* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Decision Making for Food Choice by Grasshoppers
Occupancy–abundance relationship wikipedia , lookup
Ficus rubiginosa wikipedia , lookup
Biodiversity action plan wikipedia , lookup
Theoretical ecology wikipedia , lookup
Latitudinal gradients in species diversity wikipedia , lookup
Island restoration wikipedia , lookup
Introduced species wikipedia , lookup
PLANT-INSECT INTERACTIONS Decision Making for Food Choice by Grasshoppers (Orthoptera: Acrididae): Comparison Between a Specialist Species on a Shrubby Legume and Three Graminivorous Species F. PICAUD,1 E. BONNET,1 V. GLOAGUEN,2 AND D. PETIT Faculté des Sciences, E.A. 3176, 123, av. A. Thomas, 87060 Limoges, France Environ. Entomol. 32(3): 680Ð688 (2003) ABSTRACT Dwarf gorse bush (Ulex minor) heathlands in Limousin, France, are ecological islands often separated by tens of kilometers of grasslands and hedges, where several species of grasshoppers belonging to the genus Chorthippus (Acrididae: Gomphocerinae) coexist. Chorthippus binotatus (Charpentier) feeds only on Ulex minor; nymphs feed exclusively on leaves whereas adults become ßorivorous at the end of the season. The other species studied (C. biguttulus (L.), C. albomarginatus (De Geer), and C. parallelus (Zetterstedt)) are all graminivorous. The importance of sugars, nitrogen content, sparteine (a quinolizidine alkaloid), and plant architecture in food selection was investigated. Chorthippus binotatus is sensitive to sucrose and fructose, consistent with the high sugar content of Ulex minor ßowers. Experiments with grass coated with sparteine showed that this molecule associated with sucrose is a phagostimulant for this grasshopper. Different behavioral responses of graminivorous species are observed with sparteine alone, but never phagostimulation. We compared the response times corresponding to decision-making between the different species toward several components involved in food selection. The food choice toward host plant and sugars is as quick for C. binotatus as for the two graminivorous species (C. parallelus and C. albomarginatus), whereas C. biguttulus is slower and exhibits atypical reactions. Chorthippus binotatus can feed on Poaceae, but with more time spent, leading to an increasing predation risk. This situation is a limitation toward dispersal between different heathlands (patchy habitats). KEY WORDS decision making, patchy habitats, alkaloids, Chorthippus, diet shift, graminivory CHAPMAN (1982) NOTED that in the majority of phytophagous insect orders, ⬎50% of the species are oligophagous (they feed on one plant family only) or monophagous. The Orthoptera, and particularly Acridoidea, stand apart from the other orders: 60% of grasshoppers have been classiÞed as polyphagous, and a further 25% are graminivorous. In North America, at least two Gomphocerinae species have evolved specialized diets from graminivory under conditions in which grasses were often very sparse (Otte and Joern 1977). Ligurotettix coquilletti McNeill feeds on Larrea (Zygophyllaceae), Atriplex (Chenopodiaceae), and Lycium (Solanaceae) (Chapman and Joern 1990). Bootettix argentatus Bruner lives and feeds on the creosote bush Larrea tridentata (Zygophyllaceae) (White and Chapman 1990). The basis for this preference depends partly on the presence of the lignan nordihydroguaiaretic acid (NDGA) (Chapman et al. 1988). In France, the genus Chorthippus (Gomphocerinae) comprises 16 species (Bellmann and Lucquet 1993). The diet of all species was characterized by 1 2 Société Entomologique du Limousin, Limoges, France. Laboratoire CSN, Faculté des Sciences de Limoges, France. Gangwere and Morales-Agacino (1973). All species are graminivorous with the exception of Chorthippus binotatus (Charpentier) (Picaud et al. 1999). This species is present in France, Spain, and Morocco and comprises six subspecies, Þve of which occur in France (Defaut 1999). Chorthippus binotatus binotatus feeds almost exclusively on various species belonging to the Genisteae tribe (Fabaceae) (Grassé 1929), with some regional specialization: Ulex minor Roth in Périgord and Limousin, Ulex europaeus L. and Cytisus scoparius (L.) Link in Charente-Maritime, Genista scorpius (L.) DC. In the French Pyrenees (Defaut 1999), and Ulex parviflorus Pourret in Vaucluse (Mossot and Petit 2000). In Limousin, the dietary preference of C. binotatus binotatus during the season is synchronized with the life history of Ulex minor: the grasshopper feeds shoots before blooming and ßowers during the summer (Picaud et al. 1999, 2002). In the laboratory, the entire life cycle can be completed with Ulex minor as the only food supply. It is certain that C. binotatus evolved from graminivorous species: the 15 other species in the genus Chorthippus feed on Poaceae, as well as the French species of the sister genera Omocestus and Stenobothrus (Bonnet 1996). It is well known that grass-feeding 0046-225X/03/0680Ð0688$04.00/0 䉷 2003 Entomological Society of America June 2003 PICAUD ET AL.: DECISION MAKING FOR FOOD CHOICE BY GRASSHOPPERS 681 Fig. 1. Distribution map of Chorthippus binotatus subspecies binotatus in France (hatched parts) and in Limousin Region (open circles), divided in three departments (after Kruseman, 1982; Defaut, 1999). Plant samples for analyses were collected in Ceinturat heathland (black circle). grasshoppers feed mainly on the most abundant Poaceae species (Bernays and Chapman 1994). Because the chemical composition of secondary metabolites of Graminae are rather uniform (Bruneton 1999), one can assume that grass-feeding grasshoppers are oligophagous and therefore relatively specialist species. In Limousin, the heathlands correspond to ecological islands (Brugel et al. 2001). Thus, for species using food resources that are limited and patchy, the distance between these bush formations can be a constraint for dispersal. A less obvious constraint that may be associated with oligophagy is the problem of food quality, including nitrogen and/or carbohydrates levels, and the balance between deterrents and phagostimulants. Bernays and Bright (2001) showed that food choice of Schistocerca americana (Drury) between different combinations of two plant species causes interrupted feeding as a result of inefÞcient decision making. So, feeding on only one plant species saves time and potentially beneÞts grasshoppers. One can ask several questions about the properties linked to feeding upon a given plant: (1) in the case of binary choice (Ulex minor versus Poaceae), do the insects feed exclusively on their host plants; (2) what are the characteristics of Ulex minor involved in the diet preference of C. binotatus; and (3) is the specialist species on Genisteae bushes as efÞcient as each graminivorous species in their food selection? In this work, we examined the feeding behavior and the food selection of all the Chorthippus species coexisting with Chorthippus binotatus in Limousin heathlands: C. biguttulus (L.) (subgenus Glyptobothrus, as C. binotatus), C. parallelus (Zetterstedt), and C. albomarginatus (De Geer) (subgenus Chorthippus). It is known that the three latter species are graminivorous (Bonnet 1996). We investigated food quality as the basis of food selection in grass feeding and Ulex minor specialist grasshoppers. We analyzed sugars and nitrogen content in the plant species with a special focus on Ulex minor and Poaceae. As for secondary compounds, it is well known that numerous quinolizidine alkaloids characterize the tribe of Genisteae (Bruneton 1999). Our investigation was limited to sparteine, the major alkaloid in the genus Ulex (Picaud et al., unpublished data). The inßuence of plant architecture was also tested: ramiÞed in the case of Ulex, straight in the case of Poaceae. We discuss the Þndings in relation to the beneÞt of quick decision times (Bernays and Bright 2001, Abrams and Schmitz 1999) and the disadvantage of being a specialist on Ulex minor, as the Ulex minor heathlands form islands between grasslands and pastures. Materials and Methods Experimental Animals. Chorthippus adults (C. binotatus, C. biguttulus, C. albomarginatus, and C. parallelus) were collected in September and October 1993Ð 2001 in heathlands in the Limousin region (45Ð 46 degrees N. latitude, 1Ð2 degrees E. longitude), France (Fig. 1). If we take into account the mean of absolute plant cover in seven heathlands where C. binotatus was present, the most abundant plant species are: Ulex minor (48%), Calluna vulgaris (34%), Molinia caerulea (23%), Erica cinerea (17%), Pteris aquilinum (7%), and Erica tetralix (4%). Insects were placed individually in glass jars (6 cm diameter ⫻ 10 cm height) and kept at room temperature (20 ⫾ 2⬚C), under a L:D 12:12 h photoregime and 50% humidity for the duration of the experiments. They were fed daily with fresh plants collected at the Limoges University: Ulex minor for C. binotatus and a mix of Agrostis tenuis, Holcus lanatus, and Lolium perenne for the graminivorous species. 682 ENVIRONMENTAL ENTOMOLOGY Every week after the experiments, all living insects were returned to the wild and replaced by newly collected specimens. Tested Plants and Food Selection Experiments. We chose Ulex minor (ßowering time in JulyÐOctober) and the most abundant grass in heathlands (Molinia caerulea) for testing. Another grass species, Lolium perenne, was also tested because of its abundance at the Limoges University. Before each experiment, food was withheld from insects for 4 Ð 6 h (Bernays and Chapman 1970). Food choice tests with 16 Ð32 individual adults of both sexes of each species were performed in a Petri dish (14.2 cm diameter ⫻ 2 cm height) isolated in a green enclosure. A 40 W-lamp above the center of the dish provided constant light and minimized temperature ßuctuations. Each experiment was limited to 15 min to avoid postingestive feed-backs. Thus, decisions by grasshoppers depended on receptor sensitivity and the physiological state of individuals. The grasshopper was transferred to the center of a dish in a small glass jar. It was given a choice between two separate sources, each consisting of fresh plants (⬇1 g), cut with scissors just before the start of the bioassay. Three different behavioral responses were recorded (Williams 1954, Bernays and Chapman 1970, Uvarov 1977): orientation (the grasshopper walked to the plant (item 1), palpated the plant (item 2), and ate the plant (item 3). To help understand food choice, we examined sugars, architecture, and the secondary metabolite sparteine. During plant choice experiments, the sum of orientation and palpation times from initiation of movement until the decision (feeding upon or rejecting it) was documented. Concerning the experiments with sugars and the alkaloid, non - volatile chemical compounds, only palpation time before decision was taken into account. Chemical Analysis of Plants (soluble carbohydrates and nitrogen content). The samples of Ulex minor ßowers, shoots, and stems were collected from Ceinturat heathland (Fig. 1), Þve in September 1997 and four in July 1999 at the beginning of blooming for a series of analyses. Four samples of two species of Graminae were collected in grasslands of the Limoges University in early July 1999. The separation and identiÞcation of soluble sugars (glucose, fructose and sucrose) was adapted from Harisson et al. (1997) and Cataldi et al. (1999) and performed by high performance liquid chromatography (HPAEC-PAD). Elution of the samples was carried out in isocratic conditions by using 0.16 M aqueous NaOH at a ßow rate of 1 ml ⫻ min⫺1. Calculations of mono- and disaccharide concentrations were made from dry weight extraction residues, and then expressed relative to initial sample dry weight. The measurement precision was tested by duplicating analyses of the samples. The nitrogen content was evaluated by Kjehldahl analysis (AOAC 1984). Sugar Sensitivity. The experimental apparatus was the same as that described above, with the exception that the length of the test was reduced to 5 min to Vol. 32, no. 3 Fig. 2. Drawings presented to insects in plant architecture experiments. avoid consumption of the piece of Þlter paper. The choice consisted of two pieces of Þlter paper (each one measuring 6 ⫻ 6 cm, and four folds), placed at random on opposite edges of the dish, one with one M sugar solution (1 ml), and one with distilled water (20 Ð21 individuals for each species). Decomposition Analysis of Shape of the Plant. Individual grasshoppers were placed for 10 min in a Y-maze with opaque walls (30 ⫻ 8 ⫻ 11 cm) covered with Plexiglas. The grasshoppers had to choose between two drawings (8 ⫻ 11 cm) placed vertically and at random ends of the two branches (18 ⫻ 8 ⫻ 11 cm). Choice between the drawings was recorded when the insect reached the end of the Y-maze and climbed on the drawing. The time from initiation of movement until climbing onto the drawing was recorded. Two pairs of green patterns on a yellow background, to mimic Ulex branches in the environment, were presented: each individual (19 Ð21 by species) had to choose between straight versus branching aspects and straight versus crossed aspects in separate bioassays (Fig. 2). Alkaloid Sensitivity. Preliminary experiments demonstrated (data not shown) that C. binotatus and C. biguttulus have no preference or rejection behavior for 1% acetic acid versus water, on Þlter paper. Alkaloid content in Leguminosae vary generally between 0.1 and 3% of plant dry weight (Bruneton 1999). Our analyses of different organs of Ulex minor (Picaud et al., unpublished data) have shown that sparteine represents between 30 Ð 48% of nonlinked alkaloids. Different concentrations (in 1% acetic acid) of sparteine 99% (Sigma Aldrich, Saint-Quentin, France) were tested under the same conditions as described in “sugar sensitivity” trials, with a 1% acetic acid treatment as a control. Experiments were repeated three times with different sets of 16 Ð24 individuals of C. binotatus, tested separately with 0.1%, 0.5%, 1% and 2% of sparteine. The principal behavioral response recorded was the arrestment on the Þlter paper. InterspeciÞc comparisons of behavioral responses toward sparteine were undertaken with a solution of 1% sparteine in 1% acetic acid on Þlter paper in the center of a glass container (5 cm diameter at base, 7 cm high), with a 1% acetic acid treatment as a control. For each species, we observed 20 Ð24 separated individuals. For each individual, we recorded 10 items during a 5-min continuous observation. The behaviors were: palpation in the air (Pair); palpation of Þlter paper (Palp); arrestment on the Þlter paper (Arrest); put- June 2003 PICAUD ET AL.: DECISION MAKING FOR FOOD CHOICE BY GRASSHOPPERS 683 Fig. 3. InterspeciÞc comparison of three behaviors of Chorthippus binotatus (bin), C. biguttulus (big), C. albomarginatus (alb), and C. parallelus (par) in binary food plant experiments. (a) Results (mean ⫾ SEM of differences) from Wilcoxon test on behaviors (orientation: 1, palpation: 2, consumption: 3) for plant choice. Kruskal-Wallis tests of all combined data are noted on each Þgures. Different letters correspond to signiÞcant differences between species (MannÐWhitney U tests, P ⬍ 0.05). (b) Mean reaction times ⫾ SEM (seconds, Log10-transformed data) to move and palpate the plant. ANOVA of the combined data of all species concerning reaction time for ßowered Ulex minor: F3,27 ⫽ 3.62, P ⫽ 0.026. ANOVA for Lolium perenne: F3,30 ⫽ 17.16, P ⬍ 0.001. Different letters correspond to signiÞcant differences between species (TukeyÕs test, P ⬍ 0.001). ting the fore legs on the Þlter paper and touching the mouth parts (Leg); putting the labrum on the Þlter paper (Lab); biting and masticating (Bmas); leg withdrawing above the Þlter paper (Wleg); regurgitation (Reg); leaving Þlter paper (Leave); jumping resulting from Þlter paper contact (Jump). We tried to enhance grass consumption by C. binotatus in adding some characteristic molecules present in Ulex minor: we studied its behavior with Lolium perenne covered, with a brush, with different combinations of solution diluted in 1% acetic acid: sucrose one M, sparteine 1%. Lolium perenne covered with 1% acetic acid were used as control. We recorded the time spent palpitating before decision and the frequency of individuals (16 Ð22 individuals for each experiment) consuming Lolium perenne. Statistical Tests. The individual behaviors for each choice were analyzed with Wilcoxon tests. Differences given by these tests were used to perform Kruskal-Wallis tests for interspeciÞc comparisons using SYSTAT ver. 7.0 (SPSS 1997). The reaction times were compared using a multiway analysis of variance (ANOVA) because all of the data were normally distributed. Log-transformed palpation times were compared using multi-way ANOVA for interspeciÞc comparisons. In each case, TukeyÕs tests were performed for pair-wise comparisons. Frequencies of different items with sparteine trials and coated grass experiments were compared with a MannÐWhitney U test. Results Behavioral Comparison among Species. Chorthippus binotatus and C. biguttulus preferred Ulex minor ßowers (Fig. 3a) to Lolium perenne (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 18.166, N ⫽ 88, P ⬍ 0.001) and Molinia caerulea (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 16.30, N ⫽ 79, P ⫽ 0.001). When the Ulex minor branches have not ßowered yet, the graminivorous species (C. biguttulus, C. albomarginatus, and C. parallelus) preferred Graminae (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3⬍10.96, 74⬍N⬍79, P ⬍ 0.015 with both binary plants choice). The shortest time (Fig. 3b) between the beginning of the experiment and the palpation of Ulex minor in the case of binary choices was observed for C. binotatus (ANOVA: F3,27 ⫽ 3.62, P ⫽ 0.026). C. albomarginatus and C. parallelus show the quickest reaction time to Lolium perenne (ANOVA: F3,30 ⫽ 17.16, P ⬍ 0.001). 684 ENVIRONMENTAL ENTOMOLOGY Vol. 32, no. 3 Fig. 4. InterspeciÞc comparison of three behavioral items of Chorthippus binotatus (bin), C. biguttulus (big), C. albomarginatus (alb), and C. parallelus (par) in an experiment testing the response to different sugars versus water. (a) Mean ⫾ SEM of differences from Wilcoxon test on behaviors (orientation: 1, palpation: 2, consumption: 3). Kruskal-Wallis test of all combined data are noted on each Þgures. Different letters correspond to signiÞcant differences between species (MannÐ Whitney U tests, P ⬍ 0.05). (b) Mean palpation time ⫾ SEM (seconds, log-transformed data) of soluble carbohydrate solution (1M). ANOVA of the combined data of all species for each sugar experiment: see text. Different letters correspond to signiÞcant differences between species (TukeyÕs test: P ⬍ 0.05). The most marked preference for sucrose (Fig. 4a) was observed for C. binotatus (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 10.49, N ⫽ 80, P ⫽ 0.015). This result is corroborated by the fact that C. binotatus showed the quickest palpation time (Fig. 4b) before making a decision in the presence of sucrose (ANOVA: F3,28 ⫽ 3.91, P ⫽ 0.019). Curiously, C. biguttulus responded slowly to sucrose. As for fructose, the most complete series of behaviors was performed by C. binotatus and C. parallelus (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 7.56, N ⫽ 82, P ⬍ 0.056). Unexpectedly, C. biguttulus had the quickest reaction time (Fig. 4b, ANOVA: F3,31 ⫽ 3.53, P ⫽ 0.026) in spite of its low motivation for fructose (Fig. 4a). C. biguttulus is the only species sensitive to glucose (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 10.23, N ⫽ 81, P ⫽ 0.017), but no difference between decision time was recorded (ANOVA: F3,29 ⫽ 0.77, P ⫽ 0.52). Finally, C. binotatus was the only species attracted (Fig. 5) by branching aspects (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 13.41, N ⫽ 82, P ⫽ 0.004). Soluble Carbohydrates and Nitrogen Contents. Flowers of Ulex minor had high levels of sucrose (mean ⫾ SEM ⫽ 18.9 ⫾ 0.7 mg ⫻ g⫺1 of extract residue) in the autumn but not summer (Table 1). Glucose and fructose were both present at very high levels in Ulex minor (⬎58 mg 䡠 g⫺1 of extract residue). Shoots of Ulex minor also contained fructose (mean ⫾ SEM ⫽ 3 ⫾ 1.4 mg 䡠 g⫺1) and sucrose (mean ⫾ SEM ⫽ Fig. 5. InterspeciÞc comparison of orientation behavior of Chorthippus binotatus (bin), C. biguttulus (big), C. albomarginatus (alb), and C. parallelus (par) in a Y-maze. Results (mean ⫾ SEM of the differences) of Wilcoxon test on orientation behaviors an experiment comparing different plant architecture. Kruskal-Wallis test of all combined data are noted on each Þgures. Different letters correspond to signiÞcant differences between species (Mann-Whitney U tests, P ⬍ 0.05). June 2003 PICAUD ET AL.: DECISION MAKING FOR FOOD CHOICE BY GRASSHOPPERS 685 Table 1. Determination of soluble carbohydrates by HPAEC-PAD analyses and nitrogen content by Kjehldahl analysis. Sugars and nitrogen (mean ⴞ SEM) are expressed in mg.gⴚ1 for dry weight. Analyses were performed on 500-mg plant tissues of 5 samples of Ulex minor and 4 samples of each grass Flowers of Ulex minor (end of July) Flowers of Ulex minor (September) Stems of Ulex minor Shoots of Ulex minor Leaves of Agrostis tenuis Leaves of Holcus lanatus Glucose Fructose Sucrose Nitrogen 58.3 ⫾ 0.9 69.1 ⫾ 0.5 5.6 ⫾ 2.1 3.95 ⫾ 0.8 7 ⫾ 2.5 4.2 ⫾ 1.7 67.7 ⫾ 6.7 77.4 ⫾ 0.9 2.6 ⫾ 0.4 3 ⫾ 1.4 8.4 ⫾ 4.1 3.6 ⫾ 2 3.5 ⫾ 0.2 18.9 ⫾ 0.7 8 ⫾ 0.8 6.6 ⫾ 0.8 9.6 ⫾ 0.9 8.7 ⫾ 1 5.16 ⫾ 0.5 No data 9.6 ⫾ 1.2 9.8 ⫾ 0.6 No data No data 6.6 ⫾ 0.8 mg 䡠 g⫺1), representing ⬍10% of the quantity present in ßowers. The two species of grasses contained sugars in comparable amounts to stems of Ulex minor (Table 1). Ulex minor ßowers were low in nitrogen content (0.5Ð 0.6% of dry weight). Nitrogen made up ⬇1% of the dry weight of shoots and stems of Ulex minor. Sparteine Sensitivity. At concentrations between 0.5 and 2% sparteine, 25% of C. binotatus individuals were arrested by the piece of Þlter paper. For weaker concentrations, the percentages of responses clearly fall (12% of individuals for 0.1% sparteine). Consequently, the sparteine concentration used in interspeciÞc experiments was 1%. Among the 10 behaviors considered, four were of special interest because of statistical differences between species (Fig. 6): air palpation, arrestment on the piece of Þlter paper, leaving Þlter paper and regurgi- tation. It should be noticed that except one individual of C. albomarginatus, arrestment and leaving Þlter paper were never associated. C. binotatus differed from remaining species by three behaviors when sparteine is dropped on Þlter paper. C. binotatus palpated the air more than the other species (KruskalWallis test: H3 ⫽ 18.94, N ⫽ 57, P ⬍ 0.001). Arrestment on the piece of Þlter paper characterized C. binotatus (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 16.27, N ⫽ 57, P ⫽ 0.001). In the same way, C. binotatus was the only species never leaving Þlter paper (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 19.03, N ⫽ 57, P ⬍ 0.001). Otherwise, regurgitation characterized C. albomarginatus compared with other species (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 21.12, N ⫽ 57, P ⬍ 0.001). Within each species, we tested to determine whether there was a signiÞcant difference between the frequencies of each behavior in response to acetic Fig. 6. Frequency of different behaviors in response to 1% sparteine diluted in 1% acetic acid on Þlter paper (shaded boxes) or to 1% acetic acid alone (open boxes). Asterisks indicate signiÞcantly different values from all other species tested (Kruskal-Wallis test, P ⱕ 0.001 in each case). The circles indicate signiÞcant differences between frequencies of response to sparteine and the acetic acid control (Mann-Whitney U test, P ⬍ 5%) in each case), when both frequencies are not null. In the case of acetic acid alone, the behavior “no-response” is not represented. Pair: palpation of air; Palp: palpation of Þlter paper; Arrest: arrestment on Þlter paper; Leg: placing fore legs on Þlter paper and touching with mouth parts; Lab: touching Þlter paper with labrum; Bmas: biting and masticating; Wleg: withdrawing leg above Þlter paper; Reg: regurgitation; Leave: leaving Þlter paper; Jump: jumping in response to contact with Þlter paper. 686 ENVIRONMENTAL ENTOMOLOGY Vol. 32, no. 3 acid versus sparteine diluted in acetic acid. The responses of C. binotatus are generally stronger when sparteine is present (MannÐWhitney U test, P ⬍ 0.01 in each case), except for biting behavior. The behaviors related to avoidance (leaving Þlter paper, jumping, leg withdrawing, regurgitation) are generally expressed in graminivorous species when sparteine is present. Curiously, C. parallelus is the only graminivorous species palpitating Þlter paper with sparteine at a higher frequency than acetic acid (MannÐWhitney U test, P ⬍ 0.05). Responses of C. binotatus toward Lolium perenne Coated with Different Combinations of Solutions. The time spent on palpation of L. perenne before decision (consumption or rejection) is shown in Fig. 7a. The shortest time, i.e., the quickest decision corresponded to the combination of sucrose and sparteine. This time differs signiÞcantly from the one recorded with L. perenne coated with acetic acid only (ANOVA: F1,26 ⫽ 5.97, P ⫽ 0.02). Consumption frequency of Lolium perenne by C. binotatus (Fig. 7b) was enhanced by the same combination of sparteine ⫹ sucrose (Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 8.24, N ⫽ 69, P ⫽ 0.04). The effect of addition of sucrose or sparteine alone did not signiÞcantly differ from the test. Discussion Analysis of Food Choice. Chorthippus binotatus prefers Ulex minor, especially ßowers. Both C. albomarginatus and C. parallelus prefer Graminae to Ulex, whether blooming or not. Our experiments lead us to deÞne three feeding groups. The Þrst one is represented by C. binotatus, which specializes on Ulex minor, and the second one comprises C. albomarginatus and C. parallelus, which are “purely graminivorous.” The third group consists of C. biguttulus, which behaves like the purely graminivorous species when Ulex is not ßowering, but like the specialist species on Genisteae when Ulex is ßowering. We can call this species “atypically graminivorous.” Thus, both C. binotatus and C. biguttulus respond to Ulex minor ßowers, which probably contain one or more attractants. These observations are conÞrmed by measuring time to decision making. The two purely graminivorous species have the quickest reaction time for Graminae and the longest one for Ulex minor, whereas the specialist species on Genisteae shows the opposite behavior. Curiously, the “atypical graminivorous species” (C. biguttulus) is slow to make a decision, regardless of its choice (Ulex minor or Graminae) and has the broadest diet, accepting Ulex as well as grasses. This agrees with the Þndings of Bernays (1998, 2001) that the broader the food types accepted, the slower the response (assuming grasses to be rather uniform in secondary chemical compounds). This disadvantage is somewhat balanced by the fact that C. biguttulus has a larger habitat niche than either of the other species (Bellmann and Luquet 1993). Chorthippus binotatus is the species most responsive to sucrose, and it is as responsive to fructose as Fig. 7. Behavior of Chorthippus binotatus in response to Lolium perenne covered with different combinations of solutions diluted in acetic acid 1% (sucrose: 1 M, sparteine: 1%). (a) Mean time ⫾ SEM spent (seconds, log-transformed data) in palpation (global ANOVA: F3,52 ⫽ 3.41, P ⬍ 0.05). Different letters correspond to signiÞcant differences between treatments (TukeyÕs test: P ⬍ 0.05). (b) Consumption frequency. Combined data of each combination are compared with Kruskal-Wallis test: H3 ⫽ 8.24, N ⫽ 69, P ⬍ 0.05). Different letters correspond to signiÞcant different frequencies between treatments (Mann-Whitney U tests, P ⬍ 0.05). C. parallelus. Chorthippus biguttulus is most sensitive to glucose. As a result, sucrose and fructose are probably the major feeding stimulants, among sugars, on Ulex minor. The sugar analyses show that Ulex minor ßowers in September had the highest content of sugars. However, the attraction of C. binotatus to its host plant in July is not explained by sugar content. Presence of receptors that can detect sugars has been demonstrated in numerous insects (Mitchell 1981) and especially in grasshoppers (Bernays and Chapman 1994): the pyranose site is maximally sensitive to sucrose and glucose and the furanose site to fructose. Feeding on Ulex minor ßowers is partly explained by sensitivity of C. binotatus to sucrose and of June 2003 PICAUD ET AL.: DECISION MAKING FOR FOOD CHOICE BY GRASSHOPPERS C. biguttulus to glucose. Fructose does not play any role as the “strictly graminivorous” species are not attracted by Ulex minor ßowers. Sparteine alone leads to different behavioral responses according to the species. Leaving the substrate containing sparteine is a response characteristic for graminivorous species. C. albomarginatus regurgitates in response to sparteine. Contrary to the other species, C. binotatus palpates the air and remains on the substrate. Thus, sparteine is at least involved in the food selection of the insect species enabling it to feed on Genisteae. This situation can be compared with the phagostimulant power of pyrrolizidine alkaloids to Zonocerus elegans Thunberg (Boppré et al. 1984), although it is a polyphagous and opportunistic species. Our study on C. binotatus with Lolium perenne coated with different solutions showed that the combination of sparteine and sucrose is probably involved in food choice of C. binotatus. This is in agreement with our results that the highest consumption level was recorded in presence of the same combination, demonstrating its role as a phagostimulant. With respect to the plant architecture, we found that C. binotatus was attracted to the branchy drawing over the straight one. This can partly explain its preference to bushes versus grasses. However, plant architecture cannot be the only explanation for the feeding behavior of this species, because it is never attracted to other ßowering bushes in heathlands, e.g., Calluna vulgaris, Erica tetralix, and Ulex europaeus. Contrary to the results of Williams (1954) and Bernays and Chapman (1970), graminivorous species were not signiÞcantly attracted to a given architecture in our experiments. These authors used only three-dimensional lures and never drawings. Benefits and Disadvantages of Being a Specialist on Fabaceae Bushes. Chorthippus binotatus has evolved from graminivorous species. We hypothesize that the shift could have been the consequence of an evolved sensibility to sparteine (and/or other quinolizidine alkaloids) in the shoots and not to Genisteae ßowers because (1) young instars of the subspecies C. b. binotatus are never exposed to ßowers, whereas (2) the adults of subspecies C. b. moralesii feed on both grasses and nonßowering Genisteae (Picaud et al. 1999). However, the diet shift within the genus Chorthippus has ecological consequences on nutrient balance, dispersal, and possibly predation risk. Grasses growing on rich soils in spring contain as much nitrogen as Genisteae shoots and stems. However, nitrogen concentration of grasses decreases with plant maturity (Chapuis and Lefeuvre 1980). Finally, Genisteae in the autumn are richer in nitrogen than grasses growing on the poor soils of heathlands (Rozé 1980, Glyphis and Puttick 1989). Consequently, feeding on Genisteae represents a gain in terms of nitrogen supply, at least when Ulex minor shoots and stems are consumed. However, when the specialist species shifts its diet toward U. minor ßowers, we assume that the nitrogen supply falls. If compensatory feeding occurs, C. binotatus should eat twice as much on ßowers as shoots. Nevertheless, the major advantage of this shift 687 should be an increase of Þtness because of the presence of carbohydrates. We hypothesize that the sugars present in ßowers favor the reproductive investment that occurs from the end of August until October, i.e., the main ßowering season. Graminivorous species have no constraint for dispersal as grasslands and hedges are connected. The occurrence of C. binotatus, however, is limited by the patchy distribution of Ulex minor heathlands. However, we showed that this specialist species can feed on Poaceae when there is no other available resource. Guéguen et al. (1975) found Poaceae phytolithes in C. binotatus faeces in Brittanny. It should be pointed out that when the species specializing on Genisteae feeds on Poaceae, its vigilance level probably decreases, leading to a predation risk (Dukas 1998). Thus, population migrations between relatively close heathlands are possible but certainly infrequent. It would be interesting to know the percentage of individuals able to complete their life cycle with Poaceae as the only food source. Our preliminary experiments (Picaud et al. 2002) suggest that this percentage is very low at best. If a few generations could develop on grass, it would allow the species to colonize distant heathlands islands. Genetic distances between populations should be measured to estimate gene ßow to address whether these populations are part of one or several metapopulations, and thus the extinction risk of each population. Acknowledgments We are grateful to Cécile Nouhaut for her help in translation, and A. Bellet, S. Verot, and I. Vannier for their technical assistance. References Cited Abrams, P. A., and O. J. Schmitz. 1999. The effect of risk of mortality on the foraging behaviour of animals faced with time and digestive capacity constraints. Evol. Ecol. Res. 1: 285Ð301. [AOAC] Association of Official Agricultural Chemist. 1984. OfÞcial Methods of Analysis. A.O.A.C., Washington, DC. Bellmann, H., and G. Luquet. 1993. Guide des Sauterelles, Grillons et Criquets dÕEurope occidentale. Delachaux et Niestlé, Lausanne. Bernays, E. A. 1998. The value of being a resource specialist: behavioral support for a neural hypothesis. Am. Nat. 151: 451Ð 464. Bernays, E. A. 2001. Neural limitation in phytophagous insects: implications for diet breath and evolution of host afÞliation. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46: 703Ð727. Bernays, E. A., and K. L. Bright. 2001. Food choice causes interrupted feeding in the generalist grasshopper Schistocerca americana: further evidence for inefÞcient decision-making. J. Insect Physiol. 47: 63Ð 41. Bernays, E. A., and R. F. Chapman. 1970. Experiments to determine the basis of food selection by Chorthippus parallelus (Zetterstedt) (Orthoptera, Acrididae) in the Þeld. J. Anim. Ecol. 39: 761Ð776. Bernays, E. A., and R. F. Chapman. 1994. Host plant selection by phytophagous insects. Chapman & Hall, New York. 688 ENVIRONMENTAL ENTOMOLOGY Bonnet, E. 1996. Phylogénie et partage des ressources chez les Orthoptères Acridiens. Thèse doctorat, Université de Limoges, Limoges, France. Boppré, M., U. Seibt, and W. Wickler. 1984. Pharmacophagy in grasshoppers? Entomol. Exp. Appl. 35: 115Ð117. Brugel, E., L. Brunerye, and A. Vilks. 2001. Plantes et végétation en Limousin. Atlas de la ßore vasculaire. Conservatoire Régional des Espaces Naturels, St-Gence, France. Bruneton, J. 1999. Pharmacognosie, Phytochimie, Plantes médicinales. Techniques et Documentation, Paris. Cataldi T. R., Campa, C., Casella, I. G., and S. A. Bufo. 1999. Determination of maltitol, isomaltitol, and lactitol by high-pH anion-exchange chromatography with pulsed amperometric detection. J. Agric. Food Chem. 47: 157Ð 163. Chapman, R. F. 1982. Chemoreception: the signiÞcance of receptor numbers. Adv. Insect Physiol. 16: 247Ð356. Chapman, R. F., and A. Joern. 1990. Biology of grasshoppers. Wiley, New York. Chapman, R. F., E. A. Bernays, and T. Wyatt. 1988. Chemical aspect of host-plant speciÞcity in three Larrea-feeding grasshoppers. J. Chem. Ecol. 14: 561Ð579. Chapuis J. L., and J. C. Lefeuvre. 1980. Evolution saisonnière du régime alimentaire du Lapin de Garenne, Oryctolagus cuniculus (L.), en lande: résultats de deux ans dÕanalyses. Bull. Ecol. 11: 587Ð597. Defaut, B. 1999. Synopsis des Orthoptères de France. Association pour la Caractérisation et lÕétude des Entomocénoses. Bédeilhac, France. Dukas, R. 1998. Constraints on information processing and their effects on behaviour, pp. 89 Ð128. In R. Dukas [ed.], Cognitive ecology: The evolutionary ecology of information processing and decision making. Chicago University Press, Chicago, IL. Gangwere, S. K., and E. Morales-Agacino. 1973. Food selection and feeding behaviour in iberian Orthopteroidea. An. Inst. Nacl. Inv. Agr., Serie Pr. Veg. 3: 251Ð337. Glyphis, J. P., and G. M. Puttick. 1989. Phenolics, nutrition and insect herbivory in some garrigue and maquis plant species. Oecologia (Berl.) 78: 259 Ð263. Grassé, P. P. 1929. Etudes écologiques et biogéographiques sur les Orthoptères français. Bull. Biol. Entomol. Fr. Belg. 63: 489 Ð539. Guéguen, A., A. Chenouvrier, and J. C. Lefeuvre. 1975. Essai de détermination du régime alimentaire dÕanimaux herbivores à lÕaide des phytolithes siliceux des Graminées Vol. 32, no. 3 et des Cypéraces. 2. Application à lÕétude du régime alimentaire des Orthoptères Acridiens. C.R.A.S., Paris 281 (D): 929 Ð932. Harisson, J., J. A. Gallagher, and C. J. Pollock. 1997. A simple and rapid method for the analysis of water-soluble carbohydrates from small segments of cereal leaf tissue. J. Plant Physiol. 151: 654 Ð 659. Kruseman, G. 1982. Matériaux pour la faunistique des Orthoptères de France. Fasc II. Les acridiens des Musées de Paris et dÕAmsterdam. Instituut voor taxonomische zoölogie (Zoölogish Museum), Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Mitchell, R. 1981. Insect behaviour, resource exploitation, and Þtness. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 26: 373Ð396. Mossot, M., and D. Petit. 2000. Présence de Chorthippus binotatus binotatus (Charpentier, 1825) dans le Vaucluse (Orth., Acrididae). Bull. Soc. Entomol. Fr. 105: 251Ð252. Otte, D., and A. Joern. 1977. On feeding pattern in desert grasshoppers and the evolution of specialized diets. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 128: 89 Ð126. Picaud, F., C. Nanthieras, I. Coudon, A. Bellet, and D. Petit. 1999. Graminivory abandonment gradation among two subspecies of Chorthippus binotatus (Acrididae, Gomphocerinae): descriptive and experimental approaches. Ann. Soc. Entomol. Fr. 35: 242Ð247. Picaud, F., Gloaguen, V. and D. Petit. 2002. Mechanistic aspects of feeding preference in Chorthippus binotatus (Acrididae, Gomphocerinae). J. Insect Behav. 15: 513Ð 526. Rozé, F. 1980. Répartition et évolution de lÕazote dans quelques landes bretonnes. Bull. Ecol. 11: 315Ð321. SPSS Inc. 1997. SYSTAT ver. 7, The system for statistics. SysStat Inc., Evanston, IL. Uvarov, B. P. 1977. Grasshoppers and locustsÑA handbook of general acridology, vol. 2. Centre for Overseas Pest Research, London, England. White, P. R., and R. F. Chapman. 1990. Olfactory sensitivity of gomphocerine grasshoppers to the odours of host and non-host plants. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 55: 205Ð212. Williams, L. H. 1954. The feeding habits and food preferences of Acrididae and the factors which determine them. Trans. Royal Entomol. Soc. Lond. 105: 423Ð 454. Received for publication 1 April 2002; accepted 18 February 2003.