* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Joash Gambarage Johannes
Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup
Malay grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ukrainian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Compound (linguistics) wikipedia , lookup
Arabic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Modern Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ojibwe grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup
Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Navajo grammar wikipedia , lookup
Proto-Indo-European nominals wikipedia , lookup
Arabic nouns and adjectives wikipedia , lookup
Esperanto grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old Norse morphology wikipedia , lookup
Russian declension wikipedia , lookup
French grammar wikipedia , lookup
Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Vietnamese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Turkish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Sotho parts of speech wikipedia , lookup
Sotho nouns wikipedia , lookup
Romanian nouns wikipedia , lookup
Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup
THE KI-NATA NOUN STRUCTURE By Joash Gambarage Johannes A Dissertation Submitted in (Partial) Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts (Linguistics) of the University of Dar es Salaam University of Dar es Salaam June, 2007 2 THE KI-NATA NOUN STRUCTURE Joash Gambarage Johannes MA (Linguistics) Dissertation University of Dar es Salaam June 2007 3 CERTIFICATION The undersigned certifies that he has read and hereby recommends for the acceptance by the University of Dar es Salaam a dissertation entitled: The Ki-Nata Noun Structure, in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts (Linguistics) of the University of Dar es Salaam. ………………………………. Prof. D.P.B. Massamba (Supervisor) Date: ------------------------------- 4 DECLARATION AND COPYRIGHT I, Joash Gambarage Johannes, declare that this dissertation is my own original work and that it has not been presented and will not be presented to any other University for a similar or any other degree award. Signature ------------------------------------ This dissertation is copyright material protected under the Berne Convention, the Copyright Act of 1999 and other international and national enactments, in that behalf, on intellectual property. It may not be reproduced by any means, in full or in part, except for short extracts in fair dealings for research or private study, critical scholarly review or discourse with an acknowledgement, without the written permission of the Dean of the School of Graduate Studies, on behalf of both the author and the University of Dar es Salaam. 5 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am highly indebted to many people who assisted me in one way or another in accomplishing this work. To them all I would like to express my sincere thanks. In particular, Prof. D.P.B. Massamba, my supervisor, deserves my warm and heartfelt thanks for his patience, forbearance, constant advice and timely guidance, despite his many other important academic and social responsibilities. Without such a commitment, this work would not have reached this stage. A similar note of thanks goes to my sponsor, the Languages of Tanzania (LOT) Project, funded by the Swedish International Development Agency (SIDA), through its research cooperation wing, the Swedish Agency for Research Cooperation (SAREC), and the University of Dar es Salaam, without whose support financially and materially, it would not be possible for me to reach this academic level. Many native speakers of Ki-Nata have unselfishly and objectively spared part of their valuable time responding to my questions. They also provided valuable criticisms, checking the correctness and appropriateness of my own Ki-Nata. Since it would be unjust to name only some of them, but also impossible to list 6 all of them by name, I shall kindly ask them to accept my collective appreciation: Mukoore βwaheene βoose, ‘Thank you very much to you all’. My gratitude is also due to my lecturers and classmates, who I have drawn gratefully on their valuable comments in the course of writing the research proposal. They also built a favourable climate for a smooth running of the course, culminating in this work. Unfortunately, for lack of space, I cannot mention each by name. Last, but far from least, I am more than ordinarily grateful for the patience and understanding shown by my wife, Neema, for a difficult and tiresome task of taking care of our newborn, Jones/Gambarage, at the time this study was in progress. 7 DEDICATION To my beloved son, Jones/Gambarage and my wife Neema. 8 ABSTRACT This study presents a descriptive analysis of Ki-Nata noun structure. The only descriptive study on Ki-Nata is that of Mekacha (1985) that only touches upon a few aspects of the noun structure. In the light of recent researches and approaches to linguistic analyses, there are peculiarities so striking and so unlike the Ki-Nata language spoken today that the author has felt it important to work on. The study is based on the major assumption of Lexical Phonology, which is level ordered-morphology. The basic insight of level ordered-morphology is that derivational and inflectional processes of languages can be arranged in a series of levels. This derives from the line of argument by Kiparsky (1982) that morphological rules are sensitive to any output of phonological rules. Thus, phonological rules operate alongside with morphology in the lexicon. The work is organised into four chapters. Chapter One presents background information to the study such as statement of the problem, objectives and significance of the study, research hypotheses, theoretical framework, literature review and the sound inventory of Ki-Nata. Chapter Two presents an overview of the Bantu Noun Structure making reference to three aspects: augment, noun class prefix and nominal derivation. Chapter Three discusses the Ki-Nata noun structure based on such aspects as noun augments, nominal prefixes, singular- 9 plural pairing of the noun prefixes and nominal derivation processes. Chapter Four gives the summary and conclusion. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Certification------------------------------------------------------------------------ i Declaration and Copyright------------------------------------------------------ ii Acknowledgement---------------------------------------------------------------- iii Dedication-------------------------------------------------------------------------- v Abstract----------------------------------------------------------------------------- vi Abbreviations --------------------------------------------------------------------- x List of Symbols ------------------------------------------------------------------- xii LIST OF TABLES Table1: Ki-Nata Consonant System--------------------------------------------- 8 Table 2: Noun class prefixes of Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria ------------------- 21 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Ki-Nata Vowel Systems----------------------------------------------- 11 Figure 2: Noun Class Pairing in Ki-Nata ----------------------------------- 65 CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION 1.1 General Introduction ------------------------------------------------- 1 1.2 Geographical Location------------------------------------------------ 1 1.3 Background to the Problem------------------------------------------- 2 1.4 Statement of the Problem--------------------------------------------- 4 10 1.5 Objectives of the Study ------------------------------------------------ 4 1.6 Significance of the Study---------------------------------------------- 4 1.7 Research hypotheses-------------------------------------------------- 5 1.8 Theoretical Framework ------------------------------------------------ 5 1.9 Research Questions---------------------------------------------------- 7 1.10 Scope and Limitation of the Study---------------------------------- 7 1.11 Literature Review ------------------------------------------------------- 7 1.11.1 Sound Inventory of Ki-Nata------------------------------------------- 8 1.11.1.1 Consonant System--------------------------------------------------- 8 1.11.1.2 The Vowel System-------------------------------------------------- 11 1.11.2 Studies on Noun Morphology in Ki-Nata --------------------- 12 1.11.3 The Missing Link ----------------------------------------------------- 20 1.11.4 Other Related Literature -------------------------------------------- 21 1.12. Research Methodology-------------------------------------------------- 28 1.13. Summary-------------------------------------------------------------------- 29 CHAPTER TWO: THE BANTU NOUN STRUCTURE: AN OVERVIEW 2.1 Introduction---------------------------------------------------------------- 30 2.2 The Bantu Noun Structure: An overview---------------------------- 30 2.2.1 The Augment----------------------------------------------------------- 31 2.2.2 The Noun Class Prefix ---------------------------------------------- 37 2.2.3 Noun Derivation--------------------------------------------------------- 42 11 2.2.3.1 Deriving nouns by changing classes----------------------------- 42 2.2.3.2 Noun stems formed from verb stems/roots-------------------- 43 2.2.3.2.1 Using a nominalizing suffix {-i}------------------------------------ 43 2.2.3.3 Nouns formed with the nominalizing suffix {-o}--------------- 45 2.2.3.4 Nouns formed with the nominalizing suffix {-e}--------------- 45 2.3 Summary-------------------------------------------------------------------- 46 CHAPTER THREE: THE KI-NATA NOUN STRUCTURE 3.1 Introduction--------------------------------------------------------------- 48 3.2 Augment in Ki-Nata Nouns-------------------------------------------- 48 3.2.1 Use and Omission of the Augment--------------------------------- 55 3.3 The Noun Class Prefix in Ki-Nata ---------------------------------- 61 Data Analyisis--------------------------------------------------------- 64 3.3.1.1 Pairing system and a brief semantic survey------------------- 65 3.3.1.2 Surface representation of prefixes ---------------------------- 69 3.4 Noun Derivation-------------------------------------------------------- 85 3.5 Summary ---------------------------------------------------------------- 97 3.3.1 CHAPTER FOUR: SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION 4.1 Summary ------------------------------------------------------------------ 98 4.2 Conclusion---------------------------------------------------------------- 100 REFERENCES-------------------------------------------------------------------- 103 APPENDIX I----------------------------------------------------------------------- 107 12 ABBREVIATIONS 1/2/3PER sg/pl A 1st /2nd /3rd person singular/plural Aspect AG Augment Cas Causative Ci/Cu Consonant followed by i or u CV Consonant followed by a Vowel VCV Vowel-Consonant-Vowel cluster C/Cl Class A-CONECT A-connective CP Copula EMGP Extended Model of Generative Phonology Fut Future FV Final vowel H&K Hyman and Katamba Inf Infinitive MA Mood/Aspect NEG Negative N&P Nurse and Philipson NPx Noun Class Prefix 13 Loc Locative LOT Languages of Tanzania Project LP Lexical Phonology Pass Passive PERF Perfective SC Subject Concord ST Stem SUF Suffix TA Tense and Aspect TAM Tense, Aspect and Mood R Verb Root W.F.P Word Final Position 14 LIST OF SYMBOLS + Morpheme boundary Ø Zero morpheme // Underlying Representation [ ] Surface Representation → Realized as * The structure is unacceptable # Word initial position ≠ X not equal to/not same as Y § Section/Subsection Cο Voiceless consonant V: Long vowel 15 CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION 1.1 General Introduction This chapter presents preliminary information on the study. The chapter deals with such aspects as geographical location, background to the problem, statement of the problem, objectives of the study, significance of the problem, theoretical framework and research questions. Fieldwork information as well as review of the related literature are also included in the chapter. 1.2 Geographical Location The Ki-Nata language is spoken by βa-Nata1, who inhabit parts of two Tanzanian districts, namely Serengeti and Bunda in Mara Region. In Serengeti District, Ki-Nata is spoken in such areas as Isenye, Nata-Motukeri, Nata-Mbiso, Makondose, Nyichoka and Burunga. In Bunda District it is spoken in Mogeta and Kyandege. In the mentioned districts the βa-Nata interact with other speech communities. In Serengeti, they interact with Kuria, Ikoma and Isenye while in Bunda they interact with Kuria, Ikizo and Zanake. 1 Although speakers of the language ‘aβa-Nata’ call their language ‘IKi-Nata’, the labels ‘Ki-nata’ and ‘βa-Nata’ are used throughout this presentation. The βa-Nata range from approximately 20,000 to 25,000 in number (cf. National Bureau of Statistics, 2003:168) 16 1.3 Background to the Problem According to Greenberg (1963), Ki-Nata belongs to the Bantu family of languages, which falls under the Benue-Congo group. The latter falls under a major group known as Niger-Congo. Within the Bantu family, Ki-Nata belongs to the subgroup of Eastern Bantu languages, in which some languages have not been seriously studied while some are hardly known. Ki-Nata is among the languages that have been scantly studied. According to Guthrie (1948), Ki-Nata falls under Zone E, Group 40, Index 5. Ki-Nata is placed along with Ke-Ragoli (E41), Ke-Gusii (42), γi-Kuria (43), Ke-Zanake (E44), γiIsenye (E44b), Ci-Ndali (E44c), γi-Siora (E44g) and γi-Sweta (E44e). Others include γiKiroβa (E44f), γi-Ikizo (E44g), Ki-γirango (E44h), γi-Sembete (E44k) and Ki-Sonjo (E46). In his classification Guthrie treats Ki-Nata and γi-Ikoma as one and the same language. Bryan (1959) is another scholar who attempted a classification of the language under description. In her classification, Bryan followed closely Guthrie’s classification of KiNata. The only thing she added was that Ki-Nata was spoken around Ikoma2, south of River Mara; and that, to the east it borders Sonjo, Sukuma to the South, Ikizo to the West, and Ngoreme to the North. 2 Ikoma, Sonjo, Sukuma, Ikizu and Ngoreme as cited here by Bryan are place names of the respective languages. 17 Heine (1976: 51), focusing on vernacular languages of Tanzania spoken in Mara Region gives a classification that places Ki-Nata in the γi-Ikoma subgroup under the Ke-Zanake group. The subgroup has the following languages: γi-Isenye, Ki-Nata, and γi-Ikoma. Other sister languages to Ki-Nata like γi-Kuria and Ke-Ngoreme form different groups. Heine adds, however, that his classification is not based on objective analytic procedures, but rather on people’s subjective views based on degrees of mutual intelligibility. Nurse (1977: 71) places Ki-Nata in the Eastern Nyanza subgroup. Within this subgroup he includes such languages as Ke-Gusii, γi-Kuria, Ke-Zanake, Ki-Nata, Ke-Ngoreme and Ki-Shashi. This treatment is also maintained in the classification of Nurse and Philipson (1980). In the above classifications, we have the following reservations: Bryan (1959) and Guthrie (1967-71) treat Ki-Nata and γi-Ikoma as one and the same language. We tend to agree with Mekacha (1985) that a convenient Ki-Nata classification should have shown that Ki-Nata and γi-Ikoma are closely related languages but not that they are one and the same. Heine’s classification too should have, in our view, indicated that Ki-Nata is more closely related to Ke-Ngoreme than to Ke-Zanake. 18 1.4 Statement of the Problem In view of the reviewed literature (cf. section 10.2), it seems that little has been covered in as far as the structure of the Ki-Nata noun is concerned. Issues pertaining to various aspects such as shapes of augments and morphosyntactic contexts that license their distribution on nouns, allomorphemic variation of noun prefixes and processes involved in derived nouns have not been well covered. 1.5 Objectives of the Study The central objective of this study is to make a descriptive study of the noun structure of Ki-Nata. More specifically, the study seeks to give a detailed description of: i) The morphological structure of the Ki-Nata noun. ii) Shapes of augments in Ki-Nata nouns and morphosyntactic contexts licensing their distribution on nouns. iii) Various forms of noun prefixes and rules governing their manifestation. iv) Processes involved in deriving nouns from verbs, etc. 1.6 Significance of the Study This study is useful in that: • The Languages of Tanzania Project (LOT) is currently making efforts in researching on and documenting Tanzanian languages. The completion of this study is, therefore, a contribution to this undertaking. • This study is also useful to linguists who are interested in Bantu studies. 19 1.7 Research Hypotheses In this study the following hypotheses are attested: • Augment in Ki-Nata is associated with semantic and syntactic concepts like head-marker, determiner, etc. and its shape is controlled by vowel harmony rule. • Surface manifestations of noun class prefixes are accounted for by certain morphophonological rules. • Noun class prefix in Ki-Nata represents several concepts such as the form of the thing involved (i.e. small, broad, abstract etc.), singular and plural, etc. • Morphological processes are involved in deriving nouns from verbs, adjectives and nouns. 1.8 Theoretical Framework This study is based on the framework of Lexical Phonology (LP), an Extended Model of Generative Phonology (henceforth EMGP). LP has been arrived at through a combination of ideas and theories from independent researches. The major ones are: the theory of Morphology and the Lexicon, Level Ordered Morphology and the Problem of Constraining Lexical Representations and Phonological Rules. The basic assumptions of this theory, as stipulated by Kirpasky (1982), are well summarized by Massamba (1996: 165-173). Let us briefly examine a few things in this theory that are relevant to our area of study. 20 LP theory deals much with level ordered-morphology. The basic insight of level ordered-morphology is that derivational and inflectional processes of languages can be arranged in a series of levels. According to this theory, each step of word formation process is tied to rules of a certain level. Within this approach, it is assumed that the output of each word-formation process within the lexicon itself is accounted for by phonological rules of its level. At a lexicon level, such rules are referred to as Lexical Phonology Rules. Such rules apply after each step of word-formation. In LP, as it is also widely accepted elsewhere, phonology is integrated with morphology in the lexicon. This interdependence is explicitly governed by rule ordering that morphological rules may be sensitive to any output of phonological rules. In other words, phonological rules operate alongside morphology in the lexicon. In relation to Ki-Nata noun structure, it is thought that the derivational aspects of the noun are well covered in the LP framework. This is because Ki-Nata nouns derived from verbs, for instance, are a result of different morphological processes such as prefixation and suffixation, of which according to LP, can be assigned to level 1 and 2 of morphology and phonology. We also argue that phonological and morphological rules that account for surface manifestations of formatives in nouns derived from verbs are possibly applicable to surface forms of underived nouns in Ki-Nata. This theory is, therefore, ideal for this study. 21 1.9 Research Questions In this study the following questions are addressed: • What is the shape/form of the noun in Ki-Nata? • What kinds of morphosyntactic generalizations are associated with noun augments? • What are the various shapes of the noun prefixes and rules governing them? • What morphological processes are involved in deriving nouns from verbs? 1.10 Scope and Limitation of the Study While there are aspects like tone and syllable structure that could be dealt with in connection with the Ki-Nata noun, in this study, due to time constraint, we will limit ourselves to the description of the noun structure. 1.11 Literature Review This section has two parts. The first part is a survey of works done on Ki-Nata noun morphology. The second part locates the gap in relation to our area of study. Before we do this, however, it will be instructive to begin by examining the Ki-Nata sound inventory. 22 1.11.1 Sound Inventory of Ki-Nata Ki-Nata sound system has relatively simple consonant and vowel systems. 1.11.1.1 Ki-Nata Consonant System Bilabial Alveolar Postalveolar Palatal Velar Glottal Plosive t Nasal m c ɲ n Trill k ŋ r Fricative β Approx. w s ∫ γ h j Table1: Ki-Nata Consonant System. The consonant system shown here, except for sounds [b], [d], [g], is clearly the same as that of Mekacha (1985: 13). One thing we do not agree with Mekacha is his assumptions that all the above Ki-Nata sounds are found at underlying level. We will cite some examples to show that his assumption is misguided. The alveolar nasal /n/ in a word like /okojόnga/ ‘to put through a sieve’ is phonetically realized as a velar nasal [ŋ], thus [okojόŋga]. In generative phonology this phonological fact is accounted for by the Homorganic Nasal Assimilation rule represented informally as: Rule 1: /n/ → [ŋ] ⁄ — g 23 This rule is formally represented as: +cons -syll +nas +cons → [α place] -syll α place This rule means that a nasal consonant is articulated at the same point with the following non-nasal consonant. This shows that when /n/ precedes the voiced velar stop [g] it is phonetically realized as [ŋ], in its surface form. Moreover, consonants like [β], [γ] and [d] occur in certain phonetic environments only. We will now show their phonetic realizations: 1.11.1.1.1 Phonetic realization of /β/ The bilabial voiced fricative /β/ occurs both word initially and intervocalically, except where it is preceded by a bilabial nasal [m]. This phonological fact can be stated informally by the rule: Rule 2: /β/ → [b] / m— Formal representation of this rule is: +cons -syll +bil +con → [-cont] -syll +cons +bil +nas This rule means that a voiced bilabial fricative /β/ is realized as a voiced bilabial stop [b] when it is preceded by a bilabial nasal [m]. Since the voiced bilabial stop [b] and the voiced bilabial fricative [β] occur in mutually exclusive environments, the two sounds are said to be allophones of the same phoneme. 24 1.11.1.1.2 Phonetic realization of /γ/ The voiced velar fricative /γ/ is realized as a [γ] in all environments except where it is preceded by a velar nasal [ŋ]. Whenever the phoneme /γ/ is preceded by the velar nasal [ŋ], it surfaces as a voiced velar stop [g]. This phonological fact is accounted for by the following informal rule: Rule 3: /γ/ → [g] /ŋ — This rule is formally represented as: + cons -syll +cont → [-cont] +back +voice +high -syll +cons +back +high +nas ________ Like the phones [β] and [b], the voiceless velar fricative [γ] and the voiced velar stop [g] occur in environments that are mutually exclusive. This means that whereas [γ] occurs both word initially and intervocalically, [g] occurs only in environments where it is preceded by a velar nasal [ŋ]. 1.11.1.1.3 Phonetic realization of /r/ The trill /r/ is realized as [r] both word initially and intervocalically except if it is preceded by the alveolar nasal [n], in which case it consistently surfaces as a voiced alveolar stop [d]. This phonological fact can be taken care of by the rule stated informally as: Rule 4: /r/ → [d] /n — 25 This rule is formally represented as follows: +cons -syll +son +cont → -cont -son +cons -syll _______ +nas +cor The rule means that while the voiced alveolar stop [d] occurs in the environment where it is preceded by the alveolar nasal [n], the trill [r] occurs both word initially and intervocalically. This means that the phones [d] and [r] are in complementary distribution. 1.11.1.2 The vowel system Ki-Nata has a seven-vowel system. This is opposed to the five-vowel system that Mekacha (1985: 19) posits for the language. The seven vowels are presented below: i u e o ɔ ε a Figure 1: Ki-Nata Vowel System The lax vowels /ε ɔ/ in Ki-Nata are phonemic as exemplified below by minimal pairs: a) /o ɔ / rόra ‘be sour’ rɔra ‘see’ 26 b) /e ε/ kόŋo:ta ‘shake the dust’ kɔŋɔ:ta ‘beat severely’ réra ‘cry’ rέra ‘bring up a baby’ méra ‘swallow’ mέra ‘germinate’ This system is in agreement with the one Guthrie (1948: 18) posits for the languages of Zone E. It is clear that Mekacha’s five vowel system excluded the lax vowels /ε ɔ/, which are clearly manifested in Ki-Nata. 1.11.2 Studies on noun morphology in Ki-Nata Mekacha (1985) describes various phonological processes affecting Ki-Nata vowels. Although the work basically concerns itself with phonological phenomena, it also touches on aspects like augments and noun classes. Mekacha analyses phonological processes affecting vowels in different word categories. A few examples from his analysis are here presented, showing the effects of phonological processes within the nominal category. 1.11.2.1 Vowel deletion There is an outright loss of a vowel segment in certain phonetic contexts, as exemplified below: 1. a) mu+taki → [mtaki] ‘an officer’ 27 b) a+aka+ramu → [akaram] ‘a pen’ However, here Mekacha (1985: 36) needs to make it clear that with the exception of nouns in classes 1a, 2a and the locative classes (16-18), all Ki-Nata nouns occur with augments when they are in isolation. For this case, what we see in example 1(a) is a matter of inconsistence. Moreover, having aka as a prefix is an error. Again, the vowel a in both noun stems in 1(a) and (b) above is long and not short as he puts it. The nouns can therefore be accurately represented as [υmta:ki] and [akara:m]. The morphological parsing of akara:mu therefore, should be a+ka+ra:mu, and not a+aka+ramu. 1.11.2.2 Glide formation A vowel changes into a glide in certain phonetic environments, as exemplified below: 2. a) mu+ana→ [mwana] ‘a child’ The two vowels in 2(a) above are non-identical, thus the [u] changes into a glide. Mekacha also discusses the concept of vowel harmony with respect to prefixes and preprefixes. He contends that pre-prefix vowels harmonize with prefix vowels. He cites two kinds of vowel harmony, namely complete and partial vowel harmony. With regard to complete vowel harmony, he gives the following evidence: 3. a) e+me+te → b) a+ma+βere → [emete] ‘trees’ [amaβe:re] ‘milk’ c) o+mo+ri:si → [omori:si] ‘ herder’ As we can see, in examples 3 (a-c), the pre-prefix is a copy of the prefix vowel. 28 As for partial vowel harmony, he gives the following examples: 4. a) o+mu+kari → [omukari] b) e+γi+ta:nda → [eγita:nda] ‘woman’ ‘bed’ As seen in examples 4(a-b), the pre-prefix is [o] when the noun class prefix vowel is [u], as in 4(a) but [e] when the prefix vowel is [i], as in 4(b). This is to say that the pre-prefix is in partial harmony with the prefix vowel in that, in 4(a) the pre-prefix and the prefix vowel are all back vowels while in 4(b) the pre-prefix and the prefix vowel are front vowels. The question, however, is why in this language it is unacceptable to have the following forms: 5. a) *u+mu+kari ’woman’ b) *i+γi+ta:nda ‘a bed’ c) *i+γi:+se ‘flywhisk’ d) *i+βi+γero ‘things’ e) *i+mi+te:wa ‘grooms’ (augmentative) f) *u+mu+kama ‘wind’ g) *u+ku+γa:mba ‘to speak’ In answering the above fundamental question, one generalization to take into consideration is that pre-prefixes which are underlyingly high vowels, /u/ or /i/, are lowered to [o] and [e] word initially (cf. Hyman and Katamba 1993: 211, on Luganda). The tendency of lowering high pre-prefix vowels in word initial position is common in Bantu languages, especially those around the Lake Victoria area (cf. Chagas 1977: 35). 29 It seems to be the case, in view of the data we have and the researcher’s native intuition, that initial vowels /i/ and /u/, in nouns such as those in (5) above are pronounced as [I] and [υ] respectively. Given this, we can conclude that in Ki-Nata when a noun stem begins with a consonant and its prefix is either Ci or Cu, the augment is pronounced as either [I] or [υ] respectively. The appropriate phonetic transcription of the data in (5) above should, therefore, be: 6. a) /u+mu+kári/ → [υmukári] b) /i+γí+tanda/ → [Iγítanda] c) /i+γi:+sé/ → [Iγi:sé] d) /i+βi+γéro/ → [Iβiγéro] e) /i+mi+te:wá/ → [Imite:wá] f) /u+mu+káma/ → [υmukáma] g) /u+ku+γá:mba/ → [υkuγá:mba] As we can see here, when a noun stem begins with a consonant with either the Ci or Cu prefix the pre-prefix3 is pronounced as either [I] or [υ] respectively. This phonological fact can informally be stated as: Rule 5: /i/ or /u/ → [I] or [υ] _____ +Ci or Cu The same is formally accounted for by the following rule: 3 It is not possible to get examples of nouns in which initial vowels [I] and [υ] occur word medially or finally. 30 +syll -cons +hi → -high α place ___ +C +syll -cons +hi α place AG NPX That is, pre-prefixes which are underlyingly high vowels, /i/ or /u/, are lowered to [I] and [υ] respectively, if the noun prefixes are respectively Ci or Cu. Thus, the pre-prefix harmonizes partially with the vowel in the prefix. Before we account for the generalization of pre-prefixes /i/ and /u/ let us closely observe the data in 7(a-f) below: 7. a) /u+mu+aná / → [umwaná] ‘child’ b) /u+ru+ambé / → [urwambé] ‘gossip’ c) /u+βu+áŋgo/ → [uβwáŋgo] ‘quickness’ d) /i+ri+oβá/ → [iryoβá] ‘sun’ e) /i+ki+ará/ → [ikyará] ‘finger’ f) /i+βi+andá/ → [iβyandá] ‘intestine’ With regard to the data in 7(a-f), we note that the high vowels are not lowered as expected. This situation can be accounted for by having recourse to rule ordering. The thrust of the argument being that since we have two rules applying here, namely, glide formation and vowel harmony, it is evident that glide formation applies first before vowel harmony. The application of the glide formation rule bleeds the application of the vowel harmony rule. 31 Following closely the transcriptions in (6) and (7) above, we can see that by not distinguishing between vowels [i] and [I] or [u] and [υ], Mekacha’s transcription in (4) above is confusing and misleading. The fact is that pre-prefixes which are underlyingly /u/ or /i/ are respectively lowered to [I] or [υ] word initially. As for the noun classes, Mekacha uses a system that arranges independent prefixes by numbers in assigning names to different noun classes as established by Meinhof (1932) and Guthrie (1948), (Werner, 1919:34; Massamba, 1977: 72). Mekacha (1985: 97-98) presents a total number of 18 noun classes in Ki-Nata as follows: 8. 1 mu 10 n 2 βa 11 ru 3 mu 12 n (=class 10) 4 mi 13 ku (verbal infinitive) 5 ri 14 ka (diminutive) 6 ma 15 tu (diminutive) 7 γi 16 ha 8 βi 17 mu 9 n 18 γ u (augmentative) 32 Mekacha’s arrangement of Ki-Nata noun classes, however, differs from that of Meinhof and Guthrie in terms of noun class prefixes. The arrangement of noun classes by Meinhof (1932: 102), commonly referred to as Proto-Bantu, is as follows: 9. 1 *mu 8 *βi 15 *ku 2 *βa 9 *n 16 *pa 3 *mu 10 *n 17 *ku 4 *mi 11 *lu 18 *mu 5 *li 12 *ka 19 *pi 6 *ma 13 *tu 20 *γu 7 *ki 14 *βu 21*γi The above Proto Bantu4 arrangement of noun classes was then taken up by many other Bantu scholars like Meeusen (1967), Welmer (1973), Massamba (1976) and Hinnebusch (1989), (cf. Maho, 1999: 51). If we disregard the pre-prefixes, Mekacha’s classification of Ki-Nata noun class prefixes encounters the following mismatch: 10. Proto Bantu version 4 Mekacha’s version 12. *ka 12. n. 13. *tu 13. ku. 14. *βu 14. ka 15. *ku 15. tu Other scholars (cf. Bleek 1862) have simply designated the classes by their prefixes, i.e. the mu class, etc. 33 18. *mu 18. γu If we compare the above two arrangements, we note that there is a mismatch between the two systems in classes 12-15 and 20. Most Bantuists would agree that a more appropriate classification of nouns in Ki-Nata would be as follows: 11. Class Noun class prefix 12 ka 13 tu 14 βu 15 ku 20 γu As a matter of convention, it is generally agreed, from Bantu language studies, that each class number is consistently associated with its specific noun class prefix. The above arrangement matches with the Proto-Bantu order of noun classes and noun class prefixes (cf. Werner 1919), and other Bantu languages related to Ki-Nata (see Cammenga 1992, 2002, on γi-Kuria and Ke-Gusii respectively; Massamba 1976, 1995 on Ci-Ruuri). Maho (1999: 301) provides a pairing system of the Ki-Nata noun classes. Although he examines various noun classes of Bantu languages, not much information is available regarding the language under description. He gives the following class pairing system of Ki-Nata nouns: 34 12. Class 1 and 2 [mu]/[βa] [umwaaná]/[aβaaná] ‘child’/ ‘children’ Class 3 and 4 [mu]/[mi] [umwandό]/[imyandό] ‘cloth’/ ‘clothes’ Class 5 and 6 [ri]/[ma] [risosό]/[amasosό] ‘lung’/ ‘lungs’ Class 7 and 8 [ki]/[βi] [Ιkímweso]/[Ιβímweso] ‘sacrifice’/‘sacrifices’ Class 9(aN) and 10(caN) [aɲ akwahá]/[caɲ akwahá] ‘armpit’/ ‘armpits’ Class 12 and 8 [ka]/[βi] [akamorí]/[υtumo:rí] ‘calf’/calves (dim) Class 15 and 6 [ku]/[ma] [υkuβόko]/[amaβόko] ‘arm’/ ‘arms’ [amata:rό]/[υγuta:rό] ;river(s) (aug) Class 6 (aug.) and 20 [ma]/[γu] Maho points out that concords for classes 9 and 10 are identical with a nasal prefix. Moreover, we think that the pairing of noun classes by mere numbers, except for classes 9, 10 and 20, does not suffice to give a clear picture of the various singular-plural noun class prefixes in Ki-Nata. 1.11.3 The Missing Link The reviewed literature shows that little has been done with respect to the noun structure of the language under description. In fact, the cited works have dealt with noun classes and augments only superficially. Such aspects as shapes of augments and morphosyntactic contexts that license their distribution on nouns, allomorphemic variation of noun prefixes and processes involved in derived nouns have not been discussed. This study, therefore, aims at addressing these issues. 35 1.11.4 Other Related Literature As pointed out earlier, the language under description has been grouped together with Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria groups in Guthrie’s classification of Bantu languages. It is hoped that, in this part, the review of related literature from Ke-Gusii, γi-Kuria and other Bantu languages, especially Ci-Ruuri, can present noun morphological aspects more or less in the same way as Ki-Nata. Let us begin by examining literature on Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria nominal morphology. In his Phonology and Morphology of Ekegusii (2002) and Phonology and Morphology of Kuria (1992), Cammenga gives a detailed description of Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria morphology. On noun morphology, the aspect we are much interested in, he presents the noun classes, prefixes and agreement, noun derivation and allomorphs of noun class prefixes. According to Cammenga (1992: 78, 2002: 199) the Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria nouns respectively, form the following noun class prefixes: Ke-Gusii γi-Kuria Cl NPx examples gloss Cl NPx examples gloss 1 omo omoremi farmer 1 omo omokari woman 1a mo 1a ø/mo sokoro/? grandfather ? 1b ø taata father 2 aβa aβa-kari women 2 aβa aβaremi farmers 2a βa βa-sokoro grandfathers 3 omo omote tree 3 omo omote tree 4 eme emete trees 4 eme emete trees 5 eri eritɔɔke banana 5 iri/ere irisana cave 36 6 ama amatɔɔke banana 6 ama amasana caves 7 eke eγesaku clan 7 eke ekeγambɔ language 8 eβI eβisaku clans 8 iβi iβiγambɔ languages 9 e- ekaβira tribe 9 e- e-βaata duck 9a e-n endaaγera food 9a eN- embeγo seed 10 ci cikaβira tribes 10 ici- ici-βaata ducks 10a ci-n cindaaγera foods 10a iciN icimburi goats 11 oro oroko firewood 11 oro oroko firewood 12 aka akaana small child 12 aka akaana small child 14 oβo oβokombe hoe 14 oβo oβokaano sesame seed 15 oko okoγoro leg/foot 15 oko oγosoma to read 16 a- ase place 16 aha ahase place 21 Ňa ňamiira a place 17 ko kuuγikurya in Kikurya 18 mo moonse inside 19 ihi ihiβeγo small seeds 20 oγo oγoβeγo large seed 21 Ňa- Ňaβohanse avillagename name Table 2: Noun class prefixes of Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria As we can see from the above data, the two languages have much in common as far as the noun class prefixes are concerned. Cammenga suggests that both arrangements in the above data form the following singular- plural pairing system: 1/2, 3/4, 5/6, 7/8, 9/10. The data show that nouns in both languages have augments, except for kinship terms in classes 1a, 2a and 21 for Ke-Gusii, kinship terms in classes 1a and 21, and locative classes 17 and 18 for γi-Kuria. According to Cammenga, augments occur word initially (observe the ones in italic form) and all noun class prefixes in both languages follow 37 immediately after augments. With regard to the number of prefixes, one should note that while classes 17-20 are present in γi-Kuria they are not present in Ke-Gusii. Given the difference in number of class prefixes in the two languages, one can say that the number of noun class prefixes is not constant in all Bantu languages. As far as the numbering is concerned, except for class 21, Cammenga advocates Meinhof’s approach of classifying Bantu nouns. Based on Meinhof’s approach, class 21 has the prefix γi-. But what we see in Cammenga’s work is the prefix Ňa-. Again, there is no example of nouns for the mo- prefix in class 1a in both languages. Since we know that class 1a in most Bantu languages carries a null prefix we can conclude that Cammenga is possibly wrong to have had the mo- prefix in class 1a. Apart from numbering, Cammenga treats augments as part and parcel of the noun class prefixes in his morphological parsing. This is inappropriate since augments and noun class prefixes are two different morphological aspects. The issue of the shape of the augment in classes 10 and 10a, in most Bantu languages around Lake Victoria, seems to be complicated. Scholars such as Cammenga (1992: 78, 2002: 199) think that augments in classes 10 and 10a in Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria have the CV and VCV forms respectively as shown in his analysis (cf. table 2 above). This view is incorrect since the ca is not the augment but rather part of the original form of classes 10 and 10a prefixes –caN (cf. Maho 1999: 301). We will explain this by presenting two arguments: First, earlier studies of Bantu noun classes provide evidence that the prefix 38 for classes 10 and 10a was originally of a VCV structure which was later reduced to a nasal form (N) in languages like Luganda and retained in some languages (cf. Werner 1919: 23, 26). Thus, diachronically, languages like γi-Kuria, Ki-Nata, and Ke-Gusii, to mention but a few, have retained the early Proto-Bantu shape in class 10 with slight transformation to CVC (caN) while in class 9 the original VCV form has been reduced to VC, in this case aN (cf. Maho 1999: 301). Second, it is widely accepted in Bantu language studies that the noun prefix controls the concordial agreement between a noun and a verb. The following are example of sentences: 13. a) γi-Kuria: ciŋgoγé ce-era ce-keɲere ‘those baboons have fled’ b) Ki-Nata: caŋguγé ci-iri ci-ŋosire ‘those baboons have fled’ c) Ke-Zanake: zeŋgoγé ze-rya ze-ŋosire ‘those baboons have fled’ d) Ki-Isenye: ceŋguγé ci-ire ce-kaŋosa ‘those baboons have fled’ If we disregard the surface forms of augments, it is apparent that in the above languages the noun formatives in italics are part of the NPX because they are concordial elements. This makes our exercise of determining this morpheme even simpler since augments never count as markers of concordial agreement. 39 In his Umbo-Nomino Katika Ci-Ruuri, Massamba (1976) gives a detailed description of Ci-Ruuri nominal morphology. The major aspects dealt with in his study include: Noun classification, noun structure, and function of nominal morphemes. Other aspects are derivation of nouns from verbs and function of augments. According to Massamba, the structure of the Ci-Ruuri noun includes three morphemes, namely, a pre-prefix, noun prefix and a noun root or stem. In his later work, The Classification of Ci-Ruuri Nouns, Massamba (1995: 607-608) discusses various approaches to Bantu noun classification. From the description of the Nominal Prefix Approach by Meinhof, Massamba forms the following Ci-Ruuri noun class prefixes: 14. 1 mu 8 [β]i- 14 [β]u- 2 [β]a 9 n- 15 ku- 3 mu- 10 n- 16 a- 4 mi- 11 ru- 17 e- 5 li- 12 ka- 18 mu- 6 ma- 13 tu- 19 gu- 7 ci- In the above data, as it is in Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria, classes 1-10 and 12-13, odd numbered prefixes indicate the singular forms of nouns to which they are attached, while the even ones immediately following them, indicate their corresponding plurals. Although in Ke-Gusii and γi-Kuria class 13 does not exist, it does in Ci-Ruuri. With regard to class 12 [ka], Massamba (1995) explains that this prefix has disappeared in 40 some languages like Kiswahili and has been replaced by ki. Some speakers of standard Kiswahili have, however, begun using it again. Regarding the function of the prefix, Massamba argues that the prefix is a presentation of several concepts like humans (class 1 [mu-]), abstract (class 14 [βu-] etc. In Ki-Nata, as we shall see in Chapter Three, both class 12 and 13 exist and also mark diminutiveness. As it is in Ci-Ruuri, the prefixes in Ki-Nata convey different concepts. Unlike Ki-Nata, where any vowel can appear as a pre-prefix, in Ci-Ruuri, according to Massamba (1976, 1977, 1995), a vowel cannot appear as a pre-prefix if it has the features: +back +high +round The Ci-Ruuri noun pre-prefix, like it is in other Bantu languages (cf. Hyman and Katamba, 1991 on Luganda; Progovic, 1991 on Ki-Nande; Byarushengo et al, 1959 on Luhaya), deletes in certain contexts such as in negative and interrogative constructions, while in affirmative ones it is retained. In all the three languages, nouns can be derived from verbs. The main processes described in these languages (i.e. Ke-Gusii, γi-Kuria and Ci-Ruuri) include prefixation and suffixation where prefixes and suffixes are added on verbal roots/stems. Both Cammenga (1992, 2002) and Massamba (1976) explicitly show that suffixes added to verbal roots/stems to form nouns appear in different patterns such as -i, -o, -e, etc. and 41 that they count for some semantic motivation of nouns to which they are attached. Below are some examples from Ci-Ruuri (Massamba 1976: 74) and Ke-Gusii (Cammenga 2002: 531): 15. a) Ci-Ruuri Stem/root lim- Derived noun ‘cultivate’ o-mu-lim-i ‘cultivator’ (translation is mine) b) Ke-Gusii Input -rem- Suffixation -i Prefixation mo- Augmentation o- Output ‘dig’ omoremi As seen from the above data, the output from both Ci-Ruuri and Ke-Gusii derived nouns has involved prefixation and suffixation of affixes. Furthermore, the nominalizing suffix –i in (a) and (b) above carries the connotation of the performer of the action. The data we have in Ki-Nata reveal that nominal derivation uses prefixation and suffixation in deriving nouns from verbs (cf. §. 3.3). 42 1.12 Research Methodology 1.12.1 Study Site This research was conducted in Nata Ward, Serengeti District, Mara Region. This is because Nata is the heartland of Ki-Nata. Due to this, it was hoped that the researcher would be able to elicit the necessary linguistic information. 1.12.2 Population Sample As a native speaker of the language, the researcher has furnished some data. But in order to avoid imposing idiosyncratic features on the data, ten informants (of different age groups and sex) were randomly chosen to provide a body of vocabulary (nouns), phrases and sentences in the language under discussion. The selection of informants was based on the requirement that the informant had to be a native speaker of the language. 1.12.3 Data Collection Procedure Since the intention of this study was to collect a corpus of lexical items (nouns), phrases and sentences in Ki-Nata, the procedure used was face-to-face interviews. This means, the researcher went to the field to elicit data from the informants. 1.12.4 Instruments of Data Collection 1.12.4.1 Questionnaire In this study, a close-ended type of questionnaire was used as a guide for the interview. The questionnaire was written in English and included about 200 nouns denoting different body parts, natural phenomena, nouns derived from adjectives, verbs, etc. 43 Also, for the sake of generalizations pertaining to augments in Ki-Nata, various phrases and sentences were recorded. 1.12.4.2 Tape recorder A tape recorder was used during the fieldwork to record some conversations, corpus of lexical items, phrases and sentences. 1.13 Summary In this chapter, we have presented preliminary information on our study such as geographical location, background to the problem, statement of the problem, objectives of the study, significance of the problem, theoretical framework and research questions. Also, fieldwork information as well as review of related literature on Ki-Nata and other related languages have been provided. 44 CHAPTER TWO THE BANTU NOUN STRUCTURE: AN OVERVIEW 2.1 Introduction In this chapter we examine the Bantu noun structure in detail. Since we believe that Bantu languages share certain nominal morphological features, it is hoped that an examination of Bantu noun structure will shed light in the course of exploring the noun structure of Ki-Nata. 2.2 The Bantu noun structure: an overview There are different forms of nouns in Bantu languages. One is the form in which a noun is formed by a free morpheme. Such nouns have no affixes. Observe Kiswahili nouns below: 16. mama ‘mother’ maji ‘water’ Faraja ‘person name’ Kaka ‘elder brother’ All Kiswahili nouns under 16 above are formed by free morphemes. A Bantu noun can also be formed by bound morphemes. Such nouns are analyzed as consisting of a stem/root and an affix. An affix, in the morphology of a noun, can be a 45 noun prefix, or in nouns derived from verbs a noun suffix (cf. Schadeberg, 2003: 71). In most Bantu nouns, a noun root is that part which is no more analyzable morphologically. Except for nouns formed by free morphemes, in most Bantu nouns a root/stem combines with either an NPx or suffix or both to form a word (Adonijah, 2003: 43). In Kiswahili, quick examples here would be words under 17 below: 17. m-zee ‘old man’ lind-o ‘guard’ ki-lim-o ‘cultivation’ In 17 above, the prefix in the word m-zee, is m- and the stem is –zee. In lind-o, the word has the root/stem lind- and the suffix –o. The word ki-lim-o is formed by the prefix ki-, the root/stem –lim- and the suffix –o. However, as we have already seen, in some Bantu languages, a noun prefix can be preceded by an augment. We shall now try to examine this element more closely: 2.2.1 The Augment In some Bantu languages nominal prefixes are sometimes preceded by a formative that has been given different names by different Bantu scholars. Gregersen (1967), for example refers to it as pre-prefix. De Blois (1970) calls it the augment; Guthrie (196771) calls it double prefix; and Mould (1974) calls it initial vowel. Of these labels, the label augment seems to be used more commonly by Bantu scholars. As a matter of consistency we will use this label throughout this work. 46 Most Bantuists tend to agree that an augment is a morphological segment retained from an earlier Proto-Bantu CV form (Chagas 1977: 35). However, the occurrence of this formative is reported with variations in terms of shape and function from one Bantu language to another. It is important to point out here that some Bantu languages like KiSwahili (G42), Ki-Matuumbi (P13), Ki-Nkutšu (C73), to mention but a few, have no augments (cf. Maho 1999: 61). Some Bantu scholars report that augments in some languages (i.e. Luganda, Ruhaya, Isizulu, etc.) are simply initial vowels (cf. Ashton et al (1954: 402); Byarushengo et al (1977: 35); Maho (1999: 61) respectively). As is widely accepted in other Bantu languages, it is still possible to say that the shapes of augments are mostly controlled by the vowel harmony rule. Furthermore, there seems to be restrictions on the distribution of augments in Bantu languages. For instance, while in Otjiherero no other vowel will appear as an augment apart from [o] and [e], in CiRuuri, as we pointed out earlier (cf. Massamba 1977: 52), and Ruhaya (cf. Chagas 1977: 35), [u] does not appear as an augment. The most debatable issue pertaining to the augment is its function. This is because the function of this formative seems to be difficult to pinpoint. There are various views regarding the function of the augment in Bantu languages. One view is that of treating augments as part and parcel of the class prefix. This is the view held by Guthrie (1970: 83, 87). Let us closely observe the way he treats this formative in the following examples from Ki-Nata and Ci-Bemba: 47 18. a) Ki-Nata (E.45) 18. b) Ci-Bemba (M.42) Cl 7 eβe eβi-sara ‘roofs’ eβe-sima ‘wells’ eβe-kuβa ‘chests’ eβε-rεrɔ ‘vegetables’ (relish) Cl 1 umu umu-londo ‘fisherman’ 2 aβa aβa-londo ‘fishermen’ 3 umu umu-koŋkwa ‘gully’ 7 ici ici-kolwe ‘ancestor’ 7 icy icy-alo ‘country’ As we can see from his morphological parsing of Ki-Bemba and Ki-Nata above, Guthrie does not separate the augment from the noun class prefix. This shows that, to him, the augment is part and parcel of the class prefix. Guthrie calls the unit in bold the “prefix unit.” Such units, according to him, will always be quoted without a hyphen between an augment and NPx (Guthrie 1970: 80). However, researches in Bantu languages show that the augment and the prefix are two distinct formatives. Augments tend to disappear in certain grammatical contexts (cf. Massamba 1976: 70; Hyman and Katamba (henceforth H&K, 1993: 223-228). Early scholars who worked on South African Bantu languages like Isizulu and Isixhosa, wondered whether this formative should be called an “article” or not. Bourquin (1946) in his The So-called Article in Xhosa, (cf. Massamba, 1976: 54), presents various views from different scholars on the augment. He shows that Dohme (1857) is the pioneer of 48 the idea that augments function the same as “definite articles” in English. In the ZuluKafir Dictionary by Bourquin, (Massamba, 1976: 55) contends that this formative in Isizulu shows definiteness of the noun. Other scholars supporting this view include Bleek (1862) Comparative Grammar of South African Languages, who maintains that this formative is an “article” and it deletes in certain environments such as when it appears after a negative verb. Again according to Massamba (1976: 56), Mcharen (1886) in Outline of Xhosa Grammar, and Torrend (1887) in Outline of Xhosa Grammar contribute to the idea that the augment in Isixhosa is the “article” and it conveys the concept of definiteness. Massamba (1976: 56) reports that other scholars do not subscribe to the view that augments in Bantu function as “articles”. Massamba (op. cit.) argues that Grout (1893) in his Grammar of the Zulu Languages refutes the argument that augments are the same as “articles.” He adds that in Isizulu or other languages closer to Isizulu there is nothing like the so-called “article.” Bryant (1905) in his the Zulu-English Dictionary, apart from agreeing with Grout, goes even further to add that “articles” do not exist in Bantu languages (cf. Massamba 1976: 56). Massamba (1976: 57-58) reports further that other scholars who go along with Grout are: Werner (1919) in her Introductory Sketch of the Bantu Languages, who argues that the function of augments in Bantu languages is not the same as that of English articles “a” and “the.” Like Werner (ibid: 49), Samuelson (1915) in his A Zulu Grammar rejects 49 the argument that there is a “definite article” in Isizulu. He agrees that augments are present in Isizulu and play the role of definiteness and that their manifestation depends on the context. H&K (1993: 211), also discuss the function of the augment, arguing that some scholars claim that the presence or absence of an augment in Bantu languages is licensed by, among other things, pragmatic/semantic or syntactic generalizations. On pragmatic grounds, they review the argument of Givon (1971) that augment conditioning is pragmatic. On the other hand, Progovic (1993: 257-267), who advocates the syntactic function of augment, takes the view that augments are syntactically conditioned. H&K (1993: 221) take the following Ki-Nande example from Progovic: 19. a) Yohani si anzire o-mukali [+def, +spec] ‘John doesn’t like the woman’ b) Yohani si anzire mukali [-def, -spec] ‘John doesn’t like any woman’ c) *Yohani anzire mukali [-spec] ‘John likes any woman’ H&K show that the presence of an augment after a negative verb, such as in 19(a), indicates a definite and specific referent, while its absence, as in 19(b), designates an indefinite, none specific referent. According to H&K, specificity is here taken to mean that there is referentiality. In other words, a specific referent exists or it is intended. The 50 ungrammaticality of 19(c) suggests that the non-augmented form cannot appear after an affirmative verb. On Givon’s side, the above examples support his contention in that concepts such as specificity and definiteness are reduced to pragmatic or semantic primes. That is, the use or non-use of augments still depends upon context. Apart from specificity and definiteness concepts, the augment is viewed as a determiner marker (cf. Meeussen 1959). Addressing the Ki-Rundi case, Meeussen (1959: 63) contends that the augment marks headness or initiality within an immediate NP. This is to say that Meeussen is also of the opinion that augments are syntactically motivated. Moreover, the presence of the augment in some Bantu languages has been viewed as part of inflectional morphology. H&K (op .cit: 218) refer to Anderson (1988), who argues that the function of the augment is conditioned by a combination of syntactic and morphological requirements. According to H&K (op. cit: 218), Anderson (ibid) argues further that there is no semantic or pragmatic generalization underlying the use of augment; instead, the partial pragmatic generalizations concerning definiteness are limited to specific construction, and for that reason, cannot serve as a general account of the phenomenon. However, the above generalizations may not necessarily capture all cases pertaining to the function of augments in all Bantu languages. The study of augment in Tonga language by Carter (1963: 34) (cf. H&K op. cit), for instance, reveals that the augment 51 has no defining function. According to H&K, Carter shows that it is impossible to set up rules of use for the augment, since it has no use in the sense of function. She concludes that the augment has no place in the structure of the language, since there are no conditions under which its appearance is obligatory. A closer examination of the above discussion on augment function in Bantu languages shows that the issue of augment function is still elusive. Since the issue of function of the augment in Bantu languages is rather complicated, there is a need for more research in this area. 2.2.2 The Noun Class Prefix (NPx) In his description of the characteristic features of Bantu languages, Guthrie (1948: 11) makes mention of the system of grammatical gender. What needs to be stressed here is that the use of the term “gender” by Guthrie differs from that of Indo-European languages. Nouns in languages like German, Latin and Greek are classified according to gender classes –a distinction between male entities (masculine), female entities (feminine) and sexless entities or animals (neuter) (Werner 1919; Dixon 1968). To come to grips with gender in Indo-European languages, let us observe the following examples from German and Spanish by Yule (1985: 71): 20. a) German Grammatical Gender Masculine Example der mond Gloss ‘the moon’ 52 b) Spanish Feminine die sonne ‘the sun’ Neuter das feuer ‘the fire’ Masculine el sol ‘the sun’ la luna ‘the moon’ Feminine As we can see from both German and Spanish examples, formatives in bold take different forms depending on the noun category of grammatical gender –masculine, feminine or neuter. To Guthrie, nouns in Bantu languages are not classified by masculine, feminine and neuter class genders but rather by independent prefixes –noun class prefixes. A noun prefix in this case, according to him, is a sign of “gender.” A prefix as a feature of grammatical gender is, therefore, used to assort words into a number of classes varying roughly from 10 to 205. According to Guthrie, there is a closer association between independent prefixes with identical stems and “gender.” This means that all nouns having independent prefixes with identical stems will form one gender. To understand what Guthrie means by “class genders” let us examine the following examples from three different Bantu Languages: 21. a) Ki-Nata: I Iki:-rema II υβu-i:ro 5 ‘darkness’ ‘loyalty’ Since Guthrie’s noun class prefixes do not differ from those of Pro-Bantu by Meinhof (1932) we refer to Proto Bantu ones as elaborated in (§. 1.8.2) 53 III ama-γuta b) c) Kiswahili: Chasu: ‘oil’ I m-vulana---------------wa-vulana ‘boy/boys’ II m-ti---------------------mi-ti ‘tree/trees’ III ki-ti----------------------vi-ti ‘chair/chairs’ IV ji-we---------------------ma-we ‘stone/stones’ I m-bwange ‘young man’ II va-bwange ‘young men’ III u-bwange ‘youth’ In Guthrie’s sense, 21(a) is an example of one class gender. Specifically, there are three different one-class genders: the Iki-, υβu and ama-. Each noun in 21(a) is an example of one-class gender simply because each independent prefix can only be associated with a single identical stem. 21(b) has examples of two class-genders. Thus, each pair in 21(b) is an example of two class-genders. The reason being that each identical stem in those pairs can only be associated with two independent prefixes. As we can see in 21(b), in each pair of independent prefixes –m-/wa, m-/mi-, ki-/vi-, and ji-/ma-, one independent prefix denotes singular and the other plural. Chasu nouns in 21(c) are examples of multiple-class genders. That is, a single identical stem can be associated with more than two independent prefixes (cf. m-, va, and u- in 21c). Although Guthrie himself does not define what gender means in Bantu languages, given the above treatment of noun pairings in 21(a-c), to him gender seems to mean a combination of any identical stem with an independent prefix. The combination of this sort, therefore, forms gender classes as exemplified in 21(a-c). 54 As we have seen, the concept “gender” is so broad and it means different things to different linguists (cf. Guthrie’s gender concept in Bantu and gender in Indo-European languages). To most linguists, however, “gender” is used in the sense that is understood in the perspective of the Indo-European languages. In this case, one would argue that the concept of “gender” does not exist in Bantu languages. Moreover, the classification of Bantu nouns as accepted by most Bantuists, can best be done by using a system of noun classes and not a system of class genders as Guthrie suggests. 2.2.2.1 Semantic Motivation of Noun Classes Most Bantuists agree that the Bantu noun class system appears to be at least partially semantically motivated. This is because some nouns that share a particular feature, e.g. [+HUMAN], or [+ABSTRACT], belong to the same class (Kahigi 2003). For instance, in most Bantu languages nouns whose singulars begin with m(u)- and plurals with wagenerally refer to human beings or animates (cf. Massamba 1995: 606) and are normally placed in classes 1 and 2 respectively; Classes 3 and 4 (mu-mi) contain, besides trees, parts and organs of the body, etc. (cf. Werner 1919); Class 11 (lu-) (or u- in languages like Ki-Swahili) consists mainly of long, thin, broad objects etc. Despite the opacity and blurred nature of the semantic criterion in Bantu noun classes, there is still evidence which shows that members of a given noun class at least belong to the same semantic realms i.e. [+HUMAN], [+ANIMATE], [+INANIMATE]. The later 55 makes us tend to agree with Massamba (1995) that the classification of Bantu noun was initially based on clear semantic criteria, perhaps during Proto-Bantu period. The term ‘noun prefix’ according to Guthrie (1970: 41), refers to ‘the concord element of an ‘independent nominal’6 as in the following Ci-Bemba (M42) example: Umulondo umbi akaya ‘another fisherman will go’ The nominal prefix umu- is taken as the controller of the grammatical agreement that forms the part of a set umu-, u-, a-. All these are, therefore, placed in the same class, numbered class 1 in this case. Guthrie takes the approach in which each noun class is identified by a specific nominal prefix. A number (an Arabic numeral) is then assigned to a nominal prefix, which counts as a noun class marker, and the number assigned to it as its class number. In such a case, a noun class prefix is thus a complex unit having both a noun prefix and a noun class number. 6 The term “nominal” in this case does not simply refer to nouns alone but it is a cover term for adnominals, pronouns and nouns. 56 2.2.3 Noun Derivation Noun derivation in Bantu covers the formation of noun stems from verbs, adjectives and other elements and the specification of the classes with which the noun stems may be associated in the construction of nouns. Quite typical of the Bantu situation is the fact that a noun stem, whether underived or derived, gets associated with as many groups of classes as are allowed by the meaning restrictions of the stem/or the derivational affix (cf. Kahigi 2003: 17). Studies on Bantu noun derivation (cf. Polomé, 1967; Massamba, 1977; Kahigi, 2003; Rugemalira, 2005, to mention but a few) reveal that nouns are mainly derived through affixation. The following affixation processes are identified in noun derivation: 2.2.3.1 Deriving nouns by changing classes Rugemalira (2005: 67) gives the following examples from Runyambo: 22. a) en-ju house [standard] b) aka-ju (nice) small house; hut [pejorative if seen as inappropriately small] c) oru-ju large house, hall; [pejorative if seen as inappropriately large/designed] d) eci-ju house [pejorative: too big or not well built/kept] What happens here is that one noun stem –ju is associated with different noun class prefixes such as class 9, 12, 11 and 7 as in 22 a, b, c and d above to modify the meaning of the noun by change of the class. Basically, what we see here is meaning modification or expansion of the same noun by the change of class. 57 2.2.3.2 Noun stems formed from verb stems/roots The majority of derived nouns are ‘deverbative’. This means adding a nominal suffix [or nominal class prefix] to simple verbal root/stem forms derived nouns (cf. Polomé, 1967: 77). There are different ways of deriving nouns from verb stems, some of which include: i) using a nominalizing suffix {-i}. ii) using a nominalizing suffix {–o}. iii) by use of suffix {-e}. 2.2.3.2.1 Using a nominalizing suffix {-i}. In Bantu languages the pattern {-i} is associated with derived nouns of different classes. For instance: 23. Luganda o-mu-som-i ‘reader’ from soma ‘read,’ Werner (1919: 200). Ci-Ruuri o-βu-lim-i ‘cultivation’ from lima ‘cultivate,’ Massamba (1977: 75). Sisumbwa li-lamuz*-i ‘huge or strong judge’ from lamula, ‘judge’ Kahigi (2003: 18). Ki-Swahili ki-ongoz-i ‘leader’ from ongoza ‘lead.’ ma-lez*-i ‘up bringing’ from lea ‘bring up’ Polomé (1967: 77) The agentive nominalizer suffix {-i} mostly indicates the person or thing performing the action as for derived nouns in Luganda, Sisumbwa and Kiswahili above. Nouns derived 58 with suffix {-i} belonging to class prefix {βu-}, like in Ci-Ruuri above, express abstract concepts, while those belonging to class prefix {ma-}, like in Ki-Swahili, express totality, (cf. Polomé 1967: 77). However, one should note that the use of nominalizing suffix {-i} may fricatize some consonants, i.e. nouns indicated by an asterisk above, their root-final consonant /l/ changes to [z], (cf. Kahigi 2003: 19). Apart from the agentive suffix {-i} Polomé (1967: 77) reports that the allomorph {-ji} is also used to derive nouns from verbs. He gives examples of nouns like m-soma-ji ‘habitual reader’ from soma ‘read’, u-omba-ji ‘professional begging’ from omba ‘beg’, ki-nywa-ji ‘beverage’/’a drink’ from kunywa ‘drink. Werner (1919: 43) concludes that nouns derived by the Ki-Swahili suffix {-ji} taking the {m(u)-} or {u-} class prefixes denote habitual actions. While on the one hand we agree with the idea by both Werner (1919) and Polomé (1967) that Ki-Swahili nouns derived by suffix {-ji} taking the {m(u)-} class prefix denote habitual action, on the other hand we are of the opinion that Ki-Swahili nouns derived by suffix {-ji} taking the {u-} class prefix denote ‘manner of doing something’ and not habitual actions as Werner (1919) states. We think that nouns derived by { u-} NPX, and {ji-} suffix sound more of abstract nouns than nouns denoting habitual actions. 59 2.2.3.3 Nouns formed with the nominalizing suffix (-o} Polomé (1967: 78) contends that the suffix {-o} in most Bantu languages has essentially a double function, indicating a) the implement which performs the action, and b) the ultimate result of the action. He gives the following examples from Ki-Swahili: 24. (i) ki-zib-o ‘cork’ from ziba ‘stop up’. (ii) ki-funik-o ‘lid’ from funika ‘cover’ (iii) ma-lish-o ‘pasture’ from lisha ‘feed’ (iv) m-shon-o ‘seam’ from shona ‘sew’ (v) u-kom-o ‘end/conclusion’ from koma ‘stop/reach the end’ (vi) siki-o ‘ear’ from sikia ‘hear’ Here we should note that the suffix {-o} occurs with a number of class prefixes, the {ki}, {ma-}, {m-}, {u-}, Ø, etc. The two afore-mentioned functions of the suffix {-o}, according to Polomé, may be assumed simultaneously in all of the above examples. However, the later statement seems to be too general since (vi) above does not accommodate role (b) that is the ultimate result of the action. The noun siki-o ‘ear’, masiki-o ‘ears’ is its plural, simply means the implement which is used to perform the action –kusikia ‘to hear’ but not the ultimate result of the action. 2.2.3.4 Nouns formed with the nominalizing suffix {-e} Kahigi (2003: 20) gives the following examples of Sisumbwa nouns: 25. βu-konde ‘gentleness’ from konda ‘become soft’ 60 βu-moote ‘nice smell’ from moota ‘smell nice’ Regarding the function of the suffix {-e} Polomé (1967: 79-80) argues that deverbatives formed by this suffix indicate the person or object undergoing the action. While in Sisumbwa, according to Kahigi (2003: 20), the nominalizing suffix {-e} is used to form derived nouns with the {βu-} class prefix only, in Ki-Swahili it is used with a variety of noun class prefixes i.e. m-tum-e ‘apostle’ from tuma ‘send’, ki-umb-e ‘creature’ from umba ‘create’ u-tum-e ‘apostasy’ from tuma ‘send’, etc. Other productive patterns in deriving nouns from verbs include: use of suffix {-u}, i.e. in Ki-Swahili word m-tukuf*-u ‘exalted person’ from tukuka ‘be exalted’. An asterisk means that there is a morphophonological process by which a nominalizing suffix {-u} has changed the voiceless velar plosive into the voiceless labial-dental fricative. However, apart from affixation patterns, other processes like reduplication and compounding are also productive in Bantu nominal derivation (cf. Polomé 1967; Kahigi 2003; Nurse and Phillipson 2003). 2.3 Summary In this chapter we have attempted an overview description of the Bantu noun structure by examining three aspects, namely noun augments, noun class prefixes and derivation. It is evident that the canonical structure of underived nouns takes the rule (AUGMENT) + NPX + STEM, while the rule for derived nouns is (AUGMENT) + NPX + STEM/ROOT + (NOMINALIZING SUFFIX). Moreover, it is important to point out 61 that morphophonological, syntactic and semantic criteria become handy in the description of Bantu noun structure. 62 CHAPTER THREE THE KI-NATA NOUN STRUCTURE 3.1 Introduction In this chapter we describe three aspects of the noun structure in Ki-Nata, namely AG, NPx and nominal derivation. In augmented nouns the canonical shape of a Ki-Nata noun with AG, ie. AG- NPx- NOUN STEM/ROOT- SUFFIX, shows that it is the AG that precedes the NPx. If a noun has an AG, NPx and a noun stem/root, the noun stem/root occupies the third slot and the noun suffix fills the last slot. We will now begin with the AG. 3.2 AGs in Ki-Nata Nouns In Ki-Nata, the AG manifests itself as a single vowel preceding a CV prefix. However, nouns in the locative classes 16-18, including some nouns in class 5 with stems beginning with consonants never take AGs. Moreover, proper nouns in this language occur without AGs. Examples of proper nouns are nouns denoting names of people, place names and country names as shown below: 26. a) Wambura b) Ro:βanda Saβiti Mo:tukeri Nyakorema Ti:rina c) βuraya ‘Europe’ βu:ngereca ‘United Kingdom’ 63 As we can see, proper nouns in 26(a-c) never take AGs. Let us now go back and examine the following data in which AGs appear with nouns: 27. Class AG 1 1a 3 5 Examples Class AG Examples Gloss 2 aβâ:to person(s)/people umwa:ná aβa:ná child(ren) υmusa:ní aβasa:ní friend(s) βa:tá:ta father(s) o/u/υ omô:to ta:tá a 2a misé:ŋge βamíse:ŋge so:kόro βasó:koro o/u/υ/ɔ omoté 4 e/i/Ι/ε emeté grandfather(s) tree(s)/medicine(s) umwε:rí imyε:rí υmutwé Imitwé head(s) ɔ mɔrɔrɔ εmέrɔrɔ fire(s) amâ:to people (aug) e/i/(Ι) erê:to 6 a iryo:βá - riso:sό 7 paternal aunt(s) moon(s)/month(s) sun amasό:so lung(s) eβériβici girdle(s) ikya:rá iβya:rá finger(s) Ιkímwe:so Ιβímwe:so sacrifice(s) eγekúβa eβekúβa εkέmεrɔ εβέmεrɔ ε/e/i/Ι ekériβici 8 ε/e/i/Ι chest(s)/bosom(s) throat(s) 64 9 11 amborí ca:mborí goat(s) á:nda cá:nda stomach(s) aɲakwa:há ca:ɲakwa:há armpit(s) aŋguγé ca:ŋguγé baboon(s) atwí:γa cátwi:γa giraffe(s) ca:siko day(s) o/u/υ orosíko 10 10 urwa:mbé 12 13 14 a - gossip υrute:wa ca:te:wa broom(s) υrúβaru cá:mbaru rib(s) ɔ rɔhɔ:ŋgɔ ca:hɔ:ŋgɔ sieve(s) aka:ná 19 /i/e/Ι/ε ihya:ná child(ren) (dim) aγácoγu ehécoγu elephant(s)(dim) aγasáka Ιhisáka bush(es) (dim) akárɔrɔ εhέrɔrɔ fire(s) (dim) o/u/υ otόcoγu elephants (dim) utwa:ná children (dim) utwo:má iron bars (dim) υtumo:rí calves (dim) o/u/υ oβόruγi manner of cooking oβoβése quality unripe if being 65 Class AG 15 Examples Class AG Examples Gloss uβwá:ŋgo quickness υβucόmu beauty/goodness o/u/υ okohéta to pass ukwa:ŋgá to refuse υkuhέ:cεra to breath oγoténa to cut uγwa:sáma to open the mouth wider υγukáma 15a 16 o/u/υ okόγoro milking 6 a amáγoro leg(s) υkuβόko amaβόko arm(s) oγotwé amatwé ear(s) ha:sé down/on the ground 17 18 ha:rí there háno here kume:cá on the table komutwé on the head kúmotoka on the car múno in here mu:rí in there 66 Class AG 19 Examples Class AG i/Ι/e/ε/ Examples Gloss mwiβa:ncá in the cattle shed ihya:ná children (dim) Ιhíta:mbara 20 rags (dim) ehéβeγu seeds (dim) ehécoγu elephants (dim) εhέmεrɔ throats (dim) amasúβe husband(s) (aug) uγwa:ná ama:ná child(ren) (aug) υγuta:rό amata:rό river(s) (aug) ɔ γɔrɔrɔ amárɔrɔ fire(s) (aug) o/u/υ oγosúβe 6 a AGs which manifest as single vowels are identified as [i-], [u-], [e-], [ε-], [o-], [ɔ-] and [a-]. AGs [Ι-] and [υ-] appear here as variant forms of [i-] and [u-] respectively. It is also evident that some nouns (cf. nouns in classes 1a/2a, 9/10, 16-18 and some nouns in class 5, whose stems begin with consonants, occur without AGs. As we can see, in this language, nouns in locative classes (16-18) and proper nouns (1a/2a) never take AGs. Since there is no evidence from this language, which shows that nouns in locative classes, or class 1a/2a occur with AGs, we can conclude that it is the nature of nouns in these classes to occur without AGs. From our informants’ intuitions, it would seem that these nouns initially occurred with AGs but due to diachronic changes the nouns have lost their AGs. 67 Viewed superficially, one tends to think that the vowel in class 9 is the AG, but the fact is, this vowel is part of the NPx, just like the ca in class 10 (cf. our discussion in §. 1.11.4 in Chapter One). In all classes other than 1a/2a, 9/10 and 16-18, strict vowel harmony is observed between the AG and the prefix vowel. The AG is [e-] when the prefix vowel is [e-] as in classes (4), (5), (6), (7), (8) and (19). The AG is [ε] when the prefix vowel is [ε] as in classes 4, 7, 8 and 19; and the AG is [ɔ] when the prefix vowel is also [ɔ] as classes 3, 11and 20. When the vowel of the prefix is [o-] the AG is also [o-] as in classes (1), (3), (11), (13), (14), (15/15a) and (20); and when the AG is [a-] the vowel of the prefix is also [a-] as in classes (2), (6) and (12). This paradigm shows that AGs are in complete harmony with the prefix vowels. As explained earlier, (cf. examples 6 and 7 in Chapter One), generalization pertaining to AGs [i-]/[I-] and [u-]/[υ-] are accounted for by rule ordering. We have two rules applying here namely glide formation and vowel harmony. The glide formation rule applies before the vowel harmony rule. Once the glide formation rule has applied, vowel harmony rule cannot. Observe the following nouns: 28. Cl 3 and 4 /umuε:rí/ → [umwε:rí] /imiε:rí/ → [imyε:rí] ‘moon(s)/month(s)’ /umua:ndó/ → [umwa:ndó] /imia:ndó/ → [imya:ndó] ‘cloth(es)’ Cl 7 and 8 /iγio:tó/ → [iγyo:tó] /iβio:tó/ → [iβyo:tó] 68 ‘fire(s) for warming’ /ikia:rá/ → [ikya:rá] /iβia:rá/ → [iβya:rá] ‘finger(s)’ Cl 11 /urua:mbé/ → [urwa:mbé] ‘gossip’ As we can see, AGs [i] and [u] are accounted for by glide formation rule. Once glide formation rule has applied, vowel harmony rule cannot. Glide formation occurs when prefix vowels, underlyingly /i/ or /u/ change into glides [y] or [w] respectively when followed by non-identical vowels. AGs [Ι-] and [υ-] are accounted for by vowel harmony. AG is [Ι-] or [υ-] when the prefix vowel is /i/ or /u/ respectively. Observe what happens in the following nouns: 29. Cl. 7 and 8 /iγítanda/ → [Iγítanda] /iβítanda/ → [Iβítanda] /iγi:sé/ → [Iγi:sé] /iβi:sé/ → [Iβi:sé] ‘bedstead(s)’ ‘flywhisks’ Cl. 3 /imite:wá/ → [Imite:wá] ‘brooms’ (aug). Cl. 19 /ihítamba:ra/ → [Ιhítamba:ra] ‘rags. dim’ Cl. 1 /umukári/ → [υmukári] ‘woman’ Cl. 2 /umukáma/ → [υmukáma] ‘wind’ Cl. 15 /ukuγá:mba/ → [υkuγá:mba] ‘to speak’ From the above data, it is evident that AGs are underlyingly /i-/ or /u-/ but they are lowered to [Ι-] and [υ-] respectively when they occur word initially if the noun stems 69 begin with consonants. (cf. rule 5 in Chapter One). In this regard we can still say that there is a partial vowel harmony between AG [Ι-] or [υ-] and the prefix vowels. 3.2.1 Use and Omission of the AG One of our working hypothesis claims that in Ki-Nata the use or non-use of AG in a particular construction is essentially determined by semantic and syntactic attributes. An attempt is made here to determine whether or not this is true. Let us examine various constructions and see under which contexts AGs are retained or omitted: 30) a. u-mw-aná na-koréra. AG-NPx1- ST TA-crying ‘the child is crying.’ b). mw-aná na-koréra? NPx1-ST TA-crying ‘child is crying?’ c). *mw-aná na-koréra. NPx1-ST TA-crying ‘child is crying.’ d). o - mo - t(e)7 o - γw - í:re. ‘the tree has fallen’ AG-NPx1-ST SC2-fall-PERF e). mo - t(e) o - γw - í:re? NPx1-ST SC2-fall-PERF ‘tree has fallen?’ f). *mo - t(e) o - γw - í:re. NPx1-ST SC2-fall-PERF ‘tree has fallen’ 31 a). N - gor - (e) a - ma - βere. ‘I should buy some milk’ 1PER-buy-MA AG-NPx6-ST b). N-gor-(e) ma-βere? 1PER-buy-MA NPx6-ST 7 ‘should I buy any milk?’ The brackets mean that the relevant vowel deletes before the AG in the adjacent word. 70 c). * N-gor-(e) ma-βere. 1PER-buy-MA NPx6-ST 32 a). ruγ-(a) o- βo - kima. cook AG-NPx14-ST b). βo - kima ke? NPx14-ST which c) *o - βo - kima ke? AG-NPx14-ST which ‘I should buy any milk’ ‘cook some stiff porridge’ ‘which ugali?’ ‘which some ugali’ d). o- βo - kima βw(a) o - βu - ta:ka. ‘some ugali of cassava flour’ AG-NPx14-ST A-CONECT AG-NPx14-ST 33 a). mo-suβe ke a - het-ire hano? NPx1-ST which SC1-Pass-PERF here b). mo-suβe wond(e) a - het - ire; n’timweche. NPx1-ST certain SC1-Pass-PERF ‘which man has passed here?’ ‘a certain man passed; I don’t know him’ c). o-mo-suβe wond(e) a - het - ire; n’timweche. ‘a certain man passed; AG-NPx1-ST certain SC1-Pass-PERF I don’t know him’ 34 a) N - no: - kuγeγ(a) e - γi - ko Wambura? 1PER-TA-carry AG-NPx7-ST b) N - te - kuγeγa γi-ko. 1PER-TA-carry NPx7-ST c) *N - te - kuγeγ(a) e -γi – ko. 1PER-NEG-carry AG-NPx7-ST d) ri: -si u-mw-ana! eat-Cas AG- NPx1-ST ‘Wambura, will you carry the calabash?’ ‘I won’t carry any calabash’ ‘I won’t carry the calabash’ ‘cause the child to eat’ e) N - te - korisi mw-ana. 1PER-NEG-feed NPx1-ST ‘I won’t cause any child to eat’ f) Ne - korisi u-mw-ana. 1PER-feed NPx1-ST ‘I will cause the child to eat’ 71 35. a) Uyo n(i) o:-mo - γo:ndo. That CP AG-NPx2-ST ‘that is the farm’ b) Uyo m’mo - γo:ndo? That CP NPx2-ST ‘Is that a farm’? c) *Uyo m’o-mo - γo:ndo? That CP NPx2-ST ‘Is that a farm’? d) tanyi’mo - γo:ndo. Isn’t NPx2-ST 36. ‘that isn’t the farm’ e) * tanyi’o-mo - γo:ndo. Isn’t AG-NPx2-ST ‘that isn’t the farm’ f) Kiyo n(i) e: -kí - γeso. That CP AG-NPx7-ST ‘that is the knife’ g) Kiyo ŋ’kí - γeso? That CP NPx7-ST ‘Is that a knife?’ h) tanyi’kí - γeso. isn’t NPx7- ST ‘It isn’t a knife’ i) *tanyi’e – kí - γeso. isn’t AG-NPx7-ST ‘It isn’t the knife’ a) kira ŋ-gúγe. every 9-baboon ‘every baboon’ b) *kira a-ŋ-gúγe. ‘every baboon’ c) kira ro-síko. every AG-11-day ‘every day’ d) *kira o-ro-síko. ‘every day’ We now examine more closely these constructions to see in which contexts the AG is omitted or retained. Examination of sentences 30(a-f) shows that sentences 30(a and d) are affirmative, while those in 30(b and e) are interrogative. While the affirmative sentences (30a and d) are acceptable in Ki-Nata, sentences 30(c and f) are unacceptable. This is because nouns in affirmative sentences such as those in 30(c and f) have to 72 appear with AGs. The AG disappears when such sentences are changed into questions (cf. 30b and e). As in sentences 30(a-f), sentence 31(a), which is affirmative, the AG is obligatorily required. Sentence 31(c) is unacceptable because nouns in affirmative sentences have to appear with AGs. When 31(a), which is affirmative, changes into an interrogative sentence, i.e. 31(b), the noun loses its AG. In sentences 32(a-d) we have the following forms of sentences: Sentence 32(a) is a normal affirmative, while 32(d) is the reply of the question in 32(c). As we can see, in all affirmative sentences (32a and d) nouns appear with AGs. On the other hand, while sentence 32(b) is acceptable as a Wh-question in this language, sentence 32(c), which is also a Wh-question, is unacceptable simply because the noun has occurred without an augment. What we need to point out here is that in the affirmative sentences 30(a and d), 31(a) and 32(a and d) the presence of the augment specifies the noun in question. This means in 30(a and d) the speaker has the knowledge of “the child” and “the tree” in question; in 31(a) the speaker knows which kind of milk he/she is talking about; likewise in sentences 32(a and d) the speaker has some prior information on the type of ugali he/she is talking about. The omission of AG in some interrogative sentences renders the objects (child, tree, milk and ugali) unspecified. Furthermore, we emphasize here that 73 while in any case in Wh-questions 32(b) and 33(a) the noun must appear without an AG, it is possible for the noun in questions 30(b and e) and 31(b) to appear with an AG. However, this is possible only if the speaker has prior information on the noun (umwa:na and omote). Sentence 33(a) is a question followed by two replies; 33(b) and 33(c). The same treatment of the omission of AG in Wh-questions is maintained in sentence 33(a). Unlike languages like Ki-Nande (cf. Progovic 1993) which, whenever a negative verb is used a noun must appear without an AG, sentences 33(b) and 33(c) are acceptable in Ki-Nata. The two sentences have a relationship of some kind in that the speaker in both sentences talks of an unknown person. However, while the speaker in sentence 33(b) does not have a clear image of the man who passed there, the speaker in sentence 33(c) does. It is in view of this difference that other Ki-Nata speakers would reject sentence 33(b) as being ungrammatical simply because a noun, in this case denoting the unknown person, should not carry an AG. To recapitulate, sentence 33(c) is acceptable in this language if a speaker has a clear image of the man in question, though unknown by name. Sentence 34(a) is a request followed by two negative replies in 34(b) and 34(c). In making this request the speaker assumes that the hearer knows or sees the calabash in question. Because the speaker has special information regarding the calabash, this noun, therefore, appears with the AG. Negative sentence 34(c) is unacceptable in this language simply because the noun has occurred with an augment. A noun in a negative sentence 74 (particularly when it is before a negative verb) must appear without an AG, which is the case in sentence 34(b). This is because, as we have explained earlier, the speaker in sentence 34(b) does not specify which calabash he/she does not want to carry. In other words, the speaker means that he/she won’t carry any calabash at all. Although both sentences 33(c) and 34 (b) are replies, there is a sharp difference between them. The difference is that while the noun in 33(c) occurs with an AG before a negative verb, in 34(b) the noun loses its AG after a negative verb. Sentence 34(d) is imperative followed by two negative replies in 34(d and e). In this language nouns in imperative sentences must appear with AG. While the presence of AG in sentence 34(f), shows that the speaker has knowledge of the child in question, sentence 34(e) does not specify which particular child the speaker is not willing to feed. Sentences 35(a-f) involve the use of a copula verb, which has the underlying form /N/ with different surface manifestations such as [m’] and [ŋ’] due to homorganic nasal assimilation. Unlike other languages like Luganda (cf. H&K 1993), in which the use of a copula in any construction does not permit a noun to occur with an AG, in Ki-Nata sentences 35(a and f) show that in affirmative sentences, especially those with deictic words (i.e. uyo ‘that’), the copula and the AG co-occur. As can be seen, the AG deletes in question sentences with affirmative copula (35b and g), and in sentences with prenominally negativized copula (35d and h). The reverse of this is the ungrammaticality of sentences (35c, e and i). 75 As we can see, the use of kira restricts the use of the AG in 36(b and d). It seems that kira and the AG are mutually exclusive morphemes. The later means that while the AG specifies the noun kira, does not, a result of which the two cannot co-exist in the same NP. 3.2.2 Summary Our discussion regarding the function of AG has revealed that in Ki-Nata the AG serves two major roles, which are syntactic and semantic. The syntactic role operates when syntactic operators such as negation, Wh-enclitics, post-verbal position (copula), etc. control non-augmented forms. The specificity concept, as we have seen, is partially reflected in semantic/pragmatic primes. However, although these two roles show up, the surveyed Ki-Nata data show that at some point the two roles function together. In other words, the AG function largely depends upon the context –the retention or deletion of AG depends on whether the speaker or hearer has prior information of the object in question. 3.3 The Noun Class Prefix in Ki-Nata In this subsection we identify and describe specific noun prefixes in relation to their noun class numbers. We also describe surface representations of various NPxs. Since any attempt to classify Bantu nouns cannot ignore the semantic aspect, we also look into this aspect and see what concepts are portrayed by these prefixes. Following the 76 approach of Bantu noun classification established by Bleek (1862) and modified by Meinhof (1932), Ki-Nata nouns can be classified as follows: 37. Class prefix examples 1 mu- 1a 2 2a 3 4 5 6 7 βa- βa- mu- mi- ri- ma- ki- Gloss u-mu-a:ná → [umwa:ná] child u-mu-kári → [υmukári] woman/wife ta:tá → [ta:tá] father misénge → [miséŋge] paternal aunt a-βa:-ná → [aβa:ná] children a-βa-kári → [aβakári] women βa:tá:ta → [βa:tá:ta] father βa:míse:nge → [βa:míse:ŋge] paternal aunt u-mu-e:rí → [umwε:rí] moon/month u-mu-ô:yo → [umwô:yo] heart i-mi-e:rí → [imyε:rí] moons/months i-mi-ô:yo → [imyô:yo] hearts rí-iβuri → [rí:βuri] feather ri:-so:sό → [ri:so:sό] lung a-má-βuri → [amáβuri] feathers a-ma-sό:so → [amasό:so] lungs i-ki-a:rá → [ikya:rá] finger i-kí-mue:so → [Ιkímwe:so] sacrifice 77 Class prefix examples 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 βi- N- N- Gloss i-βi-a:rá → [iβya:rá] fingers i-βí-mue:so → [Ιβímwe:so] sacrifices a-m-borí → [amborí] goat a-ɲ-akwa:há → [aɲakwa:há] armpit ci-a:m-borí [ca:mborí] goats → ci-a:ɲ-akwahá → [ca:ɲakwa:há] armpits u-ru-a:mbé → [urwa:mbé] gossip u-rú-βaru → [υrúβaru] rib a-ka-:ná → [aka:ná] child (dim) a-ka-mo:rí → [akamo:rí] calf (dim) u-tu-a:ná → [utwa:ná] children (dim) u-tu-mo:rí → [υtumo:rí] calves (dim) u-βu-á:ngo → [uβwá:ŋgo] quickness u-βu-cόmu → [υβucόmu] u-ku-a:ngá → [ukwa:ŋgá] to refuse u-ku-hέ:cεra → [υkuhέ:cεra] to breath ku- u-ku-βόko → [υkuβόko] arm ha- ha:sé → [ha:sé] down/on the ground ha:rí → [ha:rí] there ku:maγúta → [ku:maγúta] on the oil kú:mance → [ku:mance] on the water ru- ka- tu- βu- ku- ku- beauty/goodness 15a 78 Class prefix examples 18 19 20 mu- hi- γu- Gloss mu:maγúta → [mu:maγúta] in the oil mu:mance → [mu:mance] in the water i-hi-a:ná → [ihya:ná] children (dim) i-hí-ta:mbara → [Ιhíta:mbara] rags (dim) u-γu-a:ná → [uγua:ná] child (aug) u-γu-ta:rό → [υγuta:rό] river (aug) 3.3.1 Data Analysis A close look on this data tends to reveal the following facts: First, some NPxs have similar shapes: for instance, mu is found in class 1 and class 3; ku is found in classes 15 and 15a. As we pointed out earlier, classes 16-18 are locative classes. The mu- in class 18 and ku- in class 17, therefore, are locative class markers. The fact that the same forms appear in these NPxs, (mu- for example, appears as NPx for class 1, 3 and 18 and kuappears both as NPx for class 15 and 17), denotes that in this language the semantic criterion of noun classification has been blurred. Second, the difference between classes 1a/2a and 1/2 is that while nouns included in classes 1a/2a are those denoting kinship terms such as ma:mé ‘uncle’, ta:tá ‘father’etc. and proper nouns like Masato, Winyanya etc., classes 1/2 carry common names denoting human beings, eg. umwa:ná ‘child’, etc. Classes 15 and 15a differ in that while in class 15 we have gerundive nouns, class 15a includes nouns denoting two body parts, which are υkuβόko ‘arm’ and okόγoro ‘leg’. Third, following what we discussed in Chapter One (cf. §.1.10.4), we emphasize that the NPx in class 9 is a vowel followed by a nasal (VC) while in class 10 79 it is a CVC form. However, both NPxs are underlyingly represented as N in the above data. Four, locative markers in classes 17 (ku-) and 18 (mu-) precede prefix markers in italic form (ma and mɔ). 3.3.1.1 Pairing system and a brief semantic survey From the above data we note that Ki-Nata has a total number of 20 noun classes marked by different Independent Prefixes (IPs). The NPxs in this language form several singular-plural pairs as shown below: Singular 1 1a 3 5 7 9 11 12 15a 20 Plural 2 2a 4 6 8 10 13 14 19 Figure 2: Noun class pairings in Ki-Nata. The above figure shows that class 1 mu- and class 2 βa- form one pair. Class 1a ø and class 2a βa- form another pair. Other pairs are: 3 mu- and 4 mi-, 5 ri- and 6 ma, 7 ki- and 8 βi-, 9 N- and 10 N-, 11 ru- and 10 N-, 12 ka- and 13 tu-, 15a ku- and 6 ma-, 7 ki- and 19 hi-, and 20 γu- and 6 ma-. Class 12 ka- can also pair with classes 19 hi- and 8 βi- all of which are diminutive markers. However, in some cases class 12 ka- can also form diminutive pairs with class 14 βu- and class 11 ru-. This shows that, in this language, the classification of nouns on the basis of singularity and plurality is rather opaque. Suffice 80 it to say that various surface representations of prefixes that manifest themselves in this language are dealt with in the subsequent subsections. 3.3.1.1.1 A Brief Semantic Survey Our discussion in Chapter Two revealed that nouns in Bantu languages are partially semantically motivated. This is in the sense that nouns belonging to the same class tend to share some semantic features. In Ki-Nata, like in most other Bantu languages, nouns whose singulars begin with m(u)- and whose plurals begin with βa- (or wa- in languages like Kiswahili) generally refer to human beings or animates; nouns beginning with ru(with cognates like lu- or u in other Bantu languages) generally refer to things that are thin, broader, elongated or circular; nouns which begin with βu- (with cognates like u, vu or bu in other Bantu languages) generally refer to abstract concepts. However, in this language, some nouns like υβuta ‘bow’, υβuka:no ‘sesame’, υβurwe ‘finger millet’ which are in this class, refer to concrete things. In short, we can say that nouns in KiNata are partially-semantically motivated. Although in the discussion above we tried to show that Ki-Nata noun classification is to some extent semantically motivated, the semantic criteria cannot be seriously considered as sufficient in the classification of all noun classes in this language. There are some obvious reasons: One, based on our argument on noun classification as shown in 37, (cf. §.3.2 above), we can say that there is some overlapping of NPxs, e.g. mu- in class 1 is repeated in class 3 and in class 18; ku- in classes 15, 15a and 17, where each mu- and ku- 81 in the respective classes denotes nouns of different kinds. This makes the semantic criterion unstable in the classification of Ki-Nata nouns. Two, the overlapping of the singular- plural parings of noun classes (cf. §.3.2.1.1) is another point that weakens the semantic criterion as a concrete parameter for Ki-Nata noun classification. Observe the following data on the singular-plural pairings of diminutive classes: 38. a) Class 12 Class 19 akaβόri ‘small goat’ (+derog, - derog) b) Class 1 eheβόri ‘minute; nice goats’ Class 8 akaβόri ‘small goat’ (+derog, - derog) c) Class 12 eβeβόri ‘small goats (+derog) Class 13 akaβόri ‘small goat’ (+derog, - derog) otoβόri ‘small goats’ (+derog, -derog) d) Class 7 ekeβόri ‘small goat’ (+derog.) e) Class 11 Class 8 eβeβόri ‘small goats’ (+derog) Class 14 oroβόri ‘dwarf; small goat’ (+derog.) oβoβόri ‘(minute) goats’ (+derog.) f) Class 12 Class 14 akaβόri ‘small goat’ (+derog, - derog) oβoβόri ‘(minute) goats’ (+derog.) 82 As we can see in the above data, there are varying degrees of diminutiveness. However, the singular- plural paring of diminutive classes has no clear demarcation line since one diminutive class, 12 ka for instance, pairs with various classes (cf. cl 8, 13, 14 and 19 above). This shows that the semantic criterion is no longer a concrete parameter for KiNata noun classification. Class 19 hi-, originally *pi- in Proto-Bantu, is a diminutive class. However, in Ki-Nata the prefix in class 19 differs from other Bantu languages. The difference is that while in Proto-Bantu the prefix in class 19 denotes singularity (cf. Werner 1919: 62) in Ki-Nata it denotes plurality. Although, classes 7 and 12 have their plural counterparts, they are still used alternatively as singular prefixes for class 19. It is important to point out here that, given the data we have, there is no clear demarcation line in the pairing of diminutive classes. Therefore, with regard to the pairing of diminutive classes, one can easily conclude that clear distinctions have been blurred. Three, it is also evident that in classifying Ki-Nata nouns by semantic criterion not only is it difficult but also impossible to classify nouns by grouping them into classes such as diminutives, augmentatives, inanimate, body parts, concrete things, etc. For example, class one which is said to be the human class has other entities like Nyámuha:nga ‘God’, rinani ‘ogre in Ki-Nata tales’ etc. that feature in this class. 83 The above arguments reveal that the semantic criterion in the classification of Ki-Nata nouns has a lot of problems. Some of the reasons that account for non-stability of the semantic criterion of noun classification in most Bantu languages (cf. Massamba, 1993: 606), include: First, due to passage of time meanings change, expand, shift or get different shades. This makes it possible for other things to be either included in or excluded from the meanings of words. Second, due to language contact and language adaptation loan words can interfere with the already established morphological structure of the target language. Lastly, language development process may trigger changes in the forms of the words themselves. These and other problems that relate to them, interfere with the semantic criterion of Ki-Nata and other Bantu noun classification. It is the researcher’s view that the classification of nouns in Ki-Nata using the semantic criterion is opaque and faces lots of irregularities. 3.3.1.2 Surface representation of prefixes Our Ki-Nata data reveal that NPxs have different variant forms (allomorphs) ranging in number between one and five. Except for class 1a, which occurs without any prefix, NPxs that exhibit only one basic form are in classes 2 βa-, 2a βa-, 6 ma-and 16 ha-. We will now present the variant forms of NPxs with an account of the morphophonological processes involved: 84 Class 1 and 3 are marked by the prefix mu- and variant forms namely mu-, mw-, mo- and mɔ. However, in certain phonetic environments the prefix mu- loses the vowel u- as the case in the following words: 39. Class 1 Class 3 /umuháβe/ → [υmháβe] ‘poor person’ /umukári/ → [υmkári] ‘woman’ or ‘wife’ /umúnwa/ → [υmnwa] ‘mouth’ /umutwé/ → [υmtwé] ‘head’ This process can be informally accounted for by the following rule: Rule 6: /u/ → [ø]/ N____ [+C] Formally this rule can be represented as: +syll -cons +high +back → [ø] +cons -syll +nas +bil _____ +cons + -syll That is, a high back vowel deletes when it occurs between a bilabial nasal and any consonant. The deletion of /u/ in the above examples is accounted for by the phonological process known as syncope; a phonological process that allows segment deletion in a word in an environment other than the final position (cf. Massamba 1996: 98) 85 Another variant form in classes 1 and 3 is the prefix mw-. Observe the following examples: 40. Cl 1 Cl 3 /umuá:na / → [umwa:ná] ‘child’ /umuí:ti/ → [umwí:ti] ‘killer’ /umuε:rí/ → [umwε:rí] ‘moon’ or ‘month’ /umuô:yo/ → [umwô:yo] ‘heart’ The variant form mw- can be accounted for by the glide formation rule. This can be informally written as: Rule 7(a): /u/ → [w]/___ +V 7(b) V → V:/mw____ Cond: V ≠ u As we can see, glide formation in rule 7(a) triggers vowel lengthening as seen in 7(b). This rule can be formally represented as: V1 +syll -cons +high +back → - syll -cons ______ V2 syll + -cons Condition: V1 ≠ V2 That is, a high back vowel changes into a glide [w] when followed by a non-identical vowel across morpheme boundary. Another allomorph that surfaces in the above mentioned classes is the mo-. Examples of nouns are: 86 41. Cl 1 Cl 3 /umukiri:sito/ → [omokíri:sito] ‘a christian’ /umusúβe/ → [omosúβe] ‘man’/‘husband’ /umukéra/ → [omokéra] ‘tail’ /umuté/ → ‘tree’/‘medicine’ [omoté] As we have already pointed out, the underlying form in classes 1 and 3 is mu-. What happens with the variant mo- in the noun /umuté/ ‘tree’ is that the prefix vowel /u/ becomes [o] when a noun stem/root contains a close-mid front vowel, /e/. This phonological fact can informally be presented as: Rule 8: /u/ → [o]/____ [e] NPx STEM Formally the same rule is represented as: +syll -cons +high +back → -high -low +syll ____ -cons -high -low -back That is, a high back vowel is realized as a close-mid back vowel if a noun stem contains a close-mid front, [e]. In the nouns /umokíri:sito/ ‘a christian’ and /umosúβe/ ‘man’/‘husband’ the prefix vowel /u/ becomes [o] when the vowel in the noun final position is also a close-mid vowel. As we can see, the prefix vowel in the noun [omokéra] ‘tail’ is in harmony with the vowel in the penultiment syllable. In Ki-Nata the surface form mɔ- occurs in a limited number of nouns. The few examples available are: 87 42. Cl 3 /umurɔrɔ/ → [ɔmɔ rɔrɔ]8 ‘fire’ /umurεmɔ/ → [ɔmɔ rεmɔ] ‘responsibility’ The underlying form in classes 1 and 3 is mu-. As we can see, the prefix vowel /u/ lowers to [ɔ] when a noun ends with an open-mid back vowel. This vowel harmony rule can informally be represented as: Rule 9: /u/ → [ɔ ]/____ [ɔ ] NPx W.F.P Formally this rule can be represented as: +syll -cons +high +back +syll → -high _____ -cons -high -low +back The rule means that a high front vowel is realized as a open-mid back vowel (ɔ) if the noun final position is also occupied by an open-mid back vowels. This occurs whereby at the surface level the prefix vowel acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in word final position. Class 4, which is the plural form for class 3, is marked by four prefixes, namely mi-, my-, me- and mε. As seen in (37) above and from the Proto-Bantu point of view (cf. Chapter One, p.17 example 9), the underlying form is mi-. The allomorphs my-, me- and 8 Note that in a quick speech, the word ɔmɔ rɔrɔ ‘fire’ is articulated as ɔmɔ rrɔ. Thus there is a deletion of segment /ɔ/. 88 mε- manifest in the following examples:8 Note that in a quick speech, the word ɔmɔ rɔrɔ ‘fire’ is articulated as ɔmɔ rrɔ. Thus there is a deletion of segment /ɔ/. 43. Cl 4 a) b) my- me- /imi:aka/ → [imya:ka] ‘years’ /imi:eri/ → [imyε:rí] ‘moons’/‘months’ /imiô:βo/ → ‘holes’ /imite/ [imyô:βo] → [emeté] ‘trees’ /imiγό:ndo/ → [emeγό:ndo] ‘farms’ c) mε /imirɔrɔ/ → [εmέrɔrɔ] ‘fires’ The allomorph my- in (a) above is accounted for by the glide formation rule while allomorphs me- and mε- are accounted for by the vowel harmony rule. In the allomorph my-, the / i-/ glides into /y-/ when it precedes any vowel other than [i]. The above phonological process can informally be represented as follows: Rule 10(a): /i/ → [y]/___ +V 10(b) V → V:/my____ Cond: V ≠ i As we can see, the glide formation in rule 7(a) triggers vowel lengthening as shown in rule 7(b). 89 This rule can formally be represented as: V1 +syll -cons +high -back → -syll -cons ______ V2 syll + + -cons Condition: V1 ≠ V2 That is, when a high front vowel is followed by a non-identical vowel it changes into its corresponding glide across a morpheme boundary. The allomorphs me- and mε- are accounted for by the vowel harmony rule. Prefixes meand mε- manifest whereby at surface level the vowel in the prefix acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in the stem. For the prefix me-, this phonological fact is informally accounted for by the following rule: Rule 11: /i/ → [e]/____ [o]/[e] NPx STEM This rule is formally represented as: +syll -cons +high -back +tense NPx → -low -high +syll _____ -cons -high -low STEM That is, at the surface level the prefix vowel acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in the root/stem. 90 Likewise, the allomorph mε- is accounted for by vowel harmony rule. There is partial vowel harmony between the prefix vowel and the vowel in the root/stem. The prefix vowel acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in the root/stem. This phonological fact be informally represented by the following rule: → [ε]/____ [ɔ ] NPx STEM This rule is formally represented as: Rule 12: /i/ +syll -cons → +high -back +tense NPx -low -high -tense +syll _____ -cons -high -low +back STEM That is, at the surface level the prefix vowel acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in the root/stem. Class 5 has the prefix ri- in its underlying representation. Allomorphs in this class are ri, ry- and re-. Examples are: 44. Cl 5 a) ry- /irio:βá/ → [iryo:βá] ‘sun’ /irio:kí/ → [iryo:kí] ‘smoke’ b) re- /irí:to/ → [eré:to] ‘person (aug) In class 5(a) the high front vowel /i/ glides into [y] when it is followed by a non-identical vowel (cf. Rule 10a and b above). In 5(b) above, the prefix vowel [e] is in harmony with the vowel in the stem, which is also a mid vowel (cf. Rule 11 above). 91 Class 7 is marked by the prefix ki- and five allomorphs, which are ki-, ky-, ke-, γi- γyand γe-. In this class allomorphs are accounted for by either the glide formation rule or vowel harmony rule. Observe examples of nouns from class 7: 45. Cl 7 a) b) c) d) e) ky- ke- γi- γy- γe- /ikia:rá / → [ikya:rá] ‘finger’ /ikia:má/ → [ikya:má] ‘party’ or ‘club’ /ikiβόno/ → [ekeβόno] ‘buttock’ / ikiriβici / → [ekériβici] ‘girdle’ /ikisa:kwá/ → [Ιγisa:kwá] ‘small animal skin’ /ikisára/ → [Ιγisára] ‘roof’ /ikio:tό/ → [iγyo:tό] ‘fire for warming purposes’ /ikie:kíβu/ → [iγye:kíβu] ‘something being burnt’ /ikisόβe/ → [eγesόβe] ‘seed’ /ikitú:mbe/ → [eγetú:mbe] ‘a three-legged traditional stool’ As we can see, while variants ky- in 7(b) and γy- in 7(d) operate under the glide formation rule (cf. Rule 10 above), variants ke- in 7(b) and γe- in 7(e) are operative under the vowel harmony rule (cf. Rule 11 above). Here the prefix vowel occurs as the front or back close-mid vowel if the stem has any close-mid vowel [o]/[e]. Class 15 is marked by the prefix ku- and five allomorphs, which are ku-, kw-, ko-, γu-, γw and γo-. As the case in variant forms in class 7, in this class too, allomorphs are 92 accounted for by either glide formation rule or vowel harmony rule. Let us first observe the following examples of nouns from class 15: 46. Cl 15 a) kw- b) ko- c) γu- d) γw- /ukua:ŋgá/ → [ukwa:ŋgá] ‘to refuse’ /ukua:ndéka/ → [ukwa:ndéka] ‘to write’ /ukurúγa/ → [okorúγa] ‘to cook’ /ukuréma/ → [okoréma] ‘to cultivate’ /ukukáma/ → [υγukáma] ‘milking’ /υkusáβa/ → [υγusáβa] ‘to beg’ /ukua:sáma/ → [uγwa:sáma] ‘to the mouth wider’ /ukua:kéra/ → [uγwa:kéra] ‘to swallow without chewing’ e) γo- /ukuténa/ → [oγoténa] ‘to cut’ /okotwé/ → [oγotwé] ‘ear’ The vowels in variants kw- in 15(a) and γw- in 15(d) operate under the glide formation rule (cf. Rule 7 above). The variants ko- in 15(b) and γo- in 15(e) are operative under the vowel harmony rule (cf. Rule 8 above). That is, at the surface level the prefix vowel acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in the stem. As explained in Chapter One, the sounds [β] and [b], likewise [γ] and [g], in this language are in complementary distribution (cf. §. 10.1.1.1 and §. 10.1.1.2). The voiced bilabial fricative /β/ occurs both word initially and intervocalically. The voiced velar fricative /γ/ is realized as /γ/ in all environments except where it is preceded by a velar 93 nasal [ŋ]. Whenever the phoneme /γ/ is preceded by the velar nasal [ŋ] it surfaces as a voiced velar stop [g]. Like phones [β] and [b], the voiceless velar fricative [γ] and the voiced velar stop [g] occur in environments that are mutually exclusive. This means that whereas [γ] or [β] occurs both word initially and intervocalically, [g] or [b] occurs only in environments preceded by a velar nasal [ŋ] and bilabial nasal [m] respectively (cf. Rule 2 and 3). However, the question we should ask here is, when does the surface form ke- become γein class 7(e) or ko- becomes γo- in class 15? This can be explained by using Dahl’s Law. Dahl’s Law states that a voiceless consonant becomes voiced if the consonant in the next syllable is also voiceless (cf. N&P 2003: 56). However, regarding variants γy- in 7(d) and γw- in 15 (d) we must stress that two rules are in order here. One is the glide formation rule (cf. rule 7 and 10 above) and the other is consonantal voicing (Dahl’s Law). That is consonantal voicing (Dahl’s Law) takes place only after the glide formation rule. The manifestation of [γ] therefore can be accounted for by the following informal rule: Rule 13: 9 /k/ → [γ]/______+Cо9 A diacritic (о) means that a consonant is voiceless. 94 The above rule can formally be represented as: + cons -syll -cont +back -voice +high → +cont +voice _____ + +cons -voice That is, a voiceless velar stop /k/ changes to a voiced velar fricative [γ] if it precedes a syllable with a voiceless consonant. Nouns in class 8 are marked by the prefix βi- and variants βi-, βe- and βy-. Observe the following nouns: 47. Cl 8 a) b) βe- βy- /iβihό:nco/ → [eβehό:nco] ‘plates’ /iβiβό:nda/ → [eβeβό:nda] ‘smoking pipes’ /iβia:rá/ → [iβya:rá] ‘fingers’ /iβia:ndá/ → [iβya:ndá] ‘intestines’ The βi- is the underlying form. The glide y- in (b) is a result of the juxtaposition of two non-identical vowels, /i/ and /a/, thus, the high front vowel /i/ changes into glide /y/ (cf. Rule 10 above). The vowel in variant βe- is in harmony with the vowel in the root. The vowel in the prefix βe- is a mid front vowel if the noun stem has a mid-vowel (cf. Rule 11 above). 95 Class 11 is marked by the prefix ru- and allomorphs such as ru-, rw- and ro-. Let us examine the following examples: 48. Cl 11 a) b) rw- ro- /urua:mbé/ → [urwa:mbé] ‘gossip’ /uruo:ki/ → [υrwo:ki] ‘vapour’ /urusíko/ → [orosíko] ‘day’ /uruhόγo/ → [orohόγo] ‘highland’ As we see, in class 11(a) the variant rw- is accounted for by glide formation rule. That is, a high back vowel /u/ changes into glide /w/ if it is followed by a non-identical vowel (cf. Rule 7 above). The vowel in variant ro- is in harmony with the vowel in the root whereby the vowel in the prefix ro- is a close-mid back vowel, if the noun stem contains a close-mid vowel (cf. Rule 8 above). Nouns in class 12 are marked by the prefix ka- and variants such as γa- and ko-. Below are examples of nouns that manifest such allomorphs. 49. Cl 12 a) γa- /akácoγu/ → [aγácoγu] ‘small elephant’ (+derog, -derog) /akáta:nda/ → [aγáta:nda] ‘small bed stead’ (+derog, derog) b) ko- /akaô:ro/ → [akô:ro] (-derog.) ‘little fire’ /akaô:βo/ → [akô:βo] (-derog.) small; shallow ‘hole’ - 96 What we see in (a) is the operation of Dahl’s Law. Since in the next syllable there is a voiceless plosive, the voiceless velar stop /k/ changes into the voiced velar fricative [γ]. Thus, in (b) above, the prefix vowel /a/ has been deleted and its place filled in with the preceding vowel. As seen in the outputs, deletion of the prefix vowel /a/ is accompanied by a compensatory lengthening of the remaining vowel. Nouns in class 13, which are actually the plurals of nouns in class 12, are marked by the prefix tu- and the variants tw- and to-. These are found in the following examples: 50. Cl 13 a) tw- /utua:ná/ → [utwa:ná] ‘small children’ (+derog, -derog) /utuo:má/ → [utwo:má] ‘small iron bars’ (+derog, -derog) b) to- /utúcoγu/ → [otόcoγu] ‘very small elephants’ (+derog, -derog) /utúγoro/ → [otόγoro] ‘inappropriately small legs’ (+derog) Allomorphs in (a) operate under the glide formation rule (cf. Rule 7 above). In (b) the vowel in the prefix to- is a close-mid back and this is possible if the noun stem has also a close-mid vowel (cf. Rule 8 above). Nouns in class 14 have the prefix βu- and variants βw-and βo-. The variants are evident in the following noun examples: 97 51. Cl 14 a) βw- b) βo- /uβuá:ŋgo/ → [uβwá:ŋgo] ‘quickness’ /uβua:ná/ → [uβwa:ná] ‘childhood’ /uβuγé:mba/ → [oβoγé:mba] ‘manner of rain making’ /uβuβése/ → [oβoβése] ‘quality if unripe’ The glide formation in (a) is due to the fact that the prefix vowel underlyingly /u/ is preceded by the /a/, which is non identical to it. In (b) above, the vowel in the prefix is operative under vowel harmony (cf. Rule 8 above). That is, the vowel in the prefix occurs as a close-mid back vowel if the stem also has a mid-vowel. The Proto-Bantu prefix for class 19 is *pi. What happened in Ki-Nata is a sound shift from the Proto-Bantu /*p/ to [h]. Nouns in this class are, therefore, marked by the prefix hi- and variants hy-, he- and hε-. Observe the examples below: 52. Cl 19 a) hy- /ihia:ná/ → [ihya:ná] (nice) small children [pejorative if inappropriately small] /ihia:rá/ → [ihya:rá] (nice) small fingers [pejorative if inappropriately small] b) he- /ihíβeγu/ → ehéβeγu (nice) small seeds [pejorative if inappropriately small] /ihícoγu/ → ehécoγu (nice) small elephants [pejorative if inappropriately small] c) hε- /ihímεrɔ/ → εhέmεrɔ small throats [pejorative if inappropriately 98 small] In (a) above, the prefix vowel is underlyingly /i/ and it changes into the glide [y], since it is followed by a non-identical vowel (cf. Rule 11). In (b) above, the vowel /e/ in the prefix occurs as the front mid vowel if the stem has a mid-vowel (cf. Rule 12). Likewise, the allomorph hε- is accounted for by vowel harmony. There is partial vowel harmony between the prefix vowel and the vowel in the root/stem. The prefix vowel acquires the same phonetic features as the vowel in the root/stem (cf. Rule 12 above). Nouns in class 20 are marked by the prefix γu- and variants γw- and γo-. Let us examine the following examples: 53. Cl 20 a) γw- /ukua:ná/ → [uγwa:ná] badly shaped/ poorly disciplined child [also pejorative if /uγwε:rí/ → seen as extraordinarily fat] [uγwε:rí] huge moon [pejorative if seen as inappropriately huge] b) γo- /oγosúβe/ → [oγosúβe] extraor[pejorative] dinarily fat; man/husband with bad attributes; /oγoβόri/ → [oγoβόri] huge goat [pejorative if seen as inappropriately huge] Like other allomorphs, in 20(a) above, the allomorph is γw- since the vowel that is underlyingly /i/, has changed into the glide [y] (cf. Rule 11). In 20(b) above, the vowel in the prefix, which is a mid back vowel, is in harmony with the mid vowel in the stem (cf. Rule 9 above). 99 3.3.2 Summary In our discussion on surface representation of NPxs we have revealed that, first, the NPxs in this language are realized in various surface forms. Second, each surface form is derived from its underlying form. Third, all surface forms examined in this work are accounted for by such morphophonological rules as glide formation and vowel harmony. 3.4 Noun Derivation The data we have show that the majority of the nouns in this language are derived from verbs and by changing the noun class. However, this does not mean that there are no other processes involved in noun derivation. Apart from the above mentioned processes, nouns in Ki-Nata are also derived from adjectives, compounding and reduplication processes. 3.4.1 Deriving nouns through change of class In this method of noun derivation, a derived or underived noun stem gets associated with different groups of noun classes. However, when we change an NPx of a derived or underived noun, we must consider the meaning restriction of the stem and/or the derivational affix. To come to grips with this, some examples are in order: 3.4.1.1 Underived 54. Cl 2 υmutwé ‘standard head’ Cl 4 Imitwé ‘standard heads’ 100 Cl12 aγatwé (nice) small head [pejorative if inappropriately small] Cl 8 Iβitwé (nice) small head [pejorative if inappropriately small] Cl 20 υγutwé huge head [pejorative if seen as inappropriately huge] In 3.4.1.1 (a) above as we can see, nouns are derived from nouns by change of noun class. When different NPxs are associated with one stem like -twe in 3.4.1.1 (a) above, the meaning of the derived noun becomes modified. 3.4.1.2 Derived 55. Cl 1 o-mό-rem-i ‘farmer’ Cl 2 a-βá-rem-i ‘farmers’ from réma ‘cultivate’ Cl 12 a-ká-rem-i ‘small farmer’ Cl 13 ‘small farmers (derog.)’ e-hé-rem-i Cl 14 o- βό-rem-i ‘cultivation’ Cl 20 o-γό-rem-i ‘huge farmer’ What we see here is that the change of class in both derived and underived nouns, like in 3.4.1.1 (a) and 3.4.1.2 (b), modifies the meaning of the noun. The striking difference between the derived and underived nouns is that while in underived nouns NPxs are only associated with noun stems, in derived nouns both the NPxs and the suffix must be associated with the verb root –rem-. 101 3.4.2. Deriving nouns from verbal roots/stems Of the methods used to derive nouns, quite typical of Ki-Nata is deriving nouns from verbs. Deriving nouns from verbs involves two major steps. (i) Adding an AG and an NPx before a verb stem/root. This method is commonly known as prefixation. As it will soon be evident, nouns derived from verbs by mere prefixation receive no derivational suffixes. However, derived nouns such as those found in class 5, (cf. §. 3.2), do not have augments. (ii) Adding an AG and an NPx before a verb stem/root and adding a noun suffix at the end of the noun stem/root. In other words, both prefixation and suffixation are applied to the verb stem/root. We should point out here that nouns derived by suffixation are formed when verbal stem/root receive additional extension suffixes. Suffixes in this case seem to take different shapes and different semantic implications. We will now start with nouns derived from verbs by prefixation. 3.4.2.1 (AG) + NPx Deverbatives (nouns derived from verbs) formed by merely adding an NPx to a verbal stem or an AG and an NPx are: STEM/ROOT 56. a) ruγ- DERIVED NOUN ‘cook’ o-ko-rúγ-a ‘manner of cooking’ AG-C15-R-FV 102 b) a:ŋg- ‘refuse’ u-kw-a:ŋg-á ‘manner of refusing’ AG-C15-R-FV c) rem- cultivate o-ko-rém-a AG-C15-R-FV ‘manner of cultivating’ d) ) ruγ- ‘cook’ ko-rúγ-a C15-R-FV ‘to cook’ e) a:ŋg- ‘refuse’ kw-a:ŋg-á C15-R-FV ‘to refuse’ f) ) rem- ‘cultivate’ ko-rém-a C15-R-FV ‘to cultivate’ g) rok- ‘vomit’ ri-rok-á C5-R-FV ‘the vomiting’ h) rem- ‘cultivate’ rí-rem-a 5-R-FV ‘the farming’ i) he:β- ‘chase’ ri- heβ-á 5-R-FV ‘the chasing’ Nouns derived from verbs by prefixation are found in classes 5 and 15, and all refer to non-human objects. As we can see, from 56(a-c) both the AG and the NPx are used in the formation of deverbative nouns. However, we note here that nouns without AGs, like nouns in 56(d-f), differ in meaning from the augmented ones (cf. 56a-c), though they all belong to the same class. As we can see, in this language, if gerundive nouns occur with an AG, they carry a concept of “manner” or “a way of doing something”. We would argue here that the suffix –a in the above examples remains with the status of a final vowel. The reason is that, according to the data we have, all verbs in 56(a-i) have the –a as their final vowel. We form deverbative nouns, therefore, by merely adding a class prefix to the verbal stems. This means that the noun suffix remains that of the verb stem. 103 3.4.2.2 AG + NPx + ROOT + SUFFIX There are many possibilities in which nouns can be derived from verbs using the above morphological processes. Ki-Nata has a number of suffixes that together with the AG and NPx, change verbs into derived nouns. These include the nominalizing suffix -i, and -o, the passive marker -u/-βu, the applicative marker -er- and the negation marker ta-. In this language the suffixes -e and -ε are not productive. 3.4.2.2.1 Pattern -i: Examples are: STEM/ROOT 57. a) γor- DERIVED NOUN ‘buy’ o-mo- γόr-i ‘buyer’ A-C1-R-SUF b) rwan- ‘fight’ υ-mú-rwan-i ‘fighter’ A-C1-R-SUF c) saβ- ‘beg’ υ-mú-saβ-i ‘begger’ A-C1-R-SUF d) ro:t- ‘dream’ υ-mú-ro:t-i ‘dreamer’ A-C1-R-SUF e) andek- ‘write’ u-βw-ándek-i ‘job of writing’ A-C14-R-SUF f) ruγ- ‘cook’ o-βό-ruγ-i ‘job of cooking’ A-C14-R-SUF g) sar- ‘circumcise’ υ-βú- sa:r-i ‘skill of circumcising’ A-C1-R-SUF h) som- ‘read’ υ-βú-som-i ‘job of studying’ A-C1-R-SUF 104 The above examples show that the agentive nominalizer –i together with the NPx omu-, change verbs into derived nouns in classes 1/2 and 14. There are two concepts coming out clearly through the suffix –i. One is that when the agentive nominalizer –i and the prefix omu- in class 1 are affixed to a verb root, the derived noun resulting from this process denotes a doer of an action. This is verified by the derived nouns in 57(a-d). However, the concept of the actor manifests also in derived nouns in class 2 i.e. a-βasaβ-i ‘beggars’ from saβ- ‘beg’; nouns denoting animates in class 9 and 10, i.e. a-mbiem-i ‘predator’ from βyem- ‘hunt’. Also, when the suffix –i and class 14 βu- are affixed to a verb root, the nouns derived pertain to abstract nouns (cf. 57e-h). 3.4.2.2.2 Pattern –o The following examples of nouns are derived from verbs using the nominalizing suffix – o: STEM/ROOT 58. DERIVED NOUN a) mεr- ‘swallow’ ε-kέ-mεr-o ‘throat’ A-C7-R-SUF b) tir- ‘climb’ o-mό-tir-o ‘ladder’ A-C3-R-SUF c) andek- ‘write’ i-ri-ándek-o ‘that which has been written’ A-C5-R-SUF d) γamb- ‘say’ Ι-kí-γamb-o ‘a say’ A-C7-R-SUF e) γend- ‘walk’ υ-rú-γend-o ‘journey’ A-C11-R-SUF f) huŋgur- ‘open’ o-ro- huŋgur-ό ‘key’ 105 A-C11-R-SUF g) tum- ‘sew’ o-mό-tum-o A-C3-R-SUF h) βin- ‘sing’ ri-βin-á C5-R-SUF ‘seam’ ‘song’ The derived nouns in 58(a-h) show that the nominalizing suffix –o can occur with various noun classes to form nouns belonging to different classes. Occurring with the suffix –o are nouns in classes 3, 5, 7, and 11. Although the plural counterparts of the above cited nouns have not been shown, it is evident, from the data we have, that they too occur with the suffix –o. The nominalizing suffix –o occurring with NPxs in the above examples seems to perform triple functions: First, it is the implement performing the action. In this function the suffix –o occurs with the ki- prefix of class 7 and ru- in class 11 as in examples 58(a and f). Second, it is the ultimate result of the action as in 58(c-e, g-h). Suffix –o here occurs with prefixes ri-, ki- and ru- of classes 5, 7, and 11 respectively. Third, it is the instrument used to perform the action. The prefixes that occur with the suffix –o are mu- in class 3 and ru- in class 11 as in 58(b and f). However, as we can see, it is possible for one NPx occurring with the suffix –o to perform different roles (cf. 58a and d, e and f). 3.4.2.2.3 Pattern –u/-βu This pattern uses the passive suffix to form deverbatives. Examples are: STEM/ROOT 59. a) tem- DERIVED NOUN ‘beat’ υ-mu-tém-u ‘beaten one’ 106 A-C1-R-SUF b) γeγ- ‘carry’ υ-mu-γéγ-u ‘carried one’ A-C1-R-SUF c) sek- ‘laugh’ υ-mu-sék-u ‘someone laughed at’ A-C1-R-SUF d) keri- ‘salute’ υ-mu-kéri-βu ‘saluted one’ A-C1-R-SUF e) ha- ‘give’ υ-mu-há-βu ‘given one’ A-C1-R-SUF The extension suffixes –u/-βu, which are passive markers in this language, occur with all NPxs except for those in classes 1a/2a, 15 and the locatives (16-18). We need to comment here that the verb stems/roots, which the derivational suffix –βu can occur with in forming deverbatives, are either monosyllabic verbs, except for –ku ‘die’, (cf. υmu-há:-βu ‘given one’ from -ha ‘give’ Ι-kí-nywe:-βu ‘a drink’ from nywa ‘drink’) or verb stems ending with a high front vowel /i/ (cf. υ-mu-keri-βu ‘saluted one’ from keri ‘salute’, υ-mu-hiki-βu ‘one reached in destination’ The suffix –u/-βu occurring with AG [υ] or [Ι] with the prefix mu- denotes the concept of the undergoer of the action. 3.4.2.3 Nouns formed from adjective Examples are: STEM 60. a) –re b) –nene DERIVED NOUN ‘tall’/‘long’ υ-βú-re ‘length’ ‘fat’ υ-βu-néne ‘fatness’ 107 c) –γuhe ‘short’ o-βo-γúhe ‘shortness’ d) –sohu ‘selfish’ υ-βu-sόhu ‘selfishness’ e) –comu ‘good’/‘beautiful’ υ-βu-cόmu ‘goodness’/‘beauty’ f) –γoγo ‘mean’ υ-βu-γoγό ‘meanness’ Most adjective stems occur with the prefix in class 14 βu- to form abstract nouns denoting different states of affairs. However, it is possible in this language to affix the above adjective stems with prefixes of classes denoting augmentativeness and diminutiveness to form augmentative and diminutive nouns. 3.4.2.4 Proper nouns: Ki-Nata has proper nouns that take different morpho-semantic characteristics: Proper nouns formed by a free morph: 61. a) ta:tá b) mmá ‘father’ ‘mother’ c) βukoβá ‘Bukoba region’ d) tetú ‘black cow’ e) shorí ‘white cow’ f) βirirí ‘red cow’ All nouns in 61(a-f) are formed by a free morph. As we can see, nouns such as these denote many things, including kinship terms (cf. 61a-b); place names (cf. 61c) and individual heads of cattle (cf. 61d-f). One thing to observe clearly is that although most 108 animals enter classes 9 for singular and 10 for plural, in Ki-Nata, all nouns denoting individual animates are placed in class 1a for singular and class 2a for plural. This is to say, except for 61(c) above, the rest of the proper nouns take the βa- prefix of class 2a in their plurality. 3.4.2.5 Negation marker In this language, nominal derivation using the negation marker ta- is not very productive. There is a limited number of nouns formed by the negation marker. In the noun ɲantahete:ra ‘one who is never passed by anything’, we see that the verb root -hetoccurs with, among other things, the ɲa- and the negation marker ta- to form the deverbative noun. What we see here is that due to simplification in pronunciation the intensive marker er-er- has been shortened to e:r-. Thus, there is a deletion of segment /r/. 3.4.2.6 Reduplication. This process does not appear to be very productive in Ki-Nata nouns. However, few nouns manifest a partial reduplication where the noun appears to have two CV sequences that are identical and which contain the ‘repetitive notion’ as a recurring element of meaning (N&P 2003: 79). Examples are: 62. a) omukarakára ‘castor oil plant’ b) riβaβá ‘wing’ c) riβaβajό ‘pawpaw’ 109 d) oro:teté ‘back bone’ e) riŋoŋoí ‘crane’ [bird] Almost all nouns in this language manifest a complete reduplication. In this, two identical nouns appear in sequence: 63. a) omosúβesuβé ‘real man’ aβasúβesuβé ‘real men’ or ‘ordinary men’ b) ekehurerόhurerό ‘real pan’ eβehurerόhurerό ‘real pans’ or ‘ordinary pans’ c) riγuháγuhá ‘real bone’ amaγúhaγuhá ‘real bones’ or ‘ordinary bones’ It is evident that the nouns in (a-c) manifest a complete reduplication. From this process, one can easily see that the first noun in this process should appear with its prefix, while in the duplicate, only the stem appears. However, the rule is inapplicable to the noun omo:tomo:to ‘real man’ its plural aβa:toβa:to ‘real persons’ or ‘ordinary people’ where the prefix is retained in the second noun and only the augment is dropped. Furthermore, we see that nouns in singular forms denote the concept of ‘realness’ while nouns in plural forms denote both ‘realness’ and ‘ordinariness.’ 3.4.2.7 Compounding Nouns of the structure A+B generally refer to a “B-like kind of A” structure (cf. N&P 2003: 86). The first is the head of the compound while the second is the modifier. Let us examine the few available examples: 3.4.2.7.1 Verb-Noun Compounds: 64. a) rihiriŋgiti-máβi ‘dung beetle’ [roller of faeces] 110 b) εkεmεra-ncόka ‘kind of crane that eats snakes’ c) riβututuri-mánce ‘cuckow’ d) Ιγikaŋga-βáremi ‘kind of lizard that scares farmers by running closer to their feet’ e) Ιγikaŋgata-βúkwe ‘bride price advance’ The Ki-Nata data we have show that nouns formed by this process are rare. The verb stem (in bold) in the above compounds is prefixed with an augment (except for 64a and c) and NPx; the noun retains the NPx but drops the augment. 3.4.2.7.2 Noun-Noun compounds In this language, this type of derivation is not common. Noun-Noun compounds available here are those formed by the sa- and ɲa- prefixes. Examples are: 65. a) Sa-mitwe:-na:né ‘female unicorn with eight heads’ [Ki-Nata tales] b) Sa-koro-Wambúra ‘Wambura’s grand father’ c) ɲa-koro-Wambúra ‘Wambura’s grand mother’ Nouns in 65(b and c) are found in Ki-Nata greetings. The prefixes sa- and ɲa- in some occurrences denote gender (sa- for males and ɲa- for females). In 65(a) the nouns retain the NPx but drop the augment. iii) Noun-Adjective compounds Examples of N-A compounds are: 111 66. a) ta:ta-moɲi ‘young father’ b) ma:-moɲi ‘young mother’ c) ta:ta-mokoro ‘big father’ In Ki-Nata, this type of derivation is not very productive. We only find such few forms of N-V compounds in kinship terms like in 66(a-c) above. 3.5 Summary In this chapter we attempted a description of Ki-Nata noun structure in three major aspects, namely noun AGs, NPx and nominal derivation. It is evident that augments in this language manifest in various shapes as [i], [Ι] [e], [ε], [ɔ], [o], [υ], [u] and [a]. Their distribution is controlled by the vowel harmony rule. However, the nature of the stem counts in making some generalizations pertaining to the shapes of prefixes. Furthermore, in this language, AGs delete in certain contexts and are retained in others. As far as the number of NPxs is concerned, this language seems to have 20 noun classes marked by various prefixes. Although the semantic criterion of classifying nouns seems to be blurred, one can still see that some Ki-Nata nouns are semantically motivated. Various processes are involved in noun derivation. Of the processes employed, the analysis shows that most nouns are derived from verbs. Patterns like -i, -o, -u/βu- are productive in changing verbs into nouns. Other processes discussed are reduplication and compounding. 112 CHAPTER FOUR SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION 4.1 Summary In the three preceding chapters, we have attempted a description of three aspects of KiNata noun structure, namely the noun AG, NPx and Nominal Derivation. These elements combine to form the canonical form of the Ki-Nata noun structure. The nature of their combination differs from one kind of noun to another. For instance, in this language the canonical structure of underived nouns is (AUGMENT) + NPx + STEM, while that of the derived nouns is (AUGMENT) + NPx + STEM/ROOT + (NOMINALIZING SUFFIX). Moreover, through our analysis, morphophonological, syntactic and semantic criteria are quite valuable in the description of the Ki-Nata noun structure. In Chapter One we presented preliminary information to our study such as geographical location, background to the problem, statement of the problem, objectives of the study, significance of the problem, theoretical framework and research questions. The chapter also included fieldwork information as well as a review of the related literature. In the second chapter, we surveyed the Bantu noun structure in three aspects –AG, NPx and Nominal Derivation. Regarding the AG, researches in Bantu languages tend to show that the AG and the NPx are two distinct formatives. This is because augments tend to 113 disappear in certain grammatical contexts. Moreover, we have seen that the AG shape is mostly governed by the vowel harmony rule. Following our data presentation and analysis in Chapter Three, the following things have been addressed: First, the discussion has revealed that the AG in Ki-Nata serves two major roles, syntactic and semantic. The syntactic role operates when syntactic operators such as negation, Wh-enclitics, post-verbal position (copula), etc. control nonaugmented forms. The specificity concept, as we have seen, is partially reflected in semantic/pragmatic primes. However, although these two roles show up, the surveyed Ki-Nata data show that at some point the two roles function together. In other words, the AG function largely depends upon the context in the sense that the retention or deletion of AG depends on whether the speaker or hearer has prior information of the object in question. Second, regarding AGs [i] or [u] and [I] or [υ], we have shown that there are two rules applying here exclusively; the glide formation and vowel harmony. The application of the glide formation rule bleeds the application of the vowel harmony rule. In other words, once the glide formation rule has applied the vowel harmony rule cannot. Although there are some restrictions in some other Bantu languages for certain vowels to occur as AGs, in Ki-Nata all vowels occur as AGs. 114 Third, regarding the NPx, this language seems to have 20 noun classes marked by various prefixes. Although the semantic criterion of classifying nouns seems to be blurred, one can still say that this criterion was initially clear perhaps during the ProtoBantu period (cf. Massamba 1995). This is because even today some nouns, especially those in classes 1, 2, 3, 4 and 14, in Ki-Nata and other Bantu languages (cf. §.2.1.2.1) are partially semantically motivated. Fourth, on nominal derivation, we have seen in this chapter that various processes are involved. Of these processes, the analysis shows that most nouns in Ki-Nata are derived from verbs. Patterns like -i, -o, -u/βu- are productive in changing verbs into nouns. Other productive processes discussed are reduplication and compounding. We have also seen that only few nouns can be derived through compounding by (i) VERB + NOUN (ii) NOUN +NOUN (iii) NOUN + ADJECTIVE. 4.2 Conclusion Our aim in this study has been to explore the aspects of the Ki-Nata noun structure by using Lexical Phonology as our framework. We have seen that where there is rule ordering (morphophonological rules) phonological rules apply first before the morphological ones. For example, the glide formation rule applies before the vowel harmony rule. Again, as we have seen, where the glide formation rule has applied the vowel harmony rule cannot. Moreover, the process of deriving nouns from verbs and 115 adjectives is arranged in a series of levels. Each step of word formation process is tied to Lexical Phonology Rules i.e. augmentation, prefixation and suffixation. This study, as per the hypotheses made in Chapter One, has testified that in this language the AG is associated with concepts like specificity as its deletion or retention depends upon the context; and its shape is controlled by the vowel harmony rule. The surface manifestations of AGs and NPxs are accounted for by morphophonological rules. We have seen that the vowel harmony rule governing AG occurs between the AG itself and the vowel in the prefix, while the surface manifestation of the prefix is accounted for by the nature of the stem or vowel harmony between the vowel in the prefix and that of the stem/root. Although noun class prefixes in Ki-Nata represent several concepts such as the form of the thing involved (i.e. small, broad, abstract etc.), singular and plural, etc., we have seen that the semantic criterion, as explicitly shown in Chapter Two, cannot give a satisfactory Ki-Nata Noun classification. It was clear that only certain noun classes, i.e. class 1 mu-, refer to particular kinds of objects. Obviously, there is a lot that we have not attempted to do. We have not, for instance, explored the syllable structure of the noun in this language. Over and above, apart from mentioning at the outset that this language is tonal, we have not done any description of the Suprasegmental features of the Ki-Nata noun. In effect, although the three features of 116 the noun–AG, NPx and Nominal Derivation have been dealt with in this study, the full picture of the Ki-Nata noun structure remains incomplete. While we cannot claim to have explored each and every aspect in our area of study, we hope that we have been able to shed some light on the major three components of the KiNata noun structure. We also hope that this study is a stimulus for further exploration and detailed reading and examination, not only on the issues highlighted in our study, but also on this largely undescribed Bantu language. 117 REFERENCES Adonijah, A. (2003). Swahili Noun Class System: A Critical Survey of the Loan words. Unpublished M.A. Dissertation. University of Dar es Salaam. Ashton, E.O. et al. (1954). A Luganda Grammar. London: Longmans, Green and Co. Bleek, W. (1862). A Comparative Grammar of South African Languages. London: Trubner & Co. Bryan, M.A. (1959) The Bantu Languages of Africa. London: Oxford University Press. Byarushengo, E.R. et al. (eds). (1977). Haya Grammatical Structure. In Occasional Papers in Linguistics No. 6. University of Southern California. Chagas, J.E. (1977). The Pre-prefix. In Byarushengo et al. (eds). Haya Grammatical Structure. In Occasional Papers in Linguistics No. 6. University of Southern California. Cammenga, J. (1992). Kuria Phonology Morphology. Unpublished M.A.Thesis. University of Amsterdam. ---------(2002). Phonology and Morphology of Ekegusii. A Bantu Language of Kenya. Köln: Rüdiger Köpee Verlag. De Blois, K. (1970). The Augment In Bantu Languages. In Africana Linguistica IV. pp. 85-165. Tervuren. Givon, T. (1971). Studies in Chibemba and Bantu Grammar. Supplement 3 to Studies in African Linguistics. Greenberg J. H. (1963) Studies in African Linguistic Classification: Part 1. The Niger Congo Family. In Southwestern Journal of Anthropology. pp. 309-317. Gregersen, E.A. (1967). Prefix and Pronoun in Bantu. In IJAL, Memoirs 21. Baltmore: Waverly Press. Guthrie, M. (1948). The Classification of Bantu Languages. London: Oxford University Press. -------------(1967-71). Comparative Bantu. Farnborough: Gregg Internatinal Publishers. 118 -------------(1970). Collected Papers on Bantu Linguistics. Farnborough: Gregg International Publishers. Heine, B. (1976). Knowledge and Use the Second Language in Musoma Region: A Qualitative Survey. In Kiswahili. Vol 46. pp. 49-59. Hinnebusch, T.J. (1989). Bantu. In The Niger-Congo Languages. Nework and London: University Press. Hyman, L. and F. Katamba (1993). The Augment in Luganda: Syntax or Pragmatic? In Mchombo, S.A. Theoretical Aspect of Bantu Grammar 1. California: CSLI Publication. Kahigi, K. K. (2003). The Sisumbwa Noun: Its Classes and Derivation. In Occasional Papers in Linguistics. No.1. pp.1-86. University of Dar es Salaam: LOT Publications. Kiparsky, P. (1982). From Cyclic Phonology to Lexical Phonology. In Hust, H and Smith, N. (eds). The Structure of Phonological Representation. Part I. Dordrecht-Holland/ Cinnaminson: Foris Publications. Maho, J. (1999). A Comparative Study of Bantu Noun Classes. Cothoburgensin: Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis. Massamba, D. P. B. (1976). Umbo-Nomino katika Ci-Ruuri. Unpublished Undergraduate Dissertation. University of Dar es Salaam. ---------(1977). A Comparative Study of Jita, Ruri and Kwaya Languages of the Eastern Shores of Lake Nyanza. Unpublished M.A.Thesis. University of Dar es Salaam. ---------(1995). The Classification of Ci-Ruuri Nouns. In Journal of Asian and African Studies. Nos. 48-49. ---------(1996). Phonological Theory: History and Development. Dar es Salaam: Dar es Salaam University Press. Meeussen, A.E. (1959). Essai de Grammaire Rundi. Tervuren. Annales du Musée Royal du Congo Belge (de l’Afrique centrale) 119 Meinhof, C. (1932). Introduction to the Phonology of the Bantu Languages. Berlin. ----------(1899). Grundris Einer Lautlehre der Bantuspraschen. Leipzig: F.A. Brochhaus. Mekacha, R.D.K. (1985). Phonological Processes Affecting Ki-Nata Vowels. Unpublished M.A. Dissertation. University of Dar es Salaam. Mould, M. (1974). The Syntax and Semantics of the Initial Vowel in Luganda. In Voeltz, E. (ed.). Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference on African Linguistics. pp.223-230. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.. National Bureau of Statistics. (2003). 2002 Population and Housing Census General Report. Dar es Salaam: Government Printer. Nurse, D. (1977). Classification of the Chaga Dialects. Humburg: Buske. Nurse, D. and Phillipson, G. (1980). Bantu Languages of East Africa: a Lexicostatistical Survey. In Polomé. E. C. and Hill, C.P. (eds.) Languages in Tanzania. London: Oxford University Press. Nurse, D. and Phillipson, G. (eds.) (2003). The Bantu Languages. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Polomé. E. C. (1967). Swahili Language Handbook. 1717 Massachusetts Avenue: Centre for Applied Linguistics. Progovic, L. (1993). Non Augmented NPs in Kinande as Negative Polarity. In Mchombo, S.A. Theoretical Aspect of Bantu Grammar I. California: CSLI Publication. Rugemarila, J. M. (2005). A Grammar of Runyambo. Dar es Salaam: Languages of Tanzania (LOT). Schadeberg T. C. (1995). Spirantization and the 7-to-5 Vowel Merger in Bantu. In Belgian Journal of Linguistics. Vol. 9. pp. 73-84 Schadeberg T. C. (2003). Derivation. In Nurse, D. and Phillipson, G. (eds.). The Bantu Languages. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Wermer, W. E. (1973). Noun class and Concord Systems. In African Languages Structures. Berkeley: University of California Press. 120 Werner, A. (1919). Introductory Sketch of the Bantu Languages. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co. Ltd.. Yule, G. (1985). The Study of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 121 APPENDIX Working Data S/No 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 32 33 34 35 36 37 English person people a christian person christian people child children father fathers maternal uncle maternal uncles woman women lip/mouth lips/mouths tail tails load loads head heads tree trees lung lungs sun people (aug) People (aug) feather feathers urine oil, fat bottle (augm.) bottles (augm.) thing things girdle Kiswahili mtu watu mkristo wakristo mtoto watoto baba kina baba mjomba wajomba mwanamke wanawake mdomo midomo mkia mikia mzigo mizigo kichwa vichwa mti miti pafu mapafu jua jitu majitu unyoya manyoya mkojo mafuta lichupa* machupa* kitu vitu kiuno Ki-Nata omô:to aβâ:to omokírisito aβakírisito umwa:ná aβa:ná ta:tá βatá:ta ma:mé βa:ma:mé υmukári aβakári υmúnwa Imínwa omokéra emekéra omoríγo emeríγo υmutwé Imitwé omoté emeté ri:so:sό amasό:so iryo:βá erê:to amâ:to rí:βuri amáβuri amasíno:ro amaγúta oγocuβá amacuβá Ikiγéro Iβiγéro ekériβici 122 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 girdles animal skin (dim) animal skins (dim) bosom/chest bosoms/chests English water (dim) darkness house(s) goat(s) armpit(s) viuno ngozi ngozi kifua vifua kingereza/Vingereza vinywaji giza nyumba mbuzi kwapa 49 50 51 hyena(s) table(s) pen(s) fisi meza kalamu 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 torch(es) stomach(s) duck(s) rib ribs day(s) elbow hair hairs door(s) razor(s) kulunzi/tochi tumbo bata ubavu mbavu siku kiwiko unywele nywele mlango/milango wembe/nyembe 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 spider’s web bead(s) elephant (dim.) elephants (dim) water (dim) husband (dim) husbands (dim) teaching profession beauty/goodness humanity bow(s) snuff night childhood utando wa buibui ushanga/shanga katembo* tutembo* tumaji* kanaume twanaume ualimu uzuri utu uta ugoro usiku utoto eβériβici aγase:rό amase:rό eγekúβa eβekúβa eγecu:ŋgú ama:ncé Iki:réma aɲu:mbá/caɲumbá amborí/ca:mborí aɲakwa:há/caɲak wa:há ahití/ca:hití amecá/ca:mecá akará:m(u)/cakará :m(u) atό:ci/cáto:ci ánda/cá:nda ambá:ta/cá:mba:ta υrúβaru cá:mbaru orosíko/ca:sikό aŋgokόro υrucwé:re ca:ncwe:ré eγéseku/eβéseku υrúγe:mbe/cá:ŋge mbe υru:ɲáwamboβe υrúre:sa/ υβúre:sa aγácoγu ehécoγu Ihi:ncé aγasúβe otosúβe uβwarim(ú) υβucόm(u) oβό:to υβutá υβuγorό oβotíko uβwa:ná 123 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 brain matter leg legs ear ears arm arms to refuse winnowing to cut to laugh place there here at the head at the book on the head on the table yonder on the house on the car in there in here inside the head inside the cupboard inside the cattle shade in the grandmother’s house seeds (dim) oranges (dim) rags (dim) goats (dim) children (dim) pus (dim) river (aug) rivers (aug) husband(aug) husbands (aug) finger (aug) fingers (aug) flour (aug) water (aug) ubongo mguu miguu sikio masikio mkono mikono kukataa pepeta/pembua kukata kucheka mahali pale hapa kwenye kichwa kwenye kitabu juu ya kichwa juu ya meza kule/huko juu ya nyumba juu ya gari mule humu ndani kichwani ndani ya kabati ndani ya zizi ndani ya nyumba ya bibi vimbegu* vimachungwa* vitambara* vimbuzi* vitoto* viusaa* jito* mamito* jianaume* mianaume/manaume* jidole* mavidole* miunga/maunga* mimaji* uβwoŋgό okόγoro amáγoro oγotwé amatwé υkuβόko amaβόko ukwaŋgá okohόŋga γoténa γuséka aháse ha:rí háno mo:mutwé mό:γitaβo ko:mutwé ku:mecá yi:rí/íyo ko:ɲumbá kú:motoka mu:rí múno mo:mutwé mu:kaβáti mwi:βancá mwa:γo:kό ehéβeγu ehecu:ŋgwá Ihíta:su eheβόri ihya:ná Ihíβoro υγuta:rό amata:rό oγosúβe amasúβe uγwa:rá ama:rá amátu Imi:ncé 124 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 cat (aug) cats (aug) cook cooks shepherd shepherds reader readers speker speker extraction tool extraction tools throat throats ladder ladders activity activities writings kind of hair platting journey a cry key seam agreement song play boil boils beloved the bitten one the circumcised one the thatched one he who has been eaten dead people the given one something drinkable manner of farming manners of farming manner of cooking manners of cooking manner of harvesting jipaka* mapaka* mpishi wapishi mchungaji wachungaji msomaji wasomaji msemaji/muongeaji Wasemaji/waongeaji king’oleo ving’oleo koo makoo ngazi ngazi shughuli/kazi shughuli/kazi maandishi msuko safari kilio ufunguo mshono mapatano wimbo mchezo jipu majipu mpedwa mpigwaji mtahiriwa iliyoezekwa aliyeliwa mfu/wafu aliyepewa kinachonyweka kilimo kilimo upishi mapishi uvunaji υγúɲa:m(u) amáɲa:m(u) omόruγi aβáruγi omori:si aβarí:si υmúsomi aβásomi υmúγa:mbi aβáγa:mbi Ikí:hero Iβí:hero ekémero eβémero omόti:ri eméti:ro omόremo eméremo amá:ndeko omόsuko υruγe:ndo ekérero orohu:ŋgurό υmú∫ono am(a)ise(r)erano iryé:mbo rí:βarana ri:βi:mbá amaβí:mba υmuséγu υmutému υmusá:ru anseré:tu omoré:βu omokú υmuhá:βu Ikiɲwé:βu okoréma amárema okorúγa amáruγa υkuγésa 125 162 163 164 165 167 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 manners of harvesting manner of eating manners of eating smallness quickness shortness length/tallness quality if being unripe beauty/goodness sourness quantity/numerous state childhood foolishness let him/her winnow dog cry care/nurse a child month months heart hearts intestines pancreas pancreases shake the dust a kind of beating* son’s wife/daughter in law scarecrow* dung beetle [roller of faeses] bride price’s advance crane cuckow shadow butterfly someone who is never passed by anything a male unicorn with 8 heads uvunaji ulaji ulaji udogo uharaka ufupi urefu ubichi uzuri uchachu wingi amáγesa υkurá:γera amará:γera oβosú:hu uβwá:ŋgo oβoγúhe υβúre oβoβése υβucόmu oβόroro uβwa:rú utoto ujinga apepete mbwa lia lea mwezi miezi moyo mioyo utumbo kongosho kongosho kung’uta kung’uta mkamwana uβwa:ná υβuráγa:nu aho:ŋgé/i:rúre ase:sé réra rέra umwε:rí imyεrí umwό:yo imyό:yo υβúra ikya:ná iβya:ná kόŋo:ta kɔŋɔ:ta υmukamwá:na aina ya mjusi wa mashambani mdudu aviringishaye mavi posa* korongo dudumizi kivuli kipepeo mtu asiyepitwa na jambo au kitu zimwi lenye vichwa nane Ιγikaŋga-βáremi rihiriŋgiti-máβi Ιγikaŋgata-βúkwe εkεmεra-ncόka riβututuri-mánce Ιki:ri:rí Ιkiβa:βáyo ɲantaheteramόhumo sa-mitwe:-na:né 126 198 199 200 young father young mother Wambura’s grand father’ baba mdogo mama mdogo Babu yake Wambura Constructions Translate the following constructions into Ki-Nata: 1. A child is crying. 2. The child is crying. 3. Bring a book 4. Buy good books. 5. Go with a calabash. 6. Bring some water and some milk. 7. An old man and an old woman have arrived. 8. A/the thief entered in the house. 9. Saβiti likes a/the woman. 10. Saβiti doesn’t like any woman. 11. Don’t go with a calabash 12. A child is not crying. 13. Children are not crying. 14. Bedsteads won’t be taken away. 15. Speaking is a cure. 16. Speaking isn’t a cure. ta:ta-mόɲi ma:-mόɲi sa-koro-Wambúra 127 17. The cups belong to the woman. 18. The cups do not belong to the woman. 19. He didn’t see these cups. 20. Put the book on the table 21. Put the cups in the cupboard. 22. Every book should be read. 23. The owner the book has arrived. 24. Every person should study. 25. The owner (dim) of the house has come. 26. She is the farmer. 27. They are the farmers. 28. You fool! 29. You thieves! 30. Have you brought my flywhisk? 31. I have brought it. 32. Give me a chair. 33. Which chair? 34. Which shoes do you like? 35. The one in the house.