Download Are Anxiety and Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms Related to

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders wikipedia , lookup

Anorexia nervosa wikipedia , lookup

Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup

Abnormal psychology wikipedia , lookup

Classification of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup

Pyotr Gannushkin wikipedia , lookup

History of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup

Selective mutism wikipedia , lookup

Panic disorder wikipedia , lookup

Conversion disorder wikipedia , lookup

Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup

Body image wikipedia , lookup

Child psychopathology wikipedia , lookup

Spectrum disorder wikipedia , lookup

Anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Generalized anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Separation anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Are Anxiety and Obsessive-Compulsive
Symptoms Related to Muscle Dysmorphia?
CHRISTOPHER G. CHANDLER
FREDERICK G. GRIEVE
W. PITT DERRYBERRY
PHILLIP O. PEGG
Western Kentucky University
The present study examines how muscle dysmorphia (MD), a clinically significant
preoccupation that one’s body is inadequately muscular, relates to trait anxiety
and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. 97 college-age men completed the MD Inventory, the Drive for Muscularity Scale, the Male Body Attitudes Scale, the Social Physique Anxiety Scale, the trait scale of the Speilberger State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory , and an abbreviated version of the Yale Brown Obsessive-Compulsive
Scale. Bivariate correlation analyses revealed that trait anxiety and obsessivecompulsive symptoms demonstrated strong relationships with both social
physique anxiety and overall MD symptomology. Path analysis indicated that anxiety-related variables accounted for 77 percent of the variance in MD symptoms.
The findings lend support to the assertion that MD should be placed within the obsessive-compulsive spectrum of disorders.
Keywords: muscle dysmorphia, anxiety, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, men
Although not officially listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (American Psychiatric Association, 2000)
(DSM-IV-TR), muscle dysmorphia (MD) is recognized by many researchers as a legitimate psychological disorder. Olivardia (2001) provides thorough diagnostic criteria for MD. First, one must be preoccupied with the idea that his or her body is not
sufficiently lean or muscular. Second, the preoccupation must cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational or other important areas of functioning as demonstrated by (a) giving up participating in important activities in these
areas because of a compulsive need to maintain diet and workout schedules, (b) avoiding situations in which one’s body is exposed to others, (c) exhibiting clinically significant distress in these areas of functioning, (d) continuing exercising, dieting, and
using performance enhancing substances despite negative physical or psychological
Christopher G. Chandler, Frederick G. Grieve, W. Pitt Derryberry, and Phillip O. Pegg, Western Kentucky University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Frederick G. Grieve, Department of
Psychology, 1906 College Heights Boulevard #21030, Bowling Green, KY 42101-1030. Email:
[email protected]
International Journal of Men’s Health, Vol. 8, No. 2, Summer 2009, 143-154.
© 2009 by the Men’s Studies Press, LLC. http://www.mensstudies.com. All rights reserved.
jmh.0802.143/$14.00 • DOI: 10.3149/jmh.0802.143 • Url: http://dx.doi.org/10.3149/jmh.0802.143
143
CHANDLER ET AL.
consequences, and (e) being preoccupied with being too small or inadequately muscular rather than on being overweight.
Grieve (2007) discusses factors that influence the development of MD. These include socio-environmental (media presentation of ideal images and sports participation), emotional (negative affect and body image dissatisfaction), psychological
(idealized body image and low self-esteem), and cognitive (perfectionism) influences
(Grieve). Of most interest to the present study are the emotional influences, especially
negative affect.
Maida and Armstrong (2005) noted positive correlations between symptoms of
MD and variables such as anxiety and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. They examined a model in which obsessive-compulsive symptoms were related to symptoms of
eating disorders, body dysmorphic disorder, and MD. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms were also directly related to symptoms of MD. These findings indicate strong
positive relationships between obsessive-compulsive symptoms, anxiety, symptoms of
body dysmorphic disorder, and symptoms of MD, with the strongest relationship being
between obsessive-compulsive symptoms and symptoms of MD. However, since Maida
and Armstrong measured anxiety using the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis,
1984), which is a screening instrument for psychiatric disorders, replication of these
findings with a more comprehensive evaluation of anxiety is in order. Moreover, Maida
and Armstrong did not directly evaluate symptoms of MD. They used a combination
of measures to suggest symptoms of MD. Thus, replication with a measure designed
to evaluate symptoms of MD is warranted.
Another variable of interest in terms of negative affect is social physique anxiety,
a body-related anxiety that stems from the fear of others evaluating one’s appearance
(Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989). Conceptually, symptoms of MD include concern over
body shape and hiding of the body (Olivardia, 2001). Further, prior research has demonstrated a link between MD and social physique anxiety. Grieve, Jackson, Reece,
Marklin, and Delaney (in press) found that men who reported higher levels of social
physique anxiety also reported higher levels of MD symptoms.
Two other variables—drive for muscularity and body focus—are notable in the
development of MD (Grieve, 2007). A drive for muscularity underlies the behavioral
symptoms of MD in that people with the disorder want to be larger and more muscular than what they actually are (Olivardia, 2001). There is a large body of research that
ties a drive for muscularity to MD (Thompson & Cafri, 2007). A second factor important in the development of MD is body focus. Individuals high in MD exhibit a number of body checking behaviors such as looking at their bodies in mirrors (Olivardia,
2001).
The present study was designed to expand upon the work of Maida and Armstrong,
and evaluate the relationship that anxiety symptoms have with MD. The model proposed for the study includes obsessive-compulsive symptoms, trait anxiety, social
physique anxiety, drive for muscularity and media influences as factors that are related
to MD. The general model under study was taken from Grieve (2007), and was modified to include the anxiety symptoms of interest to the present research.
144
MUSCLE DYSMORPHIA
Levels of anxiety are presumed to be motivational factors that will influence both
drive for muscularity and body focus. If individuals are anxious about their appearance
or display obsessive ideas about appearance, it is likely that they will experience a
higher level of body focus. Because the social ideal for the male body is a hypermesomorphic one (Grieve, Newton, Kelly, Miller, & Kerr, 2005; Parks & Read, 1997; Ridgeway & Tylka, 2005), an acceptable way to decrease body-related anxiety is to develop
a drive for muscularity.
The study was designed to address two limitations noted in the research by Maida
and Armstrong: use of a screening instrument for anxiety and not directly measuring
the symptoms of MD. For the present study, the trait section of the Speilberger StateTrait Anxiety Inventory (Speilberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1983) was used to evaluate anxiety, and the Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (Short, 2005) was used to evaluate
symptoms of MD.
The specific hypotheses under study are:
1. Higher levels of anxiety, measured as obsessive-compulsive symptoms, levels of trait anxiety, and level of social physique anxiety, will be
associated with higher levels of MD symptoms.
2. Higher levels of anxiety will be associated with a higher body focus
and a higher drive for muscularity.
3. Higher levels of body focus and drive for muscularity will be associated with higher levels of MD symptoms.
Methods
Participants
Participants in the study were 97 male volunteers recruited from a mid-sized university in the mid-South. The mean age of the participants was 21.75 years (SD = 4.52);
the average height was 70.86 inches (SD = 2.71); and the mean weight was 184.11
pounds (SD = 44.12). Participants had a mean body mass index of 26.29 (SD = 5.38).
In all, 74 (85.1%) participants were Caucasian, 5 (5.7%) were African American, 4
(4.6%) were Asian, 1 (1.1%) was Hispanic, 1 (1.1%) was Pacific Islander, and 2 (2.3%)
were bi-racial.
Thirty-two participants (36.8%) acknowledged using some form of supplement
while working out. Creatine, protein shakes, and vitamins were the supplements mentioned most often. Forty-four (50.6%) reported having an active gym membership; 64
(73.6%) said they participated in cardiovascular exercise; 6 (6.9%) said they used topical analgesics before working out and 12 (13.8%) reported using them afterwards; and
12 (13.8%) participants said they used pain medication before a workout while 21
(24.1%) reported using them afterwards.
145
CHANDLER ET AL.
Measures
Demographics. Participants completed a 22-item demographics survey assessing
age, weight, height, and ethnicity. They were also asked about active gym membership, frequency of working out, and use of supplements to gain body mass.
Anxiety. The Speilberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Speilberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1983) was used to assess for state and trait-based levels of anxiety.
The STAI is a 40-item self report measure that assesses both how a person feels at the
moment (state anxiety) and how he or she generally feels (trait anxiety). The STAI includes such statements as “I feel calm” and “I make decisions easily.” These items are
rated on a four-point Likert-type scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much so). Because
the focus of this study is on the overall personality and long-term characteristics of the
participants, only the scores on trait anxiety were used in the analysis. Higher scores
indicate higher levels of trait anxiety. Sansoni, Vellone, and Piras (2004) report a Cronbach’s Alpha of .95 for the trait version of the STAI. In the present study, the Cronbach’s
alpha for the STAI was .95.
The Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS; Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989) was administered to evaluate body anxiety. The SPAS consists of 12 items that participants rate
on a five-point Likert-type scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Higher scores on
the SPAS indicate higher levels of social physique anxiety. Hart et al. (1989) found
that the SPAS demonstrates good internal consistency (r = .90) and test-retest reliability (r = .82). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .92.
An abbreviated version of the Yale Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (YBOCS;
Goodman, Price, Rasmussen, & Mazure, 1989) was administered to assess symptoms
of obsessive-compulsive disorder. This version of the YBOCS consists of 10 self-report
items on which participants rank, on a scale of 0 to 4, how much control they perceive
themselves as having over their obsessions or compulsions. Higher scores indicate
higher levels of obsessive-compulsive features. Frost, Steketee, Krause, & Trepanier
(1995) reported Cronbach’s alphas for the total score as .88. The Cronbach’s alpha for
the total score present study was .92.
Muscle Dysmorphia. To assess for symptoms of MD, the Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (MDI; Short, 2005) was used. The MDI is a 25-item questionnaire that assesses eight components of MD: Inadequacy, Preoccupation, Muscularity Drive,
Increased Muscularity, Compulsivity, Body Anxiety, Social Sacrifice, and Persistence.
Included in the MDI are such items as “I feel badly when I do not get to work out.” Participants rank these items on a six-point Likert-type scale, from 1 (strongly disagree)
to 6 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicate a greater endorsement of MD symptomatology. In previous research, the MDI has an overall Cronbach’s alpha of .87 (Short,
2005). For the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was .82.
146
MUSCLE DYSMORPHIA
Drive for Muscularity. To assess a participant’s drive for muscularity, the Drive for
Muscularity Scale (DMS; McCreary & Sasse, 2004) was administered. The DMS is a
15-item questionnaire with statements such as “I wish I were more muscular” and
“Other people think I work out with weights too much.” Participants rank these items
on a six-point Likert-type scale, from 1 (always) to 6 (never). The DMS is reverse
scored so that high scores indicate higher levels of drive for muscularity (McCreary,
2007). McCreary and Sasse (2004) found that the DMS had a Cronbach’s alpha of .87
among male participants. For the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was .92.
Body Focus. To assess attitude about body image, participants were given the Male
Body Attitudes Scale (MBAS; Tylka, Bergeron, & Schwartz, 2005). This 29-item measure allows participants to answer on a seven-point Likert-type scale from 0 (never) to
6 (always). Lower scores indicate higher levels of body focus and negative body opinion. The MBAS assesses body focus on four dimensions, including Muscularity, Body
Fat, Height, and Overall Body. The MBAS has adequate total scale reliability (r = .88)
for test-retest procedures (Tylka et al., 2005). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was
.92.
Descriptive statistics for all measures are presented in Table 1.
Participants completed the demographics survey followed by the MDI, DMS,
MBAS, SPAS, YBOCS, and STAI. The entire process took about 20 minutes for each
participant.
Results
Before addressing the conducted analyses, it is important to note that there were
ten participants who had missing data. This data was replaced using mean substitution,
and the participants were included in the analyses.
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for the Measures
Measure
YBOCS
DMS
MBAS
MDI
SPAS
STAI
Sample Size
(N)
95
97
96
93
97
94
Mean
SD
Range of Scores
7.76
39.70
97.28
66.26
16.75
72.68
6.82
15.21
21.43
19.61
6.39
22.36
0.00-27.00
16.00-75.00
50.00-141.00
27.00-126.00
7.00-35.00
40.00-139.00
Note: YBOCS = Yale Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale; DMS = Drive for Muscularity Scale,;
MBAS = Male Body Attitudes Scale; MDI = MD Inventory; SPAS = Social Physique Anxiety
Scale; STAI = Spielberg State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
147
CHANDLER ET AL.
Path analysis via a series of regressions (Pedhazur, 1997) was used to examine relationships in the specified recursive model. As illustrated in Figure 1, considered predictors of MD comprise three exogenous variables, including Social Physique Anxiety,
Obsessive Compulsive Features, and Trait Anxiety, and two endogenous variables, including Drive for Muscularity and Body Attitude. The path analysis is the culmination
of 12 regressions. Specifically, two regressions were conducted assessing the contribution from each exogenous variable to each endogenous variable. Then one regression
was conducted from each exogenous variable to MD, and one regression was conducted from each endogenous variable to MD while accounting for the contributions
from the upstream exogenous variables. Path coefficients specified in Figure 2 are
therefore the standardized beta weights from each regression. The model also accounts
for correlations between Social Physique Anxiety and Trait Anxiety, Social Physique
Anxiety and Obsessive Compulsive Features, and Trait Anxiety and Obsessive Compulsive Features.
Social Physique
Anxiety
(SPAS)
Trait Anxiety
(STAXI)
Obsessive
Compulsive
Features
(YBOCS)
Drive for
Muscularity
(DMS)
Body Attitude
(self)
(MBAS)
MD
(MDI)
Figure 1. A model for MD symptoms that incorporates trait anxiety and obsessive compulsive
features.
Figure 2 illustrates the path coefficients from the exogenous and endogenous variables included in the model and also the correlations among the exogenous variables.
Most of the specified pathways had statistically significant relationships. There were
only two pathways that did not achieve statistical significance. These were from trait
anxiety to drive for muscularity and from obsessive-compulsive features to drive for
muscularity. Overall, the total model R2 denotes that the predictors accounted for 73 percent of the variance in the symptoms of MD. Given this large R2, it is apparent that the
model possess acceptable goodness of fit.
148
MUSCLE DYSMORPHIA
Social Physique
Anxiety
(SPAS)
.55***
Trait Anxiety
(STAXI)
Obsessive
Compulsive
Features
(YBOCS)
.48***
-.74***
.48***
.19
-.45***
.24*
.18
Drive for
Muscularity
(DMS)
-.35***
.45***
.76***
.30***
Body Attitude
(self)
(MBAS)
.50***
-.25**
MD
(MDI)
* = p > .05
** = p > .01
*** = p > .001
R2 = .73
Figure 2. Beta weights for the relationships among anxiety, drive for muscularity, body focus and
MD symptoms. Note: Path coefficients reflect standardized beta weights from a series of conducted regressions.
Discussion
The results of the study support all of the hypotheses under study. There were several strong relationships between trait anxiety, social physique anxiety, obsessive-compulsive features, and MD symptoms. Trait anxiety was found to be highly predictive of
social physique anxiety. A similar pattern of association was reported in women with
disturbed eating patterns (Bas, Asci, Karabudak, & Kiziltan, 2004). Trait anxiety was
also significantly negatively associated with body attitude; that is, a higher level of trait
anxiety was a reliable predictor of low body attitude. This finding is consistent with past
research completed with women who have eating disorders.
Kashima, Tatsuhisa, Okamoto, Nogoshi, Wada, Tadai, et al. (2003) found that
higher scores on the Body Attitude Test (BAT) correlated with higher scores on both the
STAI and Self-Rating Depression Scale, suggesting that those who suffer from eating
disorders also experience negative appreciation of body size, unfamiliarity with their
bodies, and general body dissatisfaction, and, therefore, have high trait anxiety. Finally,
trait anxiety also had a strong, predictive relationship with overall MD symptoms, as
149
CHANDLER ET AL.
illustrated by the relationship between the STAI and MDI. The results suggest that trait
anxiety integrates well into existing models of MD. Because MD involves high levels
of exercise to obtain and maintain a certain body shape, the findings also support research indicating that people who exercise excessively have higher levels of obsessivecompulsive symptoms (Gulker, Laskis, & Kuba, 2001).
Similar to trait anxiety, obsessive-compulsive features are strongly related to and
were predictive of several factors of MD. In the present study, a strong predictive relationship was found between obsessive-compulsive features and body focus. This relationship could indicate unhealthy obsessions in each of these three areas for men who
have high levels of MD symptoms. Individuals may, for example, be able to find relief
from their intrusive, obsessive thoughts about their bodies by working out excessively
or consuming a high protein diet or supplements.
Obsessive-compulsive features were also strongly correlated with and highly predictive of social physique anxiety and overall MD symptoms. This finding suggests
that a man’s underlying obsession with his own body magnifies the concerns he has regarding evaluation. This could also be a sign of underlying obsessive-compulsive features. Body focus could, therefore, be the primary focus of obsessional thinking or
compulsive behaviors.
Obsessive-compulsive features and overall MD symptoms were also strongly related in this study. It could be that a predisposition towards obsessive thinking and
compulsive behaviors, when paired with other factors linked to MD, lead to the expression of those obsessions and compulsions through bodily preoccupation. For example, an individual who suffers from obsessive thinking and is exposed to media
images of muscular men may manifest his or her compulsions by working out in an effort to obtain an idealized body shape.
Social physique anxiety was also strongly positively correlated with MD symptoms. These findings support past research of Grieve et al. (in press), who found that
men with higher levels of social physique anxiety also had higher levels of MD symptoms. Conceptually, a relationship between social physique anxiety and MD is understandable. Even though they have very muscular body shapes, people with MD are
worried about how their bodies look and do not believe they measure up to their standards. This discrepancy can lead to the development of social physique anxiety.
Taken together, the findings support and extend the findings Maida and Armstrong
(2005). Similar relationships were noted for both studies between obsessive-compulsive features, levels of anxiety, and drive for muscularity. The present study also found
a relationship between anxiety, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, social physique anxiety, and symptoms of Muscle Dysmorphia.
In general, the findings of the present study lend support to Maida and Armstrong’s
(2005) assertion that MD should be placed within the obsessive-compulsive spectrum
of disorders. There is presently a lack of consensus about the classification of MD. In
addition to being classified in the obsessive-compulsive spectrum of disorders, it has
also been classified as a Body Dysmorphic Disorder (Pope, Katz, & Hudson, 1993), as
an Eating Disorder (Goodale, Watkins, & Cardinal, 2001), and as part of the Obsessive150
MUSCLE DYSMORPHIA
Compulsive spectrum of disorders (Maida & Armstrong, 2005). That obsessive-compulsive features were found to be strong predictors for MD symptomatology, body attitude, and social physique anxiety lends support to Maida and Armstrong’s placement
of MD under the obsessive-compulsive spectrum.
The classification of MD is not simply a philosophical debate. Correct classification has important implications for diagnosis and, especially, fpr treatment. Placing the
disorder within an appropriate spectrum allows clinicians to devise appropriate treatments for the disorder. Inappropriate placement may lead to the development of potentially ineffective treatments and possible client endangerment. On the treatment of
MD, see Olivardia (2007).
Interestingly, this study found a relationship between aspects of negative affect
and MD, whereas previous research (Henson, 2004; Jonda, 2007) did not. The current
study found both trait anxiety and obsessive-compulsive features to be predictive of MD
symptomatology. Jonda’s model of MD symptomatology noted a weak standardized
beta-weight (.101) between negative affect and MD symptomatology. This difference
may be the result of how each study measured negative mood states. Whereas the current study used specific assessment tools to assess trait anxiety and obsessive-compulsive features, the study by Jonda used the Positive and Negative Affect Scales (PANAS;
Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), an instrument designed to measure normal mood
fluctuations. Thus, it may be that negative emotional states need to be of clinical significance before they substantively influence MD symptoms.
Because the current study found that both obsessive-compulsive features and trait
anxiety were predictive of MD, future research may wish explore how other psychological mood states relate to MD symptomatology. Future research could expand the
current study’s findings by incorporating, for example, clinical measures of depression
and anger.
The present study also has some limitations worth addressing. The most obvious
improvement would be a larger sample size. While the overall reliability and the overall goodness of fit of the model specified in the model of the study was strong, more
participants would improve statistical power and give opportunity to find greater numbers of participants with clinical levels of MD. This study also lacked a measure for
media influence. As the existing literature has shown how important media influence
is in people with MD, future studies should incorporate such a measure.
Another possibility to consider would be that MD may not fit into any existing
spectrum of disorders. While the literature attempts to classify MD under a particular
heading, it may also be possible that MD presents us with a unique symptomatology
and etiology that warrants its own classification in a manner analogous to eating disorders.
Perhaps the most effective way to treat patients with MD is to treat it as its own
disorder rather than trying to classify it within the body of existing disorders. In fact,
it may be fruitful to consider the diagnostic history of eating disorders which only attained the status of a separate diagnostic category in the DSM-IV (Williamson, Zucker,
Martin, & Smeets, 2004). Such diagnostic clarification has led both to a greater speci151
CHANDLER ET AL.
ficity of the subtypes within the eating disorders (the binging-eating/purging and the restricting subtypes of anorexia nervosa) and to the genesis of diagnosis specific treatments of choice (interpersonal or cognitive-behavioral therapies for anorexia nervosa).
So, too, we can imagine the improved ability to identify at-risk individuals, render accurate diagnoses, and advance the treatment of individuals with MD, all as a consequence of finding a diagnostically apt placement for the disorder outside of existing
groupings.
Finally, one limitation of the study is that it evaluates symptoms of anxiety and MD
within a normal college population. Participants who met diagnostic criteria for MD
were not recruited for participation; therefore, the findings may only be generalizable
to sub-clinical populations. However, the range of scores on the MDI in this study (27
to 126) indicates that there were some participants who reported high levels of MD
symptoms. Further, college-age men are at a higher risk for developing MD than other
age groups of men or of women. While women can meet diagnostic criteria for MD,
the disorder is much more prevalent in men than women. Therefore, college-age men
are the population to which these findings should be generalized.
In conclusion, while this study does not make any definitive statement regarding
the classification of MD, it does reveal that anxiety and obsessive-compulsive symptoms are strongly related to high levels of MD symptoms, lending support to Maida and
Armstrong’s (2005) assertion that MD should be classified under the obsessive-compulsive spectrum of disorders. This study brings researchers one step closer to being
able to classify MD appropriately based upon its etiology and psychological comorbidity.
References
American Psychological Association (APA). (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision. Washington, DC: APA.
Baird, A., & Grieve, F. G. (2006). Exposure to male models in advertisements leads to a decrease
in men’s body satisfaction. North American Journal of Psychology, 1, 115-122.
Bas, M., Asci, F. H., Karabudak, E., & Kiziltan, G. (2004). Eating attitudes and their psychological correlates among Turkish adolescents. Adolescence, 39, 593-599.
Bienvenu, O. J., Samuels, J. F., Jack, F., & Riddle, M. A. (2000). The relationship of obsessivecompulsive disorder to possible spectrum disorders: Results from a family study. Biology
Psychiatry, 48, 287-293.
Derogatis, L. (1984). Brief symptom inventory. Minneapolis, MN: NCS, Inc.
Godart, N. T., Flament, M. F., Perdereau, F., & Jeammet, P. (2002). Comorbidity between eating
disorders and anxiety disorders: A review. Eating Disorders, 32, 253-270.
Goodale, K. R., Watkins, P., & Cardinal, B. J. (2001). Muscle dysmorphia: A new form of eating disorder? American Journal of Health Education, 32, 260-266.
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., & Rasmussen, S. A. (1989). The Yale-Brown obsessive-compulsive scale. I: Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 10061011.
152
MUSCLE DYSMORPHIA
Grieve, F. G. (2007). A conceptual model of factors contributing to the development of muscle
dysmorphia. Eating Disorders, 15, 63-80.
Grieve, F. G., Jackson, L., Reece, T., Marklin, L., & Delaney, A. (in press). Correlates of social
physique anxiety in men. Journal of Sport Behavior.
Grieve, F. G., Newton, C. C., Kelley, L., Miller, R. C. Jr., & Kerr, N. A. (2005). The preferred
male body shapes of college men and women. Individual Differences Research, 3, 188-192.
Hart, E. A., Leary, M. R., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). The measure of social physique anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 94-104.
Henson, C. (2004). Potential antecedents of muscle dysmorphia. Unpublished MA thesis, Western Kentucky Univeristy, Bowling Green, KY.
Jonda, J. (2007). Similarities between etiological models of eating disorder symptomatology and
muscle dysmorphia symptomatology. Unpublished MA thesis, Western Kentucky University,
Bowling Green, KY.
Kashima, A., Yamashita, T., Okamoto, A., Nagoshi, Y., Wada, Y., Tadai, T., et al. (2003). Japanese version of the body attitudes test: Its reliability and validity. Psychiatry and Neurosciences,
57, 511-515.
Leit, R. A., Gray, J. J., & Pope, H. G. (2002). The media’s representation of the ideal male body:
A cause for muscle dysmorphia? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31(3), 334-338.
Lorenzen, L. A., Grieve, F. G., & Thomas, A. (2004). Exposure to muscular male models decreases men’s body satisfaction. Sex Roles, 51, 743-748.
Maida, D. M., & Armstrong, S. L. (2005). The classification of muscle dysmorphia. International Journal of Men’s Health, 4, 73-91.
McCreary, D. R. (2007). The Drive for Muscularity Scale: Description, psychometrics, and research findings. In J. K. Thompson & G. Cafri (Eds.), The muscular ideal: Psychological, social, and medical perspectives (pp. 87-106). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration of the drive for muscularity in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health, 48, 297-304.
Olivardia, R. (2001). Mirror, mirror on the wall, who’s the largest of them all? The features and
phenomenology of muscle dysmorphia. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 9, 254-259.
Olivardia, R. (2007). Muscle dysmorphia: Characteristics, assessment, and treatment. In J. K.
Thompson & G. Cafri (Eds.), The muscular ideal: Psychological, social, and medical perspectives (pp. 123-140). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Olivardia, R., Pope, H. G., & Hudson, J. I. (2000). Muscle dysmorphia in male weightlifters: A
case-control study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1291-1296.
Parks P. S. M., & Read, M. H. (1997). Adolescent male athletes: Body image, diet, and exercise.
Adolescence, 32, 593-603.
Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research (3rd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.
Pope, H. G. Jr., Katz, D. L., & Hudson, J. I. (1993). Anorexia nervosa and “reverse anorexia”
among 108 male bodybuilders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 406-409.
Ridgeway, R. T., & Tylka, T. L. (2005). College men’s perceptions of ideal body composition and
shape. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 6, 209-220.
Sansoni, J., Vellone, E., & Piras, G. (2004). Anxiety and depression in community dwelling Italian Alzheimer’s disease caregivers. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 10, 93-100.
Short, J. L. (2005). Creating an assessment tool for muscle dysmorphia. Unpublished MA thesis, Western Kentucky University, Bowling Green, KY.
153
CHANDLER ET AL.
Speilberger, C. D., Gorsuch, D. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1983). State-trait anxiety inventory. San
Francisco, CA: Consulting Psychologists’ Press.
Thompson, J. K., & Cafri, G. (2007). The muscular ideal: Psychological, social, and medical perspectives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Tylka, T. L., Bergeron, D., & Schwartz, J. P. (2005). Development and psychometric evaluation
of the male body attitudes scale (MBAS). Body Image, 2, 161-175.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures
of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.
Williamson, D. A., Zucker, N. L., Martin, C. K., & Smeets, M. A. M. (2004). Etiology and management of eating disorders. In H. E. Adams & P. B. Sutker (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology, 3rd ed. (pp. 641-670). New York: Springer.
154