Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
July1985] Short Communications erated from cells with inadequate sample sizes. No test was conducted on the Tule Lake NWR data be- cause t-tests indicated the mean water-depth categorieswere not different. Bear River NWR and Lower Klamath NWR data sets revealed no difference in water-depth use by Western Grebe color morphs. I also tested for differences in the proportion of each color morph at each water-depth zone (Miller 1981: 219). Of 15 casestested, only 3 were significantly different: medium and high zones at Lower Klamath NWR and the low zone at Upper Klamath Lake. The data support the hypothesisthat light-phase Western Grebes feed farther from shore than dark- phase birds. However, the data are not totally consistentand, thus,this questionwarrantsfurther study. The data do not supportthe hypothesisthat lightphase Western Grebes feed at greater water depth than dark-phasebirds. However, the data must be viewed with cautiondue to severalconfoundingfactors:(1) water-depthcategorieswere not directly related to distancecategories;(2) 3 of 5 study areas were artificial water impoundmentswith relatively little change in water depth throughout the water basin (with the exceptionof "borrow ditches"adjacent to dike roads);and (3) the depth categorieswere only relative to other measurements on the samelake. Future research should concentrate on natural water basins to minimize these factors. Unfortunately, however, nearly all of the sympatric breeding populations in the U.S. are on artificial impoundments. One notable exception is Upper Klamath Lake. A general hypothesiscan be developedfrom these data. Most natural water basins have the shallowest water zones adjacent to shore, and water depth increaseswith approachto the approximatecenter of the basin. Light-phase birds may have evolved behavioral patternscloselyassociated with water depth. Useof a particularwater-depthzonecommonlymay occurat a fairly consistentdistancefrom shore.Thus, distancefrom shoremay be an environmental"cue" regarding water depth and niche partitioning between the color morphs.Reducedniche overlap may 637 limit competition for food resources,i.e. a "coexistencemechanism"(Cody 1974:7). An alternative hypothesisis that niche partitioning is accomplishedby spatialseparationof near shorevs. farther from shore, and water depth and the evolution of "springing dives" are secondaryto spatialfactors.In either case, at artificial water impoundment areas where mean water depth is relatively similar (comparedto natural water impoundments), distance from shore may influence spatial behavior of birds more than water depth. Travel support was provided by the Wildlife Biology Program,WashingtonStateUniversity. Dr. Tom McCabe assisted with field work and reviewed the manuscript. Duane Diefenbach assistedwith data analysis. Sincere thanks to Drs. Rich Alldredge and Gary White for statisticalconsultation,and Drs. Fred Gilbert, RichardJohnson,John Kadlec,Gary Nuechterlein, Vince Schultz, John Thompson, and Kerry Reesefor valuable commentson the manuscript. LITERATURE CITED CODY,M.L. 1974. Competition and the structureof bird communities. Monographs in Population Biology No. 7. Princeton,New Jersey,Princeton Univ. Press. GRINNELL,J. 1904. The origin and distribution of the Chestnut-backed MILLER, R. G. 1981. Chickadee. Simultaneous Auk 21: 364-382. statistical infer- ence, 2rid ed. New York, Springer-Verlag. NUECHTERLEIN, G.L. 1981. Courtship behavior and reproductive isolation between Western Grebe color morphs. Auk 98: 335-349. RATTI,J. T. 1979. Reproductive separation and isolating mechanismsbetween syrupatticdark- and light-phase Western Grebes. Auk 96: 573-586. 1981. Identification Clark's Grebe. Western and distribution of Birds 12: 41-46. STORER, R.W. 1965. The color phasesof the Western Grebe. Living Bird 4: 59-63. Received18 ]une 1984,accepted19 December1984. The Importanceof Open Habitat to the Occurrenceof Kleptoparasitism DENNIS R. PAULSON BurkeMuseumDB-10,University of Washington, Seattle,Washington 98195USA Direct competitionamong predatory birds for potential prey often is observedbecauseof the large sizeof predatorand prey, the often lengthy pursuit, and the open terrain in which many of thesebirds live. Suchcompetitionwas brought to my attention vividly when I watchedthe interactionamongfour speciesof raptorsover an open grassyprairie near Clewiston,Hendry County, Florida on 26 November 1961.A Merlin (Falcocolumbarius) flew rapidly over the prairie about0.5 m abovethe ground and flushed two male Red-winged Blackbirds(Agelaiusphoeniceus).It followed one blackbird closelyuntil a male Northern Harrier (Circuscyaneus)rose from the prairie in front of them and struck at but missed the passing blackbird. The blackbird dropped to the ground and flew up again, and the Merlin forced it 638 ShortCommunications [Auk, Vol. 102 down a few metersfarther on. The harrier caughtup with them and captured the blackbird on the ground, where it restedwith its prey asthe Merlin flew away. The harrier then flew up without the blackbird a longer distance;(3) hiding from kleptoparasitesis difficultor impossible;and (4) prey itemscanbe found easily after they are relinquished by the host. The following observationssupporteachof theseconclu- and landed a short distance away, and an adult Red- sions. tailed Hawk (Buteojamaicensis) landed on the blackbird a few secondslater. The harrier presumablysaw the other hawk approaching and relinquished its prey. The Red-tailedHawk restedon the ground for a few minutes, but then it also flew up with the blackbirdas two CrestedCaracaras(Polyborus plancus) (1) Kleptoparasitescan observe and follow hosts more easily in open habitats. African Fish Eagles (Haliaeetus vocifer)were important kleptoparasiteson Goliath Herons (Ardeagoliath,Mock and Mock 1980), and it may have been the eagles' ability to observe several hunting herons simultaneously that made arrived from a distant palm clump and began to dive food stealing a successfulstrategy (S. Carroll pers. at it. The Red-tailed Hawk was joined by another comm.). In Washington I have observed most miRed-tailed Hawk, possibly its mate, and they flew grant Parasitic Jaegers (Stercorariusparasiticus)near away with the blackbird, the caracarasin pursuit. The flocksof their host species,Bonaparte'sGulls (Larus two hawks and the pair of caracarasrepeatedlydived philadelphia)and Common Terns (Sterna hirundo). at one another in flight until the Red-tailed Hawk LaughingGulls (Larusatricilla)and Heermann'sGulls with the blackbird landed about 1 km from the cap- (Larusheermanni)follow Brown Pelicans (Pelecanusocture site and the caracarasflew away. I had lost sight cidentalis) as the pelicansforage and attempt to take of the second Red-tailed Hawk, the harrier, and the prey from each successfulpelican (Anthony 1906, Merlin by that time. Schnell et al. 1983). Similarly, I watched six LaughThis interactioninvolved six raptorsof four species ing Gulls follow two Great Egrets(Casmerodius albus) for about 10 min. The first three specieswere vying that were foragingon a sandflat in Florida. Although for possession of the prey, but the caracarasmay have I did not observe the egrets capture prey, it seemed been displaying interspecific territoriality (Newton clearthat the gulls were attending the egretsfor pos1979: 45) or aggressiontoward potential predators. sible kleptoparasitism. Caracarasmay nestduring Novemberin Florida(Kale (2) Kleptoparasitescan observe prey capture and 1978), and the catholic food habits of Red-tailed carryingmore easilyin open habitats.The Red-tailed Hawks (Craigheadand Craighead1956) make them Hawk that took the blackbird from the harrier (dis- possiblepredatorsof nestlings.All of theseraptors cussed above) was not visible to me when the interhave been reported as kleptoparasites(Brockmann action began and seemingly flew from a distance, and Barnard 1979). Most reportsof kleptoparasitism perhapsbeginning its approachas the Merlin chased involve two species,but Bent (1938: 133) cited a rethe blackbird.Similarly, I have seenParasiticJaegers port of a crow (Corvussp.) taking a mouse from a that were in cruising flight suddenly accelerate to Northern Harrier and losing it to a CrestedCaracara. harassa gull or tern with prey as far as 0.5 km away. The successionof interspecific interactions proba- Clearly, the visibility of prey is directly related to bly wasdeterminedby size,with the Red-tailedHawk the opennessof a specifichabitat, an important vari(1,220 g) dominant to the Northern Harrier (530 g), able to birds with long-distancevision. which was dominant to the Merlin (215 g; mean fe(3) Hostsare lessable to hide from kleptoparasites male weights from Newton 1979). Birds are not the in open habitats.Furhess(1978) found that Parasitic primary prey of either Red-tailedHawks or harriers Jaegerswere more successful in taking prey from a (Craigheadand Craighead 1956);therefore, pursuit speciesof gull and a speciesof tern (33-44% of chases by the Merlin and capture by the harrier made the successful) than from three speciesof alcidsthat could blackbird more accessibleto the larger species.The rapid reactionof the larger raptorsindicatesthat avian predatorsare often aware of the activitiesof other predators. Brockmann and Barnard (1979) enumerated six ecologicalconditions facilitating the evolution of kleptoparasitism: (1) largeconcentrations of hosts;(2) large quantities of food; (3) large, high-quality food items; (4) predictable food supply; (5) food visible; and (6) shortageof food. I believe their "food visible" conditionis a consequence of another important condition, the opennessof the habitat in which both the host and the parasitelive. The opennesscondition has four effects: (1) potential and actual hosts can be watched, even continuously, at a longer distance;(2) capture and carrying of prey is visible for dive to escape(11-21% successful).The ability of the host to "hide" was important in determining the successof these kleptoparasiticattempts.Similarly, Atlantic Puffins (Fraterculaarctica)gave up their prey in 51% of kleptoparasiticattempts by ParasiticJaegers when flying over land and only 22% of similar attempts when above the sea, where they could dive or descendto the surface(Andersson 1976). I rarely saw an intended host escape with its prey during many observationsof Parasiticand Pomarine (Ster- corarius pomarinus) jaegerskleptoparasitizing gullsand terns at sea. These birds can neither dive below the surfacenor escapeto the relative safetyof a breeding colony.For the majorityof theseobservations,only thosebirds that swallowedprey during the chasewere able to retain it. July1985] Short Communications 639 anthracinus, Buteo nitidus, B.lineatus, B.platyp(4) Relinquishedprey items can be found by klep- teogallus and B. albonotatus. The difference toparasitesmore easily in open habitats.Andersson terus,B. brachyurus, (1976) observed that Parasitic Jaegerssecured 15 of betweenthesetwo groupsis significant(Fisher'sex18itemsdroppedby AtlanticPuffins.AlthoughI have act test, P = 0.01). I believe the observationspresentedhere support few recorded data, my impressionafter many years of watching jaegerswith their many host speciesis what is an intuitively reasonablehypothesis,even by humansbeingableto that prey items droppedby hostsare rarely lost by thougha biasis introduced interactions moreeasilyin open jaegers.Most fish probablyare disabledduring the detectkleptoparasitic time it takes the parasite to force the host to drop terrain. I thank Greg Butcher,ScottCarroll,Gary Schnell, them and are unlikely to escapethe parasite. This is certainlytrue for a fish carried by a bird to feed its and an anonymousbut excellentreviewerfor their constructivecommentson the manuscript.Preparing young. this note has impressedon me the value of preserv"habitataswell asthe behaviorof the hostmayaffect ing field notes;without mine from 23 yearsago I its chancesof being parasitized," but they did not would not have had thesenew thoughtson the subspecifyopennessof habitatand overwaterforaging ject. as important considerations.Further, they suggested Brockmann and Barnard (1979: 494) stated that that "mixed coloniesof fishing birds provide the ideal environment for kleptoparasitismwith a plentiful LITERATURE supplyof food itemswhich are easilystolen"(Brockmann and Barnard 1979: 498) but did not mention ANDERSSON, g. CITED 1976. Predation and kleptoparasi- that the majorgroupsof marinekleptoparasites (frigtismby skuasin a Shetlandseabirdcolony.Ibis atebirds,jaegers,and gulls) often display this behav118: 208-217. ior while away from thesecolonies.This is a reason- ANTHONY,g.W. 1906. Random notes on Pacific coast ableoversight,becausethe best-knownkleptoparasite, gulls. Auk 23: 129-137. the ParasiticJaeger,has been studied at seabirdcol- BENT,A. C. 1938. Life histories of North American onies in the north Atlantic rather than in the north Pacific,where it is a predatoron its breedinggrounds (Maher 1974) and a kleptoparasite at sea. Although mostseabirdsuse open habitats,raptors also can be used to test the hypothesisthat openness of habitat is an important factor leading to kleptoparasiticbehavior. The prediction is that raptors of open country are likely to be kleptoparasiteswhile those of woodland are not. I used the list of kleptoparasitic raptors presentedby Brockmannand Barnard (1979), with the addition of Falcosparveriusas a kleptoparasite(S. Carroll pets.comm.),to testthe hypothesis.The categorizationis my own, basedon extensive experiencewith all specieson the list and information summarized by Brown and Areadon (1968). Of 22 North American falconiforms that forage primarily in open country, t0 (marked with asterisks) are known to be kleptoparasites: Coragyps atratus,Cathartesaura*, Gymnogypscalifornianus, Pandion haliaetus,Elanuscaeruleus,Rostrhamussociabilis,Ic- birdsof prey. Order Falconiformes (part 2). U.S. Natl. Mus. Bull. 170. BROCKMANN, H. J., & C. J. BARNARD. 1979. Klepto- parasitismin birds.Anita. Behav.27: 487-514. BROWN,L., & D. AMADON. 1968. Eagles,hawks and falcons of the world. New York, McGraw-Hill. CRAIGHEAD, J. J., & F. C. CRAIGHEAD, JR. 1956. Hawks, owls and wildlife. Harrisburg, Pennsylvania, Stackpole Co. FURNESS, R. W. 1978. Kleptoparasitism by Great Skuas(CatharactaskuaBrunn.) and Arctic Skuas (Stercorarius parasiticus L.) at a Shetland seabird colony.Anita. Behav.26: 1167-1177. KALE,H. W., II. (Ed.). 1978. Rare and endangered biota of Florida. Vol. 2, Birds. Tallahassee, Florida Game & Fresh Water Fish Comm. MAHER,W.J. 1974. Ecologyof Pomarine, Parasitic, and Long-tailedjaegersin northern Alaska.Pacific Coast Avifauna No. 37. MOCK,D. W., & K. C. MOCK. 1980. Feeding behavior tinia mississippiensis, Haliaeetusleucocephalus*, Circus and ecologyof the Goliath Heron. Auk 97: 433cyaneus*,Parabuteo unicinctus, Buteoswainsoni, B. albi448. caudatus, B. jamaicensis*, B. regalis,B. lagopus*,Aquila NEWTON,I. 1979. Population biology of raptors. chrysaetos, Polyborus plancus*,Falcosparverius*,F. coVermillion, South Dakota, Buteo Books. lumbarius*,F. peregrinus*, F. rusticolus, and F. mexica- SCHNELL,G. D., B. L. WOODS, & B. J. PI,OGER. 1983. nus*. However, none of the following 10 species, Brown Pelican foraging successand kleptoparawhich forage primarily within forest and woodland sitismby Laughing Gulls. Auk tOO:636-644. in the same region, displays this behavior: Elanoides forficatus, Accipiterstriatus,A. cooperil, A. gentills,Bu- Received18 June1984, accepted23 December1984.