Download More Than Meets the Eye - Association for Consumer Research

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
1
More Than Meets the Eye:
The Influence of Implicit Self-Esteem on Materialism
JI KYUNG PARK
DEBORAH ROEDDER JOHN*
2
*Ji Kyung Park (Tel: 612-616-9788; Fax: 612-624-8804; E-mail; [email protected]) is PhD
Candidate and Deborah Roedder John (Tel: 612-624-9563; Fax: 612-626-8328; E-mail:
[email protected]) is Curtis L. Carlson Chair and professor of marketing, Carlson School of
Management, University of Minnesota, 321 19th Avenue South, Minneapolis, MN 55455. Both
authors contributed equally to this manuscript. Correspondence: Deborah Roedder John.
3
ABSTRACT
Materialism researchers have consistently found that individuals with high self-esteem are less
materialistic than those with low self-esteem. Self-esteem has been measured with self-report
scales, which tap conscious evaluations of self-worth, known as explicit self-esteem. In this
article, we propose that implicit self-esteem, defined as unconscious evaluations of self-worth, is
also an important determinant of materialism. We explore the joint effect of implicit and explicit
self-esteem on materialistic tendencies. Contrary to prior research, we find that individuals with
high explicit self-esteem can be quite materialistic when these feelings of self-worth are
accompanied by low implicit self-esteem.
4
EXTENDED ABSTRACT
One of the most consistent findings reported in the materialism literature is the link between selfesteem and materialism, with lower feelings of self-worth related to higher levels of materialism
(Chaplin and John 2007; Richins and Dawson 1992). Material goods are viewed as a way for
individuals with low self-esteem to cope with or compensate for doubts about their self-worth.
However, recent self-esteem research suggests that the relationship between self-esteem and
materialism may be more complicated than current findings suggest. Two forms of self-esteem
have been identified: explicit versus implicit self-esteem (Greenwald and Banaji 1995). Explicit
self-esteem is defined as conscious evaluations of the self, whereas implicit self-esteem is defined
as unconscious evaluations of the self. Consistent with Wilson, Linsey and Schooler (2000)’s
dual attitude model, people can have two different attitudes toward the self, such as implicit
versus explicit self-esteem. In fact, explicit self-esteem is only weakly correlated with implicit
self-esteem, which is considered a distinct dimension of self-esteem (Bosson, Brown, and
Zeigler-Hill 2003).
In this article, we propose that explicit self-esteem alone is not sufficient to capture the
relationship between self-esteem and materialism, and that implicit self-esteem is an important
determinant in the adoption of materialism. We report three studies that demonstrate the
usefulness of this construct for understanding materialism. Each of the studies and findings are
described below.
Study 1: The Joint Effect of Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem on Materialism
Prior research has found that individuals with high (explicit) self-esteem exhibit lower levels of
materialism. However, individuals with high (explicit) self-esteem are not a homogeneous group.
In fact, this group includes two distinct subgroups of individuals: those with congruent high selfesteem (high explicit/high implicit self-esteem) and those with discrepant high self-esteem (high
explicit/low implicit self-esteem).
Not surprisingly, these two groups exhibit different behavioral patterns. Individuals with
discrepant high self-esteem have underlying negative self-feelings associated with low implicit
self-esteem. To conceal such nagging self-doubts, these individuals use various forms of selfenhancing strategies. Relative to individuals with congruent high self-esteem, they tend to
exhibit overt grandiosity, higher levels of narcissism, and indirect forms of self-enhancement,
such as out-group derogation and in-group biases (Bosson et al. 2003; Jordan et al. 2003; Kernis
et al. 2005). In sum, individuals with discrepant high self-esteem possess some of the same
characteristics usually attributed to individuals with low (explicit) self-esteem, with both groups
engaging in self-enhancing strategies to compensate for negative self-feelings.
Thus, we predict that individuals with discrepant high self-esteem will exhibit higher levels of
materialism than individuals with congruent high self-esteem. We measured intact levels of
implicit and explicit self-esteem, comparing how combinations of these types of self-esteem
relate to materialism. Explicit self-esteem was measured using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
(Rosenberg 1965); implicit self-esteem was measured using the self-esteem Implicit Association
5
Test (IAT: Greenwald and Farnham 2000). As a measure of materialism, we used a qualitative
task in which levels of materialism were revealed by asking participants to construct a collage to
answer the question: “What makes me happy?” (see Chaplin and John 2007).
The results support the idea that materialism depends not only on the levels of explicit selfesteem, but also on implicit self-esteem. Contrary to prior materialism research, individuals with
high (explicit) self-esteem are not always immune to the appeals of materialism. Indeed,
individuals with discrepant high self-esteem (high explicit/low implicit self-esteem) are more
materialistic than individuals with congruent high self-esteem (high explicit/high implicit selfesteem).
Study 2: The Causal Impact of Implicit Self-Esteem on Materialism
Next, we proceed with our examination of implicit self-esteem by examining the causal
relationship between implicit self-esteem and materialism. In Study 1, intact levels of implicit
and explicit self-esteem were measured rather than manipulated, leaving open the possibility that
materialism affects implicit self-esteem rather than the reverse. In Study 2, while measuring the
intact levels of explicit self-esteem, we primed high implicit self-esteem through subliminal
evaluative conditioning (Dijksterhuis 2004).
The results replicated those of Study 1. High implicit self-esteem priming reduced materialism;
indeed, among participants with high explicit self-esteem, those in the control group who did not
receive a prime were more materialistic than those primed with high implicit self-esteem.
Study 3: Why Does Discrepant High Self-Esteem Cause Materialism?
Individuals with discrepant high self-esteem, who tend to have relatively strong materialistic
tendencies, present the most interesting case when compared to prior materialism research. In
Study 3, we explore why discrepant high self-esteem can lead to materialism in more detail.
They tend to use self-protective and self-enhancing strategies, not only to compensate for
underlying negative self-feelings, but also to deal with threats to the self (McGregor et al. 2005).
Because these individuals are concerned about protecting their exaggerated self-images, they
overly react to self-threats.
We thus predict that individuals with discrepant high self-esteem will respond to a situation
where a self-threat is present by focusing on material possessions as a self-enhancing behavior.
Further, we expect their response to self-threats to be unique, in that not all individuals with low
implicit self-esteem will react this way. We manipulated the level of self-threat in performing a
memory task. As in Study 1, we measured the intact levels of explicit and implicit self-esteem,
and used Richins and Dawson (1992)’s materialism scale as a measure of materialism.
We found that individuals with discrepant high self-esteem exhibit defensive responses to threats
to their self-image. When a self-threat was present, individuals with discrepant high self-esteem
exhibited heightened materialistic tendencies, which was not the case for individuals with low
explicit and low implicit self-esteem.
6
Summary
Prior research has found that individuals with high self-esteem are less materialistic than those
with low self-esteem. We add to these findings by distinguishing two types of self-esteem-explicit and implicit self-esteem--and demonstrating that they have a joint influence on
materialism. Specifically, we find that individuals with high explicit self-esteem vary in their
tendency to self-enhance through material things as a function of their levels of implicit selfesteem. Thus, contrary to prior research, individuals with high explicit self-esteem can be
susceptible to materialism.
REFERENCES
Bosson, Jennifer K., Ryan, P. Brown, Virgil Zeigler-Hill, and William B. Swann, Jr. (2003),
“Self-Enhancement Tendencies among People with High Explicit Self-Esteem: The
Moderating Role of Implicit Self-Esteem,” Self and Identity, 2, 169-187.
Chaplin, Lan Nguyen and Deborah Roedder John (2007), “Growing Up in a Material World:
Age Differences in Materialism in Child and Adolescents,” Journal of Consumer
Research, 34 (4), 480-93.
Dijksterhuis, Ap (2004), “I Like Myself but I Don’t Know Why: Enhancing Implicit Self-Esteem
by Subliminal Evaluative Conditioning,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
86 (2), 345-355.
Greenwald, Anthony G. and Mahzarin R. Banaji (1995), “Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes,
Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes,” Psychological Review, 102 (1), 4-27.
______________ and Shelly D. Farham (2000), “Using the Implicit Association Test to Measure
Self-Esteem and Self-Concept,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6),
1022-1038.
Jordan, Christian H., Steven J. Spender, Mark P. Zanna, Etsuko Hoshino-Browne, and Joshua
Correll (2003), “Secure and Defensive High Self-Esteem,” Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85 (5), 969-978.
Kernis, Michael H., Teresa A. Abend, Brian M. Goldman, Ilan Shrira, Andrew N. Paradise, and
Christian Hampton (2005), “Self-Serving Responses Arising from Discrepancies between
Explicit and Implicit Self-Esteem,” Self and Identity, 4, 311-330.
McGregor, Ian, Paul R. Nail, Denise C. Marigold, and So-Jin Kang (2005), “Defensive Pride
and Consensus: Strength in Imaginary Numbers,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89 (6), 978-96.
Richins, Marsha L. and Scott Dawson (1992), “A Consumer Values Orientation for Materialism
and Its Measurement: Scale Development and Validation,” Journal of Consumer
Research, 19 (December), 303-16.
Wilson, Timothy D., Samuel Lindsey, and Tonya Y. Schooler (2000), “A Model of Dual
Attitudes,” Psychological Review, 107 (1), 101-126.