Download Mild Thyroid Hormone Insufficiency During Development

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
ORIGINAL
RESEARCH
Mild Thyroid Hormone Insufficiency During
Development Compromises Activity-Dependent
Neuroplasticity in the Hippocampus of Adult Male
Rats
M. E. Gilbert, K. Sanchez-Huerta, and C. Wood
Toxicity Assessment Division (M.E.G., C.W.), National Health and Environmental Effects Research
Laboratory, United States Environmental Protection Agency, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina
27709; Departamento de Fisiología “Mauricio Russek” (K.S.-H.), Escuela Nacional de Ciencias Biológicas,
Instituto Politécnico Nacional, Mexico City, Mexico 07738
Severe thyroid hormone (TH) deficiency during critical phases of brain development results in
irreversible neurological and cognitive impairments. The mechanisms accounting for this are likely
multifactorial, and are not fully understood. Here we pursue the possibility that one important
element is that TH affects basal and activity-dependent neurotrophin expression in brain regions
important for neural processing. Graded exposure to propylthiouracil (PTU) during development
produced dose-dependent reductions in mRNA expression of nerve growth factor (Ngf) in whole
hippocampus of neonates. These changes in basal expression persisted to adulthood despite the
return to euthyroid conditions in blood. In contrast to small PTU-induced reductions in basal
expression of several genes, developmental PTU treatment dramatically reduced the activity-dependent expression of neurotrophins and related genes (Bdnft, Bdnfiv, Arc, and Klf9) in adulthood
and was accompanied by deficits in hippocampal-based learning. These data demonstrate that
mild TH insufficiency during development not only reduces expression of important neurotrophins
that persists into adulthood but also severely restricts the activity-dependent induction of these
genes. Considering the importance of these neurotrophins for sculpting the structural and functional synaptic architecture in the developing and the mature brain, it is likely that TH-mediated
deficits in these plasticity mechanisms contribute to the cognitive deficiencies that accompany
developmental TH compromise. (Endocrinology 157: 774 –787, 2016)
T
hyroid hormone (TH) is essential for normal development of the central nervous system; severe TH deficiency in the fetus and neonate produces severe neurological deficits including cognitive impairment and mental
retardation (1–3). However, even mild TH insufficiency in
pregnant women can produce cognitive deficits in the offspring (1, 4). Recently, animal models have confirmed that
modest degrees of maternal TH insufficiency produce
dose-dependent changes in molecular, anatomical, electrophysiological, and behavioral effects in the offspring
(5– 8). Despite advances in our recognition that the developing brain is sensitive to mild TH insufficiency, the spe-
cific mechanisms by which TH affects brain development,
and the developmental events that are disturbed by TH
insufficiency, remain poorly understood. This is important both because an understanding of the mechanisms
and developmental events controlled by TH may provide
insight into strategies for treating children of women with
TH insufficiency but will also identify the kinds of molecular and developmental endpoints that could be captured in tests of chemicals that may interfere with TH
action.
TH receptors are ligand-activated transcription factors
(9) that regulate the expression of specific genes. This reg-
ISSN Print 0013-7227 ISSN Online 1945-7170
Printed in USA
Copyright © 2016 by the Endocrine Society
Received July 24, 2015. Accepted November 18, 2015.
First Published Online November 24, 2015
Abbreviations: BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CA, Cornu Ammonis; DG, dentate gyrus; dCt, delta cycle threshold; EPSP, excitatory postsynaptic potential; FC, fold
change; LTP, long-term potentiation; MWM, Morris water maze; PN, postnatal day; ppm,
parts-per-million; PS, population spike; PTU, propylthiouracil; TH, thyroid hormone.
774
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
ulation is cell-type and developmental stage specific, making the study of TH action in brain development complex.
We and others have previously identified TH-responsive
elements in the cortex and hippocampus of the neonatal
rat (6, 10 –12) and postulated that neurotrophins may be
important mediators of TH action in the developing hippocampus (11, 13, 14). This possibility is provocative, in
part because neurotrophins in general are strongly implicated in mechanisms of neuronal plasticity during development and in adulthood. The mechanisms of neuronal
plasticity during brain development are similar to those
proposed to occur during learning and neurotrophinbased molecular signals subserving both are conserved
from development to adulthood. TH insufficiency may
interfere with these mechanisms. The present studies were
designed to test this hypothesis in 3 ways. First, we examined the effect of TH insufficiency in pregnant rats on the
basal expression of a number of neurotrophin genes in
young (neonatal) and adult offspring. Secondly, learning
was assessed because learning initiates alterations in glutamate-dependent excitatory synaptic transmission that
are subsequently stabilized through brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-induced structural changes at
the synapse (15–17). We examined learning and memory
in adult offspring of hypthyroxenemic dams using specific
hippocampally mediated spatial learning and contextual
fear conditioning paradigms. Finally, we examined the
effect of induction of long-term potentiation (LTP), the
glutamate-dependent cellular correlate of associative
learning, on neurotrophin expression. Our findings reveal
what may be a critical deficit in the brains of adult offspring exposed to developmental hypothyroxinemia. Specifically, although neurotrophin gene expression was only
modestly affected by developmental hypothyroxinemia,
hippocampal-based learning was impaired, and LTP-induced neurotrophin expression was severely suppressed
by this treatment. Thus, the ability of the brain to respond
to neural activity may be an important mechanism by
which developmental hypothyroidism alters neuronal network formation in the developing brain and contributes to
cognitive deficits in the adult.
Materials and Methods
Developmental exposure
Pregnant Long-Evans rats were obtained from Charles River
on gestational day 2 and housed individually in standard plastic
hanging cages in an Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care-certified animal facility. All animal treatments were in strict accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory
Animals. Animal rooms were maintained on a 12-hour light,
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
775
12-hour dark schedule, and rats were permitted free access to
food (Purina 5008 rat chow) and deionized water. Beginning on
gestational day 6 dams (15/dose group) were administered via
the drinking water, 0, 1, 2, 3, or 10 parts-per-million (ppm,
0.0001%, 0.0002%, 0.0003%, and 0.001%, wt/vol) of the antithyroid agent propylthiouracil (PTU) (Sigma) in deionized water until postnatal day (PN)21. The day of birth was designated
PN0, and all litters were culled to 10 pups (5 males and 5 females
to the extent possible) on PN4. One male and 1 female from each
litter were killed on PN14 and PN90 and blood and hippocampus collected. With the exception of the high-dose group, pups
were weaned on PN21, transferred to plastic hanging cages (2/
cage), and permitted free access to food (Purina 5001) and filtered tap water. Dams exposed to 10 ppm were removed from
PTU water on PN21 but remained with pups for an additional
week to increase survivability of the pups before weaning. Dam
weights were monitored frequently throughout pregnancy and
offspring weights were recorded during the first postnatal month
and once again on PN90. Dams were killed when pups were
weaned and blood collected for TH analysis.
Hippocampus collected from male offspring (n ⫽ 8 litters/
dose group) killed on PN14 and PN90 were flash frozen for gene
expression analysis. Two behavioral tests sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction were conducted on independent groups of adult
male offspring. Finally, LTP was induced and activity-induced
expression of a set of neurotrophin-related genes were assessed.
Behavioral assessments
Two hippocampal-dependent memory tests were evaluated in
different groups of adult male offspring. Spatial learning using
the Morris water maze (MWM) began at approximately 3
months of age in 1 male from each of 13–14 litters/dose group
according to previously described procedures (18). Briefly, 4 trials per day for 5 consecutive days were conducted with animals
introduced into the pool from a different start point on every trial
each day. Animals were allowed to swim freely in search of an
escape platform hidden below the surface of the water, and once
found, to rest upon it for 15 seconds. If not found within 60
seconds, animals were guided to it by the experimenter. On trials
6, 11, 14, and 20, the platform was removed and reinserted after
60 seconds of free swim, and animals rested on it for 15 seconds
before removal from the pool. The time spent searching the correct quadrant and the number of times the animal’s path crossed
the platform position were recorded. Cue testing with a visible
platform raised above the water surface was conducted on the
sixth day of testing.
Beginning at approximately 5 months of age, trace fear conditioning was conducted in a different set of male offspring than
used for MWM. Testing was performed in 9 –12 animals/dose
from 6 –11 litters with no more than 2 animals represented from
any given litter. Procedures followed slight modifications to
those employed previously in our laboratory in hypothyroid animals (7) and were identical to those described by Oshiro et al
(19). Briefly, a total of 2 training trials were given. After a 2-minute baseline period, a compound 15-second light/tone stimulus
was presented, terminating 30 seconds before (the trace interval)
administration of a brief, mild footshock (0.5 seconds, 1 mA). An
intermittent noncontingent “distractor” flashing light stimulus
was randomly presented throughout the training period. Activity
was monitored via a motion detector. The next day, animals were
returned to the same training box and activity monitored for 5
776
Gilbert et al
Hypothyroidism and Activity-Dependent Plasticity
minutes as an index of contextual learning. About 1–2 hours
later, animals were placed in a different testing box with visual,
tactile, and olfactory cues distinct from the training/context testing box and housed in a different test room. Activity was monitored for 2 minutes before and after presentation of the compound light/tone stimulus previously paired with footshock to
evaluate conditioning to cue.
Surgical procedures
Between 9 and 12 months of age, LTP was induced in a subset
of animals previously tested in MWM or fear conditioning several months earlier. Animals were anesthetized with urethane
(1.5–2 g/kg, ip), mounted in a stereotaxic frame, stimulating
electrodes were lowered into the angular bundle of the perforant
path of the left hemisphere according to standard procedures
described previously (20). A recording electrode was lowered
into the ipsilateral dentate gyrus (DG). Optimal depth placement
was achieved through electrophysiological monitoring of the response evoked in the DG after single pulse perforant path stimulation. Only animals for which an optimal waveform morphology, minimum field potential amplitude of 3 mV, an excitatory
postsynaptic potential (EPSP) threshold of less than 200 ␮A, and
stable recordings over the course of electrophysiological monitoring were included in analysis for activity-dependent gene expression. These criteria were achieved in offspring from 8, 6, 10,
and 8 animals from the 0-, 2-, 3-, and 10-ppm dose groups,
respectively. The positive component of the field potential provides an index of synaptic activation resulting from the summed
EPSPs. The slope of the EPSP was calculated as the rate of amplitude change for the initial positive component of the field
potential before population spike (PS) onset.
Electrophysiological recordings and induction of
LTP
Once optimal electrode placement was achieved, responses
were monitored for approximately 2 hours to ensure stability.
Responses were amplified, digitized (33-kHz sampling rate), averaged using LabWindows (National Instruments) and custom
designed software, and stored on a PC for later analysis. Once
stable, intensity was adjusted to produce a PS 30% of the maximal recorded at 1500 ␮A, and a series of sweeps were collected
at 10-second intervals. Five 5-sweep averages sampled at 5-minute intervals were collected at this “test stimulus” intensity just
before application of LTP-inducing trains. Train parameters effective in inducing BDNF expression (21) were applied to the
perforant path (4 8-pulse train bursts, 400 Hz, 1500 ␮A delivered at 10-s intervals, repeated 3 times at 5-min intervals). Sampling of 5-sweep averages at 15-minute intervals at the pretrain
test stimulus intensity resumed 1 minute after the final train
delivery and continued for the next 5 hours when animals were
killed by decapitation. The brain was removed from the skull and
the hippocampus immediately dissected. The DG was separated
from the CA (Cornu Ammonis) regions (primarily CA1 was harvested) from both the stimulated and the unstimulated hemispheres according to procedures outlined in Hagihara et al (22).
Briefly, on removal from the brain, the hippocampus was laid
medial surface up on a cold plate moistened with ice cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid. A 30 gauge hypodermic needle was
used to section along the fissure that separates DG from CA
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
regions. DG and CA1 were collected, flash frozen and stored at
⫺80°C until processing.
Quantitative RT-PCR
RNA was extracted from whole hippocampus in 1 set of
animals killed on PN14 and PN90, and from the DG and CA1
regions of the stimulated and nonstimulated hemispheres of
animals after LTP induction using Tri reagent (Sigma) according to manufacturer’s instructions. RNA pellets were resuspended in nuclease-free H20 and the concentration determined on a Nanodrop ND-100. Two micrograms of total
RNA were initially digested using 2-U DNase I (Promega).
Some of this RNA was quantified using a Quant-iT RiboGreen
RNA assay kit (Life Technologies). Another portion of the
DNased RNA was reverse transcribed using a High Capacity
cDNA Archive kit (Life Technologies) according to the provided protocol. Amplification was performed on an Applied
Biosystems 7900HT Fast Real-Time PCR System in duplicate
using 25-ng cDNA and TaqMan Universal PCR Master Mix
in a total volume of 12 ␮L. PCR cycling conditions were 2
minutes at 50°C, 10 minutes at 95°C, then 40 cycles of 15
seconds at 95°C and 1 minute at 60°C. The TaqMan primers
used are listed in Supplemental Table 1. Selection of ␤-2microglobulin (B2m) and gyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase (Gadph) as endogenous reference genes was based
on expression at uniform levels across all treatments as determined by ANOVA. Relative quantification was measured
by the delta-delta cycle threshold (ddCt) method and mean
fold change (FC) calculated by normalizing to the mean dCt
of the control group.
In the first experiment, probes for BDNF-total (Bdnft),
nerve growth factor (Ngf), and neurotrophin factor 3 (Ntf3)
were evaluated in whole hippocampus. The BDNF exon iv
(Bdnfiv) was also examined as it has been specifically implicated in activity-dependent plasticity (13). Another target
gene for the calcium-binding protein, parvalbumin (Pvalb),
was included as a positive control. In the second experiment,
gene expression was performed independently in the CA1 and
DG regions of the hippocampus, including probes for the neurotrophins above in addition to other activity-dependent immediate early genes Arc (activity-regulated gene, also known
as Arg3.1), early growth factor 1 (Egr-1) (also known as
Zif268, Krox24), mammalian target of rapamycin (mTor),
and the TH-dependent transcription factor Klf9. Gene expression in the nonstimulated hemisphere was taken as estimate of “basal” expression and the reference for induction by
LTP in the stimulated hemisphere.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging has revealed bilateral
hippocampal activation after unilateral stimulation to induce
LTP (23). Bilateral increases in the expression of neurotrophins
have also been reported after unilateral LTP (21), raising a potential confound in the use of the unstimulated hemisphere as the
reference for expressing LTP-induced augmentation in neurotrophin expression. For this reason expression of a subset of gene
targets (Arc, Bdnfiv, Klf9, and Ngf) was examined in the DG
harvested from anesthetized but not implanted animals (surgical
controls) and dCts compared with those obtained from the nonstimulated hemisphere in the 0-ppm group.
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
Statistical analyses
Standard general linear model ANOVA was conducted using
SAS (version 9.2; SAS Institute). Baseline EPSP and PS amplitudes, dCts in DG contrasting surgical control unstimulated
hemisphere expression levels, activity counts during training,
and testing in fear conditioning were evaluated using one-way
ANOVAs with ␣-level set to P ⬍ .05. LTP and MWM acquisition
were assessed with repeated measures ANOVA. Contrast tests
within ANOVA were performed for each dose group relative to
control if significant main effects or interactions were observed.
In MWM, quadrant dwelling time was also evaluated by determining search time that was greater than chance (25%) for each
dose group using t-tests. For gene expression of whole hippocampus, statistical significance of FC from control was determined
by one-way ANOVA followed by the Dunnett’s post hoc test.
Repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for DG and CA1
regions independently in the samples taken after LTP. Factors
included in the analyses were dose (which collapses across hemisphere), stimulation (which collapses across dose), and dose ⫻
hemisphere interaction terms. Significant main effects of dose
indicate PTU-induced alteration in gene expression. Significant
main effects of hemisphere indicate LTP-induced alteration in
gene expression. Most importantly, significant dose ⫻ hemisphere interactions indicate differential effects of PTU on activity-induced gene expression. Because a number of ANOVAs was
performed to evaluate the expression of many genes in the same
samples, an ␣-level of 0.025 was applied to reduce experimentwise error rate for both whole hippocampus and LTP-induced
gene expression experiments.
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
777
PTU effects on neurotrophin gene expression in
hippocampus of the neonate and adult
We selected the neurotrophins Bdnft, Bdnfiv, Ngf, and
Ntf3 because of their role in neurodevelopment and activity-dependent plasticity. No changes in expression of
the neurotrophins Bdnft, Bdnfiv, and Ntf3 were detected in
the hippocampus of offspring of PTU-treated dams (all
P ⬎ .50) (Figure 1A). Ngf was reduced in a dose-dependent
manner in the neonate at the 2 highest dose levels (P ⬍
.0001), a suppression that persisted in the adult hippocampus at PN90 (P ⬍ .0003) (Figure 1B). Although no change
in the expression of Bdnfiv was observed in the neonate
(P ⬎ .065), surprisingly, small but statistically significant
increases were detected in the adult hippocampus at the 2
Results
Characterization of the low-dose PTU model
Serum TH and body weights in dams and pups from
this cohort of animals were previously reported in Johnstone et al (24) but for completeness are summarized in
Supplemental Table 2. Body weight of the dams was not
altered, body weight of pups was reduced in the 10-ppm
dose group from PN4, remaining below controls weights
throughout lactation. The 2- and 3-ppm dose groups did
not differ from controls until PN21 when a small but significant reduction was seen. Body weight deficits disappeared in adulthood for these dose levels, but persisted in
the 10-ppm dose group. Maternal T4 values measured at
weaning of the pups were dose dependently reduced, TSH
was increased in the 2- and 3-ppm dose groups. As described in Materials and Methods, pups were weaned 1
week later in the high-dose group to optimize pup survival
during which time dam and pup TH and TSH had completely recovered. No changes in maternal T3 measures
were observed at any dose. Pup serum T4 on PN14 was
dose dependently reduced and to a greater degree than that
observed in the dam. Reductions in serum T3 in pups were
restricted to the high-dose group.
Figure 1. Neurotrophins in neonate and adult hippocampus. Mean
(⫾SEM, n ⫽ 8/dose group) FC in expression level of 4 neurotrophin
genes in the whole hippocampus from male offspring on (A) PN14 and
(B) PN90. The expression level of Ngf was dose dependently reduced
on PN14 (P ⬍ .0001), and this reduction persisted into adulthood (P ⬍
.0003). No change was detected in the other neurotrophins assessed
on PN14 (Bdnft, Bdnfiv, and Ntf3, all P ⬎ .05) or on PN90 with the
exception of a slight but statistically significant increase in Bdnfiv
expression (P ⬍ .0024). The gene probe for the calcium-binding
protein Pvalb was included as a positive control gene (see text) and
was dose dependently reduced at all dose levels in the neonate (P ⬍
.0001) but exhibited no change in adulthood. *, P ⬍ .05, in Dunnett’s
t test conducted after significant effect of dose was detected in
ANOVA with stringent ␣-level of 0.025 applied to reduce experimentwise error rate.
778
Gilbert et al
Hypothyroidism and Activity-Dependent Plasticity
highest dose levels (P ⬍ .0024). Pvalb was included as a
positive control as it has previously been shown to be
markedly reduced in the hippocampus by perinatal exposure to graded levels of PTU (6, 10). Pvalb expression was
reduced at all dose levels in the neonatal (P ⬍ .0001) (Figure 1A), but was not different from control levels in the
adult hippocampus (P ⬎ .78) (Figure 1B).
Behavioral assessments
Morris water maze
PTU-exposed animals exhibited spatial learning deficits as assessed in the MWM. Latency to find the escape
platform was longer across several trials in the PTU-exposed animals relative to controls (dose, P ⬍ .0001;
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
dose ⫻ trial interaction, P ⬍ .009) (Figure 2A). Acquisition deficits were most pronounced in animals exposed to
10-ppm PTU (mean contrast test in ANOVA, 0 vs 10 ppm,
P ⬍ .0001), but impairments were also evident in the
3-ppm dose group (0 vs 3 ppm, P ⬍ .039), and limited to
the early trials. Performance improved with additional
training, but deficits persisted in the high dose group. Temporary removal of the escape platform during “probe”
trials intermittently inserted throughout acquisition, support these observations. Deficits were seen at all dose levels
on the first probe trial in that a greater than chance dwelling time in the correct quadrant (ie, evidence of learning)
was seen only in controls (Figure 2B). Fewer crossings over
the position where the escape platform had been reliably
placed on previous trails were also observed in dosed animals (Figure 2C) on the first 2 probe
trials (trials 6 and 11, both P ⬍ .01).
No differences between groups were
seen on performance in cue learning
when the platform was made visible
or in swim speed (data not shown).
Figure 2. Morris Water Maze. A, Mean (⫾SEM, n ⫽ 13–14/dose group) latency to find the
hidden platform was increased in 3- and 10-ppm animals indicative of impaired acquisition.
Significant main effects of dose and trial were detected in the overall analysis, and 10- and 3ppm dose groups were different from controls in contrast tests collapsed across trial (⫹, P ⬍ .03;
⫹⫹, P ⬍ .0007). Deficits were restricted to the early learning trials in the 3-ppm dose group but
persisted in the high dose group. Duncan t test contrast to control; *, P ⬍ .05 for 10 ppm; #,
P ⬍ .05 for 3 ppm on individual trials. B, Probe trials were interspersed throughout training on
trials 6, 11, 15, and 20. Chance performance was seen in all dosed groups but not in controls on
the in initial probe. *, P ⬍ .05, indicates significantly different from chance performance (25%
time in correct quadrant, dotted line). C, Mean number of platform crossings were significantly
lower in PTU-treated animals relative to controls (dose, P ⬍ .001), and these differences were
more prevalent in the first (P ⬍ .001) and second (P ⬍ .01) probe trials.
Distract trace fear conditioning
Learning impairments were evident in trace fear conditioning, confirming and extending a previous report by Gilbert (7). No differences in
baseline activity counts were seen during training (data not shown), but animals placed back into the context previously associated with shock did not
suppress their activity to the same extent as controls (dose, P ⬍ .003) (Figure 3A). Cue learning, as evidenced by
suppression of activity during the trace
period, also appeared to be impaired
in exposed offspring, although the
overall ANOVA failed to reveal an
overall effect of dose (P ⫽ .10). Mean
contrast tests at each dose however
suggest impairment at the 3-ppm (P ⬍
.04) and 10-ppm (P ⬍ .03) dose
groups (Figure 3B).
DG field potentials in response
to PTU
Baseline synaptic transmission for
all animals with single electrode penetrations, minimal response amplitudes, and thresholds criteria are
summarized in Table 1. As expected
from previous work (7, 20), the max-
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
779
Distinct from previous work, these reductions in excitatory synaptic transmission were statistically limited to
the highest dose group, likely reflecting the smaller sample
size of the present study based on strict criteria for inclusion for gene analysis (ie, single hemisphere penetration
only). Analysis of baseline synaptic transmission with all
animals included (ie, penetration of both hemispheres)
echoed deficits at lower doses reported previously (data
not shown).
Figure 3. Trace Fear Conditioning. A, Mean (⫾SEM, n ⫽ 9 –12/dose
group) activity counts after reintroduction to the training box in which
shock was received 24 hours earlier were higher in all PTU-treated
groups relative to controls, indicative of a deficit in context learning
(dose, P ⬍ .0028). No differences were detected in baseline activity
levels during training (data not shown). B, Mean (⫾SEM) percent of
baseline activity counts corresponding to the trace interval after tone/
light stimulus 24 hours after training but presented in a unique
context. Higher activity counts were suggestive of impaired learning to
cue but the overall effect of dose failed to reach statistically significant
levels (P ⫽ .10).
imal amplitude of the EPSP slope (P ⬍ .0034) and PS (P ⬍
.0189) of the field potential recorded from the DG were
reduced in PTU-exposed animals. The EPSP slope of the
test stimulus was also smaller as a function of developmental PTU exposure (P ⬍ .0372), but the stimulus intensity required to evoke it did not differ by dose (P ⬎ .36).
DG LTP
Distinct from previous reports with this low-dose
PTU model (7, 20), a more robust stimulation regimen
was followed to maximally induce LTP and neurotrophin expression (see Figure 4A). The EPSP slope amplitude was increased immediately after train delivery in
all groups, but the 3- and 10-ppm dose groups demonstrated a significantly smaller magnitude of LTP at these
early time points (Figure 4B). All groups demonstrated
a decay from maximally induced LTP over the next 5
hours, with declines to pretrain baseline in the 2 highest
dose groups. The 2-ppm group tracked a similar path to
that observed in controls and could not be discriminated
statistically from the 0-ppm dose group. These observations are supported by the results of the repeated measures ANOVA which revealed a significant main effect
of dose (P ⬍ .0061) and time (P ⬍ .0001). The dose ⫻
time interaction trended towards but failed to reach
statistical significance (P ⬍ .0894). Mean contrast tests
to examine the significant main effect of dose revealed
significant differences from controls for the 3-ppm (P ⬍
.0491) and the 10-ppm (P ⬍ .0064) dose levels, with
no difference between 0- and 2-ppm dose group (P ⬎
.47).
Basal and activity-dependent induction of
neurotrophin expression
Minor differences in basal expression were detected
in DG and CA1
PTU-induced effects on basal levels of expression
were examined in the nonstimulated hemisphere as
Table 1. Mean (⫾SEM) EPSP Slope of Probe Stimulus, Set at Intensity Required to Produce a PS 30% of Maximal,
the Intensity Required to Evoke Probe, EPSP Slope, and PS at Maximal Stimulus Intensity of 1500 ␮A
a
PTU Dose
(ppm)
n
Probe Intensity
(␮A)
Probe EPSP
Slope (mv/ms)
Maximal EPSP
Slope (mv/ms)
Maximal Population
Spike (mv)
0
2
3
10
8
6
10
8
533.8 ⫾ 48.7
435.0 ⫾ 48.9
428.0 ⫾ 54.4
537.1 ⫾ 73.5
3.4 ⫾ 0.5
3.3 ⫾ 0.7
3.0 ⫾ 0.4
1.5 ⫾ 0.2a
5.2 ⫾ 0.5
5.4 ⫾ 0.8
4.8 ⫾ 0.7
2.1 ⫾ 0.3a
10.4 ⫾ 1.9
10.6 ⫾ 2.2
9.4 ⫾ 1.6
3.5 ⫾ 0.6a
P ⬍ .05 Dunnett’s t test after significant effect of dose in ANOVA.
780
Gilbert et al
Hypothyroidism and Activity-Dependent Plasticity
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
CA1 regions were independently assessed (both P ⬎
.36) (Figure 6, A and B). However, basal levels of Bdnft
(Figure 6C) and Ntf3 (Figure 5C) that were unchanged
in whole hippocampus (Figure 1B), were slightly reduced in the DG (both P ⬍ .02) but not significantly
altered in CA1 (both P ⬎ .05) (Figures 5D and 6D).
Similarly, basal expression levels of the transcription
factor Klf9 were reduced in DG (P ⬍ .0001) (Figure 6E),
but unchanged in CA1 (P ⬎ .31) (Figure 6F). Basal expression levels of these 3 transcripts, Bdnft, Ntf3, and
Klf9, exhibited a nonmonotonic pattern of change in the
DG, with reductions at lower doses and return to controls levels at the highest PTU dose (Figure 6C and E and
5C, respectively). No effects on basal levels of the immediate early genes Arc (P ⬎ .72) (Figure 6G), mTor, or
Erg-1 (P ⬎ .80) (Supplemental Figure 1, A and B) were
seen in DG. Basal levels of Arc in area CA1 were variable, displaying a large increase at the lower doses and
approaching control levels at the highest dose (P ⬍
.007) (Figure 6H). Despite these changes in basal expression levels that were often restricted to DG, the
magnitude of change paled in comparison with the large
increases in neurotrophins induced by LTP as described
below.
Figure 4. Long Term Potentiation. LTP was reduced in adult offspring
of PTU-exposed dams. A, Train stimulation protocol optimized to
induce LTP-dependent neurotrophin expression. Field potential from
the adult offspring of a control dam taken before (dotted line),
immediately after, and at 15-minute intervals after delivery of a series
of high frequency trains to induce a robust and long lasting (⬎5 h)
LTP. A leftward shift in the slope of the rising phase of the EPSP and a
dramatic increase in the size of the negative component of the
potential (PS) are indicative of LTP. B, Mean (⫾SEM) percent of
baseline (BL) EPSP slope amplitude before (BL), immediately after (h ⫽
0), and at 15-minute intervals for the next 5 hours. Only animals with
single penetration in one hemisphere, minimal response amplitudes of
3mV, EPSP thresholds of less than 200 ␮A, and stable recordings
throughout the pre- and posttrain periods were included (see Table 1).
Significant main effects of dose (P ⬍ .0061) and time (P ⬍ .0001) were
detected. There was no difference in the magnitude of LTP seen
between the 0- and 2-ppm dose groups (P ⬎ .47), but LTP was
significantly impaired in both the 3-ppm (⫹, P ⬍ .05) and 10-ppm
(⫹⫹, P ⬍ .0064) dose groups.
these could influence interpretation of any observed effects of stimulation. In general, findings were consistent
with results in whole hippocampus of the adult offspring. Ngf was reduced in a dose-dependent manner in
both DG and CA1 subregions (both P ⬍ .006) (Figure
5, A and B), reflecting the same pattern as seen in whole
hippocampus (Figure 1B). The very modest increase in
expression of Bdnfiv seen in whole hippocampus (Figure
1B) was not detected under conditions where DG and
Activity-dependent expression in DG induced by LTP
Activity-dependent changes in gene expression were examined by contrasting expression levels in the stimulated and
nonstimulated hemispheres. Statistically, LTP-induced
changes in gene expression are confirmed by a main effect of
hemisphere (line above LTP histograms in Figures 5 and 6
indicates significant change from basal levels). LTP induced
robust increases in expression of Ngf, Bdnfiv, Bdnft,Klf9, and
Arc, and in the DG (LTP, all P ⬍ .0001) (Figures 5A and 6,
A, C, E, and G). A significant effect of hemisphere was also
obtained for Ntf3, but a reduction rather an increase over
basal levels was seen with LTP (P ⬍ .0001) (Figure 5C).
These data indicate that the regulation of these genes is modulated by neuronal activity and are consistent with previous
reports investigating DG LTP using in situ hybridization to
examine neurotrophin expression (21, 25, 26).
However, no differential effects of hypothyroidism
on activity-dependent induction of these neurotrophins
was detected. Differential effects of PTU exposure on
activity-dependent induction of gene transcripts are indicated by significant dose ⫻ hemisphere interactions
and summarized in Figure 6, left panel. Only under conditions of a significant interaction were mean contrast
tests conducted to identify PTU-induced effects on activity-dependent induction. A strong suppression in activity-induced expression of both Bdnf transcripts was
evident at all dose levels of PTU tested (LTP, both P ⬍
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
781
Figure 5. Basal but not activity-dependent changes in Ngf and Ntf3. Only animals passing strict criteria were included in genomic analysis (text
and Figure 4), resulting in sample sizes of 8 0-ppm, 6 2-ppm, 10 3-ppm, and 8 10-ppm animals. Basal levels of expression of Ngf were dose
dependently reduced in PTU-exposed rats in both DG (A) and CA1 (B) regions (dose, both P ⬍ .0035). Stimulation to induce LTP produced a large
fold increase in expression of Ngf in the DG (hemisphere, P ⬍ .0001), but this was not differentially altered in PTU-exposed animals, because no
dose ⫻ hemisphere interaction was detected (P ⬎ .93). Only modest changes in Ngf expression in CA1 followed LTP stimulation, reductions rather
than increases, were observed (hemisphere, P ⬍ .0011), but no differential effect of PTU was detected (dose ⫻ hemisphere interactions, P ⬎ .05).
C, Ntf3 expression was reduced in response to LTP stimulation in both DG (C) and CA1 (D) (significant effect of hemisphere, both P ⬍ .0001), but
as with Ngf, not differentially impacted by PTU exposure. Significant main effects of hemisphere are depicted by a line with asterisk above LTP
grouping. Mean contrast tests for basal expression revealed by Dunnett’s t test, *, P ⬍ .05 after significant main effect of dose.
.005) (Figure 6, A and C) and in Arc expression at the
2 highest dose levels (P ⬍ .025) (Figure 6G). Expression
of Klf9 was reduced at the 2 lower doses, with some
recovery evident in the highest dose group (P ⬍ .0007)
(Figure 6E), mirroring the pattern seen under basal conditions for this gene. Although Ngf was increased (Figure 5A), and Ntf3 reduced (Figure 5C) as a function of
LTP, neither were differentially affected by PTU under
stimulation conditions (dose ⫻ hemisphere interaction,
P ⬎ .16). Neither were the immediate early genes Erg1
and mTor, downstream targets of BDNF, induced by
LTP or affected by PTU in the basal or stimulated state
(Supplemental Figure 2).
Site specificity
Hippocampal CA subregions of both hemispheres were
assessed as a control for site specificity of stimulation-induced changes. As in DG, LTP-inducing stimulation up upregulated expression of Bdnfiv, Bdnft, Klf9, and Arc in the
CA1 region (significant main effect of hemisphere, P ⬍ .004)
(Figure 6, B, D, F, and H), and modestly reduced expression
of Ngf and Ntf3 (P ⬍ .0001) (Figure 5, B and D). Significant
dose ⫻ hemisphere interactions in the analyses of CA1 were
largely absent, indicating no differential effect of PTU on
activity-dependent induction of Ngf, Ntf, Bdnfiv, Bdnft, and
Klf9. Although a significant interaction was seen in area CA1
for Arc (P ⬍ .002) (Figure 6H), it was not sufficiently robust
to statistically distinguish any PTU dose group from controls.
Importantly, all changes induced by stimulation and affected
by developmental PTU exposure were of a much smaller
magnitude in CA1 relative to DG (LTP) (Figures 5 and 6, left
vs right panels).
Surgical controls
A third “surgical control experiment” was conducted to
evaluate expression of a subset of these gene targets in DG
harvested from anesthetized but not implanted animals (n ⫽
9). A comparison of dCts revealed very similar levels of expression of Bdnfiv and Klf9 in the DG of surgical control and
the nonstimulated hemisphere of 0-ppm control animals un-
782
Gilbert et al
Hypothyroidism and Activity-Dependent Plasticity
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
Figure 6. Basal and LTP-induced expression of BDNF-related transcripts are suppressed by PTU. Basal levels of expression of Bdnfiv (A), and Arc (G)
in the DG were largely unchanged in PTU-exposed animals. Modest reductions were observed in Bdnft (C, P ⬍ .008) and Klf9 transcripts (E, P ⬍
.0001). Stimulation to induce LTP produced large increases in expression of all of these transcripts in the DG of control animals that were
dramatically blunted in those exposed to PTU. Activity-dependent changes induced by LTP are ascertained by significant main effects of
hemisphere depicted as line with asterisk in LTP panels (all P ⬍ .0001). Differential effects in control vs PTU-exposed groups are supported by
significant dose ⫻ hemisphere interactions (all P ⬍ .005) followed by mean contrast tests (#, P ⬍ .05, mean contrast Dunnett t test relative to LTPFC in 0-ppm group). Although significant main effects of hemisphere were also evident in area CA1 (B, D, F, and H), these were smaller in
magnitude and not differentially affected by PTU (no significant dose ⫻ hemisphere interactions).
dergoing LTP (both P ⬎ .35). However, expression of Arc
was lower (dCt means ⫽ 7.59 ⫾ 0.18 and 6.07 ⫾ 0.17, P ⬍
.0001) and Ngf slightly higher (6.83 ⫾ 0.08 and 7.28 ⫾ 0.05,
P ⬍ .0002) in the nonstimulated DG relative to the surgical
control DG, respectively. The latter is consistent with previ-
ous reports of bilateral hippocampal activation and induction of Ngf after unilateral administration of LTP-inducing
stimulation (21, 23). Although these differences in basal levels of expression in animals undergoing surgery may result in
an underestimation of the magnitude of activity-dependent
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
increase in expression for some transcripts, they are not likely
to alter the pattern of change observed or counter the conclusions drawn.
Discussion
In this study, we have demonstrated that small changes in
neurotrophin expression were detected under basal conditions, but activity-dependent induction of neurotrophin-related gene expression was severely impaired by developmental hypothyroxinemia. This deficit was
accompanied by impairments in synaptic plasticity and in
tests of hippocampal-dependent learning and memory.
These findings suggest that disruption of neurotrophin
signaling may represent a fundamental deficiency underlying neurological impairments induced by developmental
TH insufficiency.
Basal levels of neurotrophins
Basal levels of expression of neurotrophins at the doses
examined were largely unaffected, either in whole hippocampus of the neonate and adult, or in DG and CA1
subdivisions of adult hippocampus. Ngf was a noteworthy
exception - expression levels of this gene were reduced in
the hippocampus of the neonate, changes persisted to
adulthood, and a pattern was recapitulated with microdissections of hippocampal subregions in an independent
group of animals. The findings in the neonatal hippocampus are consistent with reports by others after more severe
TH insufficiency (27–29). As such, Ngf may represent a
robust biomarker of altered TH-dependent action during
hippocampal development that imparts a lingering signature that can be detected after recovery of TH status in
serum. By contrast, transcripts of Bdnft and Bdnfiv were
not changed in the hippocampus of the neonate, and although very modest increases in Bdnfiv expression were
detected in the adult hippocampus, these were not reproduced in independent assessments of DG and CA1. So
although neurotrophins have been previously implicated
in TH-induced neurotoxicity, changes in basal levels of
expression are not readily apparent in response to moderate TH insults.
Hippocampal learning and memory
Both tests of hippocampal-dependent memory were
impaired in adult offspring of hypothyroxenemic dams.
Spatial learning acquisition deficits were most profound in
the high-dose animals, but longer latencies to find the escape platform were also present in animals exposed to 3
ppm, replicating previous findings with this task (20), and
consistent with impairments reported using other spatial
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
783
learning tests (30, 31). Insertion of probe trials throughout
the acquisition phase revealed that all PTU-exposed animals, but not controls, did not differ from chance performance in quadrant dwelling time on the first probe (trial
6 of acquisition), and the 2 higher doses remained at
chance levels on subsequent probe trials. Less time in the
correct quadrant was also accompanied by fewer platform
position crossings at all dose levels relative to controls on
the first probe trial. Similarly, in a trace fear conditioning
paradigm, conditioning to context was impaired at all
dose levels. Conditioning to cue failed to reach statistical
significance but showed a trend with a similar profile of
impairment to that observed for context conditioning.
Training in similar hippocampal-based learning paradigms increases Bdnf mRNA expression (15) and impairment of these tasks is observed in mice with hippocampusspecific deletion of BDNF (32) Learning impairments in
PTU-exposed animals may reflect deficiencies in BDNFrelated activity-dependent signaling pathways as described below.
Activity-dependent induction of neurotrophins in
controls
A very robust stimulation paradigm was used to elicit
maximal LTP and optimize induction of activity-dependent gene expression. Impairments in EPSP slope LTP
were observed in PTU-exposed offspring, consistent with
previous reports from our laboratory using a similar dosing regimen but a much milder stimulation (7, 20). Also
consistent with previous LTP reports, control animals exhibited large increases in the expression of Ngf, both Bdnf
transcripts, the immediate early gene Arc, and reductions
in Ntf3 expression in response to LTP (16, 17, 21, 25,
33–36). We report for the first time LTP-induced increases
in the TH-dependent transcription factor Klf9, a TH-responsive gene first identified in amphibians (37), and subsequently observed in neonatal rat brain (38). Klf9 is predominantly expressed in the outer fields of granules cells
of the developing rat DG, but can be induced by seizures
in the adult and has been proposed as a regulator of activity-dependent integration of neurons into the DG during adult neurogenesis (39). Our data support this role for
Klf9 as a regulator of activity-dependent neuronal change
in the DG. They extend the observations of Scobie et al
(39) demonstrating Klf9 activation in response to the
milder form of neuronal activation that accompanies LTP.
Collectively, these findings further implicate TH as a modulator of activity-dependent plasticity that subserves
learning and memory.
784
Gilbert et al
Hypothyroidism and Activity-Dependent Plasticity
TH insufficiency and activity-dependent induction
of neurotrophins
The activity-dependent induction in Bdnft, Bdnfiv, Arc,
and Klf9 expression in control animals was severely
blunted in animals experiencing developmental TH insufficiency. During LTP, Bdnf and its downstream target Arc
translocate to the dendrites and accumulate at active synapses in response to neuronal activation (17, 40). These
actions serve to support both the persistent enhancements
in synaptic transmission of LTP and the structural remodeling of the active synaptic zone (16, 33, 36, 41), and were
clearly compromised by TH insufficiency. Robust LTPinduced increases in expression of the transcription factor
Klf9 were also strongly suppressed in DG of PTU-exposed
animals. DG LTP is impaired in the Klf9 knockout mouse
(39). To our knowledge the present data are not only the
first report of Klf9 induction by LTP stimulation in control
animals, but also the first to reveal persistent impairments
in its basal and activity-induced expression by developmental hypothyroidism.
Interestingly, a nonmonotonic pattern in basal and activity-induced expression was observed in the DG for Klf9
and the Bdnft transcript, the significance of which is unclear. It is possible that the delayed weaning in the highdose group (see Materials and Methods) may have served
as an early enrichment, countering some of the effects of
hypothyroidism (42). Alternatively, the severity of the hypothyroidism associated with 10-ppm PTU may have been
sufficient to activate injury-induced responses of neurotrophins and related genes (43, 44).
Differential effects of TH insufficiency on
neurotrophins
Not all LTP-responsive neurotrophins were altered in
the same way in response to TH insufficiency. Although
basal expression of Ngf and Ntf3 was reduced in the DG
of PTU-exposed animals, and both transcripts were responsive to LTP stimulation, distinct from BDNF transcripts, no differential effect of PTU on activity-dependent
induction of expression was detected. Neither were the
immediate early genes Egr-1 and mTor impacted by hypothyroidism or LTP. Egr-1 and mTor are BDNF-associated transcripts that are rapidly and transiently induced
during synaptic plasticity, memory consolidation, and recall (40, 41, 45– 47). Failure to demonstrate changes in
these transcripts is not surprising given the rapid onset and
transient responses of these genes, a profile that was not
compatible with the single 5-hour sampling time of the
present study chosen to optimize BDNF expression.
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
Which came first, LTP impairments or
neurotrophin deficiencies?
The suppression of activity-dependent up-regulation of
Bdnft, Bdnfiv, Arc, and Klf9 are largely consistent with the
observed impairment in LTP induction and decay. It is
difficult to ascertain whether the LTP impairments are the
consequence of deficiencies in mechanisms controlling activation of critical activity-dependent genes, or whether
the deficiencies in gene activation are the consequence of
failed LTP. However, comparable levels of LTP between
controls and the lowest dose group in the presence of suppressed gene expression suggest that developmental TH
insufficiency permanently disrupts the underlying machinery controlling activity-dependent plasticity. Although the nonmonotonic pattern of “recovery” of expression of Bdnft and Klf9 at the highest dose was not
reflected in functional measures of LTP, spatial and context learning deficits were evident at all doses of PTU.
These data indicate that the milder degrees of TH disruption sufficient to suppress activity-dependent genes are
associated with persistent neurological impairments in the
adult.
Implications of TH-dependent disruption of
neurotrophin-based plasticity
The mechanisms underlying plasticity involve strengthening (LTP) and weakening (long-term depression) of synaptic connections, synaptogenesis, structural remodeling,
and neurogenesis (27, 36, 48). All these processes are active in the developing brain and are reliant on neurotrophin pathways. These same biological substrates are also
conserved in the adult brain where they support learning
and memory. Direct infusion of BDNF into the hippocampus enhances synaptic strength, modulates the induction
of LTP, and induces structural changes that support learning. Blockade or hippocampus-specific deletion of BDNF
impairs spatial memory and LTP, and learning induces a
time-dependent induction of BDNF expression (15, 17,
32, 34, 36, 41, 49 –51). Compromise of this key neurotrophin pathway by developmental TH insufficiency may
contribute to altered connectivity in the developing hippocampus (11, 52–54) in addition to reducing structural
(27, 55–57), physiological, and behavioral plasticity in the
adult (7, 20). As such, disruption of neurotrophin-based
activity-dependent processes as reported here represents a
unifying mechanism whereby TH can influence structural
and functional synaptic architecture of the developing
brain and impair learning in the adult.
Willoughby et al (3) recently described deficiencies in
memory function and hippocampal size in children of adolescent age born to hypothyroxinemic women. Román et
al (58) reported an association between maternal hypo-
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
thyroxinemia and increased risk of autistic symptoms, including cognitive impairment in offspring. Others have
argued, largely based on models of severe TH deficiencies,
that diminished BDNF and related molecules are responsible for abnormal dendritic and synaptic differentiation
and altered network connectivity (14, 59) and underlie the
clinical manifestations described by Willoughby et al (3)
and Román et al (58). The present data represent the first
direct empirical support of TH-mediated disruption of activity-dependent neurotrophin signaling essential for synaptic plasticity and support this contention. They demonstrate further that these deficits are induced after moderate
degrees of TH insufficiency and that TH-mediated deficiencies in neurotrophin signaling may underlie neurological and cognitive deficits in both the neonate and adult
offspring after maternal hypothyroxenemia.
Conclusions
In conclusion, TH insufficiency decreased basal expression levels of the neurotrophin Ngf in the hippocampus of
the hypothyroxenemic neonate, and these decrements persisted to adulthood despite complete recovery of TH status. In contrast to relatively minor TH-dependent changes
in basal expression of other neurotrophins, dramatic deficiencies in the induction of BDNF and related activitydependent plasticity markers in adult offspring were unveiled by LTP stimulation. In addition, LTP-induced
increases in the expression of the TH-responsive transcription factor Klf9 and its vulnerability to developmental TH
insufficiency were demonstrated for the first time. These
alterations in expression of activity-dependent genes after
moderate degrees of developmental TH insufficiency were
accompanied by deficits in spatial learning and context
fear conditioning. Considering the importance of these
neurotrophin-based plasticity mechanisms in synaptogenesis and network connectivity in the developing brain, it is
likely that TH-mediated impairments contribute to altered structural and functional pathways in both the developing and the adult brain.
Acknowledgments
We thank Andrew Johnstone, Masashi Hasegawa, Joan Hedge,
and Michelle Taylor for technical assistance. We also thank Dr
Stephen Lasley, Dr Christopher Lau, and Dr William Mundy for
their comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We
especially thank the comments of Dr R. Thomas Zoeller in revision of this article. This document has been subjected to review
by the National Health and Environmental Effects Research
Laboratory and approved for publication. Approval does not
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
785
signify that the contents reflect the views of the Agency, nor does
mention of trade names or commercial products constitute endorsement or recommendation for use.
Address all correspondence and requests for reprints to: M. E.
Gilbert, PhD, Toxicity Assessment Division (MD-B105-05), National Health and Environmental Effects Research Laboratory,
United States Environmental Protection Agency, Research Triangle Park, NC 27711. E-mail: [email protected].
Disclosure Summary: The authors have nothing to disclose.
References
1. Rovet JF, Willoughby KA. Maternal thyroid function during
pregnancy: effects on the developing fetal brain. In: Maternal
Influences on Fetal Neurodevelopment. New York, NY: Springer;
2010:55–77.
2. Williams, G.R. Neurodevelopmental and neurophysiological actions of thyroid hormone. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20(6):784 –
794.
3. Willoughby KA, McAndrews MP, Rovet JF. Effects of maternal
hypothyroidism on offspring hippocampus and memory. Thyroid.
2014;24(3):576 –584.
4. Morreale de Escobar G, Obregón MJ, Escobar del Rey F. Is neuropsychological development related to maternal hypothyroidism or
to maternal hypothyroxinemia? J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2000;
85(11):3975–3987.
5. Ausó E, Lavado-Autric R, Cuevas E, Del Rey FE, Morreale De Escobar G, Berbel P. A moderate and transient deficiency of maternal
thyroid function at the beginning of fetal neocorticogenesis alters
neuronal migration. Endocrinology. 2004;145(9):4037– 4047.
6. Bastian TW, Prohaska JR, Georgieff MK, Anderson GW. Fetal and
neonatal iron deficiency exacerbates mild thyroid hormone insufficiency effects on male thyroid hormone levels and brain thyroid
hormone-responsive gene expression. Endocrinology. 2014;155(3):
1157–1167.
7. Gilbert ME. Impact of low-level thyroid hormone disruption induced by propylthiouracil on brain development and function. Toxicol Sci. 2011;124(2):432– 445.
8. Gilbert ME, Ramos RL, McCloskey DP, Goodman JH. Subcortical
band heterotopia in rat offspring following maternal hypothyroxinaemia: structural and functional characteristics. J Neuroendocrinol. 2014;26(8):528 –541.
9. Brent GA. Mechanisms of thyroid hormone action. J Clin Invest.
2012;122(9):3035–3043.
10. Royland JE, Parker JS, Gilbert ME. A genomic analysis of subclinical
hypothyroidism in hippocampus and neocortex of the developing
rat brain. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20(12):1319 –1338.
11. Chakraborty G, Magagna-Poveda A, Parratt C, Umans JG, MacLusky NJ, Scharfman HE. Reduced hippocampal brain-derived
neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in neonatal rats after prenatal exposure
to propylthiouracil (PTU). Endocrinology. 2012;153(3):1311–
1316.
12. Bernal J. Action of thyroid hormone in brain. J Endocrinol Invest.
2002;25(3):268 –288.
13. Gilbert ME, Lasley SM. Developmental thyroid hormone insufficiency and brain development: a role for brain-derived neurotrophic
factor (BDNF)? Neuroscience. 2013;239:253–270.
14. Berbel P, Navarro D, Roman GC. An evo-devo approach to thyroid
hormones in cerebral and cerebellar cortical development: etiological implications for autism. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2014;5:
146.
15. Tyler WJ, Alonso M, Bramham CR, Pozzo-Miller LD. From acqui-
786
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
Gilbert et al
Hypothyroidism and Activity-Dependent Plasticity
sition to consolidation: on the role of brain-derived neurotrophic
factor signaling in hippocampal-dependent learning. Learn Mem.
2002;9(5):224 –237.
Bramham CR, Messaoudi E. BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: the synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Prog Neurobiol. 2005;
76(2):99 –125.
Messaoudi E, Kanhema T, Soulé J, et al. Sustained Arc/Arg3.1 synthesis controls long-term potentiation consolidation through regulation of local actin polymerization in the dentate gyrus in vivo.
J Neurosci. 2007;27(39):10445–10455.
Gilbert ME, Hedge JM, Valentín-Blasini L, et al. An animal model
of marginal iodine deficiency during development: the thyroid axis
and neurodevelopmental outcome. Toxicol Sci. 2013;132(1):177–
195.
Oshiro WM, Beasley TE, McDaniel KL, et al. Selective cognitive
deficits in adult rats after prenatal exposure to inhaled ethanol. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2014;45:44 –58.
Gilbert ME, Sui L. Dose-dependent reductions in spatial learning
and synaptic function in the dentate gyrus of adult rats following
developmental thyroid hormone insufficiency. Brain Res. 2006;
1069(1):10 –22.
Bramham CR, Southard T, Sarvey JM, Herkenham M, Brady LS.
Unilateral LTP triggers bilateral increases in hippocampal neurotrophin and trk receptor mRNA expression in behaving rats: evidence
for interhemispheric communication. J Comp Neurol. 1996;368(3):
371–382.
Hagihara H, Toyama K, Yamasaki N, Miyakawa T. Dissection of
hippocampal dentate gyrus from adult mouse. J Vis Exp. 2009;(33):
e1543.
Alvarez-Salvado E, Pallarés V, Moreno A, Canals S. Functional MRI
of long-term potentiation: imaging network plasticity. Philos Trans
R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2014;369(1633):20130152.
Johnstone AF, Gilbert ME, Aydin C, Grace CE, Hasegawa M, Gordon CJ. Thermoregulatory deficits in adult Long Evans rat exposed
perinatally to the antithyroidal drug, propylthiouracil. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;39:1– 8.
Castrén E, Pitkänen M, Sirviö J, et al. The induction of LTP increases
BDNF and NGF mRNA but decreases NT-3 mRNA in the dentate
gyrus. Neuroreport. 1993;4(7):895– 898.
Lindholm D, Castrén E, Berzaghi M, Blöchl A, Thoenen H. Activitydependent and hormonal regulation of neurotrophin mRNA levels
in the brain–implications for neuronal plasticity. J Neurobiol. 1994;
25(11):1362–1372.
Zhang L, Blomgren K, Kuhn HG, Cooper-Kuhn CM. Effects of
postnatal thyroid hormone deficiency on neurogenesis in the juvenile and adult rat. Neurobiol Dis. 2009;34(2):366 –374.
Alvarez-Dolado M, Iglesias T, Rodríguez-Peña A, Bernal J, Muñoz
A. Expression of neurotrophins and the trk family of neurotrophin
receptors in normal and hypothyroid rat brain. Brain Res Mol Brain
Res. 1994;27(2):249 –257.
Oh JD, Butcher LL, Woolf NJ. Thyroid hormone modulates the
development of cholinergic terminal fields in the rat forebrain: relation to nerve growth factor receptor. Brain Res Dev Brain Res.
1991;59(2):133–142.
Axelstad M, Hansen PR, Boberg J, et al. Developmental neurotoxicity of Propylthiouracil (PTU) in rats: relationship between transient hypothyroxinemia during development and long-lasting behavioural and functional changes. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2008;
232(1):1–13.
Akaike M, Kato N, Ohno H, Kobayashi T. Hyperactivity and spatial
maze learning impairment of adult rats with temporary neonatal
hypothyroidism. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1991;13(3):317–322.
Heldt SA, Stanek L, Chhatwal JP, Ressler KJ. Hippocampus-specific
deletion of BDNF in adult mice impairs spatial memory and extinction of aversive memories. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(7):656 – 670.
Panja D, Bramham CR. BDNF mechanisms in late LTP formation:
Endocrinology, February 2016, 157(2):774 –787
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
41.
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
51.
52.
a synthesis and breakdown. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(pt C):
664 – 676.
Messaoudi E, Ying SW, Kanhema T, Croll SD, Bramham CR. Brainderived neurotrophic factor triggers transcription-dependent, late
phase long-term potentiation in vivo. J Neurosci. 2002;22(17):
7453–7461.
Wibrand K, Messaoudi E, Håvik B, et al. Identification of genes
co-upregulated with Arc during BDNF-induced long-term potentiation in adult rat dentate gyrus in vivo. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23(6):
1501–1511.
Bruel-Jungerman E, Davis S, Laroche S. Brain plasticity mechanisms
and memory: a party of four. Neuroscientist. 2007;13(5):492–505.
Hoopfer ED, Huang L, Denver RJ. Basic transcription element binding protein is a thyroid hormone-regulated transcription factor expressed during metamorphosis in Xenopus laevis. Dev Growth Differ. 2002;44(5):365–381.
Denver RJ, Williamson KE. Identification of a thyroid hormone
response element in the mouse Kruppel-like factor 9 gene to explain
its postnatal expression in the brain. Endocrinology. 2009;150(8):
3935–3943.
Scobie KN, Hall BJ, Wilke SA, et al. Krüppel-like factor 9 is necessary for late-phase neuronal maturation in the developing dentate
gyrus and during adult hippocampal neurogenesis. J Neurosci.
2009;29(31):9875–9887.
Li L, Carter J, Gao X, Whitehead J, Tourtellotte WG. The neuroplasticity-associated arc gene is a direct transcriptional target of
early growth response (Egr) transcription factors. Mol Cell Biol.
2005;25(23):10286 –10300.
Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Medina JH. BDNF and memory
processing. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(pt C):677– 683.
Branchi I, D’Andrea I, Fiore M, Di Fausto V, Aloe L, Alleva E. Early
social enrichment shapes social behavior and nerve growth factor
and brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels in the adult mouse
brain. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(7):690 – 696.
Lasley SM, Gilbert ME. Developmental thyroid hormone insufficiency reduces expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor
(BDNF) in adults but not in neonates. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2011;
33(4):464 – 472.
Cortés C, Eugenin E, Aliaga E, et al. Hypothyroidism in the adult rat
causes incremental changes in brain-derived neurotrophic factor,
neuronal and astrocyte apoptosis, gliosis, and deterioration of postsynaptic density. Thyroid. 2012;22(9):951–963.
Tang SJ, Reis G, Kang H, Gingras AC, Sonenberg N, Schuman EM.
A rapamycin-sensitive signaling pathway contributes to long-term
synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
2002;99(1):467– 472.
Knapska E, Kaczmarek L. A gene for neuronal plasticity in the mammalian brain: Zif268/Egr-1/NGFI-A/Krox-24/TIS8/ZENK? Prog
Neurobiol. 2004;74(4):183–211.
Slipczuk L, Bekinschtein P, Katche C, Cammarota M, Izquierdo I,
Medina JH. BDNF activates mTOR to regulate GluR1 expression
required for memory formation. PLoS One. 2009;4(6):e6007.
Caroni P, Donato F, Muller D. Structural plasticity upon learning:
regulation and functions. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(7):478 – 490.
Kuipers SD, Tiron A, Soule J, Messaoudi E, Trentani A, Bramham
CR. Selective survival and maturation of adult-born dentate granule
cells expressing the immediate early gene Arc/Arg3.1. PLoS One.
2009;4(3):e4885.
Kameda M, Taylor CJ, Walker TL, Black DM, Abraham WC, Bartlett PF. Activation of latent precursors in the hippocampus is dependent on long-term potentiation. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e72.
Waterhouse EG, An JJ, Orefice LL, et al. BDNF promotes differentiation and maturation of adult-born neurons through GABAergic
transmission. J Neurosci. 2012;32(41):14318 –14330.
Madeira MD, Paula-Barbosa MM. Reorganization of mossy fiber
synapses in male and female hypothyroid rats: a stereological study.
J Comp Neurol. 1993;337(2):334 –352.
doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1643
53. Rami A, Patel AJ, Rabié A. Thyroid hormone and development of
the rat hippocampus: morphological alterations in granule and pyramidal cells. Neuroscience. 1986;19(4):1217–1226.
54. Rami A, Rabié A. Delayed synaptogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the
thyroid-deficient developing rat. Dev Neurosci. 1990;12(6):398 –
405.
55. Ambrogini P, Cuppini R, Ferri P, et al. Thyroid hormones affect
neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of adult rat. Neuroendocrinology.
2005;81(4):244 –253.
56. Desouza LA, Ladiwala U, Daniel SM, Agashe S, Vaidya RA,
Vaidya VA. Thyroid hormone regulates hippocampal neurogen-
press.endocrine.org/journal/endo
787
esis in the adult rat brain. Mol Cell Neurosci. 2005;29(3):414 –
426.
57. Kapoor R, Fanibunda SE, Desouza LA, Guha SK, Vaidya VA. Perspectives on thyroid hormone action in adult neurogenesis. J Neurochem. 2015;133(5):599 – 616.
58. Román GC, Ghassabian A, Bongers-Schokking JJ, et al. Association
of gestational maternal hypothyroxinemia and increased autism
risk. Ann Neurol. 2013;74(5):733–742.
59. Chang K, Shin JI. Association of gestational maternal hypothyroxinemia and increased autism risk: the role of brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Ann Neurol. 2014;75(6):971.