Download Dysthymia: a review of pharmacological and behavioral

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
Molecular Psychiatry (2000) 5, 242–261
 2000 Macmillan Publishers Ltd All rights reserved 1359-4184/00 $15.00
www.nature.com/mp
REVIEW ARTICLE
Dysthymia: a review of pharmacological and behavioral
factors
J Griffiths1, AV Ravindran1, Z Merali1,2 and H Anisman1,3
1
Department of Psychiatry, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada; 2Institute of Cellular and Molecular Medicine, Ottawa,
Canada; 3Institute of Neuroscience, Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada
Although dysthymia, a chronic, low-grade form of depression, has a morbidity rate as high
as that of major depression, and increases the risk for major depressive disorder, limited
information is available concerning the etiology of this illness. In the present report we review
literature concerning the biological and characterological features of dysthymia, the effectiveness of antidepressant treatments, the influence of stressors in the precipitation and maintenance of the disorder, and both quality of life and psychosocial correlates of the illness. We
also provisionally suggest that dysthymia may stem from disturbances of neuroendocrine
and neurotransmitter functioning (eg, corticotropin releasing hormone and arginine vasopressin within the hypothalamus, or alternatively monoamine variations within several extrahypothalamic sites), and may also involve cytokine activation. The central disturbances may reflect
phenotypic variations of neuroendocrine processes or sensitization of such mechanisms. It
is suggested that chronic stressor experiences or stressors encountered early in life lead to
the phenotypic neurochemical alterations, which then favor the development of the dysthymic
state. Owing to the persistence of the neurochemical disturbances, vulnerability to double
depression is increased, and in this instance treatment with antidepressants may attenuate
the symptoms of major depression but not those of the basal dysthymic state. Moreover, the
residual features of depression following treatment may be indicative of underlying neurochemical disturbances, and may also serve to increase the probability of illness recurrence
or relapse. Molecular Psychiatry (2000) 5, 242–261.
Keywords: depression; antidepressants; corticotropin releasing hormone; arginine vasopressin;
stress; illness recurrence; residual features
Introduction
Dysthymia, a chronic, low-grade form of depression,
occurs in a substantial portion of the population, and
increases the risk for major depressive disorder. Yet,
relative to major depression, limited information is
available concerning the behavioral concomitants, as
well as the physiological correlates of dysthymia. Dysthymia and major depression share several features
regarding stress/coping, and the response to pharmacotherapy. However, it appears that they can be distinguished from one another with respect to hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) functioning, and there is
evidence that these depressive subtypes can be differentiated with regard to their cytokine correlates. Yet,
there is reason to suppose that, owing to its chronic
nature, dysthymia may be associated with persistent
functional changes of HPA activity, as well as several
adjunctive features, including altered stressor and
uplift perceptions, coping styles, and quality of life.1–4
Together, these factors may perpetuate the illness, pro-
Correspondence: H Anisman, Institute of Neuroscience, Life
Science Research Centre, Ottawa, Ontario, K1S 5B6, Canada.
E-mail: hanisman얀ccs.carleton.ca
Received 10 June 1999; revised and accepted 30 September 1999
mote relapse following treatment, and increase the risk
for
superimposed
major
depression
(double
depression).
The broad purpose of the present review is to elucidate several behavioral, neuroendocrine and immune/
cytokine characteristics of dysthymia. To this end, we
provide an overview of the characteristics of dysthymia, including a description of the clinical and epidemiological aspects of the illness, comorbid features of
dysthymia, as well as a review of the data suggesting
a role for genetic factors. Given that dysthymia, or at
least some subtypes of the illness, may involve biological underpinnings, a brief review is provided regarding
the neuroendocrine, neurochemical and cytokine correlates of dysthymia, and an overview is provided concerning the efficacy of pharmacotherapy in the treatment of this disorder.
It is proposed that dysthymia may be related to subtle effects of stressors and inadequate coping styles,
and may be exacerbated by the presence of ongoing
psychosocial impairments. A highly provisional model
is proposed concerning the etiological processes subserving dysthymia, including various facets of the HPA
axis (eg, phenotypic variations of corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) and arginine vasopressin (AVP),
down-regulation of adrenal functioning) and forebrain
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
serotonergic mechanisms. However, given the paucity
of data concerning the effects of various challenges (eg,
dexamethasone, ACTH, CRH, TSH, stressors, as well as
challenges in the presence or absence of metyrapone)
on HPA hormones among dysthymic individuals, the
conclusions that can be derived are tentative and must
necessarily await further data.
Dysthymia: clinical and epidemiological features
Dysthymia, literally meaning ‘being of bad mood’ or
‘ill-humor’ is an illness characterized by a number of
affective, neurovegetative and cognitive symptoms.
The diagnosis of dysthymic disorder was introduced
in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III) to characterize
chronic depression of 2 or more years and to
encompass disorders which had previously been considered characterologically based, including neurotic
depression, chronic minor depression, and characterological depression.5 While the severity of dysthymia is
usually less profound than that of acute major depressive disorder, symptoms may fluctuate in intensity. Furthermore, several subtypes of dysthymia have been
proposed based on specific symptoms, family history,
and age of onset; subaffective dysthymia is thought to
be of biological origin, while character spectrum dysthymia is more personality-based.6,7 Currently, DSMIV stipulates that a diagnosis of dysthymia includes
depressed mood, coupled with two or more of the following: poor appetite or overeating, insomnia or hypersomnia, low energy/fatigue, low self-esteem, poor concentration or difficulty making decisions, and feelings
of hopelessness.5 Dysthymia also frequently has an
early and insidious onset, and is associated with pathology of character, albeit these may not play an etiological role. There has been some debate as to whether
these symptoms are, in fact, most characteristic of dysthymia, and based on field trials, the DSM-IV
(Appendix B) offers an alternative set of criteria. These
include: (a) low self-esteem, self confidence, or feelings
of inadequacy; (b) feelings of pessimism, despair, and
hopelessness; (c) anhedonia (generalized loss of interest or pleasure); (d) social withdrawal; (e) chronic
fatigue or tiredness; (f) feelings of guilt, or brooding
about the past; (g) irritability or excessive anger; (h)
reduced activity, effectiveness, or productivity; and (i)
difficulty in thinking, as reflected by poor concentration, memory or decisiveness.
Like the DSM-IV, the ICD-10 defines dysthymia as a
chronic depression which fails to meet the severity and
duration criteria for recurrent depressive disorder.
However, depressive illness, of at least mild duration,
may have occurred previously.8 According to the ICD10, dysthymia often begins early in adult life, and
when late onset dysthymia occurs it is often secondary
to a severe major depressive disorder or in response to
environmental stressors (eg, bereavement). Moreover,
dysthymia is thought to include persistent anxietydepression, depressive neurosis, depressive person-
ality disorder, as well as neurotic depression of more
than 2 years duration.7
The characteristics of dysthymia in many ways overlap with those of major depression, although in dysthymia, symptoms outnumber signs (ie, objective characteristics such as vegetative symptoms and psychomotor
changes are typically absent).9 Relative to major
depressive disorder, symptoms occur at a lower frequency in dysthymic patients, but are qualitatively
similar. Moreover, relative to neurovegetative and psychomotor features, social-motivational impairments
tend to be more characteristic of dysthymia.3,10 In many
dysthymic patients an intermittent emergence of major
depression may occur (double depression), with the
dysthymic state usually recurring upon remission of
the major depressive episode.9 Likewise, dysthymia
may emerge as a residual syndrome of acute
depression;6,11 however, the diagnosis of dysthymia
would not be applied if the initial episode of chronic
depressive symptoms met the criteria for and was sufficiently severe to be diagnosed as a major depressive
episode.
Dysthymia is more common among women than
among men (2:1), has a 1-year prevalence rate (for the
United States) estimated as high as 5.4%,12 and the
highest life-time prevalence of the affective disorders.13
However, only a relatively small proportion of dysthymic individuals seek treatment for their illness,
likely owing to the mild nature of the symptoms and
their insidious onset, coupled with the individual’s
lack of insight. Indeed, the illness often appears in
childhood and adolescence,14,15 and as a result of the
long-term, low-grade nature of the illness, dysthymia
might not be perceived as differing from the individual’s norm. It is of interest to note that in a study of a
fairly large number of dysthymic patients, only 41%
had received any form of pharmacotherapy, and just
56% had received psychotherapy, attesting to the
under-treatment of this disorder.16
Although it had been suggested that pharmacotherapy may be less effective in the treatment of dysthymia relative to major depressive disorder, recent
evidence has indicated that such intervention may, in
fact, be a treatment of choice for dysthymia. However,
the treatment response may vary with the specific subtype of the illness.4,17 Indeed, it appeared that
pharmacotherapy was less effective in ‘pure dysthymia’ than in dysthymia with a history of major
depression or in patients with concurrent major
depression.18 Because dysthymia was initially thought
to be more of a characterological disturbance than a
biologically-based illness, and because of the oft-noted
superiority of pharmacotherapy in major depression
relative to that observed in dysthymia,17 the illness was
typically treated using different forms of psychotherapy.19–21 As such, the lack of controlled studies
comparing the relative efficacy of psychotherapy and
pharmacotherapy (either alone or in combination) is
surprising. It was recently reported, however, that in
the short-run, pharmacotherapy (using a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor) was more efficacious than
243
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
244
group cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) in alleviating
symptoms of dysthymia,22 although CBT attenuated
several functional aspects of the illness. Using a somewhat more protracted regimen (16 weeks), CBT was
effective in a study involving a small number of dysthymic subjects. While this effect appeared to be
somewhat reduced relative to that associated with
fluoxetine, the effects of the treatments were not significantly different.23
Symptom and illness comorbidity
The presence of comorbid features with dysthymia can
be related to any number of factors. On the one hand,
the comorbidity may simply reflect the nosology of the
syndromes, which have overlapping symptoms.1 On
the other hand, comorbidity may reflect common
underlying mechanisms, or in the case of debilitating
medical conditions, the dysthymic state may represent
a subsyndromal depression resulting from the primary
illness. It is possible, as well, that the development of
dysthymia may be secondary to features such as personality disorders or to excessive anxiety, or conversely, dysthymia may give rise to these features.
Whatever the case, it is of obvious diagnostic and
therapeutic value to discern the presence and progression of comorbid features.1
Major depression is often superimposed on a dysthymic state (double depression), and is associated
with a high rate of illness recurrence.24,25 In their epidemiological study, Weissman et al26 reported that
40% of dysthymic patients exhibited comorbid major
depression, while more recent evidence suggested that
as many as 62% of dysthymic patients met criteria for
current major depression, and 80% for lifetime major
depression.14 Treatment outcome was significantly better in major depressives than among double depressives, as was the recurrence rate over 2 years.25 It has
been suggested that the chronology of dysthymia relative to major depression may influence subsequent
treatment response. For example, it was reported that
patients who had experienced dysthymia subsequent
to their first major depressive episode, showed a
reduced treatment response relative to those patients
in whom onset of dysthymia had preceded their first
major depression.27
As many as 75% of dysthymic patients suffer from
some comorbid psychiatric disorder, of which
depression, anxiety, and substance abuse are the most
common.26 Indeed, approximately one-half (50.7%) of
Shelton et al’s16 sample of 410 dysthymic patients
reported a history of major depression, while 26.3%
had a history of substance abuse, and 68.2% were diagnosed with a comorbid personality disorder. With
respect to comorbid anxiety, it was demonstrated that
while social phobia and panic attacks were significantly more common among unipolar and bipolar
depressives (respectively) than dysthymic patients, the
latter exhibited a higher prevalence of generalized anxiety disorder.28 As such, dysthymia ought to be more
closely scrutinized for comorbid anxiety disorder, as it
Molecular Psychiatry
might be associated with a more severe and enduring
symptom profile, as observed in major depression with
co-existing anxiety disorder,29 and might thus require
adjunctive forms of pharmacotherapy. In fact, it was
recently reported that a coexisting anxiety disorder
may indicate increased risk for persistent depression.30
In addition to comorbid anxiety, dysthymia often coexists with personality disorders.31 As well, more double depressive patients than pure dysthymics met criteria for at least one personality disorder. Conversely,
compared to episodic major depressives, significantly
more dysthymics met criteria for an axis II disorder.
Interestingly, dysthymic patients scored significantly
higher than major depressive patients on all of the 13
dimensions of the Personality Disorder Examination,
with the most common axis II disorders being borderline, avoidant and histrionic.31 Essentially, dysthymic
subjects were found to display personality disorders in
the DSM cluster B (antisocial, narcissistic, borderline
and histrionic)31,32 although high rates of avoidant and
dependent personality disorders were also apparent.31–33
Indeed, it was reported that subaffective and character
spectrum dysthymics could be distinguished from one
another on the basis of DSM cluster C characteristics,
with subaffective subjects exhibiting greater avoidant
personality and dependent personality relative to
character spectrum patients.34 It was further reported
that the occurrence of such personality disorder comorbidity was particularly notable in early-onset dysthymic patients.35,36 Finally, it appeared that depressive personality disorder (DPD) was more closely
aligned with dysthymia than with major depression.37
As in the case of other depressive syndromes,38
substance abuse was found to occur at a fairly high rate
among dysthymic patients. For instance, it has been
reported that subjects in alcohol treatment programs
frequently met the criteria for dysthymic disorder.39
In a sample of subjects with substance-related disorder,
a substantial portion (苲10%) exhibited comorbid
dysthymia.40,41 The dysthymic group also had a surprisingly early age of first use of caffeine (7.3 ± 7.0
years), and the investigators speculated that this may
have constituted an early attempt to self-medicate.40
In considering comorbid features, it may be
important to distinguish between subtypes of dysthymia based on age of illness onset. It had been reported
that early- and late-onset dysthymic patients with
superimposed major depression did not differ with
respect to several clinical, psychosocial and cognitive
indices, nor in terms of family history of depression
and alcoholism.42 Subsequent studies, however, indicated that early-onset dysthymics exhibited greater use
of emotion-focused coping strategies, a higher frequency of personality disorders, and increased lifetime substance abuse disorder than their late-onset
counterparts.36,43–50 Further, a greater number of earlyonset dysthymic patients had a family history of a
mood disorder, and displayed earlier onset and longer
duration of the index major depressive episode.36,50 To
be sure, as indicated by Klein et al,36 the earlier onset
of the index major depressive episode might simply
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
reflect greater opportunity for such an occurrence given
that dysthymia increases the risk of major depression.
Moreover, early onset dysthymia being associated with
poor interpersonal and psychosocial skills, may have
favored the development of personality disorders and
substance abuse. Yet, there is reason to suppose that
biological factors may be more closely aligned with
early onset of the illness, while late onset may be associated with character spectrum disorder.4 In fact, as
will be discussed shortly, there have been several
reports indicating that these subgroups may differ with
respect to several neuroendocrine substrates, as well as
in response to some antidepressant medications.
In addition to the high psychiatric comorbidity, dysthymia has also been associated with various other
medical conditions. Given the similarity between dysthymia and chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS), particularly with respect to lethargy, lassitude, impaired
concentration, and diminished drive, the possibility
exists that a subgroup of chronic fatigue patients represents a variant of dysthymia.51 Indeed, it was
reported that patients identified with fatigue were
between six and 10 times more likely to suffer from
dysthymia than the population at large.52 Similarly,
fibromyalgia syndrome, which overlaps with CFS, has
been associated with major depression and dysthymia.53,54 Interestingly, fibromyalgia syndrome has been
associated with adverse childhood experiences. Moreover, specific personality traits which may be related
to such adverse experiences (eg, unstable self-esteem),
may also be associated with fibromyalgia, just as they
are associated with dysthymia.54 It is unclear, however,
whether dysthymia is associated simply with the
symptom of chronic fatigue, as opposed to the syndrome.51 It is significant, as will be discussed shortly,
that dysthymia is associated with elevated production
of interleukin-1 (IL-1␤),55 a cytokine released by activated macrophages, just as CFS has been associated
with an elevation of this cytokine.56,57
In addition to CFS, like other mood and anxietyrelated disorders, dysthymia has also been associated
with increased co-occurrence of migraine (with aura),
tension, and non-organic headaches.58–60 In this
respect, it was reported that of the mood disorders, the
prevalence of coexisting dysthymia was particularly
notable.60 These data raise the possibility that headache and dysthymia share common underlying processes (eg, serotonergic mechanisms, given that both
are positively influenced by serotonin reuptake
inhibitors) or that environmental triggers, such as stressors, may be associated with both pathologies.
Comorbidity involving CFS and headaches in dysthymia is not unexpected given the potential for common mechanisms or environmental precipitants. More
surprising, however, was the finding that a large number of patients suffering from Parkinson’s disease exhibited concurrent major depressive disorder and/or dysthymia. Of course, such comorbidity might be
predicted given that the stress of the neurodegenerative
disturbance might lead to depressive affect. Interestingly, however, in a recent review of this literature,
depressive features, including introversion and
inflexibility, were reported to serve as premorbid predictors of the onset of Parkinson’s disease or an accelerated cognitive decline.61 These investigators have
gone so far as to suggest that psychological and pharmacological interventions may be appropriate in the
treatment of Parkinsonian depression.
As in the case of neurodegenerative disorders,
patients suffering from traumatic brain injury (TBI)
reportedly suffer from a higher prevalence of mood disorders than the general population (ie, 25–50% major
depression, 15–30% dysthymia, 9% mania).62 Given
the large number of brain regions that may be primarily
or secondarily influenced by head trauma, it is difficult
to identify the specific nuclei or pathways subserving
the depression. However, since brain trauma is associated with focal increases of IL-1,63 as are neurodegenerative disorders, the possibility should be considered
that the behavioral effects observed in TBI and
Parkinsonian patients are associated with heightened
cytokine levels.
245
Biological aspects of dysthymia
Depression has been associated with a variety of neurochemical changes, including deficiencies of norepinephrine (NE),64 serotonin (5-HT)65,66 and dopamine
(DA),67–69 or to variations of DA autoreceptors, 5-HT2
receptors, ␣1-NE or ␤-NE receptors.70–72 Of course,
depression is likely a biochemically heterogeneous disorder, such that the neurochemical underpinnings for
the illness, as well as the symptom profile exhibited,
vary across subjects.66,73 Moreover, given the abnormal
responses to various endocrine challenges (eg, the
dexamethasone suppression test (DST), CRH, as well
as thyroid releasing hormone (TRH) challenges), there
is reason to suppose that hormonal variations contribute to the provocation or expression of depressive
symptoms, and that considerable interindividual variability exists with respect to the contribution of these
endocrine factors.74–77
It has been suggested that stressful events or failure
experiences are associated with depressive illness.78–80
Such an outcome may stem from the stressor experience provoking the formation of attributions, which
give rise to negative expectancies of future performance and may result in the development of cognitive
disturbances, such as helplessness.81,82 Alternatively,
stressor experiences may give rise to neurochemical
alterations that favor depressed mood.83,84
It is difficult, using animals, to adequately model
depressive illness let alone to reflect dysthymia. Nevertheless, it may be productive to examine some of the
neurochemical correlates of stressors. Although
environmental insults initiate a series of neurochemical changes that may be of adaptive significance, when
these neurochemical alterations are insufficient to deal
with environmental demands (or neurochemical adaptation does not occur readily), vulnerability to pathology is increased.83 Indeed, animal studies indicated
that stressors will induce many of the central neuroMolecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
246
transmitter alterations (eg, variations of NE, DA and
5-HT turnover and levels) that have been proposed to
subserve the depressive symptoms in humans.
Specifically, in response to acute stressors the
increased utilization of NE, DA and 5-HT is ordinarily
met by adequate synthesis and hence transmitter levels
remain stable. However, under conditions that favor
amine utilization exceeding synthesis (ie, if the stressor
is sufficiently severe and uncontrollable), amine levels
may decline in several brain regions.83–86 These amine
alterations typically persist for only a few hours,
depending on the stressor severity and several organismic variables (eg, age, species).84,87,88 However,
re-exposure to a mild stressor enhances the utilization
of hypothalamic NE89,90 and mesocortical DA
(sensitization effect),91–93 even if the re-exposure
involves a different stressor.94–96
Since humans typically encounter chronic, intermittent and unpredictable stressors, it may be more relevant to assess the effects of such regimens in animal
studies. In contrast to the amine reductions induced
by acute stressors, transmitter levels equal or exceed
control values following protracted or repeated stressors,93,97–100 owing to increased amine synthesis and/or
moderation of excessive utilization.98,101 Moreover,
chronic insults affect the amine variations engendered
by later stressors. Specifically, in addition to a sensitization with respect to amine utilization (as seen following acute stressors), chronic stressors also induce sensitization of amine synthesis, thereby assuring adequate
transmitter levels upon later stressor encounters.99 In
addition, a chronic stressor may result in the downregulation of ␤-NE receptor activity and the NE-sensitive cAMP response.102 Interestingly, upon application
of a chronic unpredictable stressor the neurochemical
adaptation was slower to develop.83,103 It has been suggested that when inadequate neurochemical coping
mechanisms are generated (eg, in genetically vulnerable animals, and when the stressor occurs unpredictably and involves a series of different insults),
depressive-like characteristics may evolve.104 In fact,
Anisman and Merali105 indicated that a regimen of
mild, unpredictable stressors may be precisely the
antecedent events most closely aligned with dysthymic-like states. It is important to emphasize at this
juncture that while some degree of behavioral and
neurochemical adaptation may occur in response to
chronic stressor experiences, the view has been
expressed that the wear and tear induced by attempts
to adapt to a chronic stressor (allostatic load), when
sufficiently protracted and/or intense, may culminate
in pathological outcomes.106 As will be discussed later,
chronic insults may, in fact, promote persistent neurochemical alterations which favor the development of
depressive characteristics.
While there is considerable evidence supporting a
relationship between major depression and central
neurochemical disturbances,107–110 scant information is
available regarding the biological substrates of dysthymia. However, the frequent abnormal DSTs seen in
major depressive patients were absent in dysthymia,2,4
Molecular Psychiatry
and the latter might actually be associated with hypocortisol responding.111 Indeed, it has been reported that
the elevated salivary cortisol associated with exercise
in pre-adolescent children was not apparent among
dysthymic children of the same age.112 Paralleling
these alterations of pituitary-adrenal activity, differences appeared between major depression and dysthymia in growth hormone secretion in response to
physiological challenges, as well as TSH blunting in
the TRH stimulation test.113
As part of a study assessing the role of psychosocial
and biological variables in chronic and non-chronic
major depression and dysthymia, a lower rate of DST
non-suppression was observed in dysthymic patients
(52% vs 8.5% non-suppression in major depression vs
dysthymia). However, the rate of DST non-suppression
was higher in the early-onset than in the late-onset dysthymics,113 although, when double depressives were
excluded from the analysis (39 of the 75 dysthymic
patients), there was no difference between early and
late onset groups (9% and 8% respectively). Interestingly, paralleling the DST response, among late-onset
dysthymics, the blunted TSH response to TRH administration was absent, whereas early-onset dysthymics
(who parenthetically reported more traumatic and frustrating childhood backgrounds) had a higher rate of
DST nonsuppression, and more frequently exhibited a
blunted TSH response. In effect, these data suggest that
early-onset dysthymia may represent a biologically distinct subgroup of chronically depressed patients.113
Indeed, it was noted that early-onset dysthymics
responded preferentially to moclobemide relative to
imipramine, while no such distinction was found
among the late-onset dysthymics, suggesting that
monoamine oxidase A might be more imbalanced
among early-onset dysthymics.114
While limited attention has been devoted to the
analysis of monoamine abnormalities in dysthymia,
reduced levels of plasma NE coupled with elevated
platelet and free 5-HT were evident in dysthymia.115
Further, following exercise, changes of epinephrine
levels were relatively modest in dysthymic patients
relative to control subjects. Thus, it was posited that
dysthymia may be associated with altered adrenal
responsivity to environmental challenges, as well as
heightened sympathetic tone as reflected by the elevated free 5-HT levels. Ravindran et al111 observed
reduced platelet MAO activity in primary, early-onset
dysthymics relative to control subjects. Moreover,
MAO activity prior to treatment was lower among nonresponders than among the drug responders. Along the
same line, relative to endogenous depressives, MAO
levels were low among neurotic depressives,116 while
MAO activity correlated positively with clinical state
in the endogenous group. Thus, low MAO activity may
represent a marker for vulnerability to neurotic
depression. Consistent with the potential involvement
of serotonergic mechanisms in dysthymia, prior to
treatment, lower urinary 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid
(5-HIAA) levels were observed among treatment
responders relative to nonresponders, and the reduced
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
levels in responders normalized following treatment.117
There have been few studies that assessed the electrophysiological correlates of dysthymia. However, it
was reported that in anticipation of aversive stimuli,
dysthymics exhibited hyporesponsiveness of skin conductance, and displayed subtle cognitive processing
disturbances (possibly reflecting difficulties in the processing of complex information), as reflected by evoked
potentials in response to task-relevant stimuli. It was
posited that dysthymia may be associated with an
impoverished ability to respond appropriately to external task demands, possibly owing to inappropriate
allocation of processing resources. Furthermore, it was
suggested that owing to their impaired resource allocation strategies, dysthymic subjects may be more generally impaired in their ability to cope with day-today stressors.118,119
Akiskal et al120,121 reported differences in the sleep
architecture between subtypes of dysthymic patients.
While subaffective dysthymics exhibited shortened
REM latencies relative to controls, this was not the case
among character spectrum disorder patients. In
addition, dysthymia was associated with excessive and
abnormal distribution of REM during the early part of
the night121 as observed in major depression.2,122 However, while the major depressive patients displayed
reduced total sleep time, sleep latency, morning wake
time and sleep efficiency, the sleep architecture of dysthymics in terms of stage percentages, and REM sleep
features, were identical to those of major depressives.
In effect, these data are consistent with the notion that
the two disorders are variants of the same illness,123 or
share common underlying mechanisms.
Pharmacological contributions to the analysis of
dysthymia
The most persuasive data favoring a biological substrate for dysthymia originate from studies which
assessed pharmacological agents in the treatment of
dysthymic illness. The early pharmacological studies
in dysthymia revealed that although MAO inhibitors
and tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) had superior
therapeutic efficacy to placebo, their effects were not
as marked as in major depression.4,17,124–130 However,
it appears that reliable and impressive effects of antidepressant medications can be garnered in dysthymia.
This stems from the development of medications, such
as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs),
which have fewer side effects, thus permitting the use
of higher doses. Moreover, it has been suggested that
optimal drug effects would be obtained when administered primarily to patients with subaffective, rather
than character spectrum disorder.4 In fact, studies
which employed rigorous diagnostic criteria,
established the efficacy of tricyclic agents, such as
imipramine and desipramine,131–137 MAOIs,136,138,139
the reversible monoamine oxidase inhibitor, moclobemide,114,137,140–143 SSRIs, such as fluoxetine and sertraline,22,23,111,132,134,144–146 as well as other agents, such
as the 5HT2 antagonist, ritanserin,147,148 the selective
norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor, reboxetine,149 and
the serotonin/norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor
(SNRI), venlafaxine150,151 (Table 1). The use of well tolerated compounds, including moclobemide and sertraline, may be effective in the long-term management of
dysthymia. This is particularly important since discontinuation of antidepressant treatment was found to be
associated with an 89% rate of relapse in a 4-year
maintenance study.18
In addition to the effects of the aforementioned antidepressants, hormonal manipulations have also been
shown to influence dysthymic symptoms. Specifically,
in a crossover-study involving a small number of subjects, it was observed that administration of the adrenal
androgen, dehydroepiandrosterone, alleviated dysthymic symptoms, primarily comprising anhedonia,
loss of motivation and energy, inability to cope, worry,
emotional numbness, and sadness.163 Interestingly,
these effects were obtained after only 3 weeks of treatment. Furthermore, the thyroid hormone, thyroxine,
potentiated the effects of a variety of antidepressant
medications in dysthymic and treatment-resistant
chronic depressive patients.164 Moreover, in a small
study of five patients it was observed that chromium
supplementation enhanced the antidepressant effects
of more traditional therapeutic agents.165
As indicated earlier, we observed in a double-blind
placebo-controlled study, that sertraline was generally
more effective than group CBT in treating the symptoms of dysthymia, as measured by the Hamilton
Depression Scale.22 Of course, these data need to be
considered as highly provisional, since the trial was
short-term (12 weeks), and the CBT consisted of group
rather than individual treatment. It is possible that
individual CBT, or a program designed specifically for
dysthymia may be more conducive to the treatment of
the disorder. Further, given that anhedonia is a characteristic and persistent feature of dysthymia, it may have
been beneficial to employ cognitive techniques which
focused specifically on the inability of patients to
experience or perceive positive events. Indeed, using
cognitive behavioral psychotherapy, which focuses on
the helplessness and hopelessness associated with dysthymia, and also teaches adaptive coping skills,
McCullough21 reported that nine of 10 dysthymic
patients were still in remission after a 2-year period.
Given the high rate of depressive relapse/recurrence
ordinarily observed following cessation of treatment,
it will be interesting to establish whether combination
therapy minimizes recurrence of illness relative to that
seen among patients who had received only pharmacotherapy. This is particularly the case since group CBT
enhanced the effects of sertraline with respect to some
functional behaviors (eg, cognitive coping styles, and
several indices of quality of life) which, in turn, may
have important implications with respect to illness
recurrence.22
Owing to dysthymia’s fluctuating and chronic nature, several studies evaluated the efficacy of antidepressants in the maintenance treatment of the illness.
247
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
248
Table 1
Pharmacological studies of DSM-III/DSM-III-R/DSM-IV diagnosed dysthymia
Reference
Medication
Outcome
Placebo-controlled
Stewart et al (1985)130
Harrison et al (1986)152
Kocsis et al (1988)133
Tyrer et al (1988)153
Stewart et al (1989)154,a
Desipramine
Phenelzine
Imipramine
Doxepin
Imipramine, phenelzine
Costa-e-Silva et al (1990)155
Bersani et al (1991)148
Botte et al (1992)141,b
Versiani et al (1992)137
Hellerstein et al (1993)144
Stewart et al (1993)136,a
Versiani (1993)143
Bakish et al (1993)147
Kocsis et al (1994)134
Thase et al (1996)145
Vanelle et al (1997)146
Lecrubier et al (1997)156,a
Versiani (1997)114,a
Boyer et al (1999)157
Ravindran et al (1999)22
Amisulpride
Ritanserin
Moclobemide
Moclobemide, imipramine
Fluoxetine
Imipramine, phenelzine
Moclobemide, imipramine
Ritanserin, imipramine
Sertraline, imipramine
Sertraline, imipramine
Fluoxetine
Amisulpride, imipramine
Moclobemide, imipramine
Amisulpride, amineptine
Sertraline
Desipramine = placebo
Phenelzine ⬎ placebo
Imipramine ⬎ placebo
Doxepin ⬎ placebo
Imipramine ⬎ placebo; phenelzine = placebo;
imipramine = phenelzine
Amisulpride ⬎ placebo
Ritanserin ⬎ placebo
Moclobemide ⬎ placebo
Moclobermide = imipramine ⬎ placebo
Fluoxetine ⬎ placebo
Imipramine ⬎ placebo = phenelzine
Moclobemide ⬎ placebo; imipramine ⬎ placebo
Ritanserin = imipramine ⬎ placebo
Sertraline = imipramine ⬎ placebo
Sertraline = imipramine ⬎ placebo
Fluoxetine ⬎ placebo
Amisulpride = imipramine ⬎ placebo
Moclobemide ⬎ placebo; imipramine ⬎ placebo
Amisulpride = amineptine ⬎ placebo
Sertraline ⬎ CBT = placebo
Non-placebo-controlled
Vallejo et al (1987)139
Baumhackl et al (1989)140,b
Ravindran et al (1994)158
Marin et al (1994)135,a
Friedman et al (1995)159,a
Kocsis et al (1996)160,a
Dunner et al (1996)23
Dunner et al (1997)150
Keller et al (1998)132,c
Santagostino et al (1998)161
Smeraldi (1998)162
Ravindran et al (1998)151
Imipramine, phenelzine
Moclobemide, imipramine
Fluoxetine
Desipramine
Desipramine
Desipramine
Fluoxetine
Venlafaxine
Sertraline, imipramine
Alprazolam
Amisulpride, fluoxetine
Venlafaxine
Phenelzine ⬎ imipramine
Moclobemide = imipramine
65% response
70% response
61% response
71% response
Fluoxetine = CBT
⬎70% response
Sertraline = imipramine
73% response
Amisulpride = fluoxetine
73% response
a
Involved dysthymic and double depressive patients for whom data are reported independently (or authors indicate comparable
effects in the two populations).
b
Neurotic depression.
c
Major depressive or double depressive patients were assessed.
Kocsis et al166 indicated that the higher relapse rate in
those dysthymic patients who were randomized to placebo exceeded that of patients who continued on the
maintenance desipramine treatment. Paralleling these
findings, symptom improvement was sustained, and
the rate of relapse reduced, among dysthymic patients
who were maintained on either trazodone or fluoxetine
over a 40-week interval compared to those who discontinued medication.167 Commensurate with the notion
that a long-standing illness, such as dysthymia, might
require prolonged pharmacotherapy, continuous
improvement was observed among dysthymic patients
treated over a 6-month period,146 while Kocsis et al160
found a substantially reduced rate of relapse (11%)
among dysthymic patients maintained on desipramine
over a 2-year period, relative to the 52% relapse rate
in the placebo group. Although these data do not
necessarily speak to the mechanisms subserving dysMolecular Psychiatry
thymia, the results from these controlled clinical trials
are congruent with the proposition that antidepressants are effective for a substantial portion of dysthymic
patients (primarily the subaffective variety), and that
prolonged maintenance treatment may be beneficial.
While most antidepressant trials have focused on the
effects of 5-HT and NE manipulations on the symptoms
of dysthymia and major depression, there have been
several studies implicating a role for dopamine (DA).
Since DA has been thought to subserve reward processes,168 and anhedonia is a characteristic feature of
depression, the view has been taken that reduced DA
activity might contribute to the depressive profile.104,169,170 It will be recalled, however, that in contrast to major depression, anhedonia is not one of the
fundamental symptoms of dysthymia according to the
DSM-IV criteria. Yet, it has been suggested that
anhedonia may be a cardinal feature of dysthymic indi-
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
viduals.4 Unfortunately, there have been few studies
that evaluated the effects of DA manipulations in dysthymia. The administration of the selective D2 and D3
antagonist amisulpride, an agent most often used as an
antipsychotic when administered in high doses (400–
1200 mg), has agonistic DA properties at low doses
(50 mg), likely owing to preferential presynaptic binding. Consistent with the proposition that DA may play
a role in dysthymia, amisulpride was effective in attenuating the symptoms of both dysthymia and major
depressive illness.154–156,169,171,172 In fact, amisulpride
was as effective as imipramine in alleviating depressive symptoms in dysthymia, and both agents were significantly better than placebo in this respect (Table 1).
Genetic factors in dysthymia
Since the prevalence rates of various affective illnesses
differ in families with dysthymic, major depressive and
double depressive probands, it was suggested that dysthymia and major depression are independent disorders.2,47 While dysthymia may represent a trait factor
predicting increased risk for major depression, it may
be important to distinguish between early- and lateonset illness. In fact, while major depression and dysthymia appear to be distinct illnesses, relatives of probands with early-onset depression were at increased
risk for both major depression and dysthymia.173 Likewise, Goodman and Barnhill174 reported the results of
a study comparing the rates of dysthymia in relatives
of probands with either panic disorder, major
depression, or both conditions (a subset of 33 patients
were also dysthymic). Increased rates of dysthymia
were observed in relatives of early-onset major depressives, and among relatives of dysthymic probands, thus
supporting a relationship between early-onset major
depression and dysthymia. Unfortunately, the small
number of subjects tested makes it difficult to discern
whether the risk of dysthymia varies as a function of
early- vs late-onset of the disorder in the dysthymic
proband. Paralleling these findings, rates of major
depression in relatives of early-onset dysthymics compared to relatives of controls, confirmed a familial
association between dysthymia and major depression.
It was also observed that relatives of the dysthymics
had higher rates of chronic depression than relatives
of episodic depressives. Thus, there appears to be support for familial aggregation in dysthymia, as well as
for the validity of dysthymia as a distinct diagnostic
category.175
Donaldson et al176 found higher rates of dysthymia
among relatives of pure dysthymics and of double
depressives, than among relatives of major depressive
probands and normal controls. Furthermore, the rates
of pure dysthymia did not differ between relatives of
pure dysthymics and those of double depressives, nor
did they differ between relatives of major depressive
and normal control probands. Once again, these data
are consistent with the notion that while dysthymia
may be distinct from major depressive disorder, dysthymia and double depression may be more closely
related. The conclusions are clouded, however, by the
finding that there was a higher rate of pure major
depression among the relatives of pure major depressive probands, as well as among the relatives of double
depressives, than among normal controls. These investigators suggested that dysthymia may be associated
with two distinct etiological profiles. That is: (a)
increased vulnerability to depression occurs in all relatives of unipolar depressive illness, irrespective of subtype; and (b) that risk for dysthymia may be particularly notable among relatives of dysthymic patients
and those suffering from double depression.
The high comorbidity of dysthymia with personality
disorders has consistently been noted.177 It has also
been reported that the relatives of dysthymic patients,
regardless of the presence of cluster B personality disorder (antisocial, borderline, histrionic, narcissistic) in
the proband, exhibited increased frequency of dysthymia with and without cluster B personality disorder,
as well as cluster B personality disorder without dysthymia. Thus, these results supported the notion that
dysthymia and cluster B personality disorder share
etiological factors such as genetic or familial factors, or
early home environment.178
The contribution of genetic factors to dysthymia
prompted Akiskal6 to categorize patients, in part, on
the basis of genetic history. It was suggested that subaffective dysthymics frequently had a family history of
depression, whereas character spectrum dysthymics
tended to have a significant family history of
alcoholism/drug abuse, but not of depressive disorder.
Partial support for Akiskal’s classification of subaffective vs character spectrum disorder was obtained from
the finding that there was a higher rate of alcoholism
among the relatives of the character spectrum disorder
dysthymics, while the subaffective dysthymics exhibited higher rates of depressive symptoms, as well as
personality and cognitive features.34 Unlike Akiskal’s
classification, however, these investigators did not
observe differences between groups with respect to
early home environment, family history of mood disorders, gender, or personality disorder.
Few studies have examined the genotypic expression
of factors that might be related to dysthymia. However,
it was demonstrated in a Japanese sample, that patients
diagnosed with depressive disorders exhibited higher
rates of genotypes coding for low activity catechol-omethyltransferase relative to non-depressed controls.179 Although only five dysthymic patients were
represented in the sample, the results obtained were
consistent with those observed in a larger set of major
depressive patients (n = 66) in this study. Of course,
given the small number of subjects tested, these data
must be considered cautiously. Nevertheless, they are
suggestive of disturbances of enzyme activity related to
catecholamine function in dysthymic patients, just as
such effects may occur in major depressive disorder.
While the preceding studies supported a genetic contribution to dysthymia, other studies challenged this
conclusion. For instance, monozygotic and dizygotic
twins did not differ in their concordance rates for dys-
249
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
250
thymic illness (7.4% vs 8.7% respectively) as they did
with respect to major depression.180 It was concluded
that the shared or family environment may contribute
more to the etiology of dysthymia than to major
depression. It was argued that severity of depression
and early-onset of the illness may be aligned with a
genetic association, while the milder depressive illness
spectrum (including dysthymia) may be more closely
tied to environmental factors. As the dysthymic
patients were not subdivided into character spectrum
vs subaffective, it is unclear from these data whether
the conclusion applies to both subtypes equally.
Psychosocial factors and stressors in major
depressive disorder and dysthymia
There is considerable evidence supporting the contention that a relationship exists between stressful events,
coping deficits, and the development or exacerbation of
major depressive disorder. These data have frequently
been reviewed and thus will not be reiterated
here.78,81,181 In view of the shared attributes between
dysthymia and major depression, it is somewhat surprising how little information is available concerning
the contribution of stress to the provocation of dysthymic disorder. However, inasmuch as dysthymia is
a chronic illness, it may be difficult to identify specific
life events that precipitated illness onset. Nevertheless,
it would not be unreasonable to propose that some
adverse life events, particularly the inability to cope
with day-to-day annoyances, or alternatively chronic
stressor experiences, may precipitate or aggravate the
illness.
Not unexpectedly, some stressors are conducive to
the provocation of depressive symptoms (eg, social
loss),78,182 whereas others are more closely aligned with
anxiety disorders (eg, threats or impending stress).183
Although major life events often precede depression,
the antecedents of affective illness may involve a series
of minor stressors (day-to-day hassles). Indeed, these
stressors may have particularly profound effects when
applied onto a backdrop of major stressors.182,184 Of
course, the potency of a stressor in promoting
depression may be related to characteristics of the
individual, coupled with the nature of the stressor
encountered.78,185,186 Finally, as alluded to earlier,
acute stressors may have very different implications
than chronic predictable or chronic intermittent stressors.182 Chronic intermittent stressors may not lend
themselves to neurochemical adaptation and may be
most likely to result in behavioral disturbances.83,104
It has been reported that dysthymic patients, like
major depressives, perceived a markedly greater frequency of day-to-day annoyances than did nondepressed subjects. In contrast, stressful life events
were only marginally greater in the depressive groups.
The elevated stress perception was accompanied by
coping styles wherein emotion-based strategies predominated (eg, blame, emotional expression, emotional
containment, avoidance/denial). With successful pharmacotherapy, the elevated stress perceptions were
Molecular Psychiatry
reduced, and the reliance on maladaptive coping was
attenuated.187,188 It is unclear whether these effects
reflect changes in appraisal processes and consequently altered coping styles, or simply a proportionate
decline in the reliance upon emotion-focused coping.
Of course, these data do not suggest that altered stress
perception and coping were etiological factors in dysthymia, as they may simply have been correlates of
the illness.
Interestingly, in a prospective study of dysthymic
patients over a 9-month period, McCullough et al189
identified several features that distinguished the remitters from the nonremitters. Specifically, the nonremitters tended to exhibit a stable depressive attributional
style and tended to employ inappropriate coping strategies. Additionally, the nonremitters did not deal with
their major stresses effectively, and tended to use emotion-focused coping (eg, wishing away their problems,
blame) and social support seeking rather than a problem-focused style. These individuals also displayed an
interpersonal style characterized by shyness and lack
of sociability, coupled with submissiveness and compliance. It was suggested that this profile represents a
maladaptive behavioral pattern that predisposes the
individual to the persistent nature of dysthymia.
While there is reason to suppose that stressful events
contribute to the provocation of dysthymia, prospective studies have not been conducted to assess the contribution of life stressors to this illness. Several studies,
however, reported that stressful life events preceded
the onset of both neurotic and non-neurotic
depression.190,191 Given the frequent early onset of dysthymia, coupled with its chronic nature, the paucity of
prospective studies concerning the stressful antecedents of the illness is not surprising. However, it was
reported that among adults who acted as care-givers for
a spouse with a progressive dementia (care-giving itself
is a profound stressor), the incidence of major
depression and dysthymia greatly exceeded that seen
in a matched control sample.192 Thus, dysthymia, like
major depression, may be provoked by chronic uncontrollable stressor conditions. It remains to be established whether a chronic regimen of minor stressors
would likewise be associated with dysthymic symptoms.
In addition to altered stress perception and coping
styles, perceived daily positive or uplifting events were
reduced in dysthymic individuals relative to controls.187,188 This effect, however, was not limited to
day-to-day uplift perception, but was also evident with
respect to several quality of life indices, including
social interaction, health perception, cognitive functioning, alertness, energy/vitality, and life satisfaction.
Among responders to treatment (either pharmacotherapy or group CBT), each of these quality of life
indices increased significantly.22,159,193–195 Studies that
focused on social and interpersonal impairments likewise indicated marked deficits among dysthymic
patients.22,159,193,196–199 These reports indicated that
social impairment correlated positively with increasing
severity and chronicity of the illness, and worsened
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
with onset of double depression. In addition to alleviating the depressive symptoms, antidepressants attenuated the social impairment characteristic of dysthymic
patients. Markowitz et al200 demonstrated that acute
treatment (10 weeks) with desipramine significantly
improved interpersonal functioning in dysthymics, as
measured by the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems
(IIP). While improvement continued over a 16-week
maintenance phase, this was not significant, and
although the social impairment scores approached normative values, they did not attain this level. Further to
this point, life satisfaction and psychosocial functioning improved among dysthymics who responded to
antidepressant treatment. Thus, it was posited that the
reduced capacity to enjoy leisure time may be a state
marker of chronic depression.131,159,201
It is interesting that while significant improvement
in quality of life was observed among dysthymic
patients treated with either imipramine or sertraline
over 12 weeks,202 self-reported social functioning
showed greater improvement than did participation in
leisure activity. It was suggested that the delayed
alleviation of certain aspects of psychosocial functioning may have been due to the relatively short duration of pharmacotherapy relative to the patients’ often
life-long impairment. Further to this same point, it
should also be considered that the well entrenched
anhedonia and impaired psychosocial functioning
among dysthymics may contribute to the unremitting
nature of the illness. In this respect, it also appears that
the response to desipramine was inferior among dysthymic patients characterized by the most pronounced
overall social impairment and family dysfunction.131
Despite the fact that antidepressant treatment diminished the functional impairments characteristic of dysthymic patients, it is of particular interest that the
stress profile, although improved, continued to show
significant residuum. For instance, while antidepressant medication was associated with reduced daily hassle perception, increased uplift perception, diminished
reliance on emotion-focused coping styles, and
reduced feelings of loneliness, none of these behavioral
parameters had returned to levels of nondepressed subjects.188 Similarly, Klein et al203 reported that adolescents with a history of dysthymia displayed persistent perception of increased daily hassles, difficulties
in psychosocial functioning, and residual symptoms of
depression. Finally, although improvement of psychosocial functioning was apparent among chronic
depressive patients within 4 weeks of sertraline or
imipramine treatment, the level of functioning typically did not reach that of a community control population.204 However, among those patients who displayed full remission, psychosocial functioning was
comparable to that of a community sample. Thus, it
appeared that in some chronically depressed patients,
antidepressant medication effectively alleviated the
functional psychosocial disturbances. Indeed, it was
recently observed that among patients who received
either sertraline, CBT, or a combination of the two
treatments, only a modest overall elevation of uplift
perception and quality of life was observed. However,
when these scores were assessed among treatment
responders, it was clear that uplift perception and
quality of life scores approached or reached those of
nondysthymic controls.22
As indicated earlier, it has been suggested that
depressive illness, and particularly dysthymia, may be
a life-long disorder. Moreover, the high rate of recurrence of major depressive disorder may stem from
undertreatment of the illness, as reflected by the persistence of residual symptoms.20,205–207 It is similarly
possible that those treatments which permit residual
dysthymic features to persist (eg, inadequate antidepressant dosage or insufficient duration of treatment)
may also favor recurrence of this illness. It is certainly
conceivable that by virtue of its effects on the secondary features of dysthymia, CBT may act to limit illness
recurrence. Yet, as will be discussed later, the neurochemical underpinnings of dysthymia may persist
despite a positive treatment response, thus necessitating sustained maintenance treatment.
One further issue warrants some consideration. It has
been observed that among dysthymic patients treated
with sertraline, the reduction of clinician-rated
depression scores (Hamilton Depression, Montgomery–
Asberg, and Cornell Dysthymia Rating Scales) was
greater than among patients treated with group CBT.
The scores in the latter group were, in fact, no different
from those of placebo-treated subjects. However, like
drug-treated patients who showed a positive treatment
response, the CBT patients who showed significant
clinical improvement also reported a marked increase
of quality of life. In contrast, no such increase was
apparent in placebo responders (ie, their quality of life
was comparable to treatment nonresponders). Thus,
despite the comparable clinical depression scores, the
functional effects of drug treatment, CBT, and placebo
were readily distinguishable. Given that quality of life
changes may be a fundamental characteristic in identifying the efficacy of treatment response,22 such a functional measure may also be useful in distinguishing
genuine drug responders from drug-treated patients
actually exhibiting a placebo-like response, and thus
may prove to be a valuable tool in predicting relapse.
251
Cytokines and depressive illness
Increasing evidence has indicated that depressive illness is accompanied by immune dysregulation. While
it had typically been assumed that depression promoted immunosuppression, it has been argued that the
compromised immunity may actually be secondary to
an initial immune activation. Furthermore, this notion
has led to the possibility that products of an activated
immune system may come to promote central neurochemical changes, hence provoking depressive symptoms.208 Commensurate with this view, depressed
patients exhibited signs of immune activation, including increased plasma concentrations of complement
proteins, C3 and C4, and immunoglobulin (Ig) M, as
well as positive acute phase proteins, haptoglobin, ␣1Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
252
antitrypsin, ␣1 and ␣2 macroglobulin, coupled with
reduced levels of negative acute phase proteins. Also,
depression was accompanied by an increased number
of activated T cells (CD25+ and HLA-DR+), secretion of
neopterin, prostaglandin E2 and thromboxane. Furthermore, it appears that depression may be associated
with variations of either circulating cytokine levels (ie,
cell signalling factors released from activated
macrophages), or cytokine production from mitogenstimulated lymphocytes, including interleukin-2 (IL-2),
soluble IL-2 receptors (sIL-2R), IL-1␤, IL-1 receptor
antagonist (IL-1Ra), IL-6, soluble IL-6 receptor (sIL-6R),
and ␥-interferon (IFN).209–216 While there have been
reports that the elevated levels of IL-1␤, IL-6 and ␣1acid glycoprotein normalized with antidepressant
medication,217 the unregulated production of sIL-2R,
IL-6 and sIL-6R was not attenuated with antidepressant
agents, leading to the suggestion that the latter factors
may be trait markers of the illness.208
Although severity of depressive illness is likely fundamental in determining cytokine levels,208 the possibility cannot be ignored that chronic depression (or
chronic stress) may induce cytokine changes to a
greater extent than those observed following acute episodes (as in the case of major depression). Consistent
with reports in melancholic patients,208 levels of
mitogen-stimulated IL-1␤ production were enhanced
in dysthymia.55 However, in this particular study, IL1␤ production was stimulated by the T cell mitogen,
phytohemagglutinin (PHA), and thus may have
reflected primarily T cell rather than macrophage-produced IL-1␤. While not excluding the possibility that
illness severity may be a pertinent feature in promoting
the enhanced IL-1␤ production, it seems likely that illness chronicity or age of onset may also be important
in this respect. Indeed, it was observed that age of onset
was inversely related to IL-1␤ production, while duration of illness was directly related to production of
this cytokine. Additionally, the altered IL-1␤ production was evident irrespective of whether a typical
or atypical (reversed neurovegetative) profile was evident. Thus, it is unlikely that the altered cytokine production was related to the neurovegetative alterations
that may appear in depression. It might be noted, as
well, that IL-1␤ production was not markedly reduced
with the alleviation of dysthymic symptoms following
12 weeks of SSRI treatment.55,218 However, given that
dysthymia is a long-standing illness, it is certainly
possible that more prolonged treatment would have
been necessary to realize changes of IL-1␤, just as relatively protracted treatment was previously reported to
promote variations of circulating natural killer cells.219
In contrast to the elevated IL-1␤ in supernatants of
mitogen-stimulated lymphocytes, we observed that circulating serum IL-1␤, presumably derived from macrophages and T cells, was not increased in either typical
major depressive or in dysthymic patients. However,
among atypical major depressive patients, circulating
IL-1␤ levels were greatly increased, and normalized
with treatment response.220 It could be assumed that
the elevated levels of serum IL-1␤ in atypical
Molecular Psychiatry
depression were secondary to the neurovegetative features of this depressive subtype. However, animal studies have shown that IL-1␤ provokes some symptoms
characteristic of atypical depression (including,
increased sleep and fatigue),99 and thus it is just as
likely that elevated circulating IL-1␤ contributes to the
neurovegetative features of this depressive subtype.
The finding that illnesses involving atypical depressive
features (eg, chronic fatigue syndrome) may be associated with HPA disturbances (eg, reduced plasma cortisol, increased ACTH, and reduced ACTH release following oCRH challenge),221,222 coupled with the fact
that IL-1␤ is a potent stimulator of CRH release,223,224
raises the possibility that elevated circulating IL-1␤
levels contribute to the pathophysiology of atypical
depressive symptoms.
In support of the immune activation view of depressive illness, Maes225 indicated that in addition to the
altered cytokine, acute phase protein, and hormonal
changes ordinarily elicited as part of the inflammatory
response, elevations of IL-1␤ may be evident in
depression associated with a variety of medical illnesses. These include not only infectious diseases
(influenza, herpes virus, HIV, Borna virus), but also
numerous noninfectious illnesses, such as neurodegenerative disorders, autoimmune disorders and brain
injury.61,225 Thus, it was suggested that the cytokine
activation associated with these illnesses and/or injuries may have provoked variations of HPA activity, as
well as central neurochemical alterations, which may
then have favored the development of depression. In
effect, these comorbid conditions may have contributed to the illness owing to the cascade of cytokine,
hormone and transmitter alterations engendered. It
will be recalled that dysthymia likewise is associated
with a large number of comorbid conditions, such as
neurodegenerative disorders,1,61 traumatic brain
injury,62 and illnesses potentially involving viral
components, such as fibromyalgia syndrome and
chronic fatigue syndrome.53,54 As such, the possibility
ought to be considered that such coexisting illnesses
are not simply correlates of the mood disorder, but may
actually act to either precipitate or aggravate dysthymia.
The data presently available concerning cytokine
changes in depressive illness (ie, studies showing elevations of the cytokines in severe major depression) are
largely correlational. Thus, it is unclear whether the
cytokine alterations seen in affective disorders are secondary to the illness (or the stress associated with the
illness), or play an etiological role in the provocation of
the disorder. Yet, administration of high doses of IL-2,
IFN-␣ and tumor necrosis factor-␣ (TNF-␣) in humans
undergoing immunotherapy have been shown to
induce
neuropsychiatric
symptoms,
including
depression, and these effects were related to the cytokine treatment rather than to the primary illness.226–229
Of course, the doses administered in these studies were
in the pathophysiological range, and thus their relevance to depression per se must be interpreted cautiously. However, as indicated by Meyers,230 even
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
when administered at relatively low doses, cytokines
such as IFN-␣ may elicit depressive-like symptoms.
A provisional model of chronic depressive illness
It is clear from the preceding sections that limited data
are available concerning the mechanisms underlying
dysthymia. Because of dysthymia’s chronic, low-grade
nature, animal models of the illness have yet to be
developed. Nevertheless, data derived from animal
studies offer some clues as to the potential persistent
effects of stressor experiences that may be relevant to
dysthymia. In particular, it seems that in addition to
any immediate consequences, stressors may also proactively influence the neurochemical response to subsequently encountered aversive stimuli (sensitization),
hence favoring long-term behavioral repercussions.
Post and Weiss231 indicated that although the variations of certain peptides persist for relatively brief
periods following a single stressor session, with
repeated challenges the release of some peptides will
be more readily induced and will be more persistent
(eg, CRH, and to a greater extent TRH). It was suggested
that depressive illness may initially stem from the neuroendocrine alterations provoked by a stressor. However, with each subsequent stressor experience, or with
each episode of depression, the sensitization becomes
more pronounced, such that progressively less intense
psychosocial stressors are required to provoke the
onset of a depressive episode. Ultimately, episodes of
depression may occur in the absence of obvious stress
triggers. In fact, it was reported that unlike the initial
episode, recurrence was less likely to be preceded by
antecedent stressors196,232 and even occurred spontaneously.233,234 Of course, it is often difficult to identify significant or meaningful stressors that may be pertinent to a given individual, thus conclusions
concerning the presence or absence of stressful precipitants of depression may be difficult to validate. It is
also conceivable that in addition to stressors of a
psychological nature, a physiological stressor, such as
a virus, may be interpreted by the CNS in the same way
as a psychosocial stressor, hence triggering the cascade
of events resulting in neuroendocrine, cytokine, and
mood alterations.235
Although it is often thought that HPA disturbances
are likely only a reflection of depression, it has been
proposed that alterations of HPA activity could be the
primary abnormality in depression, rather than simply
an illness response.236,237 Moreover, it has been suggested that among biologically predisposed individuals, chronic stressors may come to promote sustained
HPA activation which leads to adverse effects.74 As
alluded to earlier, the view has even been offered that
the monoamine variations often associated with
depression may actually stem from endocrine alterations.236,237 In this respect, it was suggested that stressful events promote CRH variations in the central amygdala, which in turn may affect forebrain serotonin
alterations. The former may reflect a basic stress-
response, while the latter, presumably, entails the
appraisal of the stressor situation.238
While the model developed by Post and Weiss231 was
meant to accommodate recurrent depression, it may
also be applicable to the analysis of dysthymia. In this
respect, however, it is important to underscore that
sensitization effects are not limited to the neuroendocrine factors discussed by Post, nor is such a sensitization limited to antecedent stressors. In fact, it has been
demonstrated that several neurochemical alterations
associated with stressors, psychostimulant use
(amphetamine and cocaine), and electrical stimulation
of the amygdala or the piriform cortex, may permanently enhance the neuronal response to subsequent
manipulations (sensitization).239 As will be seen
shortly, this applies to the effects of cytokine treatments as well.
In modelling dysthymic disorders, several features of
this illness need to be considered, and it is important
to distinguish these from other types of depression.
Depressive disorders may be associated with profound
interindividual differences in the symptoms subserving the illness, the response to pharmacotherapy,
as well as the neuroendocrine correlates of the disorder. Further, subtypes of depression may differ in
terms of their response to specific pharmacological
treatments, and with respect to their neuroendocrine
factors. For instance, major depression is characterized
by HPA alterations, including elevated plasma ACTH
and cortisol levels, nonsuppression of cortisol release
following dexamethasone challenge, and a blunted
ACTH response to CRH challenge.222,240 While limited
data are available, it appears that in illnesses involving
atypical features (eg, bulimia, seasonal affective disorder, and chronic fatigue syndrome) the elevated
ACTH levels are accompanied by reduced cortisol, a
blunted ACTH response to CRH challenge,221,222,240,241
and absence of the CRH hypersecretion characteristic
of typical depression.240 Among dysthymic patients the
profile is different yet again, and as indicated earlier,
there is reason to believe that cortisol levels may actually be reduced,4 although a contradictory finding has
been reported with respect to plasma cortisol and CRH
concentrations.242 Moreover, in response to a stressor
challenge, the cortisol response may be minimal in
dysthymic patients relative to that seen in other types
of depression and in nondepressed subjects.112 It
remains to be established whether subaffective and
character-spectrum dysthymia can be distinguished on
the basis of these parameters. In any event, in providing a model of the mechanisms subserving dysthymia
it needs to be understood why this disorder is not associated with cortisol abnormalities like those seen in
major depressive illness. Furthermore, with respect to
double depression, it would be of obvious benefit to
determine why, following successful treatment,
patients typically return to their dysthymic states
rather than to an euthymic state.
The sensitization model described earlier introduces
an important facet of stressor actions that may be relevant to understanding the relationship between
253
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
254
stressor-induced neurochemical alterations and
chronic depressive illness. Tilders and his associates243–246 indicated that in response to repeated
stressor experiences, or with the passage of time following a stressor or IL-1␤ challenge,247 phenotypic
variations may occur within hypothalamic neurons
that are ordinarily responsive to stressors. In particular,
increased coexpression of CRH and AVP was observed
within CRH containing neurons originating in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and having terminals in the
external zone of the median eminence. As the coreleased peptides act synergistically to promote ACTH
secretion, the chronic stressor regimen increases the
potential for elevated HPA functioning.243,246 It is of
particular significance, as well, that the altered coexpression of CRH and AVP was exceptionally long
lasting, and was even evident as long as 60 days following stressor exposure.
Given the chronic nature of dysthymia, characterized
by increased stress perception and inadequate coping
styles, it is conceivable that this illness would be
accompanied by the neuroendocrine characteristics
ordinarily associated with chronic stressors. For
instance, dysthymia may be associated with increased
CRH and AVP coexpression within the external zone
of the median eminence, as occurs with chronic stressors,246 and this may represent a permanent (or
persistent) characteristic. The fact that dysthymic
patients do not exhibit increased ACTH and cortisol,
however, raises the possibility that the protracted
CRH/AVP may have given rise to the down-regulation
of pituitary and adrenal sensitivity. Clearly, this
suggestion is highly speculative given that experiments
have not been performed to assess, in detail, the
characteristics of HPA functioning in dysthymic
patients. Since an abnormal DST response has typically not been noted in dysthymia, it was taken for
granted that this illness is not accompanied by any
HPA disturbances. Yet, it may be the case that dysthymia is associated with adrenal hypofunctioning (as
observed in atypical depression), rather than the hyperfunctioning seen in major depressive disorder.4 There
are few neuroendocrine studies, however, that
observed distinctive differences between dysthymic
and non-depressed subjects. In part, this may stem
from the subtle pathophysiological disturbances in
dysthymia, and the use of neuroendocrine analyses
that tap circulating hormonal levels rather than the
dynamic, temporal patterns of hormone release.1 Also,
assessment of HPA functioning in dysthymia requires
evaluation of the effects of various challenges (eg,
ACTH, CRH, AVP, as well as serotonergic acting
agents) in order to identify the nature of any dysregulation that may exist.107,248 Further, it is essential to
evaluate these processes independently in the character-spectrum and subaffective variants of the illness.
Inasmuch as dysthymia is a chronic illness, often of
mild-moderate severity and typically without clear
precipitating events, it is unlikely that it is related to
a single strong stressor experience. It is more reasonable to speculate that dysthymia reflects the actions of
Molecular Psychiatry
more sustained, variable, and probably less intense
stressors, coupled with the use of inadequate or inappropriate methods of coping, culminating in the
phenotypic CRH/AVP coexpression. Thus, it would
not be altogether surprising to find that double
depression may be related to the superimposition of a
further stressor on the backdrop of dysthymia, which
would then promote the increased release of these peptides (and the ensuing neurochemical cascade). Given
this scenario, it might further be expected that after
treatment of double depression, the CRH/AVP coexpression would persist and hence the dysthymic profile would be maintained. In these individuals the risk
for further major depressive episodes would, of course,
be heightened. In effect, we are suggesting that dysthymia may reflect a chronic state of altered endocrine and
central neurotransmitter functioning which may be
related to sustained stressor experiences together with
inadequate coping. Indeed, even with the remission of
symptoms, the persistent neuropeptide disturbances
would increase the likelihood of symptom recurrence.249 Obviously, it would be of particular interest to
establish whether the alleviation of double depressive
symptoms would be accompanied by abnormal
responses to CRH challenge, and whether such an
effect differed from that seen following recovery from
a major depressive episode. These factors, coupled
with the long standing nature of the disorder, and the
comorbid features discussed earlier, may necessitate a
more sustained regimen of pharmacotherapy. Moreover, given the personality disturbances and the maladaptive cognitive coping strategies characteristic of dysthymia, in the absence of cognitive therapy or
psychotherapy (as adjunctive or maintenance
treatment) susceptibility to recurrence of illness may
be increased. Indeed, we have shown previously that
in spite of clinical improvement following pharmacotherapy, functional disturbances (as reflected by
compromised quality of life, anhedonia) may persist in
dysthymia. It was hypothesized that these residual features may actually be predictive of illness recurrence
following cessation of pharmacotherapy.22
In relating stressful events to the mechanisms underlying dysthymia, we have defined stressors in a fairly
broad way. As discussed earlier, it has been posited
that, among other things, the immune system acts like
a sensory organ informing the brain of antigenic challenge.250,251 Furthermore, given the nature of the neurochemical changes elicited by antigens and cytokines, it
was suggested that immune activation may be interpreted by the CNS as a stressor.44,223,235,251 To be sure,
the effects of systemic stressors (eg, those associated
with viral insults, bacterial endotoxins, cytokines) are
not entirely congruous with those elicited by processive stressors (ie, those involving higher order sensory processing).252 Nevertheless, cytokines may be
part of a regulatory loop that, by virtue of their effects
on CNS functioning, might influence behavioral outputs and may even contribute to the symptoms of
behavioral pathologies, including mood and anxietyrelated disorders.44,235,253 It is certainly the case that
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
both processive and systemic stressors effectively
increase HPA activity. However, while processive
stressors do so via limbic circuits, the HPA alterations
elicited by systemic stressors may result from limbicindependent processes.245 Yet, it ought to be underscored that systemic stressors, including IL-1␤, IL-2
and TNF␣ have all been shown to influence central
monoamine activity at both hypothalamic and extrahypothalamic sites, including hippocampal 5-HT
activity, as well as that of NE and DA in hypothalamus,
locus coeruleus and mesolimbic regions.235,254–256
Thus, the possibility exists that cytokine elevations, by
virtue of these monoamine effects, may come to promote or exacerbate depressive disorders, quite apart
from any actions involving the HPA axis. It remains
to be determined whether the IL-1␤ variations seen in
dysthymia are secondary to the illness or, in fact, play
an etiological role. Yet, as indicated earlier, this cytokine provokes behavioral changes, some of which are
reminiscent of the characteristics of atypical
depression, including increased sleep, lethargy and
reduced locomotor activity,257 and may provoke anxiety.235 Given that the production of IL-1␤ in mitogenstimulated lymphocytes is greater among dysthymic
than among major depressive patients, particularly in
those reporting early onset of the illness,55 the possibility exists that dysthymia may be associated with
excessive cytokine reactivity. Increased IL-1␤ activation would then stimulate CRH functioning, and the
ensuing neuroendocrine cascade. In effect, it may be
that in dysthymic patients, stressors in the form of viral
or bacterial challenges, may be particularly potent in
provoking major depressive symptoms and hence promoting double depression.
Concluding remarks
The paucity of data from human studies, coupled with
the lack of a suitable animal model for dysthymia, have
limited the conclusions that can be drawn concerning
the etiology of this disorder. Nevertheless, the available
data have made it clear that elucidation of the mechanisms underlying dysthymia, and the development of
adequate treatment strategies, will require that several
fundamental features be included in experimental
analyses. Foremost in this respect is the need to subtype subjects according to definite criteria. In particular, it will be of obvious advantage to distinguish
between pure dysthymia, double depression, and other
forms of chronic depression. Additionally, patients
need to be characterized into homogeneous subgroups
(eg, early- vs late-onset; subaffective vs characterspectrum), and the symptom profile of the dysthymic
patients ought to be considered (vis-à-vis the presence
of typical or atypical neurovegetative symptoms).
Although genetic factors likely contribute to the
expression of dysthymia, there is also reason to believe
that experiential factors play a cogent role in this
respect. There is no information, however, as to
whether early-life experiences contribute to the biological (subaffective) type of dysthymia. It is interest-
ing, however, that studies in rodents have indicated
that early life maternal deprivation may give rise to a
cascade of neurochemical alterations much like those
purported to occur in dysthymia. These include
increased CRH mRNA expression in the amygdala and
CRH concentrations in the median eminence, as well
as increases of CRH receptors in the prefrontal cortex,
amygdala, hypothalamus and cerebellum. As adults,
rats that had undergone maternal deprivation display
increased stress-elicited arousal and elevated HPA
functioning.258,259 The possibility ought to be explored
that in humans, early-life stress or ‘neglect’ may give
rise to these neurochemical disturbances, hence
increasing vulnerability to later stressor-induced neuroendocrine and neurotransmitter alterations and ultimately the dysthymic profile. Of course, the failure to
establish appropriate coping strategies (and this
includes affiliation, attachment and support systems,
particularly with parents) may augment stressor
effects, thereby encouraging the development of dysthymia.
Finally, the identification of subtypes of dysthymia
may be an important feature in determining the optimal treatment strategy employed. From the outset, it
must be acknowledged that because dysthymia is a
chronic condition, relatively sustained treatment may
be required to alleviate the symptoms.260 Thus, drugs
that are relatively well tolerated will be most efficacious in treatment, particularly when these agents do
not elicit sexual dysfunction or somatic complaints,
symptoms which themselves typically are not characteristic of dysthymia.18 Further, it is likely, as indicated
earlier, that the effectiveness of various treatment strategies may be related to factors such as age of onset,
and the presence of characterological features. In this
respect, determining whether a given episode is associated with neuroendocrine disturbances (eg, reduced
cortisol secretion secondary to excessive CRH
activation) may offer insights into whether pharmacotherapy (and the type of agents used) would be most
efficacious in treating the disorder. Of course, family
history of psychiatric illness and the effectiveness of
specific pharmacotherapy therein, would be of obvious
value in planning a treatment strategy. Finally, it ought
to be underscored that the effectiveness of cognitive
therapy has not been extensively evaluated in dysthymic patients. Nevertheless, it would appear that this
therapeutic modality may be useful in treating some
dysthymic patients. It remains to be established what
characteristics of the illness might be predictive of
those patients who would benefit most from this form
of therapy. In this respect, it may be useful to consider
functional parameters related to quality of life (eg, cognitive disturbances, social interaction, life satisfaction),
as opposed to relying simply on clinical indices of
depression. Given the particularly high rate of relapse
in dysthymia upon cessation of pharmacotherapy,18 the
possibility ought to be considered that cognitive therapy, particularly when focusing on residual functional
disturbances, would be useful as an adjunctive or
maintenance treatment strategy.
255
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
256
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Medical Research
Council of Canada. HA is an Ontario Mental Health
Foundation Senior Research Fellow.
References
1 Akiskal HS, Bolis CL, Cazzullo C, Costa e Silva JA, Gentil V, Lecrubier Y et al. Dysthymia in neurological disorders. Mol Psychiatry 1996; 1: 478–491.
2 Howland RH, Thase ME. Biological studies of dysthymia. Biol
Psychiatry 1991; 30: 283–304.
3 Klein DN, Kocsis JH, McCullough JP, Holzer CE, Hirschfeld RM,
Keller MB. Symptomatology in dysthymic and major depressive
disorder. Psychiat Clin North Am 1996; 19: 41–53.
4 Ravindran AV, Merali Z, Anisman H. Dysthymia: a biological perspective. In: Licinio J, Bolis CL, Gold P (eds). Dysthymia: From
Clinical Neuroscience to Treatment. World Health Organization:
Geneva, 1997, pp 21–44.
5 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders. Editions 3 and 4; 1980, 1994.
6 Akiskal HS. Dysthymic disorder: psychopathology of proposed
chronic depressive subtypes. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140: 11–20.
7 Freeman H. How the concept of dysthymia has developed.: a biological perspective. In: Licinio J, Bolis CL, Gold P (eds). Dysthymia: From Clinical Neuroscience to Treatment. World Health
Organization: Geneva, 1997, pp 1–8.
8 World Health Organization. The ICD–10 classification of mental
and behavioural disorders—clinical descriptions and diagnostic
guidelines. WHO: Geneva, 1992.
9 Akiskal HS. Towards a definition of dysthymia: boundaries with
personality and mood disorders. In: Burton SW, Akiskal HS (eds).
Dysthymic Disorders. Gaskell: London, 1990, pp 1–12.
10 McCullough JP, Kornstein SG, McCullough JP, Belyea-Caldwell S,
Kaye AL, Roberts C et al. Differential diagnosis of chronic depressive disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1996; 19: 41–53.
11 Keller MB, Sessa FM. Dysthymia: development and clinical
course. In: Burton SW, Akiskal HS (eds). Dysthymic Disorders.
Gaskell: London, 1990, pp 13–23.
12 Regier DA, Narrow WE, Rae DS, Manderscheid RW, Locke BZ,
Goodwin FK. The de facto US mental and addictive disorders service system: Epidemiologic Catchment Area prospective 1-year
prevalence rates of disorders and services. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1993; 50: 85–94.
13 Regier DA, Boyd JH, Burke JD, Rae DS, Myers JK, Kramer M et al.
One-month prevalence of mental disorders in the United States.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45: 977–986.
14 Keller MB, Klein DN, Hirschfeld RMA, Docsis JH, McCullough JP,
Miller I et al. Results of the DSM-IV Mood Disorders Field Trial.
Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 843–849.
15 Kovacs M, Feinberg TL, Crouse-Novak MA, Paulauskas SL, Finkelstein R. Depressive disorders in childhood, I: a longitudinal prospective study of characteristics and recovery. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41: 229–237.
16 Shelton RC, Davidson J, Yonkers KA, Koran L, Thase ME,
Pearlstein T et al. The undertreatment of dysthymia. J Clin Psychiatry 1997; 58: 59–65.
17 Howland RH. Pharmacotherapy of dysthymia: a review. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1991; 11: 83–92.
18 Versiani M. Pharmacotherapy of dysthymic and chronic depressive disorders: overview with focus on moclobemide. J Affect Disord 1998; 51: 323–332.
19 Akiskal HS, Weise RE. The clinical spectrum of so-called ‘minor’
depressions. Am J Psychother 1992; 46: 9–22.
20 Markowitz JC. Psychotherapy of dysthymia. Am J Psychiatry 1994;
151: 1114–1121.
21 McCullogh JP. Psychotherapy for dysthymia: a naturalistic study
of ten patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 1991; 179: 734–740.
22 Ravindran AV, Anisman H, Merali Z, Charbonneau Y, Telner J,
Bialik RJ et al. Treatment of primary dysthymia with group cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy: clinical symptoms and functional impairments. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156: 1608–1617.
Molecular Psychiatry
23 Dunner DL, Schmaling KB, Hendrickson H, Becker J, Lehman A,
Bea C. Cognitive therapy versus fluoxetine in the treatment of dysthymic disorder. Depression 1996; 4: 34–41.
24 Keller MB, Shapiro RW. Double depression: superimposition of
acute depressive episodes on chronic depressive disorders. Am J
Psychiatry 1982; 139: 438–442.
25 Keller MB, Lavori PW, Endicott J, Coryell W, Klerman GL. Double
depression: two year follow up. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:
689–694.
26 Weissman MM, Leaf PJ, Bruce ML, Florio L. The epidemiology
of dysthymia in five communities: rates, risks, comorbidity and
treatment. Am J Psychiatry 1988; 145: 815–819.
27 Levitt AJ, Joffe RT, Sokolov STH. Does the chronological relationship between the onset of dysthymia and major depression influence subsequent response to antidepressants? J Affect Disord
1998; 47: 169–175.
28 Pini S, Cassano GB, Simonini E, Savino M, Russo A, Montgomery
SA. Prevalence of anxiety disorders comorbidity in bipolar
depression, unipolar depression and dysthymia. J Affect Disord
1997; 42: 145–153.
29 Coryell W, Endicott J, Andreasen N, Keller MB, Clayton PJ,
Hirschfeld RM et al. Depression and panic attacks: the significance
of overlap as reflected in follow-up and family study data. Am J
Psychiatry 1988; 145: 293–300.
30 Gaynes BN, Magruder KM, Burns BJ, Wagner R, Yarnall KSH,
Broadhead WE. Does a coexisting anxiety disorder predict persistence of depressive illness in primary care patients with major
depression? Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1999; 21: 158–167.
31 Pepper CM, Klein DN, Anderson RL, Riso LP, Ouimette P, Lizardi
H. DSM-III-R axis II comorbidity in dysthymia and major
depression. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 239–247.
32 Markowitz JC, Moran MM, Kocsis JH, Frances AJ. Prevalence and
comorbidity of dysthymic disorder among psychiatric outpatients.
J Affect Disord 1992; 24: 63–67.
33 Marin DB, Kocsis JH, Frances AJ, Klerman GL. Personality disorders in dysthymia. J Personality Disord 1993; 7: 223–231.
34 Anderson RL, Klein DN, Riso LP, Ouimette PC, Lizardi H,
Schwartz JE. The subaffective-character spectrum subtyping distinction primary early-onset dysthymia: a clinical and familial
study. J Affect Disord 1996; 38: 13–22.
35 Garyfallos G, Adamopoulou A, Karastergiou A, Voikli M, Sotiropoulou A, Donias S et al. Personality disorders in dysthymia and
major depression. Acta Psychiat Scand 1999; 99: 332–340.
36 Klein DN, Schatzberg AF, McCullough JP, Keller MB, Dowling F,
Goodman D et al. Early- versus late-onset dysthymic disorder:
comparison in out-patients with superimposed major depressive
episodes. J Affect Disord 1999; 52: 187–196.
37 Klein DN, Shih JH. Depressive personality: associations with
DSM-III-R mood and personality disorders and negative and positive affectivity, 30-month stability, and prediction of course of
Axis I depressive disorders. J Abn Psychol 1998; 107: 319–327.
38 Davidson KM, Ritson FB. The relationship between alcohol
dependence and depression. Alcohol Alcohol 1993; 28: 147–155.
39 Lynskey MT. The comorbidity of alcohol dependence and affective disorders: treatment implications. Drug Alcohol Depend 1998;
52: 201–209.
40 Eames SL, Westermeyer J, Crosby RD. Substance use and abuse
among patients with comorbid dysthymia and substance disorder.
Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 1998; 24: 541–550.
41 Westermeyer J, Eames SL, Nugent S. Comorbid dysthymia and
substance disorder: treatment history and cost. Am J Psychiatry
1998; 155: 1556–1560.
42 Miller IW, Norman WH, Dow MG. Psychosocial characteristics of
‘double depression’. Am J Psychiatry 1986; 143: 1042–1044.
43 Akiskal HS, King D, Rosenthal TL, Robinson S, Scott-Strauss A.
Chronic depressions, 1: clinical and familial characteristics in 137
probands. J Affect Disord 1981; 3: 297–315.
44 Anisman H, Zalcman S, Zacharko RM. The impact of stressors on
immune and central transmitter activity: bidirectional communication. Rev Neurosci 1993; 4: 147–180.
45 Hirschfeld RMA. Personality and dysthymia. In: Burton SW,
Akiskal HS (eds). Dysthymic Disorders. Gaskell: London, 1990,
pp 69–77.
46 Klein DN, Taylor EB, Dickstein S, Harding K. The early-late onset
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
distinction in DSM-III-R dysthymia. J Affect Disord 1988; 14:
25–33.
Kocsis JH, Frances AJ. A critical discussion of DSM-III dysthymic
disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144: 1534–1542.
McCullough JP, Braith, JA, Chapman RC, Kasnetz MD, Carr KF,
Cones JH et al. Comparison of early and late onset dysthymia. J
Nerv Ment Dis 1990; 178: 577–581.
Sansone RA, Wiederman MW, Sansone LA, Touchet B. Earlyonset dysthymia and personality disturbance among patients in a
primary care setting. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186: 57–58.
Klein DN, Taylor EB, Dickstein S, Harding K. Primary early-onset
dysthymia: comparison with primary non-bipolar non-chronic
major depression on demographic, clinical, familial, personality,
and socioenvironmental characteristics and short-term outcome. J
Abn Psychol 1988; 97: 387–398.
Brunello N, Akiskal H, Boyer P, Gessa GL, Howland RH, Langer
SZ et al. Dysthymia: clinical picture, extent of overlap with
chronic fatigue syndrome, neuropharmacological considerations,
and new therapeutic vistas. J Affect Disord 1999; 52: 275–290.
Walker E, Katon W, Jemelka R. Psychiatric disorders and medical
care utilization among people who report fatigue in the general
population. J Gen Intern Med 1993; 8: 436–440.
Epstein SA, Kay G, Clauw D, Heaton R, Klein D, Krupp L et al.
Psychiatric disorders in patients with fibromyalgia: a multicentre
investigation. Psychosomatics 1999; 40: 57–63.
Keel P. Psychological and psychiatric aspects of fibromyalgia syndrome (FMS). Z Rheumatol 1998; 57: 97–100.
Anisman H, Ravindran AV, Griffiths J, Merali Z. Behavioral, endocrine and cytokine correlates of major depression and dysthymia
with typical or atypical features. Mol Psychiatry 1999; 4: 182–188.
Cannon JG, Angel JB, Abad LW, Vannier E, Mileno MD, Fagioli
L et al. Interleukin-1 beta, interleukin-1 receptor antagonist, and
soluble interleukin-1 type II secretion in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Clin Immunol 1997; 17: 253–261.
Chao CC, Janoff EN, Hu SX, Thomas K, Gallagher M, Tsang M et
al. Altered cytokine release in peripheral blood mononuclear cell
cultures from patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome. Cytokine 1991; 3: 292–298.
Mitsikostas DD, Thomas AM. Comorbidity of headache and
depressive disorders. Cephalalgia 1999; 19: 211–217.
Okasha A, Ismail MK, Khalil AH, EI Fiki R, Soliman A, Okasha
T. A psychiatric study of nonorganic chronic headache patients.
Psychosomatics 1999; 40: 233–238.
Puca F, Genco S, Prudenzano MP, Savarese M, Bussone G, D’
Amico D et al. Psychiatric comorbidity and psychosocial stress in
patients with tension-type headache from headache centers in
Italy. Cephalalgia 1999; 19: 159–164.
Poewe W, Luginger E. Depression in Parkinson’s disease: impediments to recognition and treatment options. Neurology 1999; 52:
S2–S6.
Taylor CA, Jung HY. Disorders of mood after traumatic brain
injury. Sem Clin Neuropsychiat 1998; 3: 224–231.
Rothwell NJ. Cytokines—killers in the brain. J Physiol 1999; 514:
3–17.
Schildkraut JJ. Current status of the catecholamine hypothesis of
affective disorders. In: Lipton MA, DiMascio A, Killam KF (eds).
Psychopharmacology: A Generation of Progress. Raven Press: New
York, 1978, pp 1223–1234.
Coppen A, Prange AJ Jr, Whybrow PC, Noguera R. Abnormalities
of indolamines in affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972;
26: 474–478.
van Praag HM. Amine hypotheses of affective disorders. In:
Iversen LL, Iversen SD, Snyder SH (eds). Handbook of Psychopharmacology. Plenum: New York, 1978, pp 187–298.
Brown AS, Gershon S. Dopamine and depression. J Neurotrans
1993; 91: 75–109.
Jimerson DC. Role of dopamine mechanisms in the affective disorders. In: Meltzer HY (ed). Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation of Progress. Raven: New York, 1987, pp 505–511.
Siever LJ. Role of noradrenergic mechanisms in the etiology of the
affective disorders. In: Meltzer HY (ed). Psychopharmacology: The
Third Generation of Progress. Raven: New York, 1987, pp 493–
504.
Antelman SM, Chiodo LA. Amphetamine as a stressor. In: Creese
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
I (ed). Stimulants: Neurochemical, Behavioral and Clinical Perspectives. Raven Press: New York, 1983, pp 269–300.
Meltzer HY, Lowy MT. The serotonin hypothesis of depression.
In: Meltzer HY (ed). Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation
of Progress. Raven Press: New York, 1987, pp 513–526.
Sulser F. Antidepressant treatments and regulation of norepinephrine-receptor-coupled adenylate cyclase systems in brain. In:
Usdin E, Asberg M, Bertilsson L, Sjoqvist F (eds). Frontiers in Biochemical and Pharmacological Research in Depression. Raven:
New York, 1984, pp 249–262.
Maas JW. Biogenic amines and depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1975; 32: 1357–1361.
Checkley SA. Neuroendocrine mechanisms and the precipitation
of depression by life events. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 160: 7–17.
Joffe RT, Bagby RM, Levit AJ. The thyroid and melancholia. Psychiatr Res 1992; 42: 73–80.
Prange AJ, Loosen PT, Wilson IC, Lipton MA. The therapeutic use
of hormones of the thyroid axis in depression. In: Post RM, Ballenger JC (eds). Neurobiology of Mood Disorders. Williams & Wilkins: Baltimore, 1984, pp 311–322.
Rubin RT, Poland RE. The dexamethasone suppression test in
depression: advantages and limitation. In: Burrows GD, Norman
TR, McGuire KP (eds). Biological Psychiatry: Recent Studies. Libbey: London, 1984, pp 76–83.
Brown GW, Harris TO. Life Events and Illness. Guilford Press:
New York, 1989.
Brown GW, Harris TO. Social Origins of Depression: A Study of
Psychiatric Disorder in Women. Free Press: New York, 1978.
Robins CJ, Block P. Personal vulnerability, life events, and
depressive symptoms: a test of a specific interactional model. J
Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 847–852.
Abramson LY, Seligman MEP, Teasdale JD. Learned helplessness
in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abn Psychol 1978; 87:
49–74.
Alloy LB, Clements CM. Illusion of control: invulnerability to
negative affect and depressive symptoms after laboratory and
natural stressors. J Abn Psychol 1992; 2: 234–245.
Anisman H, Zalcman S, Shanks N, Zacharko RM. Multisystem
regulation of performance deficits induced by stressors: an animal
model of depression. In: Boulton A, Baker G, Martin-Iverson M
(eds). Neuromethods, vol. 19: Animal Models of Psychiatry, II.
Humana Press: New Jersey, 1991, pp 1–59.
Weiss JM, Simson PE. Electrophysiology of the locus coeruleus:
implications for stress-induced depression. In: Koob GF, Ehlers
CL, Kupfer DJ (eds). Animal Models of Depression. Birkhauser:
Boston, 1989, pp 111–134.
Heinsbroek R, van Haaren F, Feenstra M, van de Poll N. Changes
in dopamine and noradrenaline activity in the frontal cortex produced by controllable and uncontrollable shock. Behav Pharmacol 1989; 1: 61.
Petty F, Sherman A. A neurochemical differentiation between
exposure to stress and the development of learned helplessness.
Drug Devel Res 1982; 2: 43–45.
Ritter S, Pelzer NL. Magnitude of stress induced norepinephrine
depletion varies with age. Brain Res 1978; 152: 1701–1705.
Tanaka M, Kohno Y, Nakagawa R et al. Time related differences
in noradrenaline turnover in rat brain regions by stress. Pharm
Biochem Behav 1982; 16: 315–319.
Anisman H, Sklar LS. Catecholamine depletion upon reexposure
to stress: mediation of the escape deficits produced by inescapable
shock. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1979; 93: 610–625.
Cassens G, Roffman M, Kuruc A, Orsulak PJ, Schildkraut JJ. Alterations of brain norepinephrine metabolism induced by environmental stimuli paired with inescapable shock. Science 1980; 209:
1138–1140.
Doherty MD, Gratton A. High-speed chronoamperometric
measurements of mesolimbic and nigostriatal dopamine release
associated with repeated daily stress. Brain Res 1992; 586: 295–
302.
Herman JP, Guillonneau D, Dantzer R, Scatton B, Semerdjian-Rouquier L, LeMoal M. Differential effects of inescapable footshock
and stimuli previously paired with inescapable footshocks on
dopamine turnover in cortical and limbic areas of the rat. Life Sci
1982; 30: 2207–2214.
257
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
258
93 Roth RH, Tam S-Y, Ida Y, Yang J-X, Deutch Y. Stress and mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Ann NY Acad Sci 1988; 537: 138–
147.
94 Antelman SM. Time-dependent sensitization as the cornerstone
for a new approach to pharmacotherapy: drugs as foreign/stressful
stimuli. Drug Dev Res 1988; 14: 1–30.
95 Nisenbaum LK, Abercrombie ED. Enhanced tyrosine hydroxylation in hippocampus of chronically stressed rats upon exposure
to a novel stressor. J Neurochem 1992; 58: 276–281.
96 Nisenbaum LK, Zigmond MJ, Sved AF, Abercrombie ED. Prior
exposure to chronic stress results in enhanced synthesis and
release of hippocampal norepinephrine in response to a novel
stressor. J Neurosci 1991; 11: 1478–1484.
97 Deutch AY, Roth RH. The determinants of stress-induced activation of the prefrontal cortical dopamine system. Prog Brain Res
1990; 85: 367–402.
98 Herman JP, Stinus L, Le Moal M. Repeated stress increases locomotor response to amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 1984; 84:
431–435.
99 Irwin M. Psychoneuroimmunology of depression. In: Bloom FE,
Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of
Progress. Raven Press: New York, 1995, pp 983–998.
100 Kvetnansky R. Recent progress in catecholamines under stress. In:
Usdin E, Kvetnansky R, Kopin IJ (eds). Catecholamines and
Stress: Recent Advances. Elsevier: New York 1980, pp 7–20.
101 Zacharko RM, Anisman H. Pharmacological, biochemical and
behavioral analyses of depression: animal models. In: Koob GF,
Ehlers CL, Kupfer DJ (eds). Animal Models of Depression. Birkhauser: Boston, 1989, pp 204–238.
102 Stone EA. Central cyclic-AMP-linked noradrenergic receptors:
new findings on properties as related to the actions of stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1987; 11: 391–398.
103 Molina VA, Volosin M, Cancela L, Keller E, Murua VS, Basso AM.
Effect of chronic variable stress on monoamine receptors: influence of imipramine administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
1990; 35: 335–340.
104 Willner P. The anatomy of melancholy: the catecholamine
hypothesis of depression revisted. Rev Neurosci 1987; 1: 77–99.
105 Anisman H, Merali Z. Chronic stressors and animal models of
depression: distinguishing characteristics and individual profiles.
Psychopharmacology 1997; 134: 330–332.
106 Schulkin J, Gold PW, McEwen BS. Induction of corticotropinreleasing hormone gene expression by glucocorticoids: implication for understanding the states of fear and anxiety and allostatic load. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1998; 23: 219–243.
107 Holsboer F. Neuroendocrinology of mood disorders. In: Bloom FE,
Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of
Progress. Raven Press: New York, 1995, pp 957–969.
108 Maes M, Meltzer HY. The serotonin hypothesis of major
depression. In: Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology:
The Fourth Generation of Progress. Raven Press: New York, 1995,
pp 933–944.
109 Plotsky PM, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB. Neuropeptide alterations in
mood disorders. In: Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. Raven Press: New York,
1995, pp 971–981.
110 Schatzberg AF, Schildkraut JJ. Recent studies on norepinephrine
systems on mood disorders. In: Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. Raven: New
York, 1995, pp 911–920.
111 Ravindran AV, Bialik RJ, Lapierre YD. Primary early onset dysthymia, biochemical correlates of the therapeutic response to fluoxetine: 1. Platelet monoamine oxidase and the dexamethasone suppression test. J Affect Disord 1994; 31: 111–117.
112 Jansen LMC, Gispen-de Wied CC, Jansen MA, van der Gaag RJ,
Matthys W, van Engeland H. Pituitary-adrenal reactivity in a child
psychiatric population: salivary cortisol response to stressors. Eur
Psychopharmacol 1999; 9: 67–75.
113 Szadoczky E, Sazekas I, Rihmer Z, Arato M. The role of psychosocial and biological variables in separating chronic and nonchronic major depression and early-late onset dysthymia. J Affect
Disord 1994; 32: 1–11.
114 Versiani M, Amrein R, Stabl M, the International Collaborative
Study Group. Moclobemide and imipramine in chronic
Molecular Psychiatry
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
depression (dysthymia): an international double-blind, placebocontrolled trial. Int Clin Psychopharm 1997; 12: 183–193.
Lechin F, van der Dijs B, Orozco B, Lechin AE. Plasma neurotransmitters, blood pressure, and heart rate during supine resting,
orthostasis, and moderate exercise in dysthymic depressed
patients. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 37: 884–891.
Wahlund B, Saaf J, Wetterberg L. Clinical symptoms and platelet
monoamine oxidase in subgroups and different states of affective
disorders. J Affect Disord 1995; 35: 75–87.
Ravindran AV, Bialik RJ, Brown GM, Lapierre YD. Primary early
onset dysthymia, biochemical correlates of the therapeutic
response to fluoxetine: II. Urinary metabolites of serotonin, norepinephrine, epinephrine and melatonin. J Affect Disord 1994; 31:
119–123.
Yee CM, Miller GA. A dual-task analysis of resource allocation in
dysthymia and anhedonia. J Abn Psychol 1994; 103: 625–636.
Yee CM, Deldin PJ, Miller GA. Early stimulus processing in dysthymia and anhedonia. J Abn Psychol 1992; 101: 230–233.
Akiskal HS, Lemmi H, Dickson H, King D, Yerevanian B, Van Valkenberg C. Chronic depressions. Part 2. Sleep EEG differentiation
of primary dysthymic disorders from anxious depressions. J Affect
Disord 1984; 6: 287–295.
Akiskal HS, Rosenthal TL, Haykal RF, Lemmi H, Rosenthal RT,
Scott-Strauss A. Characterological depressions: clinical and sleep
EEG findings separating ‘subaffective dysthymias’ from ‘character
spectrum’ disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980; 37: 777–783.
Akiskal HS, Judd LL, Gillin C, Lemmi H. Subthreshold
depressions: clinical and polysomnographic validation of dysthymic, residual and masked forms. J Affect Disord 1997; 45:
53–63.
Arriaga F, Cavaglia F, Matospires A, Lara E, Paiva T. EEG sleep
characteristics in dysthymia and major depressive disorder.
Neuropsychobiology 1995; 32: 128–131.
Baldwin D, Rudge S, Thomas S. Dysthymia: options in pharmacotherapy. CNS Drugs 1995; 4: 422–431.
Conte HR, Karasu TB. A review of treatment studies of minor
depression: 1980–1991. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 46: 58–74.
Dunner DL. Treatment of dysthymic disorder. Depress Anxiety
1998; 8: 54–58.
Frank E, Thase ME. Natural history and preventative treatment of
recurrent mood disorders. Ann Rev Med 1999; 50: 453–468.
Gorman JM, Kent JM. SSRIs and SNRIs: broad spectrum of efficacy
beyond major depression. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60: 33–38.
Lopez Ibor JJ, Frances A, Jones C. Dysthymic disorder: a comparison of DSM-IV and ICD-10 and issues in differential diagnosis.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1994; 89 (suppl 383): 12–18.
Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Liebowitz MR. Treatment outcome validation of DSM-III depressive subtypes: clinical usefulness in outpatients with mild to moderate depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1985; 42: 1148–1153.
Friedman RA, Markowitz JC, Parides M, Kocsis JH. Acute
response of social functioning in dysthymic patients with desipramine. J Affect Disord 1995; 34: 85–88.
Keller MB, Gelengerg AJ, Hirschfeld RN, Rush AJ, Thase ME,
Kocsis JH et al. The treatment of chronic depression, part 2: a
double blind, randomized trial of sertraline and imipramine. J
Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59: 598–607.
Kocsis JH, Frances AJ, Voss C, Mann JJ, Mason BJ, Sweeney J.
Imipramine treatment for chronic depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45: 253–257.
Kocsis JH, Thase M, Koran L, Halbreich U, Yonkers K. Pharmacotherapy of pure dysthymia: sertraline vs imipramine and placebo.
Eur Neuropsychopharm 1994; 4 (suppl 3): S204.
Marin DB, Kocsis JH, Frances AJ, Parides M. Desipramine for the
treatment of ‘pure’ dysthymia versus ‘double’ depression. Am J
Psychiatry 1994; 151: 1079–1080.
Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Quitkin FM, Rabkin J, Harrison W, Wager
S et al. Chronic depression: response to placebo, imipramine, and
phenelzine. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1993; 13: 391–396.
Versiani M, Nardi D, Capponi R, Costa DA, Magistris H, Ucha
Udabe R. Moclobemide compared with imipramine in the treatment of chronic depression (dysthymia DSM-III-R): a double-blind
placebo-controlled trial. Clin Neuropharmacol 1992; 15 (suppl
1): 148b.
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
138 Petursson H. Studies of reversible and selective inhibitors of
monoamine oxidase A in dysthymia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995;
91 (suppl 386): 36–39.
139 Vallejo J, Gasto C, Catalan R, Salamero M. Double blind study
of imipramine versus phenelzine in melancholias and dysthymic
disorders. Br J Psychiatry 1987; 151: 639–642.
140 Baumhackl U, Biziere K, Fischbach R, Geretsegger C, Hebenstreit
G, Radmayr E et al. Efficacy and tolerability of moclobemide compared with imipramine in depressive disorder (DSM-III): an
Austrian double-blind multicentre study. Br J Psychiatry 1989;
suppl 6: 78–83.
141 Botte L, Evrard JL, Gilles C, Stenier P, Wolfrum C. Controlled comparison of RO 11–1163 (mocobemide) and placebo I the treatment
of depression. Acta Psychiat Belg 1992; 92: 355–369.
142 Duarte A, Camozzi CR. Moclobemide versus fluoxetine in the
treatment of dysthmia. 9th World Congress of Psychiatry abstract,
Rio de Janeiro, 1993.
143 Versiani M. Pharmacotherapy of dysthymia: a controlled study
with imipramine, moclobemide or placebo. Neuropsychopharmacology 1993; 10(3S): 298.
144 Hellerstein DJ, Yanowitch P, Rosenthal J, Samstag LW, Maurer M,
Dasch K et al. A randomized double blind study of fluoxetine
versus placebo in the treatment of dysthymia. Am J Psychiatry
1993; 150: 1169–1175.
145 Thase ME, Fava M, Halbreich U, Kocsis JH, Koran L, Davidson J
et al. A placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial comparing
sertraline and imipramine for the treatment of dysthymia. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53: 777–784.
146 Vanelle JM, Attar-Levy D, Poirier MF, Bouhassira M, Blin P, Olie
JP. Controlled efficacy study of fluoxetine in dysthymia. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 170: 345–350.
147 Bakish D, Lapierre YD, Weinstein R, Klein J, Wiens A, Jones B et
al. Ritanserin, imipramine and placebo in the treatment of dysthymic disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1993; 13: 409–414.
148 Bersani G, Pozzi F, Marini S, Grispini A, Pasini A, Ciani N. 5HT2 receptor antagonism in dysthymic disorder: a double-blind
placebo-controlled study with ritanserin. Acta Psychiatr Scand
1991; 83: 244–248.
149 Burrows GD, Maguire KP, Norman TR. Antidepressant efficacy
and tolerability of the selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor
reboxetine: a review. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59: 4–7.
150 Dunner DL, Hendrickson HE, Bea C, Budech CB. Venlafaxine in
dysthymic disorder. J Clin Psychiat 1997; 58: 528–531.
151 Ravindran AV, Charbonneau Y, Zaharia M, Al-Zaid K, Wiens A,
Anisman H. Efficacy and tolerability of venlafaxine in the treatment of primary dysthymia. J Psychiat Neurosci 1998; 23: 288–
292.
152 Harrison W, Rabkin J, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Tricamo E, Quitkin
F. Phenelzine for chronic depressions: a study of continuation
treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 1986; 47: 346–349.
153 Tyrer P, Seivewright N, Murphy S, Ferguson B, Kingdon D, Barczak P et al. The Nottingham study of neurotic disorder: comparison of drug and psychological treatments. Lancet 1988; 8605:
235–240.
154 Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Quitkin FM, Harrison W, Markowitz J,
Wager S et al. Relevance of DSM-III depressive subtype and
chronicity to antidepressant efficacy in atypical depression: differential response to phenelzine, imipramine, and placebo. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1989; 46: 1080–1087.
155 Costa-e-Silva JA. Treatment of dysthymic disorder with low-dose
amisulpride. A comparative study of 50 mg/day amisulpride versus placebo. Ann Psychiatry 1990; 5: 242–249.
156 Lecrubier Y, Boyer P, Turjanski S, Rein W, the Amisulpride Study
Group. Amisulpride versus imipramine and placebo in dysthymia
and major depression. J Affect Disord 1997; 43: 95–103.
157 Boyer P, Lecrubier Y, Stalla-Bourdillon A, Fleurot O. Amisulpride
versus amineptine and placebo for the treatment of dysthymia.
Neuropsychobiology 1999; 39: 25–32.
158 Ravindran AV, Bialik RJ, Lapierre YD. Therapeutic efficacy of specific serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in dysthymia. Can J
Psychiatry 1994; 39: 21–26.
159 Friedman RA, Parides M, Baff R, Moran M, Kocsis JH. Predictors
of response to desipramine in dysthymia. J Clin Psychopharm
1995; 15: 280–283.
160 Kocsis JH, Friedman RA, Markowitz JC, Leon AC, Miller NL,
Gniwesch L et al. Maintenance therapy for chronic depression. A
controlled clinical trial of desipramine. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;
53: 769–774.
161 Santagostino G, Cucchi ML, Frattini P, Zerbi F, Di Paolo E, Preda
S et al. The influence of alprazolam on the monoaminergic neurotransmitter systems in dysthymic patients. Relationship to clinical
response. Pharmacopsychiatry 1998; 31: 131–136.
162 Smeraldi E. Amisulpride versus fluoxetine in patients with dysthymia or major depression in partial remission. A double-blind,
comparative study. J Affect Disord 1998; 48: 47–56.
163 Bloch M, Schmidt PJ, Danaceau MA, Adams LF, Rubinow DR.
Dehydroepiandrosterone treatment of midlife dysthymia. Biol
Psychiatry 1999; 45: 1533–1541.
164 Rudas S, Schmitz M, Pichler P, Baumgartner A. Treatment of
refractory chronic depression and dysthymia with high-dose thyroxin. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45: 229–233.
165 McCleod MN, Gaynes BN, Golden RN. Chromium potentiation of
antidepressant pharmacotherapy for dysthymic disorder in 5
patients. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60: 237–240.
166 Kocsis JH, Sutton BM, Frances AJ. Long-term follow-up of chronic
depression treated with imipramine. J Clin Psychiatry 1991; 52:
56–59.
167 Hellerstein DJ, Samstag LW, Cantillon M, Maurer M, Rosenthal J,
Yanowitch P et al. Follow-up assessment of medication-treated
dysthymia. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1996;
20: 427–442
168 Wise RA. The anhedonia hypothesis: mark III. Behav Brain Sci
1985; 8: 178–186.
169 Fibiger HC. Neurobiology of depression: focus on dopamine. In:
Gessa GL, Fratta W, Pani L, Serra G (eds). Depression and Mania:
From Neurobiology to Treatment. Lippincott-Raven: Philadelphia,
1995, pp 1–42.
170 Kapur S, Mann JJ. Role of the dopaminergic system in depression.
Biol Psychiatry 1992; 32: 1–17.
171 Maier W, Benkert O. Treatment of chronic depression with sulpiride: evidence of efficacy in placebo-controlled single case studies.
Psychopharmacology 1994; 115: 495–501.
172 Leon CA, Vigoya J, Conde S, Campo G, Castrillon E, Leon A. Comparison of the effect of amisulpride and viloxazine in the treatment of dysthymia. Acta Psiquiatr Psicol Am Lat 1994; 40: 41–49.
173 Goldstein RB, Weissman MM, Adams PB, Horwath E, Lish JD,
Charney D et al. Psychiatric disorders in relatives of probands
with panic disorder and/or major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1994; 51: 383–394.
174 Goodman DW, Barnhill J. Family and genetic epidemiologic studies. In: Kocsis JH, Klein DN (eds). Diagnosis and Treatment of
Chronic Depression. Guilford Press: New York, 1995, pp 103–123.
175 Klein DN, Riso LP, Donaldson SK, Schwartz JE, Anderson RL,
Ouimette PL et al. Family study of early-onset dysthymia. Mood
and personality disorders in relatives of outpatients with dysthymia and episodic major depression and normal controls. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1995; 52: 487–496.
176 Donaldson SK, Klein DN, Riso LP, Schwartz JE. Comorbidity
between dysthymic and major depressive disorders: a family
study analysis. J Affect Disord 1997; 42: 103–111.
177 Klein DN, Riso LP, Anderson RL. DSM-III-R dysthymia: antecedents and underlying assumptions. In: Chapman LJ, Chapman
JP, Fowles DC (eds). Progress in Experimental Personality and
Psychopathology Research, Vol 16. Springer: New York, 1993,
pp 222–253.
178 Riso LP, Klein DN, Ferro T, Kasch KL, Pepper CM, Schwartz JE et
al. Understanding the comorbidity between early-onset dysthymia
and cluster B personality disorders: a family study. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153: 900–906.
179 Ohara K, Nagai M, Suzuki Y, Ohara K. Low activity allele of catechol-o-methyltransferase gene and Japanese unipolar depression.
NeuroReport 1998; 9: 1305–1308.
180 Lyons MJ, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True W, Lin N, Meyer JM et al.
A registry-based twin study of depression in men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55: 468–472.
181 Monroe SM, Depue RA. Life stress and depression. In: Becker J,
Kleinman A (eds). Psychosocial Aspects of Depression. Erlbaum:
Hillsdale, 1991, pp 101–130.
259
Molecular Psychiatry
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
260
182 Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context
of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders.
Psychol Bull 1991; 110: 406–425.
183 Finlay-Jones R, Brown GW. Types of stressful life events and the
onset of anxiety and depressive disorders. Psychol Med 1981; 11:
803–816.
184 Lazarus RS. Coping theory and research: past, present, and future.
Psychosom Med 1993; 55: 234–247.
185 Hammen C. Generation of stress in the course of unipolar
depression. J Abn Psychol 1991; 100: 555–561.
186 Overholser JC. Emotional reliance and social loss: effects on
depressive symptomatology. J Pers Assess 1990; 55: 618–629.
187 Ravindran AV, Griffiths J, Merali Z, Anisman H. Primary dysthymia: a study of several psychosocial, endocrine and immune
correlates. J Affect Disord 1996; 40: 73–84.
188 Ravindran A, Griffiths J, Waddell C, Anisman H. Stressful life
events and coping styles in dysthymia and major depressive
disorder: variations associated with alleviation of symptoms
following pharmacotherapy. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol
Psychiatry 1995; 19: 637–653.
189 McCullough JP, Kasnetz MD, Braith JA, Carr KF, Cones JH, Fielo
J et al. A longitudinal study of an untreated sample of predominantly late onset characterological dysthymia. J Nerv Ment Dis
1988; 176: 658–667.
190 Benjaminsen S. Stressful life events preceding the onset of neurotic depression. Psychol Med 1981; 11: 369–378.
191 Monroe SM, Thase ME, Hersen M, Himmelhoch JM, Bellack AS.
Life events and the endogenous-non-endogenous distinction in
the treatment and posttreatment course of depression. Comp Psychiatry 1985; 26: 175–186.
192 Dura JR, Stukenberg KW, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Chronic stress and
depressive disorder in older adults. J Abn Psychol 1990; 99:
284–290.
193 Friedman RA. Social impairment in dysthymia. Psychiat Ann
1993; 23: 632–637.
194 Walker V, Streiner DL, Novosel S, Rocchi A, Levine MAH, Dean
DM. Health-related quality of life in patients with major
depression who are treated with moclobemide. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1995; 15: 60S–67S.
195 Wells K, Stewart A, Hays R, Burnam M, Rogers W, Daniels M et
al. The functioning and well-being of depressed patients: results
from the Medical Outcome Study. JAMA 1989: 262: 914–919.
196 Cassano GB, Perugi G, Maremmani I, Akiskal HS. Social adjustment in dysthymia. In: Burton SW, Akiskal HS (eds). Dysthymic
Disorders. Gaskell: London, 1990, pp 78–85.
197 De Lisio G, Maremmani I, Perugi G, Cassano GB, Deltito J, Akiskal
HS. Impairment of work and leisure in depressed outpatients. J
Affect Disord 1986; 10: 79–84.
198 Freeman HL. Historical and nosological aspects of dysthymia.
Acta Psychiat Scand 1994; 89: 7–11.
199 Leader JB, Klein DN. Social adjustment in dysthymia, double
depression and episodic major depression. J Affect Disord 1996;
37: 91–101.
200 Markowitz JC, Friedman RA, Miller N, Spielman LA, Moran ME,
Kocsis JH. Interpersonal improvement in chronically depressed
patients treated with desipramine. J Affect Disord 1996; 41: 59–62.
201 Agosti V, Stewart JW, Quitkin FM. Life satisfaction and psychosocial functioniong in chronic depression: effect of acute treatment with antidepressants. J Affect Disord 1991; 23: 35–41.
202 Kocsis JH, Zisook S, Davidson J, Shelton R, Yonkers K, Hellerstein
DJ et al. Double-blind comparison of sertraline, imipramine, and
placebo in the treatment of dysthymia: psychosocial outcomes.
Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154: 390–395.
203 Klein DN, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. Psychosocial characteristics
of adolescents with a past history of dysthymic disorder: comparison with adolescents with past histories of major depression and
non affective disorders, and never mentally ill controls. J Affect
Disord 1997; 42: 127–135.
204 Miller IW, Keitner GI, Schatzberg AF, Klein DN, Thase ME, Rush
AJ et al. The treatment of chronic depression, part 3: psychosocial
functioning before and after treatment with sertraline and imipramine. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59: 608–619.
205 Fawcett J. Antidepressants: partial response in chronic
depression. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1994; 26: 37–41.
Molecular Psychiatry
206 Paykel ES, Prusoff BA, Uhlenhuth EH. Scaling of life events. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1971; 25: 340–347.
207 Ramana R, Paykel ES, Cooper Z, Hayhurst H, Saxty M, Surtees
PG. Remission and relapse in major depression: a two year prospective follow-up study. Psychol Med 1995; 25: 1161–1170.
208 Maes M. Evidence for an immune response in major depression:
a review and hypothesis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1995; 19: 11–38.
209 Maes M, Bosmans E, Meltzer HY, Scharpe S, Suy E. Interleukin1 ␤: a putative mediator of HPA axis hyperactivity in major
depression? Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150: 1189–1193.
210 Maes M, Bosmans E, Suy E, Vandervorst C, de Jonckheere C,
Minner B et al. Depression-related disturbances in mitogeninduced lymphocyte responses and interleukin-1␤ and soluble
interleukin-2 receptor production. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1991; 84:
379–386.
211 Maes M, Lambrechts J, Bosmans E, Jacobs J, Suy E, Vandervorst
C et al. Evidence for a systemic immune activation during
depression: results of leukocyte enumeration by flow cytometry
in conjunction with monoclonal antibody staining. Psychol Med
1992; 22: 45–53.
212 Maes M, Meltzer HY, Bosmans E, Bergmans R, Vandoolaeghe E,
Ranjan R et al. Increased plasma concentrations of interleukin-6,
soluble interleukin-6, soluble interleukin-2 and transferrin receptor in major depression. J Affect Disord 1995; 34: 301–309.
213 Mullar N, Ackenheil M. Psychoneuroimmunology and the cytokine action in the CNS: implications for psychiatric disorders.
Prog Neuropsychopharm Biol Psychiatry 1998; 22: 1–33.
214 Nassberger L, Traskman-Bendz L. Increased soluble interleukin-2
receptor concentrations in suicide attempters. Acta Psychiatr
Scand 1993; 88: 48–52.
215 Smith RS. The macrophage theory of depression. Med Hypoth
1991; 35: 298–306.
216 Song C, Dinan T, Leonard BE. Changes in immunoglobulin, complement and acute phase protein levels in depressed patients and
normal controls. J Affect Disord 1994; 30: 283–288.
217 Sluzewska A, Rybakowski JK, Laciak M, Mackiewicz A, Sobieska
M, Wiktorowicz K. Interleukin-6 serum levels in depressed
patients before and after treatment with fluoxetine. Ann NY Acad
Sci 1995; 762: 474–476.
218 Anisman H, Ravindran AV, Griffiths J, Merali H. Interleukin-1
variations associated with dysthymia prior to and following antidepressant medication. Biol Psychiatry (in press).
219 Ravindran A, Griffiths J, Merali Z, Anisman H. Lymphocyte subsets in major depression and dysthymia: modification by antidepressant treatment. Psychosomatic Med 1995; 57: 555–563.
220 Griffiths J, Ravindran AV, Merali Z, Anisman H. Immune and
behavioral correlates of typical and atypical depression. Soc Neurosci Abst 1996; 22: 1350.
221 Demitrack MA, Dale JK, Straus SE, Laue L, Listwak SJ, Kruesi
MJP et al. Evidence for impaired activation of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal axis in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1991; 148: 337–344.
222 Gold PW, Licinio J, Wong ML, Chrousos GP. Corticotropin releasing hormone in pathophysiology of melancholic and atypical
depression and in the mechanism of action of antidepressant
drugs. Ann NY Acad Sci 1995; 771: 716–729.
223 Dunn AJ. Interactions between the nervous system and the
immune system: implications for psychopharmacology. In: Bloom
FE, Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation
of Progress. Raven Press: New York, 1995, pp 719–731.
224 Rivier C. Effect of peripheral and central cytokines on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of the rat. Ann NY Acad Sci 1993;
697: 97–105.
225 Maes M. Major depression and activation of the inflammatory
response system. Adv Exp Med Biol 1999; 461: 25–46.
226 Capuron L, Ravaud A, Radat F, Dantzer R, Goodall G. Affects of
interleukin-2 and alpha-interferon cytokine immunotherapy on
the mood and cognitive performance of cancer patients. Neuroimmunomodulation 1998; 5: 9.
227 Caraceni A, Martini C, Belli F, Mascheroni L, Rivoltini L, Arienti
F et al. Neuropsychological and neurophysiological assessment of
the central effects of interleukin-2 administration. Eur J Cancer
1992; 29A: 1266–1269.
Dysthymia
J Griffiths et al
228 Denicoff KD, Rubinow DR, Papa MZ, Simpson L, Seipp LA, Lotze
MT et al. The neuropsychiatric effects of treatment with interleukin-2 and lymphokine-activated killer cells. Ann Int Med 1987;
107: 293–300.
229 Meyers CA, Valentine AD. Neurological and psychiatric adverse
effects of immunological therapy. CNS Drugs 1995; 3: 56–68.
230 Meyers CA. Mood and cognitive disorders in cancer patients
receiving cytokine therapy. Adv Exp Med Biol 1999; 461: 75–81.
231 Post RM, Weiss SRB. The neurobiology of treatment-resistant
mood disorders. In: Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (eds). Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. Raven Press: New
York, 1995, pp 1155–1170.
232 Perris H. Life events and depression. Part 2. Results in diagnostic
subgroups, and in relation to the recurrence of depression. J Affect
Disord 1984; 7: 25–36.
233 Thase ME, Sullivan LR. Relapse and recurrence of depression: a
practical approach for prevention. CNS Drugs 1995; 4: 261–277.
234 Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:
999–1010.
235 Anisman H, Merali Z. Anhedonic and anxiogenic effects of cytokine exposure. Adv Exp Med Biol 1999; 461: 199–233.
236 De Kloet ER, Reul JMH. Feedback action and tonic influence of
corticosteroids on brain function: a concept arising from heterogeneity of brain receptor systems. Psychoneuroendocrinology
1987; 12: 83–105.
237 Dinan TG. Glucocorticoids and the genesis of depressive illness:
a psychobiological model. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 164: 365–371.
238 Merali Z, McIntosh J, Kent P, Michaud D, Anisman H. Aversive
as well as appetitive events evoke the release of corticotropin
releasing hormone and bombesin-like peptides at the central
nucleus of the amygdala. J Neurosci 1998; 18: 4758–4766.
239 Kalivas PW, Stewart J. Dopamine transmission in the initiation
and expression of drug-and stress-induced sensitization of motor
activity. Brain Res Rev 1991; 16: 223–244.
240 Nemeroff CB. The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) hypothsesis
of depression: new findings and new directions. Mol Psychiatry
1996; 1; 336–342.
241 Levitan RD, Kaplan AS, Brown GM, Joffe RT, Levitt AJ, Vaccarino
FJ et al. Low plasma cortisol in bulimia nervosa patients with
reversed neurovegetative symptoms of depression. Biol Psychiatry
1997; 41: 366–368.
242 Catalan R, Gallart JM, Castellanos JM, Galard R. Plasma corticotropin-releasing factor in depressive disorders. Biol Psychiatry
1998; 44: 15–20.
243 Bartanusz V, Jezova D, Bertini LT, Tilders FJ, Aubry JM, Kiss JZ.
Stress-induced increase in vasopressin and corticotropin-releasing factor expression in hypophysiotrophic paraventricular neurons. Endocrinology 1993; 132: 895–902.
244 Schmidt ED, Binnbekade R, Janszen AWJW, Tilders FJH. Short
stressor induced long-lasting increases of vasopressin stores in
hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) neurons in
adult rats. J Neuroendocrinol 1996; 8: 703–712.
245 Schmidt E, Janszen AWJW, Wouterlood FG, Tilders FJH. Interleukin-1 induced long-lasting changes in hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) neurons and hyperresponsiveness
of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. J Neurosci 1995; 15:
7417–7426.
246 Tilders FJH, Schmidt ED, De Goeij DCE. Phenotypic plasticity of
CRF neurons during stress. Ann NY Acad Sci 1993; 697: 39–52.
247 Tilders FJH, Schmidt ED. Interleukin-1-induced plasticity of
hypothalamic CRH neurons and long-term stress hyperresponsiveness. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998; 840: 65–73.
248 Gold PW, Chrousos GP, Kellner C, Post RM, Roy A, Augerinas P
et al. Psychiatric implications of basic and clinical studies with
corticotropin-releasing factor. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141: 619–
627.
249 Banki CM, Karmacsi L, Bissette G, Nemeroff CB. CSF corticotropin-releasing hormone and somatostatin in major depression:
response to antidepressant treatment and relapse. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1992; 2: 107–113.
250 Blalock JE. The syntax of immune-neuroendocrine communication. Immunol Today 1994; 15: 504–511.
251 Dunn AJ. Interleukin-1 as a stimulator of hormone secretion. Prog
NeuroEndocrinImmunol 1990; 3: 26–34.
252 Herman JP, Cullinan WE, Neurocircuitry of stress: central control
of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci
1997; 20: 78–84.
253 Crnic LS. Behavioral consequences of viral infection. In: Ader R,
Felten DL, Cohen N (eds). Psychoneuroimmunology. Academic
Press: San Diego, 1991, pp 749–770.
254 Lacosta S, Merali Z, Anisman H. Influence of interleukin-1 on
exploratory behaviors, plasma ACTH and cortisol, and central biogenic amines in mice. Psychopharmacology 1998; 137: 351–361.
255 Linthorst ACE, Flachskamm C, Muller-Preuss P, Holsboer F, Reul
JMHM. Effect of bacterial endotoxin and interleukin-1␤ on hippocampal serotonergic neurotransmission, behavioral activity, and
free corticosterone levels: an in vivo microdialysis study. J Neurosci 1995; 15: 2920–2934.
256 Song C, Merali Z, Anisman H. Systemically administered IL-1, IL2 and IL-6 and mild stress influence in vivo monoamine variations
in the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience 1998; 88: 823–836.
257 Kent S, Bluthe RM, Kelley KW, Dantzer R. Sickness behavior as
a new target for drug development. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1992;
13: 24–28.
258 Anisman H, Zaharia MD, Meaney MJ, Merali Z. Proactive hormonal, neurochemical and behavioral effects of early life stimulation; genetic differences. Int J Dev Neurosci 1998; 16: 149–164.
259 Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, Widdowson J, LaPlante P, Caldji
C et al. Early environmental regulation of forebrain glucocorticoid
receptor gene expression: implications for adrenocortical
responses to stress. Dev Neuroscience 1996; 18: 49–72.
260 Akiskal HS, Cassano GB. Dysthymia and the Spectrum of Chronic
Depressions. Guilford Press: New York, 1997.
261
Molecular Psychiatry