Download Living with males

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Behavioral ecology wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Södertörns högskola | Institutionen för Livsvetenskaper
Magisteruppsats 30 hp | Biologi | Vårterminen 2009
Living with males
– benefits and costs to females of resident males
in Colobus vellerosus
Av: Johanna Hedlund
Handledare: Tomas Bollner och Eva Wikberg
2
Living with males
- benefits and costs to females of resident males in Colobus vellerosus
Johanna Hedlund
Supervisors: Ewa Wikberg, Tomas Bollner
3
ABSTRACT
Only in primates is permanent male-female association the most widespread social
structure of all. The continuous presence of resident males in the social group can have
significant impacts on female fitness, both in forms of costs and benefits. In this study I
investigate particular short-term benefits and costs of resident males to females in a
population of ursine colobus (Colobus vellerosus). I hypothesise that for females
permanent association with males result in certain benefits and certain costs, exceeding
those provided or imposed by other females. The results indicate that female derive
greater benefits from males than from females during intergroup encounters and in the
form of vigilance since males were the main participants in intergroup encounter and
were more vigilant than females. I could not confirm any type of behaviour employed by
resident males that is costly to females. However, the rarity and subtleness of some costly
male behaviours imply that more data is needed before making a conclusion on their
absence or occurrence in this population and I purpose that herding behaviour could
occur at my study site. Moreover, multi-male groups (MM-groups) showed higher rates
of vigilance than single-male groups (SM-groups) and had a tendency to experiencing
fewer intergroup encounters than SM-groups. I interpret the former as a result of the
demanding social conditions in the MM-groups. The latter indicate that females may
benefit from MM-group living through a decrease in intergroup encounters.
4
INTRODUCTION
Almost all primate taxa live in groups and the reproductive strategies of females is considered to
be the main determinant of primate gregariousness and the predominant factor that shapes
primate social system as a whole (Emlen and Oring, 1977; Wrangham, 1980). While females are
believed to be the founders of groups, aggregating in order to optimize resource utilization and to
gain protection, male reproductive success is suggested to be most dependant on mating
opportunities (Trivers, 1972; Smuts et al, 1987; van Schaik, 1996) resulting in a male allocation
primarily based on the distribution of females (Altman, 1990). Unlike most group living
mammals, male and female primates have a social relationship which make them truly unusually.
Only in primates is permanent male-female association the most widespread social structure of
all, reaching almost 100% in diurnal species (Smuts et al, 1987). The continuous presence of
resident males in the social group has a significant impact on female fitness, resulting in
situations that alternately are costly or beneficiary. Sexual coercion, aggression and resource
competition (Wrangham, 1980; van Schaik, 1983; Dunbar, 1988; Smuts and Smuts, 1993) are
examples when males may impose a cost on females and protection against infanticide, defence
against predators and enhanced competitive ability of the group (Wrangham, 1980; Robinson,
1988; van Schaik, 1989; Nunn, 2000) are examples when males may provide a benefit to females.
Of the numerous types of costs resident primate males can place on females sexual coercion is
perhaps the most conspicuous. Smuts and Smuts (1993) defined sexual coercion as the force or
intimidation used by a male to increase the likelihood that a female will mate with him and to
decrease the prospect that she will mate with other males. For females costs associated with
sexual coercion include deprivation of active mate choice by forced copulations (Smuts and
Smuts, 1993; Muller et al, 2007), inhibition of promiscuity and free migration brought on by
herding (Watts, 1989; Smuts and Smuts, 1993; Steenbeek, 1999) and injuries and fatal wounds
caused by physical punishment for promiscuity (Goodall, 1986). There are also stress-related
effects of male sexual harassment and coercion; abortion (Pireira, 1983), disruption of oestrus
cycle (Goodall, 1986), increased glucocorticoid secretion (Muller et al, 2007) and temporary
infertility (Dunbar, 1987). However the predominant and most well-studied form of sexual
coercion is infanticide, the killing of unweaned young by conspecifics (Wrangham, 1979; Smuts
and Smuts, 1993; Borries et al, 1999; Kappeler, 1999). Infanticide constitute a enormous cost to
females and the risk of being exposed to this form of male behaviour can have a substantial
impact on female behaviour (Palombit 1999, Steenbeek 1999), but notably, it is not a behaviour
typically exhibited by resident males (Struhsaker and Leland, 1987).
Other cost to females of a continuous association with males is resource competition. Males
may divert resources that would otherwise accrue to females and their relatives and male presence
in the group increases group size and thereby possibly also resource competition (Wrangham,
1980; van Schaik, 1983; Dunbar, 1988; O’Brien, 1991). In white-faced capuchins as much as
75% of all male-female agonism takes place during feeding (Rose, 1994). Competition over food
also exists between females, but because of sexual dimorphism in size, strength and weaponry
males are the dominant sex in the majority of primate species and females are assigned a
disadvantageous position when competing with the opposite sex, thus suffering higher costs in
food competition with males (Crook and Gartland, 1966; Smuts, 1987; Rose, 1994; Strier, 1994;
Campbell, 2003).
The risk of receiving aggression from males may oblige females to maintain vigilance towards
potentially dangerous males (Smuts and Smuts, 1993; Alberts, 1994, Kutsukake 2007). Caine and
Marra (1988) suggested that a high demand for social within-group surveillance may handicap
individuals with respect to other types of vigilance, i.e. vigilance aimed at the detection of
predators. Thus in species where females are compelled to engage in a high frequency of social
surveillance, either directed towards males or indeed also other females, they may suffer costs of
decreased vigilance directed towards predator detection (Caine and Marra, 1988) and also loose
time and energy otherwise spent (Smuts and Smuts, 1993).
Balancing the disadvantages females may suffer from permanent male presence in the group
are the many possible and important benefits males also provide. For instance, resident males can
constitute a defence system against intrusion of infanticidal males and in species where
5
infanticide is a major threat female primates are known to associate with particular males for
offspring protection (Wrangham, 1979; Watts, 1989; O’Brien, 1991; Smuts and Smuts, 1993;
Sterck et al, 1997; Fashing, 2001; Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004; van Schaik et al, 2004; van
Schaik, 1996). In one newly documented case, Vogel and Fuentes-Jiménez (2006) observed a
wedge-capped capuchin male, in spite of high costs to himself, rescue an infant and its mother
from an attack by strange males.
Furthermore females can benefit from of having more than one male in their group. Infanticide
is expected to be less likely in a group containing multiple reproductively active males (MMgroup) since potentially infanticidal intruders face several competitors, of whom all are possible
sires of the infants and consequently will protect them and resist take-over (of course this
assumes promiscuous matings in multi-male groups) (Hrdy, 1979; Struhsaker and Leland, 1987;
van Schaik, 1996; Altmann, 1990; Janson and van Schaik, 2000; Borries et al, 1999).
Accordingly, two case studies have shown that infanticide risk is greater in single-male groups
(SM) than in MM-groups (Hanuman langurs: Newton, 1986; mountain gorillas: Robbins, 1995).
Numerous reports confirm that primate males take greater responsibility for anti-predatory
tactics than primate females, thus suggesting that continuous male presence in the group can
decrease female mortality and consequently have a substantial beneficiary influence on female
fitness (Wrangham, 1979, 1980; van Schaik, 1989). For example, there are reports of male group
members being the first to confront predators (white-fronted capuchin and brown capuchin: van
Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; vervet monkey: Baldellou and Henzi, 1992), the last to leave if
the troop turns to flee (white-fronted capuchin and brown capuchin: van Schaik and van
Noordwijk, 1989; Phayre's leaf monkey: Lloyd et al, 2006) and the most prominent in attacking,
mobbing or chasing predators off (chacma baboon: Busse, 1980; white-fronted capuchin and
brown capuchin: van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; spectral tarsier: Gursky, 2005; red
colobus: Stanford, 2002). Also, in many species, males are more vigilant than females (de Ruiter,
1986; van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; Baldellou and Henzi, 1992; Rose, 1994; Rose and
Fedigan, 1995, Gould et al, 1997) and since there is a strong correlation between vigilance and
predator detection (Caine, 1984; de Ruiter, 1986; Boinski, 1988; Caine and Marra, 1988; van
Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; Fragaszy, 1990; Rose, 1994), male’s greater vigilance make
them likely to detect predators sooner than females and thereby they may reduce the burden of
staying alert for female group members (Eisenberg et al, 1972; van Schaik and van Noordwijk,
1989; Artiss and Martin, 1995). Furthermore, it is expected that the positive influence males may
have on female safety lead to a covariance between threat increase and male group numbers, i.e.
where predation risk is high groups should contain multiple males (Cheney and Wrangham, 1987;
van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989). This potential connection was investigate and confirmed
by Anderson (1986) and van Schiak and van Höstermann (1994) who preformed comparisons of
predation pressure and male group numbers across taxa and found that where primate groups
suffered from strong predation pressure they contained a lager number of males.
I will conclude this basic outline of benefits and costs of primate males to females by looking
in to one final benefit males can provide females; competition potential. Because female
reproductive success depends largely on access to food (Trivers, 1972; van Schaik, 1989),
acquiring this resource is of utmost importance to female fitness. Whenever food is limited and,
depending on the distribution and quality of it, females will compete within the group or between
groups to increase their access to it. Intergroup encounters are situations that can act as means of
contest competition over resources between neighbouring groups (Wrangham, 1979, 1980).
Females are known to participate aggressively in these interactions (guereza: Fashing, 2001;
ursine colobus: Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004; western black-and-white-colobus: Korstjens et al,
2002), i.e. for the reason of female resource defence (Cheney, 1987; Fashing, 2001), but males
are usually the most active participators (guereza: Oates, 1977; Fashing 2001; Harris 2006; ursine
colobus: Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004). Since male fitness is governed by mating opportunities
not food (Trivers, 1972), males are assumed to engage in intergroup encounters for other reasons
than females, principally mate defence (Cheney, 1987). But by excluding non-group conspecifics,
either through direct mate defence (driving rival males away) (Cheney, 1987) or indirect mate
defence (defending the groups resources or territory and thereby attracting females, e. g. resource
defence polygyny) (Emlen and Oring, 1977), males provide female group members with benefits
6
of increased resource access (Wrangham, 1980; van Schaik, 1989; O’Brien, 1991) and ultimately
increase female reproductive success.
In this study I will investigate the particular short-term benefits and costs that males place on
females in a wild population of ursine colobus (Colobus vellerosus) (Fig. 1b) at Boabeng-Fiema
Monkey Sanctuary (BFMS) in central Ghana. I hypothesis that; I) For females permanent
association with males result in certain benefits, exceeding those provided by other females; and
II) For females permanent association with males result in certain costs, exceeding those imposed
by other females.
The predictions associated with hypotheses I are; a) males are more vigilant than females,
thereby relieving females of the burden of vigilance (van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; Rose,
1994) ; b) females will have a tendency to scan less when males are in proximity than when other
female group members are in proximity, suggesting that females rely more on male vigilance than
on female vigilance (van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989); c) males are more active than
females in intergroup encounters thus contributing more than females in between-group
competition (van Schaik 1989; O’Brien 1991).
The predictions associated with hypotheses II are; a) during foraging females engage in
aggressive interaction more often with males than with other females, suggesting that there is a
greater resource competition between the sexes than within (Rose 1994); b) males will be
aggressive towards female group members while females will tend to direct submissive signals
towards males, suggesting that females are disadvantaged in antagonistic encounters with male
group members (Rose, 1994; Smuts, 1987; Campbell, 2003), c) female foraging is more often
interrupted by males than by other females, suggesting that female feeding success suffers in
male presence, d) female foraging success decreases with increased time spent in close vicinity to
males, implying that males inflict a negative influence on female foraging (O’Brien 1991).
In addition, since BFMS is an unusual study site in the sense that both MM and SM-groups
exists within the same population, I will also examine if group type (i.e. the presence of one or
several adult resident males in the group) can explain any observed difference between the groups
in certain behaviours, namely vigilance, intergroup interaction and male aggression. As
mentioned in the sections above, several researchers have found that the number of males per
group influence a wide range of behaviours, conditions and phenomenon, such as female
reproductive rate (Dunbar, 1987), infanticide risk (van Schaik, 1996), vigilance rates (Rose and
Fedigan, 1995) and group competition potential (Wallace, 2007) to name a few. Consequently, I
find group type an interesting additional parameter worth investigating in the context of benefits
and costs of males to females.
MATERIAL & METHODS
Study site
This study was conducted at the Boabeng-Fiema Monkey Sanctuary (BFMS) in the Nkoranza
District in the Brong-Ahafo region, central Ghana (7° 43’N and 1° 42’W) (Fig. 1a). BFMS
encompasses 192ha of dry semi-deciduous forest in the forest-savanna transition zone in western
Africa. The sanctuary lies on the border of the small twin-community of Boabeng-Fiema
(Teichroeb et al 2008) and is surrounded by farmland separating it from the nearest large forest
block by 50km (Beier et al, 2002). However, there are riparian forest strips and patches of
secondary forest that links BFMS with surrounding smaller forest fragments (Wong and Sicotte
2006).
BFMS has two species of monkey; ursine colobus (Colobus vellerosus) and mona monkey
(Cercopithecus campbeli). C. vellerosus is also present in the forest fragments surrounding BFMS
and it is believed that migration occur between them (Kankam, 1997; Wong and Sicotte, 2006).
BFMS is a community-based conservation effort, founded in 1974 when the twin-community
of Boabeng and Fiema passed a District Council by-law prohibiting harm to the monkeys in the
area (Ghana Forestry Commission; Saj et al, 2005). Within the sanctuary it is not allowed to fell
7
a)
b)
Fig 1 (a). A map showing Ghana’s geographical position in West Africa and the location of BoabengFiema Monkey Sanctuary (BFMS) in relation to the country’s largest cities, Kumasi and Accra, the capitol.
(b). A young female of C. vellerosus. Photo: Johanna Hedlund
large live trees but the hunting ban only incorporates the two species of monkey and villagers use
the forest for medicinal plants, to graze sheep and to collect fire wood. The presence of exotic
plant species and patches of regenerating farm land, the rarity of large predators and
encroachment along the edges of the forest all bear witness to the long-term anthropogenic
influence in the area (Teichroeb et al, 2008; Saj et al, 2005).
Study Species
Ursine colobus (Fig.1b) is an arboreal folivore with a diet consisting mainly of young and mature
leaves, though seeds constitute a significant proportion when available (Saj and Sicotte, 2007a).
The species is one of five representatives of the genus Colobus, of which all are generally
restricted to the tropical rainforests of Africa (Struhsaker and Leland, 1987; Oates, 1994). C.
vellerosus is the least known of all black and white colobus species (Sicotte and Macintosh,
2004).
Throughout most of its range in, C. vellerosus seems to be declining and the species is listed as
Vulnerable by IUCN (2007). However, BFMS appears to be an exception; here the population
has been growing in the past 15 years and increasing as much as 78,9% between 1991 and 2003
(Wong and Sicotte, 2006; Saj et al, 2005). According to the latest population census of C.
vellerosus at BFMS conducted in 2003, the sanctuary inhabits 217-241 individuals in 15 groups
(Wong and Sicotte, 2006). There is considerable variation (4-32) in group size in the area (Saj et
al., 2004) and groups are both uni- and multi-male (Saj et al, 2005), with female transfer known
to occur (Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004).
Intergroup encounters are frequent and often aggressive at BFMS and home ranges are small
and overlap with neighbouring groups’ (Sicotte and MacIntosh, 2004). Incidents of infanticide,
male take-over, extra-group matings and occurrence of all-male-bands have been documented in
the population. Female dispersal is presumably facultative and individuals are known to stay and
breed in their natal groups (Saj and Sicotte, 2005; Teichroeb et al, 2005; Saj et al, 2007; Sicotte et
al, 2007).
8
Table 1. Sex and age-class composition of study groups.
Group
name
Nippy, NP
Bentesua, BS
Splinter, SP
Red tail, RT
Dadie, DA
Wawa, WW
Odum, OD
Group
type
SM
SM
SM
Mean
MM
MM
MM
MM
Mean
Total group
size
11
11
13
11.7
15
19
20
21
18.8
#
adult ♀
4
4
4
#
subadult ♀
2
2
0
#
adult ♂
1
1
1
#
subadult ♂
0
0
0
#
juv. & infants
4
4
8
6
6
9
5
0
2
1
3
3
4
2
6
0
0
1
1
6
7
7
6
The first column lists all names used for the study groups during data collection in the field. In the data set
the names were shortened to a two-letter code, also seen in the table, and I will only use these codes when
referring to the different groups. Group type refers to the number of males present in the group, SM-groups
being single-male (NP, BS, SP) and MM-groups being multi-male (RT, DA, WW, OD). A mean group size
is presented for both group types (11.7 for SM-groups and 18.8 for MM.groups). Adult denotes individuals
known to be sexually mature, subadult denotes individuals that are fully grown or nearly fully grown and
believed to be sexually mature or very soon sexually mature, juv. denotes juvenile individuals not yet fully
grown nor sexually mature and infants denotes individuals that are not yet fully grown nor sexually mature
and that are very dependant on their mothers. The symbol ♀ represents females and ♂ represents males. No
sex difference is considered for juveniles or infants.
Study Groups
Behavioural data was collected for seven study groups (DA, BS, NP, OD, RT, SP, WW) of C.
vellerosus (Table 1). All groups had been under observation before and three (BS, RT, WW)
since the beginning of Pascal Sicotte’s research at the site in 2000. All groups were more or less
habituated and were not disturbed by human presence as long as a distance of at least 10 meters
between observer and the nearest individual was maintained.
Three of the seven research groups were single-male (NP, BS, SP) and four were multi-male
(RT, DA, WW, OD). The group NP was part of DA in 2006, but split off from DA between
August 2006 and May 2007. The group OD was the least known of the seven groups and was at
the time of this study subject of a major influx of new males. All members of the seven groups
could be identified individually by facial characteristics, size, scars and/or tail shape. In addition
to the seven study groups, three additional groups (SK, VI, BO) could be identified on group
level and were included in the analysis on occasions when they participated in intergroup
encounters with any of the study groups.
Data collection
Data were collected through May to the beginning of August 2007 and observations took place
between sunrise (06:30-07:30) and sunset (17:00-19:00).
Group scans were executed once every hour. Each group scan included the behaviours of five
randomly sampled adult or subadult individuals of the same group. Since there could be a risk of
sampling the same individuals several times in a row if the animals stayed stationary, sampling
changed directionality every other sampling occasion, going left to right or right to left. Some
groups had only one adult male and to avoid low male representation in the data, efforts were
made to include a male in each scan even if this meant diverting from the sampling scheme.
Focal observations of the duration of 10 minutes (Altmann, 1974) were conducted on adult
and subadult females. All exhibited and received behaviours of the focal individual were
documented. Also, for each focal observation five point samples (one every 2.5 minute) were
9
taken, registering the focal animal’s behaviour at that precise moment and the proximities of all
other individuals within 5 meters. Three different proximity categories were used to assign
distance between the focal animal and other individuals; within 1 tail length, within 3 meters and
within 5 meters.
To ensure independence, focal observations on one individual were never collected twice in
one hour and a 10 minute interlude was also used to separate samples of individuals that had been
closer than 1 meter of the last sampled individual. The dense terrain did not allow for a strict
sampling scheme; valuable time would have been lost waiting for the next animal in turn to
appear. However, the aim was to finish all females in one group before resampling any of the
females from that group or before moving on to the next study group.
When considered to be foraging, focal feeding follows (Altmann, 1974) of the duration of 1
minute were conducted on adult and subadult females. All exhibited and received behaviours of
the focal individual were noted as well as the proximity of all other individuals within 5 meters of
the focal animal. Feeding follows were also extracted from focal observations. To be considered a
valid feeding follow for extraction, the foraging period had to include the ingestion of at least 2
items within a one minute period. To ensure independence no more than one feeding follow was
extracted from each focal observation.
Intergroup encounters were documented ad libitum and were defined as occasions where one
or several groups came in 50 meter of the focal group (Oates, 1977; Fashing, 2001; Sicotte and
Macintosh, 2004). Encounters in which none of the involved groups exhibited agonistic
behaviours were excluded from the analysis on sex difference in intergroup behaviour but kept
for calculation on the hourly rate of intergroup encounters per group. Agonistic behaviours were
characterized as low intensity only or high intensity, where low intensity aggression include;
jump display, open mouth display or stiff leg display (Oates, 1977) and high intensity aggression
include; chase or contact aggression (Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004). During encounters,
participants (aggressors and targets) were individually identified as often as possible, but
otherwise categorized as male or female. The hourly rate of intergroup encounters per group was
calculated by dividing the number of intergroup encounters per group with the hours spent
observing the group.
All observations were conducted with binoculars (8x42) and behavioural data was registered
with behaviour codes on hand-held computer (Psion Organizer, model LZ64). During intergroup
encounters or other highly eventful situations observations were documented by tape recorder and
later transcribed into behaviour codes.
Data Analysis
Data on vigilance behaviour was extracted from group scan observations and expressed as
occasions of scans versus occasions of other observed behaviours and tested for sex, group and
group type (SM or MM) differences using a generalized linear model.
Differences in female vigilance depending on the sex of a neighbouring individual was
extrapolated from two different data localities; point samples and focal follows and both
calculations were done using a linear mixed-effects model. Data from point samples were
expressed as occasions of scans versus occasions of other observed behaviours when the focal
animal was alone or in the vicinity of males or other females. Three proximity classes were used
to assign vicinity of neighbours: within 1 meters, within 3 meters and within 5 meters. Data from
focal follows were expressed as hourly rate of scanning per individual, i.e. proportion of time
spent vigilant out of the total observation time with a female or a male neighbour or any
neighbour irrespective of sex.
Intergroup encounters were divided into low intensity and high intensity classes. Sex
differences in participation was tested for all encounters as well as for the two intensity
categories, using Fisher’s exact test. Participation differences between groups and group types
(SM or MM) were also tested using Fisher’s exact test.
Since no occasions of male aggression, female submissive signals or male interruption of
female feeding was witnessed, data analyses on these predictions were not needed.
10
Differences in foraging success when the focal was in the vicinity of males or females or alone
were documented in two ways; foraging success and feeding efficiency and tested using linear
mixed-effects model. Foraging success was expressed as proportion of foraging time spent
ingesting food and feeding efficiency was expressed as ingested items per minute. No distinction
on the type of item (fruit, leaf or seed) ingested was made and neither on the quality (young or
mature leaf).
All statistical analyses were done in the statistical computer program R and statistical
significance was set at the 5% level.
RESULTS
In total, 478 observation hours and 63 days were spent in the field. I collected 40.5 hours of focal
data (N = 243), 83 (39 + 44) feeding follows and observed 49 intergroup encounters. Mean
observation time per study group was 68 hours, but varied between groups (Table 2).
Table 2. Observation time spent per group. Number (#) of days spent collecting data per group and the total amount (#)
of hours spent collecting data per group. Approximately 14 days and 68 hours were spent observing each group.
Groups
NP
BS
SP
RT
DA
WW
OD
mean
# of days / group
# of hours / group
16
77
9
41
17
68
11
61
14
105
23
87
9
34
14
68
I) Benefits of males to females
Vigilance.
A comparison between female and male vigilance level showed a significant difference in the
proportion of vigilance behaviour of each sex. Males were considerably more vigilant than
females (P<0.001). However, an intragroup comparison revealed a more variable situation. In
two of the seven study groups, one being MM (DA) and one SM (NP), males were significantly
more vigilant than females (P<0.05 resp. P<0.001), but in the remaining five the distinction was
no longer statistically different even if male vigilance showed a tendency to exceed female
vigilance in all but one (BS) group (Fig.2). There was no significant difference in vigilance
between the seven groups when all adult and subadult individuals were considered, and likewise,
no significant difference in vigilance between the different groups’ females or between the
different groups’ males. However, when comparing MM and SM-groups there was a significant
difference in vigilance; members of MM-groups scanned more than individuals in SM-groups
(P< 0.05). However, the difference disappeared in a sex dependent analysis; i.e there was no
difference in vigilance between females from MM groups and females from SM groups or males
from MM groups and males from SM-groups.
Vigilance and neighbours
Results from focal follows and point samples were unanimous. There was no statistical difference
in the amount of time a female spent scanning depending on the sex of her neighbours. However,
if no regard was taken to the sex of the neighbour, females spent significantly more time scanning
alone (P<0.05) than when in company of another group member. Data was not comprehensive
enough to allow for an analysis on group type differences.
Intergroup encounters
A total of 49 intergroup encounters were documented and all were used for calculating the hourly
rate of intergroup encounters experienced by each study group. SP had the highest frequency of
Proportion (%) of vigilant behaviour
11
Fig 2. Sex and group differences in the proportion (%) of vigilant behaviour observed during group scans. Percentages
represent vigilance in relation to all other types of behaviour sampled. All groups are represented with one bar for each
sex (♀ = females, ♂ = males). In all groups, except BS, males showed a higher percentage of vigilant behaviour than
females.
intergroup encounters (0.265 encounters per hour) and DA experienced the lowest frequency
(0.105 enoucnters per hour) (Table 3). MM-groups generally experienced fewer intergroups
encounters than SM-groups. The hourly rate of intergroup encounters for MM-groups was 0.157
compared to 0.223 for SM-groups, however, the difference was not statistically significant.
For the analysis on sex differences in intergroup behaviour 12 of the 49 observed cases of
intergroup encounters hade to be removed since they continued without any indication of
particular intergroup related behaviours or aggressiveness and an additional 8 cases hade to be
Table 3. The hourly rate of intergroup encounters per group. The approximate hourly rate of intergroup encounters
(IGE) was calculated by dividing the number of IGE per group with the observation hours (Table 2) per group. The
mean hourly rate of IGE for SM-groups (NP, BS, SP) was 0.223 and the mean hourly rate of IGE for MM-groups (RT,
DA, WW, OD) was 0.157. There was no statistically significant difference between the mean hourly rate of IGE of SM
and MM-groups. Note that the IGE presented in this table (N=49) are not fully equivalent to the encounters (N=29)
used for the analysis of sex differences in intergroup behaviour (see Table 4).
Groups
NP
# of IGE / group
16
Hourly rate of IGE / group
0.208
Hourly rate of IGE for SM-groups
Hourly rate of IGE for MM-groups
BS
8
0.195
SP
18
0.265
RT
7
0.115
DA
11
0.105
WW
20
0.230
OD
6
0.176
Mean
0.185
0.223
0.157
excluded because of uncertainties concerning the sex of the participating individuals. Of the
remaining 29 cases 18 were categorized as high intensity encounters and 11 as low intensity
encounters. The result showed that males were more active in intergroup encounters than females.
While males participated in all 29 between-group encounters, females were active in a
significantly lower proportion (P < 0.05) (Table 4). Males were also the most active sex in
intergroup encounters of high intensity, participating with high intensity behaviours in 94.44% of
all cases, significantly more than the 61.11% for females (P < 0.05) (Table 4). Male
predominance prevailed in intergroup encounters of low intensity, where male participation was
100%, against the female participation at 63.63%. However, the difference was not statistically
significant in encounters of low intensity.
12
Table 4. Sex differences in intergroup behaviour. A total of 29 intergroup encounters (IGE) were included in the
analysis on sex differences in intergroup behaviour. The number of IGE with female participants (N = 22, 75.86%)
were significantly fewer (P < 0.05) than the number of IGE in which males participated (N = 29, 100%). A total of 18
encounters were categorized as being of high intensity and of these females participated with high intensity behaviours
in 11 (61.11%) and males in 17 (94.44%). Eleven of the IGE were categorized as being of low intensity and of these
females participated with low intensity behaviours in 7 (63.63%) and males in 11 (100%).
IGE
High intensity IGE
Low intensity IGE
Total
#
29
18
11
Female
participation (#)
22
11
7
Female
participation (%)
75.86
61.11
63.63
Male
participation (#)
29
17
11
Male
participation (%)
100
94.44
100
II) The costs of males to females
Male aggression.
No aggressive interactions between male and female group members were observed. The only
documented form of male aggression towards females occurred when individuals from different
groups intermingled, i.e. during instances of intergroup encounters and male incursions.
Female submissive signals
No observations of female submissive signals toward male group members were observed.
Foraging and male interruption.
The degree of within-group aggression during foraging was very low in all of the seven study
groups and there were no documentation of male interruption of female feeding. However, there
were some rare occasions of female-female agonism in foraging context.
Feedings success and neighbours
There was no statistical indication of a difference in foraging success for females depending on
the sex of the neighbouring group member and neither did the female ingestion rate (items
ingested per hour) differ significantly depending on whether the female was feeding in the
proximity of males or other females.
DISCUSSION
Benefits of males to females
Vigilance
The result of this study confirmed my prediction that ursine colobus males are more vigilant than
females. An intragroup comparison revealed a corresponding situation, where males tended to be
the most vigilant sex in all groups except one, BS-group and since all other groups had noticeably
higher male vigilance BS-group stands out as an inconsistency. One possible explanation for BSgroup’s dissimilarity with the other study groups is insufficient data. I spent a mean observation
time of 68 hours per group, but BS was only observed for 41 hours and it is possible that
additional time spent with this group would generate data that would be more in line with that of
the other groups.
This is the first study on sex difference in vigilance behaviour in a black and white colobus
species. Greater male than female vigilance has been confirmed in several other primate species,
but notably not in many old world species and in very few colobines (New world species: redbacked squirrel monkey (Boinski, 1988); white-fronted capuchin and brown capuchin (van
Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989); wedge-capped capuchin (Fragaszy, 1990); white-faced
capuchin (Rose, 1994; Gould et al, 1997); Old world species: vervet monkey (Baldellou and
13
Henzi, 1992); colobine species: Thomas’s langur (Steenbeek et al, 1999a). Since vigilance
behaviour is positively correlated with predator detection (Caine, 1984; Caine and Marra, 1988;
de Ruiter, 1986; van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; Rose, 1994), this study indicates that
females may derive greater benefits from males than from other female group members in the
form of vigilance and that males may provide superior anti-predatory services compared to
females in this sense. This is particular interesting since predation risk could be the major force
behind gregariousness in primates (Alexander, 1974; van Schaik, 1983; van Schaik and van
Hooff, 1983; Terborgh and Janson, 1986; Dunbar, 1988; Janson, 1992; Sterck et al, 1997). Thus
greater male than female vigilance suggest, as Rose (1994) noted in her study on white-faced
capuchin monkeys, that females derive the greatest benefit of group living from males. However,
it would be precipitated to assume that male vigilance is exclusively, or even primarily, aimed
towards discovering potential predators in light of the fact that vigilance also can serve as withingroup surveillance (Caine and Marra 1988, Alberts 1994, Treves 1999, Jack 2001), a component
of male mate defence (Treves 2000) and to detect conspecific groups (Baldellou and Henzi 1992,
Rose and Fedigan 1995). So even if male vigilance surpasses female vigilance, the male motive
may not be predator detection but enhancing male reproductive success. In the case of the latter,
could male vigilance still be beneficiary for females? Yes and no. Yes since it would be highly
unlikely that a vigilant male scanning for competitors would ignore an approaching predator if he
spotted it and also because strange males are not just competition for the group’s resident male,
they also pose a threat of infanticide. The benefit for females could thus still stand even if the
motive of male vigilance is aimed towards other males. However, if the male in question is a
member of a MM-group, the motive for his vigilance behaviour could be monitoring competitors
within the group. In such circumstances male vigilance would be of lesser value for females,
partly because males already part of the group pose little threat of infanticide and partly because
vigilance used for within-group surveillance takes time from vigilance aimed at external threats,
such as predators (Caine and Marra, 1988). Additionally, on occasions when males are
monitoring competitors for the purpose of mate defence, male vigilance may in fact inhibit
female mate choice and constitute a cost (Treves, 1999). Consequently, the aim of male vigilance
is of significance to primate females; when males effectively spot danger and relieve females
from requirements of vigilance they provide a benefit, but when the purpose of male vigilance is
within-group surveillance or mate defence, it is less valuable to females and could even be costly
to them.
The directionality and purpose of male vigilant behaviour is a noteworthy issue for other
reasons. When I examined group type differences in vigilance I found that when no regard to sex
was taken individuals from MM-groups were statistically more vigilant than SM-group
individuals. This is surprising for two reasons. First, you would expect individuals of MM-groups
to be less vigilant than individuals of SM-groups since the greater number of resident males in
MM-groups means that there are more animals of the more vigilant sex present and that this in
turn should lower requirements on individual vigilance in MM-groups (van Schaik and van
Noordwijk, 1989; Rose and Fedigan, 1995; but see Baldellou and Henzi, 1992). Secondly, at
BFMS all MM-groups were larger in size than the SM-groups which according to the “group size
effect” is another reason to expect a decrease in individual scanning rate in MM-groups (Burger
and Gochfeld, 1992; for review on vigilance group size effect see Bednekoff and Lima, 1998).
However I propose that the tendency for MM-group individuals to be more vigilant than
individuals of SM-groups can be clarified if focusing on the directionality and purpose of
vigilance. When performing a group scan I made a distinction between long-distance scanning
behaviour and close-range scanning behaviour since these two actions could serve different
functions. The function of long-distance scanning / non-social vigilance is presumably predator
detection (or detection of other threats such approaching groups) whereas the function of closerange scanning is food-related or social, for example used for within-group surveillance (Caine
and Marra 1988, Treves 1999, Jack 2001). Subsequently, I only used long-distance scanning in
my analysis on vigilance. Though, in retrospect, it may have been arbitrary to generalise the
function of an individual’s vigilance behaviour based on the distance of the object the animal
seemed to be watching without actually confirming what this object was. There is a possibility
that a certain amount of the vigilance I denoted long-distance in fact could have been caused by
14
social conditions within the group, i.e. even though individuals did not seem to be watching a
group member or food item in close proximity they could still have been looking at another group
member further away. Chapman and Pavelka (2005) have argued that larger groups in black-andwhite colobus (C. guereza) are unstable since an increase in the number of resident males (a
result of the increase of females) cause an increase in aggression. In fact, three of the four MMgroups (but non of the SM groups) in this study experienced intense male-male interactions (DA,
WW) or exceptional male related conditions (OD) that could increase the entire group’s
uneasiness and with it it’s group member’s tendency to be vigilant towards individuals within the
group. DA-group was followed by an extra-group male that on several occasions was engaged in
high-intensity interactions with the group’s resident males, in WW-group one of the two resident
males was repeatedly seen attacking and even wounding the other male and OD-group had within
the previous 8 months acquired 6 new males. Females have good reason for increasing their
vigilance in these conditions since high-intensity male-male competition can induce certain costs
(Chapman and Pavelka, 2005) such as stress (Dunbar, 1987) and the presence of strange males
increase the risk of infanticide (Struhsaker and Leland, 1987; Kappeler, 1999). Additionally, for
males competitive conditions can be a reason to increase alertness towards other male group
members (Baldellou and Henzi, 1992; Treves, 1999). Subsequently I believe that the social
situations could be the reason behind the higher vigilance rates of the individuals of MM-groups.
In another analysis on group type, I found that there was no statistical difference in the vigilance
rates between females or between males when comparing SM and MM-groups. This implies that
neither males nor females reduce their vigilance rates in response to the number of resident males in
the group. This is surprising since males proved to be the most vigilant sex and more male group
members imply lowered requirements on individual vigilance. It is possible that in order to
significantly influence vigilance behaviour in all group members, the number of resident males in
the group needs to be greater than it was in any of my study groups, i.e. >7. This being said, even
though there was no difference in vigilance rates within the sexes depending on group type, there
might still be other vigilance-depending differences between group types, namely the purpose of the
vigilance behaviour. For instance, and as exemplified in the previous section, males of MM-groups
have competitors present continuously and are therefore potentially more exposed to male-male
interactions than males of SM-groups. This might force them to engage in with-group surveillance
more frequently than males of SM-groups (Treves, 1999; Cameron and du Toit, 2005). Conversely,
males of SM-groups might show more vigilance directed towards neighbouring groups than males in
MM-groups because of the slightly greater frequency of inter-group encounters SM-groups
experience (this study, further discussed in the section on intergroup encounters). Hence, the motive
for male vigilance might differ depending on group type and as noted earlier, for females the motive
of male vigilance can be of importance. If SM-group males tend to direct more vigilance outwards
the group than MM-group males, females in SM-groups may derive greater benefits from male
vigilance than females in MM-groups. To be able to distinguish male vigilance directionality and
consequently the motive, a more detailed sampling method than group scans is required, for example
continuous focal sampling of males.
Vigilance and male neighbours
In contrast to my predictions, female vigilance did not differ depending on the sex of the nearest
group member. This contradicts a previous study by van Schaik and van Noordwijk (1989) who
found that vigilance in capuchin monkeys was lower when one or more of the neighbours of the
focal animal were adult males. It is indeed surprising that I did not find indications attesting that
male presence influence female vigilant behaviour since males proved to be the more vigilant sex.
There is the possibility that this study is not extensive enough. Since all study groups contained
more females than males data points of male presence were underrepresented in the analysis. To
evaluate whether this unevenness could have biased the result more data needs to be collected and
the analysis repeated with the added sample. Still, presuming that data deficiency is not the reason
for my results there are other conditions and variables that can deliver possible answers as to why
my prediction could not be confirmed.
Firstly there could be a need for supplementary parameters. In addition to looking at sex
differences when evaluating differences in vigilance, several authors have also considered age
15
(Alberts, 1994) and rank (Alberts, 1994; Gould et al, 1997), existence of offspring (Alberts, 1994;
Steenbeek et al, 1999a; Treves, 1999; Kutsukake, 2007), closeness to neighbouring group’s home
range (Steenbeek et al, 1999a), the individuals’ position in relation to the rest of the group and
height above ground (van Schaik and van Noordwijk, 1989; Steenbeek et al, 1999a). Not all of these
factors are of interest to this study, but a few of them deserves further attention. Note for example,
that females without depending infants could be the only females that would regulate their vigilance
rates depending on the proximity and sex of neighbours since females that have infants have much
more to loose and would maintain the same level of vigilance irrespective of the sex of neighbouring
individuals. Thus, separating vigilance data from females in different life history stages might render
other results then those presented here. Similarly, the threat towards an individual differs depending
on spatial factors. For example, on occasions when foraging is taking place near ground or in the
periphery of the group the animal is potentially subjected to higher levels of danger and should thus
regulate vigilance rate accordingly. Changes in vigilance caused by the position of the focal might
then obscure an analysis on the importance of the focal’s neighbours.
Secondly there is yet again the issue of vigilance directionality. As mentioned in the precious
section on vigilance, there are difficulties in separating social from non-social vigilance when the
object of the focal animals’ vigilance is not known. Hence, the lack of difference in vigilance rate
between females with female neighbours and females with male neighbours, might not
necessarily mean a lack of importance of male vigilance to the females, but could simply reflect
the possibility that I mistook some social vigilance and visual foraging for non-social vigilance.
In fact, since within-group surveillance is expected to increase with group size (Treves, 1999;
Treves and Baguma, 2002), the overall higher vigilance rate in the larger MM-groups may
indicate that within-group surveillance might be an issue at BFMS, which I also noted earlier. In
opposition, female ursine colobus suffer a very low risk of aggression from other group members
(Saj and Sicotte, 2007b; this study) so with the exception of the high-intensity male-male
interactions in MM-groups, there should be little cause for within-group surveillance in this
species (aggression rates will be further discussed in the section on the cost of males to females).
In accordance, females did spend significantly more time scanning alone than when in company
of another group member when no regard to the sex of the neighbour was taken, thus implying
that the purpose of female vigilance is non-social vigilance. These contradictions might have been
entangled if the data would have permitted an analysis on group type differences in female
vigilance and neighbours, but unfortunately a comparison of this sort could not be preformed
without an addition in datapoints.
Intergroup encounters
As predicted, males were the main participants in inter-group encounters and were active in all
low intensity cases and 94,44% of all high intensity cases. Sicotte and Macintosh (2004) also
found that males were the most active sex in inter-group encounters in their study on one C.
vellerosus group at BFMS (BS/B2). My data underlines their findings, confirms that males are
more active in between-group encounters in an additional five groups at this site and further
strengthen the notion that male predomination in inter-group encounters is the norm in this
population.
Female primates can derive great benefits from having male group members participating
extensively in intergroup encounters. If a group is successful during agonistic interactions with
neighbouring groups it can increase its inter-group dominance. In turn, a raised position in the
inter-group hierarchy could grant the group extended access to resources such as food and water
and may also serve to intimidate extra-group males from performing incursions or take-overs,
instances where the group’s infants are at risk of infanticide. Benefits for females of extensive
male participation in intergroup encounters thus include increased resource access and infant
safety (Wrangham 1980). Whether or not these particular benefits are made available to females
of ursine colobus at BFMS is another matter, which will be discussed further in the following part
of this section where I also will address the competitive ability of the study groups and the
implications of group type in inter-group hierarchies.
Sicotte and Macintosh (2004) found indications of male resource defence in their study of
ursine colobus’ inter-group behaviour at BFMS. This would indicate that resource competition
16
occurs between groups in this population and that males take part in defending the group’s
resources during intergroup encounters. Hence one of the female benefits I suggested is implied.
However, the theory that extensive male involvement in between-groups interactions lead to a
reduction in infanticide risk has to remain speculative. Infanticide is known to occur at BFMS
(Sicotte et al, 2007), and thus means of reducing it would be of great importance for females
(Steenbeek et al, 1999a), but it is a rare and difficult phenomenon to study (van Schaik and
Jansen, 2000) and at this point there is no available data confirming whether inter-group
dominance lessens infanticide risk or not.
Shifting the focus to group type, did the number of males in the group influence competition
potential of the group? Unfortunately, the dense vegetation often made it difficult to determine
which group “won” an encounter and I could not comprise an intergroup hierarchy based on the
data I collected. But assuming that males have the ability to increase the competitive capacity of
their group, groups containing more than one adult male would have greater competitive ability
than groups having just one male. A group’s position within the group hierarchy would thus
reflect group type (SM or MM). A similar relationship between the number of males per group
and group rank have been previously predicted by Wrangham (1980) and Nunn (2000) and
confirmed in a study on wedge-capped capuchin monkeys (Robinson, 1988). Thus if intergroup
encounters function as a way of between-group competition over resources in C. vellerosus,
which is possible (Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004), and male group numbers influence the outcome
of such competitive interactions, females can benefit from MM-group living. In light of this
hypothesis it is regrettable that I could not establish a group hierarchy of the study groups at
BFMS and investigate whether group rank was predisposed by group type. However, in the few
cases where a winner of an inter-group encounter could be established, no clear-cut tendency
concerning group type emerged. For instance, the SM-group BS was observed retreating several
times when approached by a MM-group, but in at least one occasion the SM-group SP was
observed driving away the MM-group WW. It is possible that a potential intergroup hierarchy
existing at BFMS is not fixed and linear as in guerezas (Harris, 2006) but depending on the
groups that meet and when, as in Japanese macaques (Sugiura et al, 1999). If this is the case,
female benefits of group type in between-group interaction are less distinct.
As previously mentioned, all MM-groups were larger than the SM-groups (Table 1). Hence it
is difficult to distinguish whether a behaviour expressed by individuals of MM-groups is
associated with the greater number of males or the greater number of individuals of the group, or
perhaps a combination of both. This complication being noted, there is still some debate on the
importance of group size in between-group ranking (Harris 2006).
Even though I was unsuccessful in extracting any data on group hierarchy from my
observations, it was possible to calculate the frequency of inter-group encounters per group type.
MM-groups experiences a slightly lower frequency of between-groups encounters than SMgroups, a tendency which is in line with the four studies compiled by van Schaik (1996)
indicating that groups containing multiple adult males are less likely to suffer conflicts with
neighbouring groups. van Schaik suggested that this could be explained by between-group
avoidance. For females, the advantages of a decrease in, or low rate of, inter-group encounters
would be facilitated infant defence and perhaps a reduction of infanticide risk and injuries.
However, the difference in intergroup encounter frequency between SM and MM-groups was not
statistically significant and I suggest that this is might be a prolonged consequence of population
density. There are examples among several species suggesting that density is the critical factor
affecting between-group aggression (lion: Heinsohn, 1997; Japanese macaque: Saito et al, 1998;
bonnet macaque: Cooper et al, 2004; but see mantled howler monkey: Cristóbal-Azkarate et al,
2003) and intergroup encounter frequency (mungo: Gilchrist and Otali, 2002). BFMS is a small
reserve with a population of colobus monkeys that have been growing for at least 15 years (Beier
et al, 2002; Wong and Sicotte, 2006). The higher density of individuals could be increasing
intergroup encounters and in turn, an overall augmentation in intergroup encounters could dilute
the significance of group type. Strengthening this explanation is an example from one of the study
groups, BS. This group has seen an increase in the hourly rate of intergroup encounters since
2001, from 0.099 (34 encounters in 343 hours: Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004) to 0.195 (8
encounters in 41 hours, this study).
17
Costs of males to females
I did not expect to find extensive or severe within-group aggression between the sexes since this
is mainly common in species socially and / or ecologically different from black and white colobus
species (e.g. orang-hutan: Mitani, 1985; Fox 2001; Japanese macaques: Eaton, 1984; Matsubara
and Sprague, 2004; hamadryas baboon: Smuts, 1995; chimpanzee: Goodall, 1986; Muller et al,
2007; wedge-capped capuchin: O’Brien, 1991; Rose, 1994). I did, however, wish to investigate
whether aggression, when it did occur, was more frequent between than within the sexes.
Nevertheless, the results of this study did not support any of the predictions I put forward; there
was a total absence of male aggression towards female group members, males did not interrupt
female foraging and male presence did not have a negative influence on female feeding or
foraging success. In short, this study suggests that ursine colobus males do not impose costs on
females in form of aggression nor constitute a cost component in resource competition with
females. It is possible that within-group agonistic interaction between the sexes still occur even
though I did not observe it, but even so, most definitely at a very low rate.
The implications of my findings result in two questions; i) why did I not observe any male
aggression towards females within the groups? and ii) if resident ursine colobus males do not
impose cost to females in the form of aggression or feeding competition, is there any other way in
which they impose costs on females?
The answer to the first question is linked to the ecology and social life of C. vellerosus. If we
start by looking at the ecology of ursine colobus we find that it is primarily a folivorous species
(Saj et al, 2005) and folivores usually depend on food resources that are evenly distributed and
plentiful, i.e. hard to monopolize. A resource that is not monopolizable does usually not generate
direct competition (within-group contest competition, WGCC) (Wrangham, 1980; van Schaik,
1989), thus rates of agonistic interactions over food should be infrequent in folivorous
populations. Just recently Saj and Sicotte (2007a) presented results indicating that the potential
for WGCC at BFMS indeed is low and in another study (2007b) they also found that femalefemale aggression during feeding within the group is rare. The result of this study, that malefemale aggression during feeding also is rare, is added evidence for absence of WGCC at BFMS.
Besides feeding competition, the other major circumstance in which female-directed male
aggression is observed in primates is sexual coercion. Herding, intimidation, physical punishment
and forced copulations are all variant types of sexual coercion exhibited by male primates (Smuts
and Smuts 1993). Why were not any of these forms of male aggression observed at BFMS?
Firstly, it is important to remember that sexual coercion can occur at a very low rate. This
study is based on seven study groups and only occasionally were we able to have more than one
group under surveillance and even then dense cover prevented us from a clear view of all
individuals. Hence there were several opportunities for males to exhibit certain rare aggressive
behaviours without our knowing. To take an example, infanticide was only recently documented
in the black-and-white colobus genus (Harris and Monfort, 2003; Onderdonk, 2000) even though
these species have been studied extensively since before the 1970s and at BFMS it was officially
confirmed as late as in 2007 (Sicotte et al, 2007).
Secondly, the majority of these types of male sexual coercion are reported in primate species
socially and physically different from ursine colobus. For example; orang-utan (Pongo
pygmaeus), Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata), hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas) and
chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). In orang-utans, forced copulations are common, but unlike ursine
colobus orang-utan females are solitary and therefore lack protection from other group members
(Mitani, 1985; Fox, 2002). In Japanese macaques, female-directed male aggression and violence
is common, but unlike ursine colobus, Japanese macaques are seasonal breeders and male
aggression escalate and intensify especially during the breeding season (Eaton, 1984; Matsubara
and Sprague, 2004). In hamadryas baboons, male aggression towards females occurs at a regular
basis, but unlike ursine colobus hamadryas males form tightly held harems through abducting
child brides. By continuous threats and intimidation the hamadryas males are able to lay claim to
females long before they are sexually mature and thus make sure that the females will reproduce
with their harem master and not solicit matings with another male (Smuts, 1995). In chimpanzees,
male aggression against females occurs frequently, but unlike ursine colobus chimpanzee females
18
have sexual swellings that provoke high intensity male-male competition and it is believed that
male aggression against females in this species is augmented by this physical attribute (Goodall,
1986; Muller et al, 2007). This being said, ursine colobus also have fair similarities, such as a
folivorous diet and female dispersal, with species in which male aggression is common, e.g.
mountain gorilla (Gorilla gorilla beringei). However, since female-female affiliation is greater
than female-male affiliation in ursine colobus it is likely that female dispersal is more facultative
in ursine colobus than in mountain gorilla (Saj et al, 2007). In the latter the male-female bond is
stronger than the female-female bond and the frequent displays of female-directed aggression
from silver-back males has been proposed to constitute a male demonstration of strength,
exhibited in order to influence female mate choice and female dispersal as well as to keep female
within-group competition from escalating (Watts, 1992; Sicotte, 2002). Also, the considerably
greater sexual dimorphism in seize in mountain gorillas make male dominance maintenance less
costly for males and females more vulnerable to male force than in ursine colobus (Sicotte, 2002;
Slater et al, 2008). Summarizing the above, ursine colobus lack various social and physical traits,
such as solitary living (Saj et al, 2005), seasonal breeding (Teichroeb and Sicotte, 2008), tightly
held harems of unrelated females (Saj et al, 2005; Wong and Sicotte, 2006), sexual swellings
(personal observation) and generalized female dispersal (Saj et al, 2007) present in other species
that exhibit intense forms of sexual coercion and frequent male aggression.
Answering the second question, I propose the possibility that there is one particular
phenomenon of male coercive behaviour I might have overlooked; male herding behaviour.
Herding is a form of male mate defence employed by males in order to keep female group
members from leaving the group (Steenbeek, 1999; Sicotte, 1993) or getting in contact with
extra-group males for copulation (Steenbeek, 1999). Males can achieve this either through
leading his group of females, i.e. herding them, away from other males or other groups, or
through displays of aggression towards female group members when these approach or attempt to
interact with other males (Majolo et al, 2005). Herding is considered to be a form of sexual
coercion since it aims to manipulate mating opportunities.
Herding can be difficult to distinguish from general group movement, especially since it
usually is associated with intergroup encounters, situations typically involving many rapidly
moving individuals. This may be the major reason as to why I overlooked it. Herding is found in
primates species socially or ecologically different from C. vellerosus (chimpanzee: Muller et al,
2007; Japanese macaque: Majolo et al, 2005) and in species living in a different (more open)
habitat (hamadryas baboon: Swedell, 2002) but has also been suggested in black and white
colobus species (C. guereza: Fashing, 2001). Since herding is male tactic designed to keep
females from moving freely and engage in promiscuity it should only exist in species where
female dispersal and extra-group matings occur. At BFMS, both female dispersal (Saj et al, 2007)
and extragroup copulation attempts (Sicotte and Macintosh, 2004) have been documented. Also,
males at BFMS are known to be the main aggressors during intergroup encounters (Sicotte and
Macintosh, 2004; this study), which suggests existence of male mate defence. In light of these
facts I propose the possibility that herding behaviour occurs in my study population of ursine
colobus. At this time the only evidence I can present to support this theory is an observation of
one possible herding attempt witnessed in the group SP. At this occasion the resident male of SP,
ED, was observed following one of the group’s females as she approached members of OD
during an intergroup encounter. The female later returned to SP tailed by ED. The purpose of the
female’s behaviour will not be discussed here, but interestingly, one year later the female IO
(possibly the female seen approaching OD) was missing from SP (Ewa Wikberg, personal
comm.). Had IO been investigating dispersal opportunities in 2007 and than sometime between
August 2007 and May 2008 decided to leave SP? Assuming this to be the case it is possible that
ED had been employing herding behaviour when following IO. For females the costs associated
with male herding behaviour include impediment of free female dispersal and free mate choice
(Watts 1989; Smuts and Smuts, 1993; Steenbeek, 1999).
Finally I would like to address group type and cost of males to females. Since the results in this
study could not confirm any form of cost to females linked to the presence of resident males I
also lack data for a comparison depending on group type. However, if one broadens the view and
includes results and settings other than those of this study, infanticide is often mentioned in
19
relation to male group numbers. This since infanticide and male takeovers (that can lead to
infanticide) is expected to be more likely in groups with low male group number (Watts, 1989;
Sommer, 1994, Rose and Fedigan, 1995; van Schaik, 1996; Perry, 1998). Thus females can
reduce the risk of infanticide in MM-groups and are also known to prefer MM-groups if given the
choice (Steenbeek et al. 1999b; Watts, 1999). A further negative consequence of SM-groups is an
increase in female-female reproductive competition and restrained mate choice (Smuts, 1987;
Altmann, 1990; Kappeler, 1999). Alternately, female members of MM-group experience a higher
risk of intense sexual coercion (Smuts and Smuts, 1993) and a possible increase in food
competition (Wrangham, 1980; van Schaik, 1983; Dunbar, 1988). MM-groups are also more
likely to experience demanding social situations because of the coexistence of several competing
males, which can induce stress-related cost on females (Dunbar, 1987; Chapman and Pavelka,
2005). To add a further level to the issue, all benefits and all costs of males to females are not of
the same magnitude or importance for female fitness and different females would differ in their
susceptibility to certain cost or benefits depending on their age, rank, sexual maturation and if
they have offspring or not. Nevertheless, it is plausible to conclude that it would be in the interest
of all females of all life history stages to manipulate male numbers to their advantage or at least
not be passive in their choice of group if given the opportunity to choose.
CONCLUSIONS
The results of this study suggest that female ursine colobus can derive greater benefits from male
than from female group members when it comes to vigilance and intergroup encounters.
However, no indication of a male behaviour costly to females could be found. Thus, two of the
predictions associated with my first hypothesis, that female’s permanent association with males
result in certain benefits exceeding those provided by other females, was confirmed. However,
my second hypothesis, that female’s permanent association with males result in certain costs
exceeding those imposed by other females, could not be confirmed. I found no clear cut
indication that group type could explain any differences in benefits or costs of males to females,
other than a tendency for SM-groups to experience higher rates of intergroup encounters than
MM-groups and that members of MM-groups appeared to be more vigilant than members of SMgroups. The first I interpret as an indication of group avoidance, beneficiary to the females of
MM-groups, and the latter as a mere consequence of social conditions within the MM-groups.
The absence of obvious costly male behaviour, such as aggression and resource competition, does
not mean that female permanent association with male ursine colobus has to be entirely without
costs but just that the costs may be of a very subtle nature (herding behaviour). For the future, I
request more studies on this species, the least known of all black and white colobus, and
emphasise that by evaluating benefits and costs of resident males to females we may increase the
knowledge of basic female behaviours such as vigilance, foraging and intergroup behaviour as
well as ultimately contribute to the understanding of the underlying motives for female dispersal
and migratory behaviour.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I thank my supervisor Eva Wikberg for her support and help without which this master thesis
would not have been possible. I thank Lucy-Dee Anderson, Dancia Stark and Robert Koranteng
for assistance in data collection and Julie Teichroeb for helping identifying monkeys and sharing
her expertise. I am grateful for the advice and help of Mickael Lönn in analysing statistical data. I
also thank my supervisor at Södertörns högskola, Tomas Bollner and I am grateful for the reviews
of Michael J Ulfbåge and Eva Wikberg. I thank the Ghana Wildlife Division for permission to
perform research at BFMS and the Senior Wildlife Officer at BFMS, Anthony N’Dassah. Finally
I would like to express my gratitude to the caretakers of the BFMS Guesthouse, Alfred and
Beatrice Donyina. This project was founded by a Minor-Field Study Scholarship from The
Swedish International Development Cooperation Agency (Sida).
20
REFERENCES
Alberts S (1994). Vigilance in young
baboons - effects of habitat, age, sex and
maternal rank on glance rate. Anim
Behav, 47:749-755.
Alexander RD (1974). The evolution of
social behaviour. Ann Rev Ecol Syst,
5:325-383.
Altmann J (1974). Observational study of
behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour,
49:227-265.
Altmann J (1990) Primate males go where
the females are. Anim Behav, 39:193-195.
Anderson C M (1986). Predation and
primate evolution. Primates, 27(1):1539.
Artiss T and Martin K (1995). Male
vigilance in white-tailed ptarmigan,
Lagopus leucurus: mate guarding or
predator detection? Anim Behav,
49:1249–1258.
Baldellou M, Henzi SP (1992). Vigilance,
predator detection and the presence of
supernumerary males in vervet monkey
troops. Anim Behav, 43:451-461
Bednekoff PA and Lima SL (1998).
Randomness, chaos and confusion in the
study of antipredator vigilance. Trends
Ecol Evol, 13:284–287.
Beier P, Van Drielen M, Kankam BO
(2002). Avifaunal collapse in West
African forest fragments. Conserv. Biol,
16:1097–1111.
Boinski S (1988). Sex differences in the
foraging behaviour of squirrel monkeys in a
seasonal habitat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol,
23:177-186.
Borries C, Launhardt K, Epplen C, Epplen J
T and Winkler P (1999). Males as infant
protectors in Hanuman langurs (Presbytis
entellus) living in multimale groups –
defence pattern, paternity and sexual
behaviour. Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 46:
350-356.
Burger J and Gochfeld M (1992). Effect of
group size on vigilance while drinking in
the coati, Nasua narica in Costa Rica.
Anim Behav, 44:1053-1057.
Busse C (1980). Leopard and lion predation
upon chacma baboons living in the
Moremi Wildlife Reserve. Botswana Not
Rec, 12:15-21.
Caine NG (1984). Visual scanning by
tamarins. A description of the behaviour
and test of two derived hypothesis. Folia
Primatol, 43:59-67.
Caine NG and Marra SL (1988). Vigilance
and social organisation in two species of
primates. Anim Behav, 36:897-904.
Cameron EZ and du Toit JT (2005). Social
influences on vigilance behaviour in
giraffes, (Giraffa camelopardalis). Anim
Behav, 69:1337-1344.
Campbell CJ (2003). Female-directed
aggression in free-ranging Ateles
geoffroyi. Int J Primatol, 24:223-237.
Chapman CA and Pavelka MSM (2005).
Group size in folivorous primates:
ecological constraints and the possible
influence of social factors. Primates, 46:1–9.
Cheney DL (1987). Interactions and
relationships between groups. In: Smuts
BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM,
Wrangham RW and Struhsaker TT (eds)
Primate Societies. Univ Chicago Press,
Chicago, pp 227-239.
Cheney DL and Wrangham RW (1987).
Predation. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL,
Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW and
Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate Societies.
Univ Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 227239.
Cooper M, Aureli F and Singh M (2004).
Between-group encounters among bonnet
macaques (Macaca radiata). Behav Ecol
Sociobiol, 56:217-227.
Cristóbal-Azkarate J, Dias PAD and Vèa JJ
(2003). Causes of intraspecific aggression
in Alouatta palliata mexicana: evidence
from injuries, demography, and habitat.
Int J Primatol, 25: 939-953.
Crook JH and Gartland JS (1966).
Evolution of primate societies. Nature,
210:1200-1203.
Dunbar RIM (1987). Habitat quality,
population dynamics, and group
composition in colobus monkeys
(Colobus guereza). Int J Primatol, 8:299329.
Dunbar RIM (1988). Primate Social Systems.
Chapman & Hall: London and Cornell
University Press: Ithaca pp.33-54.
Eaton GG (1984). Aggression in adult male
primates: a comparison of confined Japanese
macaques and free-ranging olive baboons.
Int J Primatol, 5:145-160.
Eisenberg JF, Muckenhirn NA and Rudran
21
R (1972). The relation between ecology
and social structure in primates. Science,
176:863-874.
Emlen ST and Oring, LW (1977). Ecology,
sexual selection and the evolution of
mating systems. Science 197:215-223.
Fashing PJ (2001). Male and female
strategies during intergroup encounters in
guerezas (Colobus guereza): evidence for
resource defense mediated through males
and a comparison with other primates.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 50:219-230.
Fedigan LM and Baxter MJ (1984). Sex
differences and social organisation in
free-ranging spider monkeys (Ateles
geoffroyi). Primates, 25:279-294.
Fox EA (2002). Female tactics to reduce
sexual harassment in the Sumatran
orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii). Behav
Ecol Sociobiol, 52:93–101.
Fragaszy DM (1990). Age and sex differences
in the organization of behaviour in wedgecapped capuchins (Cebus olivaceus). Behav
Ecol, 1:81-94.
Ghana Forestry Commision,
http://www.fcghana.com
Gilchrist JS and Otali E (2002). The effects
of refuse-feeding on home-range use,
group size, and intergroup encounters in
the banded mongoose. Can J
Zool, 80(10):1795–1802.
Goodall J (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe
- patterns of behaviour. Harvard Uni. Press,
Cambrdige, MA.
Gould L, Fedigan LM, Rose LM (1997).
Why be vigilant? The case of the alpha
animal. Int J Primatol, 18:401-414.
Gursky S (2005). Predator mobbing in
Tarsius spectrum. Int J Primatol, 26:207221
Harcourt AH and Greenberg J (2001). Do
gorilla females join males to avoid
infanticide? A quantitative model. Anim
Behav 62: 905–915.
Harris TR (2005). Roaring, intergroup
aggression, and feeding competition in
black and white colobus monkeys
(Colobus guereza) at Kanyawara, Kibale
National Park, Uganda. Ph.D.dissertation,
Yale University, New Haven, CT.
Harris TR (2006). Between-group contest
competition for food in a higly folivorous
population of black and white colobus
monkeys (Colobus guereza). Behav Ecol
Sociobiol, 61:317-329.
Harris TR and Monfort SL (2003).
Behavioral and endocrine dynamics
associated with infanticide in a black and
white colobus monkey (Colobus
guereza). Am J Primatol, 61:135–142.
Heinsohn R (1997). Group territoriality in
two populations of African lions. Anim
Behav, 53:1143–1147.
Hrdy SB (1979). Infanticide among
animals: a review, classification, and
examination of the implications for the
reproductive strategies of females. Ethol
Sociobiol, I: 13-40.
IUCN. 2007. 2007 IUCN red list of threatened
species. Available at http://www.redlist.org.
Jack KM (2001). Effect of male emigration
on the vigilance behavior of coresident
males in white-faced capuchins (Cebus
capucinus). Int J Primatol, 22:715-732.
Janson CH (1992). Evolutionary ecology of
primate social structure. In: Smith EA,
Winterhalder B (eds) Evolutionary ecology
and human behavior. Aldine de Gruyter,
New York, pp 95-130.
Kankam BO (1997). The population of black
and white colobus (Colobus polykomos) and
the Mona monkeys (Cercopithecus mona) at
the Boabeng-Fiema Monkey Sanctuary and
surrounding villages. Ph.D. dissertation,
Kumasi University of Science and
Technology, Kumasi, Ghana.
Kappeler PM (1999). Primate socioecology:
new insights from males.
Naturwissenschaften, 85:18–29
Koenig A (2000). Competitive regimes in
forest-dwelling Hanuman langur females
(Semnopithecus entellus). Behav Ecol
Sociobiol, 48:93–109.
Korstjens A.H, Sterck EHM and Noe R
(2002). How adaptive or phylogenetically
inert is primate social behaviour? A test
with two sympatric colobines. Behaviour,
139:203-225.
Kutsukake N (2007). Conspecific influences
on vigilance behavior in wild
chimpanzees. Int J Primatology, 28:907–
918.
Lloyd E, Kreetiyutanont K, Prabnasuk J,
Grassman LI and Borries C (2006).
Observation of Phayre's leaf monkeys
mobbing a clouded leopard at Phu Khieo
Wildlife Sanctuary (Thailand).
Mammalia, 70:158–159.
Majolo B, Ventura R and Koyama NF
(2005). Sex, rank and age differences in
22
the Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata
yakui) participation in inter-group
encounters. Ethology, 111:455-468.
Matsubara M and Sprague DS (2004). Mating
tactics in response to costs incurred by
mating with multiple males in wild female
Japanese macaques. Int J Primatol, 25:901917.
Mitani JC (1985). Mating behaviour of male
orangutans in the Kutai Rserve. Anim
Behav.
33:392-402.
Pireira ME (1983). Abortion following the
immigration of an adult male baboon
(Papio cynocephalus). Am J Primatol, 4:9398.
Muller MN, Kahlenberg SM, Emery
Thompson M and Wrangham RW (2007).
Male coercion and the costs of
promiscuous mating for female
chimpanzees . Proc R Soc B, 274:1009–
1014.
Newton PN (1986). Infanticide in an
undisturbed forest population of hanuman
langurs, Presbytis entellus. Anim Behav,
34:785-789.
Nunn CL (2000). Collective benefits, freeriders, and male extragroup conflict. In:
Kappeler P (ed) Primate males: causes
and consequences of variation in group
composition. Cambridge Univ. Press,
Cambridge, UK, pp 192–204.
Oates JF (1977). The social life of a black
and white colobus monkey (Colobus
guereza). Z. Tierpsychol. 45:1-60.
Oates JF (1994). The natural history of
African colobines. In: Colobine Monkeys:
Their Ecology, Behaviour and Evolution, A.
G. Davies and J. F. Oates (eds.), Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, pp.75-128.
O’Brien T (1991). Female-male social
interactions in wedge-capped capuchin
monkeys: benefits and costs of group
living. Anim Behav, 41:555-567.
Onderdonk DA (2000). Infanticide of a
newborn black-and-white colobus monkey
(Colobus guereza) in Kibale National Park,
Uganda. Primates, 41:209–212.
Palombit RA (1999). Infanticide and the
evolution of pair bonds in nonhuman
primates. Evol Anthropol, 7:117-129.
Perry SE (1998).A case report of a male rank
reversal in wild white-faced capuchins
(Cebus capucinus). Primates 39:477–510.
Robbins MM, (1995). A demographic analysis
of male life history and social structure of
Mountain Gorillas. Behaviour, 132: 21-47.
Robinson JG (1988). Group size in wedgecapped capuchin monkeys Cebus olivaceus
and the reproductive success of males and
females. Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 23:187-197.
Rose LM, Fedigan LM (1995). Vigilance in
white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus, in
Costa Rica. Anim Behav, 49:63-70.
Rose LM (1994). Benefits and costs of resident
males to females in white-faced capuchins
(Cebus capucinus). Am J Primatol, 32:235248.
Ruiter J de (1986). The influence of group size
on predator scanning and foraging behaviour
of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys (Cebus
olivaceus). Behaviour, 98:240-258.
Saito C, Sato S, Suzuki S, Sugiura H,
Agetsuma N, Takahata Y, Sasaki C,
Takahashi H, Tanaka T, Yamagiwa J
(1998). Aggressive intergroup encounters
in two populations of Japanese macaques
(Macaca fuscata). Primates, 39:303–312.
Saj TL and Sicotte P (2005). Male takeover
of a bisexual group in Colobus vellerosus
at Boabeng Fiema MS, Central Ghana.
Primates, 46:211–214.
Saj TL and Sicotte P (2007a). Predicting the
competitive regime of female Colobus
vellerosus from the distribution of food
resources. Int J Primatol, 28:315–336.
Saj TL and Sicotte P (2007b). Scramble
competition among Colobus vellerosus at
Boabeng-Fiema, Ghana. Int J Primatol,
28:337–355.
Saj TL, Teichroeb JA, Sicotte P (2005). The
population status of the ursine colobus
(Colobus vellerosus) at Boabeng-Fiema,
Ghana. In: Paterson JD, Wallis J, editors.
Commensalism and conflict: the human
primate interface. Special topics in
primatology. Vol. 4. Norman, OK:
American Society of Primatologists, pp
350–375.
Saj TL, Marteinson S, Chapman CA and
Sicotte P (2007). Controversy over the
application of current socioecological
models to folivorous primates: Colobus
vellerosus fits the predictions. Am J Phys
Anthropol, 133:994–1003.
Schaik CP van (1983). Why are diurnal
primates living in groups? Behaviour
87:120-144.
Schaik CP van (1989). The ecology of
social relationships amongst female
23
primates. In Standen V, Foley RA (eds)
Comparative socioecology: the
behavioral ecology of humans and other
mammals. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 195–
218.
Schaik CP van (1996). Social evolution in
primates: the role of ecological factors
and male behaviour. Proc. Br. Acad.
88:9-31.
Schaik C.P van and Hooff JARAM van
(1983). On the ultimate causes of primate
social systems. Behaviour, 85:91-117.
Schaik CP van and Höstermann M (1994).
Predation risk and the number of adult
males in a primate group: a comparative
test. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 3: 261–272.
Schaik CP van and Janson CH. 2000.
Infanticide by males and its implications.
Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press, pp 16.
Schaik CP van and Noordwijk MA van
(1989). The special role of male Cebus in
predation avoidance and its effect on
group composition. Behav Ecol
Sociobiol, 4:265-276.
Schaik CP van, Pradhan GR and Noordwijk
MA van (2004). Mating conflict in
primates: infanticide, sexual harassment
and female sexuality. In: Sexual selection
in primates: new and comparative
perspectives (eds P Kappeler and CP van
Schaik). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Univ Press, pp. 131–150.
Sicotte P (1993) Inter-group encounters and
female transfer in mountain gorillas:
influence of group composition on male
behavior. Am J Primatol, 30:21–36
Sicotte P (2002). The function of male
aggressive displays towards females in
mountain gorillas. Primates, 43: 277-289.
Sicotte P and MacIntosh AJ (2004).
Intergroup encounters and male
incursions in Colobus vellerosus in
central Ghana. Behaviour, 141: 533–553.
Sicotte P, Teichroeb and Saj TL (2007).
Aspects of male competition in Colobus
vellerosus: preliminary data on male and
female loud calling, and Infant deaths
after a takeover. Int J Primatol, 28:627636.
Slater KY, Schaffner CM and Aureli F,
(2008). Female-directed male aggression in
wild Ateles geoffroyi yucatanensis. Int J
Primatol, 29:1657-1669.
Smuts BB, (1987). Gender, aggression and
influence. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL,
Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW and
Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate Societies.
Univ Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 400412.
Smuts BB, (1995). Apes of wrath. Discover
magazine. Published online August 1.
Smuts BB and Smuts RW (1993). Male
aggression and sexual coercion of
females in nonhuman primates and other
mammals: evidence and theoretical
implications. Adv Study Behav, 22:1-63.
Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM,
Wrangham RW and Struhsaker TT, (eds),
(1987). Primate Societies. Univ Chicago
Press, Chicago.
Sommer V (1994). Infanticide among langurs
of Jodhpur: testing the sexual selection
hypothesis with a long-term record. In:
Parmigiani S, vom Saal F, (eds). Infanticide
and parental care. New York: Harwood
Academic Publishers, pp 155–198.
Steenbeek R (1999). Tenure related changes
in wild Thomas’s langurs I: Betweengroup interactions. Behaviour, 136:595–
625.
Stanford CB (2002). Avoiding predators:
expectations and evidence in primate
antipredator behavior. Int J Primatol,
23:741-757.
Steenbeek R, Ruben CP, Buul M van and
Hooff JARAM van (1999a). Vigilance in
wild Thomas’s langurs (Presbytis
thomasi): the importance of infanticide
risk. Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 45:137–150.
Sterck EHM, Watts DP, Schaik CP van
(1997). The evolution of female social
relationships in nonhuman primates.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 41:291-309.
Steenbeek R, Sterck EHM, de Vries H, van
Hooff J (1999b). Costs and benefits of the
one-male, age-graded and all-male phase in
wild Thomas langur groups. In: Kappeler
PM (ed) Primate males. Cambridge Univ
Press, Cambridge.
Strier KB, (1994). Brotherhoods among
atelines: Kinship, affiliation, and
competition. Behaviour 130:151–167.
Struhsaker, T.S. & Leland, L. (1987).
Colobines: Infanticide by adult males. In:
Primate societies (B.B. Smuts, D.L.
Cheney, R.M. Seyfarth, R.W.Wrangham
& T.T. Stushaker, eds). Chicago Univ
Press, Chicago, p. 83-97.
Sugiura H, Saito C, Sato S, Agetsuma N,
24
Takahashi H, Tanaka T, Furuichi T and
Takahata Y (1999). Variation in
intergroup encounters in two populations
of Japanese macaques. Int J Primatol,
21:519-535.
Swedell L (2002). Affiliation among
females in wild hamadryas baboons
(Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Int J
Primatol, 23:1205-1226.
Teichroeb JA, Marteinson S and Sicotte P
(2005). Individuals’ behavior following
dye marking in wild black-and-white
colobus (Colobus vellerosus). Am J
Primatol, 65:197–203.
Teichroeb JA and Sicotte P, 2008. Social
correlates of fecal testosterone in male
ursine colobus monkeys (Colobus
vellerosus): the effect of male reproductive
competition in aseasonal breeders. Horm
Behav, 54:417–423.
Teichroeb JA, Saj TL, Paterson JD and
Sicotte P (2008). Effect of group size on
activity budgets of Colobus vellerosus in
Ghana. Int J Primatol, 24:743-758.
Terborgh J and Janson CH (1986). The
socioecology of primate groups. Ann Rev
Ecol Systemat, 17:111-135.
Treves A (1999). Within-group vigilance in
red colobus and redtail monkeys. Am J
Primatol, 48:113–126.
Treves A (2000). Theory and method in
studies of vigilance and aggregation.
Anim Behav, 60:711–722.
Treves A and Baguma P (2002).
Interindividual proximity and
surveillance of associates in comparative
perspective. In: Glenn ME and Cords M
(eds). The Guenons: Diversity and
Adaptation in African Monkeys Part II,
Springer US, pp 161-172.
Trivers RL (1972). Parental investment and
sexual selection. In: Sexual selection and
the descent of man, 1871-1971 (B.
Campbell, ed.). Chicago Univ Press,
Chicago, pp 136-179.
Wallace RB (2007). Towing the Party Line:
Territoriality, Risky Boundaries and Male
Group Size in Spider Monkey Fission–
Fusion Societies. Am J Primatol, 69:1-16.
Watts DP (1989). Infanticide in mountain
gorillas: new cases and a reconsideration
of the evidence. Ethology, 81:1-18.
Watts DP (1992). Social relationships of
immigrant and resident female mountain
gorillas I. Male-female elationships. Am J
Primatol, 28:159-181.
Watts DP (1999) Causes and consequences of
variation in male mountain gorilla life
histories and group membership. In:
Kappeler PM (ed), Primate males.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Wrangham RW (1979). On the evolution of
ape social system. Soc Sc Inform, 18:334368.
Wrangham RW (1980). An ecological
model of female social relationships.
Behaviour, 75:262-300.
Wong SNP and Sicotte P (2006). Population
size and density of Colobus vellerosus at
the Boabeng-Fiema Monkey Sanctuary
and surrounding forest fragments in
Ghana. Am J Primatol, 68:1–12.
25