain verbs.
The pattern of eye gaze agreement we observed with both native and fluent signers can be
summed up as follows:
A. Gaze occurring with agreeing verbs marks the object
-the direct object for transitive verbs
-the indirect object for ditransitive verbs
B. Eye gaze with spatial verbs marks the locative
We propose to account for this pattern by appealing to the Accessibility Hierarchy put forth by
Keenan and Comrie (1977). The hierarchy, originally proposed to account for patterns of relative
clause formation across languages appears to capture a universal ‘natural’ ordering of arguments
and has been used to account for other phenomena such as causativization and case marking (see
Comrie 1976; Croft 1988). The Accessibility Hierarchy applies to verbal arguments, not
adjuncts, and we argue that locatives are in fact arguments of spatial verbs in ASL. Evidence for
this proposal is the fact that locatives are required by spatial verbs. ASL spatial verbs are
produced at a location in signing space (e.g., the one-handed form of STAND); however, when
no specific location in signing space is encoded, a neutral base hand must be added to the sign.
We propose that when a base hand is added to a spatial verb. It serves as an argument filler for
the locative. In addition, our eye-tracking results revealed that signers consistently directed their
gaze lower in signing space to mark locative agreement than when marking object agreement.
Thus, signers produced a clear distinction between locative and object agreement with respect to
eye gaze.
To capture the facts about eye gaze behavior for both spatial and agreeing verbs, we propose the
following eye gaze agreement hierarchy: Subject < Direct Object < Indirect Object < Locative.
Within this hierarchy, eye gaze marks the lowest argument. Thus, a unique agreement marker,
namely eye gaze, still follows a predictable pattern in natural languages. Further, while fluent
late-learners correctly apply this hierarchy to their eye gaze patterns, they overgeneralize and
employ it when producing plain verbs as well. By following a universal language pattern (the
Accessibility Hierarchy) and ignoring an idiosyncratic feature of ASL (marking agreement on
only a subset of verbs), fluent signers create a divergent grammar that is more consistent with
language universals (see Sorace 1993). In conclusion, our proposed hierarchy can account for
both eye gaze and manual agreement and may have larger applications within the study of
natural language processes.
References
Comrie, Bernard. 1976. The syntax of causative constructions: cross-linguistic similarities and
differences. In M. Shibatani (ed), The syntax of causative constructions. Syntax and
semantics,Vol. 6, Academic Press, New York.
Croft, William. 1988. Agreement vs. case marking and direct objects. In M. Barlow and C.
Ferguson (eds.), Agreement in natural language: Approaches, theories, descriptions,
Center for the Study of Language and Information, Stanford, CA, pp. 159-179.
Keenan, Edward. & Comrie, Bernard. 1977. Noun phrase accessibility and universal
grammar.Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 63-99.
Neidle, Carol, Judy Kegl, Dawn MacLaughlin, Benjamin Bahan, and Robert Lee. 2000. The
Syntax of American Sign Language: Functional categories and hierarchical structure.
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Sorace, Antonella. 1993. Incomplete vs divergent representations of unaccusativity in non-native
grammars of Italian. Second Language Research, 9, 22-47.