Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Cognitive Structure and Real Life Implementation of Scripts in Late Adulthood L. Godbout, PhD M. Fiola, MA C. M. J. Braun, PhD S. Gagnon, PhD ABSTRACT. Previous research has demonstrated that healthy senescent cohorts manifest marked impairment in cognitive performance, particularly on tests of executive functions. Studies directly investigating ADL have found mild and tardive impairment in senescence, and a relation with certain executive functions, but the targeted ADL were very simple tasks such as memorizing a telephone number or walking a few meters and have always been strictly limited to the accuracy domain–excluding any speed factor. The purpose of the present study was (1) to investigate performance, in an experimentally controlled manner, in normal senescent cohorts, on one of the most complex ADL (planning and preparing a meal under time pressure), more indicative of true quality of life of seL. Godbout is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois Rivières, and the Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Montréal. M. Fiola is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois Rivières. C. M. J. Braun is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Montréal. S. Gagnon is affiliated with the School of Psychology, University of Ottawa. Address correspondence to: C. M. J. Braun, PhD, Professeur Titulaire, Centre de Neurosciences Cognitives, UQAM, C.P. 8888, Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, Québec, Canada, H3C 3P8. This research was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Grants (Canada) to the third and fourth authors, and a Fonds de Recherche en Santé du Québec grant to the first and third authors. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics, Vol. 23(1) 2004 Available online at http://www.haworthpress.com/web/POTG 2004 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved. Digital Object Identifier: 10.1300/J148v23n01_03 25 26 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS nior citizens, and (2) to scrutinize its cognitive structure. A large battery of tests of executive function, including a script generation task were also administered. It was found that despite numerous marked impairments on tests of executive function, this particular ADL was not globally impaired even in advanced senescence. This finding suggests that the combination of deep proceduralization over a lifetime and continued daily practice suffice to maintain complex ADL, i.e., quality of life, well into late senescence, despite important decline in cognitive resources. [Article copies available for a fee from The Haworth Document Delivery Service: 1-800-HAWORTH. E-mail address: <[email protected]> Website: <http://www.HaworthPress.com> 2004 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved.] KEYWORDS. Aging, frontal, scripts, ADL, supervisory attentional system, contention scheduler, central executive system, prospective memory, planning INTRODUCTION Cognitive decline in late life primarily involves executive functions (De Luca, Wood, Anderson, Buchanan, Proffitt, Mahony & Pantelis, 2003), memory (Rabbitt & Lowe, 2000) and cognitive and behavioural slowing (Schretlen et al., 2000). Each of these three factors contributes significant distinct variance to the decline of quality of life in the late years. Several executive functions depend more on frontal lobe function than on that of other lobes (Burgess, 2000; Godefroy, Cabaret, PetitChenal, Pruvo & Rousseaux, 1999; Stuss & Alexander, 2000). Executive decline in late life is explainable by regional age-related decline of brain function (Debettignles, Swihart, Green & Pirozzolo, 1997; Matsuda, Ohnishi, Asada, Li, Kanetaka, Imabayashi, Tanaka & Nakano, 2003) and anatomy (O’Sullivan, Jones, Summers, Morris, Williams & Markus, 2001) which starts before senescence to a great extent, if not primarily, in the frontal cortex (Cowell, Turetsky, Gur & Grossman, 1994; Schretlen et al., 2000). Normal senescence is eventually associated with decline in self reported activities of daily living, at least in very late life (Demura et al., 2003; Femia, Zarit & Johansson, 1997; Willis, 1996). Some studies have observed correlations between neuropsychological tests of executive function and self ratings of ADL in the elderly Godbout et al. 27 (Cahn-Weiner et al., 2000; Carlson, Fried, Xue, Bandeen-Roche, Zeger & Brandt, 1999; Royall, Chiodo & Polk, 2000). It also appears that neuropsychological tests of executive function manifest impairment in normal aging long before any impairment of ADL can be documented with self rating scales (Corey-Bloom, Wiederholt, Edelstein, Salmon, Cahn & Barrett-Connor, 1996; Crawford & Channon, 2002; Grigsby, Kaye, Baxter, Shetterly & Hamman, 1998; Steen, Sonn, Hanson & Steen, 2001). One must be concerned that a face-saving mechanism could cause a bias against self-reported age-related decline in ADL, a problem which can be overcome by direct measurement of ADL. Investigating cohorts of normal elderly, several research groups have found that neuropsychological tests of executive function significantly correlated with functional status on the Independent Living Scales (ILS), a direct measure of basic ADL such as simple memory items, managing money, problem solving regarding transportation, home maintenance, and social adjustment (Cahn-Weiner, Boyle & Malloy, 2000; Carlson et al., 1999), even when corrected for education, socioeconomic factors and disease-related processes. With these factors controlled, age itself contributes little variance to the ILS (Bell-McGinty, Podell, Franzen, Baird & Williams, 2000). Much larger cohorts (Grigsby et al., 1998; Carlson et al., 1999) did present a significant correlation between age and the ILS, but the actual explained variance is unimpressive, as in the smaller scale studies (1 to 7%). The effect of frontal lobe dysfunction on executive function and personality, and ensuing behavioral disturbance or impairment in real life situations in young adult patients is well established. However, very little is known about how decline of executive function in normal late life might lead to behavioral disturbance or impairment in ADL. Script generation is a mental representation of long action chains leading toward a goal. Shallice (1982) first proposed a frontal lobe/basal ganglia model of script generation in 1982. He proposed that the nonroutine, more abstract aspects of script generation are processed by a frontal lobe module which he named the supervisory attentional system (SAS). He also proposed that the more routine subcomponents of script generation, individual actions, are processed automatically and procedurally by a basal ganglia module which he named the contention scheduler (CS). Grafman (1989) has since further developed the neuropsychological articulation of script generation. He insisted on the sequencing aspect of script generation wherein time management is a constant operant and wherein managerial knowledge units (MKUs) are 28 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS arranged to form chains. Rather than distinguishing the SAS from the CS, he preferred to state that script generation has the same structure whether the scripts be abstract or concrete, and that in both cases the frontal lobe ought to play a critical role. Exploration of executive components of ADL also has a relatively recent history. Frontal lobe lesions definitely impair ADL (Goel et al., 1997; Schwartz, Reed, Montgomery, Palmer & Mayer, 1991) including ADL formatted as routine everyday action chains which must be performed in chronological order toward a goal (Fortin, Godbout & Braun, 2003). Impairment of ADL is believed to involve primarily (or at least importantly) executive functions of several types, including planning, flexibility and prospective memory (Acker, 1990; Bassett & Folstein, 1991; Grafman, Sirigu, Spector & Hendler, 1993; Mazaux, Masson, Levin, Maurette & Barat, 1997; Shallice & Burgess, 1991). A few attempts have recently been made to operationalize script generation tasks as ADL. Schwartz and colleagues (1991; 1998) concluded that the impairment of ADL observed in their cohorts of head trauma patients did not resemble a frontal dysexecutive syndrome as much as a globally limited attentional capacity. However, the ADL investigated were very simple (e.g., making a cup of coffee). Though normal late life is significantly associated with decline of performance on numerous paper-pencil tests of executive function, it remains unclear whether there is any real decline of mental or real life implementation of lengthy routine scripts. Indeed, early senescence does not seem to involve any decline in mental representation of routine scripts (Godbout & Soucy, 1996; Godbout, Doucet & Fiola, 2000; Roman, Brownell, Potter & Seibold, 1987; Rosen, Caplan, Sheesley, Rodriguez & Grafman, 2003). On the other hand, when script recitation tasks involve non-routine situations, impairment is observed in early senescence (Godbout, Cloutier, Bouchard, Braun & Gagnon, 2004). The present investigation had four purposes concerning normal aging in healthy, non-demented community dwellers: (1) to confirm significant weakness of executive functions as determined with neuropsychological tests in early senescence, (2) to determine whether mental representation of routine scripts eventually breaks down in late senescence, and more importantly, the originality of this project was to (3) determine whether a highly routine, yet complex and lengthy multitask real life activity sequence (a highly constrained and quantifiable behavioural ADL script under time pressure) is impaired in normal late life, and (4) to determine how it is related to assessments of executive func- Godbout et al. 29 tions and mental representations of routine scripts. The proposed methodology is identical to Fortin et al. (2003). METHOD Participants. Two cohorts of normal women were tested. The study was limited to women in order to increase homogeneity of the participants’ experience with planning and preparing meals. All participants were living autonomously and were preparing meals on a daily basis. A young adult group comprised 23 female students aged 18 to 35. An elderly group consisted of 20 women 65 to 83 years of age. Half the participants of the last group were in early senescence (between 64 and 73 years) and will henceforth be referred to as “young old” and half were in late senescence (between 74 and 83 years) and will henceforth be referred to as “old old.” In light of previous significant findings using the same methods and sample sizes in frontal patients (Fortin et al., 2003; Godbout et al., 2004), it was felt that statistical power of this experiment would be sufficient. In the approach proposed here, the classical criterion of “large groups from which easily collectible data are obtained” is replaced by “extremely time consuming data collection from small small cohorts.” All participants were recruited via a local newspaper. Exclusion criteria included substance abuse and psychiatric or neurological consultation, or any major health problem (ex: hospitalization during the last year, cancer, diabetes, vascular, endocrinological, or other diseases). Elderly participants were also required to be dementia free as estimated from the Modified Mental Status Scale (Teng & Chui, 1987; limen = 80%) and the Mini Mental Status Examination (Folstein, Folstein & McHugh, 1975; limen = 24/30). An important inclusion criterion was that all participants be autonomous with regard to meal preparation on a daily basis. There was one left hander in the young adult group and one in the young old group. All participants signed a consent form before participating (see Table 1). Standardized tests. Tests selected for routine assessment of executive functions covered the following four cognitive domains. 1. organization and planning: The Picture Arrangement subtest of the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981) requires the subject to place in sequence, under time pressure, cards describing elements of a situation unfolding in time. The Porteus Mazes-Revised (Arthur, 1947) 30 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS 2. 3. 4. 5. is considered a measure of planning because the subject must analyze and memorize a trajectory through a maze before starting to draw a pathway towards the exit. mental flexibility and resistance to interference: The Trail Making Test (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985) requires linking numbers on a sheet of paper with a pencil (Part A), in order of increasing magnitude, under time pressure. The second part of the test requires adapting to a new assignment (shifting sets) in order to link numbers to letters in alternation (Part B). lexical access and categorization: Thurstone’s Verbal Fluency Test (Thurstone & Thurstone, 1962), requires that the subject state words that start with a given letter (the phonological fluency test) or representing an animal (the semantic fluency test) without repetition during 10 seconds–requiring rapid lexical access and effortful attention to avoid repetitions and category errors. selective and sustained attention: Ruff’s 2-and-7 Test (Baillargeon, 1994; Ruff & Allen, 1996), comprises a phonological attention task, then a visuospatial attention task, then a task combining the two previous ones. The last task is far more demanding on working memory. mental sequencing: Luria’s Graphic Sequences (Luria, 1966) requires the subject to resist maintaining a simple behavioral set by requiring that the subject alternate between drawing simple and slightly more complex designs. Script recitation task. As in Godbout and Doyon (1995), participants were asked to enumerate out loud “10 to 20 actions, that people generally do, in proper order,” for a specific ADL. Participants were asked to avoid idiosyncrasies and were given two examples, “getting up in the TABLE 1. Biographic characteristics of the participants Age (years) Groups Education (years) Mini Mental State Examination M SD M SD Modified Mental Status Scale M SD M SD Young adult 22.78 3.44 15.70 1.06 - - - - Young old 67.00 2.24 14.30 2.28 29.3 0.7 96.4 2.1 Old old 77.40 2.76 10.10 3.86 28.5 1.2 92.6 4.7 Note. The cut off score for the Mini Mental State Examination is 24/30, and for the Modified Mental Status Examination, it is 80% out of 100% Godbout et al. 31 morning” and “putting on your coat and leaving.” After repeating the instructions correctly, they were asked to generate and recite aloud a script representing “going to the restaurant” and another representing “doing the groceries.” The semantic structure of the recited actions was measured using inclusion criteria based on four types of action (% major, % minor, % trivial and % relevant intrusion) as in Godbout and Doyon (1995) and Godbout and Bouchard (1999). To be included, an action had to be mentioned by at least 25% of the participants of the normative study (see Godbout and Doyon, 1995). Actions that met this criterion were subsequently classified as major (mentioned by more than 65% of the participants), minor (mentioned by 45-64% of the participants) or trivial (mentioned by 25%-44% of the participants). A relevant intrusion was marked when the action did not meet the inclusion criteria but belonged to the particular script. Sequence errors, perseverations and irrelevant intrusions were tallied as in Godbout and Doyon (1995). Examples of sequence errors, perseverations and irrelevant intrusions in the restaurant script are (1) “paying the bill and then eating the dessert,” (2) mentioning “paying the bill” twice, and (3) “stopping at the pharmacy,” respectively. Because of the large number of actions which must be sequenced in the subject’s mind, without any concrete support (no test materials), beyond the limits of immediate rote memory (which is 7 items plus or minus two), the script task is considered a planning and sequencing task which necessarily solicits working memory (effortful action-oriented attention). The ADL task. The assignment consisted of planning and preparing a complete meal. It was determined that all participants had had frequent experience with that particular ADL in real life. Participants were required to (1) prepare a menu (2) shop for groceries, and (3) prepare the meal selected. Each of these tasks comprises a sequence which is incontrovertible. For example, doing the groceries requires taking a cart, walking the aisles, selecting items, unloading the cart onto the counter, and taking the bags. The menu selection task. The participant was seated before a table with meal suggestion cards (4 entrées, 4 main courses, 4 desserts), an envelope containing money ($10.00) and a subset of ingredients required for each meal. The participant was required to plan a 3 course meal within the budget allotted that could be prepared within 45-60 minutes. Only one menu was feasible, canned soup as entrée, boiled potatoes and ground beef for the main course and brownies for dessert. The task required that the participant take cognizance of the budget, select the menu accordingly and prepare the list of missing ingredients. 32 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS The setting was a hospital cuisinette with two experimenters, one providing the instructions and the other handling the video camera. Participants who prepared an inappropriate menu were requested, afterwards, to prepare the soup-beef/potatoes-brownie menu so as to create a more standard context for the subsequent task. The grocery shopping task. Once the menu was planned, the participant was required to go to the grocery and purchase the missing ingredients, having been instructed to select the smallest quantities of the essential ingredients only, and to stay within the budget. The experimenters accompanied the participant and took note of the behavior. The meal preparation task. Once back at the cuisinette, the participant was required to prepare the meal as if for a guest arriving in an hour–including setting the table for two. To respect this constraint, the participant had to understand that the dessert had to be prepared first (30 minutes preparation time). Participants were filmed and the experimenters limited their interactions with the participant to a strict minimum. The neuropsychological tests, script recitation task and ADL were completed in counterbalanced order. Scoring System of the ADL Tasks ADL tasks were scored in two different ways. In a first step, the level of success in completing the tasks was assessed. Errors belonging to this category are severe enough to prohibit appropriate completion of the task. In a second step, all action errors were examined in detail. Inability to achieve the goal. When errors of this type are made, chances that the task will be completed successfully decrease. The nature of those errors vary according to the ADL task. For instance, in the menu selection two errors could be made: selecting the wrong menu (based on the criteria already mentioned) or the wrong ingredients. For the grocery shopping, the following errors could occur: purchasing the wrong items or the wrong quantities, not limiting their purchases to the given budget or using extra money to buy food items. Finally, for the meal preparation, four type of errors were also of interest: not following the recipe, not completing each course of the meal successfully, not preparing the meal within the allotted time, and not finishing the courses simultaneously (not cold or overcooked). For each ADL task, the previous errors were summed up. Action errors. Action errors are similar to those tallied in the script recitation task. For the three ADL tasks, sequence, perseveration, intrusion and omission errors were compiled. In some cases, those errors Godbout et al. 33 might significantly influence the outcome of the ADL task (e.g., sequence, perseveration, intrusions errors might increase time duration and decrease the quality of the outcome). Other types of error were specific to the meal preparation task. This ADL task is the only one that comprises subscripts corresponding to each element of the meal (cooking the soup, the meat, the potatoes and preparing and baking the dessert). One error was associated with strategic planning and anticipation and was termed a first action error. The second was termed an end point timing error. Appropriate planning of the meal requires that the participant start by preparing the dessert first (first action error). It had to be prepared first because the recipe specified that it took far longer to cook than any other course. Participants also had to anticipate when each course would be ready in order to make sure that the four courses arrive at completion more or less simultaneously (end point timing error). In addition to anticipation, self-correction and time estimation are most likely involved in assuring that the meal is ready on time (Fortin et al., 2003). For this last variable, more than 12 minutes between two courses was the criterion for desynchrony of courses, based on the fact that none of the controls of the present study or of the 10 frontally lesioned patients (Godbout, Cloutier, Bouchard, Braun & Gagnon, 2004) or 10 controls of another study (Fortin et al., 2003) had surpassed 11 minutes. These latter functions are increasingly being recognized as none other than prospective memory (Wilkins & Baddeley, 1978). Indeed, when a lot of interference occurs during a substantial time interval, “planning” becomes “prospective memory”: the ability to remember to do something in the future. Alternation behavior. Alternation behavior was also examined in the meal preparation task. When cooking a meal it is normal to oversee the various components, in alternation. The cook can lower the heat if a course seems to be cooking too fast or raise the heat if a course is getting behind. This requires abandoning a course (ex: cooking the potatoes) for another (ex: cooking the beef) and vice versa and back and forth. Of course, alternation can be insufficient or it can be excessive. At any rate, this behavior was tallied for the meal preparation task only. Time-on-task. Finally, for all ADL tasks, one last variable was considered. Time on task was noted in minutes. This measure varied considerably and was thus transformed [log10(time required + 1)]. Macrostructure and microstructure of the ADL. In such a complex multitask ADL, we have proposed (Fortin et al., 2003; Godbout et al., 2004) that mental operations can be conceptualized and operationalized in terms of a macrostructure and a microstructure. The macrostructure 34 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS is the representation and implementation of the script as a whole (i.e., the actual plan towards a goal). This is a relatively abstract level of representation, germaine to the SAS (Shallice, 1988). The microstructure is the representation of the highly routine (overlearned and relatively short) sub-sequences of the script in which individual actions are termed “nodes” by Grafman (1989) (e.g., greasing the pan, putting the ground beef in the pan, turning off the element). This is a relatively concrete level of representation, which relates to the CS (see Fortin et al., 2003). These macro and microstructures cannot be drawn quantitatively from recited scripts, or even from the menu preparation or grocery shopping tasks. We believe this is why previous attempts to dissociate the CS from the SAS, with script recitation tasks, have failed. Only the meal preparation task lent itself to an operationalization of macrostructure/microstructure construct. We propose that the script’s macrostructure, in the cooking phase, is reflected in the first node and the ending. There is a deficit of macrostructure if the participant does not prepare the dessert first (first action error) and if the courses do not arrive on time or in fast enough sequence (end point timing error). The former represents a deficit of anticipation and strategic planning and the second represents an impairment of planning, self-correction and time estimation (Fortin et al., 2003). These latter functions are increasingly being recognized as none other than prospective memory (Wilkins & Baddeley, 1978). The dessert had to be prepared first because the recipe specified that it took far longer to cook than any other course. Errors of the microstructure included sequencing, perseverative, intrusive and omission errors. These errors do not necessarily result from an inability to abstract the script as a whole. They reflect more concrete and simple cognitive operations. The scoring system for our ADL has been developed in the course of several previous research projects on normal aging (Godbout, Doucet & Fiola, 2000; Godbout & Soucy, 1996) and frontally lesioned patients (Fortin et al., 2003; Godbout et al., 2004) as well as normal control groups in each of these studies. Scoring was done from the videotapes and notes by two independent raters. In the results section which follows, the alpha criterion is set at .05, and all inference tests are twotailed. Self-rating of competence on ADL. Finally, the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ) was used to evaluate ADL (Broadbent et al., 1982). The CFQ is a self-rating questionnaire which measures frequencies of failures of behavior in daily life. Godbout et al. 35 RESULTS Matching of the Experimental and Control Groups An ANOVA revealed a significant difference between groups in education (F(2,40) = 19.4, p < .001), a nearly inevitable effect of generations. The Tukey post hoc test determined that only the old old group (M = 10.1 years, SD = 3.9) differed significantly from the young adult (M = 15.7 years, SD = 1.1) and young old (M = 14.3 years, SD = 2.3) groups (see Table 1). Neuropsychological Tests For each test measure, the 23 young adult and 20 elderly participants were compared with ANOVA. As expected, most of the measures of executive function (14/17) yielded significant group effects compatible with an age-related decline in performance (see Table 2). We then proceeded to subdivide the elderly group into young old (N = 10) and old old (N = 10). For each test measure, the three groups were compared with ANOVA followed by the Tukey post hoc test for determination of simple effects. Exactly the same measures of executive function yielded significant group effects as previously, demonstrating an age-related decline in performance. In addition, significant weaknesses of the old old group relative to the young old group were observed on Trails B (time), Trails B minus A (time), Luria’s sequences B, and Semantic fluency. Though most neuropsychological measures of the present investigation were significantly correlated with education, the word fluency and Trails B minus A measures were not. This suggests that the effects reported above concerning these two particular measures can confidently be attributed to age, not education. Furthermore, statistical correction for education via ANCOVA did not bring any of the significant effects under the alpha criterion. These results confirm the “classical” cognitive impairment profile reviewed in the introduction. The Script Recitation Task Total actions. The total number of actions generated did not differ as a function of the two age groups (F(2, 40) = 1.1). This result allowed us to proceed with the analysis of the semantic structure and pattern of errors. 36 10.83 (2.61) 1.91 (2.21) 25.39 (7.67) 52.04 (16.75) 26.65 (17.25) 45.48 (17.54) .22 (.52) 22.83 (8.62) .22 (.74) 39.70 (10.24) .39 (.50) 1.26 (1.18) 44.57 (9.32) .96 (1.15) .52 (1.56) 94.04 (15.18) 94.35 (13.61) 6.30 (1.68) 4.60 (2.62) 52.90 (20.09) 91.50 (20.75) 38.60 (20.21) 72.10 (16.40) .60 (.66) 46.20 (12.77) .20 (.40) 34.80 (9.31) 1.20 (1.94) 2.70 (2.15) 39.70 (7.50) .90 (.70) .20 (.60) 60.50 (17.63) 58.40 (15.12) Group Young old n = 10 5.40 (1.50) 5.60 (1.69) 50.90 (7.18) 170.90 (103.67) 120.00 (103.28) 76.70 (19.84) 1.40 (1.28) 58.60 (14.29) .20 (.40) 29.00 (6.13) 1.40 (1.56) 2.00 (1.48) 30.70 (7.34) .70 (1.19) .50 (.81) 50.80 (9.84) 47.60 (13.60) Old old n = 10 26.92 11.11 25.79 16.71 10.70 13.52 7.30 38.17 .00 4.61 2.68 3.07 8.93 .19 .24 37.17 48.43 F .0001a, b .0001a, b .0001a, b .0001b, c .0012b, c .0001a,b .002b .0001a,b,c .9957 .0158b .0810 .0574 .0006b, c .8255 .7879 .0001a, b .0001a, b p Note. a = significant difference between the young adult and young old groups, b = significant difference between the young adult and old old groups, c = significant difference between the young old and old old groups. Tests Picture Arrangement (WAIS)–Scale score performance Porteus Mazes–Trials required to complete correctly Trail Making A–Seconds required to complete Trail Making B–Seconds required to complete Trail Making (B minus A)–Seconds required to complete Luria’s Series A–Seconds required to complete Luria’s Series A–Perseverative errors Luria’s Series B–Seconds required to complete Luria’s Series B–Perseverative errors Verbal Fluency–Number of words stated in 10 seconds Verbal Fluency–Perseverative errors Verbal Fluency–Rule breaking errors Semantic Fluency–Number of words stated in 10 seconds Semantic Fluency–Perseverative errors Semantic fluency–Rule breaking errors Ruff’s 2-7 Single Task–Items correct Ruff’s 2-7 Dual Task–Items correct Young adult n = 23 TABLE 2. Means (and standard deviations) on standardized tests of executive function and tests of the inference of group differences Godbout et al. 37 Semantic content. A 2 ⫻ 4 repeated measures ANOVA was calculated on responses within each of four semantic categories as a function of the 2 age groups. The semantic categories were major, minor, trivial or relevant intrusion. Though there was a significant effect of semantic category (F(1,41) = 14.5, p < .0001), none of the effects involving age reached significance. Tukey post hoc tests revealed that relevant intrusions were significantly more frequent than each of the three other semantic categories. When the three age groups were compared, these ANOVAs yielded the same results, confirming the results reported by Rosen et al. (2002). Error analysis. Three types of errors were scored, namely sequence errors, perseverations and irrelevant intrusions. Because of low rates, non-parametric analyses (Fisher exact probability test) were performed on cases dichotomized as having committed or not each type of error. The two group comparison did not reach significance for sequence errors (4/23, 9/20), perseverative errors (1/23, 5/20) or irrelevant intrusions (0/23, 3/20). With a three group model, there was a significant deterioration with age for sequence errors and irrelevant intrusion errors, but not perseverative errors. See Table 3. None of the script generation task measures were significantly correlated with education, indicating that education was not a confounding factor of age effects. TABLE 3. Percentages of cases presenting various types of errors on the script generation task and probabilities of significant effects based on the Fisher Exact Test Types of errors Young n = 23 Group Young old n = 10 Old old n = 10 17% 30% 60% .049 b Perseveration 4% 20% 30% .12 Irrelevant intrusion 0% 0% 30% .005 b Sequence error p Note. a = significant difference between the young adult and young old groups, b = significant difference between the young adult and old old groups, c = significant difference between the young old and old old groups. 38 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS The Activity of Daily Living (ADL) Inter-judge agreement for ratings of the ADL were at least 98% for the young adult group data and 97% for the senescent groups, with respect to all ratings except alternation (80%). Inability to achieve the goal. The rates for this type of error were too low to support parametric analysis, so for this error measure cases were dichotomized as presenting one or more errors versus none. Fisher exact probability tests revealed no group differences in preparation of the menu (10/23, 3/20), in grocery shopping (8/23, 5/20) or in meal preparation (9/23, 6/10). Nor were there group effects in a three group model (10/23, 2/10, 1/10), (8/23, 4/10, 1/10) (9/23, 3/10, 3/10). Time-on-task. An ANOVA (two groups) revealed a significant main effect of age for the grocery shopping condition (t(1,41) = 4.6, p < .0001) and for the meal preparation condition (t(1,41) = 3.9, p < .0005) but not for the menu preparation condition. The younger subjects performed better. An ANOVA designed for the same inference tests with three groups instead of two revealed a significant effect of age for the menu preparation condition (F(2,40) = 6.7, p < .01), for the grocery shopping condition (F(2,340) = 13.7, p < .001) and for the meal preparation condition (F(2,40) = 6.5, p < .01). In all three cases, post hoc tests revealed that the old old group was significantly slower than the two other groups. Alternation behavior. Alternation behavior was analyzed with ANOVA. In a two group model, the young adult group presented more alternation than the elderly group (F(1,40) = 14.4, p < .001). In the three group model, there was significantly more alternation behavior (F(2,40) = 6.8, p < .01) in the young adult group (M = 53, SD = 18.2) than in the young old (M = 36, SD = 10.8) or old old groups (M = 37, SD = 9.3), and the latter two groups did not differ from each other. Microstructure. In the two group model, there was no main effect of age in ANOVA with regard to perseverative errors. However, there were more sequencing errors and irrelevant intrusions in the elderly group but fewer omissions. The results were similar in the three group model (see Table 4). Macrostructure. Errors of the macrostructure consisted of two eventualities: not starting with the dessert in the meal preparation task and overpassing a synchronization criterion (12 minutes per course) in serving the meal. Taken separately, these two measures did not differ significantly as a function of group as determined by ANOVA in either the two or three group models. In the three group model, if summed, these Godbout et al. 39 TABLE 4. Means and standard deviations of four types of errors relevant to the microstructure on the activity of daily living (ADL) task and tests of the inference of group differences Error type Group F p Young adult Young old Old old 1.35 (1.53) .70 (.78) 1.40 (1.36) .90 .4155 .13 (.34) 1.00 (1.00) 2.10 (3.27) 4.62 .0157b Sequence 2.39 (1.31) 3.30 (1.68) 3.60 (2.11) 2.26 .1173 Omission 1.04 (1.26) 2.70 (1.62) 2.00 (1.26) 5.38 .0086a Perseveration Irrelevant intrusion Note. a = significant difference between the young adult and young old groups, b = significant difference between the young adult and old old groups, c = significant difference between the young old and old old groups. two error types yielded a significant difference, but only between the young adult and old old groups. See Table 5. None of the ADL task measures were significantly correlated with education, indicating that education was not a confounding factor of age effects. Regarding the question of relative magnitude of the effects observed in neuropsychological test measures and ADL performance task measures (continuous variables only), we converted the F values to Pearson correlations using Guilford’s (1965) formula; we also calculated intercorrelations of all these measures, and then applied Guilford’s (1965) inference test of a difference between correlation coefficients. Script generation tasks measures could not be thus treated as none were continuous. A first test of interest, serving to determine whether the neuropsychological tests are more sensitive than the ADL tasks, was to compare Ruff’s Dual Task measure with the omission error score on the ADL task. Both of these were the most sensitive to age in their respective domain (neuropsychological tests versus ADL tasks). The difference was not statistically significant, indicating that ADL tasks are just as sensitive to the effect of aging as the most sensitive neuropsychological tests of the present investigation. Likewise, the “overall planning” ADL score (i.e., the macrostructure) did not differ significantly from Ruff’s Dual Task in its ability to distinguish the age groups. 40 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS TABLE 5. Macrostructure: Means and standard deviations of two types of “errors” on the activity of daily living (ADL) task and tests of the inference of group differences Error type Young adult Group Young old Old old F p Starting point error .57 (.84) .60 (.92) 1.50 (1.12) 3.59 .0368 End point time interval (seconds over the time limit) 448 (406) 467 (378) 703 (499) 1.17 .3217 Overall planning .78 (.90) .80 (1.25) 2.10 (1.30) 5.24 .0095b, c Note. a = significant difference between the young adult and young old groups, b = significant difference between the young adult and old old groups, c = significant difference between the young old and old old groups. Planning represents the sum of starting point errors and of incidence of surpassing the 12 minute synchrony criterion for serving the courses warm. Prediction of performance on ADL with paper-pencil tasks and tests. ADL tasks such as the ones implemented here are far too time consuming and expensive to be of much use in a typical professional neuropsychological evaluation (though that might not be the case for occupational therapy). Yet, estimation of ADL is the prime concern of most professional neuropsychological evaluations in the elderly population. After all, the purpose of the evaluation of healthy elderly is usually not so much to plan remediation, but to recommend a life course (autonomous, semi-dependent, fully dependent). It is, thus, of interest to determine whether there are any good inexpensive predictors of ADL. The first such plausible predictor that comes to mind is the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire. This questionnaire did not distinguish the age groups nor did it correlate significantly with any of the ADL measures. On the other hand, virtually all the neuropsychological test measures significantly predicted one or several performance scores on the ADL tasks. The best such predictors, Trails-B and Ruff’s Dual Task, significantly predicted most ADL performance measures. See Table 6 for examples. These results, even the specific pattern of results, corroborates the relevant previous research cited in the introduction. The script generation task did not yield scores with distributions sufficiently continuous to justify correlational analysis. However, the sum of all error types (omissions, irrelevant intrusions, perseverations, sequence errors) was sufficiently continuous for this purpose. This index Godbout et al. 41 TABLE 6. Prediction of performance on the meal preparation ADL by the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire, a selection of neuropsychological tests and the composite error score on the script generation task (Pearson product-moment correlations and probability levels) Inability to achieve the goal CFQ Trails-B seconds Time on task Alternation Requests behavior for repetition of the instructions .07 (.629) ⫺.10 (0.518) .06 (.687) .20 (.197) .30 (.049) ⫺.29 (.058) Ruff’s Dual ⫺.09 (.551) ⫺.23 (.137) Task Script generation error score composite .45 (.002) .35 (.019) Microstructure Macrostructure .06 (.679) ⫺.01 (.938) ⫺.01 (.941) .68 (.0001) .55 (.0001) .45 (.003) .53 (.0003) ⫺.56 (.0001) ⫺.56 (.0001) ⫺.30 (.050) .69 (.0001) .69 (.0001) .50 (.0006) .26 (.086) was a remarkably good predictor of ADL (see Table 6 for examples), suggesting that a brief script generation task, scored in this manner might be a new and useful component of a gerontological neuropsychological test battery. DISCUSSION As expected, early senescence was characterized by significant deterioration on most of our standardized tests of executive function, and the decline was significantly exacerbated by late senescence, a finding unexplainable by differing education levels of the age groups. Of course, some tests of executive function are more sensitive to normal aging than others. The set of tests of executive function of the present investigation included indexes of planning and anticipation (Picture Arrangement and Porteus Mazes), of visuomotor speed and control (Trail Making Test, Luria’s sequences), of verbal fluency (Phonological fluency, semantic fluency), and of sustained attention (our adaptation of the Ruff 2-7 task, visual modality only, visual and auditory dual task version). All of these functions are known to decline in normal aging: [planning and anticipation: Vakil & Agmon-Ashkenazi, 1997, visuo- 42 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS motor speed: Rabbitt, 1996, verbal fluency: Mittenberg, Seidenberg, O’Leary & DiGiulio, 1989, Thuillard & Assal, 1991, sustained attention: Craik & Byrd, 1982; Plude & Hoyer, 1986]. In the present investigation, all these categories manifested decline in early senescence except verbal fluency–which manifested significant decline only in late senescence. The relative robustness (resistance to aging), within the domain of executive functions, of verbal fluency, has been previously observed (Daigneault, Braun & Whitaker, 1991). This can easily be explained by its “vocabulary” component which is indexed as a well established “hold” measure of the WAIS tests, i.e., vocabulary has long been known to resist aging quite well (Wechsler, 1981). It is already known that mental genesis of routine scripts (indexed by recitation), such as those of the present study, is not impaired by early senescence (Godbout & Soucy, 1996; Light & Anderson, 1983; Roman, Brownell, Potter & Seibold, 1987). This was confirmed here. All age groups were comparable in total number of actions and semantic structure. In short, at all ages, participants went about the task with quite similar strategies and cognitive representations. However, there was a modestly significant decline in ability, as indexed by sequence errors (an established index of frontal lobe function), (Fortin et al., 2003; Godbout & Doyon, 1995), by late senescence. Senescent participants also tend to be generally more idiosyncratic, self-centered, in their response style (Roman et al., 1987) and we believe this may be the principal explanation of the observed significantly increasing rate of irrelevant intrusions in late senescence. This confirms there exists a certain robustness of representation of routine scripts in normal aging, but at the same time specifies that there is a limit to this robustness. With regard to real life activities of daily living (ADL), this investigation presents the advantage and innovation of an exhaustive quantitative scoring grid of a very complex set of sequential activities under demanding time constraints. Though routine, these activities could well be the most complex and demanding in the everyday lives of senescent retirees. The outcome of this particular component of the present investigation is basically a demonstration of a remarkable robustness of complex routine ADL in normal aging, including late into senescence. Though the senescent participants were significantly slow (as indexed by time-on-task which surpassed the allotted limit on average) and significantly idiosyncratic (as indexed by irrelevant intrusions) they were quite able to carry out the task, to plan and deliver a three course meal. There are three arguments supporting the view that experience with these ADL protects them from decline in late life: (1) healthy elderly Godbout et al. 43 women continue to plan and prepare meals on a daily basis in high proportion (Moane, 1993), (2) significantly greater rates of omission errors and of alternation behavior of the young adult group in this study suggest that the senescent participants had a stronger procedural memory for this type of task, and (3) script recitation is not impaired in early senescence when the content of the recitation is routine (overlearned), but the same task reveals impairment in early senescence when the script must be recited in reverse order (a condition adding novelty, i.e., load, to working memory) or when a novel script must be created to deal with a novel goal (Bourassa, 2000; Bourassa & Godbout, 1996; Godbout & Bouchard, 1997). Nevertheless, it seems that frontal lobe-based executive function decline must inevitably impose limits on complex routine ADL requiring long sequences of actions under time constraints. Indeed, though there was a deficit of microstructure in senescence, this can best be explained as a manifestation of the previously described idiosyncratic style of senescence (Roman et al., 1987) rather than as an impairment per se. Our senescent participants made remarks about the meal not having to do with their own food preferences and intolerances and had difficulty de-centering their attitude from their own gastronomic style to this “generic” task which was not adapted to their specific lifestyle. For example, one old old participant declared that she needed more fruits and vegetables and proceeded, inefficiently, down those aisles in the grocery store. In addition, there was a modestly significant decline in the macrostructure by late senescence. The old old participants were slightly impaired in their ability to counter-intuitively sequence the cooking agenda and to adjust pro-actively all the ongoing subscripts, for optimal end point synchrony of the ensemble of the courses. In short, the old old participants of this investigation seemed to be faced with mild difficulties in strategic planning and in prospective memory. This suggests that a frontal-executive impairment was emerging which had become too profound to be compensated by everyday learning. We propose that strategic planning (see the introduction) and prospective memory are important components of complex ADL and both of these draw heavily upon working memory. Prospective memory consists of remembering, in the future, to execute a specific action. Prospective memory has been demonstrated to be frontal lobe based (Cockburn, 1995; Okuda, Fujii, Yamadori, Kawashima, Tsukiura & Fukatsu, 1998), to relate to several higher order executive functions (Bisiacchi, 1996; McDaniel, Glisky, Guynn & Routhieux, 44 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS 1999; Shallice & Burgess, 1991; Shapiro, Shapiro, Russell & Alper, 1998) and to be very important in ADL (Marsh, Hicks & Landau, 1998; McDaniel & Einstein, 1992; Van den Broek, Downes, Johnson, Dayus, & Hilton, 2000). Prospective memory is known to decline in normal aging (Daum, Graber, Schugens & Mayes, 1996; Maylor, 1995; Rendell & Thomson, 1999). It is not yet known which sub-areas of the frontal lobe might contribute most to prospective memory. Burgess and colleagues (2000) recently reported a major investigation of effects of focal lesions on a multitask laboratory activity. They found that lesions to the left posterior cingulate and forceps major regions gave deficits on “task learning and remembering, plan-following and remembering one’s actions,” but not “planning.” Remembering task contingencies after a delay was also affected by lesions in the region of the left anterior cingulate, and rule-breaking and failures of task switching were additionally found in people with lesions affecting the medial and more polar aspects of Brodmann’s areas 8, 9 and especially 10. Planning deficits were associated with lesions to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. They distinguished “planning” from “prospective memory,” and considered that the brain basis of each is distinct. Working memory has been identified as an important component of ADL (Carlson, Fried, Xue, Bandeen-Roche, Zeger & Brandt, 1999). One particularly elegant conceptualization of working memory is the central executive system (CES). The CES is a high order cognitive frontal lobe module which manages multiple complex mental operations under time pressure, and which is subserved by hemispherically specialized slave systems, namely a left hemisphere phonological loop system and a right hemisphere visuospatial scratchpad (Baddeley, 1986; Baddeley & Della Sala, 1996; D’Esposito, Detre, Alsop, Shin, Atlas & Grossman, 1995; Smith & Jonides, 1997). Tasks purported to solicit one or the other slave system are considered CES tasks when they are required to be carried out simultaneously under time pressure (dual task paradigm) (Baddeley & Della Sala, 1996; Baddeley, Della Sala, Papagno & Spinnler, 1997; Della Sala, Baddeley, Papagno & Spinnler, 1995). The CES has been demonstrated to lodge principally in the frontal lobes (Baddeley et al., 1997; Collette et al., 1999). The CES manifests marked decline in normal aging (Spinnler, Della Sala, Bandera & Baddeley, 1988). Unfortunately, we have not been able to locate any reports of research on the link between the CES and ADL in brain damaged patients, but the idea has indeed been articulated (Stablum, Umilta, Mogentale, Carlan & Guerrini, 2000). The findings of the present research indirectly support the notion of a decline of the CES in normal aging. In par- Godbout et al. 45 ticular, the meal preparation task can be construed to resemble a CES task: it involves multitasking under time pressure. Recall that the old old group was significantly slower than the other two groups on the ADL task. In short, there is a frontal-executive decline in normal aging, but script recitation tasks for routine activities and routine ADL under-represent this frontal-executive impairment, even though they draw heavily upon it, probably because complex routine mental representations and everyday activities continue to be learned and consolidated (a sort of proceduralization and automatisation, an increasing easiness or unhesitatingness) even into senescence. Eventually, however, the frontal-executive deterioration of senescence attacks the least routine and least automatized aspects of such tasks and activities: they make their execution less strategic, less adaptable, on the one hand, and they make it difficult for the senescent participants to handle complex multitasking under time constraint, i.e., in working memory. CONCLUSION Frontal lobe deterioration is an important aspect of normal aging; it is reflected in neuropsychological tests of executive function, especially those most demanding–working memory. Activities of daily living (even complex ones), however, are protected from this decline by deep proceduralization and daily practice, but eventually cannot escape being compromised by it. REFERENCES Acker, M.B.A. (1990). Review of the ecological validity of neuropsychological tests. In D.E. Tupper & K.D. Cicerone (Eds.), The neuropsychology of everyday life: Assessment and basic competencies (pp. 19-55). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Arthur, G. (1947). A point scale performance tests revised form II. New York: The Psychological Corporation. Baddeley, A. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Baddeley, A., & Della Sala, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society of London Biological Science, 351, 1397-1404. Baddeley, A., Della Sala, S., Papagno, C., & Spinnler, H. (1997). Dual-task performance in dysexecutive and nondysexecutive patients with a frontal lesion. Neuropsychology, 11, 187-194. 46 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS Baillargeon, J. (1994). Adaptation française du “Test 2 et 7 de Ruff.” Unpublished manuscript, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières. Bassett, S.S., & Folstein, M.F. (1991). Cognitive impairment and functional disability in absence of psychiatric diagnosis. Psychological Medicine, 21, 77-84. Bell-McGinty, S., Podell, K., Franzen, M, Baird, A.D., & Williams, M.J. (2002). Standard measures of executive function in predicting instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. International Journal of the Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 828-834. Bisiacchi, P.S. (1996). The neuropsychological approach in the study of prospective memory. In M. Brandimonte, G.O. Einstein, & M.A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 297-318). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Bourassa, J. (2000). Troubles dans l’élaboration des schémas cognitifs au cours du vieillissement normal. Unpublished master’s thesis, Université du Québec à TroisRivières. Bourassa, J., & Godbout, L. (1996). Trouble dans l’élaboration des schémas cognitifs lors du vieillissement normal. XIXe Congrès de la Société Québécoise pour la Recherche en Psychologie, Trois-Rivières, Canada. Broadbent, D.E., Cooper, P.F., FitzGerald, P., & Parkes, K.R. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 1-16. Burgess, P.W. (2000). Strategy application disorder: The role of the frontal lobes in human multitasking. Psychological Research, 63, 279-288. Burgess, P.W., Veitch, E., de Lacy-Costello, A., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38, 848-863. Cahn-Weiner, D.A., Malloy, P.F., Boyle, P.A., Marran, M., & Salloway, S. (2000). Prediction of functional status from neuropsychological tests in community-dwelling elderly individuals. Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 187-195. Carlson, M.C., Fried, L.P., Xue, Q.L., Bandeen-Roche, K., Zeger, S.L., & Brandt, J. (1999). Association between executive attention and physical functional performance in community-dwelling older women. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54, S262-270. Cockburn, J. (1995). Task interruption in prospective memory: A frontal lobe function? Cortex, 31, 87-97. Collette, F., Salmon, E., Van der Linden, M., Chicherio, C., Belleville, S., Degueldre, C., Delfiore, G., & Franck, G. (1999). Regional brain activity during tasks devoted to the central executive of working memory. Brain Research [Cognitive Brain Research], 7, 411-417. Corey-Bloom, J., Wiederholt, W.C., Edelstein, S., Salmon, D.P., Cahn, D., & BarrettConnor, E. (1996). Cognitive and functional status of the oldest old. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 44, 671-674. Cowell, P.E., Turetsky, B.I., Gur, R.C., & Grossman, R.I. (1994). Sex differences in aging of the human frontal and temporal lobes. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 4748-4755. Craik, F.I.M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits: The role of attentional resources. In F.I.M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (Vol. 8, pp. 191-211). New York: Plenum Press. Godbout et al. 47 Crawford, S., & Channon, S. (2002). Dissociation between performance on abstract tests of executive function and problem solving in real-life-type situations in normal aging. Aging and Mental Health, 6, 12-21. D’Esposito, M., Detre, J.A., Alsop, D.C., Shin, R.K., Atlas, S., & Grossman, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature, 378, 279-281. Daigneault, S., Braun, C.M.J. & Whitaker, H.A. (1991). Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non-perseverative prefrontal measures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 99-114. Daum, I., Graber, S., Schugens, M.M., & Mayes, A.R. (1996). Memory dysfunction of the frontal type in normal aging. Neuroreport, 7, 2625-2628. De Luca, C.R., Wood, S.J., Anderson, V., Buchanan, J.A., Proffitt, T.M., Mahony, K., & Pantelis, C. (2003). Normative data from the CANTAB. I: Development of executive function over the lifespan. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 242-254. Debettignles, B.H., Swihart, A.A., Green, L.A., & Pirozzolo, F.J. (1997). The neuropsychology of normal aging and dementia: An introduction. In A.M.N. Horton, D. Wedding, & J. Webster (Eds.), The neuropsychology handbook (pp. 173-210). New York: Springer. Della Sala, S., Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Spinnler, H. (1995). Dual-task paradigm: A means to examine the central executive system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 161-171. Demura, S., Sato, S., Minami, M., & Kasuga, K. (2003). Gender and age differences in basic ADL ability on the elderly: Comparison between the independent and the dependent elderly. Journal of Physiology, Anthropology and Applied Human Science, 22, 19-27. Femia, E. E., Zarit, S. H., & Johansson, B. (1997). Predicting change in activities of daily living: A longitudinal study of the oldest old in Sweden. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences, 52B(6), 294-302. Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A practical method of grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Fortin, S., Godbout, L., & Braun, C.M.J. (2003). A test of Shallice’s and Grafman’s neuropsychological models of executive functions with head trauma patients performing activities of daily living. Cortex, 39, 273-91. Godbout, L., Cloutier, P., Bouchard, C., Braun, C.M.J., & Gagnon, S. (2004). Script generation following frontal and parietal lesions. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology (in press). Godbout, L., Doucet, C., & Fiola, M. (2000). The scripting of activities of daily living in normal .aging: Anticipation and shifting deficits with preservation of sequencing. Brain and Cognition, 43, 220-224. Godbout, L., Grenier, M.-C., Braun, C.M.J. & Gagnon, S. (2004). Cognitive structure of executive deficits in patients with frontal lesions performing activities of daily living. Brain Injury (in press). 48 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS Godbout, L., & Bouchard, C. (1997). Semantic memory and aging: Is it more difficult to organize information in time or in space? Seventh Annual Conference, Rotman Research Institute. Toronto, Canada. Godbout, L., Cloutier, P., Bouchard, C., Braun, C.M.J. & Gagnon, S. (2003). Script generation following frontal and parietal lesions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (in press). Godbout, L., & Bouchard, C. (1999). Processing time and space components of semantic memory: A study of frontal-lobe related impairments. Brain & Cognition, 40, 136-139. Godbout, L., & Doyon, J. (1995). Mental representation of knowledge following frontal-lobe or postrolandic lesions. Neuropsychologia, 33(12), 1671-1696. Godbout, L., & Soucy, M.-J. (1996). Trouble dans l’élaboration des schémas cognitifs lors du vieillissement normal. XIXe Congrès de la Société Québécoise pour la Recherche en Psychologie. Trois-Rivières, Québec. Godefroy, O., Cabaret, M., Petit-Chenal, V., Pruvo, J.P., & Rousseaux, M. (1999). Control functions of the frontal lobes. Modularity of the central-supervisory system? Cortex, 35, 1-20. Goel, V., Grafman, J., Tajik, J., Gana, S., & Danto, D. (1997). A study of the performance of patients with frontal lobe lesions in a financial planning task. Brain, 120, 1805-1822. Grafman, J. (1989). Plans, actions and mental sets: Managerial knowledge units in the frontal lobes. In Perecman, E. (Ed.), Integrating theory and practice in clinical neuropsychology. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Ch. 4, pp. 93-138. Grafman, J., Sirigu, A., Spector, L., & Hendler, J. (1993). Damage to the prefrontal cortex leads to decomposition of structured event complexes. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8, 73-87. Grigsby, J., Kaye, K., Baxter, J., Shetterly, S.M., & Hamman, R.F. (1998). Executive cognitive abilities and functional status among community-dwelling older persons in the San Luis Valley Health and Aging Study. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 46, 590-596. Light, L.L., & Anderson, A.P. (1983). Memory for scripts in young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 11, 435-444. Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books. Marsh, R.L., Hicks, J.L., & Landau, J.D. (1998). An investigation of everyday prospective memory. Memory and Cognition, 26, 633-643. Matsuda, H., Ohnishi, T., Asada, T., Li, Z.J., Kanetaka, H., Imabayashi, E., Tanaka, F., & Nakano, S. (2003). Correction for partial-volume effects on brain perfusion SPECT in healthy men. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 44, 1243-1252. Maylor, E.A. (1995). Prospective memory in normal ageing and dementia. Neurocase, 1, 285-289. Mazaux, J.M., Masson, F., Levin, H.S., Maurette, P., & Barat, M. (1997). Long-term neuropsychological outcome and loss of social autonomy after traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 78, 1316-1320. McDaniel, M.A., & Einstein, G.O. (1992). Aging and prospective memory: Basic findings and practical applications. Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities, 1, 23-41. Godbout et al. 49 McDaniel, M.A., Glisky, E.L., Guynn, M.J, & Routhieaux, B.C. (1999). Prospective memory: A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 13, 103-110. Mittenberg, W., Seidenberg, M., O’Leary, D. S., & DiGiulio, D. V. (1989). Changes in cerebral functioning associated with normal aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 918-932. Moane, G. (1993). Dependency and caring needs among the elderly. Irish Journal of Psychology, 14, 189-203. Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Yamadori, A., Kawashima, R., Tsukiura, T., & Fukatsu, R. (1998). Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: Evidence from a PET study in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 253, 127-130. O’Sullivan, M., Jones, D.K., Summers, P.E., Morris, R.G., Williams, S.C., & Markus, H.S. (2001). Evidence for cortical “disconnection” as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology, 57, 632-638. Plude, D.J., & Hoyer, W.J. (1986). Age and the selectivity of visual information processing. Journal of Psychology and Aging, 1, 4-10. Rabbitt, P. (1996). Speed of processing and ageing In R.T. Woods (Ed.), Handbook of the clinical psychology of ageing. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Rabbitt, P., & Lowe, C. (2000). Patterns of cognitive ageing. Psychological Research, 63, 308-316. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychological Press. Roman, M., Brownell, H. H., Potter, H. H., & Seibold, M. S. (1987). Script knowledge in right hemisphere-damaged and in normal elderly adults. Brain & Language, 31, 151-170. Rosen, V.M., Caplan, L., Sheesley, L., Rodriguez, Rr., & Grafman, J. (2003). An examination of daily activities and their scripts across the adult lifespan. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 35, 32-48. Royall, D.R., Chiodo, L.K., & Polk, M.J. (2000). Correlates of disability among elderly retirees with “subclinical” cognitive impairment. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 55, M541-546. Ruff, R.M., & Allen, C.C. (1996). Ruff 2 and 7 selective attention test, professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. Schretlen, D., Pearlson, G.D., Anthony, J.C., Aylward, E.H., Augustine, AM., Davis, A., & Barta, P. (2000). Elucidating the contributions of processing speed, executive ability, and frontal lobe volume to normal age-related differences in fluid intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 52-61. Schwartz, M.F., Montgomery, M.W., Buxbaum, L.J., Lee, S.S., Carew, T.G., & Coslett, H.B. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13-28. Schwartz, M.F., Reed, E.S., Montgomery, M., Palmer, C., & Mayer, N.H. (1991). The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 381-414. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199-209. Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 50 PHYSICAL & OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY IN GERIATRICS Shallice, T., & Burgess, P.W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727-741. Shapiro, J.K, Shapiro, L.R., Russell, S., & Alper, J.S. (1998). A role for executive processes in prospective remembering. Brain & Cognition, 37, 175-177. Smith, E.E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5-42. Spinnler, H., Della Sala, S., Bandera, R., & Baddeley, A. (1988). Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 193-211. Stablum, F., Umilta, C., Mogentale, C., Carlan, M., & Guerrini, C. (2000). Rehabilitation of executive deficits in closed head injury and anterior communicating artery aneurysm patients. Psychological Research, 63, 265-278. Steen, G., Sonn, U., Hanson, A.B., & Steen, B. (2001). Cognitive function and functional ability. A cross-sectional and longitudinal study at ages 85 and 95 in a non-demented population. Aging (Milano), 13, 68-77. Stuss, D.T., & Alexander, M.P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63, 289-298. Teng, E. L., & Chui, H. C. (1987). The modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 314-318. Thuillard, F., & Assal, G. (1991). Données neuropsychologiques chez le sujet âgé normal. In M. Habib, Y. Joanette, & M. Puel (Eds.), Démences et syndromes démentiels: Approche neuropsychologique (pp. 125-133). Paris: Masson. Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, T. G. (1962). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: Science Research Associates. Vakil, E., & Agmon-Ashkenazi, D. (1997). Baseline performance and learning rate of procedural and declarative memory tasks: Younger versus older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series-B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 52B, 229-234. Van den Broek, M.D., Downes, J., Johnson, Z., Dayus, B., & Hilton, N. (2000). Evaluation of an electronic memory aid in the neuropsychological rehabilitation of prospective memory deficits. Brain Injury, 14, 455-462. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised manual. New York: Psychological Corporation. Wilkins, A.J., & Baddeley, A.D. (1978). Remembering to recall in everyday life: An approach to absentmindedness. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (vol. 2, pp. 27-34). Chichester: Wiley. Willis, S. L. (1996). Everyday cognitive competence in elderly persons: Conceptual issues and empirical findings. The Gerontologist, 36, 595-601.