* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Patch area, substrate depth, and richness affect giving
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
Oikos 118: 17211731, 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2008.17473.x, # 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation # 2009 Oikos Subject Editor: Thomas Valone. Accepted 7 May 2008 Patch area, substrate depth, and richness affect giving-up densities: a test with mourning doves and cottontail rabbits Mohammad A. Abu Baker and Joel S. Brown M. A. Abu Baker ([email protected]) and J. S. Brown, Dept of Biological Sciences, Univ. of Illinois at Chicago, 845 W. Taylor St. (M/C 066), Chicago, IL 60607, USA. We compared the foraging behavior of mourning doves Zenaida macroura and cottontail rabbits Sylvilagus floridanus in patches that varied in initial food abundance, surface area and substrate depth. We measured giving-up densities (GUD), food harvest and proportion of food harvested to investigate their ability to respond to characteristics of resource patches. GUDs have been analyzed in three ways: grams of per patch, grams per unit surface area (GUDAREA), and grams per unit volume of sand (GUDVOL). Mourning doves and cottontails exhibited similar responses to resource density and sand depth. Both foragers detected and responded to variation in initial food abundance. The proportion of food harvested from a patch increased from 40.7, 43.8 to 48.3% (for the doves) and 34.9, 35.8 to 38.4% (for the rabbits) in patches of low, medium and high initial food abundance, respectively. Deeper substrates reduced the foragers’ encounter probability with food, decreased patch quality and resulted in higher GUDs (60% higher in the deepest relative to shallowest substrate) and lower harvests. A significant interaction between initial food abundance and substrate depth showed that both species were willing to dig deeper in patches with higher resource density. Patch size (surface area) had no effect on food harvest or the proportion of food harvested. Consequently, GUDAREA and GUDVOL increased in patches with a smaller surface area. Smaller patches appeared to hamper the dove’s and cottontail’s movement across the surface. Our results revealed that mourning doves and cottontails forage under imperfect information. Both species were able to respond to patch properties by biasing their feeding efforts toward rich and easy opportunities, however, mourning doves were more efficient at food harvesting. The interaction of patch area, volume and food abundance directly influenced food harvest. Such resource characters occur under natural situations where food varies in abundance, area of distribution, and accessibility. Food harvest represents an interplay between resource characteristics and the animals’ ability to respond to these characteristics. Diet choice, habitat selection and patch use are the three main contexts of foraging theory aimed at how feeding animals exploit opportunities and avoid hazards (Stephens et al. 2007). Patch use theory considers how much effort a forager should devote to depleting the resources of a localized area before moving on in search of a new ‘patch’ (Charnov 1976, Stephens and Krebs 1986). Patch use applies to circumstances where foragers detect and bias their efforts towards spatial aggregations of resources. A forager may increase its benefits from the degree of aggregation and its ability to detect and respond to these aggregations (Brown 2000). Foragers should travel through poor patches and harvest rich ones in order to maximize their fitness rewards (Stephens and Krebs 1986). These rewards may take the form of energy, safety (Sih 1980, Brown and Kotler 2004), trace nutrients (Pulliam 1975), and/or variance of intake rates (Caraco 1980). Several patch characteristics are known to influence resource harvest and forager effort (see Meyer and Valone 1999 for an example with multiple foraging costs). First and foremost is initial food abundance. Foragers should aim to harvest more from rich patches than poor. Yet, information constraints may cause foragers to overutilize poor patches while underutilizing rich patches (Valone and Brown 1989, Valone 2006). Other patch characteristics include the type of food (Brown and Morgan 1995), the type of substrate from which the forager must detect and extract resources (Price and Heinz 1984, Price and Podolsky 1989, Kotler et al. 2001), climate (Kilpatrick 2003) and predation risk (Brown et al. 1988). Less investigated, yet of likely importance are substrate depth (Nolet et al. 2006), and patch area (Schmidt and Brown 1996). Furthermore, what happens when several factors simultaneously influence the properties of a single patch? Here, we investigate simultaneously three of these important factors: initial food abundance, substrate depth, and patch area. We show how several factors acting together yield a more sophisticated and diverse set of predictions. The actual response of the foragers then yields insights into how they assess and respond to resource heterogeneity. We investigate changes 1721 in giving-up density (GUD), cumulative harvest, and proportion of food harvested from patches that vary in elements that create diminishing returns in natural situations, namely: area, substrate depth and initial food abundance. Foraging strategies and sensory perceptions such as visual, olfactory and tactile cues allow for varying degrees of information acquisition. Foragers use this information to assess and respond to heterogeneity in prey abundance, patch size and volume. For instance, the long beaks of storks, the long-mobile snouts of elephant shrews, olfactory sense of kangaroo rats and digging abilities of gerbils may allow them to forage deeper in substrates while de-emphasizing the role of vision. Whereas diurnal seed-eating birds may rely on vision to find seeds, enhancing seed harvest from the surface at the expense of seeds deeper in the substrate (Kotler and Brown 1999, Vander Wall et al. 2003, Skinner and Chimimba 2005). Varying patch attributes has been used to investigate environmental features that influence a forager’s patch use behavior. Using behavioral titration approaches from foraging theory, one can better elucidate how habitat heterogeneity at both the macro- and micro-scales may influence resource exploitation and reveal mechanistic bases of patch use, diet choice, community organization and species coexistence (Moermond 1986, 1990, Brown 1989, Brown et al. 1994, Kotler and Blaustein 1995, Whelan 2001, Shochat et al. 2004). Diet choice can also be affected by the distribution and abundance of different foods (Brown and Morgan 1995). Fox squirrels were partially selective on the food with higher initial abundance when presented apart, while the higher encounter probability of peanuts increased their selectivity at low quitting harvest rates and the preference of sunflower seeds increased their selectivity at high quitting harvest rates (Brown and Morgan 1995). Studies on the effects of patch characteristics on foraging behavior generally vary just initial prey abundance. Exceptions include studies that vary predation risk by placing food patches in safe or risky habitats (Brown and Kotler 2004), or by changing the distribution of food within an experimental food patch (e.g. micropatch partitioning, Schmidt and Brown 1996). The relationship between harvest and time spent exploiting a depletable food patch (gain curve) directly affect the forager’s patch departure. A gain curve emerges as an interaction between structural resource properties, the abundance and distribution of food within the patch, the forager’s sensory abilities and mechanics of patch exploitation (Olsson et al. 2001, 2002). When changing patch characteristics such as initial prey abundance or within patch resource distributions, the forager experiences a change in its gain curve that should influence how long and how thoroughly the forager uses the patch (Olsson et al. 2001, 2002). As might be expected, increasing the initial prey abundance elevates the gain curve (Olsson et al. 2001) and increases the amount of food harvested before the patch is abandoned. In general, both the amount of food harvested and the giving up density increase with increasing initial prey abundance (Valone and Brown 1989, Morgan et al. 1997). Furthermore, as an indication of patch assessment by the forager, the proportion of food harvested generally increases 1722 with initial food density. The failure to equalize GUDs seems to result from imperfect patch assessment (Olsson et al. 1999) or a gain curve in which the per time encounter probability changes with patch depletion (the gain curve deviates from the ideal of random search see Olsson et al. 2002 for an example with starlings). Foraging behavior is also influenced by the substrate from which an animal feeds. Price and Podolsky (1989) and Price and Hienz (1984) showed how coarser substrates than sand reduce the harvest rates of desert granivores. Furthermore, adding gravel and small rocks to the surface of a patch increased the GUD relative to their absence (Kotler and Brown 1999). Davidson and Morris (2001) reported on increased GUDs in fine sand due to its higher bulk densities compared to coarse sand. Such a result is consistent with quitting harvest rate rules under elevated costs of foraging (Davidson and Morris 2001). Under natural and experimental conditions, foraging behavior of warblers gleaning insects appeared to be strongly influenced by fine-scale foliage structure, mainly: the species of leaf, accessibility to perches, and the leaf surface qualities (Whelan 1989, 2001). Often, giving-up density is used as a surrogate for quitting harvest rate. However, when patches vary in characteristics such as area, volume, and substrate depth the simple notion of a giving-up density can be expressed in different ways. Most studies present giving-up densities as grams of food remaining per patch, where other patch characteristics remain constant across experimental food patches. But when patches vary in surface area, would it be more appropriate to express giving-up densities as per unit surface area, or as GUDs per unit volume when patches vary in substrate depth? In fact, measuring GUDs per patch, GUDs per unit area, and GUDs per unit volume from patches that vary in surface area and in substrate volume provides different and complementary ways of viewing attributes of patch use under natural situations. We used these metrics to examine and compare foraging behavior and patch assessment by mourning doves and cottontails for patches varying in depth, area and initial prey abundance. We measured the giving-up-densities (grams of food remaining per patch) of mourning doves and cottontails in experimental food patches. We studied the effect of patch characteristics on foraging to reveal factors influencing quitting harvest rates. We explored the reaction of two very different species to the same patch properties to test for characteristics of resource patches to which foragers can respond and whether these characteristics are general or species specific, and to evaluated patch design for measuring GUDs. In what follows we develop testable predictions from foraging theory. We then present our study methods for measuring the giving-up densities of doves and then cottontails from patches that varied with respect to initial food density, surface area and sand depth, and then present the results. Predictions and results are couched in terms of how patch characteristics and the forager’s patch use strategies may influence the amount of food harvested, proportion of food harvested, GUD per patch, GUD per unit surface area (GUDAREA), and GUD per unit volume of sand (GUDVOL). Predictions In order to increase energy benefits and reduce effort, foragers should bias their efforts towards patches with smaller areas (higher concentration of food per unit area), shallower substrates (greater ease of food encounter) and higher total resource abundance (for a fixed area and substrate depth). The model of Morgan et al. (1997) on the effect of spatial scale on the functional response (food harvest as a function of initial food abundance) of fox squirrels, predicts that increasing patch richness should increase proportion of food harvested. Here we consider within-environment effects of increasing patch richness by varying independently three properties of patch quality: initial food abundance (traditional sense of patch richness), patch size, and substrate depth. Effect of initial food abundance (IPA) If the foragers are able to detect and respond to the initial abundance of food within the patch, they should bias their efforts towards patches with higher initial resource abundances. The size of this bias and its effect on GUDs and the proportions of food harvested depend on the forager’s ability to accurately assess patch quality. At one extreme the forager may be ‘prescient’ (sensu Valone and Brown 1989) and able to use sensory cues such as vision and olfaction to accurately estimate patch quality. As it forages, its estimate of remaining density is simply the difference between its initial estimate and its cumulative harvest from the patch. Such a forager should leave each patch at the same quitting harvest rate which under random search yields the same GUD independent of initial food abundance. At the other extreme, the forager may be unable to make any assessment of current patch quality. Such a forager should devote the same amount of search time to each patch resulting in the same proportions of food harvested regardless of initial food abundance. In between is the ‘ordinary’ forager that has imperfect information, but which gains some insights into patch quality as it forages the patch. Such a forager (e.g. Bayesian) will not leave each patch at the same quitting harvest rate (GUDs will often increase with initial food abundance see Olsson and Brown 2006) but it will favor rich patches with more search time (the proportion of food harvested will also increase with initial food abundance). Our data revealed (see results) that both mourning doves and cottontails forage under imperfect information. Their GUD, food harvest and proportion of food harvested all increased significantly with initial prey abundance. This knowledge about their patch use becomes essential in establishing the next two sets of predictions. Effect of substrate depth When holding depth or patch area fixed, there are no ambiguities regarding the units of initial prey abundance and GUD they are simply grams per tray. However, in considering the effects of substrate depth, GUD and initial prey abundance can be treated as either grams per patch (controlling for patch area) or grams per unit volume of substrate. We will reserve ‘GUD’ for grams per patch, and ‘GUDVOL’ when referring to grams per unit volume. (a) More and deeper substrate should reduce harvest rates for any remaining amount of food both because of the greater volume of substrate and the encumbrance of having to dig deeper. Shallow patches should be in all ways more favorable than deeper patches. Hence, we expect giving-up densities, food harvest and the proportions of food harvested to vary as follows: GUD1:5 cm BGUD3:0 cm BGUD4:5 cm food harvest1:5 cm food harvest3:0 cm food harvest4:5 cm proportion of food harvested1.5 cm proportion of food harvested3.0 cm proportion of food harvested4.5 cm (b) When evaluated as GUDVOL (food remaining per unit substrate volume) there is now the confounding effect of initial prey abundance per unit volume declining with depth. An initial abundance of say 2 g per patch represents only one third as much food per unit volume when comparing a depth of 4.5 cm with 1.5 cm. From the results of prediction 1 we know that GUD increases with initial prey abundance and hence we can expect the same effect to carry over for GUDVOL in response to initial prey abundance per unit volume: GUDVOL; 1:5 cm GUDVOL; 3:0 cm GUDVOL; 4:5 cm This prediction presupposes that depth provides little encumbrance to foraging. However, if the forager is either unable or unwilling to forage to the depth of the deepest trays, or if the forager’s encounter probability with deeper seeds is substantially lower than on shallower seeds, then substrate volume will have a disproportionate effect on GUDs and: Food harvest1:5 cm Food harvest3:0 cm Food harvest4:5 cm 3GUD1:5 cm B2GUD3:0 cm B GUD4:5 cm GUDVOL; 1:5 cm BGUDVOL; 3:0 cm B GUDVOL; 4:5 cm These predictions examine how GUDs change with substrate volume for a fixed initial abundance of food within the entire patch. Which of the alternative predictions holds for how GUDVOL changes with substrate depth indicates the degree to which depth itself is a large encumbrance to the forager. One can use the switch point in this relationship as a gauge for when depth becomes troublesome to a forager. (c) We can evaluate the effect of depth on GUDVOL by fixing the initial prey abundance per unit volume. This can be done by using the three treatment combinations of A (1.5 cm depth and 2 g initial prey abundance), B (3 cm depth and 4 g) and C (4.5 cm depth and 6 g). We expect the shallow trays to offer a more favorable encounter probability on seeds because as substrate depth and volume increases there is the encumbrance of having to dig more and deeper in search of food items. We expect that GUDVOL will increase with depth: GUDVOL; 1:5 cm BGUDVOL; 3:0 cm B GUDVOL; 4:5 cm We test this prediction by only considering the treatments shown by the shaded boxes in Table 1. 1723 resource density 8 6.25 5.128 4 3.158 2.575 2.667 2.069 1.714 sand volume 0.75 0.96 1.17 1.5 1.9 2.33 2.25 2.9 3.5 GUDsmall tray B GUDmedium tray BGUDlarge tray Food harvestsmall tray Food harvestmedium tray Food harvestlarge tray Proportion of food harvestedsmall tray Proportion of food harvestedmedium tray small medium large small medium large small medium large Proportion of food harvestedlarge tray However, there is a confounding effect when evaluating giving-up density per unit area (GUDAREA). For a given initial food abundance the patch richness per unit area goes up in smaller areas. Since GUDs increase with initial prey abundance this effect should manifest as: GUDAREA; 5.333 4.167 3.419 2.667 2.105 1.717 1.778 1.379 1.143 resource density tray size 6 gram Like the effects of depth, we expect smaller patches to offer more favorable encounter probabilities as the seeds are now concentrated into a smaller area. Hence: small tray GUDAREA; medium tray GUDAREA; large tray 0.75 0.96 1.17 1.5 1.9 2.33 2.25 2.9 3.5 small medium large small medium large small medium large 2.667 2.083 1.709 1.333 1.053 0.858 0.889 0.69 0.571 (C) 4.5 cm (B) 3.0 cm small medium large small medium large small medium large (A) 1.5 cm 0.75 0.96 1.17 1.5 1.9 2.33 2.25 2.9 3.5 sand volume sand volume tray size Depth 2 gram resource density tray size 4 gram Material and methods IPA Table 1. The experimental layout of food patches used at each site. The 27 artificial food patches established into three stations (A, B and C) of nine trays each. 1724 Effect of patch size (area) Experiments on mourning doves were conducted in two residential backyards of Oak Park, a suburb of the Chicago Metropolitan Area, Illinois, USA. As food, we used commercially available millet seeds. Following periods of pre-baiting and acclimation, data were collected for nine days within each backyard between 18 December 2005 and 10 April 2006. Days were not necessarily consecutive and were spaced to correspond with favorable weather. First one yard and then the other were sampled. Each yard contained a community of seed-eating birds. Based on direct observations, up to 12 mourning doves Zenaida macroura, six European collared doves Streptopelia decaocto (just in the first yard) and ca 25 house sparrows Passer montanus used these trays (in the second yard sparrows could number beyond 50). Based on direct observations, mourning doves contributed to the bulk of foraging and essentially all of the foraging beyond a certain point. More so than the sparrows, mourning doves were willing to dig deeper into the substrate by swishing their beaks back and forth through the sand. For experiments on the eastern cottontail, we used the same patches but for food we used commercial alfalfa pellets. We selected three sites spaced ca 30 m apart at the greenhouse compound of the Univ. of Illinois at Chicago. Eighteen days of data (six days per site) were collected intermittently from 22 May 2006 until 4 August 2006. Cottontails were the only foragers within these trays and the total number of cottontails at the compound appeared to range from ca 1015 individuals. Each of the five sites (two backyards for doves and three greenhouse sites for cottontails) received 27 artificial food patches arranged as three stations of nine trays (Table 1). The 27 food patches represented all combinations of three trays sizes (circular, plastic trays of 27, 32 and 36 cm in diameter), three substrate depths (1.5, 3 and 4.5 cm depth), Results The experiments yielded a total of 486 data points from each species (doves: 2 sites 9 days per site 27 trays per day; cottontails: 3 sites 6 days per site 27 trays per day). GUD data were log-transformed both to reduce heteroscadisticity (correlation between mean GUD and variance) and to remove problems associated with evaluating the variance of data created by taking the ratio of two variables. We used the general linear model of SYSTAT 10 to test for differences in the dependent variables (GUD, food harvest, proportion of food harvested, GUDAREA and GUDVOL) separately against the independent variables patch depth, initial food abundance, tray size and species. Two-way and three-way interactions between the independent variables were also incorporated in the analyses (Appendix 1). We conducted separate analyses on the different independent variables. We feel this successfully identifies the roles of patch characteristics and the forager’s patch use strategy in influencing these related but complementary metrics of patch use. Effect of initial food abundance GUD, food harvest and the proportion of food harvested all increased significantly with the initial food abundance. Mean giving-up densities (in grams) ranged between 1.19, 2.25 and 3.10 (for the doves) and 5.86, 11.57 and 16.63 (for cottontails) for patches of low, medium and high initial food abundance, respectively. Both doves and cottontails were able to bias their efforts towards the richer patches. The proportion of food harvested from a patch increased from 40.7, 43.8 to 48.3% (for the doves) and 34.9, 35.8 to 38.4% (for the rabbits) in patches of low, medium and high initial food abundance, respectively (Fig. 1, Appendix 1). Both doves and cottontails responded somewhat similarly to 7 Mourning doves Grams of food 6 Giving-up densities Food harvest 5 4 3 2 1 0 0 2 4 6 8 Initial abundance of food 30 Cottontails Grams of food and three initial food abundances (2, 4 and 6 g of millet per patch for doves, and 9, 18 and 27 g of pellets per patch for rabbits), (Table 1). The nine trays of a station were randomly arranged next to each other in a circle. On a given day, each station received either all of the low depth, medium depth or high depth trays. Within a ring all nine combinations of tray size and initial food density were present. Over a three day period, the three substrate depths were rotated as a 33 latin square design across the three stations. For doves the latin square was repeated three times for each site generating a total of nine days per site. For cottontails the latin square was repeated twice for a total of six days per site. Prior to a day (from mourning doves) or night (from cottontails) of data collection, trays were filled with food mixed thoroughly into the sifted sand substrate (commercial bank sand). For doves, trays were established between 07:00 and 08:00, left out all day, and then the remaining seeds were sieved from each tray between 16:00 and 18:00 h. For cottontails, trays were established around noon and then left for 24 h prior to sieving out the remaining food. The bulk of cottontail foraging occurred at night with a peak of activity just after dusk. Giving-up densities Food harvest 20 10 0 0 9 18 27 36 Initial abundance of food Figure 1. Mean giving-up densities and amounts of food harvested by mourning doves and cottontails were significantly higher in patches with high initial abundance of food. changes in initial food abundance, however, mourning doves harvested a higher proportion of food from patches than did the cottontails. Effect of substrate depth GUD, food harvest and the proportion of food harvested all varied significantly with substrate depth (F2,458 138.8, 108.4 and 142.4 for the doves and F2,459 30.18, 20.19 and 26.77 for the cottontails respectively with pB0.001 for all three variables). Mean giving-up-densities increased from 1.4 and 9.9 g in shallow patches to 2.8 and 12.7 g in patches of the deepest substrate for mourning doves and cottontails respectively (Fig. 2). Foraging of both species was negatively influenced by increasing patch depth as food harvest from deep patches decreased by both species. In deep patches, mourning doves showed lower foraging efficiencies than cottontails as their proportions of food harvested went through a sharper decline from 64.2% in shallow patches to 27.9% in deep ones. This decline ranged between 45.2% and 28.9% for the cottontails. Our results showed a significant increase in food harvest with depth when opportunities for easy harvest of food were available (i.e. shallow patches). Patch depth showed a significant interaction with initial food abundance for 1725 6 (a) 5 4 3 2 1 0 0.0 1.5 3.0 4.5 Giving-up density per unit volume Giving-up density in grams of food (a) 6.0 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 0.0 1.5 (b) 30 20 10 0 0.0 1.5 3.0 4.5 6.0 Giving-up density per unit volume Giving-up density in grams of food Patch depth (b) 3.0 4.5 6.0 4.5 6.0 Patch depth 40 30 20 10 0 0.0 1.5 3.0 Patch depth Patch depth Figure 2. Mean giving-up densities of mourning doves (a) and cottontails (b) increased significantly with substrate depth. Boxes show the range in which 50% of the values fall. The whiskers show the range of observed values that fall within 1.5the spreads of 50% of the values. x are the outside values that fall beyond 1.5 the spread of 50% of the values. Figure 3. Mean giving-up densities per unit volume of sand decreased significantly with patch depth for mourning doves (a) and cottontails (b). Boxes show the range in which 50% of the values fall. The whiskers show the range of observed values that fall within 1.5 the spreads of 50% of the values. x, o are the outside and far outside values that fall beyond 1.5the spread of 50% of the values and 3 the spread of the values respectively. mourning doves with respect to food harvest (i.e. effect of depth was greatest at low initial food abundance and less so at high initial food abundance) but not with the proportion harvested (F4,458 8.24, pB0.001; F4,458 0.7, p 0.589 respectively). In contrary, patch depth and initial food abundance had no significant interactions in the cottontail data. Giving-up densities per unit volume of sand exhibited significant differences with respect to all main effects of depth, tray area and initial food abundance for both species (Appendix 1). A significant decrease in GUDVOL with depth was reported (Fig. 3), with data ranging between 1.6 and 5.9 g l1 in shallow patches to 1.1 and 2.6 g l1 in deep patches for mourning doves and cottontails respectively (F2,458 14.05 and F2,459 86.34, pB0.001). GUDVOL was also significantly lower in large trays (1.1 and 1.3 g l1) than in small ones (1.5 and 8.8 g l1 for mourning doves and cottontails respectively) (F2,458 14.93 and F2,459 522.6, pB0.001). For our final comparison of depth effects, we used different depths of sand, but with the same initial food abundance per unit volume for all trays of a given size (equal ‘standardized’ resource density, see treatments shown in grey in Table 1). Both species exhibited lower GUDs (standardized GUDs) per volume of sand in shallow patches (Fig. 4). Standardized GUDs decreased from 1.39 and 7.49 g l1 in small trays to 0.97 and 1.1 g l1 in large trays for mourning doves and cottontails respectively. 1726 Effect of patch area There was no significant effect of patch size on giving-up densities, amounts of food harvested or the proportion of food harvested (F2,459 0.22, p0.8 and F2,459 0.182, p0.83 and F2,459 0.1, p 0.9 respectively) for cottontails. However, mourning doves had significantly different GUDs (but not food harvest or proportion harvested) between patches of different sizes (F2,458 4.83, p B0.05 and F2,458 1.73, p0.18 and F2,458 1.58, p 0.21 respectively). Mean giving-up densities (in grams) ranged between 2.087, 2.176 and 2.271 (for mourning doves) and 11.46, 11.21 and 11.38 (for cottontails) for patches (a) 2 1 0 0.0 1.5 3.0 4.5 6 Giving-up density in grams of food 3 Giving-up density per unit volume (a) 5 4 3 2 1 0 6.0 large (b) 15 10 5 0 0.0 1.5 3.0 medium small Patch area 4.5 Giving-up density in grams of food (b) Giving-up density per unit volume Patch depth 30 20 10 6.0 0 large medium small Patch area Patch depth Figure 4. Mean giving-up densities per unit volume from patches of standardized resource abundance increased with depth for mourning doves (a) and cottontails (b). Boxes show the range in which 50% of the values fall. The whiskers show the range of observed values that fall within 1.5 the spreads of 50% of the values. * are the outside values that fall beyond 1.5the spread of 50% of the values. Figure 5. Patch area had no effect on the giving-up densities of mourning doves (a) and cottontail rabbits (b). There was no significant increase in food harvest from smaller food patches. Boxes show the range in which 50% of the values fall. The whiskers show the range of observed values that fall within 1.5 the spreads of 50% of the values. of small, medium and large area, respectively. While cottontails had a slight decrease in mean GUDs throughout the three patch areas, mean GUDs of mourning doves increased between small and large patches (Fig. 5). However, when GUDs were measured per unit area of the food patches (GUDAREA), they were significantly lower in large (22.33 and 24.87 g m2) than small (36.48 and 44.54 g m2) trays for mourning doves and cottontails respectively (Fig. 6). The interaction effect of patch size and initial food abundance were significant with respect to GUDAREA for both species (Appendix 1). GUDAREA was significantly lower in smaller patches through the different initial food abundances (F4,458 2.59, p B0.05 and F4,459 10.46, pB0.001). Additionally, mourning doves had significant interactions of patch depth with both patch area and initial food abundance with respect to GUDAREA. This suggests an increase in the negative influence of small patches on foraging efficiency with depth and low initial food abundance (Appendix 1). Discussion Initial prey abundance, substrate depth and patch area all influenced the patch use behavior of mourning doves and cottontail rabbits. Both species biased their efforts towards individual patches of higher resource density and shallow patches with easier opportunities for food harvest. More food and a higher proportion of food were harvested from patches with higher initial abundances (Fig. 1). As expected, deeper substrate reduced the foragers’ encounter probability with food (as they required more bulk volume of sand, which in turn reduced resource density, Table 1), decreased patch quality and resulted in higher GUDs and lower harvests (Fig. 2). Contrary to our prediction, patch size had no effect on food harvest or the proportion of food harvested by both species. However, mourning doves had significantly lower GUDs in smaller patches. Yet, GUDAREA and GUDVOL increased for both species in patches with a smaller surface area or shallow depth. 1727 120 Giving-up density per unit area (a) 100 80 60 40 20 0 large medium small Patch area 120 Giving-up density per unit area (b) 100 80 60 40 20 0 large medium small Patch area Figure 6. Mourning doves (a) and cottontails (b) had higher GUDAREA in smaller trays due to the increase in patch richness with small patch areas. Boxes show the range in which 50% of the values fall. The whiskers show the range of observed values that fall within 1.5the spreads of 50% of the values. Despite being very different taxa foraging for quite different foods, the mourning doves and cottontails exhibited some similar responses and small differences. The increased substrate depth affected mourning doves more than cottontails. Forager size and foraging technique likely explain this. Based on direct observations, both species explore the substrate by alternating bouts of churning the substrate followed by the harvest of freshly exposed food items. Mourning doves do this by swishing their beaks sideways through the sand. To access deeper seeds, the dove must partially plunge its head into the sand. The rabbits shovel backwards with their forepaws leaving larger and deeper disturbances than the doves. Hence, the GUDs of doves nearly doubled in going from 1.5 cm of sand to 4.5 cm. Whereas this increase in depth caused less than a 50% increase in the GUDs of cottontails. In the present study, we used greater volumes of sand to increase substrate depth and vary the ease of encounter to prey between the different patches. The foragers took more advantage of shallow patches by increasing their food harvest compared to deep ones (Fig. 2, 3, GUDVOL with standardized initial food abundance); the effect of deep patches was magnified with decreasing patch area and initial food abundance (Appendix 1). However, for a given initial 1728 prey abundance, GUDVOL decreased with depth through the different patches, indicating that both the mourning doves and cottontails forage under imperfect information, as they use information acquired while exploiting the patches. This effect on GUDVOL came about via a decline in initial food abundance per unit volume as more sand was added to the patch. In the present study, mourning doves’ food harvest exhibited a significant interaction between patch depth and initial food abundance. In deep patches (4.5 cm of sand), the doves increased their food harvest from 0.52, 1.03 to 1.94 g at low, medium and high initial food abundance respectively. This shows that the foragers were willing to dig deeper and more thoroughly in patches of higher resource density hence, increasing the available food motivated more intense patch use. The doves are able to choose, up to a point, how deep to dig and this depth will increase with initial food abundance. Gawlik (2002) manipulated fish density and water depth (i.e. prey accessibility) in artificial ponds accessible to eight species of free-ranging wading birds. Foraging birds increased their GUDs with increasing water depth and had an upper threshold for water depth beyond which they appeared unable to forage. A similar relationship between elevated GUDs and food accessibility was reported in a swan-pond system (Nolet et al. 2001, 2006) where GUDs on benthic tubers increased with water depth. In the swan-pond system, accessibility was more important than differences in energetic costs of foraging. Accessibility to resources determined by foliage structure also affects avian foraging behavior by influencing encounter rates with prey, prey accessibility, and energetic costs of attacking and capturing prey (Greenberg and Gradwohl 1980, Robinson and Holmes 1982, Schmidt 1998, Whelan 1989, 2001). We selected a mammalian and an avian forager as they offered wide opportunities for diverse foraging performances due to different sensory and locomotory modes. Thus, their similar responses to resource density and patch depth emerged from very different ways of foraging and perhaps sensing and collecting food. Mourning doves appeared to be more efficient foragers, harvesting a greater fraction of food from the patches than cottontails. Food quality, background availability of food, and experience with this sort of foraging may all have been more favorable for the mourning doves. Doves in these yards were already well familiar with concentrations of millet seeds from bird feeders; seeds are probably at low background abundance in the surrounding lawns, and uncovering buried seeds is probably part of doves’ normal foraging repertoire. Cottontails were foraging for a novel food (alfalfa pellets) against a background of potentially equal forage in the lawn, and digging may not be part of a cottontail’s normal foraging behavior. Experience and memory of patch distribution (pre-harvest information) played an important role in allowing northern bobwhites to selectively exploit patches high in resources (Kohlmann and Risenhoover 1998). When exploiting novel, but predictable patches, Vásquez et al. (2006) showed that degus Octodon degus made more accurate assessments of patch quality after two weeks of foraging from paired patches of poor and rich resource densities. This pattern was observed consistently from day 15 to day 21 and agrees with a prescient foraging strategy (Valone and Brown 1989). Vásquez et al. (2006) suggests that the Bayesian behavior observed in previous patch assessment studies could result from short-term evaluations and/or lack of resource predictability (Valone and Brown 1989, Olsson et al. 1999). Contrary to previous studies where mourning doves followed a fixed time patch exploitation strategy (Valone and Brown 1989, Hayslette and Mirarchi 2002), our results showed the doves discriminating appropriately between rich and poor patches. Our patches were smaller and generally offered more striking contrasts in area, depth and initial prey abundance than those used by Valone and Brown (1989) in the Sonoran Desert of Arizona. Alternatively the schedule of daily offering the same mourning doves the same suite of patch choices may have honed their ability and/or willingness to discriminate between patches (Valone and Brown 1989, Vásquez et al. 2006). Conditioning and memory may have played a role. The foragers may use the daily renewal of patches to improve their ‘within-day’ patch assessment and exploitation abilities (Vásquez et al. 2006). Contrary to our predictions, patches with smaller area had higher GUDAREA and GUDVOL. A part of this increase likely is the effect of increased initial food abundance per unit area as tray size declines. However, the foraging mode and the physical effect of foraging while standing in the food patch also contributed to the increased GUDAREA and GUDVOL in smaller patches. Our results demonstrate that while both species were willing to dig deeper in large patches, those patches were easier to manage than their equivalent size of small-sized patches. GUDs throughout all patches from mourning doves (regardless of resource density or depth) have shown a slight increase with patch area (2.087 g in small to 2.27 g in large patches) suggesting the doves taking some benefit from higher encounter probability in smaller patches. Perhaps placing our trays in close proximity caused some assessment ambiguities that caused doves and cottontails alike to fail to distinguish patches of different sizes. Kohlmann and Risenhoover (1998) found that northern bobwhites Colinus virginianus were able to distinguish among patches of different quality when the patches were spaced 1.5 m a part, but failed to do so when they were 0 or 3 m apart. Given our foragers’ ability to distinguish initial food abundance and depth we find it unlikely that they were unable to detect the area of these raised plastic trays with distinct rims and boundaries. More likely, the way the doves and cottontails stand in the trays while foraging obliterated any advantage to having a smaller tray. This behavior may have obstructed access to areas of the patch directly underfoot creating blind spots that would have increased proportionately as patch size declined. Smaller trays may exacerbate this blind spot and cancel any advantages gained from having to search a smaller area. We may have gotten quite different results had we buried the trays flush with the surrounding lawn. Curiously, with the surface of the food patches being higher than the ground (since we placed the trays directly on the ground), we never observed doves or cottontails harvesting food while standing outside of the patches. Environmental heterogeneity and patchiness represent both challenge and opportunity for foragers. If the forager can detect and respond to this heterogeneity then it can either harvest more in the same amount of time or spend less effort harvesting a given amount of food. Actually knowing what a free-ranging animal can and cannot assess is hard to achieve through direct observations, or within its uncontrolled environment. Here we let animals reveal their abilities through artificial food patches placed within their environment. Such patches can be and have been used to examine predation risk, diet preferences, habitat selection and habitat quality (Brown 1988, Kotler and Brown 1988). When using artificial food patches patch characters such as substrate selection, patch area and depth are important properties for establishing a useful tool for challenging the forager to reveal its perceptions. We combined standard methods of measuring giving-up densities from food patches with new metrics (GUDAREA and GUDVOL) in patches that vary in surface area, substrate volume and initial resource abundance. We examined and compared these metrics on the foraging performance of two different foragers. Both doves and rabbits harvested a higher proportion of food and biased their feeding efforts towards rich patches and patches with shallow substrates. The similar results exhibited by doves and cottontails suggest that behavioral titrations can be a general and useful tool for assessing an animal’s perceptions of heterogeneous environments and resources. The predictions and different GUD metrics should prove useful when developing appropriate artificial food patches for new taxa. It may be useful to first expose the study animals to a battery of patch types that vary in size, initial food abundance, and substrate. Furthermore, varying these patch characteristics may reveal those factors that most influence the forager’s efficiency. For research on animals foraging from natural food patches, this research suggests that clear measures of patch area and patch ‘depth’ may be important. Measures of food availability and harvest per patch, per unit area, and per unit volume may provide complementary information on the animal’s foraging responses and even its niche. The alternate hypotheses and predictions for how initial prey abundance, patch area and substrate depth should influence patch use behaviors apply equally to free-ranging and captive animals, and to experimental as well as natural food patches. Acknowledgements We thank Thomas Halfpenny from Chicago, Illinois for allowing us to work in his backyard. We also thank C. Whelan and S. Emerson for providing comments on earlier versions of the manuscript, and K. Schmidt for initiating discussions at ESA, 2006. We thank T. Valone, D. Morris and B. Kotler for comments that greatly improved the manuscript. References Brown, J. S. 1988. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 22: 2747. Brown, J. S. 1989. Desert rodent community structure: a test of four mechanisms of coexistence. Ecol. Monogr. 59: 120. Brown, J. S. 2000. Foraging ecology of animals in response to heterogeneous environments. In: Hutchings, J. and Stewart, A. (eds), The ecological consequences of environmental heterogeneity. Blackwell, pp. 181215. Brown, J. S. and Morgan, R. A. 1995. Effects of foraging behavior and spatial scale on diet selectivity: a test with fox squirrels. Oikos 74: 122136. 1729 Brown, J. S. and Kotler, B. P. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecol. Lett. 7: 9991014. Brown, J. S. et al. 1988. The effects of owl predation on the foraging behavior of heteromyid rodents. Oecologia 76: 408415. Brown, J. S. et al. 1994. Foraging theory, patch use and the structure of a Negev desert granivore community. Ecology 75: 22862300. Caraco, T. 1980. On foraging time allocation in a stochastic environment. Ecology 61: 119128. Charnov, E. L. 1976. Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theor. Popul. Biol. 9: 129136. Davidson, D. L and Morris, D. W. 2001. Density-dependent foraging effort of deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus). Funct. Ecol. 15: 575583. Gawlik, D. E. 2002. The effects of prey availability on the numerical response of wading birds. Ecol. Monogr. 72: 329 346. Greenberg, R. and Gradwohl, J. 1980. Leaf surface specializations of birds and arthropods in Panamanian forest. Oecologia 46: 115124. Hayslette, S. E. and Mirarchi, R. E. 2002. Foraging-patch use and within-patch diet selectivity in mourning doves, Zenaida macroura. Ecology 83: 26372641. Kilpatrick, A. M. 2003. The impact of thermoregulatory costs on foraging behaviour: a test with American crows (Corvus brachyrhynchos) and eastern grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis). Evol. Ecol. Res. 5: 781786. Kohlmann, S. G. and Risenhoover, K. L. 1998. Effects of resource distribution, patch spacing, and preharvest information on foraging decisions of northern bobwhites. Behav. Ecol. 9: 177186. Kotler, B. P. and Brown, J. S. 1988. Environmental heterogeneity and the coexistence of desert rodents. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 19: 281301. Kotler, B. P. and Blaustein, L. 1995. Titrating food and safety in a heterogeneous environment: when are the risky and safe patches of equal value? Oikos 74: 251258. Kotler, B. P. and Brown, J. S. 1999. Mechanisms of coexistence of optimal foragers as determinants of local abundances and distributions of desert granivores. J. Mammal. 80: 361374. Kotler, B. P. et al. 2001. Foraging substrate and escape substrate: patch use by three species of gerbils. Ecology 82: 1781 1790. Meyer, M. D. and Valone, T. J. 1999. Foraging under multiple costs: the importance of predation, energetic, and assessment error costs to a desert forager. Oikos 87: 571579. Moermond, T. C. 1986. A mechanistic approach to the structure of animal communities: Anolis lizards and birds. Am. Zool. 26: 2337. Moermond, T. C. 1990. A functional approach to foraging: morphology, behavior and the capacity to exploit. In: Morrison, M. L. et al. (eds), Avian foraging: theory, methodology and applications. Studies in Avian Biology No. 13. Allen Press, pp. 427430. Morgan, R. A. et al. 1997. The effect of spatial scale on the functional response of fox squirrels. Ecology 78: 10871097. Nolet, B. A. et al. 2001. Spatial variation in tuber depletion by swans explained by differences in net intake rates. Ecology 82: 16551667. 1730 Nolet, B. A. et al. 2006. Foraging costs and accessibility as determinants of giving-up densities in a swan-pondweed system. Oikos 112: 353362. Olsson, O. and Brown, J. S. 2006. The foraging benefits of information and the penalty of ignorance. Oikos 112: 260 273. Olsson, O. et al. 1999. Gaining ecological information about Bayesian foragers through their behaviour. II. A field test with woodpeckers. Oikos 87: 264276. Olsson, O. et al. 2001. Gain curves in depletable food patches: a test of five models with European starlings. Evol. Ecol. Res. 3: 285310. Olsson, O. et al. 2002. Long- and short term state-dependent foraging under predation risk: an indication of habitat quality. Anim. Behav. 63: 981989. Price, M. V. and Heinz, K. M. 1984. Effects of body size, seed density and soil characteristics on rates of seed harvest by heteromyid rodents. Oecologia 61: 420425. Price, M. V. and Podolsky, R. H. 1989. Mechanisms of seed harvest by heteromyid rodents: soil texture effects on harvest rate and seed size selection. Oecologia 81: 267273. Pulliam, H. R. 1975. Diet optimization with nutrient constraints. Am. Nat. 109: 765768. Robinson, S. K. and Holmes, R. T. 1982. Foraging behavior of forest birds: the relationship among search tactics, diet, and habitat structure. Ecology 63: 19181931. Schmidt, K. A. 1998. Consequences of partially directed search effort. Evol. Ecol. 12: 263277. Schmidt, K. A. and Brown, J. S. 1996. Patch assessment in fox squirrels: the role of resource density, patch size and patch boundaries. Am. Nat. 147: 360380. Shochat, E. et al. 2004. Linking optimal foraging behavior to bird community structure in an urban-desert landscape: field experiments with artificial food patches. Am. Nat. 164: 232243. Sih, A. 1980. Optimal behavior: Can foragers balance two conflicting demands? Science 210: 10411043. Skinner, J. D. and Chimimba, C. T. 2005. The mammals of the southern African subregion. Cambridge Univ. Press. Stephens, D. W. and Krebs, J. R. 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton Univ. Press. Stephens, D. W. et al. (eds) 2007. Foraging: behavior and ecology. Univ. of Chicago Press. Vásquez, R. A. et al. 2006. On the value of information: studying changes in patch assessment abilities through learning. Oikos 112: 298310. Valone, T. J. 2006. Are animals capable of Bayesian updating? an empirical review. Oikos 112: 252256. Valone, T. J. and Brown, J. S. 1989. Measuring patch assessment abilities of desert granivores. Ecology 70: 18001810. Vander Wall, S. B. et al. 2003. Interspecific variation in the olfactory abilities of granivorous rodents. J. Mammal. 84: 487496. Whelan, C. J. 1989. Avian foliage structure preferences for foraging and the effect of prey biomass. Anim. Behav. 38: 839846. Whelan, C. J. 2001. Foliage structure influences foraging of insectivorous forest birds: an experimental study. Ecology 82: 219231. Appendix 1. The effects of initial abundance of food, substrate depth and patch size on the giving-up-densities (in log scale), amount harvested and the proportion of food harvested by mourning doves and cottontail rabbits (IPA, initial prey abundance). *pB0.05, **pB0.01, ***pB0.001. Mourning doves Source DF Depth IPA Traysize Yard Traysizedepth IPAdepth IPAtraysize IPAtraysizedepth Error LGUD 2 2 2 1 4 4 4 8 458 Depth IPA Traysize Yard Traysizedepth IPAdepth IPAtraysize IPAtraysizedepth Error Mean-square Food harvest 2 2 2 1 4 4 4 8 458 39.396 32.857 1.371 0.009 0.595 0.234 0.063 0.056 0.284 138.823*** 115.780*** 4.833** 0.033 2.097 0.826 0.224 0.196 86.290 176.939 1.379 0.882 0.275 6.556 0.316 0.119 0.796 108.421*** 222.319*** 1.733 1.108 0.346 8.238*** 0.397 0.149 Proportion of food harvested Depth 2 5.500 IPA 2 0.240 Traysize 2 0.061 Yard 1 0.097 Traysizedepth 4 0.018 IPAdepth 4 0.027 IPAtraysize 4 0.010 IPAtraysizedepth 8 0.007 Error 458 0.039 Depth IPA Traysize Yard Traysizedepth IPAdepth IPAtraysize IPAtraysizedepth Error GUDAREA 2 15628.812 2 25382.309 2 8401.663 1 178.323 4 723.585 4 1179.823 4 408.472 8 72.872 458 157.975 Depth IPA Traysize Yard Traysizedepth IPAdepth IPAtraysize IPAtraysizedepth Error GUDVOL 2 2 2 1 4 4 4 8 458 Yard Depth Traysize Traysizedepth Error F-ratio 10.186 56.000 10.827 0.637 0.100 0.539 0.471 0.048 0.725 GUDVOL standardized 1 0.040 2 6.947 2 3.742 4 0.230 152 0.221 142.386*** 6.209** 1.581 2.522 0.453 0.704 0.270 0.181 98.932*** 160.673*** 53.183*** 1.129 4.580*** 7.468*** 2.586* 0.461 IPA traysizedepth Error 8 459 0.073 0.180 0.403 Food harvest 2 310.548 2 2124.294 2 2.794 4 9.960 4 15.103 4 2.476 8 2.146 459 15.384 20.187*** 138.089*** 0.182 0.647 0.982 0.161 0.140 Proportion of food harvested Depth 2 1.100 IPA 2 0.055 Traysize 2 0.004 Traysizedepth 4 0.041 IPA depth 4 0.028 IPA traysize 4 0.006 IPA traysizedepth 8 0.007 Error 459 0.041 26.767*** 1.333 0.102 0.999 0.685 0.141 0.160 Depth IPA Traysize Traysizedepth IPA depth IPA traysize IPA traysizedepth Error GUDAREA 2 2994.821 2 41213.132 2 16413.621 4 307.758 4 129.227 4 1461.951 8 20.528 459 139.762 21.428*** 294.881*** 117.440*** 2.202 0.925 10.460*** 0.147 Depth IPA Traysize Traysizedepth IPA depth IPA traysize IPA traysizedepth Error GUDVOL 2 2 2 4 4 4 8 459 86.341*** 118.091*** 522.624*** 28.126*** 9.973*** 45.432*** 4.144*** Depth IPA Traysize Traysizedepth IPA depth IPA traysize IPA traysizedepth Error Depth Traysize Traysizedepth Error 466.115 637.520 2821.403 151.837 53.840 245.269 22.372 5.399 GUDVOL standardized 2 16.306 2 672.583 4 8.864 151 1.881 8.668*** 357.533*** 4.712*** 14.046*** 77.222*** 14.929*** 0.879 0.138 0.744 0.650 0.066 0.179 31.413*** 16.919*** 1.038 Cottontails Depth IPA Traysize Traysizedepth IPAdepth IPAtraysize LGUD 2 2 2 4 4 4 5.445 45.713 0.039 0.160 0.117 0.060 30.176*** 253.320*** 0.218 0.887 0.650 0.333 1731