Download Perception, Memory, and Action in Frontal and Parietal Cortex

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Microneurography wikipedia , lookup

Cortical stimulation mapping wikipedia , lookup

Macropsia wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
J Neurophysiol 95: 3309 –3310, 2006;
doi:10.1152/jn.00163.2006.
Editorial Focus
Perception, Memory, and Action in Frontal and Parietal Cortex. Focus on
“Selection and Maintenance of Saccade Goals in the Human Frontal
Eye Fields”
Vincent P. Ferrera and Jack Grinband
Mahoney Center for Brain and Behavior, Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University and New
York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, New York
Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: V. Ferrera, Mahoney
Center for Brain and Behavior, Center for Neurobiology and Behavior,
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY (E-mail:
[email protected]).
www.jn.org
response. A previous study (Connolly et al. 2002) showed that
motor set modulated activity in FEF but not IPS.
Other interpretations are possible, but the most likely of
these are ruled out by the data. For example, one can imagine
that during the first delay, subjects covertly prepare all four
movements instead of (or in addition to) simply remembering
the stimulus locations. In this case, one might infer that IPS is
more closely involved in movement planning because it has the
greatest activation during the first delay. This explanation can
be rejected because IPS activation fell to baseline levels during
the second delay despite the continued need for motor response
preparation. FEF activation remained significantly above baseline. A reduction in the number of planned movements from
four to one might account for the reduced activation seen in
both FEF and IPS but not the observed inversion in the relative
strength of activation between the two areas.
The fact that the number of locations to be remembered
changes once the target is cued raises the issue of whether an
overall reduction in the level of BOLD activation between the
first and second delays is due to response selection, per se, as
opposed to the corresponding reduction in memory load. To
address this, Curtis and D’Esposito looked for lateralized
activation in IPS and FEF. It is well known that the majority of
neurons in the parietal and prefrontal cortex prefer contralateral
stimuli and/or contraversive movements (Blatt et al. 1990;
Funahashi et al. 1990). Because the initial stimulus array
covered both visual hemifields, the first delay interval showed
bilateral brain activity that was equal across the two hemispheres. However, after presenting the target cue, activity in
the second delay interval became strongly lateralized in the
hemisphere contralateral to the position of the target. The
lateralized IPS activation was short-lived, but FEF activation
was sustained and the degree of lateralization increased over
time until the response was executed. Thus the lateralization of
activity in FEF increased even though there was only a single
item in memory. More importantly, lateralized FEF activation
just prior to the motor response predicted the direction of the
eye movement; IPS activation did not.
Neurophysiological studies in monkey FEF and LIP support
the visual-motor distinction. These studies have revealed neurons with visual, movement and mixed visual-movement activity (Andersen et al. 1990; Barash et al. 1991; Bruce and
Goldberg 1985). Visual and visual-movement neurons are
found in both areas (Chafee and Goldman-Rakic 1998); however, pure movement neurons without visual responses are
common in FEF but rare in LIP. Movement-related activity in
parietal cortex tends to evolve slowly and is best demonstrated
in delayed-response tasks. If monkeys are required to make an
0022-3077/06 $8.00 Copyright © 2006 The American Physiological Society
3309
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on July 31, 2017
Visually guided behavior engages posterior parietal and
lateral prefrontal cortex, yet the unique contributions of these
brain regions are strongly contested. Recent experiments by
Curtis and D’Esposito use functional magnetic resonance imaging to differentiate the roles of parietal and prefrontal cortex
with respect to visual working memory and movement planning.
The notion of functional specialization in the visual system
has a long if not uncontroversial history (Goodale and Milner
1992; Scheider 1969; Trevarthen 1968; Ungerleider and Mishkin 1982). One current area of contention is the division of
labor between posterior parietal (PPC) and lateral prefrontal
cortex (PFC). Both are involved in visual-motor transformations, but is one inherently more “visual” and the other more
“motor”? In this issue of Journal of Neurophysiology (p.
3923–3927), a functional imaging study by Curtis and
D’Esposito (2006) shows that PPC and PFC perform a kind of
dance during visual-motor behavior with first one area taking
the lead and then the other. Their results suggest that PPC is
more active during the encoding and storage of visual stimuli
in working memory, but PFC becomes more active during
response preparation and execution. These results lend support
to the view that parietal cortex contains a general purpose
visual map (Gottlieb et al. 1998), whereas prefrontal cortex is
more action oriented.
Curtis and D’Esposito’s experiment used a delayed-response
task that evoked activity in a region of the precentral sulcus
likely to be the human frontal eye field (FEF), and a region in
the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) likely to be the human equivalent
of monkey lateral intraparietal area (LIP). Subjects were required to remember an array of four stimulus locations during
an extended delay period. At the end of this delay, a cue
identified one of the four locations as the saccadic goal. A
second delay interval required the maintenance of the target
location until a movement execution cue was given. IPS and
FEF showed opposite patterns of blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) activitation during the two delay intervals. During the “visual memory” delay, activation was stronger in IPS
than in FEF. However, during the “movement preparation”
delay, the pattern of activation reversed. These results support
the idea that neural activity in IPS is primarily influenced by
the processing and storage of visual stimuli, whereas FEF is
more heavily recruited during the preparation of the motor
Editorial Focus
3310
REFERENCES
Andersen RA, Bracewell RM, Barash S, Gnadt JW, and Fogassi L. Eye
position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP
and 7a of macaque. J Neurosci 10: 1176 –1196, 1990.
Andersen RA, Brotchie PR, and Mazzoni P. Evidence for the lateral
intraparietal area as the parietal eye field. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2: 840 – 846,
1992.
Barash S, Bracewell RM, Fogassi L, Gnadt JW, and Andersen RA.
Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. I. Temporal properties; comparison with area 7a. J Neurophysiol 66: 1095–1108, 1991.
Blatt GJ, Andersen RA, and Stoner GR. Visual receptive field organization
and cortico-cortical connections of the lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in
the macaque. J Comp Neurol 299: 421– 445, 1990.
Bruce CJ and Goldberg ME. Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons
discharging before saccades. J Neurophysiol 53: 603– 635, 1985.
J Neurophysiol • VOL
Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME, Bushnell MC, and Stanton GB. Primate frontal
eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked
eye movements. J Neurophysiol 54: 714 –734, 1985.
Chafee MV and Goldman-Rakic PS. Matching patterns of activity in primate
prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working
memory task. J Neurophysiol 79: 2919 –2940, 1998.
Connolly JD, Goodale MA, Menon RS, and Munoz DP. Human fMRI
evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set. Nat Neurosci 5:
1345–1352, 2002.
Curtis CE and D’Esposito M. Selection and maintenance of saccade goals in
the human frontal eye fields. J Neurophysiol 95: 3923–3927, 2006.
Everling S and Munoz DP. Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated
with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. J Neurosci 20: 387– 400, 2000.
Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, and Goldman-Rakic PS. Visuospatial coding in
primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms. J Neurophysiol 63: 814 – 831, 1990.
Goldberg ME, Bisley J, Powell KD, Gottlieb J, and Kusunoki M. The role
of the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey in the generation of saccades
and visuospatial attention. Ann NY Acad Sci 956: 205–215, 2002.
Goodale MA and Milner AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and
action. Trends Neurosci 15: 20 –25, 1992.
Gottlieb J and Goldberg ME. Activity of neurons in the lateral intraparietal
area of the monkey during an antisaccade task. Nat Neurosci 2: 906 –912,
1999.
Gottlieb, J, Kusunoki, M, and Goldberg, M.E. The representation of visual
salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature 391: 481– 484, 1998.
Moore T, Armstrong KM, and Fallah M. Visuomotor origins of covert
spatial attention. Neuron 40: 671– 683, 2003.
Posner MI and Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu
Rev Neurosci 13: 25– 42, 1990.
Schneider GE. Two visual systems: brain mechanisms for localization and
discrimination are dissociated by tectal and cortical lesions. Science 163:
895–902, 1969.
Shibutani H, Sakata H, and Hyvarinen J. Saccade and blinking evoked by
microstimulation of the posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey.
Exp Brain Res 55: 1– 8, 1984.
Snyder LH, Batista AP, and Andersen RA. Intention-related activity in the
posterior parietal cortex: a review. Vision Res 40: 1433–1441, 2000.
Thompson KG, Biscoe KL, and Sato TR. Neuronal basis of covert spatial
attention in the frontal eye field. J Neurosci 25: 9479 –9487, 2005.
Trevarthen CB. Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychol Forsch 31:
229 –337, 1968.
Ungerleider LG and Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems:
separation of appearance and location of objects. In: Analysis of Visual
Behavior, edited by Ingle DL, Goodale MA, and Mansfield RJW. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1982, p. 549 –586.
95 • JUNE 2006 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on July 31, 2017
immediate “anti-saccade” (a saccade directed away from the
visual stimulus) when a target appears, FEF neurons are able to
reliably signal the direction of the movement (Everling and
Munoz 2000), but LIP neurons are not (Gottlieb et al. 1999).
Finally, whereas eye movements can be elicited by stimulating
either FEF or LIP, stimulation thresholds in FEF are much
lower than LIP (Bruce et al. 1985; Shibutani et al. 1984).
FEF and LIP are key components of a fronto-parietal network that is involved in spatial attention, working memory, and
eye movements (Posner and Petersen 1990). Different labs
have come to almost diametrically opposed views on the role
of parietal cortex in this circuit. One group has proposed that
parietal cortex represents the attentional salience of visual
objects (Goldberg et al. 2002), whereas others have emphasized its role in motor planning (Snyder et al. 2000). The view
that FEF is primarily “motor” has likewise been called into
question by studies showing the involvement of FEF in attentional tasks (Moore et al. 2003; Thompson et al. 2005). Electrophysiologically, FEF and IPS show as many similarities as
differences. The value of the Curtis and D’Esposito study is
that it clearly shows, at the level of fMRI, a movement
planning signal in FEF that is not present in IPS, thus providing
evidence for functional specialization in the fronto-parietal
circuit.