Download Attitudes Towards Homosexuality: A Literature Review

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Corrective rape wikipedia , lookup

Gay pornography wikipedia , lookup

Sexual racism wikipedia , lookup

The Homosexual Matrix wikipedia , lookup

Gay wikipedia , lookup

Homosexuality wikipedia , lookup

History of homosexuality wikipedia , lookup

LGBT social movements wikipedia , lookup

Gender roles in non-heterosexual communities wikipedia , lookup

LGBT history wikipedia , lookup

Socialism and LGBT rights wikipedia , lookup

Violence against LGBT people wikipedia , lookup

Homosexualities: A Study of Diversity Among Men and Women wikipedia , lookup

Societal attitudes toward homosexuality wikipedia , lookup

Homosexuality in society wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Attitudes Towards Homosexuality: A Literature Review
Gail Mason & Mischa Barr
Sydney Institute of Criminology, Sydney Law School, University of Sydney
January 2006
Australian Hate Crime Network
http://sydney.edu.au/law/criminology/ahcn/australian_materials.shtml#M
Contact: [email protected]
Introduction
A great deal of research has been conducted into attitudes towards homosexuality,
primarily in the United States. The most recent research shows that a majority of the
American population still harbour negative attitudes towards homosexuals and
homosexuality, although the size of the majority has diminished markedly over the past
decade (American Enterprise Institute 2004). The Canadian, British and Australian
populations tend to be more accepting of homosexuality than Americans, but their
attitudes are generally predicted by the same demographic characteristics and values.
From the evidence, it is possible to conclude that those with negative attitudes
towards homosexuality are more likely to be religious, politically conservative, and less
well educated, and to have authoritarian personalities and traditional gender role beliefs.
They are more likely to live outside metropolitan areas and in the South or Midwest of
the United States. They are also more likely to believe that homosexuality is a choice and
less likely to have had contact with gay men or lesbians.
Evidence regarding the influence of age, gender and race on attitudes towards
homosexuality is less conclusive, but it can be stated with some degree of confidence that
women are more tolerant than men and younger people more tolerant than older people.
African Americans tend to be less accepting of homosexual behaviour than White
Americans, but more supportive of gay and lesbian civil rights (Lewis 2003).
The variables with the most significant impact on attitudes towards homosexuality
are generally age, education, religiosity, gender role beliefs and political ideology, as well
as contact with homosexual persons. The nature of these variables suggests that, in
addition to personal experience, personal beliefs and values may be more influential than
demographic characteristics (see Agnew et al 1993). Indeed, research has shown that
gender differences in attitudes may be attributed to differences in gender role beliefs
(Kerns and Fine 1994; Kite and Whitley 1996) and racial differences may be accounted
for by the influence of religion and education (Schulte and Battle 2004).
Limitations in the Research on Attitudes Towards Homosexuality
There are several potential limitations to or biases in the literature on attitudes
towards homosexuality. The most obvious limitation is that the overwhelming majority of
the research has been conducted using American samples. The extent to which American
1
findings can be generalised to apply to other Western or English-speaking nations is
unclear.
Much of the research also relies on convenience samples of college students. As
Overby and Barth neatly put it, ‘[t]his is problematic… because college students tend to
be wealthier, younger, better educated, and less ethnically diverse than the national
population, making it difficult to extrapolate to and draw inferences regarding the
behaviour of the general public’ (2002: 437). Many of these demographic characteristics
of student samples, especially age and education, tend to be associated with greater
tolerance of homosexuality (Herek 2002b; Overby and Barth 2002). National probability
samples are rare, but notable examples are the General Social Survey series and much of
the research conducted by Herek and his associates. Some of the research is also limited
by its use of very small samples, particularly of ethnic and racial minorities.
Another potential limitation is the diversity of measures used to assess attitudes
towards homosexuality. These variations make it difficult to make comparisons both over
time and across populations. Different researchers use different scales to measure
attitudes. Two of the most popular are Herek’s ‘Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay
Men’ (ATLG) and Hudson and Ricketts’ ‘Index of Attitudes Toward Homosexuals’
(IAH). Some instruments focus on beliefs and opinions about homosexual people and
others on feelings and emotions. Some focus on attitudes towards homosexual behaviour
and others on attitudes towards the civil rights of homosexual people. Some use only a
single item, whereas others use multi-item scales. Multi-item scales can better detect
nuances, ambiguities and inconsistencies in individuals’ attitudes.
Questions seeking similar information can elicit different responses depending on
how they are worded. Striking examples can be found in the literature on attitudes
towards gay and lesbian civil rights. Responses change depending upon whether the
question is framed in abstract or specific terms, and in terms of support for pre-existing
laws or the passing of new laws (see Lewis and Rogers 1999). The picture is further
complicated by modifications to question wording, or the addition of new questions, in
the same instruments over time (see, for example, American Enterprise Institute 2004;
CJC 1990). A particular problem in the past has been the failure to differentiate between
attitudes towards gay men and attitudes towards lesbians. Many instruments used to
measure attitudes simply used the term ‘homosexual’ to describe the attitude object. This
is problematic both because it assumes that attitudes towards lesbians and gay men are
identical and because research has shown that respondents tend to assume that the term
‘homosexual’ refers to gay men (Herek and Capitanio 1999b; Kite and Whitley 1996;
Simon 1998). More recent research has set about trying to rectify this problem.
Responses may also be affected by the negative bias in many of the questions
used. The clearest example is the commonly used item which asks whether respondents
think homosexual relations are ‘always wrong, almost always wrong, wrong only
sometimes, or not wrong at all’.
On the other hand, as homophobia becomes less
acceptable, survey data may also be limited by a potential tendency towards socially
desirable or politically correct responses, especially among well-educated samples.
The research is limited by the lack of longitudinal studies examining how the
attitudes of a particular group of people change over time. It is also difficult to measure
how individuals’ attitudes may change in different contexts. Moreover, ‘surveys seldom
indicate whether the opinion expressed reflects strong beliefs, heavily entrenched
2
attitudes, experience based judgements, “top of the head” response or just a spur of the
moment answer’ (CLC 1990:14). Responses to a ‘pencil and paper’ test may not give an
accurate indication of an individuals’ attitudes and behaviour in their daily life.
Finally, the literature emphasises prejudice rather than tolerance. It examines the
characteristics of people who hold negative attitudes towards homosexuality and
homosexual people rather than asking what qualities a tolerant person possesses (Kite and
Whitely 1996; Kite 2002).
Changes in Attitudes Towards Homosexuality in the United States
National opinion polls in the United States, such as the General Social Survey
(GSS), began charting attitudes towards homosexuality in the early 1970s. From 1973
until the early 1990s, attitudes remained fairly stable, with 70-75% of American adults
believing that homosexuality was ‘always wrong’ (Lewis and Rogers 1999; Loftus 2001;
Smith 1992; Yang 1997, 2001). Those who believed that homosexuality was ‘not wrong
at all’ constituted 12-15% of the population. The remaining 10% fell into the intermediate
categories of ‘almost always wrong’ and ‘wrong only sometimes’ (Smith 1992).
While attitudes remained fairly stable during the 1970s and 1980s, researchers
have noted a small but significant shift towards more negative attitudes in the mid to late
1980s, when the percentage of American adults who believed homosexuality was ‘always
wrong’ rose above 75% (American Enterprise Institute 2004; Loftus 2001; Pratte 1993;
Smith 1992; Yang 1997). Most attribute this shift to the combined effects of the AIDS
crisis, which captured public attention during this period and the rise of reactionary
politics during the Reagan and Bush Snr administrations (Loftus 2001; Scott 1998).
However, the impact of the AIDS epidemic on attitudes towards homosexuality has been
questioned, and this will be discussed further below.
Since the early 1990s, however, there has been a dramatic liberalisation in
attitudes towards homosexuality. Disapproval of homosexuality dropped by ten to fifteen
percentage points through the 1990s. The percentage believing homosexuality is ‘always
wrong’ dropped from 76% in 1991 to 66% in 1993 and then to around 60% in the late
1990s (American Enterprise Institute 2004). The most recent data show that in 2002 those
considering homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’ constituted just over 50% of the
population, while over 30% of the population now believe homosexuality is ‘not wrong at
all’ (American Enterprise Institute 2004). While the majority of the American population
still finds homosexuality unacceptable, it has become significantly more tolerant.
Interestingly, the most recent data suggest that the conservative politics of the George W.
Bush administration have not (yet) reversed the trend towards greater acceptance of
homosexuality (American Enterprise Institute 2004). However, the relatively stable level
of acceptance since the mid-1990s suggests that the liberal trend may have reached a
plateau, with the American population roughly evenly split on acceptance of
homosexuality.
3
While it may give a rough indication of the climate of tolerance (or intolerance),
measures of the acceptability of homosexuality are merely one dimension of heterosexual
attitudes towards homosexuality. For example, researchers have developed measures to
differentiate between attitudes towards the (moral) acceptability of homosexual
behaviour and attitudes towards gay and lesbian civil rights, and have found the trends to
be quite distinct (Bernstein and Kostelac 2002; Klamen et al 1999; Loftus 2001; Smith
1992; Yang 1997: 477). 1 While the overwhelming majority of Americans considered
homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’ throughout the 1970s and 1980s, the same period
witnessed a steady decline in the population’s willingness to restrict the civil liberties of
gay and lesbian people (Dejowski 1992; Loftus 2001; Smith 1992; Yang 1997, 2001). 2
For example, while only 56% of people supported equal employment opportunities for
homosexuals in 1977, support had increased to over 80% by the early 1990s, reaching
88% by 2003 (American Enterprise Institute 2004). A specific example is support for
employment of homosexuals as elementary school teachers, which rose from only 27%
support in 1977 to 42% in 1989 and 61% in 2003 (American Enterprise Institute 2004). 3
While most of the research is simply descriptive of trends in public opinion,
several explanations have been proposed. It is generally accepted that cohort replacement
(the displacement of intolerant generations by more tolerant generations) has had a
greater impact on the liberalisation of attitudes towards homosexuality than has intracohort change (that is, generations becoming more liberal in their opinions as they age)
(Crockett and Voas 2003; Davis 1992 cited in Lewis and Rogers 1999; Kelley 2001;
Scott 1998; Smith 1992). However, Scott (1998) has demonstrated a pattern of attitude
change within cohorts and respondents in several surveys have indicated that their own
attitudes have changed over a given period of time, mostly as a result of contact with gay
and lesbian persons (Altemeyer 2001; Howard-Hassmann 2001). A 2003 Gallup poll
found that while 59% of those surveyed said their attitudes had not changed, 32% said
that they had become more accepting (American Enterprise Institute 2004: 9).
Dejowski (1992) and Loftus (2001) have shown that some of the shifts in attitudes
towards homosexuality can be attributed to a two factors. The first is demographic
change, particularly the growth of a more educated population. 4 The second is cultural
ideological change, in particular the decreasing willingness to restrict the civil liberties of
all unpopular groups. In addition, Loftus suggests that the liberalisation in attitudes can
be attributed to the political activism and visibility of queer communities; a possible
backlash in the 1990s against the success of the radical religious right in the 1980s; the
1
I place the word ‘moral’ in parentheses because while GSS (and other similar) questions regarding
whether ‘sexual relations between two adults of the same sex’ is ‘always wrong, almost always wrong,
wrong only sometimes, or not wrong at all’ clearly have moral overtones, people may disapprove of
homosexuality for reasons other than their moral values. Lewis and Rogers (1999) argue that there has been
no trend away from moral disapproval (as of 1996) despite the decrease in the percentage of people who
consider homosexuality ‘always wrong’. They attribute this decrease to the diminishing importance of
other reasons for disapproving of homosexuality such as gender nonconformity, the belief that homosexual
people are sexually promiscuous and the belief that homosexuality is a mental illness (120). It may
therefore be strictly inaccurate to characterise the distinction in attitudes as being between the ‘morality’ of
homosexuality and acceptance of gay and lesbian civil rights, as Dejowski (1992), Klamen et al (1999) and
Loftus (2001: 778) do.
2
With the exception of 1985 and 1987: Loftus (2001).
3
Attitudes towards civil rights for gay men and lesbians will be discussed further below.
4
The impact of education on attitudes towards homosexuality is discussed below.
4
role of other liberal movements, such as the civil rights and women’s movements, in
increasing awareness of discrimination against disempowered groups; and the expansion
of media coverage of gay and lesbian issues (2001; see also Dejowski 1992; Scott 1998).
Other factors that may have influenced attitudinal trends include the declining
influence of religion (Altemeyer 2001; Scott 1998) and shifts in attitudes towards sexual
morality generally (Ficarrotto 1990; Scott 1998; Simon 1995; Smith 1992). With regard
to the latter, however, it must be noted that while attitudes towards premarital sex have
undergone a dramatic liberalisation, attitudes towards extramarital sex have remained
constant and negative attitudes towards homosexuality have only declined slowly (Scott
1998; Smith 1992). Improved understanding of the AIDS risk may also have reduced
hostility towards homosexual people (Altemeyer 2001; Scott 1998).
Attitudes Towards Homosexuality in Australia
Empirical research into attitudes towards homosexuality in Australia is virtually
non-existent. Certainly no national surveys akin to the US General Social Surveys have
been undertaken. Morgan polls conducted in 1974 and 1989 show a small increase in the
proportion of the population supporting the legalisation of male and female
homosexuality (from 54% to 58%) (Criminal Justice Commission, Queensland 1990
(CJC 1990)). By comparison, 43% of the US population supported legalisation in 1977
and 47% in 1989. In Canada in 1977, 70% of those surveyed supported decriminalising
homosexuality (Fernald 1995).
In 1989 Western Australia was the most liberal state with 74% of the population
supporting the legalisation of male homosexuality, while Tasmania was the least tolerant,
with only 47% support. Men were less supportive than women and the youngest (14-24
years) and oldest (50+ years) age groups were least supportive. Respondents with higher
levels of education and those who supported the Democrats were generally more
supportive. Australians were significantly less supportive of civil rights for gay men and
lesbians in 1989 than they were of legalisation of homosexual behaviour. For example,
the Australian public was evenly split on the question of whether it should be illegal to
discriminate against homosexual persons and only 36% believed homosexual couples
who receive the same treatment as heterosexual couples (CJC 1990). Pain and Disney
(1995) also found that 69% of students in their 1991 Australian university sample could
be considered homophobic.
Since the 1980s, however, negative attitudes towards homosexuality have
declined dramatically. In the mid-1980s, 64% of the population considered
homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’. By 1999, this figure had fallen to 48%, with 28%
considering homosexuality to be ‘not wrong at all’ (Kelley 2000). The AIDS scare
appeared to have no effect on the liberalising trend (CJC 1990; Kelley 2000). Kelley
comments that opinions on the acceptability of homosexuality ‘are among the most
strongly polarised ever reported on an attitude item in this country: the vast majority of
Australians are either unambiguously tolerant or unambiguously censorious, with very
few holding ambivalent or nuanced views in the middle’ (2000: 15). In general, it appears
that Australians are more accepting of homosexuality than Americans with less than half
the Australian population considering homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’ (see also
Spark Jones 2000).
5
Attitudes Towards Homosexuality in Britain and Canada
The first British Social Attitudes Survey in 1983 found that just over half of those
surveyed considered homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’ while 18% considered it ‘not
wrong at all’ (Crockett and Voas 2003). Intolerance increased through the 1980s, peaking
at 64% in 1987, perhaps due to AIDS panic (Crockett and Voas 2003; Scott 1998) and
the moral conservatism of the New Right (Scott 1998). Intolerance rapidly decreased
through the 1990s such that by 2000 the number of those accepting homosexuality almost
matched the number of those condemning it (Crockett and Voas 2003). Crockett and
Voas attribute this increasing acceptance partly to a liberal period effect, but mostly to
cohort replacement, whereby more tolerant generations displace those less tolerant
(2003). As in Australia and the United States, attitudes towards homosexuality in Britain
remain polarised. Levels of acceptance are roughly equal to those in Australia and higher
than those in the United States (Widmer et al 1998), possibly due to the strength of the
antigay lobby in the US and the greater influence of religion (Crockett and Voas 2003;
Ellis 2002; Ellis et al 2002; Scott 1998).
Altemeyer (2001) conducted a longitudinal study at a Canadian university which
showed that while opinions on the acceptability of homosexual behaviour changed very
little between 1984 and 1998, respondents became much less willing to restrict the civil
liberties of homosexual people. Measures of affective responses to gay and lesbian
people showed that while the sight of two men kissing still bothered respondents as much
in 1998 as it had in 1984, they were much more willing to associate with gay men and
lesbians (Altemeyer 2001). Altemeyer suggests that these more positive attitudes are
likely to be associated with a drop in religious attendance, awareness of research into the
biological origins of homosexuality, greater understanding of AIDS and media support
for homosexuality (2001: 70-71).
Attitudes Towards Homosexuality in Other Nations
International Social Surveys conducted in the 1990s show that there are stark
national differences in attitudes towards homosexuality. Summaries compiled by Widmer
et al (1998) and Kelley (2001) show that people in the Netherlands are by far the most
accepting of homosexuality, with two-thirds of the population considering homosexuality
‘not wrong at all’. Dutch liberalism may be attributable to a progressive church, a strong
and long-standing gay movement, a secular population, a strong public commitment to
pluralism and frank discussion of sexuality in the media (Widmer et al 1998: 356).
Substantial percentages of the populations of Spain (somewhat surprisingly), the Czech
Republic, Canada and Norway are also accepting of homosexuality. Australia, Britain
and New Zealand fall in the middle of the spectrum, with just over half the populations of
these countries considering homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’. The United States is
among the least accepting countries, sharing the honours with conservative religious
nations such as Poland, Northern Ireland and Ireland; Eastern European nations such as
Hungary, Bulgaria and Slovenia; and the Philippines.
The same demographic characteristics predict intolerance across the nations
surveyed, with those who are young, female, well-educated and less religious generally
the most tolerant of homosexuality. In terms of religion, Scott argues that attitudes vary
6
across countries according to the extent to which the organised church has retained its
moral authority, rather than according to the national religion per se (1998). However, by
this measure, relatively secular countries such as Britain and Australia should perhaps be
more tolerant than they are (see Scott 1998: 839). Richer countries tend to be more
tolerant than countries which are less economically developed (Kelley 2001).
Widmer et al have shown that attitudes towards homosexuality are not necessarily
predicted by attitudes towards other types of sexual behaviour, such as premarital and
extramarital sex (1998). A ‘simple permissive-nonpermissive dichotomy’ is insufficient
to account for national differences in sexual values. Homosexuality is generally more
accepted than extramarital sex, but attitudes towards homosexuality are more varied
across nations and more polarised within nations. For example, Germany and Austria are
very permissive of teen and premarital sex, but only moderately accepting of homosexual
sex. For countries such as the Netherlands, the Czech Republic and Canada, the
relationship is reversed (Widmer et al 1998: 354). These variations occur within nations
as well. For example, women are generally more sexually conservative than men but are
more accepting of homosexuality (Scott 1998; Smith 1992).
Kelley concludes that Australian attitudes towards homosexuality are
‘unexceptional’. Australia is ‘not as tolerant as some prosperous, irreligious nations (like
the Netherlands), nor as intolerant [as] some poor, religious nations (like Chile and the
Philippines). Rather it is middle of the road. Over time, Australian opinion, like that in
other nations, is likely to shift slowly but steadily towards greater tolerance’ (2001: 20).
Attitudes Towards Gay and Lesbian Civil Rights
As mentioned above, heterosexuals tend to be significantly more supportive of
gay and lesbian civil rights than they are accepting of homosexual behaviour (Bernstein
and Kostelac 2002; Klamen et al 1999; Loftus 2001; Smith 1992; Yang 1997: 477).
According to Loftus, this disjuncture is evident in majority group attitudes towards other
minority groups and epitomises the definition of ‘tolerance’ in political sociology,
whereby an individual or group grants rights to a particular group even though they
dislike that group (2001: 779; Riggle and Ellis 1994: 137). The reason for the difference
in attitudes may also lie in distinctions between cognitive responses (opinions and
beliefs) and affective responses (emotions and feelings) towards homosexual persons. For
example, heterosexuals may believe, in accordance with their political or social values,
that gay men and lesbians are entitled to equal rights (for example, equal employment
opportunities) and yet may still feel uncomfortable with homosexual persons and
behaviour (Altemeyer 2001; Davies 2004; Hudson and Ricketts 1980; Kite and Whitley
1998; LaMar and Kite 1998; Loftus 2001; Logan 1996; Norris 1992; Van de Ven 1994).
Affective attitudes appear to be less amenable to change than cognitive attitudes
(Altemeyer 2001; Van de Ven 1995).
However, heterosexual support for gay civil rights varies considerably depending
on two principal factors: first, the right at issue and the way in which it is formulated; and
secondly, whether supporting the right entails merely endorsing existing laws or
extending to homosexual persons a right which they have hitherto been denied. Survey
items framed in terms of a general or abstract right to equality receive substantial
majority if not near unanimous support, but when these rights are applied to specific
7
circumstances, support is much lower (Ellis 2002; Ellis et al 2002; Norris 1992). For
example, while Ellis found that 96% of her British university sample agreed that a
person’s sexual orientation should not block access to basic rights and freedoms, fewer
than 55% agreed that books promoting homosexuality as a positive lifestyle should be
freely available in school libraries (2002). In the United States in 1992, while 74% of
those surveyed supported equal employment opportunities for gay and lesbian people in
principle, only 37% favoured hiring gays and lesbians in all five occupations mentioned
(salesperson, elementary school teacher, clergy, doctor, armed forces) (Lewis and Rogers
1999). Ellis concludes that because it is no longer considered acceptable to express
explicitly discriminatory attitudes in the current liberal climate, respondents will usually
affirm abstract egalitarian values (2002; see also Ellis et al 2002). 5 However, respondents
may perceive the practical application of these rights or values in specific instances (such
as permitting books promoting homosexuality in public libraries) to be unfair ‘special
treatment’ for gay men and lesbians (Ellis et al 2002: 134).
Certain rights for gay and lesbian people receive more support than others. Strand
distinguishes between support for civil rights and support for civil liberties, arguing that
heterosexuals are more supportive of civil liberties for gay men and lesbians, which are
concerned with protecting individuals from undue government interference in their
private behaviour, than they are of civil rights, which involve harnessing government
power to limit the ability of private persons to ‘engage in social and occupational
exclusion’ (1998: 115). Thus, he argues that support for gay and lesbian rights is related
to people’s abstract political values as much as it is related to their attitudes towards
homosexuality. Riggle and Ellis, on the other hand, found that an individual’s dislike for
homosexuals reduced his/her support for the rights of homosexuals to a much greater
degree than did dislike for other unpopular groups such as the KKK and neo-Nazis
(1994). They attributed this difference to the strength of negative affect towards
homosexuals and perhaps to the perception that homosexuals are not a ‘political’ group
and therefore not deserving of political tolerance. Other research supports Strand’s
theory, finding that very few American respondents support government regulation of
private consensual homosexual behaviour. Again, this is attributed to a libertarian ‘live
and let live’ ideology rather than active acceptance of homosexuality (Ellis 2002; Ellis et
al 2002; Loftus 2001). For example, in 2003, 86% of American respondents opposed
state laws regulating private consensual sexual relations between an unmarried
heterosexual couple, and 82% also opposed regulation of private consensual homosexual
relations (American Enterprise Institute 2004: 52).
Support for all gay civil rights and liberties has grown substantially, especially
since the early to mid-1990s (see Lewis and Rogers 1999; Yang 1997). For example,
Torres-Reyna and Shapiro show that national support for hiring gay men and lesbians in
the US military has grown from 51% in 1977 to 72% in 2001 (2002). 6 The percentage of
those polled by Gallup who believe marriages between homosexuals should be legally
valid has risen from 27% in 1996 to 42% in 2004 (American Enterprise Institute 2004:
22). As these figures suggest, support for equality rights also differs according to the area
5
Lewis and Rogers (1999) also note that use of the word ‘equal’ may elicit socially desirable responses.
With one deviation in 1993, when controversy raged over President Clinton’s proposal to allow
homosexual persons to serve openly in the military. The public split 47/47 on the issue at this time (TorresReyna and Shapiro 2002)
6
8
of social policy involved. Heterosexuals are much more likely to favour ensuring basic
equality rights for homosexuals (such as equal access to education, housing and
employment) than they are to favour gay marriage and adoption by gay and lesbian
couples. Heterosexuals also distinguish between the types of occupations in which they
approve of the hiring of gay men and lesbians. They are generally comfortable with being
served by gay salespeople (92% approval), but are less comfortable consulting gay
doctors (82%) and even less approving of gay elementary school teachers (61%) and gay
clergy (56%) (American Enterprise Institute 2004: 13-14; see also Lewis and Rogers
1999). Lewis and Rogers suggest that heterosexuals may be concerned by the prospect of
hiring gay men and lesbians into occupations involving intimate contact (particularly
with children) or moral guidance (for example, the clergy) (1999).
While over 80% of the Canadian community leaders interviewed by HowardHassmann indicated support for basic equality rights (such as equal access to education,
housing and employment), only about half favoured equal rights to marriage and
adoption (2001). However, another quarter approved of legal domestic partnerships with
identical legal rights to marriage (see also Tygart 2000: 266). 7 The drawing of this
distinction underscores the potent symbolism of the institution of ‘marriage’ in Western
cultural tradition and religious teaching. Howard-Hassmann considers support for
marriage and adoption rights to be one indication of whether Canadians are ‘willing to go
further than merely according equal rights to gays, to according them societal respect and
acknowledgment that their relationships are as worthy a concern as heterosexual
relationships’ (2001: 130-31).
In addition to differing according to the type of right in question, respondents are
generally less willing to extend existing civil rights laws to protect lesbians and gay men
than they are to endorse existing protections or general equality principles (Lewis and
Rogers 1999; Schulte and Battle 2004). Ellis found that while just over half the
respondents viewed themselves as personally responsible for creating positive social
change, only a handful gave responses that indicated an active commitment to supporting
lesbian and gay rights (2002). Most conceived of their responsibility in terms of an
individual obligation not to engage in discriminatory behaviour and to oppose the actions
of those who did. Norris found that many of the students and college staff he surveyed
felt the need to conceal their support for lesbians and gay men for fear of appearing too
pro-gay (1992). Lewis and Rogers found significantly lower levels of support for passing
laws to protect the rights of homosexuals than for laws per se. Through the 1990s, they
found approximately 60% support for equal job opportunities for gays and lesbians but
44-48% support (and as low as 26% in some surveys) for extending civil rights
protections applicable to racial and religious minorities to cover homosexuals. They
suggest that lesbians and gay men may not be considered a ‘legitimate’ minority group,
and that people may fear that such laws would imply special treatment for homosexuals
and/or recognition of gay marriage (1999: 123).
There also appears to be a particular concern with the promotion of
homosexuality and gay rights issues. 37% of Howard-Hassmann’s sample, who in other
respects appeared quite accepting of homosexuality, volunteered a concern with gay
7
Interestingly, the group of people who approved of gay marriage and the group that approved of adoption
on equal terms with heterosexuals were not co-extensive, suggesting that different considerations may
come into play with regard to each issue.
9
‘flaunting’ (2001). Similarly, 25% of those surveyed by Ellis did not support the right of
lesbians and gays to ‘flaunt their sexuality in public at marches and demonstrations’
(2002). Moreover, Ellis found that 14% believed that ‘lesbians and gays should only be
allowed to express their views as long as they don’t offend or upset the majority’, 9%
disagreed that it was acceptable for lesbian and gay couples openly to express their
affection for their partners in public without fear of harassment or violence, and almost a
quarter agreed that society has a right to prevent lesbians and gays from publicly
promoting homosexuality as equivalent to heterosexuality (2002). Howard-Hassmann
suggests that part of this opposition may be attributable to a more general opposition to
open discussion of sexuality and a commitment to ‘a certain social decorum’ (2002: 133,
136). Respondents also express concern that the gay rights movement exerts too much
political influence or is too militant (Howard-Hassmann 2001; Norris 1992). One US
survey in 2000 found that 42% of those interviewed believed ‘gay rights organisations
have too much influence in society’, and a 2004 survey found that 45% of respondents
thought that ‘efforts to protect gay rights have gone too far in giving gays special
privileges’, a figure which represented an increase of 5% since 1996 (American
Enterprise Institute 2004: 10). Howard-Hassmann notes, however, that fear of gay
‘flaunting’ and ‘militancy’ does not necessarily diminish respondents’ support for gay
rights (2001: 133).
People in Canada and Britain appear to be significantly more supportive of gay
civil rights and liberties than are Americans. For example, support for recognition of gay
marriage in Canada rose from 24% in 1992 to 42% in 1996 to 48% in 2003 (HowardHassmann 2001; Opinion Canada 2003) and 63% of the British students surveyed by
Ellis et al agreed that lesbian and gay couples should be legally permitted to marry
(2002). This is unsurprising, at least in the Canadian context, where the political climate
is much more favourable to lesbians and gay men than in the US. For example, as of
February 2005, gay marriage is legal in seven of the ten Canadian provinces of Canada,
and in one of the three territories (Religious Tolerance.org). 8 It is difficult to assess the
extent to which support (or lack of support) for gay rights overseas would be replicated in
Australia, where the political climate is currently hostile to the interests of the gay and
lesbian communities, but where these interests rarely feature on the national political
agenda.
Influence of Demographic Variables on Attitudes Towards Homosexuality
The factors that predict positive or negative attitudes towards homosexuality and
homosexuals are generally the same as those predicting support for gay and lesbian civil
rights (with the possible exception of gender and race: see below).
Gender
Gender is by far the most researched demographic variable influencing attitudes
towards homosexuality. Gender can affect attitudes in two respects: first, there may be
differences in the attitudes of male and female heterosexuals towards homosexuality
8
On February 1 2005, the federal government of Canada introduced a bill to legalise same-sex marriage
across the nation.
10
(differences related to the gender of the respondent); secondly, there may be differences
in the attitudes of heterosexuals towards gay men and lesbians (differences related to the
gender of the target).
Numerous studies examining gender of respondent differences have shown that
men generally have more negative attitudes towards homosexuality than do women
(Black et al 1998; Finlay and Walther 2003; Hayes 1997; Herek 1988, 2000a, 2002a;
Herek and Capitanio 1999b; Hinrichs and Rosenberg 2002; Kerns and Fine 1994; Kite
and Whitley 1996, 1998; LaMar and Kite 1998; Whitley and Kite 1995). A smaller body
of research, however, has failed to reveal any significant gender differences (Oliver and
Hyde 1993, 1995; Wilson and Medora 1990; see Appendix B in Kite and Whitley 1996).
Secondly, research into gender of target differences shows that attitudes towards gay men
are generally more negative than attitudes towards lesbians (Berkman and Zinberg 1997;
Herek 2002a; Kite and Whitley 1998). The latter difference is usually accounted for by
the former, that is, the more negative attitudes of heterosexuals towards gay men are
attributable to heterosexual men’s hostility towards gay men (Bernstein and Kostelac
2002; Herek 2000a, 2002a; Herek and Capitanio 1999b; Kerns and Fine 1994; LaMar and
Kite 1998). However, some evidence suggests that Australian heterosexual men may be
no more negative towards gay men than towards lesbians, although they are still less
accepting than heterosexual women (CJC 1990; Spark Jones 2000; contra Van de Ven
1994). Heterosexual men and women display similar levels of homophobia towards
lesbians (Kite and Whitley 1996, 1998).
It is unclear whether heterosexual women are more rejecting of lesbians than of
gay men. Some research suggests that heterosexual women’s attitudes towards lesbians
and gay men do not differ significantly (Baker and Fishbein 1998; CJC 1990; Herek
1988, 2000a; Spark Jones 2000; Whitley and Kite 1995). However, other research has
shown that women are more negative towards lesbians than gay men (D’Augelli and
Rose 1990; Gentry 1987; Kite and Whitley 1996; Logan 1996; Schope and Eliason 2004;
Whitley 1987, 1990). Kite and Whitley’s meta-analysis of research into gender
differences in attitudes towards homosexuality revealed that heterosexuals do tend to
express more negative attitudes towards homosexuals of their own sex, but that the
pattern is more pronounced for men than women (1996). LaMar and Kite found that it
was only with respect to contact that women were more negative towards lesbians than
gay men (1998).
It is also unclear whether there are any significant gender differences with respect
to attitudes towards civil rights for homosexuals. There appear to be no gender
differences on most gay rights issues (Davies 2004; Ellison and Musick 1993; Hayes
1997; Herek 2002a; Kite and Whitley 1996, 1998; Lewis and Rogers 1999; Oliver and
Hyde 1993), perhaps because questions about gay rights tap beliefs about the civil rights
and liberties of minority groups rather than affective responses to gay men and lesbians
(Davies 2004). However, heterosexual men may be less supportive of rights related to
gender roles such as gay parenting and gay people serving in the armed forces (Kite and
Whitley 1996, 1998; see also Herek 2002a; LaMar and Kite 1998). Other research
suggests that men are more opposed to gay rights in general than are women, but that the
gender difference is less pronounced than for attitudes towards homosexual persons and
behaviour (Whitley and Kite 1995). Strand found that women were more supportive of
civil rights for gay men and lesbians, but that there was no difference in support for civil
11
liberties (1998). There is some evidence to suggest that attitudes towards gay rights differ
depending on sex of target. Heterosexual men appear to be less accepting of civil rights
for gay men than for lesbians and more willing to accept discrimination against gay men,
whereas heterosexual women’s support does not differ according to sex of target (LaMar
and Kite 1998; Harris and Vanderhoof 1995, cited in Kite and Whitley 1998).
Kite and Whitley attribute the inconsistencies in these results primarily to
differences in the samples and measures used. The most pronounced gender differences
appear in student samples (as opposed to large national samples) 9 and studies measuring
affective responses to gay and lesbian people (rather than support for gay rights) (1996:
see also Davies 2004; Kite and Whitley 1998; Oliver and Hyde 1995, Spark Jones 2000;
Whitley and Kite 1995;). Kite and Whitley also found that multi-item scales produce
larger sex differences than single-item scales (perhaps by allowing for a more nuanced
assessment of attitudes) (1996). The terminology used to describe the attitude object (gay
man, lesbian or homosexual) may also affect results (Kite and Whitley 1996; Whitley and
Kite 1995). People tend to assume that the term ‘homosexual’ refers to gay men (Herek
and Capitanio 1999b; Kite and Whitley 1996; Simon 1998), and it has been shown above
that heterosexual men generally hold more negative attitudes towards gay men than
towards lesbians.
Much energy has been spent theorising these apparent gender differences in
attitudes towards homosexuality. Gender differences cannot be reduced to other
demographic differences since women are more likely to have a lower income, to be
more religious and more sexually conservative than men, all of which are typically
associated with negative attitudes towards homosexuality (Herek 2002a; Kite and
Whitley 1998, Scott 1998; Smith 1992). The most popular theory holds that gender
differences in attitudes derive not from biological sex, but from gender role attitudes,
which comprise stereotypes about men and women, beliefs about appropriate social roles
for men and women, and perceptions of those who violate gender norms (Deaux and Kite
1987; Kite and Whitley 1996). Gender differences in attitudes towards homosexuality are
substantially diminished if not eliminated when differences in gender role beliefs are
controlled (Kerns and Fine 1994; Kite and Whitley 1996; Louderback and Whitley 1997).
Male and female heterosexuals with non-traditional gender role attitudes (for
example, those who support the equal division of labour in the home) tend to exhibit
more positive attitudes towards homosexuality (Basow and Johnson 2000; Black et al
1998; Deaux and Kite 1987; Hinrichs and Rosenberg 2002; Kerns and Fine 1994;
Whitley 1987, 2001; Whitley and Kite 1995). 10 Men tend to hold more traditional
attitudes because they experience stronger gender socialisation and because traditional
gender roles preserve their greater privilege and power (Kerns and Fine 1994), but
homophobia in women may also vary according to their gender role attitudes (Basow and
Johnson 2000). In fact, it has been tentatively suggested that when men and women hold
similar gender role beliefs, women may hold more negative attitudes towards
9
Does this perhaps suggest that age and levels of education mediate the relationship between gender and
attitude towards homosexuality?
10
Note that the direction of causality is not entirely clear. While it is likely that men and women who
develop non-traditional gender role attitudes become more accepting of homosexuality, it is possible that
those with positive attitudes towards lesbians and gay men develop non-traditional gender role beliefs as a
result: Kerns and Fine 1994.
12
homosexuality (Whitley 2001). However, it may be that attitudes towards homosexuality
are more strongly related to beliefs about masculinity and femininity than to beliefs about
equality of the sexes (Polimeni et al 2000; Weinberger and Millham 1979; Whitley
2001).
Gay men and lesbians are disliked partly because they are perceived to deviate
from gender norms (Basow and Johnson 2000; Corley and Pollack 1996; Kite and
Whitley 1996). These gender norms are more rigid and restrictive for men than for
women, and homosexual men are perceived to deviate more from traditional gender roles
than are lesbians (Basow 1992; Basow and Johnson 2000; Herek 2000a; Kerns and Fine
1994; Kite and Deaux 1987; Kite and Whitley 1996; Logan 1996; Polimeni et al 2000;
Whitley and Kite 1995). For example, a man with stereotypically feminine characteristics
is more likely to be assumed to be homosexual than is a woman with stereotypically
masculine traits (Deaux and Lewis 1984; Kite and Deaux 1987; Kite and Whitley 1996;
McCreary 1994; Whitley and Kite 1995). 11 Femininity is not only less rigid than
masculinity, but it is also less tied to heterosexuality. Whereas female heterosexuality is
assumed, male heterosexuality must be proved (Basow 1992; Basow and Johnson 2000;
Herek 2000a, 2002a). Moreover, violation of gender roles by men is viewed more
seriously than gender role violation by women, in part because it is perceived to threaten
their social privilege and power (Herek 2000a, 2002a; Kerns and Fine 1994; Kite and
Whitley 1996, 1998). Men thus feel more pressure to conform to gender roles and
therefore to express disapproval of homosexuality (Herek 2000a; Kite and Whitley 1996;
Whitley and Kite 1995). It also follows from the more serious nature of male gender role
deviation that gay men are viewed more negatively than lesbians, especially by
heterosexual men (Gentry 1987; Herek 1988; Kite and Whitley 1996; Whitley 1987;
Whitley and Kite 1995).
Most of the research focuses on the correlation between attitudes towards
homosexuality and gender role attitudes, and it is unclear to what extent antigay attitudes
are related to gender role self-concept or self-discrepancy, that is, the extent to which an
individual believes he or she conforms to gender stereotypes. Kite and Whitley found that
people who reported a dissimilarity between themselves and the relevant feminine or
11
This proposition comes from Kite and Whitley (1996), LaMar and Kite (1998) and
Whitley and Kite (1995). They cite Deaux and Lewis (1984). It is also supported by a
study by McCreary (1994). He argues that data in Kite and Deaux (1987) also implicitly
support this finding. However, neither Deaux and Lewis (1984) nor Kite and Deaux
(1987) were available for this literature review. Apparently what they say is that ‘when
men are described as possessing female traits, occupying female roles, or having female
physical characteristics, the estimated probability that they are also homosexual sharply
increases. Similarly, but to a lesser extent, probabilistic judgments of homosexuality
increase when women are described as occupying male roles or having male physical
characteristics’ (Kite and Whitley 1996: 337-338). McCreary (1994) also found this to be
true for women described as having male traits. The following texts may assist in
clarifying and elaborating this point: Deaux and Lewis (1984); Kite and Deaux (1987);
Storms, Stivers, Lambers and Hill (1981) ‘Sexual Scripts for Women’ 7 Sex Roles 699707; Klassen et al (1989) Sex and Morality in the US: An Empirical Enquiry under the
Auspices of the Kinsey Institute.
13
masculine stereotype were more tolerant of homosexuality (1996). Other studies have
found no relationship between antigay attitudes and ‘normal levels of psychological
investment in traditional gender roles’ (Kite and Whitley 1998; Whitley 2001). Still
others have found that extreme investment in a masculine identity or lack of
identification with positive feminine traits are correlated with negative attitudes towards
homosexuality (Kilianski 2003; Polimeni et al 2000; Whitley 2001), or have found that
gender role self-concept correlates with antigay attitudes for one gender but not the other
(Basow and Johnson 2000; Simon 1995; Theodore and Basow 2000; Wilkinson 2004).
Herek argues that whereas heterosexual men organise their attitudes towards
homosexuals in terms of gender and sexual identity, which results in different ways of
thinking about lesbians and gay men, heterosexual women seem to organise their
attitudes more in terms of a minority group paradigm, according to which they perceive
gay men and lesbians as members of a single quasi-ethnic group (2000a, 2002a). Women
with negative attitudes may reject this group for religious, moral or other reasons, while
women with positive attitudes may consider the group to be an oppressed minority with
valid political demands, but both tolerant and intolerant women are unlikely to
differentiate between gay men and lesbians (Herek 2000a: 264). The evidence suggesting
that women sometimes hold more negative attitudes towards lesbians, however, suggests
that the sexuality-gender paradigm also shapes women’s attitudes towards
homosexuality, albeit to a lesser extent than it determines men’s attitudes (Herek 2000a;
Logan 1996).
Taking a functional approach to explaining men’s and women’s attitudes, Herek
also suggests that heterosexuals’ attitudes towards homosexuals may serve a defensive
function (1986, 1988, 2002a). Because homosexuality is stigmatised, many heterosexuals
wish to avoid being thought to be homosexual, and therefore express hostility towards
homosexuals as a means of demonstrating that they themselves are heterosexual (Herek
2002a). The need to express disapproval of homosexuality may be particularly strong
among heterosexuals who are insecure about their personal adequacy in conforming to
gender norms or who have experienced homosexual desires or engaged in homosexual
activity, which they reject as inconsistent with their self-concept (Herek 1986, 2002a). If
homophobia functions to demonstrate that a heterosexual person is not homosexual, then
it follows that this person is likely to hold more negative attitudes towards homosexuals
of his/her own sex, who pose the most direct threat to his/her heterosexual identity
(Herek 2000a; Kite and Whitley 1998; Logan 1996; Polimeni et al 2000). However,
heterosexual women are likely to feel less threatened by lesbianism, partly because
female gender and sexuality norms are more flexible and partly because they may have
more experience in repelling unwanted sexual advances (Kite and Whitley 1996, 1998;
LaMar and Kite 1998). In accordance with the minority group paradigm, heterosexual
women’s attitudes may also function to affirm a personal identity based on values (such
as sexual morality or minority rights) rather than femininity and heterosexuality (Herek
2002a).
The gap between heterosexual men’s attitudes towards gay men and lesbians
might also be explained by the ‘sexualization of lesbianism’ (Kite and Whitley 1996,
1998). Several studies have shown that lesbianism has acquired positive sexual
connotations for heterosexual men that are not associated with male homosexuality (for
example, Nyberg and Alston 1977; Turnbull and Brown 1977). Most recently,
14
Louderback and Whitley (1997) found that when the erotic value of lesbianism was
controlled, men’s attitudes towards lesbians were almost identical to their attitudes
towards gay men, which suggested that the erotic value attributed to lesbianism
ameliorates men’s negative attitudes towards lesbians.
However, Bernstein and Kostelac discovered that, within the police force at least,
the intersection between gender and sexuality could produce different results (2002).
They found that although police generally believed that gay men made more competent
police officers than do lesbians, they still held more negative attitudes towards gay men
than lesbians. Citing Kite and Whitley (1998), they suggest that this may be because
heterosexual men fear unwanted sexual advances from gay men. Heterosexual male
police officers ‘may be less hostile towards lesbians in general, but their doubts about
women’s ability to perform effectively as police officers may account for their more
negative view about lesbians in policing compared to gay men in policing’ (2002: 316317).
Finally, women may have more positive attitudes towards homosexuals than men
because they tend to have had more contact with gay men and lesbians (Herek and Glunt
1993), and contact has been shown to be correlated with more positive attitudes (Herek
and Capitanio 1996; Herek and Glunt 1993; see also below). Herek and Glunt argue that
gender differences in attitudes towards homosexuality may therefore be self-reinforcing:
‘[c]ultural ideologies of gender may lead heterosexual men more than heterosexual
women to manifest hostility toward gay men. Their heightened hostility, in turn, reduces
the likelihood that lesbians or gay men will come out to them. Consequently, their
antigay attitudes are not likely to be moderated by the experience of interpersonal contact
with openly gay people’ (1993: 243). Lottes and Kuriloff suggest that women may be
tolerant of homosexuality because they have been socialised to be caring, nurturing and
supportive, and to develop a concern for social issues (1994).
The gap between male and female heterosexual attitudes towards homosexuality
is apparent across nations (Kelley 2001), and appears to be increasing (Altemeyer 2001;
Crockett and Voas 2003; Kite and Whitley 1996; Scott 1998). Scott found that women’s
attitudes tend to be more changeable over time (1998). This again suggests that women’s
attitudes are less strongly tied to their self-identities and that their attitudes are more
likely to respond to external events such as the AIDS crisis and to more general sociopolitical change (Altemeyer 2001; Loftus 2001; Scott 1998). The gender gap appears to
be widest among young people and college students (Crockett and Voas 2003; Kite and
Whitley 1996; Oliver and Hyde 1995; Whitley and Kite 1995).
Australian researchers would probably find similar gender differences in attitudes
towards homosexuality (to the extent that any firm conclusions can actually be drawn
from the data). One small Australian study found no overall sex differences in
homophobia, but did find that male university students were more negative towards gay
men and female students were more negative towards lesbians (Polimeni et al 2000).
However, it did make two findings inconsistent with the tenor of US research: first, the
intensity of men’s antigay sentiment was no greater than that of women’s antilesbian
sentiment, and secondly, women with both high and low levels of homophobia held nontraditional gender role beliefs. This confirms the theory that the need to maintain
traditional gender roles is an inadequate explanation for female homophobia. In general,
it seems that female homophobia is less well explained than male homophobia.
15
Race and Ethnicity
Research investigating racial differences in attitudes towards homosexuality has
produced inconsistent results. Some research shows that white Americans have more
negative attitudes towards homosexuality than do black Americans (Finlay and Walther
2003; Klassen et al 1989; Levitt and Klassen 1974). Other research has uncovered no
racial differences in levels of homophobia (Glenn and Weaver 1979; Herek and Capitanio
1995; Irwin and Thompson 1977; Millham et al 1976). Still other research shows that
black people hold more negative attitudes than white people (Alston 1974; Herek and
Glunt 1993; Hudson and Ricketts 1980; Nyberg and Alston 1976-77; Schneider and
Lewis 1984; Tiemeyer 1993; Waldner et al 1999). Herek suggests that more negative
attitudes among black populations may be accounted for mainly by the relatively
favourable attitudes of white women (2000b).
It has been suggested that black Americans may be less accepting of
homosexuality than white Americans but equally if not more supportive of civil rights for
gay men and lesbians (Bonilla and Porter 1990; Glenn and Weaver 1979; Haeberle 1999;
Lewis 2003; Loftus 2001). However, Dejowski made the unusual finding that black
people were more willing to restrict the civil liberties than comparable white people
(1992). Recent national surveys in the United States show that nearly three-quarters of
the black population still believe that homosexuality is ‘always wrong’ (American
Enterprise Institute 2004: 3; Lewis 2003: 75), a figure which is approximately 15
percentage points higher than the national average, whereas roughly the same percentage
of blacks and whites agree that homosexual marriage should be recognised by law
(American Enterprise Institute 2004: 23). It appears that while a greater proportion of the
black population holds negative attitudes towards homosexuality, their attitudes are less
negative than the attitudes of (the smaller proportion of) intolerant white Americans
(Lewis 2003: 75).
Greater disapproval of homosexuality among blacks may be explained by the
influence of religion and education. Black Americans are considerably more religious
than white Americans and the highly religious are more likely to hold negative opinions
of homosexuality (see below). Blacks are more likely to belong to fundamentalist
Protestant denominations, to attend church more frequently, to believe in a punitive God,
to be guided by religion in their everyday lives, and simply to accept rather than question
their religion (Bonilla and Porter 1990; Lewis 2003; Schulte and Battle 2004). Indeed,
Schulte and Battle found that racial differences in attitudes towards lesbians among
college students disappeared when religion was controlled, that is, racial differences in
attitudes were accounted for by religious values and behaviour (2004). Secondly,
education is thought to lead to greater acceptance of diversity and more liberal sexual and
political attitudes, as well as providing opportunities to interact with gay men and
lesbians (Ellison and Musick 1993; Herek and Capitanio 1996; Lewis 2003; see also
below). Lower levels of education among the black population are therefore also likely to
be correlated with higher levels of homophobia: black Americans are only two-thirds as
16
likely as whites to have graduated from college (Lewis 2003). 12 Herek and Capitanio
also found that black Americans were more likely to hold positive attitudes towards gay
people to the extent that they did not consider homosexuality to be an entirely ‘white’
phenomenon, that is, to the extent that they associated homosexuality with black as well
as white people (which would provide them with some commonality with gay people)
(1995).
On the other hand, it has been suggested that black Americans may be more
supportive of gay civil rights as a result of their personal experiences of discrimination
and the lessons of the civil rights movement (Bernstein and Kostelac 2002; Bonilla and
Porter 1990; Haeberle 1999; Lewis 2003). Indeed these experiences seem to exert so
powerful an influence on black attitudes to gay rights that variables which predict white
attitudes, such as gender, education and age, have less impact on black attitudes (Finlay
and Walther 2003; Herek 2000b; Herek and Capitanio 1995; Lewis 2003).
Research exploring gender differences in attitudes among the black population
has also generated inconsistent results. Herek and Capitanio found that white males,
black males and black females did not differ in their attitudes towards gay men and
lesbians (1995). However, other research has shown that black women are more
accepting of homosexuality than are black men (Battle and Lemelle 2002; Waldner et al
1999). Lewis found that gender differences were insignificant on most items but that
black men were significantly more likely to consider homosexuality to be ‘always wrong’
and yet also more likely to favour gay rights laws (2003). Researchers have suggested
that the need or desire to conform to black masculinity norms, which include compulsory
heterosexuality, may be one factor contributing to more homophobic attitudes among
black men (Battle and Lemelle 2002; Lemelle and Battle 2004; Lewis 2003).
Disempowered by racial discrimination and unemployment, black men may seek to
reassert their (heterosexual) masculinity by emphasising traditional gender roles. Black
women, on the other hand, may have already subverted traditional gender roles through
their greater involvement in the workforce and consequently greater financial
independence (relative to white women) such that violation of gender norms is not
perceived as threatening (Battle and Lemelle 2002; Lemelle and Battle 2004; but
compare Bonilla and Porter 1990). Whites may also have more positive attitudes because
the gay movement is more visible in white culture than in ethnic minority cultures
(Armesto and Weisman 2001).
Very little research has been conducted into the attitudes of ethnic groups other
than black and white Americans. In fact, the General Social Survey lumps all other ethnic
groups (when it includes them) into a single ‘other’ category. It appears, however, that
Hispanic Americans are generally no more homophobic than whites (Alcalay et al 198990; Bonilla and Porter 1990; Span and Vidal 2003) and may be more tolerant than blacks
(Bonilla and Porter 1990). Span and Vidal (2003) considered this somewhat surprising,
given that Hispanic Americans are subject to similar social pressures to black Americans.
Bonilla and Porter (1990) suggest that the less dogmatic approach of the Latin American
Catholic church and the more fluid construction of homosexuality in Hispanic cultures
may be responsible for these more tolerant attitudes. However, Hispanic Americans were
12
Lewis (2003) suggests that the younger average age of the black population and the greater proportion of
black female respondents may partially offset the impact of religion and education on attitudes towards
homosexuality.
17
found to be less supportive of gay civil liberties than both white and black Americans,
which Bonilla and Porter attributed to their lower levels of education and thus
‘acculturation’ into American democratic values (1990). There were no differences in the
attitudes of different Hispanic groups (for example, Mexicans versus Puerto Ricans)
(Bonilla and Porter 1990). Even less research has been dedicated to the attitudes of those
with Asian backgrounds, and it is limited by its use of the generic term ‘Asian’. There is
some suggestion that Asian students are more homophobic than white students
(Lippincott et al 2000; Span and Vidal 2003), but that homophobia decreases with length
of residency or ‘acculturation’ (Meston et al 1998). In Britain, Davies (2004) found that
‘Asians’ (Indians and Pakistanis) were considerably more homophobic than whites but
that there was no difference on support for gay civil rights. Given the very different
history and ethnic composition of Australian society, the North American research is
unlikely to have great significance for Australian researchers. In any case, there appears
to be little consensus.
Religion
There are three ways in which ‘religion’ can influence heterosexuals’ attitudes
towards homosexuality. It is unclear which is the most influential. First, whether a person
is religiously affiliated or is not religious has been shown to affect their attitude towards
homosexuality. Religious persons are generally more prejudiced against homosexuals
than non-religious persons (Crockett and Voas 2003; Fisher et al 1994; Hayes 1995;
Schulte and Battle 2004; Scott 1998). For example, Crockett and Voas found that 44% of
Christians in Britain believed that homosexuality was ‘always wrong’ in 1999/2000,
compared with 28% of non-religious people (2003). There is some evidence, however,
that the attitudes of non-practising Christians may now be similar to the attitudes of nonreligious people (Crockett and Voas 2003). Tolerance is increasing at the same rate in
religious and non-religious populations (Crockett and Voas 2003).
Secondly, the type of religion or religious denomination to which an affiliated
person belongs influences the extent to which they accept homosexuality. People
belonging to fundamentalist or evangelical Protestant denominations, including Baptists,
tend to hold the most negative attitudes towards homosexuality, followed by Catholics,
Jews and those belonging to more liberal Protestant denominations, such as
Presbyterians, Methodists and Episcopalians (Crockett and Voas 2003 (partial support);
Finlay and Walther 2003; Fisher et al 1994; Glenn and Weaver 1979; Hinrichs and
Rosenberg 2002; Kunkel and Temple 1992). Fundamentalist Christians may be more
intolerant because they are more likely to subscribe to Biblical literalism and to have
authoritarian personalities, which lead them to conform to the values of (religious)
authorities (Ellison and Musick 1993; see also below). 13 Crockett and Voas suggest that
the gap between the views of liberal and evangelical Christians appears to be widening,
as does the gap between the views of younger and older Christians (2003). Young
Anglicans and Presbyterians are no more likely to be homophobic than nonreligious
young people, but their negative attitudes increase more sharply with age, such that older
Christians are significantly more homophobic than nonreligious people of the same age.
13
Fisher et al (1994) note that it is possible that religions do not instil these conservative values, but rather
that conservative people are attracted to religions that reflect those values.
18
Thirdly, the person’s ‘religiosity’, that is, the frequency with which they attend
church and/or the extent to which religion is integral to their lives, has a significant
impact upon their attitude towards homosexuality. The research generally suggests that
those who attend church more regularly are more prejudiced against gays and lesbians
(Finlay and Walther 2003; Glenn and Weaver 1979; Herek and Glunt 1993; Hinrichs and
Rosenberg 2002; Kelley 2001; Kunkel and Temple 1992; Scott 1998). However, Fisher et
al found that frequency of worship was significantly related to antigay attitudes only
among those belonging to antigay denominations, and not to the attitudes of those
belonging to ‘gay tolerant’ religions (1994). Those attending church regularly might be
more intolerant because they are more frequently exposed to antigay messages, and also
because they tend (once again) to have more authoritarian personalities (Altemeyer 1988;
Fisher et al 1994). People with an ‘intrinsic’ religious orientation, that is, those who
consider their faith to be an end in itself, and who try to live by it, are more prejudiced
towards gays and lesbians than those with an ‘extrinsic’ orientation, who use religion as a
means to an end, for example, to gain social standing (Fisher et al 1994; Griffiths et al
2001). Griffiths et al found that the theory that those with an intrinsic orientation might
‘hate the sin, but love the sinner’ was not supported. They found that intrinsics were as
rejecting of homosexual people as they were of homosexual behaviour (2001; contra
Howard-Hassmann 2001).
Religion is likely to have considerably less influence on attitudes towards
homosexuality in Australia. Australia is a more secular society than the United States
(Griffiths et al 2001; Spark Jones 2000), and the religiously active population is
declining. The percentage of the population claiming no religion climbed from 12.9% in
1991 to 16.6% in 1996, dropping slightly to 15.5% in 2001. In 2001, people in the 18-24
years age group were most likely to state that they had no religion (20%). The percentage
of the population claiming a Christian affiliation is declining slowly, while those
belonging to religions other than Christianity have grown in number (Australian Bureau
of Statistics). No research has been conducted into the impact that affiliation with non
Judeo-Christian religions may have on attitudes towards homosexuality (Finlay and
Walther 2003).
Marital Status
It is often thought that those who have never married or who are divorced, and
who therefore deviate from social convention themselves, should be more accepting of
homosexuality (Lewis and Rogers 1999; Smith 1992). However, findings have been
mixed (Lewis and Rogers 1999). Smith (1992) and Herek and Capitanio (1995) have
found that those who had never married or who were divorced were more tolerant;
Kunkel and Temple found that those who were married were more tolerant (1992); and
Royse and Birge found no difference (1987). Bernstein and Kostelac found that police
officers who were single, divorced or cohabiting held more favourable attitudes towards
gay people, and yet were more likely to engage in discriminatory behaviour towards them
than were married people (2002).
Place of Residence
19
In terms of regional differences, those who hail from the southern states of the US
tend to be most intolerant of homosexuality, followed by residents of the mid-western
states. Those from the Pacific and north-eastern states are most tolerant (American
Enterprise Institute 2004; Dejowski 1992; Herek 2002b; Herek and Capitanio 1996).
Ellison and Musick found that southern origins or residence continued to account for
differences in attitudes even when religion, level of education and rurality were
controlled (1993). They attributed this to the cultural homogeneity of the South where
residents have less personal or political contact with minority groups, the tendency to
localism, the tendency to distrust the unfamiliar, the perception of leftwing groups
(including gays and lesbians) as threats to the status quo or social institutions, and the
pervasive influence of messages of religious intolerance even upon non-religious persons.
In Australia in 1989, Western Australia was significantly more accepting of
homosexuality than the other states, while Tasmania was the least accepting state (CJC
1990).
Those who live in metropolitan and suburban areas appear to have more positive
attitudes towards homosexuality and to be more likely to support gay rights than those
who live in rural areas (Haeberle 1999; Herek and Capitanio 1996; Levitt and Klassen
1974; Lewis and Rogers 1999)
Political Ideology
As would be expected, individuals who subscribe to a liberal political ideology
are more tolerant of homosexuality than are those who subscribe to a conservative
political ideology (American Enterprise Institute 2004; Haeberle 1999; Herek and
Capitanio 1996; Herek and Glunt 1993; Seltzer 1993; Yang 2001). This translates
roughly to a Democrat versus Republican party preference (American Enterprise Institute
2004; Haeberle 1999; Herek and Capitanio 1996), although the attitudes of each group
are not always internally consistent (Haeberle 1999).
Age
Most of the research conducted with adult samples suggests that older people are
more prejudiced towards gay men and lesbians than are younger people (Haeberle 1999;
Herek and Glunt 1993; Kelley 2001; Lewis 2003). This is attributed primarily to a liberal
cohort effect, that is, each generation being more tolerant than the last, rather than to the
‘conservatising effect of age’ (Crockett and Voas 2003; Kelley 2001; Lewis 2003; Lewis
and Rogers 1999).
However, age does not always have a linear effect on attitudes (Lewis and Rogers
1999). Studies of the attitudes of high school and college students suggest that young
people, particularly males, may in fact become more homophobic during their
adolescence, but become more tolerant when they reach adulthood (Baker and Fishbein
1998; Kurdek 1988; Van de Ven 1994). The increase in prejudice during adolescence
may perhaps be caused by defensive reactions in young men as they grapple with their
sexuality and masculinity in the context both of dating and intimate relationships and of a
male homosocial environment which demands conformity to restrictive masculinity
20
norms, which become somewhat looser as they enter tertiary education (Baker and
Fishbein 1998; Schellenberg et al 1999).
Lewis also noted non-linear fluctuations in the age-attitude relationship among the
black population (2003), while Strand observed different age effects for support of civil
rights as compared with support for civil liberties, with older people more supportive of
the former and younger people more supportive of the latter (1998).
Education
In general, ‘education’ increases acceptance of homosexuality, that is, attitudes
improve as a function of time spent in education (Eliason 1995; Herek and Capitanio
1996; Kelley 2001; Kurdek 1988; Lottes and Kuriloff 1994; Seltzer 1992; Spark Jones
2000; contra Van de Ven 1994). For example, in 2000, 65% of General Social Survey
Respondents with a high school education believed homosexual relations were always
wrong, compared with 47% of college graduates (American Enterprise Institute 2004). In
a longitudinal study conducted by Lottes and Kuriloff between 1987 and 1991,
participants scored 25% higher on measures of homosexuality tolerance (as well as other
liberalism measures) in their senior college year than they had as first-year students
(1994). Senior students reported majority approval of homosexuality, which contrasted
sharply with national findings (approximately 75% disapproval in both years: American
Enterprise Institute 2004). 14 It is generally accepted that increased tolerance is the result
of what is learnt both inside and outside the classroom. For example, education fosters
liberal attitudes, gives individuals the ability to think critically, the opportunity to interact
with a diverse range of people and a greater awareness of the negative effects of
prejudice (Lewis 2003; Lewis and Rogers 1999; Lottes and Kuriloff 1994; Schellenberg
et al 1999).
Some research has found that students studying particular subjects or taking
particular courses are more tolerant of homosexuality. Humanities, social sciences and
psychology students are generally less homophobic than students studying subjects such
as business and science (Matchinsky and Iverson 1996; Parker and Bhugra 2000;
Schellenberg et al 1999), although some studies have found no difference (Cotten-Huston
and Waite 2000; Ellis et al 2002). It is unclear whether humanities and social sciences
students are less prejudiced as a result of course content, or because these courses tend to
be chosen by less homophobic people or people predisposed to attitude change, or
because these courses are dominated by women (Corley and Pollack 1996; Schellenberg
et al 1999).
Impact of Other Variables on Attitudes Towards Homosexuality
Educational Interventions
14
Note that the questions in Lottes and Kuriloff’s (1994) study were worded differently from the questions
used in the GSS. Lottes and Kuriloff also conducted their study at the height of the AIDS crisis: the
improvement in attitudes might have been attributable to changes in socio-political context rather than the
impact of education (Schellenberg et al 1999:141)
21
Educational interventions aimed at changing students’ attitudes towards
homosexuality have had mixed results. Students who have completed human sexuality
courses are more tolerant of homosexuality, but this may be because students who enrol
in such courses are likely to be more liberal in their attitudes to begin with (Cerny and
Polyson 1984; Iyriboz and Carter 1986; Patton and Morrison 1993; Stevenson 1988).
Interventions involving simply a monologue by a gay or lesbian speaker or a
brief, one-off discussion of gay and lesbian issues appear to be less effective than
interventions which involve more personal contact, interaction and dialogue between
heterosexual students and homosexual students/speakers (Chng and Moore 1991; CottenHuston and Waite 2000; Nelson and Krieger 1997). Panel discussions with gay and
lesbian speakers have had some success in achieving short-term results, but less success
in producing long-term change (Nelson and Krieger 1997, Van de Ven 1995). Nelson and
Krieger also found that a panel discussion was more successful in dispelling myths about
homosexual people and in increasing support for gay rights than it was in changing
attitudes towards the morality of homosexuality and in improving personal reactions to
disclosure of homosexual identity by a close friend or relative (1997). There is some
suggestion that female heterosexuals may be more susceptible to attitude change (Green
et al 1993; Van de Ven 1995; contra Nelson and Krieger 1997).
Several studies have also shown that exposure to positive visual media depictions
of homosexuality and homosexual people may have some effect in reducing homophobia
(Levina et al 2000; Luhrs et al 1991; Mazur and Emmers-Sommer 2002; Riggle et al
1996).
Interpersonal Contact
Research shows that the extent to which a heterosexual individual has had contact
with gay and lesbian individuals has a strong influence on his or her attitudes towards
homosexuality. Heterosexuals who report knowing someone who is gay generally
express more positive attitudes towards homosexual people (Altemeyer 2001; Berkman
and Zinberg 1997; Finlay and Walther 2003; Herek and Capitanio 1996; Herek and Glunt
1993; Hinrichs and Rosenberg 2002; Howard-Hassmann 2001; Overby and Barth 2002).
Even in demographic groups which generally display more negative attitudes, such as the
highly religious, those who have had contact with homosexual people hold less negative
attitudes (Herek and Glunt 1993). Herek and Capitanio found that interpersonal contact
reduces anti-gay prejudice most where heterosexuals interact with more than one gay or
lesbian person, interact with gay or lesbian people with whom they have close
relationships and learn about the gay or lesbian person’s sexual orientation directly from
the gay or lesbian person (1996). In an earlier study, however, they found that contact
experience did not strongly influence the attitudes of black Americans, perhaps because
they reported contact with people with whom they were not on intimate terms or because
they mostly had contact with non-black homosexuals with whom they may have been
unable to perceive commonalities (Herek and Capitanio 1995).
It has been suggested that contact increases acceptance of gay and lesbian people
because interactions with gay and lesbian people can challenge stereotypes and
encourage the heterosexual person to think of gay people in a more individualised and
22
personal way (Altemeyer 2001; Herek and Glunt 1993; Howard-Hassmann 2001; Overby
and Barth 2002).
However, the causal link between positive attitudes and contact is uncertain.
Interpersonal contact is more likely to be reported by those who are young, female,
highly educated and politically liberal (Herek and Glunt 1993). Thus, it may be that
heterosexuals with more positive attitudes (or who belong to demographic groups which
tend to have more positive attitudes) are more likely to have contact with gay men and
lesbians, perhaps because they are more likely to choose to have homosexual friends, or
because they find themselves in environments where gay and lesbian people are more
visible, such as at university, or because gay men and lesbians are more likely to disclose
their sexual orientation to people whom they think will respond positively (Berkman and
Zinberg 1997; Herek and Capitanio 1995, 1996; Herek and Glunt 1993; Overby and
Barth 2002). However, a substantial proportion of the contact reported is with relatives,
workmates and others with whom respondents associate involuntarily, which suggests
that contact does reduce prejudice rather than (or as well as) vice versa (Berkman and
Zinberg 1997). In addition, respondents themselves often attribute changes in their
attitudes to contact with gay and lesbian people (Altemeyer 2001; Howard-Hassmann
2001).
Overby and Barth note that, in the case of minority groups other than lesbians and
gay men, contact between the majority group and the outgroups is more likely to increase
tension and prejudice than reduce it (2002). One explanation for the different effect of
contact on attitudes towards homosexuality might lie in the notion that, unlike race,
homosexuality is a ‘concealable stigma’ (Herek and Capitanio 1996). This means that
lesbians and gay men may have the opportunity to dispel stereotypes and to have others
get to know them just as ‘people’ before disclosing their sexual orientation. Because it
must be actively disclosed, homosexuality may also be more likely to provoke discussion
than, say, race (Herek and Capitanio 1996).
Researchers therefore suggest that despite the possibility of stigma and
discrimination, more gay men and lesbians ‘coming out’ may eventually lead to greater
acceptance of homosexuality (Agnew et al 1993; Altemeyer 2001; Herek and Capitanio
1996; Herek and Glunt 1993; Wills and Crawford 2000).
Influence on Attitudes of Beliefs about the Origins of Homosexuality
Those who believe homosexuality to be genetic or biological rather than learned
or freely chosen are more likely to have positive attitudes towards homosexuals and be
more supportive of gay and lesbian civil rights (Hegarty and Pratto 2001; Herek and
Capitanio 1995; King 2001; Matchinsky and Iverson 1996; Piskur and Degelman 1992;
Tygart 2000; Whitley 1990; Wood and Bartkowski 2004). This is probably because
people believe that an individual cannot be held responsible for a stigma or ‘abnormality’
which is immutable and therefore beyond the individual’s control (King 2001; Whitley
1990). 15 One study found that men and African- and Asian-Americans were more likely
to believe homosexuality to be controllable (Armesto and Weisman 2001).
15
Biological arguments are also very problematic. For example, they still present homosexuality as an
abnormality, but one which should be tolerated since it cannot be controlled (see for example Hegarty
2002; Haslam, Rothschild and Ernst 2002).
23
However, in Hegarty’s study, respondents’ judgments about whether immutability
beliefs were indicative of a tolerant or condemning view of homosexuality moderated the
relationship between tolerant attitudes and immutability beliefs. That is, the link between
tolerant attitudes and a belief in the immutability of homosexuality only held for those
respondents who considered the immutability of homosexuality to be a belief espoused
by tolerant people. Respondents who held tolerant attitudes but who did not necessarily
believe that immutability beliefs were expressive of tolerance did not always hold such
beliefs (2002). Thus Hegarty argued that beliefs about the origins of homosexuality are
constructed as expressions of particular political viewpoints: ‘immutability beliefs may
be associated with tolerant heterosexual people leading individuals who are tolerant and
condemning to take up positions in favour of either “biology” or “choice” respectively’
(2002: 163).
Herek and Capitanio found that among the white population, the belief that
homosexuality is not a choice was linked to greater contact with gay and lesbian people,
whereas among the black population, the belief that homosexuality is not a choice was
equally common among blacks who did or not have contact with gay and lesbian people
(1995). They argue that the beliefs of black Americans regarding the origins of
homosexuality may be shaped less by contact than by African American cultural
constructions of homosexuality which are more fluid.
The effect of believing that homosexuality is determined by environmental factors
such as family upbringing has not been investigated (Lewis and Rogers 1999).
Attitudes Towards Homosexuality and HIV/AIDS
Strong correlations have been found between negative attitudes towards people
with AIDS, the perception of AIDS as a ‘gay disease’ and high levels of antigay
prejudice (D’Angelo et al 1998; Herek and Capitanio 1999a; Johnson and Baer 1996;
Kunkel and Temple 1992; Larsen et al 1990; McDevitt et al 1990). While all people who
contract AIDS sexually are considered responsible for the their infection, gay men with
AIDS and men who contract HIV through homosexual sex are evaluated more negatively
than are heterosexuals with AIDS or other illnesses (Fish and Rye 1991; Herek and
Capitanio 1999a; Johnson and Baer 1996; contra D’Angelo et al 1998). Heterosexuals
with negative attitudes towards homosexuals are more likely to be poorly informed about
and unduly fearful of AIDS (Carney et al 1994; D’Angelo et al 1998; Herek and Glunt
1991; Price and Hsu 1992; Waldner et al 1999). For example, they are more likely to hold
mistaken beliefs about how AIDS is contracted. Homophobic individuals are also more
likely to endorse restrictive and punitive measures against people with AIDS (Seltzer
1993).
Negative attitudes towards people with AIDS correlate more strongly with
homophobia than with knowledge about AIDS (Johnson and Baer 1996), and possession
of greater knowledge about AIDS does not necessarily lead to greater willingness to
interact with a person with AIDS (Fish and Rye 1991). These findings may be interpreted
as evidence that attitudes towards AIDS are based on affective responses (Johnson and
Baer 1996) or beliefs about what AIDS symbolises (Herek and Capitanio 1998, 1999a) as
well as (or perhaps more so than) on cognitive reactions such as fear of infection.
24
Research shows that men are generally more negative towards or fearful of people
with AIDS than women (Bouton et al 1989; D’Angelo et al 1998; Johnson and Baer
1996; McDevitt et al 1990; contra Kunkel and Temple 1992). However, this gender
difference in attitudes towards people with AIDS is probably attributable to gender
difference generally found in attitudes towards homosexuality (Fish and Rye 1991;
Johnson and Baer 1996), and perhaps differences in levels of empathy (Fish and Rye
1991).
A considerable amount of research has been dedicated to assessing whether the
perceived association of HIV/AIDS with the gay population has increased prejudice
against homosexuals, particularly gay men. Some studies have suggested that AIDS
contributed to homophobia (Klamen et al 1999; Pleck et al 1988; Pratte 1993; Scott
1998). For example, approximately one-third of American adults surveyed in the mid1980s reported feeling more negatively towards homosexuals as a result of the AIDS
epidemic (Pleck et al 1988; Pratte 1993). However, other studies have questioned the
impact of the AIDS epidemic on attitudes towards homosexuality claim, often with the
benefit of hindsight or long-term survey data (CJC 1990; Dejowski 1992; Herek and
Capitanio 1999a; Nelson and Krieger 1997; Smith 1992; Yang 1997; Young et al 1991).
Herek and Capitanio argue that ‘AIDS attitudes were for many heterosexuals a symbolic
vehicle for expressing preexisting sexual prejudice’ (1999a: 1132), that is, homophobia
strengthened the stigma attached to AIDS, rather than vice versa. Through the 1990s,
there was an increasing awareness that AIDS was not restricted to the homosexual
population, and yet the correlation between homophobia and negative attitudes towards
people with AIDS persisted. This suggests that AIDS stigma continued to function as a
‘symbolic expression’ of attitudes towards homosexuality (Herek and Capitanio 1999a;
see also D’Angelo et al 1998).
Personality
The extent to which different personality traits affect attitudes towards
homosexuality has received some attention. Most of this research has been directed
towards examining the links between negative attitudes towards homosexuality and
‘right-wing authoritarianism’. The authoritarian personality exhibits a high degree of
deference to established political, social and especially religious authority; support for
values endorsed by that authority; and aggression towards outgroups if sanctioned by that
authority (Altemeyer 1988, 1996, 2001).
Several studies have shown that there is a strong correlation between
authoritarianism and intolerance of homosexuality (Altemeyer 1988, 1996, 2001; Basow
and Johnson 2000; Haddock and Zanna 1998; Whitley and Aegisdottir 2000; Whitley and
Lee 2000; Wilkinson 2004). People with authoritarian personalities are more likely to say
that they would help the government to harass, torture, imprison and execute gay people,
and are more likely to approve of gay bashing (Altemeyer 2001).
Limited research has also been conducted into other personality traits that may
affect attitudes towards homosexuality. These include: social dominance orientation, that
is, ‘the extent to which one desires that one’s in-group dominate and be superior to
outgroups’ (Pratto et al 1994; Whitley and Aegisdottir 2000; Whitley and Lee 2000);
self-esteem (Crandall and Cohen 1994); empathetic concern and perspective taking
25
(Johnson et al 1997); the personal need for structure (Smith and Gordon 1998); and even
whether an individual is a morning or an evening person (Smith and Gordon 1998)!
However, Cullen et al argue that the influence of personality factors on prejudice
generally and anti-gay attitudes in particular has been overemphasised (2002). They
maintain that the social, economic and political advantages associated with
discriminating against outgroups are likely to be more influential.
Relationship of Homophobia to Other Political and Social Values
Attitudes towards other oppressed groups predict attitudes towards
homosexuality, that is, people who hold racist and sexist views are more likely also to be
homophobic (Agnew et al 1993; Altemeyer 2001; Biernat et al 1996; Ficarrotto 1990;
Kite and Whitley 1996; Whitley 2001; Whitley and Aegisdottir 2000). This suggests that,
for some people at least, anti-gay attitudes are less related to gay people per se than to
their own prejudiced personalities (Altemeyer 2001). These people may be said to display
a more general ‘social reactionary attitude’ (Kite and Whitley 1996, 1998) or to subscribe
to an ‘antiegalitarian belief system’ (Whitley 2001). However, Basow and Johnson found
that homophobia was not entirely coextensive with other types of prejudice, perhaps
because homophobia is a more socially acceptable prejudice, or because it is perceived to
threaten family and gender norms in a way that other minority groups do not (2000; see
also Agnew et al 1993; Whitley and Aegisdottir 2000). Moreover, it is unlikely that the
antigay attitudes of those who are themselves members of minority groups would be
derived from a prejudiced personality, and are therefore probably predicted by other
variables (see Whitley and Aegisdottir 2000).
Attitudes towards homosexuality are also related to sexual conservatism
(Ficarrotto 1990; Scott 1998; Simon 1995; Smith 1992). For some heterosexuals,
homophobia may stem from more general beliefs and negative feelings about sexuality
and sexual morality. This may go some way to explaining some heterosexuals’
discomfort with public displays of gay and lesbian sexuality (Howard-Hassmann 2001).
The Attitudes of Certain Groups Towards Homosexuality
The Attitudes of Members of the ‘Helping Professions’ Towards Homosexuality
Doctors, psychologists, social workers and students studying to enter these
professions are generally less homophobic than the general public, with substantial
majorities holding positive views (Berkman and Zinberg 1997; Crawford et al 1999;
Klamen et al 1999; Parker and Bhugra 2000). However, most research shows that 1015% of members of these groups can be considered homophobic (Berkman and Zinberg
1997; Crawford et al 1999; Parker and Bhugra 2000), and this may compromise the
standard of care provided to gay and lesbian patients and clients.
The Attitudes of Parents of Gay Men and Lesbians Towards Homosexuality
Most of the literature is not concerned with parents’ attitudes towards
homosexuality per se, but rather with parental reactions to a child’s disclosure of his or
26
her sexual orientation. These reactions are mostly described in terms of the array of
emotions experienced such as shock, grief, guilt, shame, anger, acknowledgment and
acceptance (Armesto and Weisman 2001; Ben-Ari 1995; Savin-Williams and Dubé
1998). However, national polls show that between 1974 and 1985, 70-80% of the US
population have said that they would not be accepting of a homosexual child and think
their relationship with the child would be strained (Yang 1997).
Most parents initially experience shock, followed by a range of negative
emotions, including grief, guilt and shame (Ben-Ari 1995; Robinson et al 1989; SavinWilliams and Dubé 1998). Extreme negative reactions are relatively rare. Savin-Williams
(1995, cited in Savin-Williams and Dubé 1998) found that almost half the mothers of gay
and bisexual college students whose son’s sexual orientation had been disclosed to them
responded with disbelief, denial or negative comments, while half the fathers responded
with silence or disbelief. 18% of the parents responded with acts of intolerance, such as
attempts to convert their child and threats to cut off financial or emotional support. 11%
of mothers and 3% of fathers responded with rejection, hysteria or physical attack.
Telljohann and Price found that 35% of young gay men and lesbians reported that their
parents reacted in a positive or supportive manner (1993). Most studies have found that
mothers react more positively than do fathers (see Ben-Ari 1995; D’Augelli and
Hershberger 1993; Savin-Williams and Dubé 1998; contra Savin-Williams 1995, cited in
Savin-Williams and Dubé). Both mothers and fathers appear to be more accepting of a
gay son than a lesbian daughter, perhaps because they hold daughters more responsible
for producing grandchildren (Ben-Ari 1995). Most parents consider themselves to be
more accepting than their children perceive them to be (Ben-Ari 1995).
Most parents become more tolerant over time (Ben-Ari 1995; Robinson et al
1989; Savin-Williams and Dubé 1998). In D’Augelli and Hershberger’s study, over half
the mothers and 37% of the fathers were accepting, a quarter of mothers and 36% of
fathers were tolerant, and a quarter of mothers and 28% of fathers were intolerant or
rejecting (1993). Little research has investigated the demographic characteristics and
beliefs which predict different reactions by parents (see Savin-Williams and Dubé 1998).
Similarly, no research has investigated the child variables that may influence parental
responses, such as the child’s status in the home, the degree to which the child deviates
from gender norms, age of disclosure and involvement in a romantic relationship (SavinWilliams and Dubé 1998). Ben-Ari suggests that negative parental responses can partly
be attributed to a lack of exposure to homosexuality. Most parents had no experience of
homosexual people and issues or had never thought about homosexuality (1995).
Attitudes Towards Bisexuals
Very little research has been conducted into attitudes of heterosexuals towards
bisexual men and women, perhaps because attitudes towards bisexuals are often conflated
with attitudes towards homosexuals (Eliason 1997; Herek 2002b). The little research that
has been carried out suggests that negative attitudes towards bisexuals are widespread,
correlated with negative attitudes towards gay men and lesbians and predicted by the
same variables (Eliason 1997; Herek 2002b; Mulick and Wright 2002). This suggests that
attitudes towards homosexuals and bisexuals arise from the same ‘social milieu’ and the
same ‘general belief system’, which includes religiosity, moral conservatism and
27
traditional gender role beliefs (Herek 2002b; Mulick and Wright 2002). However,
heterosexuals appear to exhibit more negative responses and attitudes towards bisexuals
than homosexuals (Eliason 1997; Herek 2002b). Heterosexual women are generally more
positive in their feelings towards bisexuals than are heterosexual men, and bisexual
women are generally rated more highly than bisexual men (Eliason 1997; Herek 2002b).
One interesting finding is that heterosexual women rate bisexual men and women
less favourably than homosexual men and women whereas heterosexual men rate male
targets less favourably than female targets, regardless of whether the target is homosexual
or bisexual (Herek 2002b; see also Herek and Capitanio 1999b; Herek 2000a). The
consistently more negative ratings of male targets by heterosexual men suggest that
heterosexual men’s attitudes towards homosexuals and bisexuals are both linked to
concerns regarding gender, sexuality and masculinity. Heterosexual women, on the other
hand, appear to perceive homosexuals and bisexuals as distinct minority groups and their
attitudes towards each group appear to be differently motivated (Herek 2002b).
Heterosexual men’s more positive attitudes towards bisexual and lesbian women may
also be attributable to the sexualisation of lesbianism as a male fantasy (Eliason 1997). 16
The limited number of studies, however, restricts the extent to which these findings can
be relied upon.
The Dynamic Nature of Attitudes Towards Homosexuality?
This review of the literature has revealed a very large number of studies showing
that attitudes towards homosexuality are influenced by a number of different
demographic, contact and personality variables, as well as by social and political beliefs.
Some of the research has revealed the sometimes ambiguous nature of these attitudes. For
example, it has shown that heterosexual people think differently about the acceptability
of homosexual behaviour compared with the entitlement of gay men and lesbians to the
equal enjoyment of civil rights and liberties. However, the dynamic nature of attitudes
towards homosexuality remains an under-researched area, and the following findings
cannot be considered conclusive.
A limited amount of research has sought to understand the potential disjuncture
between heterosexuals’ affective reactions to homosexuality and homosexual people
(their emotions or feelings), and their cognitive attitudes towards homosexuality (their
opinions and beliefs). 17 While these two dimensions of attitudes towards homosexuality
are related, they are not identical. For example, it is possible to believe that gay men and
lesbians should enjoy civil rights equal to those enjoyed by heterosexual men and
women, and yet still feel discomfort with the expression of gay sexuality or when
interacting with a gay or lesbian person (Altemeyer 2001; Davies 2004; HowardHassmann 2001; Hudson and Ricketts 1980; Kite and Whitley 1998; LaMar and Kite
16
For example, compared with heterosexual women, heterosexual men were more likely to report that they
would have a sexual relationship with a bisexual woman, to agree that bisexuals have more sexual partners
than heterosexuals and homosexuals, and to agree that bisexuals are more likely to have more than one
sexual partner at a time than heterosexuals: Eliason 1997.
17
The third component of ‘attitudes’ in social psychology is the behavioural aspect, that is, past behaviours
or behavioural intentions. However, I deal with this separately below.
28
1998; Loftus 2001; Logan 1996; Norris 1992; Van de Ven 1994). As Van de Ven argues,
‘[t]he result may be outward tolerance but underlying hostility, perpetuated in part by
anti-discrimination policies and equal opportunity strategies that put a premium on
acceptable behaviours toward minorities and less emphasis on cognitive notions, while all
but ignoring “inner” feelings’ (1994: 118).
Most measures of homophobia tap cognitions rather than affective reactions, such
as anger, guilt, disgust and delight (Pain and Disney 1995; Van de Ven 1994, 1995).
However, as Van de Ven’s argument suggests, it becomes increasingly important to
assess more than the cognitive aspects of attitudes towards homosexuality as homophobic
beliefs become less acceptable (Spark Jones 2000). For example, Howard-Hassmann
described her respondents’ answers as ‘often internally self-contradictory, with, for
example, a liberal attitude to gay adoptions combined with revulsion against gay sexual
practices’ (2001: 129). Estrada and Weiss found that although 46% of the military
personnel they surveyed agreed that male homosexuality is merely a different lifestyle
that should not be condemned, 90% said they would feel uncomfortable sharing a room
with a homosexual (1999). Affective attitudes may be more resistant to change than
cognitive attitudes (Altemeyer 2001; Nelson and Krieger 1997; Van de Ven 1995).
The issue of the relationship between attitudes and behaviour has also generated
debate. In general, it seems to be accepted that the attitudes of individuals towards
homosexuality do not directly predict how these individuals will behave towards
homosexual people (Fernald 1995; Kite and Whitley 1998; Loftus 2001; Scott 1998),
although some studies have shown a correlation between antigay attitudes and antigay
behaviours (Bernstein and Kostelac 2002; Fishbein and Azjen 1974; Haddock and Zanna
1998; Kite and Whitley 1998). Indeed, as Bernstein and Kostelac ask, ‘if the same person
can hold different sorts of attitudes regarding sexual orientation, then which attitudes
translate into actual discriminatory behaviour?’ (2002: 305)
Contextual or situational factors may moderate the relationship between attitudes
and behaviour (Bernstein and Kostelac 2002; Haddock and Zanna 1998; Kite and
Whitley 1998). For example, in their survey of the attitudes of police officers towards
homosexuality, Bernstein and Kostelac found that line-level officers were more likely to
engage in discriminatory behaviour against gays and lesbians than superior officers
regardless of their attitudes towards homosexuality (2002). Other studies have found that
heterosexuals respond more negatively to gay men and lesbians when they are presented
in a stereotypical manner than when they are presented as ‘ordinary’ people
(Golebiowska 2000; Lord et al 1984; Laner and Laner 1979, 1980; latter three cited in
Kite and Whitley 1998). Kite found that although heterosexual men expressed more
negative attitudes towards gay men than lesbians, they did not necessarily respond more
negatively towards gay men in an interactive setting (1992). She argues that these men
had learnt to temper their prejudiced reactions in this context, noting that their behaviour
might differ in other settings, for example, where more intimate contact such as sharing a
room was proposed (Kite 1992: 1234; Kite 2002). Indeed, Norris found that students
were comfortable with and willing to extend rights to gays and lesbians only in certain
contexts. For example, while 95% of the students reported that they would not refuse to
live in a house with a gay person, one respondent commented, ‘I might object if it were
just a two-person dorm but not a house’ (1992: 109).
29
Other experiments have shown that exposure to pro-prejudice or anti-prejudice
norms can play a role in either encouraging or inhibiting expressions of prejudice
(Masser and Phillips 2003; Monteith et al 1996). It seems that where there are clear
attitudinal or behavioural norms, individuals will tend to act in accordance with those
norms, whereas personal beliefs are more likely to come into play in individuated
situations where there are no clear norms (Kite 2002; Monteith et al 1996). Highly
prejudiced individuals seem more inclined to alter their expression and behaviour in
accordance with perceived social norms than are individuals low in prejudice (Altemeyer
2001; Masser and Phillips 2003). It is possible that men’s attitudes are also more
susceptible to situational manipulation (Herek and Capitanio 1999b).
References
Agnew, Christopher R, Vaida D Thompson, Valerie A Smith, Richard H Gramzow and
David P Currey (1993) ‘Proximal and Distal Predictors of Homophobia: Framing the
Multivariate Roots of Outgroup Rejection’, 23(24) Journal of Applied Social Psychology
2013-2042.
Alcalay, R, P Sniderman, J Mitchell and R Griffin (1989) ‘Ethnic Differences in
Knowledge of AIDS Transmission and Attitudes Towards Gays and People with AIDS’,
10 International Quarterly of Community Health Education 213-222.
Alston, JP (1974) ‘Attitudes Toward Extramarital and Homosexual Relations’, 13
Journal of Scientific Studies of Religion 479-481.
Altemeyer, Bob (1988) Enemies of Freedom, Jossey Bass, San Francisco.
Altemeyer, Bob (1996) The Authoritarian Specter, Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
Altemeyer, Bob (2001) ‘Changes in Attitudes Toward Homosexuals’, 42(2) Journal of
Homosexuality 63-75.
American Enterprise Institute (July 6, 2004) ‘Attitudes About Homosexuality and Gay
Marriage’, AEI Studies in Public Opinion.
Armesto, Jorge C and Amy G Weisman (2001) ‘Attributions and Emotional Reactions to
the Identity Disclosure (“Coming Out”) of a Homosexual Child’, 40(2) Family Process
145-161.
Baker, Janet G and Harold D Fishbein (1998) ‘The Development of Prejudice Towards
Gays and Lesbians by Adolescents’, 36(1) Journal of Homosexuality 89-100.
Basow, Susan (1992) Gender: Stereotypes and Roles, 3rd ed, Brooks/Cole, Pacific Grove.
Basow, Susan A and Kelly Johnson (2000) ‘Predictors of Homophobia in Female College
Students’, 42(5/6) Sex Roles 391-404.
30
Battle, Juan and Anthony J Lemelle Jr (2002) ‘Gender Differences in African American
Attitudes Toward Gay Males’, 26(3) The Western Journal of Black Studies 134-139.
Ben-Ari, Adital (1995) ‘The Discovery that an Offspring is Gay: Parents’, Gay Men’s,
and Lesbians’ Perspectives’, 30(1) Journal of Homosexuality 89.
Berkman, Cathy S and Gail Zinberg (1997) ‘Homophobia and Heterosexism in Social
Workers’, 42(4) Social Work 319-332.
Bernstein, Mary and Constance Kostelac (2002) ‘Lavender and Blue: Attitudes About
Homosexuality and Behavior Toward Lesbians and Gay Men’, 18(3) Journal of
Contemporary Criminal Justice 302-328.
Biernat, Monica, Theresa K Vescio and Shelley A Theno (1996) ‘Violating American
Values: A “Value Congruence” Approach to Understanding Outgroup Attitudes’, 32
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 387-410.
Black, Beverly, Thomas P Oles and Linda Moore (1998) ‘The Relationship Between
Attitudes: Homophobia and Sexism among Social Work Students’, 13(2) Affilia Journal
of Women and Social Work 166-189.
Bonilla, Louis and Judith Porter (1990) ‘A Comparison of Latino, Black and NonHispanic White Attitudes Toward Homosexuality’, 12(4) Hispanic Journal of Behavioral
Sciences 437-452.
Bouton, RA, PE Gallaher, PA Garlinghouse, T Leal, LD Rosenstein and RK Young
(1989) ‘Demographic Variables Associated with Fear of AIDS and Homophobia’, 19
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 885-901.
Carney, Jamie, James L Werth and Greg Emanuelson (1994) ‘The Relationship between
Attitudes Toward Persons who are Gay and Persons with AIDS, and HIV and AIDS
Knowledge’, 72(6) Journal of Counseling and Development 646-650.
Cerny, JA and J Polyson (1984) ‘Changing Homonegative Attitudes’, 2 Journal of Social
Clinical Psychology 366-371.
Chng, CL and A Moore (1991) ‘Can Attitudes of College Students Toward AIDS and
Homosexuality be Changed in Six Weeks? The Effects of a Gay Panel’, 15 Health Values
41-49.
Corley, Tammy J and Robert H Pollack (1996) ‘Do Changes in the Stereotypic Depiction
of a Lesbian Couple Affect Heterosexuals’ Attitudes Toward Lesbianism?’ 32(2) Journal
of Homosexuality 1-17.
31
Cotten-Huston, Annie L and Bradley M Waite (2000) ‘Anti-Homosexual Attitudes in
College Students: Predictors and Classroom Interventions’ 38(3) Journal of
Homosexuality 117-133.
Crandall, Christian S and Colby Cohen (1994) ‘The Personality of the Stigmatizer:
Cultural World View, Conventionalism and Self-Esteem’ 28 Journal of Research in
Personality 461-480.
Crawford, Isiaah, Andrew McLeod, Brian D Zamboni and Michael B Jordan (1999)
‘Psychologists’ Attitudes Toward Gay and Lesbian Parenting’, 30(4) Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice 384-401.
Criminal Justice Commission Queensland (1990) Reforms in Laws Relating to
Homosexuality: An Information Paper, Research and Co-ordination Division, Brisbane.
Crockett, Alasdair and David Voas (2003) ‘A Divergence of Views: Attitude Change and
the Religious Crisis over Homosexuality’, 8(4) Sociological Research Online
http://www.socresonline.org.uk/8/4/crockett.html
Cullen, Jenifer M, Lester W Wright and Michael Alessandri (2002) ‘The Personality
Variable Openness to Experience as It Related to Homophobia’, 42(4) Journal of
Homosexuality 119-134.
D’Angelo, Robert J, David W Abbott, John M McGuire and Kathleen Sheridan (1998)
‘Homophobia and Perceptions of People with AIDS’, 28(2) Journal of Applied Social
Psychology 157-170.
D’Augelli, Anthony R and Scott L Hershberger (1993) ‘Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual
Youth in Community Settings: Personal Challenges and Mental Health Problems’, 21(4)
American Journal of Community Psychology 421-448.
D’Augelli, AR and ML Rose (1990) ‘Homophobia in a University Community: Attitudes
and Experiences of Heterosexual Freshmen’, 31 Journal of College Student Development
484-491.
Davies, Michelle (2004) ‘Correlates of Negative Attitudes Toward Gay Men: Sexism,
Male Role Norms, and Male Sexuality’, 41(3) The Journal of Sex Research 259-266.
Davis, James A (1992) ‘Changeable Weather in a Cooling Climate Atop the Liberal
Plateau: Conversion and Replacement in Forty-Two General Social Survey Items 19721989’, 56 Public Opinion Quarterly 261-306.
Deaux, K and ME Kite (1987) ‘Thinking about Gender’ in BB Hess and MM Ferree
(eds), Analyzing Gender: A Handbook of Social Science Research, Sage, Newbury Park,
92-117.
32
Deaux, K and LL Lewis (1984) ‘Structure of Gender Stereotypes: Interrelationships
Among Components and Gender Label’, 46 Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 991-1004.
Dejowski, Edmund F (1992) ‘Public Endorsement of Restrictions on Three Aspects of
Free Expression by Homosexuals: Socio-Demographic and Trends Analysis 1973-1988’,
23(4) Journal of Homosexuality 1-18.
Eliason, Michele J (1995) ‘Attitudes about Lesbians and Gay Men: A Review and
Implications for Social Service Training’, 2 Journal of Gay and Lesbian Social Services
73-90.
Eliason, Michele J (1997) ‘The Prevalence and Nature of Biphobia in Heterosexual
Undergraduate Students’ 26(3) Archives of Sexual Behavior 317-326.
Ellis, Alan L and R Brent Vasseur (1993) ‘Prior Interpersonal Contact With and Attitudes
Towards Gays and Lesbians in an Interviewing Context’, 25(4) Journal of Homosexuality
31-44.
Ellis, Sonja J (2002) ‘Student Support for Lesbian and Gay Human Rights: Findings from
a Large-scale Questionnaire Study’ in Adrian Coyle and Celia Kitzinger (eds), Lesbian
and Gay Psychology: New Perspectives, Malden, Blackwell Publishers, 239-254.
Ellis, Sonja J, Celia Kitzinger and Sue Wilkinson (2002) ‘Attitudes Towards Lesbians
and Gay Men and Support for Lesbian and Gay Human Rights Among Psychology
Students’, 44(1) Journal of Homosexuality 121-138.
Ellison, Christopher G and Marc A Musick (1993) ‘Southern Intolerance: A
Fundamentalist Effect?’, 72(2) Social Forces 379-398.
Estrada, Armando X and David J Weiss (1999) ‘Attitudes of Military Personnel toward
Homosexuals’, 37(4) Journal of Homosexuality 83-97.
Fernald, JL (1995) ‘Interpersonal Heterosexism’ in Bernice Lott and Diane Maluso (eds),
The Social Psychology of Interpersonal Discrimination, Guildford Press, New York, 80117.
Ficarrotto, Thomas J (1990) ‘Racism, Sexism, and Erotophobia: Attitudes of
Heterosexuals Toward Homosexuals’, 19(1) Journal of Homosexuality 111-116.
Finlay, Barbara and Carol S Walther (2003) ‘The Relation of Religious Affiliation,
Service Attendance, and Other Factors to Homophobic Attitudes Among University
Students’, 44(4) Review of Religious Research 370-393.
33
Fish, Thomas A and Barbara J Rye (1991) ‘Attitudes Toward a Homosexual or
Heterosexual Person with AIDS’, 21(8) Journal of Applied Social Psychology 651-667.
Fishbein, M and I Azjen (1974) Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior: An Introduction
to Theory and Research, Addison-Wesley, Reading.
Fisher, Randy D, Donna Derison, Chester F Polley III, Jennifer Cadman and Dana
Johnston (1994) ‘Religiousness, Religious Orientation, and Attitudes Toward Gays and
Lesbians’, 24(7) Journal of Applied Social Psychology 614-630.
Gentry, CS (1987) ‘Social Distance Regarding Male and Female Homosexuals’, 127
Journal of Social Psychology 199-208.
Glenn, HD and CN Weaver (1979) ‘Attitudes Toward Premarital, Extramarital and
Homosexual Relations in the US in the 1970s’, 15 Journal of Sex Research 108-118.
Golebiowska, Ewa A (2000) ‘The Etiology of Individual-Targeted Intolerance: Group
Stereotypes and Judgments of Individual Group Members’, 21(3) Political Psychology
443-464.
Green, Shelly, Paul Dixon and Valerie Gold-Neil (1993) ‘The Effects of a Gay/Lesbian
Panel Discussion on College Student Attitudes Toward Gay Men, Lesbians, and Persons
with AIDS (PWAs)’, 19(1) Journal of Sex Education and Therapy 47-63.
Griffiths, Brian, Cynthia Dixon, Gordon Stanley and Robert Weiland (2001) ‘Religious
Orientation and Attitudes Towards Homosexuality: A Functional Analysis’, 53(1)
Australian Journal of Psychology 12-17.
Haddock, Geoffrey and Mark P Zanna (1998) ‘Authoritarianism, Values, and the
Favorability and Structure of Antigay Attitudes’ in Gregory M Herek (ed), Stigma and
Sexual Orientation: Understanding Prejudice against Lesbians, Gay Men and Bisexuals,
Sage, Thousand Oaks, 82-107.
Haeberle, Steven H (1999) ‘Gay and Lesbian Rights: Emerging Trends in Public Opinion
and Voting Behavior’, in Ellen DB Riggle and Barry L Tadlock (eds), Gays and Lesbians
in the Democratic Process: Public Policy, Public Opinion, and Political Representation,
Columbia UP, New York.
Harris, MB and J Vanderhoof (1995) ‘Attitudes Towards Gays and Lesbians Serving in
the Military’, 3(4) Journal of Gay and Lesbian Social Services 23-51.
Haslam, Nick, Louis Rothschild and Donald Ernst (2002) ‘Are Essentialist Beliefs
Associated with Prejudice?’ 41 The British Journal of Social Psychology 87-100.
34
Hayes, Bernadette C (1995) ‘Religious Identification and Moral Attitudes: The British
Case’, 46(3) British Journal of Sociology 457-474.
Hayes, Bernadette C (1997) ‘The Influence of Gender on Public Attitudes Toward
Homosexual Rights in Britain’, 9(4) International Journal of Public Opinion Research
361-385.
Hegarty, Peter (2002) ‘“It’s Not a Choice, It’s the Way We’re Built”: Symbolic Beliefs
about Sexual Orientation in the US and Britain’, 12 Journal of Community and Applied
Social Psychology 153-166.
Hegarty, Peter and Felicia Pratto (2001) ‘Sexual Orientation Beliefs: Their Relationship
to Anti-Gay Attitudes and Biological Determinist Arguments’, 41(1) Journal of
Homosexuality 121-135.
Herek, Gregory M (1986) ‘On Heterosexual Masculinity: Some Psychical Consequences
of the Social Construction of Gender and Sexuality’, 29(5) American Behavioral Scientist
563-577.
Herek, Gregory M (1988) ‘Heterosexuals’ Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men:
Correlates and Gender Differences’, 25 Journal of Sex Research 451-477.
Herek, Gregory M (2000a) ‘Sexual Prejudice and Gender: Do Heterosexuals’ Attitudes
Toward Lesbians and Gay Men Differ?’ 56(2) Journal of Social Issues 251-166.
Herek, Gregory M (2000b) ‘The Psychology of Sexual Prejudice’, 9(1) Current
Directions in Psychological Science 19-22.
Herek, Gregory M (2002a) ‘Gender Gaps in Public Opinion about Lesbians and Gay
Men’, 66 Public Opinion Quarterly 40-66.
Herek, Gregory M (2002b) ‘Heterosexuals’ Attitudes Toward Bisexual Men and Women
in the United States’, 39(4) Journal of Sex Research 264-274.
Herek, Gregory M and John P Capitanio (1995) ‘Black Heterosexuals’ Attitudes Toward
Lesbians and Gay Men in the United States’, 32(2) The Journal of Sex Research 95-105.
Herek, Gregory M and John P Capitanio (1996) ‘“Some of my best friends”: Intergroup
Contact, Concealable Stigma, and Heterosexuals’ Attitudes Toward Gay Men and
Lesbians’, 22(4) Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 412-424.
Herek, Gregory M and John P Capitanio (1998) ‘Symbolic Prejudice or Fear of
Infection? A Functional Analysis of AIDS-related Stigma Among Heterosexual Adults’,
20(3) Basic and Applied Social Psychology 230-241.
35
Herek, Gregory M and John P Capitanio (1999a) ‘AIDS Stigma and Sexual Prejudice’,
42(7) The American Behavioral Scientist 1130-1147.
Herek, Gregory M and John P Capitanio (1999b) ‘Sex Differences in How Heterosexuals
Think about Lesbians and Gay Men: Evidence form Survey Context Effects’, 36(4)
Journal of Sex Research 348-360.
Herek, Gregory M and Eric K Glunt (1993) ‘Interpersonal Contact and Heterosexuals’
Attitudes Toward Gay Men: Results from a National Survey’, 30(3) Journal of Sex
Research 239-244.
Hinrichs, Donald W and Pamela J Rosenberg (2002) ‘Attitudes Toward Gay, Lesbian and
Bisexual Persons Among Heterosexual Liberal Arts College Students’, 43(1) Journal of
Homosexuality 61-84.
Howard-Hassman, Rhoda E (2001) ‘The Gay Cousin: Learning to Accept Gay Rights’,
42(1) Journal of Homosexuality 127-149.
Hudson, WW and WA Ricketts (1980) ‘A Strategy for the Measurement of
Homophobia’, 5 Journal of Homosexuality 357-372.
Irwin, Patrick and Norman L Thompson (1977) ‘Acceptance of the Rights of
Homosexuals: A Social Profile’, 3(2) Journal of Homosexuality 107-121.
Iyriboz, Y and JA Carter (1986) ‘Attitudes of a Southern University Human Sexuality
Class Toward Sexual Variance, Abortion, and Homosexuality’, 20 College Student
Journal 89-93.
Johnson, Mark E and Alice J Baer (1996) ‘College Students’ Judgments and Perceptions
of Persons with AIDS from Different Risk Groups’, 130(5) The Journal of Psychology
527-536.
Johnson, Mark E, Christiane Brems and Pat Alford-Keating (1997) ‘Personality
Correlated of Homophobia’, 34(1) Journal of Homosexuality 57-69.
Kelley, Jonathan (2001) ‘Attitudes towards Homosexuality in 29 Nations’, 4(1)
Australian Social Monitor 15-22.
Kerns, John G and Mark A Fine (1994) ‘The Relation Between Gender and Negative
Attitudes Toward Gay Men and Lesbians: Do Gender Role Attitudes Mediate This
Relation?’ 31(5/6) Sex Roles 297-307.
Kilianksi, Stephen E (2003) ‘Explaining Heterosexual Men’s Attitudes Toward Women
and Gay Men: The Theory of Exclusively Masculine Identity’, 4(1) Psychology of Men
and Masculinity 37-56.
36
King, Beverly R (2001) ‘Ranking of Stigmatization Toward Lesbians and their Children
and the Influence of Perceptions of Controllability of Homosexuality’, 41(2) Journal of
Homosexuality 77-97.
Kite, Mary E (1992) ‘Individual Differences in Males’ Reactions to Gay Males and
Lesbians’, 22(15) Journal of Applied Social Psychology 1222-1239.
Kite, Mary E (2002) ‘When Perceptions Meet Reality: Individual Differences in
Reactions to Lesbians and Gay Men’ in Adrian Coyle and Celia Kitzinger (eds), Lesbian
and Gay Psychology: New Perspectives, Malden, Blackwell Publishers, 25-53.
Kite, ME and K Deaux (1987) ‘Gender Belief Systems: Homosexuality and the Implicit
Inversion Theory’, 11 Psychology of Women Quarterly 83-96.
Kite, Mary E and Bernard E Whitley (1996) ‘Sex Differences in Attitudes Toward
Homosexual Persons, Behaviors, and Civil Rights: A Meta-Analysis’, 22(4) Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin 336-353.
Kite, Mary E and Bernard E Whitley Jr (1998) ‘Do Heterosexual Women and Men Differ
in their Attitudes Toward Homosexuality?: A Conceptual and Methodological Analysis’
in Gregory M Herek (ed), Stigma and Sexual Orientation: Understanding Prejudice
Against Lesbians, Gay Men and Bisexuals, Sage, Thousand Oaks, 39-61.
Klamen, Debra L, Linda S Grossman and David R Kopacz (1999) ‘Medical Student
Homophobia’, 37(1) Journal of Homosexuality 53-63.
Klassen AD, CJ Williams, EE Levitt and HJ O’Gorman (1989) Sex and Morality in the
US: An Empirical Enquiry under the Auspices of the Kinsey Institute, Wesleyan
University Press, Middletown.
Kunkel, Lynn E and Lori L Temple (1992) ‘Attitudes Towards AIDS and Homosexuals:
Gender, Marital-Status, and Religion’, 22(13) Journal of Applied Social Psychology
1030-1040.
Kurdek, LA (1988) ‘Correlates of Negative Attitudes Toward Homosexuals in
Heterosexual College Students’, 18 Sex Roles 727-738.
LaMar, Lisa and Mary Kite (1998) ‘Sex Differences in Attitudes Toward Gay Men and
Lesbians: A Multidimensional Perspective’, 35(2) Journal of Sex Research 189-196.
Laner, MR and RH Laner (1979) ‘Personal Style or Sexual Preference: Why Gay Men
are Disliked’, 9 International Review of Modern Sociology 215-228.
37
Laner, M and RH Laner (1980) ‘Sexual Preference or Personal Style?: Why Lesbians are
Disliked’, 5 Journal of Homosexuality 339-356.
Larsen, KS, M Serra and E Long (1990) ‘AIDS Victims and Heterosexual Attitudes’,
19(3) Journal of Homosexuality 103-116.
Lemelle, Anthony J and Juan Battle (2004) ‘Black Masculinity Matters in Attitudes
Toward Gay Males’, 47(1) Journal of Homosexuality 39-51.
Levina, Marina, Craig R Waldo and Louise F Fitzgerald (2000) ‘We’re Here, We’re
Queer, We’re on TV: The Effects of Visual Media on Heterosexuals’ Attitudes toward
Gay Men and Lesbians’, 30(4) Journal of Applied Social Psychology 738-758.
Levitt, Eugene E and Albert D Klassen (1974) ‘Public Attitudes Toward Homosexuality:
Part of the 1970 National Survey by the Institute for Sex Research’, 1(1) Journal of
Homosexuality 29-43.
Lewis, Gregory B (2003) ‘Black-White Differences in Attitudes Toward Homosexuality
and Gay Rights’, 67(1) Public Opinion Quarterly 59-78.
Lewis, Gregory B and Marc A Rogers (1999) ‘Does the Public Support Equal
Employment Rights for Gays and Lesbians?’ in Ellen DB Riggle and Barry L Tadlock
(eds), Gays and Lesbians in the Democratic Process: Public Policy, Public Opinion, and
Political Representation, Columbia UP, New York.
Lippincott, Joseph A, Brian Wlazelek and Lisa J Schumacher (2000) ‘Comparison:
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality of International and American College Students’, 87
Psychological Reports 1053-1056.
Loftus, Jeni (2001) ‘America’s Liberalization in Attitudes Toward Homosexuality, 19731998’, 66(5) American Sociological Review 762-782.
Logan, Colleen R (1996) ‘Homophobia? No, Homoprejudice’, 31(3) Journal of
Homosexuality 31-53.
Lord, CG, MR Lepper and D Mackie (1984) ‘Attitude Prototypes as Determinants of
Attitude-Behavior Consistency’, 46 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 12541266.
Lottes, Ilsa L and Peter J Kuriloff (1994) ‘The Impact of College Experience on Political
and Social Attitudes’, 31(1/2) Sex Roles 31-54.
Louderback, Laura A and Bernard A Whitley Jr (1997) ‘Perceived Erotic Value of
Homosexuality and Sex-Role Attitudes as Mediators of Sex Differences in Heterosexual
38
College Students’ Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men’, 34(2) The Journal of Sex
Research 1997.
Luhrs, T, I Crawford and J Goldberg (1991) ‘The Presence of the Defensive Function as
a Predictor of Heterosexual College Students’ Affectve Responses Toward Gay Men and
Lesbians’, 4(4) Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality 71-87.
Masser, Barbara and Lisa Phillips (2003) ‘“What do other people think?”: The Role of
Prejudice and Social Norms in the Expression of Opinions against Gay Men’, 55(3)
Australian Journal of Psychology 184-190.
Matchinsky, Debra J and Timothy G Iverson (1996) ‘Homophobia in Heterosexual
Female Undergraduates’, 31(4) Journal of Homosexuality 123-128.
Mazur, Michelle A and Tara M Emmers-Sommer (2002) ‘The Effect of Movie Portrayals
on Audience Attitudes About Nontraditional Families and Sexual Orientation’, 44(1)
Journal of Homosexuality 157-179.
McCreary, Donald R (1994) ‘The Male Role and Avoiding Femininity’, 31(9/10) Sex
Roles 517-531.
McDevitt, Teresa M, Eugene P Sheehan, Randy Lennon and Anthony L Ambrosio (1990)
‘Correlates of Attitudes Toward AIDS’, 130(5) The Journal of Social Psychology 699701.
Meston, Cindy M, Paul D Trapnell, Boris B Gorzalka (1998) ‘Ethnic, Gender and
Length-of-Residency Influences on Sexual Knowledge and Attitudes’ 35(2) Journal of
Sex Research 176-188.
Millham, Jim, Christopher L San Miguel and Richard Kellogg (1976) ‘A Factor Analytic
Conceptualization of Attitudes Toward Male and Female Homosexuality’, 2(1) Journal
of Homosexuality 3-10.
Monteith, MJ, NE Deneen and GD Tooman (1996) ‘The Effect of Social Norm
Activation on the Expression of Opinions Concerning Gay Men and Blacks’, 18 Basic
and Applied Social Psychology 267-288.
Mulick, Patrick S and Lester W Wright Jr (2002) ‘Examining the Existence of Biphobia
in the Heterosexual and Homosexual Populations’, 2(4) Journal of Bisexuality 46-64.
Nelson, Eileen S and Shirley L Krieger (1997) ‘Changes in Attitudes Toward
Homosexuality in College Students: Implementation of a Gay Men and Lesbian Peer
Panel’, 33(2) Journal of Homosexuality 63-80.
39
Norris, William P (1992) ‘Liberal Attitudes and Homophobic Acts: The Paradoxes of
Homosexual Experience in a Liberal Institution’, 22(3/4) Journal of Homosexuality 81120.
Nyberg, KL and JP Alston (1977) ‘Homosexual Labeling by University Youths’, 12
Adolescence 541-546.
Oliver, Mary Beth and Janet Shibley Hyde (1995) ‘Gender Differences in Attitudes
Toward Homosexuality: A Reply to Whitley and Kite’, 117(1) Psychological Bulletin
155-158.
Opinion Canada, ‘Canadians split on same-sex marriage, but most side with courts over Parliament’
(2003) 5(39) Opinion Canada, available at http://www.opinioncanada.ca/en/articles/article_31.html, visited 28/2/05.
Overby, L Marvin and Jay Barth (2002) ‘Contact, Community Context, and Public
Attitudes Toward Gay Men and Lesbians’, 34(3) Polity 433-456.
Pain, Michelle D and Monique E Disney (1995) ‘Testing the Reliability and Validity of
the Index of Attitudes Toward Homosexuals (IAH) in Austalia’, 30(2) 99-110.
Parker, Andrew and Dinesh Bhugra (2000) ‘Attitudes of British Medical Students
Towards Male Homosexuality’, 15(2) Sexual and Relationship Therapy 141-149.
Patton, W and M Morrison (1993) ‘Effects of a University Subject on Attitudes Toward
Human Sexuality’, 19(2) Journal of Sex Education and Therapy 93-107.
Piskur, J and D Degelman (1992) ‘Effect of Reading a Summary of Research about
Biological Bases of Homosexual Orientation on Attitudes Toward Homosexuals’, 71(3)
Psychological Reports 1219-1225.
Pleck, JH, L O’Donnell, C O’Donnell and J Snarey (1988) ‘AIDS phobia, contact with
AIDS, and AIDS-related job stress in hospital workers’, 15 Journal of Homosexuality 4154.
Polimeni, Anne-Maree, Elizabeth Hardie, Simone Buzwell (2000) ‘Homophobia Among
Australian Heterosexuals: The Role of Sex, Gender Role Ideology, and Gender Role
Traits’, 5(4) Current Research in Social Psychology 47-62.
Pratte, Trish (1993) ‘A Comparative Study of Attitudes Toward Homosexuality: 1986
and 1991’, 26(1) Journal of Homosexuality 77-83.
Pratto, F, J Sidanius, LM Stallworth and BF Malle (1994) ‘Social Dominance
Orientation: A Personality Variable Predicting Social and Political Attitudes’, 67 Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 741-763.
40
Price, V and ML Hsu (1992) ‘Public Opinion about AIDS Policies: The Role of
Misinformation and Attitudes Towards Homosexuals’, 56 Public Opinion Quarterly 22952.
Religious Tolerance.org, ‘Same-Sex Marriages in Canada’, available at
http://www.religioustolerance.org/hom_marb.htm, visited 28/2/05.
Riggle, Ellen D and Alan L Ellis (1994) ‘Political Tolerance of Homosexuals: The Role
of Group Attitudes and Legal Principles’, 26(4) Journal of Homosexuality 135-147.
Riggle, Ellen DB, Alan L Ellis and Anne M Crawford (1996) ‘The Impact of “Media
Contact” on Attitudes Toward Gay Men’, 31(3) Journal of Homosexuality 55-69.
Robinson, BE, LH Walters and P Skeen (1989) ‘Response of Parents to Learning that
their child is homosexual and concern over AIDS: A National Study’, 18 Journal of
Homosexuality 59-80.
Royse, D and B Birge (1987) ‘Homophobia and Attitudes Towards AIDS Patients
Among Medical, Nursing, and Paramedical Students’, 55 Psychological Reports 779786.
Savin-Williams, Ritch C (1995) ‘Parents’ Reactions to the Discovery of Child’s Sexual
orientation’, Paper presented at the Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities and the Family:
Psychological Perspectives Conference, Pennsylvania State University, University Park,
Pennsylvania (cited in Savin-Williams and Dubé 1998).
Savin-Williams, Ritch C and Eric M Dubé (1998) ‘Parental Reactions to their Child’s
Disclosure of a Gay/Lesbian Identity’, 47(1) Family Relations 7-13.
Schellenberg, E Glenn, Jessie Hirt and Alan Sears (1999) ‘Attitudes Toward
Homosexuals Among Students at a Canadian University’ 40(1/2) Sex Roles 139-152.
Schneider, William and IA Lewis (1984) ‘The Straight Story on Homosexuality
and Gay Rights’, 7 Public Opinion 16-20, 59-60.
Schope, Robert D and Michele J Eliason (2004) ‘Sissies and Tomboys: Gender Role
Behaviors and Homophobia’, 16(2) Journal of Gay and Lesbian Social Services 73-97.
Schulte, Lisa J and Juan Battle (2004) ‘The Relative Importance of Ethnicity and
Religion in Predicting Attitudes Towards Gays and Lesbians’, 47(2) Journal of
Homosexuality 127-141.
Scott, Jacqueline (1998) ‘Changing Attitudes to Sexual Morality: A Cross-National
Comparison’, 32(4) Sociology 815-845.
41
Seltzer, Richard (1992) ‘The Social Location of those Holding Antihomosexual
Attitudes’, 26 Sex Roles 391-398.
Seltzer, Richard (1993) ‘AIDS, Homosexuality, Public Opinion, and Changing Correlates
Over Time’, 26(1) Journal of Homosexuality 85-97.
Simon, Angela (1995) ‘Some Correlates of Individuals’ Attitudes Toward Lesbians’,
29(1) Journal of Homosexuality 89-103.
Simon, Angela (1998) ‘The Relationship Between Stereotypes of and Attitudes Toward
Lesbians and Gays’ in Gregory M Herek (ed), Stigma and Sexual Orientation:
Understanding Prejudice Against Lesbians, Gay Men and Bisexuals, Sage, Thousand
Oaks, 62-81.
Smith, Tom W (1992) ‘Attitudes Towards Sexual Permissiveness: Trends, Correlates,
and Behavioral Connections’, National Opinion Research Center, University of Chicago.
Smith, Michael R and Randall A Gordon (1998) ‘Personal Need for Structure and
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality’, 138(1) The Journal of Social Psychology 83-87.
Span, Sherry A and Lourdes A Vidal (2003) ‘Cross-Cultural Differences in Female
University Students’ Attitudes Toward Homosexuals: A Preliminary Study’, 92
Psychological Reports 565-572.
Spark Jones, Lee (2000) ‘Attitudes of Psychologists and Psychologists-in-Training to
Homosexual Women and Men: An Australian Study’, 39(2) Journal of Homosexuality
113-132.
Stevenson, MR (1988) ‘Promoting Tolerance for Homosexuality: An Evaluation of
Intervention Strategies’, 25 Journal of Sex Research 500-511.
Strand, Douglas Alan (1998) ‘Civil Liberties, Civil Rights, and Stigma: Voter Attitudes
and Behavior in the Politics of Homosexuality’ in Gregory M Herek (ed), Stigma and
Sexual Orientation: Understanding Prejudice Against Lesbians, Gay Men and Bisexuals,
Sage, Thousand Oaks, 108-137.
Telljohann, SK and JP Price (1993) ‘A Qualitative Examination of Adolescent
Homosexuals’ Life Experiences: Ramifications for Secondary School Personnel’, 26
Journal of Homosexuality 41-56.
Theodore, PS and SA Basow (2000) ‘Heterosexual Masculinity and Homophobia: A
Reaction to the Self?’ 40(2) Journal of Homosexuality 31-48.
Tiemeyer, Peter E (1993) ‘Relevant Public Opinion’ in National Defense Research
Institute, Sexual Orientation and US Military Personnel Policy: Options and
Assessments, RAND, Santa Monica.
42
Torres-Reyna, Oscar and Robert Y Shapiro (2002) ‘The Polls – Trends: Women and
Sexual Orientation in the Military’, 66(4) Public Opinion Quarterly 618-632.
Turnbull, D and M Brown (1977) ‘Attitudes Towards Homosexuality and Male and
Female Reactions to Homosexual and Heterosexual Slides’, 9 Canadian Journal of
Behavioral Science 68-80.
Tygart, CE (2000) ‘Genetic Causation Attribution and Public Support of Gay Rights’,
12(3) International Journal of Public Opinion Research 259-275.
Van de Ven, Paul (1994) ‘Comparisons among homophobic reactions of undergraduates,
high school students, and young offenders’, 31(2) Journal of Sex Research 117-124.
Van de Ven, Paul (1995) ‘Effects on High School Students of a Teaching Module for
Reducing Homophobia’, 17(1/2) Basic and Applied Social Psychology 153-172.
Waldner, Lisa K, Anjoo Sikka and Salman Baig (1999) ‘Ethnicity and Sex Differences in
University Students’ Knowledge of AIDS, Fear of AIDS, and Homophobia’, 37(3)
Journal of Homosexuality 117-133.
Weinberger, LE and J Millham (1979) ‘Attitudinal Homophobia and Support of
Traditional Sex Roles’, 4 Journal of Homosexuality 237-246.
Weishut, Daniel JN (2000) ‘Attitudes towards Homosexuality: An Overview’, 37(4)
Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences 308-319.
Whitley, Bernard E (1987) ‘The Relationship of Sex-Role Orientation to Heterosexuals’
Attitudes Toward Homosexuals’, 17 Sex Roles 103-113.
Whitley, Bernard E (1990) ‘The Relationship of Heterosexuals’ Attributions for the
Causes of Homosexuality to Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men’, 16(2) Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin 369-377.
Whitley Jr, Bernard E (2001) ‘Gender-Role Variables and Attitudes Toward
Homosexuality’, 45(11/12) Sex Roles 691-721.
Whitley Jr, Bernard E and Stefania Aegisdottir (2000) ‘The Gender Belief System,
Authoritarianism, Social Dominance Orientation and Heterosexuals’ Attitudes Toward
Lesbians and Gay Men’, 42(11/12) Sex Roles 947-967.
Whitley Jr, Bernard E, and Mary E Kite (1995) ‘Sex Differences in Attitudes Toward
Homosexuality: A Comment on Oliver and Hyde (1993)’, 117(1) Psychological Bulletin
146-154.
43
Whitley Jr, Bernard E and Sarah E Lee (2000) ‘The Relationship of Authoritarianism and
Related Constructs to Attitudes Toward Homosexuality’, 30(1) Journal of Applied Social
Psychology 144-170.
Widmer, Eric D, Judith Treas, and Robert Newcomb (1998) ‘Attitudes Toward NonMarital Sex in 24 Countries’, 35(4) Journal of Sex Research 349-358.
Wilkinson, Wayne W (2004) ‘Authoritarian Hegemony, Dimensions of Masculinity, and
Male Antigay Attitudes’, 5(2) Psychology of Men and Masculinity 121-131.
Wills, Georgia and Ryan Crawford (2000) ‘Attitudes Towards Homosexuality in
Shreveport-Bossier City, Louisiana’, 38(3) Journal of Homosexuality 97-116.
Wilson, Stephan M and Nilufer P Medora (1990) ‘Gender Comparisons of College
Students’ Attitudes toward Sexual Behavior’, 25(99) Adolescence 615-627.
Wood, Peter B and John P Bartkowski (2004) ‘Attribution Style and Public Policy
Attitudes Toward Gay Rights’, 85(1) Social Science Quarterly 58-74.
Yang, Alan S (1997) ‘The Polls –Trends: Attitudes Towards Homosexuality’, 61(3)
Public Opinion Quarterly 477-507.
Yang, Alan S (2001) ‘The 2000 National Elections Study and Gay and Lesbian Rights:
Support for Equality Grows’, Policy Institute of the National Gay and Lesbian Task
Force Foundation.
Young, Robert K, Peggy Gallaher, Julie Belasco and Alicia Barr (1991) ‘Changes in fear
of AIDS and homophobia in a university population’, 21(22) Journal of Applied Social
Psychology 1848-1858.
44