Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications: An Evaluation of Law Enforcement Procedures for Eliciting Eyewitness Information Anita M. Scotti Argosy University—Orange County Forensic Psychology Seminar Special Topics Paper November 29, 2008 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications Abstract “It is estimated that more than 200 people a day are identified as suspects from eyewitness identification;” more than that same number of people have been imprisoned wrongly on the evidence of eyewitness identifications (Burton, 2008, p. 6). The year 2008 marks the tenth anniversary of the technical working group convened by then Attorney General Janet Reno in 1998 to review the issues surrounding eyewitness testimony, its collection, and uses throughout the legal system. This paper attempts to examine current law enforcement procedures concerning interviews, interrogations, and identification as they specifically relate to eyewitness testimony. Eyewitness Evidence: A Guide for Law Enforcement (U.S. Department Of Justice, 1999) and Eyewitness Evidence: A Trainer’s Manual for Law Enforcement (USDOJ, 2003), publications which grew out of the technical working group, as well as a large body of psychological research are the bases for the evaluation and are summarized herein. A review of relevant literature on eyewitness testimony is provided as an introduction to the topic and then followed with a discussion of police procedure and practice. Lastly, some comments are included on the consequences of a failure to fully comply with procedures designed to protect eyewitness memory along with recommendations for future research and policy direction. 1 A. M. Scotti Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications: An Evaluation of Law Enforcement Procedures for Eliciting Eyewitness Information In this age of “forensic fever,” when television shows like CSI and Law & Order have given ordinary people a passing familiarity with law enforcement, legal procedure, and scientific crime solving methods, it is hard to imagine a criminal case being closed without the aid of fingerprints, DNA, or a gas chromatograph mass spectrometer. The reality of criminalistics, however, is that often there is no physical evidence of merit or the cost of testing what little evidence there might be is prohibitive given the severity of most crimes (which are not murders). Eyewitness testimony has been the main type of evidence by which suspects have been identified and brought to justice throughout human history, long before the relatively recent advent of DNA testing in the 1980s and 90s. The simple act of one person identifying another as the perpetrator of a crime is regarded as one of the most damning types of evidence (Loftus, 1989). Whether candidly or maliciously sworn, whether accurate or erroneous, the word of a witness can send both the innocent and the guilty to prison or to their deaths. But DNA is helping to prove eyewitness identification of suspects, the preferred mode of establishing guilt for centuries, is not the infallible crime-solving juggernaut it seems to be. According to information from the Innocence Project, a group dedicated to “exonerating wrongfully convicted people” and “reforming the criminal justice system,” in 220 known cases where post-conviction DNA testing has led to exoneration, mistaken identification by eyewitnesses was implicated in 75% of the original verdicts against those found innocent (n.d.). Wrongful imprisonment and wrongful execution, in the extreme case, are potential consequences of poor eyewitness testimony. Because of the gravity of these outcomes, there is interest in investigating the factors 2 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications contributing to mistaken identifications, especially by researchers in the field of psychology, who have contributed to a young but prolific body of literature dedicated to the subject. The field of forensic psychology, that is the application of psychological knowledge and techniques to the practice of law, can be said to have begun with the work of Hermann Ebbinghaus and Hugo Münsterberg. Both men were experimental psychologists working with memory in and around the turn of the twentieth century. Ebbinghaus demonstrated, with his now famous “forgetting curve,” the rapid rate at which memories faded. Münsterberg helped to forward the cause of eyewitness testimony research and its application to legal matters thanks to a popular book, On the Witness Stand (1908), and several flamboyant turns as an expert witness on the subjects of memory fallibility and suggestibility (Wrightsman, & Fulero, 2005). The same pomposity which made Münsterberg one of the first psychologists to have a household name contributed to him making exaggerated claims about the ability of psychologists to detect truth with certainty, the ripple effects of which forensic psychologists still feel to this day in matters like polygraph use and risk assessment. In Münsterberg’s wake, forensic psychology and eyewitness identification research was largely silent until a renaissance in the 1970s. A shift in social psychology from an academic to a more applied orientation spurred new interest in the potential contributions of psychology to public policy and law (Wrightsman, & Fulero, 2005). Today, the influence of forensic psychology is felt in many aspects of the legal system, from juror selection and adjudicative competency to assessing eyewitness reliability. Currently, over 2000 publications address some facet of the latter topic—eyewitness testimony (Burton, 2008). Research in this area, just as it was in the days of Hugo Münsterberg, is primarily undertaken by cognitive and experimental psychologists rather than social psychologists due to the insurmountable difficulty of performing 3 A. M. Scotti ethically and methodologically sound research on actual participants in the legal system. Issues surrounding eyewitness testimony must often be approached obliquely. Despite these obstacles, eyewitness research is flourishing and becoming more specialized as time goes on. The focus of this paper lies at the intersection of eyewitness research and research into the form and function of police interview, interrogation, and identification procedures—all of which are means for law enforcement officers to elicit information from eyewitnesses. Consequently, the law enforcement procedures of interest within this paper are confined to activities taking place during the witness questioning portion of an interview/interrogation or during an identification process (i.e. lineup), despite the wealth of information available on other salient topics such as false confessions, recovered memories, lie detection, the Miranda warning, and questioning juveniles. To fully understand how psychological research into eyewitness testimony is related to interviews, interrogations, and identification procedures, and vice versa, a basic foundation in the terminology and theory of the two spheres of inquiry is necessary. Since the public exposure of the deplorable questioning practices colloquially known as the third degree, including physical abuse akin to torture, law enforcement has constantly sought to redefine its public image in terms of professionalism and scientific precision (Leo, 2008). One way of achieving this goal has been to distance law enforcement agencies and officers from the image of browbeating authoritarianism conjured up by words like interrogation. There has been a recent trend toward using the more neutral term interview to refer to the practice of questioning both witnesses and suspects. For purposes of this paper, however, interrogation shall refer to questioning suspects, accomplices, and hostile witnesses; the person being questioned is usually under some form of custody (i.e. arrest or imprisonment for a prior crime) and must be read their Miranda Rights. Interview, on the other hand, shall refer to the non- 4 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications custodial questioning of victims, witnesses, or informants. The purpose of both the interview and interrogation is to obtain information in furtherance of a criminal investigation, but an interrogation has the added goal of trying to elicit incriminating statements or confessions. Police also employ a variety of identification procedures in order to elicit information from eyewitnesses, including lineups, photoarrays, field identifications, mug books, and composite drawings. A lineup, or an identity parade, consists of six or more people, including a suspect and several others of similar appearance (known as foils or fillers), literally lined up sideby-side and viewed by a witness who then selects the person he or she identifies as the culprit or notes the perpetrator was absent from the lineup. A photoarray, also known as a photospread or six-pack, is the paper equivalent of a lineup with photographs of a suspect and foils standing in for real people either in a one page batch of six or as six individual photos. The photoarray is currently the most popular form of identification procedure, since it is portable. Creation and administration of a photoarray is also significantly less time consuming than for a lineup (Burton, 2008). A field identification, or showup, is an identification procedure in which a witness views only the suspect, at or near the crime scene, usually from the safety of a squad car. Mug books, or the Rogues Gallery, are collections of booking photographs through which a witness might wade in an effort to identify a suspect. Lastly, a composite drawing may be prepared by a sketch artist from the verbal description of a perpetrator’s face or by a witness using a computer program. None of these identification procedures guarantees the presence of the suspect or the accurate representation of the subject, a fact which should be told to witnesses. Cognitive and experimental psychology research extending back to Münsterberg, Ebbinghaus, and beyond has clearly shown memory to be more fragile and more malleable than is generally believed. A memory is not like a video recording of an event stored away until 5 A. M. Scotti faultless playback is required; rather a memory is a collection of diverse impressions and sensory feedback which is rarely complete and sometimes does not survive long (Loftus, 1979). The information provided by eyewitnesses is further distinguished from memory in that it is memory testimony, a statement of what is remembered (Wells, 1995 as cited in Wrightsman, & Fulero, 2005). Memory is shaped by the cognitive processes by which information is taken in and processed, but memory testimony is subject to a slew of additional reasoning and social processes. In view of this distinction, the sources of misidentifications and other eyewitness errors are clearly result from those added processes. The following section will present a brief overview of issues related to memory and eyewitness testimony as borne out by psychological research, including an explanation of some memory altering effects. Ideally, police interview, interrogation, and identification procedures should be able to cleanly access the information contained in a witness’ memory, but these post hoc procedures do exert some effect on the accuracy of eyewitness testimony. Therefore, an evaluation of how law enforcement techniques for eliciting eyewitness information contribute or detract from the resilience and integrity of witness memory is a worthy goal, to be pursued by this paper. Eyewitness Research Overview Memory and the Eyewitness The process of committing something to memory is almost universally described by psychologists as consisting of three stages: encoding (or acquisition), storage (or retention), and retrieval (Greene, Heilbrun, Fortune, & Nietzel, 2007; Loftus, 1979; Roediger, &Gallo, 2002). During encoding, the details of an event or object of attention are perceived by our five senses. The (often incomplete or misremembered) information gathered by those senses enters into the neural pathways were it is processed and goes into the memory system. During storage, the 6 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications content of the sensory input as well as information and relationships found during encoding processes is held in short- or long-term memory until forgotten or recalled during the retrieval stage. Retrieval is not a straightforward search-and-capture activity; irrelevant memories related to the target memory can surface or one may feel information is momentarily irretrievable. The memory which is retrieved may be as accurate as possible given its quality or what is dredged up may include memory illusion—remembering events and objects differently than how they actually were or even fabricating details that never were (Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). Encoding. The three stage conception of memory is also useful for discussing the many ways in which memory, and hence eyewitness accuracy, can be negatively influenced, with the encoding stage being of particular importance to the formation of eyewitness memory. Acquiring memories is a function of sensory input, so any circumstance which might result in perception being impaired, narrowed, or otherwise altered, can affect the content and quality of memory. There are two broad classes of factors which can influence eyewitness memory at the encoding stage: event factors and personal factors (Loftus, 1979; Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). Event factors are situational variables such as lighting, angle of view, distance to target, weather, etc, which limit the amount of sensory information available. What a witness cannot sense, they cannot know or remember accurately. In general, the longer the amount of time spent focused on a detail, the more frequent the opportunity to observe the detail, and the more salient and unusual the detail, then the better the recollection of that detail will be (Loftus, 1979). Salience and unusualness, however, can work against memory completeness and accuracy, as happens with the phenomenon of weapon focus. Weapon focus is a narrowing of attention which occurs in the presence of a weapon; a greater amount of the mental resources available for perception are occupied by attention to that weapon (Greene et al., 2007). With less 7 A. M. Scotti attention to spare, other sensory details may be encoded incompletely, incorrectly, or not at all. Personal factors, like event factors, can limit eyewitness accuracy, but unlike event factors, they are inherent in the witness not the situation. The presence of a weapon or other threat of violence is an event factor closely linked to the personal factor of stress. Psychological research into the effect subjective stress level has on memory has yielded mixed results. Stress is thought to be related to perception in a Yerkes-Dodson Law type relationship, represented by an inverted U or V shape graph. Stress creates emotional arousal, and thereby improved attention, up to a point of diminishing return; thereafter increased stress causes increased perceptual difficulty (Loftus, 1979; Cutler, & Penrod, 1995). Besides stress, personal factors include the static characteristics such as sex, race, age, and intelligence as well as more dynamic factors like past experience, stereotypes, temporary biases, and intoxication. Of course, personal factors also include any medical and mental health issues on the part of the witness which may limit or distort perception and encoding. The influence of the static personal factors is not as strong as that of the dynamic ones. No strong sex difference in witness ability to make an accurate identification has been seen in the research (Cutler, & Penrod, 1995). Female witnesses do show a slightly greater tendency to make correct identifications, but this is offset by an equally slight elevation in mistaken identifications (Burton, 2008). Likewise, there is no significant association between racial/ethnic group or level of intelligence and ability to correctly identify criminal suspects (Cutler, & Penrod, 1995). Age is the only static personal variable which demonstrates a clear relationship with witness accuracy. Several studies have shown percentage of correct identifications increases with age until a drop off among elderly witnesses (Cutler, & Penrod, 1995). 8 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications In contrast to the static personal factors, the dynamic factors listed above all wield considerable but variable influence on eyewitness memory. Intoxication, be it from alcohol or drugs, is a common feature of many crimes. When it is the witness or victim who is intoxicated, the result can be a decrease in the amount of information correctly recalled which manifests during questioning soon after a crime and continues past the period of intoxication (Yuille, & Tollestrup, 1990 as cited in Cutler, & Penrod, 1995). This is not surprising since alcohol and drugs impair a persons’ perceptual functioning. All of the other dynamic personal factors besides stress and intoxication—past experience, stereotypes, and temporary biases—create expectations in the mind of the witness. Past experience can distort memories of a current event through the intrusion of inferences drawn from an earlier event or can cloud perception of details which do not conform to expectations (Loftus, 1979). Stereotypical expectations work as might be anticipated. Their particular importance for eyewitness accuracy comes when the witness’ memory lacks details which are subsequently filled in according to stereotypical expectations. Temporary biases formed from recent activities or thoughts can affect the way ambiguous stimuli are perceived. In her seminal 1979 book on eyewitness testimony, Loftus illustrates this quirk of memory with a figure called the “rat-man,” which can be perceived as a rat or a man depending on whether the observer was primed to see a face or an animal (p. 44). Storage. Between the encoding of a memory and its eventual retrieval, during the stage known as storage or retention, the memory is not held in some inviolable state but rather is still subject to addition, alteration, and distortion. It may also be forgotten entirely. As mentioned above, forgetting was one of the first concerns of experimental psychologists investigating memory as well as of the earliest forensic practitioners. What common sense suggests about memory and forgetting—that the longer a memory goes without being recalled, the more it is 9 A. M. Scotti forgotten—is for the most part true. More specifically, Ebbinghaus’ experiments allowed him to plot forgetting as a curve with a steep loss of retained information soon after it is initially committed to memory. Over time, the downward trend levels out and forgetting becomes more gradual (Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). This model of forgetting portrays the type during which information which was encoded into memory becomes less sharp and complete over time, but there is also forgetting of the kind where a memory seems to have just vanished wholly when an effort is made to recall it. This total blankness is the result of a storage/retention error. The information encoded during the first stage of memory could have in a sense gotten lost as it was being moved from short- to long-term storage. Interference is also a likely culprit. Proactive interference occurs when prior memories block the effective storage of newer ones. More commonly, retroactive interference occurs when newer memories give the impression of having overwritten the older ones (Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). Both incarnations of interference are more likely when the content of the relative old and new memories is similar. Where there is some inkling that memories of interest exists but are merely inaccessible, on the other hand, both groups have historically spent considerable effort trying to get at them. Unfortunately, their labors are of questionable merit; a large body of research casts doubt on the integrity of “recovered” memories, with greater doubt cast upon them as the intervals between alleged experiences and the eventual recovery of memory lengthen. Because the vagaries of forgetting make it difficult to remedy, postevent information is instead the foremost concern of researchers who investigate the connections between law enforcement interview, interrogation, and identifications techniques on eyewitness testimony. According to Loftus, (1979, p. 55), “postevent information can not only enhance existing memories but also change a witness’ memory and even cause nonexistent details to become incorporated into a previously acquired 10 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications memory.” The challenge then for researchers is to draw the line between the postevent information which aids eyewitness accuracy and that which destroys it; the challenge for police is to walk that line while completing their primary information gathering objective. Victims and witnesses of crime rarely experience a vacuum of information after so serious an experience. They may revisit the scene and notice new features than before and they may participate in or overhear discussion of the event. If the crime is serious enough, there could be media coverage. Just as stories are embroidered with each successive telling, each instance of postevent information may present a witness with different perspective and details than in their original memory. Experimenters in one accessible example of this used five different verbs in questionnaires asking for witnesses’ approximations of the speed of the cars involved in a staged collision: contacted, hit, bumped, collided and smashed (Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). In this respective order, the verbs elicited progressively larger speed estimates. The subtle differences in the connotations of the five verbs were shown to have influenced witnesses’ retrospective analyses of an event. Postevent information of this sort does not necessarily alter the fundamental substance of recollections; it can even enhance memory by highlighting the most critical parts of an event, but the potential clearly exists for postevent information to exert real change, if minor in the given example, in the relation of that event from memory. More serious than mere exaggeration of essentially true facets of memory are the results of exposure to erroneous postevent information. If later questioning or discussion presents a witness with information contrary to what they remember, what happens? The answer largely depends on the nature of the conflicting information. When the difference is minor, such as of color or number, a compromise memory may be created in which future recollections will include a value midway between what the original memory contained and what was told in the 11 A. M. Scotti inaccurate postevent information (Loftus, 1979). Offender’s clothing remembered as yellow but described elsewhere as red may be recalled by a victim as orange. When the difference between the encoded and postevent information concerns a detail central to the understanding of an event or identity, however, memories of these salient details are more resistant to alteration (Loftus, 1979). Blatant contradiction between witness recollection and postevent information also produces resistance to memory alteration which, in addition to causing rejection of obvious errors, acts as a protective factor against incorporating minor detail errors which would otherwise yield compromise memories (Loftus, 1979). The above examples presuppose the object or event that is the subject of the descriptive error between recollection and postevent information was perceived and encoded in the first place. If no information about it was retained at all, then there is the possibility illusory detail could be incorporated into a new wrong-yet-convincing memory (Loftus, 1979). This memory may then be recalled with clarity and a feeling of having sensed the erroneous detail all along (Harley, Carlsen, & Loftus, 2004). Known as hindsight bias, this tendency is a particular danger to accuracy of eyewitness testimony. Retrieval. The bounds between each of the three stages of memory—encoding, storage, and retrieval—are vague and somewhat arbitrary distinctions. In the case of eyewitness testimony under interview, interrogation, or identification conditions, the division between the storage and retrieval stages is especially tenuous, as many of the processes, like influence by postevent information, are taking place as the witness is simultaneously being asked to recall and recount memory content. For example, people who anticipate being tested on memorized information (as a witness might expect to be interviewed by police) are more likely to store the relevant memories in such a way as to make retrieval easier (Cutler, & Penrod, 1995). A testing effect also exists in which information which is subjected to testing will subsequently be 12 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications remembered better (Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). Another closely related memory effect which bridges storage and retrieval is the freezing effect. The freezing effect manifests in how information which has been recalled previously becomes more likely to be recalled in the future (Loftus, 1979). What has happened is the neural pathway which led to the recounted item of memory became strengthened by the activation which led to that particular recollection, hence it will be more quickly accessible and seem more firmly rooted in memory the next time the pathway is used. Unfortunately, all of these memory enhancement effects of will also strengthen memories with incorrect elements with the same force as with accurate memories. The quantity, quality, and content of retrieved memories often depends on situational factors which improve or inhibit accurate recollection. Sometimes recreating features of the original memory situation, such as returning to the scene when lighting or weather conditions are similar, may provide sensory cues to witnesses who subsequently remember more information than without the benefit of those cues. It is also possible to enhance memory through recreating a similar level of emotional arousal, an example of state-dependent retrieval (Roediger, & Gallo, 2002). Typically, the more closely the environment during recall approximates that at the time the target memory was encoded, the easier accurate retrieval. The narrative form taken when recounting memories also has implications for retrieval. If questions are being asked in an attempt to elicit information, those questions may improve recall because of the inherent prompts to memory contained in their language. They may also suffer from postevent information problems and can cause witnesses to restrict their responses to answers within the scope of the question (Loftus, 1979). With a freer form of narrative, as with open-ended questions, witnesses volunteer more spontaneous, potentially useful information. Both methods have drawbacks, but the use of free recall then question-prompted recall mitigates those (Loftus, 1979). 13 A. M. Scotti One final, but potentially devastating phenomenon associated with the memory retrieval stage is that of unconscious transference, a form of source monitoring error (Dysart, Lindsay, Hammond, & Dupuis, 2001). Similar to interference effects, as described above, unconscious transference results in a memory becoming obscured by new or old information. When searching for a target memory, a witness will instead retrieve a similar memory but one which is irrelevant to the current retrieval objective Regardless of the true source of the irrelevant memory, the information it contains is misattributed to the context of the original target memory and subsequently remembered as part of it. This effect is seen more during police identification tasks, and is especially problematic for them, in comparison to interview or interrogation situations. “Victims sometimes pick from a lineup or photospread the face of a person whom they have seen before but who is not the actual criminal,” (Greene, et al., 2007, p. 131). Prior exposure of witnesses to the faces of suspects, through mug books in particular, has been implicated in misidentification of bystanders, acquaintances, and innocent suspects through unconscious transference of their faces onto that of the true offender (Dysart, et al., 2001). Unconscious transference is a problem intrinsic to identifying suspects, but research has illuminated several other unique challenges to accurate identification in the mechanics of facial recognition. The same event and personal factors which influence the encoding of incident memories are also at work in the encoding of identity memories, but additional variables specific to the perception and recognition of faces may interfere with or limit the amount and accuracy of the information encoded in those memories. Facial recognition is theorized to be an automatic and therefore swift process, leading some researchers to hypothesize correct eyewitness identifications are quicker and can be differentiated from misidentifications according to the time is took to pick out a subject from a lineup or photoarray; over 3000 studies support this notion 14 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications (Weber, Brewer, Wells, Semmler, & Keast, 2004). Dunning and Perretta (2002, as cited in Weber, et al., 2004) posited a 10-12 second rule-of-thumb for discriminating between accurate and erroneous identifications. Other research has since determined the 10-12 second rule is not useful across many identification procedures because its inflexibility does not account for the relative difficulty the identification (Weber, et al., 2004). The relative ease of identifying a particular face lessens the time needed to recognize it; characteristics of that face determine the relative ease. Distinctiveness is one of those characteristics. An intuitive example of this is how much easier it is to recall the features of faces on the extreme ends of the attractiveness spectrum as compared with average faces (Wells, & Olsen, 2003, as cited in Burton, 2008). Recall from the discussion of personal factors which affect encoding of memories that witness race did not have any effect on the innate ability to make accurate identifications, but race does have interesting implications when it comes to facial recognition. One of the most studied aspects of facial recognition is the cross-race effect, also known as the own-race effect. Proportionally, more eyewitness misidentifications occur between racial groups rather than within them (MacLin, MacLin, & Malpass, 2001). A review of research on the cross-race effect revealed nearly 80% of participants in pertinent studies were “better able to recognize persons of their own race as compared with those of other races,” (MacLin, et al., 2001, p. 135). Interestingly, this phenomenon is not related to prejudicial attitudes about race. One of the contributions of social psychologists to eyewitness research has been to suggest that the crossrace effect is caused by attentional resources which would be employed in distinguishing the facial features of an own-race/in-group face are distracted by attempts to first categorize an other-race face as a member of the correct out-group (Greene, et al., 2007). There are also some genuine difference in physiognomic variability between races (i.e. White faces exhibit greater 15 A. M. Scotti difference in hair color; Black faces have greater variety in skin tone); attention is instinctively drawn to those features which differentiate members of our own-race, which may not yield the most profitable details for making cross-race identifications (Greene, et al., 2007). Law Enforcement and the Eyewitness Up until this point in the discussion of psychological research on aspects of eyewitness testimony the great majority of factors identified as having some level of influence on eyewitness accuracy have been what are called estimator variables. The ways in which estimator variables affect changes in memory and accuracy are either fixed once the remembered event has passed or responsibility for them is located within the witness (Wrightsman, & Fulero, 2005). They are not controllable by the law enforcement or the criminal justice system. System variables, which can be managed, and their effects prevented, by these organizations are the focus of this next section. Many research studies have examined the functioning and consequences of certain elements in police interview, interrogation, and identification procedures. Much of this research centers on features of the latter, namely lineups, showups, photoarrays, mug books, and composite drawings. Still other research and theory not originally pertaining to law enforcement and eyewitnesses have been used to make comment upon these applications as well. Identification procedures. A lineup consisting of six people, including one suspect, presents witnesses with seven possible decision outcomes including that the true perpetrator of a crime is not present in the lineup. This means the probability of choosing any of the subjects at random is one in seven, approximately 14 percent. In 1974, an NBC affiliate station in New York conducted an on air six person lineup after screening a staged purse-snatching. Over 2000 people called in with their “testimony” but the rate at which they identified the assailant, who was in the lineup, was the same as if viewers selected him by chance; ignoring those viewers 16 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications who chose the “not in lineup” option, 61% of participants misidentified innocent people (Loftus, 1979). Field identifications, or showups, have been strongly spoken against as more inherently unfair than lineups, which is difficult to believe in view of the dismal performance on the televised lineup activity, especially since there is no significant difference between the number of misidentifications made in lineups versus showups (Gonzalez, Ellsworth, & Pembroke, 1993). Lineups remain the ideal law enforcement identification procedure, but their benefits have been outweighed by the convenience of photoarrays (Burton, 2008). One of the subtler differences between a lineup and its paper counterpart is the ability for photographs to be simultaneously or sequentially presented whereas the lineup subjects are seen almost exclusively in simultaneous fashion. A showup is akin to sequential presentation with a series of one. This seemingly minor divergence can have major implications for the accuracy of eyewitness testimony because research evidence points to the fact that these procedures require differing decision styles. There are two basic memory retrieval types—recall and recognition. Law enforcement identification procedures make use of recognition memory; interviews and interrogations present more opportunities to use recall memory. Recognition memory in a showup is “analogous to a true-false question” and in a lineup or photoarray “to a multiple-choice question,” (Gonzalez, et al., 1993, p. 527). In both cases, there is the equivalent of an answer or answers available to cue the retrieval of information. These same answers allow for the retrieved memory to be compared against them before an identification decision is made. Recall memory requires retrieval of appropriate information without the benefit of alternative answers, like a fill-in-the-blank question to complete the analogy. Recall memory also supplies fewer retrieved memories, but those which it does tend to be more accurate and inspire more confidence in their accuracy than answers arrived at by recognition (Robinson, & Johnson, 1996). Lineups and simultaneous 17 A. M. Scotti photoarrays, as opposed to showups and sequential photoarrays, also demonstrate another dichotomy in memory processing, that of relative versus absolute judgment. Showups and sequential presentation require absolute judgments. Lineups and simultaneous presentation, as mentioned above, provide alternatives for witnesses to measure their recollection against. Hence these types of identification procedures are characterized by relative judgment, which is why so many misidentifications are the result of witnesses choosing the lineup participant who most closely resembles the absent perpetrator (Gonzalez, et al., 1993). The issue of simultaneous versus sequential presentation of photoarrays has engendered a good deal of psychological research. Findings have been almost universally favorable to the sequential presentation of photographs, and until very recently, their advantage has been thought to be robust and able generalize across levels of lineup fairness (Carlsen, Gronlund, & Clark, 2008). That advantage lies in decreasing misidentifications rather than in increasing accurate ones; there is no real difference between simultaneous and sequential presentation on that front (Stewart, & McAllister, 2001). Sequential presentation is thought to shift witness reliance from relative to absolute judgment, (Stewart, & McAllister, 2001). “Witnesses also do not know how many faces they will see when they begin the identification process, which prevents them from feeling compelled to choose as they come to the end of the series of faces,” (Carlsen, et al., 2008, p. 118). Interestingly, the sequential presentation advantage in photoarrays does not extend to the use of mug books, and in fact can result in more false identifications when mug pictures are organized one per page (Stewart, & McAllister, 2001). This may be because the sheer volume of faces in a mug book are better culled with a relative judgment process. When detectives have a witness to a crime, but no suspect, preparation of a composite drawing can provide leads on the identity of the perpetrator. Composite drawings are a special 18 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications case identification procedure. There are limitations to what may empirically be discovered about building facial composite drawings since there is an unknown amount of variability in reliability within and between forensic artists, computer programs like FACES, the classic build-a-face kits (i.e. Identi-kit), and the witnesses who use them (Wells, Charman, & Olson, 2005). Building a composite face requires selection of features singly in a “feature-based approach” at odds with humans’ natural “holistic” way of perceiving faces (Wells, et al., 2005, p. 147). Selecting suspect features from batches of like parts also requires the witness to make relative judgments (Burton, 2008). Unlike with verbal descriptions, features cannot be left off of a composite face if the witness if unsure, resulting in potentially erroneous information being incorporated into the composite (Wells, et al., 2005). The general consequences of these factors are that such sketches tend to be poor likenesses and interfere with performance on subsequent lineup or photoarray tasks, though morphing together composites created by multiple witnesses, if available, makes for slight improvement (Bruce, Ness, Hancock, Newman, & Rarity, 2002). Creating a composite may irrevocably alter, obscure, or compromise original memory of a target face; this is especially problematic when composites are circulated by the media, since innocent suspects who resemble them face a higher risk of misidentification due to retroactive interference (Burton, 2008). After completing an identification procedure, law enforcement officers sometimes include an assessment of the witness’ confidence in the accuracy of his or her answer. The force of conviction with which eyewitnesses recount what they are purported to have seen is regularly seen as an indicator of accuracy and strength of eventual court testimony. Common police practice and legal precedent (United States v. Wade, 1967; Neil v. Biggers, 1972) have cast confidence as the second-most important segment of eyewitness testimony after identification of culprits. Psychologists performing research on the interrelation of witness confidence and 19 A. M. Scotti accuracy are dismayed by the importance laid on the former since it is rarely indicative of the latter; no matter how sure a witness seems, certainty does not guarantee accuracy. Confidence is continually represented to juries as being indicative of the probative strength of the identification in determining guilt (Burton, 2008). “A confident eyewitness tends to make [jurors] ignore the witnessing conditions themselves and believe the eyewitness at a rate that exceeds the actual rate of accuracy,” (Wells, & Bradfield, 1998, p. 361). Not all factors affecting confidence are system variables, but one in particular is cause for concern. Once a witness has made an identification, they often look for reassurance of accuracy in the form of praise or feedback from investigators. This feedback, when given, is a type of postevent information which negatively impacts memory integrity and inflates confidence. Alarmingly, this inflationary effect on confidence is greater with witnesses who make false identifications (Bradfield, Wells, & Olson, 2002). Diagnosing Misidentifications. Since confidence is not always a useful tool in establishing eyewitness accuracy, the burden of determining whether information elicited from witnesses is appropriate basis for criminal investigation or prosecution falls to other measures of accuracy. Research has shown witnesses are largely unable to report or control inappropriate influences acting on their memory and identification processes, so self-report is not a reliable method of determining accuracy either (Wells, & Bradfield, 1998; Olsson, & Juslin, 1999). That leaves law enforcement officers themselves as the last bastion against misidentification during interview, interrogation, and identification procedures. Police manuals and popular conception hold law enforcement officers with investigation experience to be accomplished lie detectors. A study of investigators’ ability to detect suspects’ lies in actual interrogation situations by Mann, Vrij, and Bull (2004) lends rare research-based credence to this assertion. Though police officers may be reasonable at detecting intentional deception, how do they fare on subtler falsehoods? 20 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications In the same way as witnesses are not attuned to the external and cognitive factors manipulating their testimony, so too are law enforcement officers unlikely to be able to monitor the source and impact of inaccuracy which remain unconscious to the witnesses themselves. Knowing the capacity of each individual witness to be swayed by those factors, including postevent information effects and judgment type, is one potential way in which police may become aware of problematic memory testimony before it impacts innocent lives adversely. Suggestibility is the term used by researchers to refer to this witness susceptibility. An assessment tool, called the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale (GSS 1), originally developed to be used in conjunction with adjudication of cases in which a confession was retracted or suspected to have been coerced, identifies several factors affecting suggestibility which apply to witnesses as well as to confessors (Gudjonsson, 2003). GSS 1 components imply suggestibility is related in some way to gender, ethnic background, age, intelligence, memory capacity, anxiety, impulsivity, sleep deprivation, social desirability effects, coping strategies, assertiveness, selfesteem, locus of control, suspiciousness, anger, and more (Gudjonsson, 2003). Note the presence of many of the same estimator variables which affect eyewitness memory in the list. Being aware of witness suggestibility is an important step for investigators as they attempt to reduce misidentifications, yet they must be aware not only of the witnesses’ susceptibility to suggestion but also their own capacity to suggest. When proctoring identification procedures, detectives typically know which lineup or photoarray member is the suspect. This foreknowledge often makes itself known through subtle changes in voice inflection and pacing, duration of eye contact, or other cues to the witness as to who is the suspect. All other things being equal—foils are fair, photo backgrounds and orientations are the same, presentation is sequential—even if all recommended steps have been taken to reduce 21 A. M. Scotti misidentification, suggestion by officers can influence a witness’ choice (Burton, 2008). Double-blind procedures are the obvious remedy. The lineup, photoarray, etc. should be administered by someone who is not a part of the particular investigation at hand and therefore does not know which person is the suspect (Bradfield, et al., 2002). Computer administered photoarrays which allow for randomization are also a technologically viable way of conducting double-blind procedures (Stewart, & McAllister, 2001). “Despite research findings showing its benefits, police are resistant to using double-blind testing because they perceive it as a loss of control and as a suggestion that they cannot conduct fair lineups,” (Haw, & Fisher, 2004, p. 1106). Some reluctant departments have devised alternatives to double-blind identification, but none has even the reliability of standard procedures (Lindsay, & Bellinger, 1999). A Closing Note About Eyewitness Research Psychological research, be it about eyewitness testimony or no, has some insights into interview, interrogation, and identification procedures which are drawn from methodology concerns shared by many studies. The way in which police investigators can influence witness responses is identical to the way in which researchers can influence participant responses (Bradfield, et al., 2002). This is the effect of the demand characteristics in a situation; witnesses may experience them as pressures to answer in a particular way or to make their own actions seem as socially acceptable as possible in the presence of the police authority, both of which can result in corrupted testimony. Witnesses and investigators also exhibit confirmation bias, which is a tendency to seek out only that information which agrees with what is already believed, sometimes to the exclusion of contradictory information (Greene, et al., 2007). Both psychologists and law enforcement officers ought to be aware of the likely consequences demand characteristics and confirmation bias have on the usefulness of obtained information. 22 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications The Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence The legal system and its constituent agencies have historically been indifferent or even downright hostile to psychology and the potential contributions of the discipline to the practice of law. It is widely believed daily interaction with people plus a little common sense is enough to make anyone an expert on human behavior. Psychologists, therefore, are sometimes vilified as usurpers of the fact-finding role held by jurors thought to be equally capable of coming to their own conclusions about psychological issues. Ironically, psychology is most useful but most ignored where research has proven the reality of studied phenomena to be more counterintuitive than generally believed. Sometimes the friction between the disciplines of law and psychology is merely an expression of the underlying paradigm differences between them; the law is concerned with making dichotomous decisions while psychology deals with human behavior as it falls along a continuum of possible manifestations. One rare period of cooperation between law enforcement, defense attorneys, prosecuting attorneys, judges, and psychological experts occurred in the late 1990s after a slew of postconviction DNA exonerations. Members of the listed groups recognized the detriment to justice (not to mention the waste of resources) posed by misidentification of innocent persons. Then Attorney General Janet Reno first mandated investigation into the role of DNA testing, but when it became apparent that faulty eyewitness identifications were the leading grounds for erroneous conviction, focus shifted from DNA to eyewitness testimony (Burton, 2008). In 1998, Reno convened the Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence (TWGEYEE). Members of the multidisciplinary TWGEYEE included representatives from the above groups, who spent more than a year identifying and discussing the reasons for the problematic nature of eyewitness evidence. The result of their labors was a set of guidelines—the 1999 report Eyewitness 23 A. M. Scotti Evidence: A Guide for Law Enforcement—for the improvement of procedures used for eliciting eyewitness testimony and identifications (Burton, 2008). Summary of Findings The content of Eyewitness Evidence: A Guide for Law Enforcement (hereafter referred to as the Guide) is organized with each group of guideline suggestions consisting of a statement of the research principle, a basic policy declaration, a procedural list, and a summary addressing the usefulness of each technique. The Guide, while not legally binding, encourages all police agencies to use it as a model for their own legislation and policy, with modifications for the unique needs and limitations of their respective jurisdictions. All of the information in the following paragraphs is drawn from the Guide (USDOJ, 1999) except where noted. Identification procedures. The Guide recognizes and comments on each of the types of eyewitness identification procedures previously discussed: mug books, composites, showups, lineups and photoarrays of both the sequential and simultaneous type. The multiple photograph per page construction scheme for mug books is preferred, with photos of like composition grouped together, followed by organization by type of crime, then by facial similarity in increasingly narrow classification by distinguishing features. A note on composite drawings warns against using the composites as stand alone evidence and reminds investigators of the low likelihood an identification based on a composite sketch will fulfill probable cause requirements. Mug book and composite procedures are discussed first despite their lower frequency of use, compared to lineups and photoarrays, since the Guide is laid out to reflect the probable order in which the procdeures discussed would be employed during an investigation. Perhaps in acquiescence to the opinion of researchers, but not police officers, that the showup is inherently more biased and suggestive than a lineup procedure, the Guide suggests 24 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications that if multiple witnesses are present, only one perform the field identification so the suspect can be put into custody while the other sequestered witnesses wait until a proper lineup or photoarray can be arranged. Instructions are included for performing simultaneous and sequential lineups as well as photoarrays much as they have already been discussed. A reminder is given to construct the lineups/photoarrays so the fillers are close approximations of the suspect, not of the theoretical perpetrator who may not be present or whom the witness may have incorrectly described. The Guide even goes so far as to suggest artificially faking or concealing features like tattoos and scars in the pursuit of lineup fairness. All of the identification procedures in the Guide require “statements of certainty” (confidence assessments) from witnesses prior to any reports regarding the selected individual (p. 37). On the one hand, this instruction is likely to limit the effect of administrator dissemination of postevent information, but on the other hand, since this same instruction is repeated for all identification procedures, the notion that witness confidence and accuracy are strongly and directly correlated may be unjustly perpetuated. Useful or not, the results of the confidence assessment and the identification made by the witness (or not made) must be zealously documented. The Guide admonishes, “preparing a complete and accurate record of the outcome of the identification procedure improves the strength and credibility of the identification or non-identification” in court and provides means for impeaching perjuring witnesses (p. 38). Interview and interrogation procedures. The Guide is heavily weighted in favor of presenting protocols for identification rather than interview or interrogation. In its limited discussion of these law enforcement functions, it assumes witnesses will be cooperative, so specific suggestions for antagonistic interrogation situations are nonexistent. To its credit, however, the Guide promotes the principles of good interviewing at the earliest levels of police 25 A. M. Scotti contact by including sections on how 911 dispatchers and first-responding officers should ask questions in the midst of their hectic duties. The first officer, after ensuring the safety of all involved persons and apprehending the culprit if possible, should begin locating witnesses and instituting simple measures (like separation of multiple witnesses) in order to preserve the integrity of the eyewitnesses’ memories. Warnings at this early stage to avoid discussions, media reports, and other sourced of postevent information are given as well. An introductory text on criminal investigation, including interviews and interrogations, by Swanson et al. (1996), conceives of the interview as a progression through three stages: establishing a rapport with the witness, acquiring information, and expressing appreciation. At all stages of eyewitness interviews, the Guide emphasizes rapport building and professional courtesy, which includes thanking witnesses for their participation. The suggestions for the questioning portion of an interview are uncomplicated and generally representative of the research identified concerns prompted by interview situations—witness guessing or hedging; interviewer leading. A step which suggests asking open-ended questions then “augmenting with close-ended, specific questions” mirrors the research supported tactic of obtaining a free recall narrative before prompting clarification and further disclosures through the use of questions (p. 23). Just as with the identification procedures, the Guide is adamant about good documentation. One final point of note is the inclusion of a step designed to let the witness recreate mentally the sensory cues and emotional arousal level of the original event to aid in recollection. The Training Guide The work begun by TWGEYEE under Janet Reno continued under Attorney General John Ashcroft with the 2003 publication of Eyewitness Evidence: A Trainer’s Manual for Law Enforcement (hereafter referred to as the Manual). The following material is drawn from the 26 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications Manual unless otherwise noted (USDOJ, 2003). Designed as a companion to the Guide (USDOJ, 1999), the Manual contains almost entirely the same information as its predecessor. The organization of the latter is a little different, however, with the presentation of topics from the former according to the timeline of their use in an investigation changed to create a division between two lesson plans on Interviewing and Identification. These sections may be taught together as part of a 2-day lecture or independently. All of the materials in the Manual, including curricula, exercises, and an accompanying CD-ROM containing audio/visual materials, were designed to be used in conjunction with the Guide. TWGEYEE instigated testing of these materials in an actual classroom environment with the Baltimore County Police Department (Maryland) to ensure their viability. The resulting Manual is sort of a teacher’s edition of the Guide, the students’ required text, and includes special notes on key issues for discussion as well as cues for a slide presentation included with the A/V supplement. As was true of the Guide, the Manual encourages law enforcement agencies to adapt its contents to suit their particular needs or to supplement existing programs on the collection and preservation of eyewitness testimony. Law Enforcement Practice and Procedure Issues Education, Training, and Policy The same aforementioned thrust to professionalize the image of law enforcement, which resulted in the aversion to terms like interrogation, led to adoption of new interrogation methods couched in the symbolic, authoritative, and scientific language of psychology to replace disgraced physically coercive methods (Leo, 2008). Law enforcement agencies are aware of many of the research findings previously discussed and incorporate the concluding suggestions of the most representative and recognizable studies into their education and practice, but the general relationship with psychology is a thorny one. The latent paradigmatic antagonism felt by 27 A. M. Scotti both disciplines is not helped by a tendency for law enforcement to lend the illusion of sanction as the fruit of modern psychological inquiry to its brand of anecdotal “research,” which has poor methodological rigor and is characterized more by a compilation of experiential evidence for certain police-invented techniques than by systematic study of them. In one discussion of talents useful for interrogators (who usually conduct both interviews and interrogations), psychology was listed along with “salesmanship and dramatics;” this use of the term psychology reflects its most manipulative and utilitarian connotations (Swanson, Chamelin, & Territo, 1996, p. 157). The assumption of the “cultural authority of science” was seen as a way of making police work into “a profession that, like medicine and the law, the public would come to trust” and as a way of “furthering the goal of professionalizing interrogation so as to insulate it from outside review or control,” (Leo, 2008, pp. 107, 109). The latter statement reflects a goal pursued by Inbau who, with Reid, coauthored one of the most prestigious procedural and training manuals for police, subsequent editions of which are still in circulation (Leo, 2008). Manuals continue to be a means for legions of police officers to learn the rudiments of interviewing, interrogation, and identification; regrettably the pseudo-psychological orientation of early manuals persists in modern training and certification and is perpetuated through manuals (Leo, 2008). Officers typically receive only minimal training in interview techniques, enough to be competent at getting information when on patrol calls in the field, but more in-depth training is rare unless an officer is assigned to an investigative position, such as a detective bureau. (D. Pearson, personal communication, November 10, 2008). Even then, the majority of training is provided on-the-job by mentors who themselves have learned through experience (Burton, 2008). The California Commission on Peace Officer Standards & Training (POST) is a law enforcement oversight group which sets minimum recruitment and training standards for 28 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications agencies which participate in its incentive-based programs (POST, 2007). POST is one of the largest and most imitated organizations of its kind, providing training in leadership and policing fundamentals as well as certification for proficiency (defined as part of POST standards) in these areas (POST, 2007). POST is also one of the few formal providers of basic training in interview and interrogation techniques (POST, 2008). Unfortunately, either the fear of hardened criminals learning to outsmart their methods or Inbau’s long-standing fear of public intrusion into police matters has lowered a veil of almost absurd secrecy over the exact content of interrogation techniques in general and POST training of the subject in specific. Even a conversation with a retired LAPD lieutenant did not reveal any more information than that POST classes provide merely an overview of issues as well as brief exposure to some of the techniques employed (D. Pearson, personal communication, November 10, 2008). POST Interview & Interrogation courses appear to be taught in a multi-day workshop format; outside companies are engaged to teach the sessions and they appear to have content leeway provided the minimum requirements are met for the appropriate police function to which the training is geared (POST, 2008). Actual Police Practice Throughout most of the discussion of police procedures for eliciting accurate eyewitness testimony within this paper, the constituent activities of interviews, interrogations, and identifications have been described in terms of the relative ideals suggested by psychological research and TWGEYEE guidelines. Lack of transparency in actual police procedures makes it unclear just how closely or not practice adheres to policy. Gonzalez, Ellsworth, and Pembroke aptly comment on the speculative absurdity of “hypotheses about the psychology of police officers and witnesses” since after “years of intensive research on eyewitness identification, [psychologists] know next to nothing about actual practice,” (1993, p. 536). From what 29 A. M. Scotti information is available about genuine police practice (D. Pearson, personal communication, November 10, 2008; Burton 2008; Swanson et al., 1996), the evidence seems to be clear that identification techniques, particularly lineups, photoarrays, and showups, are conducted almost exactly as described in the Guide (USDOJ, 1999) except for variations in verbal instructions across examined jurisdictions. There is less certainty surrounding the position of interview and interrogation techniques with respect to research and TWGEYEE suggested procedures. One unique solution to the paucity of detail surrounding actual interviews and interrogations was to review episodes of The First 48 television series (Kim, 2004-2008), a reality program in which cameras follow real detectives from cities around the US as they investigate during the first 48 hours after a homicide. There is at least one interrogation in every episode , plus interviews and photoarrays occur frequently, though it was not thought prudent to base too much of the following discussion of actual police practice on a TV show, no matter how true-to-life. Interviews. Burton (2008), himself a Sheriff’s captain and deputy division commander, notes a disparity in the amount of training certain ranks and functions receive in interview and interrogation techniques. First-responders and patrol officers are almost exclusively trained in interviewing—dealing with cooperative witnesses. Detectives on the other hand are poor in additional training with interviews, but rich in training and experience with interrogation. The one exception to this generality is the Cognitive Interview (CI) which, although developed by Fisher and Geiselman for use with cooperative witnesses, is employed more often by detectives or other follow-up investigators than by patrol officers (Fisher, Brennan, McCauley, (2002). The CI is a method for enhancing eyewitness memory, but it is not for use during identification tasks. Instead, the CI elicits descriptive information about events or objects at a greater rate than a standard police interview with no expense of accuracy (Fisher, et al., 2002). Because the CI is a 30 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications structured interview technique, it can be assumed the practice of law enforcement investigators using the CI conforms closely to the recommended procedure. There are five phases to a CI: introduction, narration, probing, review, and close (Fisher, et al., 2002). During the introduction phase, CI administrators establish rapport with the witness and give instructions for the procedure. During narration, as the witness recounts an open-ended version of the target event the investigator plans a strategy for getting the witness to remember more detail with the help of cognitive tricks (like relaying the sequence of events backwards) administered during the probing phase (Fisher, et al., 2002). The review stage is a time to check the accuracy of recorded information; the close phase recalls the appreciation stage of a standard interview (Swanson, et al., 1996; USDOJ, 1999). This form to the CI confers the benefit of decreased opportunity for leading questions and postevent disclosures on the part of the administrator (Fisher, et al., 2002). Interrogations. Because privacy, which law enforcement has enlarged into secrecy, is thought to be a necessary feature of interrogation, there is little that can be known with certainty about actual police interrogation practice. Manuals, especially those written by law enforcement for law enforcement, are therefore the best indicators of what that actual practice might look like. Assuming the study of training manuals does confer insight into interrogation theory, procedures, and policy, then practice of interrogation must be diverse indeed between jurisdictions and between proponents of the sundry techniques extolled in various manuals. Despite the prevalence of methods, the majority of attributes of the interrogation process are similar across all of them. The meagerly furnished dismally painted room with table and two chairs on adjoining sides, which is the stereotypical setting for interrogations in media portrayals, is in fact the norm. One manual offers this explanation for why; the sparseness of interrogation rooms is meant to limit the amount of stimuli experienced by the suspect to only those which come from 31 A. M. Scotti investigators (Walters, 2003). The questions they ask and the small comfort items they do or do not provide are then thought to have heightened emotional impact for the suspect. Reasoning like this illustrates how the three main themes of interrogation—preparation, control, and direction—are translated into practical aspects of the procedure. Another stereotypical conception of interrogation which does not stray too far from the mark is the idea of interrogators watching the body language of suspects to discover lies and particularly sensitive topics which can be effectively exploited in reaching a confession or other desired outcome. The Kinesic Interview and Interrogation Technique is built around the idea that verbal and non-verbal reactions during the interrogation process provide cues to deception as well as the ideal approach to use with a particular suspect (Walters, 2003). Kinesic, from the same root as kinesis meaning motion, refers in this case to communicative motion (Walters, 2003). “Neurolingustic eye movement analysis” whereby the vertical position of the suspect’s eyes is determinant of a visual, auditory, or touch memory and left-right position is indicative of veracity or fabrication, is a borderline scientific/pseudo-scientific component of the Kinesic and other interrogation techniques (Walters, 2003; Zulawski, & Wicklander, 1992). Interestingly, the naturalistic study of officer lie detection ability conducted by Mann, Vrij, and Bull, described above, found attention to physical cues to deception, “such as gaze aversion and fidgeting,” was correlated with poor lie detection performance, possibly because over-much attention was paid to them at the expense of a holistic appraisal of verbal and physical deception cues (2004, p. 137). Another method, the Reid Technique, conceptualizes interrogation as consisting of two main processes: breaking down denials and increasing suspect desire to confess (Gudjonsson, 2003). Because of the manual-based influence of Inbau and Reid, for whom the technique is named, these two objectives form the basis of many interrogation techniques; the Reid 32 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications Technique itself is one of the most commonly used paradigms of interrogation among police. Leo (as cited in Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 27) likens the subtle manipulation necessary to guide a suspect from denial to admission of guilt to “the process, sequence, and structure of a confidence game.” Interrogators, like scam artists and chess players, must be able to read their opponents and anticipate their responses. Psychology, particularly the psychology of personality is sometimes a means of “playing several moves ahead” of a suspect as well as of accomplishing the goals of breaking denials and encouraging confession (D. Pearson, personal communication, November, 10, 2008). The Kinesic Interview and Interrogation Technique, for example, borrows from the work of Carl Jung and Hans Eysenck for its suspect classification system, which includes an introversion/extroversion determination and a hypothesis about whether the suspect is emotion-, logic-, ego-, or sense-dominant (Walters, 2003). Understanding the personality of the interrogated suspect is supposed to be a key for knowing which interrogation approaches, like Good Cop/Bad Cop, are most likely to move the suspect along that road from denial to admission in the smoothest, quickest way possible. Sometimes providing incentives is the best way to motivate a suspect to confess or make an incriminating disclosure. These can be negative or positive. Negative incentives include: making accusations of definite and more serious offenses, refusing to accept suspect denials, using an evidence ploy when the “evidence” is nonexistent or less incriminating than represented, pressure to answer, repetition of questions, and general escalation of intensity (Leo, 2008). Positive incentives include: inducements, which create the hope of relief, favor, preference, or release if a suspect confesses; small favors or comforts; offers of help within the limits of the law; and suggestion of minimizing scenarios, also called rationalizations (Leo, 2008). When interrogators suggest to suspects, the crime they committed was an accident, an 33 A. M. Scotti unintended consequence, or the fault of someone else (perhaps even the victim), the natural instinct to minimize guilt and appear socially acceptable induces the suspect to agree to the conditions described in the rationalization are true (Zulawski, & Wicklander, 1992). The catch is a suspect must admit to some involvement in the crime under investigation in order to reap the benefits of the ego escape offered by an interrogator. Suggesting rationalizations like these are the fourth most successful interrogation tactic in securing confessions after appeals to conscience, trapping suspects in lies, and flattery; three of four top approaches (including rationalization) can be seen as psychologically manipulative (Gudjonsson, 2003). Conclusions Admittedly, law enforcement officers must balance deference to protecting eyewitness memory integrity and accuracy with their primary function of detecting and detaining the guilty. The long-term goal of limiting the number of innocent people accused on the basis of eyewitness testimony is the same for police agencies and psychological researchers. That said, in view of the organizational focus of the former on catching criminals, the empirically suggested means for accomplishing that long-term goal may be displaced by the realities of short-term crime solving goals. Law enforcement brass may hand down policies which follow the recommendations of psychologists and the TWGEYEE; detailed procedures for collecting and protecting accurate eyewitness testimony may be instituted; but when police officers encounter non-performing witnesses and suspects who beg to be put away on the length of their rap sheets, such procedures may be ignored if a superior tactic like using suggestive interrogation techniques will pave the way for a sought-after identification or confession. The real possibility exists for authentic law enforcement practice to not match published agency procedures or for policy and procedure both to vary significantly across jurisdictions given the limited specificity of training requirements set 34 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications by voluntary-participation organizations like POST. Regrettably, the self-imposed lack of transparency in police interview, interrogation, and identification procedures limits the ability of researchers and reformers to diagnose and address their attendant issues. That same lack of transparency and absence of uniformity in practice hampersthe formation of a final judgment on the success or failure of law enforcement procedures at preserving the integrity of the eyewitness memories they are meant to draw out. Accurate and resilient memories are not solely the product or responsibility of legal authorities, but as far as interviews and identification procedures can conform to suggested guidelines for ensuring minimal consequences from interaction with police investigators, they appear to do so. Assuming these functions are performed in accordance with stated policy, current means of administering interviews, lineups, photoarrays, and showups do provide adequate protection from memory alteration and ultimately from false attributions of guilt. The case of mug books and composite drawings is a little more unclear, but only because there is no clear empirically sound method for administering these as of yet. The state of interrogation procedures, on the other hand, is particularly concerning. There are no TWGEYEE guidelines for their content, nor is there much available scientific research on the efficacy or harm produced by the common use of psychological manipulation. This dearth of comments on and recommendations for the use of interrogations may be the result of several factors. The TWGEYEE may not view interrogation as an instance of eyewitness evidence collection, though interrogators are looking for the suspect to let slip details of a crime or crime scene known only to the perpetrator, which would have been witnessed as he or she committed the crime. A reluctance to interfere in (and thereby potentially decrease the closure rate of) crime solving could also have motivated the relative silence of TWGEYEYE. The 35 A. M. Scotti aforementioned lack of transparency in interrogation practice could also mean researchers are not privy to the information required to design a suitable test of these procedures. Interrogation is such a “calculus of personalities” (the suspect’s, the interrogator’s) that there is unlikely to be uniformity of practice from suspect to suspect. The psychologically manipulative nature of some of the components of interrogations may cause unwanted changes in eyewitness memory, in fact it is likely they do, but this cannot be known for sure without proper study. The most successful interrogation tactics would probably not be psychologically indicated for memory protection, yet the best procedures for ensuring the accuracy of identifications and recollections of events would not likely be very useful in convincing a repeat offender to confess or uncooperative witnesses to relent either. A balance must be achieved. Psychologists and police officers have differing viewpoints on eyewitness procedures; while law enforcement is concerned mostly about the utility of such techniques, psychologists see them through the lens of past research on personality, memory, and compliance (Gudjonsson, 2003). Echoes of Zimbardo’s Stanford Prison and Milgram’s obedience experiments as well as an empirical aversion to the false positive (i.e. mistaken identification, wrongful conviction) more than the false negative (i.e. true criminal goes undetected), make psychologists wary of Machiavellian motives for using certain questioning tactics and willing to remedy them. Research into eyewitness testimony has progressed far and has been to a certain extent heeded, take the TWGEYEE for example. The problem is that research does not seem to be enough to affect the magnitude of change necessary. Psychologists should continue to perform more and better studies of eyewitness procedures, especially in naturalistic settings, but they should also consider becoming advocated for change through amicus curiae briefs, development of training programs, and evaluation of current policies. 36 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications Introducing greater transparency to police practices will help to accomplish future research and policy goals. The creation of a national or state-level standard for all eyewitness procedures (interview, interrogation, and identification), one which is enforceable unlike the Guide and POST standards, will also ensure uniformity of practice and ease of evaluation. The British PEACE program (acronym reflects the five parts of their interview: Plan, Engage, Account, Close, and Evaluate) is a standardized training program in interview and interrogation skills, with enumerated procedures and government oversight (Bull, & Milne, 2004). Training is tiered, with five depths of instruction tailored to the ways officers are likely to use interviews (Bull, & Milne, 2004). PEACE is a working model of how institutional change in eyewitness procedures may be accomplished. It also may serve as a source of research data on the effect such changes have on closure and conviction rates of interest to law enforcement administrators. Eyewitness policies and procedures are far ahead of where they were just a few decades ago, but there is still room and need for improvement. The best modifications to current law enforcement practices will be those which recognize and control the hazards to accuracy and identified by social science at the same time as they attend to the needs of the police agencies which use them. With a cooperative effort to devise and implement such strategies, the number of lives destroyed by wrongful convictions on the basis of faulty eyewitness testimony may mercifully decline. 37 A. M. Scotti References Bradfield, A. L., Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (February 2002). The damaging effect of confirming feedback on the relation between eyewitness certainty and identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(1), 112-120. Bruce, V., Ness, H., Hancock, P. J. B., Newman, C., & Rarity, J. (October 2002). Four heads are better than one: Combining face composites yields improvements in face likeness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(5), 894-902. Bull, R., & Milne, B. (2004). Attempts to improve the police interviewing of suspects. In G. D. Lassiter (Ed.), Interrogations, confessions, and entrapment. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Burton, J. R. (February 2008). Eyewitness testimony: Improvements to a basic law enforcement process that will help prevent wrongful convictions. Retrieved October 1, 2008, from http://www.fdle.state.fl.us/FCJEI/Authors.htm. California Commission on Peace Officer Standards & Training. (2008). Course catalog: Interview & interrogation. Retrieved October 1, 2008, from http://www.post.ca.gov/Publications/Course_Catalog/3436.asp. California Commission on Peace Officer Standards & Training. (2007). What is POST?. Retrieved October 1, 2008, from http://www.post.ca.gov/About_Us/What_is_POST.asp. Carlson, C. A., Gronlund, S. D., & Clark, S. E. (June 2008). Lineup composition, suspect position, and the sequential lineup advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(2), 118-128. Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1995). Mistaken identification: The eyewitness, psychology, and the law. New York: Cambridge University Press. 38 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications Dysart, J. E., Lindsay, R. C. L., Hammond, R., & Dupuis, P. (December 2001). Mug shot exposure prior to lineup identification: Interference, transference, and commitment effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(6), 1280-1284. Fisher, R. P., Brennan, K. H., & McCauley, M. R. (2002). The cognitive interview method to enhance eyewitness recall. In M. L. Eisen, J. A. Quass, & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview (pp. 3-28). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gonzalez, R., Ellsworth, P. C., & Pembroke, M. (1993). Response biases in lineups and showups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 525-537. Greene, E., Heilbrun, K., Fortune W. H., &Nietzel, M. T. (2007). Wrightsman’s psychology and the legal system (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations and confessions—A handbook. Chichester, West Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons. Harley, E. M., Carlsen, K. A., & Loftus, G.R. (2004). The “saw-it-all-along” effect: Demonstrations of visual hindsight bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(5), 960-968. Haw, R. M., & Fisher, R. P. (December 2004). Effects of administrator-witness contact on eyewitness identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(6), 1106-1112. Innocence Project. (n.d.). News and information: Eyewitness identification reform. Retrieved October 1, 2008 from http://www.innocenceproject.org/Content/165.php. Kim, J. X. (Producer). (2004-2008). The First 48 [Television series]. New York: AETN. Leo, R. A. (2008). Police interrogation and American justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 39 A. M. Scotti Lindsay, R. C. L., & Bellinger, K. (1999). Alternatives to the sequential lineup: The importance of controlling the pictures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(3), 315-321. Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. MacLin, O. H., MacLin, M. K., & Malpass, R. S. (March 2001). Race, arousal, attention, exposure and delay: An examination of factors moderating face recognition. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 134-152. Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, A. (2004). Detecting true lies: Police officers’ ability to detect suspects’ lies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(1), 137-149. Olsson, N., & Juslin, P. (February 1999). Can self-reported encoding strategy and recognition skill be diagnostic of performance in eyewitness identifications? Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(1), 42-49. Robinson, M. D., & Johnson, J. T. (October 1996). Recall memory, recognition memory, and the eyewitness confidence-accuracy correlation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(5), 587-594. Roediger, H. L. , III, & Gallo, D. A. (2002). Processes affecting accuracy and distortion in memory: An overview. In M. L. Eisen, J. A. Quass, & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview (pp. 3-28). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Stewart, H. A., & McAllister, H. A. (2001). One-at-a-time versus grouped presentation of mug book pictures: Some surprising results. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(6), 13001305. Swanson, C. R., Chamelin, N. C., & Territo, L. (1996). Criminal investigation (6th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 40 Interviews, Interrogations, and Identifications U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of Justice. (October 1999). Eyewitness evidence: A guide for law enforcement (NCJ 178240). Retrieved October 1, 2008, from http://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/178240.pdf. U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of Justice. (September 2003). Eyewitness evidence: A trainer’s manual for law enforcement (NCJ 188678). Retrieved October 1, 2008, from http://www.ncjrs.gov/nij/eyewitness/188678.pdf. Walters, S. B. (2003). Principles of kinesic interview and interrogation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Weber, N., Brewer, N., Wells, G. L., Semmler, C., & Keast, A. (September 2004). Eyewitness identification accuracy and response latency: The unruly 10-12-second rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10(3), 139-147. Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (June 1998). "Good, you identified the suspect": Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(3), 360-376. Wells, G. L., Charman, S. D., & Olson, E. A. (2005). Building face composites can harm lineup identification performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(3), 147-156. Wrightsman, L. S. & Fulero, S. M. (2005). Forensic psychology. (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Zulawski, D. E., & Wicklander, D. E. (1992). Practical aspects of interview and interrogation. New York: Elsevier Science Publishing. 41