Download Who owns implicit attitudes Running Head: WHO OWNS IMPLICIT

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Confirmation bias wikipedia , lookup

False consensus effect wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Who owns implicit attitudes 1
Running Head: WHO OWNS IMPLICIT ATTITUDES?
Who owns implicit attitudes? Testing a metacognitive perspective
B. Keith Payne
Erin Cooley
Chris Loersch
Ryan Lei
Please address correspondence to:
Keith Payne
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Campus Box 3270, Chapel Hill, NC 27599
Email: [email protected]
Who owns implicit attitudes 2
Abstract
Do implicit evaluations reflect one’s personal attitude, or something external such as cultural
knowledge? We propose that the answer depends on metacognitive inferences people draw about
the meaning of their implicit responses. Participants completed an implicit measure of attitudes
toward gay men, followed by a manipulation that led them to construe their implicit responses as
belonging to themselves (owned), or as unrelated to the self (disowned). When they construed
their implicit responses as their own, they later reported explicit attitudes that were consistent
with implicit responses. Disowning their implicit responses, in contrast, led to explicit reports
that were less consistent with implicit responses. Effects were replicated by measuring individual
differences in perceptions of ownership. In addition to influencing explicit attitude reports,
inferences of ownership moderated whether implicit bias was detected on the personalized IAT
(which is designed to measure personal attitudes) but ownership did not moderate effects on a
standard IAT (which may not distinguish between personal and extra-personal associations).
Ownership of implicit attitudes may depend on metacognitions about those attitudes.
Key Words: Implicit attitudes; Automatic; Social Cognition; Extra-personal associations;
Prejudice; Heterosexism
Who owns implicit attitudes 3
Who owns implicit attitudes? Testing a metacognitive perspective
When thoughts are automatically activated, they sometimes feel as if they do not belong
to us at all. We say that such thoughts “come to mind,” or “occur to us,” portraying the thoughts
as agents and our minds as passive receivers. The feeling of foreignness is highlighted by
research on implicit attitudes, which finds that automatic evaluations often differ from explicitly
endorsed beliefs. In such cases, research participants often describe their scores on implicit tests
not as reflecting their own personal views, but instead as reflecting cultural knowledge or
stereotypes. Nonetheless, implicit attitude measures predict individual differences in a wide
range of behaviors, and do so about as well as explicit attitudes (Greenwald et al., 2009;
Cameron, Brown-Ianuzzi, & Payne, 2011). If an attitude predicts an individual’s behavior, then it
seems that it should be in some sense considered his or her own attitude. If an evaluation
automatically comes to mind, should it be considered to belong to whomever owns that mind? Or
does personal ownership depend instead on a reflective act in which someone claims it as one’s
own?
The question of whether implicit attitudes reflect a person’s “true self” is important for
interpreting instances of implicit prejudice (Arkes & Tetlock, 2004; Cameron, Payne, & Knobe,
2010; Gawronski, Peters, & LeBel, 2008). If only actions reflecting the true self are morally
blameworthy as some have suggested, then are people morally responsible for discrimination
that results from implicit bias (Dan-Cohen, 1991; Frankfurt, 1969; Sripada, 2009)? The answer
seems to depend, at least in part, on the empirical question of whether automatically activated
thoughts reflect the personal self.
Who owns implicit attitudes 4
In this paper we take a metacognitive approach to the ownership of implicit cognition.
We first review two contrasting perspectives on how personal and extra-personal associations
might be distinguished. We then consider the implications of metacognitive theories for whether,
when, and how automatically activated attitudes may become personal or extra-personal. Based
on an extension of these theories, we hypothesized that automatic evaluations provide input into
a metacognitive inference process in which they may be adopted as one’s own, or disowned.
Whether an implicit evaluation is a personal or extra-personal association therefore depends on
how the subject interprets his or her subjective experience. Automatic evaluations construed as
personal should be more likely to be expressed on explicit measures, and also more likely to be
detected by implicit measures designed to assess personal attitudes. We provide evidence across
four studies that metacognitive inferences have important consequences for both explicit and
implicit attitudes.
Theoretical Background
Some early research on the validity of the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald et
al., 1998) suggested that this measure might be sensitive to influences other than respondents’
personal attitudes. Karpinski and Hilton (2001) reported that an IAT measuring attitudes toward
apples and candy bars showed a pronounced preference for apples, but it did not predict behavior
when participants chose between the two items. The authors suggested that the IAT may reflect
environmental associations, such as a cultural norm that people should prefer apples to candy
bars. Choice behavior, in contrast, was argued to be driven by personal liking for apples and
candy bars.
Olson and Fazio (2004) extended this reasoning and argued that the IAT confounds
personal attitudes and extra-personal evaluations. They defined extra-personal associations as
Who owns implicit attitudes 5
“associations that do not contribute to one’s evaluation of an attitude object and thus do not
become activated when one encounters the object but that are nevertheless available in memory”
(p. 653). According to this view, if an evaluation is spontaneously activated when encountering
an attitude object, then it must be a personal attitude. That is, if it pops into my mind then it must
belong to me.
Measurement-based accounts of personal and extra-personal attitudes
To separate personal and extra-personal associations, Olson and Fazio (2004) developed
a “personalized” IAT, primarily by changing the category labels from “pleasant” and
“unpleasant” to “I like” and “I don’t like.” The personalized IAT was more successful than the
traditional IAT at predicting a choice between apples and candy bars. In another study, scores on
a traditional IAT were influenced by exposure to the misinformed opinions of schoolboys, but
the personalized IAT was not (Han, Olson, & Fazio, 2006). These studies suggest that personal
evaluations reflect associations between an attitude object and one’s likes and dislikes, whereas
associations between an attitude object and general categories “good” and “bad” may reflect both
personal likes and cultural associations. From this perspective, personal and extra-personal
associations refer to separate sets of associations in memory.
Ownership as a conscious judgment
An alternative view is that ownership does not reside in the memory associations per se,
but instead in the metacognitive inferences people make. Nosek and Hansen (2008) argued that
the personal versus non-personal nature of evaluations is not a feature of the associations stored
in memory, but it is instead a consequence of conscious judgments made about those
associations (see also Gawronski, Peters, & LeBel, 2008). Based on this reasoning, Banaji,
Nosek, and colleagues (Banaji, 2001; Banaji et al., 2006) argued that because the judgment of
Who owns implicit attitudes 6
ownership is conscious, “it is less sensible to think of a sharp line between person and culture
when thinking about implicit cognition” (p. xx).
Metacognitive models of implicit and explicit attitudes
Metacognition refers to thinking about one’s own thoughts, or feelings. One kind of
metacognition is making judgments about one’s own implicit associations, and researchers have
invoked metacognition as a key factor differentiating between implicit and explicit evaluations.
The role of metacognition has been developed most comprehensively in the AssociativePropositional model of Evaluation (APE; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006) and the
Metacognitive Model of attitudes (MCM; Petty, Brinol, & DeMarree, 2007). The APE claims
that automatically activated associations provide input into a propositional reasoning process that
assigns truth values. Based on propositional reasoning, people may accept the activated
information as valid or reject it as invalid. Implicit tests are said to be proxies for automatic
associations, whereas explicit tests reflect the outcome of propositional reasoning. Propositional
reasoning is therefore critical for understanding whether implicit and explicit tests will agree.
The Metacognitive model (MCM) of attitudes distinguishes between evaluative
associations which are activated from memory, and validity tags that are applied to them (Petty,
Brinol, & DeMarree, 2007). Validity tags are considered to be the result of metacognitive
inferences about whether the attitude is valid or not. The APE emphasizes reasoning processes,
whereas the MCM emphasizes attitude representations stored in memory. Nonetheless, both
models recognize that the residues of current processing may be stored as representations, and
that representations can be retrieved and modified over time. Thus, although the APE and MCM
differ on a number of important points, their implications for the present question are similar.
Who owns implicit attitudes 7
The APE and MCM focus on metacognitive inferences specifically about whether the
attitude is valid – that is, whether it is true, accurate, or justified (but for a metacognitive
approach to conscious awareness, see Hofmann & Wilson, 2010). We suggest that the basic
principle can be extended to encompass a variety of other inferences, including inferences about
whether an attitude is one’s own. One framework useful for integrating these different kinds of
inferences is the Situated Inference Model of priming (Loersch & Payne, 2011). This model was
designed to account for the wide variety of different ways that priming can affect judgment,
motivation, and behavior. For example, participants primed with the concept of aggression might
be more likely to perceive another individual as more hostile (construal priming; Higgins,
Rholes, & Jones, 1977; Srull & Wyer, 1979), behave more aggressively themselves (behavior
priming; Carver, Ganellen, Froming, & Chambers, 1983), and become motivated to actively seek
out an opportunity to aggress against someone else (goal priming; Todorov & Bargh, 2002).
The Situated Inference Model posits that priming simply increases the accessibility of
information that is related (semantically, evaluatively, or experientially) to the primed content.
People’s default assumption is that whatever thoughts and feelings come to mind refer to
whatever target is in focal attention (Higgins, 1998; Clore & Gasper, 2000). That information is
used to guide responses when people infer that the primed information is part of their own
spontaneous reaction to the target. However, the meaning of the primed information must be
constrained by the options afforded by the perceiver’s immediate context. For example, if
aggression-related thoughts come to mind while one is trying to form an impression of a new
person, one might conclude that the person is aggressive. But if the same thoughts come to mind
as one is preparing to act, one might act aggressively. The Situated Inference Model highlights
that the kinds of metacognitive inferences people make are not limited by the associations that
Who owns implicit attitudes 8
are activated; they are instead flexible interpretations constructed to meet demands of the
situation.
As applied to implicit evaluations, the Situated Inference Model suggests that people
might make a wide variety of inferences about what those evaluations mean, along many
dimensions. Truth value or validity is just one of many possible dimensions. For example, people
might also make inferences about whether the evaluation is intended; whether it is wanted; or
most important for the current question, whether it reflects one’s own personal attitude.
The Present Research
Our analysis of metacognitive models led us to predict that the inferences people make
about their implicit evaluations could influence whether they are perceived as personal or extrapersonal evaluations, with important consequences. In each study, we first measured implicit
attitudes using the Affect Misattribution Procedure (AMP; Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart,
2005). Next we either manipulated or measured inferences about whether the feelings
experienced during the task reflect participants own attitudes. Finally, we measured subsequent
explicit or implicit attitudes to assess how perceptions of ownership affected later evaluations.
First, we predicted that when participants construe implicit evaluations as their own, they
would be more likely to report them explicitly. In Study 1 we manipulated perceived ownership
and found that this inference moderated implicit-explicit consistency. In Study 2 we replicated
this effect by measuring rather than manipulating perceived ownership. In Study 3 we both
measured and manipulated perceived ownership to establish that perceived ownership was
indeed the mediator by which our manipulation influenced implicit-explicit correspondence.
Also in Study 3, we tested whether inferences of ownership could be separated from
inferences of validity. There is good reason to expect that inferences of ownership and validity
Who owns implicit attitudes 9
should be positively correlated, because people tend to believe that their own beliefs are true
(Gawronski et al., 2008). However, we provide evidence in Study 3 that inferences that an
evaluation is one’s “own” cannot be reduced to inferences that the evaluation is valid.
Inferences of ownership provide one basis for expressing attitudes explicitly, or rejecting
them. However, inferences about ownership might also have effects on implicit associations
themselves. To make the inference that “my automatic evaluations reflect my own attitudes”
requires forging an association between the evaluation and one’s personal likes and dislikes. As
highlighted by the MCM, the metacognitive judgments people make may be stored in memory
and retrieved later, and hence become linked to the attitude representation. Based on this
reasoning, in Study 4 we tested the effects of perceived ownership on a personalized IAT and a
traditional IAT. If metacognitive inferences change implicit associations in this way, then
perceiving an implicit evaluation as one’s own attitude should make it more likely to be detected
by a personalized IAT. The same relationship should not hold for the standard IAT, because it
does not distinguish between personal and extra-personal associations.
These studies help advance the theoretical debates over the role of ownership in implicit
attitudes. In agreement with Nosek and Hansen (2008) and Banaji, Nosek… (), our findings
suggest that ownership can be conferred by conscious metacognitive inferences, holding the
automatic association constant. However, unlike Nosek and Banaji and colleagues, we suggest
that those inferences can be meaningfully drawn about implicit evaluations as well as explicit
evaluations. The inferences are meaningful in that they have downstream consequences. One
consequence is that inferences about ownership contribute to the differences between
associations detected by the personalized IAT versus the traditional IAT. This provides
Who owns implicit attitudes 10
additional evidence for the validity of the personal versus extra-personal distinction and the use
of the personalized IAT to measure it (Olson and Fazio, 2004).
Experiment 1
Our research strategy was first to measure individual differences in automatic
evaluations, and then to manipulate experimentally whether participants considered that
evaluation to reflect their own attitudes. We then measured attitudes explicitly. If our hypotheses
are correct, then considering an automatic evaluation as one’s own attitude should lead
participants to report explicit attitudes that are consistent with the automatic response.
Disowning the evaluation should lead to less consistency.
The present experiments studied attitudes toward gay men, for several reasons. First,
prejudice against a stigmatized group highlights cases in which people may be motivated to
consider their implicit responses to reflect something outside of their own attitudes and beliefs.
In previous studies in our laboratory we have observed wide variability in explicit heterosexism
among our student population. Heterosexism therefore provides an ideal test case to illustrate the
malleability in prejudice that might result from metacognitive inferences about attitudes. Finally,
this topic is salient and meaningful to our subject population because these studies were
conducted in the months preceding a controversial referendum to amend the North Carolina
constitution to prohibit recognition of any form of same sex relationship. Demonstrating that
even hotly debated, salient explicit attitudes are malleable as a function of ownership inferences
highlights the importance of such inferences.
Method
Participants
Who owns implicit attitudes 11
Participants were 40 undergraduate students (31 women and 9 men) from the University
of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. They received partial course credit for participation.
Procedure
Participants were seated at one of six individual computers enclosed in cubicles for
privacy. After providing informed consent, participants completed demographic questions and
then completed the implicit measure of attitudes. After completing the implicit measure,
participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions in which they were encouraged to
interpret their affective feelings during the implicit test as reflecting their personal attitudes or as
attitudes separate from the self. Finally, participants completed the explicit measure of attitudes
and were debriefed.
Implicit measurement. Participants completed the Affect Misattribution Procedure (AMP;
Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005) to measure implicit responses to same-sex and crosssex couples. This task was selected because priming tasks avoid the task-switching demands and
dual-response mapping that is a source of controversy in interpretations of the IAT. Moreover,
the AMP demonstrates large effect sizes and high reliability, making it an effective and efficient
measure of individual differences in automatic responses (see Cameron, Brown-Iannuzzi, &
Payne, 2012; Payne, Burkley, & Stokes, 2008).
The task included 64 trials in which a photo prime was presented for 125 ms, followed by
a Chinese symbol for 100 ms, followed by a black and white pattern mask that remained on the
screen until a response was made. Participants were warned that the primes could influence their
judgments and they were instructed to try their best to ignore photo primes and to make
judgments based solely on how pleasant or unpleasant they found the Chinese symbols to be.
Participants pressed the “L” key to indicate “pleasant” and the “A” key to indicate “unpleasant.”
Who owns implicit attitudes 12
The photos used as primes were 16 photos of same-sex male couples, and 16 photos of
cross-sex couples as control primes. Participants saw each photo prime twice. The poses
included embraces, kisses, and holding hands but did not display nudity or sexual activity. The
photos were matched so that the same poses were represented equally often for same-sex and
opposite-sex couples.
Ownership manipulation. After completing the implicit measure, participants were
randomly assigned to one of two conditions. The personal inference group was encouraged to
consider their spontaneous feelings during the priming task to be their own actual attitude. The
extra-personal inference group was encouraged to consider their feelings as something other
than their own actual attitude.
To manipulate perceived ownership we drew on prior findings that simply considering a
particular hypothesis makes people more likely to believe that it is true. For example,
participants who learned that Jane acted like an extrovert in some situations but an introvert in
other situations rated Jane as more suitable for a job as a real estate agent when asked to consider
the hypothesis that she is extroverted (Snyder & Cantor, 1979). In contrast, they rated her as
more suitable for a job as a librarian when asked to consider the hypothesis that she is
introverted. Such findings illustrate the confirmation bias, a robust tendency for people to
confirm whatever hypothesis they entertain rather than disconfirm it. The bias is believed to
result from selectively searching for hypothesis-consistent evidence (Nickerson, 1998). We took
advantage of the confirmation bias by asking participants either to consider reasons that their
feelings during the implicit task reflect their own attitudes or to consider reasons that their
feelings do NOT reflect their own attitudes. The following instructions were used to manipulate
metacognitive inferences:
Who owns implicit attitudes 13
While completing the picture task, you may have had a "gut feeling" towards the
pictures of heterosexuals and homosexuals. Research has found that this gut feeling
usually reflects [does NOT reflect] people's genuine attitude towards homosexuality…
We are interested in how people generate reasons for their feelings. People can often
generate reasons why their feelings either do or do not reflect their actual attitudes. For
the purpose of this study, we'd like you to write two to three (2-3) reasons why the
feelings you felt during the picture task ARE [are NOT] your own attitude...
Finally, participants completed the Modern Homophobia Scale to assess explicit attitudes
(MHS; Raja & Stokes, 1998). The scale includes separate subscales for attitudes toward lesbians
and gay men, but in all experiments we only assessed attitudes toward gay men. Participants
responded to 22 items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Items included
questions about affective discomfort, (e.g., “I am comfortable with the thought of two men being
romantically involved” [reverse scored]), belief that homosexuality is deviant (e.g., “Male
homosexuality is a psychological disease”) discriminatory behaviors, (e.g., “I would remove my
child from class if I found out the teacher was gay”), and policy-related attitudes (e.g.,
“Marriages between gay men should be legal” [reverse scored]).
Results and Discussion
No participants reported the ability to read the Chinese characters. However, three
participants pressed the same key on all trials and were excluded from analyses, leaving a final
sample of 37. Mean AMP scores in the pre-test displayed significant levels of bias. The
proportion of pleasant responses was much higher on heterosexual prime trials (M = .86, SD =
.15) than homosexual prime trials (M = .46, SD = .30), F (1, 36) = 47.47, p < .001.
Our primary hypothesis was that explicit attitude reports would be more consistent with
implicit attitudes among participants encouraged to own their implicit responses than among
those encouraged to disown their implicit responses. We created AMP scores for each participant
by taking the difference between the proportion of pleasant responses on heterosexual prime
Who owns implicit attitudes 14
trials versus homosexual prime trials (higher scores reflect a preference for heterosexual over
homosexual couples). We then tested the hypothesis by estimating the parameters of a regression
model predicting explicit attitudes from implicit attitudes and the implicit attitude x condition
interaction. All variables were standardized prior to analysis.
Figure 1 displays the regression slopes relating implicit and explicit attitudes in each
condition. The main effect was significant neither for implicit attitudes, b = .14, p = .44, nor for
ownership condition, b = -.10, p = .57. The predicted interaction, however, was significant, b = .39, p < .05. The implicit-explicit correlation was significant in the personal condition, r = .53, p
< .05 but not in the extra-personal condition, r = -.21, p = .39. Simple slopes analysis showed
that among participants with high levels of implicit bias (estimated at 1 SD above the mean)
those in the personal inference condition scored .98 standard deviations higher in explicit
homophobia than those in the extra-personal inference condition. These effects illustrate that
metacognitive inferences of ownership can have dramatic consequences for explicit attitudes.
These results supported the hypothesis that when automatically activated evaluations are
interpreted as one’s own attitudes, they serve as a basis for explicit evaluations. When they are
disowned, they became largely irrelevant for explicit evaluations. This pattern supports the idea
that metacognitive inferences about ownership shape the downstream consequences of implicit
cognition.
One potential concern over the manipulation in Experiment 1 is that by directly
instructing participants to generate reasons why their implicit response is or is not their own
attitude, we may have induced experimental demand. Participants may have felt pressure to
respond in ways that were consistent or inconsistent with their affective responses. To address
this possibility, in Experiment 2 we replicated these findings by measuring individual differences
Who owns implicit attitudes 15
in perceived ownership rather than manipulating it. If naturally existing variability in perceptions
of ownership display the same effects then we can have greater confidence in our findings.
Experiment 2
The materials and procedure for Experiment 2 were the same as Experiment 1 except that
there was no manipulation of ownership inferences. Instead we measured individual differences
in perceptions of ownership.
Participants
Participants were 45 undergraduate students from the University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill. Three participants who reported being able to read Chinese were removed from the
sample, leaving a final sample of 42 participants (14 men and 28 women).
Procedure
After providing informed consent, participants completed a demographic questionnaire
and then completed the same AMP as in Experiment 1. Next they were asked to think about how
they felt when they saw the photo primes and then answer questions about their feelings.
Specifically, they were instructed, “Now consider the ‘gut feelings’ you had toward the pictures
of homosexuals. The following questions ask about how much you think these feelings reflect
your genuine attitude toward homosexuals. That is, do these gut feelings reflect your own
views?” They responded to the following three items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7
(strongly agree): “My immediate feelings reflect my own beliefs about homosexuals,” “My gut
reactions reflect my genuine attitude toward homosexuals,” and “My gut reactions have nothing
to do with my real attitude toward gay people” (reverse scored). Finally, participants completed
the Modern Homophobia Scale to measure explicit attitudes and then were debriefed.
Results and Discussion
Who owns implicit attitudes 16
Average AMP responses were analyzed using a repeated measures ANOVA. The
proportion of pleasant responses was much greater on heterosexual prime trials (M = .80, SD =
.16) than on homosexual prime trials (M = .42, SD = .28), F (1, 41) = 52.29, p < .001.
To examine the effects of perceived ownership on the implicit-explicit relationship we
estimated the parameters of a regression analysis predicting explicit homophobia from implicit
attitudes, perceived ownership, and their interaction. All variables were standardized using zscores prior to the analysis. The main effect of implicit attitudes was significant, b = .54, t =
3.79, p < .001, but the main effect of perceived ownership was not, b = .02, t = .16, p = .88.
Critically, the predicted interaction was significant, b = .42, t = 3.15, p < .01, indicating that
perceived ownership moderated implicit-explicit consistency.
The simple slopes relating AMP scores to explicit homophobia at high and low levels of
perceived ownership are displayed in Figure 2. Simple slopes analysis showed that the
association between implicit and explicit attitudes was significant at high levels of perceived
ownership (1 SD above the mean), b = .96, t = 5.86, p < .001. However implicit and explicit
attitudes were not significantly related at low levels of perceived ownership (1 SD below the
mean), b = .12, t = .57, p = .57.
We next examined the effects of perceived ownership separately for individuals with high
and low levels of implicit bias (+/- 1 SD from the mean). Among participants with high levels of
implicit bias, greater perceived ownership was significantly associated with greater levels of
explicit homophobia, b = 0.44, t=2.24, p < 0.05. In contrast, among those with low levels of
implicit bias perceived ownership was associated with significantly lower levels of explicit
homophobia, b = -0.40, t = -2.13, p < .05.
Who owns implicit attitudes 17
These findings suggest that perceived ownership is a powerful moderator of the
relationship between implicit and explicit attitudes, replicating the results of Experiment 1.
Unlike Experiment 1, this study relied on naturally occurring variability in perceived ownership,
making it unlikely that effects were driven by experimental demand. Together, these studies
provide converging evidence that metacognitive inferences of ownership shape the consequences
of automatic evaluations for explicit attitudes.
Experiment 3
Although Experiments 1 and 2 established the role of manipulated and measured
inferences of ownership, they do not rule out the possibility that other factors that are correlated
with ownership may have influenced the findings. In particular, some authors have suggested
that ownership may go hand in hand with endorsement of an attitude as true or valid. In our next
experiment we sought to disentangle the effects of perceived ownership from perceived validity,
as well as providing affirmative evidence for the role of perceived ownership in mediating these
effects.
In this study we combined the designs of the first two experiments by manipulating
ownership inferences and also measuring perceptions of ownership as a mediator. Moreover, we
measured perceived validity of implicit evaluations so that we could assess the effects of
ownership and validity independently. Our main hypothesis was that the manipulation of
ownership would affect implicit-explicit attitude consistency (replicating Experiment 1) and that
this effect would be mediated by the measure of perceived ownership (but not perceived validity)
of implicit responses.
Method
Participants
Who owns implicit attitudes 18
Seventy-six undergraduates participated in return for partial course credit. Data from 3
participants who reported being able to read Chinese pictographs were dropped from analyses,
leaving a final sample of 73 (46 women and 27 men).
Procedure
As in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, participants began by completing an AMP
measuring implicit attitudes toward same-sex and opposite-sex couples. Participants were then
randomly assigned to generate reasons that their feelings either reflected personal attitudes or
extra-personal attitudes. The manipulation was the same as in Experiment 1.
As a measure of our proposed mediator, participants next answered the same three
questions used in Experiment 2 to assess the degree to which they owned their implicit attitudes.
In addition to perceived ownership, we measured perceived validity of respondents’ implicit
attitudes using three additional questions. The instructions for these items read, “Still considering
the "gut feelings" you had toward the pictures of homosexuals, the following questions ask about
how much you think these feelings are valid or correct.” They then rated their agreement on a 7point scale to the following items: “My gut reactions reflect accurate beliefs about homosexuals;
my gut reactions do NOT necessarily reflect true beliefs about homosexuals (reverse scored); my
immediate reactions toward homosexuals are valid.” Internal consistency was adequate for both
perceived ownership (alpha = .72) and perceived validity (alpha = .84).
Next, participants completed the Modern Homophobia Scale to measure explicit
attitudes. After completing the explicit measure, participants were asked to reflect on the
attitudes that they expressed on that scale and to answer the same items measuring perceived
ownership and perceived validity about their explicitly reported attitudes. This provided a basis
Who owns implicit attitudes 19
for comparing metacognitions about implicit and explicit attitudes directly. Following this,
participants were debriefed about the purpose of the study.
Results and Discussion
Mean implicit attitudes
Replicating the previous studies, the proportion of pleasant responses was greater on
heterosexual prime trials (M = .76, SD = .18) than on homosexual prime trials (M = .46, SD =
.29), F (1, 71) = 47.29, p < .001.
Moderated mediation
We hypothesized that perceived ownership of implicit responses should mediate the
effects of the ownership manipulation on implicit-explicit attitude correspondence. Statistically,
this implies a pattern of moderated mediation (Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007). That is, we
expected the manipulation of ownership to affect perceived ownership, which in turn should
affect explicit attitudes. However, increasing perceived ownership of implicit responses should
have differing consequences for individuals with high versus low implicit bias. For individuals
with high implicit bias, experimentally increasing ownership should increase explicit prejudice
and this should be mediated by perceived ownership. But the experimental manipulation should
have little effect for participants with low levels of implicit bias.
The following analyses used the bootstrapping method of Preacher and Hayes (2007) to
test for moderated mediation. The analysis simultaneously estimates the effect of the
manipulation on the proposed mediator, as well as the interaction between the mediator and a
moderating factor. If these effects are significant, one can probe the conditional indirect effects,
which indicate whether the manipulation has an indirect effect via the mediator (perceived
ownership), at different levels of the moderator (implicit bias). Following the recommendations
Who owns implicit attitudes 20
of Hayes () we report the findings in three steps. First, we demonstrate that the manipulation of
perceived ownership affected the measure of perceived ownership. Second, we demonstrate that
perceived ownership moderated the association between implicit and explicit attitudes
(replicating Experiment 2). Third, we test for a conditional indirect effect. We used 10,000
bootstrap re-samples to estimate the coefficients, which are displayed in Table 1.
Effects of manipulation on perceived attitude ownership. As predicted, participants in the
personal-inference group reported greater ownership of implicit responses (M = 5.04, SD = 1.21)
than those in the extrapersonal-inference group (M = 4.27, SD = 1.34), b = 2.32, p = .01.
Effect of perceived ownership in moderating implicit-explicit attitude correspondence.
The Perceived ownership X AMP interaction was significant when predicting explicit attitudes, b
= 3.66, p < .01. As depicted in Figure 3, implicit and explicit attitudes were more strongly related
among participants who perceived their implicit responses to reflect their own attitudes.
Conditional Indirect effects via perceived ownership. Thus far the analysis demonstrates
that perceived ownership moderated implicit-explicit attitude consistency, and that our
manipulation significantly influenced perceived ownership. Probing the conditional indirect
effects revealed a significant indirect effect of the manipulation on explicit attitudes, via
perceived ownership, among participants high in implicit bias, b = .15, p = .055. There were no
significant indirect effects for participants with average levels of bias, b = .06, p = .111 nor low
levels of bias, b = -.03, p = .447. Note that 1 SD below the mean for AMP scores represents a
score of -.07, which is not significantly different from zero. Low bias in this sample thus
represents neutral or indifferent attitudes rather than pro-gay bias. Because of this, it is not
surprising that changes in perceived ownership did not increase or decrease explicit attitudes
Who owns implicit attitudes 21
among participants with low levels of implicit bias. The full moderated mediation model is
depicted in Figure 5.
Among participants with relatively negative implicit responses toward gays, the personal
inference condition increased the extent to which they perceived their implicit responses as their
own attitudes. Perceptions of ownership, in turn, tended to increase the expression of anti-gay
attitudes on the explicit measure. These effects were not evident among participants with
moderate or low levels of implicit bias. Together these effects replicate the findings of
Experiments 1 and 2, and demonstrate that when individuals infer that their implicit responses
reflect their own attitudes, these metacognitive inferences affect explicitly stated attitudes.
Perceived Validity
A parallel bootstrapping analysis was conducted to examine the mediating role of
perceived validity. Perceived validity did not reproduce the same effects as perceived ownership.
Perceived validity was not significantly higher in the personal-inference group (M = 4.49, SD =
1.36) than the extrapersonal inference group (M = 4.10, SD = 1.29), b = .15, p = .20. Thus, the
manipulation selectively affected ownership. Next, we examined the conditional indirect effects
of perceived validity. The indirect effect of the manipulation on explicit attitudes, via perceived
validity was non-significant at low implicit bias, b = -.02, p = .50, as well as at high levels of
implicit bias, b = .08, p = .22. Thus, there was no evidence that perceived validity, rather than
perceived ownership, was responsible for the effects we observed.
Although perceptions of validity were not affected by the manipulation, individual
differences in perceived validity did significantly moderate the relationship between implicit and
explicit attitudes. As displayed in Figure 4, implicit and explicit attitudes were highly correlated
for participants who perceived their implicit responses to be valid (b = .98, p < .001), but the
Who owns implicit attitudes 22
relationship was weaker for those who perceived them as less valid (b = .36, p = .02). This
finding is consistent with the APE model (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006) and the
metacognitive model (Petty, Brinol, & DeMarree, 2007), which suggest that metacognitive
inferences about the validity of one’s attitudes are an important factor differentiating implicit and
explicit attitudes. However, this finding does not conflict with our hypothesis that perceived
ownership has moderating effects distinct from perceived validity.
Experiment 4
The preceding experiments provided evidence that metacognitions about ownership
shape whether implicit attitudes are expressed explicitly. However there is reason to expect that
such metacognitions also affect implicitly activated attitudes themselves. Inferring that a
particular attitude is one’s own may entail forming an association between the attitude object and
one’s own likes and dislikes. Over time, these associations may influence not only explicit
reports, but also automatically activated evaluations. In our final experiment we tested whether
perceived ownership would moderate whether implicit evaluations were detected on an implicit
test designed to measure personal associations. To this end, we compared responses on Olson
and Fazio’s (2004) personalized IAT to the standard IAT.
Following the format of the previous studies, we first measured implicit attitudes toward
gay men using the AMP. Next, we measured perceived ownership using the same items as
experiments 2 and 3. Finally, we assessed implicit attitudes toward gay men using either the
personalized or standard IAT (randomly assigned between subjects). We predicted that the
personalized IAT would be sensitive to implicit attitudes measured by the AMP only when
participants perceived them as their own. In contrast, we did not expect perceived ownership to
Who owns implicit attitudes 23
moderate the effect for the standard IAT because it is thought to reflect both personal and extrapersonal associations.
Method
Participants
One hundred eighteen undergraduates participated in return for course credit. Seven
participants reported the ability to read Chinese characters, three additional participants pressed
the same key on all trials of the AMP, and one participant was considered an outlier due to an
IAT score more than three standard deviations above the mean. After removing these, the final
sample included 107 participants.
Procedure
The first two parts of the experiment were identical to Experiment 2. Participants
completed the AMP and then answered three questions assessing perceived ownership over the
feelings they experienced during the task. Next, participants were randomly assigned to complete
either the personalized IAT or the standard IAT. The IAT’s each used the same photos of same
sex and cross-sex couples as in the AMP. For the personal IAT, … For the standard IAT… Need
details re: stimuli, judgment task, block structure, and number of trials, and order info, etc.
Following the IAT, participants completed the Modern Homophobia Scale to assess explicit
attitudes. Because our previous studies showed that ownership inferences affected explicit
attitudes, we controlled for the MHS to ensure that any effects observed on implicit measures
were not due to shared variance with explicit attitudes.
Results and Discussion
Preliminary analyses
Who owns implicit attitudes 24
IAT scores were computed using the D score method (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji,
2003). AMP scores were computed by subtracting the proportion of pleasant judgments on
straight prime trials from the proportion pleasant on gay prime trials. All implicit measures
showed anti-gay bias that was significantly greater than zero. Average IAT scores were M = .79,
SD = .72, t(52) = 7.99, p < .001 for the personal IAT, and M = .76, SD = .66, t(53) = 8.34, p <
.001 for the standard IAT. Average AMP scores were M = .37, SD = .40, t(106) = 9.64, p < .001
(if scored as a d-score the effect size is .93, similar to the IAT’s {Check this}). Moreover, AMP
and both IAT’s were significantly correlated (AMP-personal IAT r = .57; AMP-standard IAT r =
.44), p’s < .001.
Primary analyses
Our main hypothesis was that inferences of ownership would moderate the relationship
between AMP scores and personal IAT scores, but not standard IAT scores. We tested the
hypothesis by estimating the parameters of a regression model in which IAT scores were the
dependent variable. On the first step we entered a dummy code indicating whether the participant
completed the personal or standard IAT, along with AMP scores, perceived ownership, and
explicit attitude scores as a covariate. On the second step we entered all two-way interactions,
and on the third step we entered the three way interaction between IAT condition, AMP scores,
and perceived ownership. Our hypothesis predicts a three-way interaction in which the AMP x
perceived ownership interaction is significant in the personal IAT condition but not the standard
IAT condition.
As displayed in Table 2, AMP scores and explicit attitudes were both significant
predictors of IAT scores. Most importantly, the predicted three-way interaction was significant.
To decompose the interaction, we examined the AMP x perceived ownership interaction
Who owns implicit attitudes 25
separately for the personal and standard IAT’s. As displayed in Figure 6, perceived ownership
moderated the relationship between the AMP and the personalized IAT (at marginally significant
levels, b = .18, p = .07). Simple slopes analyses showed that … Perceived ownership did not
moderate the association between the AMP and the standard IAT, b = -.08, p = .39.
These results indicate that inferences of ownership had consequences for whether implicit
evaluations measured by the AMP were reflected in other implicit measures. Perceived
ownership moderated the effects of implicit bias on the personalized IAT, which is designed to
measure personal associations, but not the standard IAT, which may measure both personal and
extra-personal associations. These effects were independent of explicit attitudes. Together, these
results suggest that inferences about ownership affect not only explicit expression of attitudes,
but also implicit evaluations themselves.
General Discussion
We presented evidence that inferences about the ownership of implicit responses have
important consequences for whether those implicit responses function like personal attitudes –
both explicitly and implicitly. When participants disowned their implicit responses, they reported
explicit attitudes that were only weakly associated with implicit responses. In contrast, when
they judged their implicit responses to reflect their own attitudes, implicit and explicit attitudes
were highly associated.
The moderating effects of ownership inferences were dramatic. Across the three studies,
the average implicit-explicit attitude correlation was r = .09 in the low ownership conditions (i.e.,
the extra-personal inference group in Experiment 1, and at 1 SD below the mean of perceived
ownership in Experiments 2 and 3), whereas it was r = .82 in high ownership conditions (i.e., the
personal inference group in Experiment 1, and at 1 SD above the mean of perceived ownership
Who owns implicit attitudes 26
in Experiments 2 and 3). When participants perceived their implicit responses to be their own,
their implicit and explicit attitudes were, in fact, nearly identical. Yet when disowned, implicit
responses were almost entirely unrelated to explicit reports.
Still, owning an evaluation does more than simply lead people to explicitly report their
attitudes. Ownership inferences rendered attitudes toward gay men more closely related to
participants’ personal likes and dislikes, as opposed to general semantic categories of good and
bad. This may have been due to a momentary construal as emphasized by the APE model
(Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006) and the Situated Inference Model (Loersch & Payne, 2011).
Alternatively, it may be due to stored traces of the inference process. Much as the MCM suggests
that people encode and store validity tags, they may also store the traces of other metacognitive
processes. If so, the present results might suggest the existence of “ownership tags.” More
research is needed, as our Experiment 4 does not distinguish between these two possibilities.
Either way, our results are the first to suggest that ownership-as-metacognition (Banaji…,
Gawronski…) and ownership-as-associations (Olson & Fazio, 2004) are not necessarily
incompatible. In fact, metacognitive inferences of ownership may be one mechanism by which
representations of an attitude object come to be linked with personal or extra-personal
evaluations.
Implications for the nature of implicit and explicit social cognition
The effects reported are consistent with dual process theories that emphasize
metacognitive inferences about implicit attitudes (Hofmann & Wilson, 2010; Gawronski &
Bodenhausem, 2006; Petty, Brinol, & DeMarree, 2007). Those theories have tended to
emphasize metacognitions about the validity of implicit responses, and the present data support
the importance of validity inferences. However, the present data also suggest an expansion of
Who owns implicit attitudes 27
these theoretical perspectives by highlighting that other kinds of inferences besides validity may
also be critical. Inferences about validity essentially ask, “is my attitude correct?” We have
focused here on inferences of ownership (i.e., “is this attitude my own?”), but other kinds of
inferences may also be important. For example, independent of these other inferences, people
might make inferences about whether they like or dislike their automatic responses (e.g., “I
spontaneously feel negative affect toward gay men but I wish I didn’t”).
Similar post-hoc inferences might be made regarding whether an individual intends or
endorses the ideas that have come automatically to mind. Intention is one of the key criteria that
distinguish automatic processes from controlled processes (Bargh, 1994). And endorsement has
long been a critical factor in distinguishing between implicit and explicit attitudes (Devine,
1989). Nonetheless, research on the experience of agency suggests that people sometimes engage
in post-hoc inferences about whether they intended to perform an action (Wegner, Fuller, &
Sparrow, 2003) or whether they authored an idea (Preston & Wegner, 2007). Such findings
highlight that intent and endorsement can be consequences of automatic processes rather than
their cause. The present findings suggest that such inferences may be important for the explicit
responses that follow from automatic processes. Post-hoc inferences about whether one intends
or endorses ideas that come automatically to mind may shape whether those ideas are overtly
expressed.
Considering the variety of metacognitive inferences one could make when reflecting on
their own automatic responses highlights other new possibilities as well. Simple affective
responses such as those measured in the AMP and other implicit measures might be interpreted
in a variety of ways by the person completing the task. Recent theories of emotion hold that
simple good/bad affective reactions take on the specific qualities of discrete emotions once they
Who owns implicit attitudes 28
are categorized as a particular emotion by the perceiver (Barrett, 2006; Lindquist & Barrett,
2008). For instance, negative affect experienced toward gay targets during the task could be
interpreted either as disliking them, or as feelings of discomfort about an unfamiliar group, or
even sympathetic distress for members of a stigmatized group.
Consistent with this idea, one study found that perceiving a group to be dislikeable
produced the same “negative” scores on an IAT as perceiving the group to be the blameless
victim of oppression (Uhlmann, Brescoll, & Paluck, 2006). Our metacognitive perspective
suggests that the affective responses assessed by implicit tests may in some cases not
differentiate between a range of qualitatively different emotions. Metacognitive inferences,
however, may transform simple affective valence into specific and nuanced emotions. Such
different appraisals of equally negative valence might have very different behavioral
consequences. Future research should explore how qualitatively different metacognitive
inferences about a single affective reaction might lead to a diversity of emotional and behavioral
effects.
Real prejudice?
In an influential critique of implicit bias research, Arkes and Tetlock (2004) argued that
implicit bias should only be considered to be prejudice in a socially and morally meaningful
sense if it reflects “personal animus,” but not if it reflects “shared cultural stereotypes,” (p. 257).
Setting aside the fact that this claim is itself controversial (), our findings suggest that any claims
about the distinction between personal versus non-personal nature of implicit attitudes should be
made with caution. Imagine two individuals who both have an equivalently high level of implicit
bias. One person assumes that the negative feelings reflect his personal attitudes, but the other
does not. Should such metacognitive inferences matter for whether we consider these evaluations
Who owns implicit attitudes 29
to be prejudice? We suggest that they should not, especially since the inferences can be made
post-hoc rather than having a causal role in forming the evaluation.
Metacognitive inferences about ownership might have other indirect effects, however,
that are relevant for whether an implicit bias is morally or socially consequential. For example,
we found that manipulating inferences about ownership affected participants’ explicit attitude
reports, including endorsement of discriminatory behaviors and policies. These results suggest
that even if ownership is inferred post-hoc, it is not simply a passive observer or epiphenomenon.
The inferences participants made at one moment had consequences for their explicit
endorsements a moment later. Most theorists, of course, agree that explicit endorsement qualifies
as morally and socially relevant. From our point of view, both automatic evaluation and
metacognitive inferences about it function as component processes -- neither of which is morally
blameworthy itself -- which give rise to more complex emergent effects. We suggest that explicit
endorsement (or rejection) is one part of a potentially complex cycle which may itself provide
feedback that modifies associations in memory.
The line between personal animus and cultural knowledge, therefore, cannot easily be
drawn in a simple or permanent way. The same evaluative association might be construed as
personal at one moment, and in this case would indeed function as a personal attitude. The next
moment, it may be construed differently and therefore function differently.
Who owns implicit attitudes 30
Who owns implicit attitudes 31
References
Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of Implicit Prejudice, or 'Would Jesse
Jackson 'Fail' the Implicit Association Test?'. Psychological Inquiry, 15(4), 257-278.
Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In H. L. Roediger, III, J. S.
Nairne, I. Neath, &
A. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp.
117-150). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen. of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency,
and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social
cognition (2nd ed., Vol. I). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the Emotion Paradox: Categorization and the Experience of
Emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 20-46.
Brunswick, E. (1952). The conceptual framework of psychology. (International Encyclopedia of
Unified Science, Volume 1, Number 10.) Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Cameron, C.D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J., & Payne, B.K. (submitted). Sequential priming measures of
implicit social cognition: A meta-analysis of associations with behaviors and explicit
attitudes.
Cameron, C.D., Payne, B.K., & Knobe, J. (2010). Do theories of implicit race bias change moral
judgments? Social Justice Research, 23, 272-279.
Dan-Cohen, M. (1992). Responsibility and the boundaries of the self. Harvard Law Review,
101(5), 959-1003.
Devine, P . G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5-18.
Who owns implicit attitudes 32
Frankfurt, H. G. (1969). Alternate possibilities and moral responsibility. The Journal of
Philosophy, 66, 829-839.
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in
evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological
Bulletin, 132, 692-731.
Gawronski, B., Peters, K. R., & LeBel, E. P. (2008). What makes mental associations personal or
extra-personal? Conceptual issues in the methodological debate about implicit attitude
measures. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1002-1023.
Gibson, J.J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Oxford England: Houghton
Mifflin.
Greenwald, A. G., Smith, C., Sriram, N. N., Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Implicit race
attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 U.S. Presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues
and Public Policy (ASAP), 9, 241-253.
Han, H., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). The influence of experimentally created
extrapersonal associations on the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 42, 259-272.
Hofmann, W., & Wilson, T. D. (2010). Consciousness, introspection, and the adaptive
unconscious. In B. Gawronski, B. Payne, B. Gawronski, B. Payne (Eds.) , Handbook of
implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 197-215). New
York, NY US: Guilford Press.
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774-788.
Who owns implicit attitudes 33
Kelley, C. M. & Jacoby, L. L. (1990). The construction of Subjective Experience: Memory
Attributions. Mind & Language, 5, 49–68.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review
of General Psychology, 2, 175-220.
Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong to us: Searching for
attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 22(4), 553-594.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the Influence of Extrapersonal Associations on
the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 86, 653-667.
Payne, B., Cheng, C., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect
misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
89, 277-293.
Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The Metacognitive Model (MCM) of
attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement, change, and strength. Social Cognition,
25, 657-686.
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Assessing moderated mediation
hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42,
185-227.
Preston, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). The Eureka error: Inadvertent plagiarism by misattribution
of effort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 575-584.
Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1979). Testing hypotheses about other people: The use of historical
knowledge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 330-342.
Who owns implicit attitudes 34
Sripada, C. (2009). The deep self model and asymmetries in folk judgments about intentional
action. Philosophical Studies, 151, 159–176.
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220-247.
Uhlmann, E. L., Brescoll, V. L., Paluck, E. L. (2006). Are members of low status groups
perceived as bad, or badly off? Egalitarian negative associations and automatic prejudice.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 491-499.
Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wegner, D. M., Fuller, V. A., & Sparrow, B. (2003). Clever hands: Uncontrolled intelligence in
facilitated communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 5-19 .
Yoshida, E., Peach, J. M., Zanna, M. P., & Spencer, S. J. (in press). Not all automatic
associations are created equal: How implicit normative evaluations are distinct from
implicit attitudes and uniquely predict meaningful behavior. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology.
Who owns implicit attitudes 35
TABLE 1
Moderated Mediation of Implicit- Explicit Attitude Correspondence
Mediator Variable Model: Outcome = Perceived ownership
Predictor
Ownership condition
B
.29
SE
.11
t
2.57
P
.012
Dependent Variable Model: Outcome = Explicit homophobia
Predictor
Ownership condition
Perceived ownership
AMP
Perceived Ownership x AMP
B
-.01
.21
.50
.30
SE
.08
.09
.10
.11
t
.20
2.41
4.92
2.74
P
.839
.019
.000
.008
Conditional Indirect Effects
Low bias (-1 SD)
High bias (+1 SD)
Indirect B
Bootstrap SE
95% confidence interval
-.03
.15
.03
.08
(-.11, .03)
(.03, .34)
Note. Significant effects are in bold type.
Who owns implicit attitudes 36
Author Note
Correspondence should be directed to Keith Payne, Department of Psychology,
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Campus Box 3270, Chapel Hill, NC 27599. Email:
[email protected]. This research was supported by the National Science Foundation 0924252.
Who owns implicit attitudes 37
Footnote
Who owns implicit attitudes 38
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Influence of ownership manipulation on correspondence between implicit and explicit
attitudes, Experiment 1.
Figure 2. Moderating effect of perceived attitude ownership on correspondence between implicit
and explicit attitudes, Experiment 2.
Figure 3. Moderating effect of perceived attitude ownership on correspondence between implicit
and explicit attitudes, Experiment 3.
Figure 4. Moderating effect of perceived attitude validity on correspondence between implicit
and explicit attitudes, Experiment 3.
Figure 5. Moderated mediation showing that ownership condition indirectly affected explicit
attitudes via perceived ownership, conditional upon implicit bias in AMP scores. * p < .05, ** p
< .01, Experiment 3.
Who owns implicit attitudes 39
Figure 1.
Who owns implicit attitudes 40
Figure 2.
Who owns implicit attitudes 41
Figure 3.
Who owns implicit attitudes 42
Figure 4.
Who owns implicit attitudes 43
Figure 5.